Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

protocol

Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish


Caio Maximino1, Thiago Marques de Brito2, Claudio Alberto Gellis de Mattos Dias1, Amauri Gouveia Jr1, 3
& Silvio Morato2
1
Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Para, Belém, Brazil. 2Laboratório de Comportamento
Exploratório, Departamento de Psicologia e Educação, FFCLRP, Ribeirão Preto, Brazil. 3Núcleo de Estudos e Pesquisa em Comportamento, Departamento de Psicologia,
Universidade Federal do Para, Belém, Brazil. Correspondence should be addressed to C.M. (caio@ufpa.br).

Published online 14 January 2010; doi:10.1038/nprot.2009.225

The scototaxis (dark/light preference) protocol is a behavioral model for fish that is being validated to assess the antianxiety
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

effects of pharmacological agents and the behavioral effects of toxic substances, and to investigate the (epi)genetic bases of
anxiety-related behavior. Briefly, a fish is placed in a central compartment of a half-black, half-white tank; following habituation,
the fish is allowed to explore the tank for 15 min; the number and duration of entries in each compartment (white or black)
are recorded by the observer for the whole session. Zebrafish, goldfish, guppies and tilapias (all species that are important in
behavioral neurosciences and neuroethology) have been shown to demonstrate a marked preference for the dark compartment. An
increase in white compartment activity (duration and/or entries) should reflect antianxiety behavior, whereas an increase in dark
compartment activity should reflect anxiety-promoting behavior. When individual animals are exposed to the apparatus on only
one occasion, results can be obtained in 20 min per fish.

INTRODUCTION
The dark/light preference model is already established as an of the gymnotid G. carapo, for which different dimensions of
‘etho­experimental’ anxiety model in rodents1. It is based on the the test tank were needed).
natural aversive quality of brightly lit environments for mice, Behavioral responses in the dark/light preference protocol are
shaping a conflict situation in which the animal must deal with readily assessable and quantifiable by an observer, without the need
its natural tendency to explore in the face of an unpleasant envi- for extensive training. Briefly, animals are placed in an intersection
ronment. The rodent dark/light preference model is an explora- compartment, located between one white and one dark compart-
tion model, in the context that it measures locomotor activity ment; these spectra can be discriminated by both zebrafish43 and
in both environments as an index of anxiety2–5. The proposed goldfish44, and even though the authors are unaware of behavio-
actinopterygian dark/light preference task is a modification of ral spectral sensitivity experiments in the other species in which
an experimental manipulation used by Edward Thorndike6 dur- scototaxis has been observed, the very existence of this trait is
ing the beginning of the twentieth century to study motivation indirect evidence for this sensitivity. This intersection compart-
and learning in fish, and afterward by Satake and Morton7 in the ment is enclosed by two sliding doors, which are removed follow-
1970s to establish the effects of noradrenergic substances on the ing a habituation interval of 5 min. The animal is then allowed to
scotophobic (i.e., dark-avoiding) behavior of pinealectomized or explore the apparatus freely, and the observer records its behavior
scotophobin-injected goldfish. Recently, the proposed model was (number and/or duration of entries in either the black or white
used to establish dark/light preference in zebrafish Danio rerio8, compartments) for 15 min. This procedure is based on the work of
bluegill Lepomis macrochirus, crucian carp Carassius langsdorfii9, Mattioli’s group8, which observed the natural preference zebrafish
goldfish Carassius auratus9–12, guppy Poecilia reticulata12, Nile tila- for the black compartment. The duration of the session was
pia Oreochromis niloticus12, lambari Astyanax altiparanae, cardi- increased to 15 min, which is 5 min more than the original experi-
nal tetra Paracheirodon axelrodi12 and banded knifefish Gymnotus ment described by Serra et al.8, because the time course of explo-
carapo12, and to screen for the neurobehavioral effects of ethanol13 ration in this apparatus reveals significant changes after the tenth
and dopaminergic drugs14 in zebrafish. The main advantage of minute of observation. Behavior in this task (i.e., activity in the
this protocol is the presentation of a clear conflict situation for white compartment) reflects a conflict between the preference of
the fish; however, most models that investigate innate ‘fear’- and the animal for protected areas (e.g., black substrata, in a process of
‘anxiety’-like behavior in fish do not use such conflict. With the crypsis) and an innate motivation to explore novel environments.
exception of predator inspection tests15–17, most innate anxiety Crypsis in fish depends on the dorsal distribution of melanophores
tests use either the exploration of an open field or the vertical in the fish surface; the presence of dorsal and dorsally oriented
distribution of the animal in the water column to measure this melanophores tends to minimize refraction and reflection of
variable14,18–32 and aim to describe individual variability across light incident on the fish, reducing the effectiveness of visual infor-
a ‘shyness–boldness’ continua 15,33–37. This continuum can be mation for predators45. Appropriate cryptic coloration requires
mapped to Budaev’s15,34,35 two dimensions of ‘temperament’ in the appropriate background and behavior (in the present task,
fish (activity-exploration and fear-avoidance), which in turn preference for dark substrata). Antianxiety effects (i.e., increased
are trait instantiations of approach–avoidance state dimen- time in the white compartment) can be determined simultane-
sions38–40. Ex hypothesi, these dimensions are best analyzed using ously with a measure of increased or decreased swimming activity
conflict models41,42. This protocol will focus on the cyprinids (i.e., total number of entries in both compartments). Other meas-
zebrafish and goldfish; however, it has been successfully applied ures that can be observed are thigmotaxis (i.e., the tendency for the
to non-cyprinids without alterations to it12 (with the exception animals to stay nearer to the walls of the apparatus, especially

nature protocols | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | 221


protocol
while exploring the white compartment)14,22,25,26,28,29,31,46,47; the instant locations, which are potentially dangerous38,40,68–72. The result is
velocity of entries in each side (i.e., mean duration of each entry, that fish will spend more time in the ‘safe’ environment (i.e., the
instead of the total duration of entries) as well as the frequency of dark compartment of the tank), making short incursions into the
body and caudal fin (BCF) transient propulsions48; frequency of potentially ‘risky’ environment (i.e., the light compartment of the
‘freezing’ events (equivalent to Webb’s ‘non-swimming’48); vertical tank)12, a pