Sunteți pe pagina 1din 222

NEUROLINGUISTICS

daniele.panizza@gmail.com
NEUROLINGUISTICS
NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course


NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)


NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)


NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture


NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture

ask ques4ons!
interrupt me any 4me you don’t understand something
NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture

ask ques4ons!
interrupt me any 4me you don’t understand something

relevant informa4on about me


NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture

ask ques4ons!
interrupt me any 4me you don’t understand something

relevant informa4on about me


psycholinguist (eye-tracking, ERPs, acquisi4on)
NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture

ask ques4ons!
interrupt me any 4me you don’t understand something

relevant informa4on about me


psycholinguist (eye-tracking, ERPs, acquisi4on)
relevant informa4on about you
NEUROLINGUISTICS

relevant informa4on about the course

program is nego4able (I’m at your service)

provide feedback (I’m at your service)

slides and readings will be uploaded a"er or before every lecture

ask ques4ons!
interrupt me any 4me you don’t understand something

relevant informa4on about me


psycholinguist (eye-tracking, ERPs, acquisi4on)
relevant informa4on about you

introduce yourself
NEUROLINGUISTICS

papers & textbooks

and maybe others…


what is

NEUROLINGUISTICS
?
what is

NEUROLINGUISTICS
?
Neurolinguistics is the study of the neural mechanisms in
the human brain that control the comprehension, production,
and acquisition of language. As an interdisciplinary field,
neurolinguistics draws methods and theories from fields such
as neuroscience, linguistics, cognitive science, neurobiology,
communication disorders, neuropsychology, and computer
science.
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?

Alexander Luria (‘50s)

Harry Whittaker
(Brain & Language, 1974)
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

Alexander Luria (‘50s)

Harry Whittaker
(Brain & Language, 1974)
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language

Alexander Luria (‘50s)

Harry Whittaker
(Brain & Language, 1974)
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

Harry Whittaker
(Brain & Language, 1974)
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

how language is implemented in the central nervous system

Harry Whittaker
(Brain & Language, 1974)
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

how language is implemented in the central nervous system

perception = hearing/reading
Harry Whittaker
production = speaking (Brain & Language, 1974)

understanding = semantics, concepts


what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

how language is implemented in the central nervous system

perception = hearing/reading
Harry Whittaker
production = speaking (Brain & Language, 1974)

understanding = semantics, concepts

2. NEURO - LINGUISTICS
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

how language is implemented in the central nervous system

perception = hearing/reading
Harry Whittaker
production = speaking (Brain & Language, 1974)

understanding = semantics, concepts

phonology/morphology
syntax
2. NEURO - LINGUISTICS
(formal) semantics
pragmatics
what is NEUROLINGUISTICS ?
two ways to conceive this term

1. neuroscience of language
Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.
Alexander Luria (‘50s)

how language is implemented in the central nervous system

perception = hearing/reading
Harry Whittaker
production = speaking (Brain & Language, 1974)

understanding = semantics, concepts

phonology/morphology
syntax
2. NEURO - LINGUISTICS
(formal) semantics
pragmatics
we will do a li-le bit of both
overview of the course 13 classes + final exam

tenta1ve program
lessons 1-2 lesson 5-6-7
SYNTAX in the brain (fMRI)
warming-up & intro
SEMANTICS and CONCEPTS (fMRI)
history, morphology of the brain
PRAGMATICS (fMRI)
physiology of the nervous system
methods in neuroscience lessons 8
NEUROLINGUISTICS electrified
lessons 3-4 (EEG and MEG studies)
neuroscience of language syntax (P600) and seman4cs (N400)
parsing and on-line sentence processing
hearing, reading, wri4ng the N400 debate
neuroimaging lessons 9-10
phonology
formal neuroseman4cs (nega4on,
composi4on, modals, nega4ve
polarity items)
pragma4cs & implicatures
what is the brain?
what is the brain?
what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans
what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of…


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)
what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)

Galen (150 ad)


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)

Galen (150 ad) observa4on of lesions in injured roman gladiators


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)

Galen (150 ad) observa4on of lesions in injured roman gladiators


nerves project to and from the brain
what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)

Galen (150 ad) observa4on of lesions in injured roman gladiators


nerves project to and from the brain

mental experiences reside in the…


what is the brain?

Aristotle (350 bc)

the ra4o of brain size to body size was greatest in more


intellectually advanced species, such as humans

cogni4on as product of… HEART!

BRAIN as cooling system


(the smarter you are, the more cooling you need)

Galen (150 ad) observa4on of lesions in injured roman gladiators


nerves project to and from the brain

mental experiences reside in the… VENTRICLES!


Vesalius (1500)
Anterior

how the brain looks..


commissure
Midbrain
Cerebellum
Brainstem Pons
Medulla Spinal cord

(E) (F)
Corpus Corpus Thalamus Basal ganglia
Internal
Cerebral cortex callosum capsule Lateral callosum
ventricle Caudate
(gray matter) Putamen
White Globus
matter Internal pallidus
Caudate capsule

Third
ventricle
Putamen
Tail of
caudate
nucleus

Temporal Lateral
lobe ventricle
(temporal
Optic
horn) Mammillary
Anterior chiasm
commissure Basal forebrain body
Amygdala nuclei Hippocampus Fornix
how the brain looks..
how the brain looks..

Connec&ng the Hemispheres:


Grey vs White Ma.er Corpus Callosum
brain: reference system
brain: reference system

Superior (above)

Anterior Posterior
(in front of) (behind)

Inferior (below)
Left Right

Lateral Medial Lateral

Posterior
(behind)
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?


brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?


brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal

ventral
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal

caudal

ventral
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal

rostral caudal

ventral
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal

rostral caudal

anterior

ventral
brain: reference system

which one is… dorsal? ventral? caudal? rostral? anterior? posterior?

dorsal

posterior
rostral caudal

anterior

ventral
brain: cortex
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)

highly convoluted folded sheet


brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)

highly convoluted folded sheet


why?
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)

highly convoluted folded sheet


why? because for evolu4onal purposes we need do stuff
more neurons into a non-expanding skull
(the benefits are manifold)
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)

highly convoluted folded sheet


why? because for evolu4onal purposes we need do stuff
more neurons into a non-expanding skull
(the benefits are manifold)

gyrus/giri = bumps
brain: cortex

cortex is grey (grey mager)


it contains cell bodies (astrocytes & neurons)
it represents the computa4onal power of the brain (lots of computers)

highly convoluted folded sheet


why? because for evolu4onal purposes we need do stuff
more neurons into a non-expanding skull
(the benefits are manifold)

gyrus/giri = bumps
sulcus/sulci = grooves
cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


cortex morphology
Central Parietal
sulcus lobe
Frontal Occipital
lobe lobe

Sylvian
fissure

Temporal lobe Cerebellum


what do you know about the brain?

what is the length of an adult’s brain?

14 cm 8 cm

24 cm 20 cm
what do you know about the brain?

what is the length of an adult’s brain?

14 cm 8 cm

24 cm 20 cm
what do you know about the brain?

how many neurons are there in a brain?

100 billion 455 million

20 billion 84 billion
what do you know about the brain?

how many neurons are there in a brain?

100 billion 455 million

20 billion 84 billion
what do you know about the brain?

which animal has the biggest brain?

whale elephant

dolphin man
it’s not the size that matters!
it’s not the size that matters!
it’s not the size that matters!
some brains..
what do you know about the brain?

which animal has the higher brain-to-body ratio ?

whale ant

dolphin man
what do you know about the brain?
what do you know about the brain?

hgp://io9.gizmodo.com/5890414/the-4-biggest-myths-about-the-human-brain

To circumvent the limitations of a basic, brain-to-body mass ratio, researchers devised a more complex measurement, known as the
"encephalization quotient" (or "EQ"), that measures the ratio of an animal's brain and body size relative to other, similarly sized animals. In
doing so, EQ not only takes into account that brain size tends to increase with body size, but that brain size does not necessarily increase at
the same rate as body size.
neuron
26 CHAPTER 2 • NEURONS AND GLIA

The Golgi Stain


The Nissl stain, however, does not tell the whole story. A Nissl-stained neu-
ron looks like little more than a lump of protoplasm containing a nucleus.
Neurons are much more than that, but how much more was not recog-
nized until the publication of the work of Italian histologist Camillo Golgi
(Figure 2.2). In 1873, Golgi discovered that by soaking brain tissue in a sil-
ver chromate solution, now called the Golgi stain, a small percentage of
neurons became darkly colored in their entirety (Figure 2.3). This revealed
that the neuronal cell body, the region of the neuron around the nucleus
that is shown with the Nissl stain, is actually only a small fraction of the
total structure of the neuron. Notice in Figures 2.1 and 2.3 how different
histological stains can provide strikingly different views of the same tissue.
Today, neurohistology remains an active field in neuroscience, along with
its credo: “The gain in brain is mainly in the stain.”
The Golgi stain shows that neurons have at least two distinguishable
parts: a central region that contains the cell nucleus, and numerous thin
tubes that radiate away from the central region. The swollen region con-
FIGURE 2.2 taining the cell nucleus has several names that are used interchangeably:
Camillo Golgi (1843–1926). cell body, soma (plural: somata), and perikaryon (plural: perikarya). The
(Source: Finger, 1994, Fig. 3.22.) thin tubes that radiate away from the soma are called neurites and are of
two types: axons and dendrites (Figure 2.4).
The cell body usually gives rise to a single axon. The axon is of uniform
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

soaked brain 4ssue in a silver chromate solu4on: neurons in their beauty Soma
diameter throughout its length, and
extend at right angles. Because axons

neuron body (a meter or more), it was imme


of the day that axons must act like
neurons. Dendrites, on the other han
26 CHAPTER 2 • NEURONS AND GLIA

The Golgi Stain


The Nissl stain, however, does not tell the whole story. A Nissl-stained neu-
ron looks like little more than a lump of protoplasm containing a nucleus.
Neurons are much more than that, but how much more was not recog-
nized until the publication of the work of Italian histologist Camillo Golgi
(Figure 2.2). In 1873, Golgi discovered that by soaking brain tissue in a sil-
ver chromate solution, now called the Golgi stain, a small percentage of
neurons became darkly colored in their entirety (Figure 2.3). This revealed
that the neuronal cell body, the region of the neuron around the nucleus
that is shown with the Nissl stain, is actually only a small fraction of the
total structure of the neuron. Notice in Figures 2.1 and 2.3 how different
histological stains can provide strikingly different views of the same tissue.
Today, neurohistology remains an active field in neuroscience, along with
its credo: “The gain in brain is mainly in the stain.”
The Golgi stain shows that neurons have at least two distinguishable
parts: a central region that contains the cell nucleus, and numerous thin
tubes that radiate away from the central region. The swollen region con-
FIGURE 2.2 taining the cell nucleus has several names that are used interchangeably:
Camillo Golgi (1843–1926). cell body, soma (plural: somata), and perikaryon (plural: perikarya). The
(Source: Finger, 1994, Fig. 3.22.) thin tubes that radiate away from the soma are called neurites and are of
two types: axons and dendrites (Figure 2.4).
The cell body usually gives rise to a single axon. The axon is of uniform
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

FIGURE 2.3
Golgi-stained neurons. (Source: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

soaked brain 4ssue in a silver chromate solu4on: neurons in their beauty Soma
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

Soma

Dendrites Neurites
Axon

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma

Dendrites Neurites
Axon

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

Dendrites Neurites
Axon

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

body = soma

Dendrites Neurites
Axon

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

body = soma

Dendrites Neurites
neurites Axon

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

body = soma

Dendrites Neurites
neurites Axon

denrites (several): INPUT

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

body = soma

Dendrites Neurites
neurites Axon

denrites (several): INPUT


axon (one): OUTPUT

: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
diameter throughout its length, and if it branches, the branches generally
extend at right angles. Because axons can travel over great distances in the
neuron
body (a meter or more), it was immediately recognized by the histologists
of the day that axons must act like “wires” that carry the output of the
neurons. Dendrites, on the other hand, rarely extend more than 2 mm in

neurons are cells

func4ons: process informa4on


Soma
transmit informa4on

body = soma

Dendrites Neurites
neurites Axon

denrites (several): INPUT


axon (one): OUTPUT
(white mager
contains axons!)
: Hubel, 1988, p. 126.)

FIGURE 2.4
The basic parts of a neuron.
bipolar, and if there are three or more, the cell is multipolar. Most neurons
in the brain are multipolar.
neuron
Classification Based on Dendrites
Dendritic trees can vary widely from one type of neuron to another. Some
have inspired elegant names like “double bouquet cells.” Others have less
interesting names, such as “alpha cells.” Classification is often unique to a
types of neurons
particular part of the brain. For example, in the cerebral cortex (the struc-
ture that lies just under the surface of the cerebrum), there are two broad
classes: stellate cells (star-shaped) and pyramidal cells (pyramid-shaped)
(Figure 2.20).

Soma

Unipolar

Multipolar

Bipolar

FIGURE 2.19
Classification of neurons based on the number of neurites.
bipolar, and if there are three or more, the cell is multipolar. Most neurons
in the brain are multipolar.
neuron
Classification Based on Dendrites 46 CHAPTER 2 • NEURONS AND GLIA

Dendritic trees can vary widely from one type of neuron to another. Some
have inspired elegant names like “double bouquet cells.” Others have less Another simple
dendrites have spin
interesting names, such as “alpha cells.” Classification is often unique to a not are called aspin

types of neurons
particular part of the brain. For example, in the cerebral cortex (the struc- For example, in th
cells, on the other
ture that lies just under the surface of the cerebrum), there are two broad
classes: stellate cells (star-shaped) and pyramidal cells (pyramid-shaped) Classification B
(Figure 2.20). Information is deliv
rites in the sensory
the eye. Cells with t
Other neurons hav
Soma mand movements;
the nervous system
to this classification

Stellate cell
Classification B
Some neurons hav
Unipolar the other; these are
neurons have short
body; these are cal
cerebral cortex, for
extend to other pa
In contrast, stellate
cortex and are ther

Classification B
The classification sc
Multipolar neurons as reveale
scientists to identify
resulted in a schem
example, the motor
the neurotransmitte
also classified as ch
Bipolar transmitter. Collect
up the brain’s neur
FIGURE 2.19
Classification of neurons based on the number of neurites. ▼ GLIA
We have devoted
While this decision
roscientists conside
day, they suppose,
tantly to informatio
At present, howev
function mainly by
be subordinate, wit
Pyramidal cell
neuron: synapse

INTRODUCING THE BRAI

A neuron KEY TERM


Neuron
A type of cell tha
up the nervous s
and supports, am
other things, cog
function.
Cell body
Part of the neuro
containing the n
and other organe
Dendrites
Branching struct
carry information
other neurons.
Neurons consist of three basic features: a cell body, dendrites that receive information and Axon
neuron: synapse

1. electrical current propagates down the axon


INTRODUCING THE BRAI

A neuron KEY TERM


Neuron
A type of cell tha
up the nervous s
and supports, am
other things, cog
function.
Cell body
Part of the neuro
containing the n
and other organe
Dendrites
Branching struct
carry information
other neurons.
Neurons consist of three basic features: a cell body, dendrites that receive information and Axon
1. Microtubules do not extend into the terminal.
neuron: synapse 2. The terminal contains numerous small bubbles of membrane, called
synaptic vesicles, that measure about 50 nm in diameter.
3. The inside surface of the membrane that faces the synapse has a partic-
ularly dense covering of proteins.
4. It has numerous mitochondria, indicating a high energy demand.

1. electrical current
propagates down the axon

Presynaptic
axon terminal

Mitochondria
reaches the synap;c bu=on Synapse

Synaptic FIG
vesicles Th
te
or
im
na
Postsynaptic dendrite fro
Synaptic Ne
cleft re
Receptors of
syn
1. Microtubules do not extend into the terminal.
neuron: synapse 2. The terminal contains numerous small bubbles of membrane, called
synaptic vesicles, that measure about 50 nm in diameter.
3. The inside surface of the membrane that faces the synapse has a partic-
ularly dense covering of proteins.
4. It has numerous mitochondria, indicating a high energy demand.

1. electrical current
propagates down the axon

Presynaptic
axon terminal

Mitochondria
reaches the synap;c bu=on Synapse

small space between


Synaptic FIG
vesicles Th
te
AXON or
im
na
Postsynaptic dendrite
DENDRITES / SOMA Synaptic
fro
Ne
cleft re
Receptors of
syn
neuron: synapse

end of an axon beginning of a dendrite


(presynap1c) (postsynap1c)
neuron: synapse

end of an axon beginning of a dendrite


(presynap1c) (postsynap1c)
2. ac4on poten4al
reaches the axon terminal
neuron: synapse

end of an axon beginning of a dendrite


(presynap1c) (postsynap1c)
2. ac4on poten4al
reaches the axon terminal

3. chemicals are
released into the
synap4c clem
(neurotransmigers)
neuron: synapse

end of an axon beginning of a dendrite


(presynap1c) (postsynap1c)
2. ac4on poten4al
reaches the axon terminal

3. chemicals are
released into the
synap4c clem
(neurotransmigers)

4. neurotransmigers
bind to the receptors
in the postsynap4c
dentrites or soma
neuron: synapse

1.Neurotransmitters

3. Electrical

2. Electrical
neuron: synapse

5. synap4c poten4al is conducted passively (without ac4on


poten4al)

1.Neurotransmitters

3. Electrical

2. Electrical
neuron: synapse

5. synap4c poten4al is conducted passively (without ac4on


poten4al)

6. if passive currents are sufficiently strong they cause an ac4on


poten4al in the neuron

1.Neurotransmitters

3. Electrical

2. Electrical
neuron: synapse

5. synap4c poten4al is conducted passively (without ac4on


poten4al)

6. if passive currents are sufficiently strong they cause an ac4on


poten4al in the neuron

7. dendrite poten4als sum up (passive current short ranged)

1.Neurotransmitters

3. Electrical

2. Electrical
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

(A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
25 Presynaptic
Connexons neuron

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

−50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
25 Presynaptic
Connexons neuron

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

−50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

−50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

−50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

−50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) −50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al
(A) Presynaptic (B)
cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


Postsynaptic 25
cell membrane 3.5 nm Postsynaptic
20 nm neuron
0
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25
neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay
0 1 2 3 4
Time (ms)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
influenced by excitatory or inhibitory forces, PASSIVE (does not regenerate)
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
influenced by excitatory or inhibitory forces, PASSIVE (does not regenerate)
it is gradable/con4nuous
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
influenced by excitatory or inhibitory forces, PASSIVE (does not regenerate)
it is gradable/con4nuous short-ranged
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
influenced by excitatory or inhibitory forces, PASSIVE (does not regenerate)
it is gradable/con4nuous short-ranged
benefits of the sum of all surrounding poten4als
time, thus facilitating a burst of hormone secretion into the circulation. The nels connect to one another, creating
fact that gap junction pores are large enough to allow molecules such as ATP electrical continuity between the two

neuron: synapse
and second messengers to diffuse intercellularly also permits electrical syn- cells. (B) Rapid transmission of signals
apses to coordinate the intracellular signaling and metabolism of coupled at an electrical synapse in the crayfish.
An action potential in the presynaptic
cells. This property may be particularly important for glial cells, which form
neuron causes the postsynaptic neuron
large intracellular signaling networks via their gap junctions. to be depolarized within a fraction of a
millisecond. (B after Furshpan and Pot-
ter, 1959.)

pre-synap4c poten4al (A) Presynaptic (B)


cell membrane
AXON 0 or 1 Connexons 25 Presynaptic
neuron

0
propagates as electric current along the axon

Membrane potential (mV)


−25

ACTIVE (regenerates) exploits myelin (electric isola4on) −50

reaches dendrites or somas


triggers the release of Postsynaptic
neurotransmigers (few
cell membrane
types,
3.5 nm
25
Postsynaptic
20 nm
eccitatory or inhibitory) 0 neuron
Pores connecting
cytoplasm of two −25

long-ranged (e.g. from motor area to spine chord)


neurons
−50
Brief (~0.1 ms)
synaptic delay

post-synap4c poten4al 0 1 2
Time (ms)
3 4

DENDRITES or SOMA
influenced by excitatory or inhibitory forces, PASSIVE (does not regenerate)
it is gradable/con4nuous short-ranged
benefits of the sum of all surrounding poten4als
is triggered by ion-channels bound to receptors (many types)
neuron: action potential

.qrk(75-100).ps 11/30/05 1:23 PM Page 83

▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

neuronal membrane at res1ng state electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium in
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m
o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


neuron: action potential

each neuron is surrounded by a cell membrane that acts as a barrier to


the passage of chemicals (Na+ = sodium, K+ = potassium)
.qrk(75-100).ps 11/30/05 1:23 PM Page 83

▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

neuronal membrane at res1ng state electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium in
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m
o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


neuron: action potential

each neuron is surrounded by a cell membrane that acts as a barrier to


the passage of chemicals (Na+ = sodium, K+ = potassium)

res4ng poten4al = -70mV (inside nega4ve, outside posi4ve)


.qrk(75-100).ps 11/30/05 1:23 PM Page 83

▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

neuronal membrane at res1ng state electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium in
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m
o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


neuron: action potential

each neuron is surrounded by a cell membrane that acts as a barrier to


the passage of chemicals (Na+ = sodium, K+ = potassium)

res4ng poten4al = -70mV (inside nega4ve, outside posi4ve)


.qrk(75-100).ps 11/30/05 1:23 PM Page 83

ion channels (= voltage gates) are found only in axons:


generate ac4on poten4al!

▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

neuronal membrane at res1ng state electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium in
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m
o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


neuron: action potential
▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) depolariza1on (posi1ve current in) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
Na K
in

K+ K+
V
m o

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
(b)
neuron: action potential
▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

if passive current is sufficiently strong:


g >> g
K Na
Outside
Sodium Potassium
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) depolariza1on (posi1ve current in) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
Na K
in

K+ K+
V
m o

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
(b)
neuron: action potential
▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

if passive current is sufficiently strong:


g >> g
K Na
Outside Na+ channelsSodium
open (posi4ve electric
Potassium flux inside the cell)
cell
K+ channel K+ channel

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) depolariza1on (posi1ve current in) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
Na K
in

K+ K+
V
m o

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
(b)
neuron: action potential
▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

if passive current is sufficiently strong:


g >> g
K Na
Outside Na+ channelsSodium
open (posi4ve electric
Potassium flux inside the cell)
cell
K+ channel K+ channel
voltage reaches -50mV
+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a) depolariza1on (posi1ve current in) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
Na K
in

K+ K+
V
m o

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
(b)
neuron: action potential
▼ THE ACTION POTENTIAL, IN REALITY

if passive current is sufficiently strong:


g >> g
K Na
Outside Na+ channelsSodium
open (posi4ve electric
Potassium flux inside the cell)
cell
K+ channel K+ channel
voltage reaches -50mV
+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + V
m
at this point the membrane becomes completely permeable:
CHARGE
– – – – – – – INSIDE
– – –ABOUT
– – –TO– CHANGE
– – – –RAPIDLY

(ac4on Kpoten4al)
+ K+
Inside – 80 mV
cell

(a) depolariza1on (posi1ve current in) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
Na K
in

K+ K+
V
m o

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
(b)
V
m

neuron: action potential


– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a)
g >> g
Na K

K+ K+
V
m

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) posi1ve charge inside the cell (maximum spike) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux in

K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m
o

+ + + + + + +

– 80 mV

(c) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


V
m

neuron: action potential


– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a)
voltage goes beyond 0 mV (posi4ve charge)
g >> g
Na K

K+ K+
V
m

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) posi1ve charge inside the cell (maximum spike) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux in

K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m
o

+ + + + + + +

– 80 mV

(c) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


V
m

neuron: action potential


– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a)
voltage goes beyond 0 mV (posi4ve charge)
g >> g
Na K

ACTION
K+ POTENTIAL! K+
V
m

– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) posi1ve charge inside the cell (maximum spike) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux in

K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m
o

+ + + + + + +

– 80 mV

(c) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


V
m

neuron: action potential


– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

Inside
K+ K+
– 80 mV
cell

(a)
voltage goes beyond 0 mV (posi4ve charge)
g >> g
Na K

ACTION
K+ POTENTIAL! K+
V
charge inside the cell is reversed (from neg to pos)m
– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) posi1ve charge inside the cell (maximum spike) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN

g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux in

K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m
o

+ + + + + + +

– 80 mV

(c) The action potential consists of a number of phases.


V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b)
g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux
K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m

+ + + + + + +

ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning


– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
K+ ion channels become open
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b)
g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux
K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m

+ + + + + + +

ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning


– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
K+ ion channels become open
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) nega4ve poten4al of the cell is restored via outward flow of K+


g >> g
K Na
Potassium efflux
K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m

+ + + + + + +

ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning


– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
K+ ion channels become open
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) nega4ve poten4al of the cell is restored via outward flow of K+


g >> g
charge inside the cell is reversed again (from pos to K Na
Potassium efflux
neg) K+ K+
– – – – – – –
V
m

+ + + + + + +

ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning


– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
K+ ion channels become open
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) nega4ve poten4al of the cell is restored via outward flow of K+


g >> g
charge inside the cell is reversed again (from pos to K Na
Potassium efflux
neg) K+ K+
– brief–hyperpolariza4on,
– – undershoot
– –(more nega4ve
– than at rest)
V
m

+ + + + + + +

ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning


– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
V
m

neuron: action potential


– –

Na+ Na+
K+ ion channels become open
Sodium influx – 80 mV

(b) nega4ve poten4al of the cell is restored via outward flow of K+


g >> g
charge inside the cell is reversed again (from pos to K Na
Potassium efflux
neg) K+ K+
– brief–hyperpolariza4on,
– – undershoot
– –(more nega4ve
– than at rest)
V
m

prevents the ac4on poten4al from traveling back


+ (cannot
+ depolarize
+ again
+ straight
+ away,
+ refractory
+ period)
ion pumps restore the gradients of K+ and Na+ (burning
– 80 mV glucose through ATP)

(c) polariza1on of the cell (coming back to res1ng state) electric poten1al INTRODUCIN
g >> g
K Na

K+ K+
in

+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
V
m o

– – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

K+ K+
– 80 mV

(d) Time
The action potential consists of a number of phases.
2. The substance must be released in exogenous transmitter mimics the post- neurotransmitters.

neuron: action potential


response to presynaptic depolarization, and
the release must be Ca2+-dependent.

Demonstrating the identity of a neurotransmitter at a synapse requires showing (1) its pres-
ence, (2) its release, and (3) the postsynaptic presence of specific receptors.

(1) (2) (3)

Action
potential
1 Neuro-
transmitter Presynaptic
present terminal

Application of
transmitter, agonists,
or antagonists
Ca2+
Ca2+

2 Neurotransmitter 3 Neurotransmitter
Postsynaptic cell
released receptors activated
2. The substance must be released in exogenous transmitter mimics the post- neurotransmitters.

neuron: action potential


response to presynaptic depolarization, and
the release must be Ca2+-dependent.

Demonstrating the identity of a neurotransmitter at a synapse requires showing (1) its pres-
ence, (2) its release, and (3) the postsynaptic presence of specific receptors.

(1) (2) (3)

Action
potential
1 Neuro-
transmitter Presynaptic
present terminal

Application of
transmitter, agonists,
or antagonists
Ca2+
Ca2+

2 Neurotransmitter 3 Neurotransmitter
Postsynaptic cell
released receptors activated

transmigers are short-lived


2. The substance must be released in exogenous transmitter mimics the post- neurotransmitters.

neuron: action potential


response to presynaptic depolarization, and
the release must be Ca2+-dependent.

Demonstrating the identity of a neurotransmitter at a synapse requires showing (1) its pres-
ence, (2) its release, and (3) the postsynaptic presence of specific receptors.

(1) (2) (3)

Action
potential
1 Neuro-
transmitter Presynaptic
present terminal

Application of
transmitter, agonists,
or antagonists
Ca2+
Ca2+

2 Neurotransmitter 3 Neurotransmitter
Postsynaptic cell
released receptors activated

transmigers are short-lived


they are destroyed and recycled (reuptake)
2. The substance must be released in exogenous transmitter mimics the post- neurotransmitters.

neuron: action potential


response to presynaptic depolarization, and
the release must be Ca2+-dependent.

Demonstrating the identity of a neurotransmitter at a synapse requires showing (1) its pres-
ence, (2) its release, and (3) the postsynaptic presence of specific receptors.

(1) (2) (3)

Action
potential
1 Neuro-
transmitter Presynaptic
present terminal

Application of
transmitter, agonists,
or antagonists
Ca2+
Ca2+

2 Neurotransmitter 3 Neurotransmitter
Postsynaptic cell
released receptors activated

transmigers are short-lived


they are destroyed and recycled (reuptake)
otherwise, they would over-excite neuron (cf. cocaine)
neuron: action potential

neurotoxins
neuron: action potential

neurotoxins
fasciculins
blocks the Ach transmiger
in the receptor (cannot be broken
down by acetylchilinesterase (AChE)
(involuntary muscle contrac4on)
toxins are enough to inhibit a cholinergic synapse. Botu- phates. These are irreversible inhibitors of AChE, and by

neuron: action potential


linum toxins are extraordinarily specific enzymes that de- preventing the degradation of ACh, they probably kill their
stroy certain of the SNARE proteins in the presynaptic victims by causing a desensitization of ACh receptors.
terminals, which are critical for transmitter release (see The organophosphates used today as insecticides, like
Box 5.3). Ironically, this specific action of the toxins made parathion, are toxic to humans only in high doses.
them important tools in the early research on SNAREs.
Although its mechanism of action is different, black
widow spider venom also exerts deadly effects by affect-
neurotoxins ing transmitter release (Figure A). The venom first in-
creases, and then eliminates, ACh release at the neuro-
fasciculins muscular junction. Electron microscopic examination of
synapses poisoned with black widow spider venom re-
veals that the axon terminals are swollen and the synap-
blocks the Ach transmiger
tic vesicles are missing. The action of the venom, a pro-
tein molecule, is not entirely understood. Venom binds
in the receptor (cannot be broken
with proteins on the outside of the presynaptic mem-
brane, perhaps forming a membrane pore that depolarizes
down by acetylchilinesterase (AChE)
the terminal and allows Ca2! to enter and trigger rapid
and total depletion of transmitter. In some cases, the
(involuntary muscle contrac4on)
venom can induce transmitter release even without the
need for Ca2!.
The bite of the Taiwanese cobra also results in the
blockade of neuromuscular transmission in its victim, but
by yet another mechanism. One of the active compounds FIGURE A
in the snake’s venom, called "-bungarotoxin, is a peptide Black widow spiders. (Source: Matthews, 1995, p. 174.)
toxins are enough to inhibit a cholinergic synapse. Botu- phates. These are irreversible inhibitors of AChE, and by

neuron: action potential


linum toxins are extraordinarily specific enzymes that de- preventing the degradation of ACh, they probably kill their
stroy certain of the SNARE proteins in the presynaptic victims by causing a desensitization of ACh receptors.
terminals, which are critical for transmitter release (see The organophosphates used today as insecticides, like
Box 5.3). Ironically, this specific action of the toxins made parathion, are toxic to humans only in high doses.
them important tools in the early research on SNAREs.
Although its mechanism of action is different, black
widow spider venom also exerts deadly effects by affect-
neurotoxins ing transmitter release (Figure A). The venom first in-
creases, and then eliminates, ACh release at the neuro-
fasciculins muscular junction. Electron microscopic examination of
synapses poisoned with black widow spider venom re-
veals that the axon terminals are swollen and the synap-
blocks the Ach transmiger
tic vesicles are missing. The action of the venom, a pro-
tein molecule, is not entirely understood. Venom binds
in the receptor (cannot be broken
with proteins on the outside of the presynaptic mem-
brane, perhaps forming a membrane pore that depolarizes
down by acetylchilinesterase (AChE)
the terminal and allows Ca2! to enter and trigger rapid
and total depletion of transmitter. In some cases, the
(involuntary muscle contrac4on)
venom can induce transmitter release even without the
need for Ca2!.
The bite of the Taiwanese cobra also results in the
blockade of neuromuscular transmission in its victim, but
by yet another mechanism. One of the active compounds FIGURE A
in the snake’s venom, called "-bungarotoxin, is a peptide Black widow spiders. (Source: Matthews, 1995, p. 174.)

tetrodotoxins dentrotoxins

blocks the ion exchange


toxins are enough to inhibit a cholinergic synapse. Botu- phates. These are irreversible inhibitors of AChE, and by

neuron: action potential


linum toxins are extraordinarily specific enzymes that de- preventing the degradation of ACh, they probably kill their
stroy certain of the SNARE proteins in the presynaptic victims by causing a desensitization of ACh receptors.
terminals, which are critical for transmitter release (see The organophosphates used today as insecticides, like
Box 5.3). Ironically, this specific action of the toxins made parathion, are toxic to humans only in high doses.
them important tools in the early research on SNAREs.
Although its mechanism of action is different, black
widow spider venom also exerts deadly effects by affect-
neurotoxins ing transmitter release (Figure A). The venom first in-
creases, and then eliminates, ACh release at the neuro-
fasciculins muscular junction. Electron microscopic examination of
synapses poisoned with black widow spider venom re-
veals that the axon terminals are swollen and the synap-
blocks the Ach transmiger
tic vesicles are missing. The action of the venom, a pro-
tein molecule, is not entirely understood. Venom binds
in the receptor (cannot be broken
with proteins on the outside of the presynaptic mem-
brane, perhaps forming a membrane pore that depolarizes
down by acetylchilinesterase (AChE)
the terminal and allows Ca2! to enter and trigger rapid
and total depletion of transmitter. In some cases, the
(involuntary muscle contrac4on)
venom can induce transmitter release even without the
need for Ca2!.
The bite of the Taiwanese cobra also results in the
blockade of neuromuscular transmission in its victim, but
by yet another mechanism. One of the active compounds FIGURE A
in the snake’s venom, called "-bungarotoxin, is a peptide Black widow spiders. (Source: Matthews, 1995, p. 174.)

tetrodotoxins dentrotoxins

blocks the ion exchange


no polariza4on/depolariza4on
toxins are enough to inhibit a cholinergic synapse. Botu- phates. These are irreversible inhibitors of AChE, and by

neuron: action potential


linum toxins are extraordinarily specific enzymes that de- preventing the degradation of ACh, they probably kill their
stroy certain of the SNARE proteins in the presynaptic victims by causing a desensitization of ACh receptors.
terminals, which are critical for transmitter release (see The organophosphates used today as insecticides, like
Box 5.3). Ironically, this specific action of the toxins made parathion, are toxic to humans only in high doses.
them important tools in the early research on SNAREs.
Although its mechanism of action is different, black
widow spider venom also exerts deadly effects by affect-
neurotoxins ing transmitter release (Figure A). The venom first in-
creases, and then eliminates, ACh release at the neuro-
fasciculins muscular junction. Electron microscopic examination of
synapses poisoned with black widow spider venom re-
veals that the axon terminals are swollen and the synap-
blocks the Ach transmiger
tic vesicles are missing. The action of the venom, a pro-
tein molecule, is not entirely understood. Venom binds
in the receptor (cannot be broken
with proteins on the outside of the presynaptic mem-
brane, perhaps forming a membrane pore that depolarizes
down by acetylchilinesterase (AChE)
the terminal and allows Ca2! to enter and trigger rapid
and total depletion of transmitter. In some cases, the
(involuntary muscle contrac4on)
venom can induce transmitter release even without the
need for Ca2!.
The bite of the Taiwanese cobra also results in the
blockade of neuromuscular transmission in its victim, but
by yet another mechanism. One of the active compounds FIGURE A
in the snake’s venom, called "-bungarotoxin, is a peptide Black widow spiders. (Source: Matthews, 1995, p. 174.)

tetrodotoxins dentrotoxins

blocks the ion exchange


no polariza4on/depolariza4on
neuron: action potential
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary


neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)

where does the informa4on processing take place?


neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors

receptors are transmitter-gated ion


channels (flow of K+, Na+. Cl-)
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors

receptors are transmitter-gated ion


channels (flow of K+, Na+. Cl-)
they create a synaptic potential
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors

2. inside the neurons: they sum up the excitatory receptors are transmitter-gated ion
and inhibitory information from various inputs channels (flow of K+, Na+. Cl-)
(coming from action potentials) they create a synaptic potential
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors

2. inside the neurons: they sum up the excitatory receptors are transmitter-gated ion
and inhibitory information from various inputs channels (flow of K+, Na+. Cl-)
(coming from action potentials) they create a synaptic potential

excitatory neurotransmigers (ACh, DA): make the neuron more likely to fire as it is more
posi4ve than normal
neuron: action potential

the amplitude of ac4on poten4al does not vary

what varies is the frequency of the spikes (ac4on


poten4al per second = spiking rate)

spiking rate = informa4onal code (010101)


1. at the postsynaptic space: between
where does the informa4on processing take place? transmitters and receptors

2. inside the neurons: they sum up the excitatory receptors are transmitter-gated ion
and inhibitory information from various inputs channels (flow of K+, Na+. Cl-)
(coming from action potentials) they create a synaptic potential

excitatory neurotransmigers (ACh, DA): make the neuron more likely to fire as it is more
BEARc05.qrk(101-132).ps 11/30/05 1:27 PM Page 128

posi4ve than normal


inhibitory neurotransmigers (e.g. GABA): make the neuron less likely to fire as it is more nega4ve
than normal128 CHAPTER 5 • SYNAPTIC TRANSMISSION

Excitatory synapse Inhibitory synapse


(active) (inactive)

Dendrite
Soma

Axon hillock
Record Vm Record Vm
let’s take a brake

What do neurons use to talk to each other? 


let’s take a brake

What do neurons use to talk to each other? 

A cellular phone.
let’s take a brake hgp://neuroscien4stryangosling.tumblr.com
let’s take a brake hgp://neuroscien4stryangosling.tumblr.com
back to history…

1800
Phrenology

Gall (1800)
Phrenology

Gall (1800)

two assump4ons:
Phrenology

Gall (1800)

two assump4ons:
1. different regions of the brain perform different func4ons
and are associated with different behaviours
Phrenology

Gall (1800)

two assump4ons:
1. different regions of the brain perform different func4ons
and are associated with different behaviours

2. size of those regions produces distor4ons of the skulls


and correlates with individual differences in cogni4on and personality
Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)
Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)

experimental abla4on in birds


Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)

experimental abla4on in birds


provided experimental evidence that
cerebellum and brain have different func4ons
Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)

experimental abla4on in birds


provided experimental evidence that
cerebellum and brain have different func4ons

But:
Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)

experimental abla4on in birds


provided experimental evidence that
cerebellum and brain have different func4ons

But:

1. the skull is not correlated with


the shape of the brain
Holism VS. Phrenology

Flourens (1830)

experimental abla4on in birds


provided experimental evidence that
cerebellum and brain have different func4ons

But:

1. the skull is not correlated with


the shape of the brain

2. all the regions of the brain par4cipate


equally in all cerebral func4ons
(holis4c view)
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
generosity, secretiveness, and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers collected and carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head with personality traits was called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
community, they did capture the popular imagination of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
One of the most vociferous critics of phrenology was Flourens, the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
twosecretiveness,
generosity, pa4ents that could not speak
and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers collected and carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head with personality traits was called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
community, they did capture the popular imagination of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
One of the most vociferous critics of phrenology was Flourens, the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
twosecretiveness,
generosity, pa4ents that could not speak
and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head with personality traits was called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
community, they did capture the popular imagination of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
One of the most vociferous critics of phrenology was Flourens, the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
twosecretiveness,
generosity, pa4ents that could not speak
and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
but other func4ons were intact
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head(including language
with personality understanding)
traits was called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
community, they did capture the popular imagination of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
One of the most vociferous critics of phrenology was Flourens, the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
twosecretiveness,
generosity, pa4ents that could not speak
and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
but other func4ons were intact
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head(including language
with personality understanding)
traits was called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
the they
community, casedidofcapture
Tan (he could imagination
the popular say only ‘Tan’)
of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
One of the most vociferous critics of phrenology was Flourens, the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
twosecretiveness,
generosity, pa4ents that could not speak
and destructiveness, could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
but other func4ons were intact
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head(including language
with personality traits wasunderstanding)
called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
the they
community, casedidofcapture
Tan (he could imagination
the popular say only ‘Tan’)
of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
conclusion:
One of the most this regioncritics
vociferous is responsible for Flourens,
of phrenology was speaking the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
perform different functions. His grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
FIGURE 1.11 could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
studying the brain of a man who had lost frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
the faculty of speech after a brain lesion (see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ferent functions could be localized to different
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
ge 11 on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
two
generosity, pa4ents
secretiveness, that could not
and destructiveness, speak
could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
but other func4ons were intact
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head(including language
with personality traits wasunderstanding)
called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
the
community, case
they
▼ THE didof TanOF(he
capture
ORIGINS the could imagination
popular
NEUROSCIENCE
say only11‘Tan’)
of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
conclusion:
One of the most this regioncritics
vociferous is responsible for Flourens,
of phrenology was speaking the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
currents to a circumscribed region of perform different functions. Hissulcus
Central grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
dog could elicit discrete movements.
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
peated these experiments with mon- ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
al of this same region of the cerebrum phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
larly, German physiologist Hermann brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
presented evidence that the occipital The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
required for vision. firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ok, we now know that there is a very ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
um,FIGURE
with1.11different parts performing could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
ps studying
of thethefunctional divisions
brain of a man who had lost of the frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
te of
the those
faculty of produced by the
speech after a brain lesion phrenol-
(see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ke ferent
the functions
phrenologists, scientists
could be localized to differenttoday
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
What about all those bumps on the brain’s surface? Do they perform
Functional specialization: Broca ferent functions as well? The idea that they do was irresistible to a y
Austrian medical student named Franz Joseph Gall. Believing that bu
ge 11 on the surface of the skull reflect the bumps on the surface of the brain
proposed in 1809 that the propensity for certain personality traits, su
two
generosity, pa4ents
secretiveness, that could not
and destructiveness, speak
could be related to th
Broca (1860, but see Dax) mensions of the head (Figure 1.10). To support his claim, Gall and hi
lowers one hadandright
collected hemiplegia
carefully measured the skulls of hundreds of pe
representing the extensive range of personality types, from the very g
but other func4ons were intact
to the criminally insane. This new “science” of correlating the structu
the head(including language
with personality traits wasunderstanding)
called phrenology. Although the c
of the phrenologists were never taken seriously by the mainstream scie
the
community, case
they
▼ THE didof TanOF(he
capture
ORIGINS the could imagination
popular
NEUROSCIENCE
say only11‘Tan’)
of the time. In fa
textbook on phrenology published in 1827 sold over 100,000 copies.
conclusion:
One of the most this regioncritics
vociferous is responsible for Flourens,
of phrenology was speaking the
man who had shown experimentally that the cerebellum and cereb
currents to a circumscribed region of perform different functions. Hissulcus
Central grounds for criticism were sound. For
thing, the shape of the skull is not correlated with the shape of the b
dog could elicit discrete movements.
In addition, Flourens performed experimental ablations showing that
peated these experiments with mon- ticular traits are not isolated to the portions of the cerebrum specifie
al of this same region of the cerebrum phrenology. Flourens also maintained, however, that all regions of the
larly, German physiologist Hermann brum participate equally in all cerebral functions, a conclusion that
was shown to be erroneous.
presented evidence that the occipital The person usually credited with tilting the scales of scientific opi
required for vision. firmly toward localization of function in the cerebrum was French ne
ok, we now know that there is a very ogist Paul Broca (Figure 1.11). Broca was presented with a patient
um,FIGURE
with1.11different parts performing could understand language but could not speak. Following the man’s d
Paul Broca (1824–1880). By carefully in 1861, Broca carefully examined his brain and found a lesion in th
ps studying
of thethefunctional divisions
brain of a man who had lost of the frontal lobe (Figure 1.12). Based on this case and several others lik
te of
the those
faculty of produced by the
speech after a brain lesion phrenol-
(see Broca concluded that this region of the human cerebrum was specif
Figure 1.12), Broca became convinced that dif-
ke ferent
the functions
phrenologists, scientists
could be localized to differenttoday
responsible for the production of speech.
parts of the cerebrum. (Source: Clarke and Solid experimental support for cerebral localization in animals qu
Functional
4.1 Processes: thespecialization: Broca
neuroanatomy of language
History and eary locationist theories

Paul Broca Leborgne („Tan“) Lelong


1824 – 1880 neuro syphilis stroke

•  apparently preserved •  apparently preserved


comprehension comprehension

•  only articulations: „tan“, •  production limited to five


„tan-tan“ words (‘oui’, ‘non’, ‘toi’,
‘toujours’ and ‘Lelo’)
•  died eight days after
admission •  died one year post-stroke

•  post-mortem inspection of •  post-mortem inspection of


the brain the brain

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


Functional specialization: Broca

4.1 Processes: the neuroanatomy of language


History and eary locationist theories

M. Lelong!

M. Leborgne !

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


Functional specialization: Broca
Inferior Frontal Gyrus
Broca’s Areathe
4.1 Processes: (BA 44, 45)
neuroanatomy (pars
of opercularis, aka frontal operculus,
language
Broca‘s Area and pars triangularis)

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


Functional specialization: Wernicke

Wernicke (1874)
Functional specialization: Wernicke

Wernicke (1874) pa4ent that could speak very fluently


Functional specialization: Wernicke

Wernicke (1874) pa4ent that could speak very fluently

but he said things that made no sense


Functional specialization: Wernicke

Wernicke (1874) pa4ent that could speak very fluently

but he said things that made no sense

he did not understand anything


Functional specialization: Wernicke

Wernicke (1874) pa4ent that could speak very fluently

but he said things that made no sense

he did not understand anything

Broca s area: Wenicke s area:


Production of Comprehension
Speech of Speech
classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)
classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)

Broca’s Aphasia: speech impaired but good comprehension


classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)

Broca’s Aphasia: speech impaired but good comprehension

Wernicke’s Aphasia: comprehension impaired but fluent speech


ncorporating this new evidence does not necessarily entail the abandon-
idea of anclassical
amodal store ofmodel (Wernicke-Geschwind)
semantic concepts. Mahon and Caramazza
ntain that an amodal system is at the core of semantic memory but that

Broca’s Aphasia: speech impaired but good comprehension


EENTH-CENTURY MODELS OF SPEECH
Wernicke’s Aphasia: comprehension impaired but fluent speech

providing
cific
in the
around
renology
ost the
terances
ca

a
e patient
The Lichtheim model of speech and aphasia links together
the
classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)
classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)
4.3 Processes: the neuroanatomy of language
Classical Model

Broca‘s Aphasia
fluentness, prosody,
Conduction Aphasia
articulation, word finding, and both comprehension and production preserved BUT
complex grammar disturbes repetition impaired
comprehension more or less
preserved

Wernicke’s Aphasia
comprehension disturbed
prosody preserved, production undisturbed to
overshooting

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)
classical model (Wernicke-Geschwind)

4.3 Processes: the neuroanatomy of language


Criticisms to classical Model

•  Functional-anatomical classification can not (or not sufficiently) account for clinical observations

•  Broca’s aphasia can be found with no lesion to Broca’s area

•  Isolated cortical lesions of the inferior frontal gyrus often lead to transient symptoms, not
corresponding to Broca’s aphasia

•  Productive speech deficits also after lesions to Wernicke’s area

•  Very rarely comprehension deficits after unilateral damage to Wernicke’s area

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


Functional specialization: Broca
a spoiler for the next classes
4.4 Processes: the neuroanatomy of language
Modern hypothesis on Broca‘s Area

phonological
processing

syntactic
processing

semantic
processing

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


back to the brain..
3.4 A short primer into the Human Brain

Gross Anatomy

Structures Formelles du Langage 03.02.2016


more on Functional specialization: Brodmann
Brodmann (1909)
more on Functional specialization: Brodmann
Brodmann (1909)
Cytoarchitectural map of cortex
more on Functional specialization: Brodmann
Brodmann (1909)
Cytoarchitectural map of cortex

each area with a common architecture


was given a number
more on Functional specialization: Brodmann
Brodmann (1909)
Cytoarchitectural map of cortex

each area with a common architecture


was given a number

why do brain areas have different


cytoarchitectonic proper4es?
more on Functional specialization: Brodmann

why do brain areas have different cytoarchitectonic proper4es?


more on Functional specialization: Brodmann

Cor$cal Layers (Cytoarchitecture)

I Dendrites of deeper cells

II Small granule cells

III Variety of cells, many pyramidal in shape

IV Mainly granule cells


V Pyramidally shaped cells
larger than in layer III

VI Heterogeneous layer of neurons


blends into white matter

White Matter
more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)
more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)
more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)

electrical s4mula4on of 400


living and awake pa4ents
more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)

electrical s4mula4on of 400


living and awake pa4ents

they were going under


brain surgery for epilepsy
more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)

electrical s4mula4on of 400


living and awake pa4ents

they were going under


brain surgery for epilepsy

occipital lobe: “a star came down toward my nose”


more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)

electrical s4mula4on of 400


living and awake pa4ents

they were going under


brain surgery for epilepsy

occipital lobe: “a star came down toward my nose”

central sulcus: “those fingers and my thumb gave a jump”


more on functional specialization: Penfield
Penfield (1950)

Penfield electrically s4mulated


the cortex (no pain receptors)

electrical s4mula4on of 400


living and awake pa4ents

they were going under


brain surgery for epilepsy

occipital lobe: “a star came down toward my nose”

central sulcus: “those fingers and my thumb gave a jump”

temporal lobe: “I hear music again”


similar and roughly resemble a trapeze artist hanging upside down, his

more on functional specialization: somatotopy


legs hooked over the top of the postcentral gyrus and dangling into the
medial cortex between the hemispheres, and his head at the opposite,
lower end of the gyrus (Figure 12.18). A somatotopic map is sometimes
called a homunculus (from the Latin diminutive of “man”; the little man in
the brain).
somatotopic mapabout
Two things are obvious of the body surface
a somatotopic map. First, onto
the mapprimary
is not somatosensory cortex (S1)
always continuous but can be broken up. Notice in Figure 12.18 that the

Trunk
Head
Neck
Elbo
For

Arm

Hip
ear

Leg
Ha

m
Fin

nd
ge
rs
Th
um
b

Ey
e
No
se Foot
Fa
ce
Up Toes
p er
lip
Lips Genitals
Lower li
p
Teeth
Gums
Jaw

gue
Ton

ry nx
ha
al

P
in
om
bd
-a
tra
In
similar and roughly resemble a trapeze artist hanging upside down, his

more on functional specialization: somatotopy


legs hooked over the top of the postcentral gyrus and dangling into the
medial cortex between the hemispheres, and his head at the opposite,
lower end of the gyrus (Figure 12.18). A somatotopic map is sometimes
called a homunculus (from the Latin diminutive of “man”; the little man in
the brain).
somatotopic mapabout
Two things are obvious of the body surface
a somatotopic map. First, onto
the mapprimary
is not somatosensory cortex (S1)
always continuous but can be broken up. Notice in Figure 12.18 that the

Trunk
Head
Neck
Elbo
For

Arm

Hip
ear

w
the homunculus!

Leg
Ha

m
Fin

nd
ge
rs
Th
um
b

Ey
e
No
se Foot
Fa
ce
Up Toes
p er
lip
Lips Genitals
Lower li
p
Teeth
Gums
Jaw

gue
Ton

ry nx
ha
al

P
in
om
bd
-a
tra
In
more on functional specialization of the brain
map of cortical areas
map of cortical areas
to know more..

and if you want to have more fun with brain func4ons..

http://www.fmriconsulting.com/brodmann/Introduction.html

readings:

Ward: chapters 1 and 2

Bear: chapters 1 to 7

credits (to serious Neurolinguists):

Einat Shetreet

Andrea San; Emiliano Zaccarella

S-ar putea să vă placă și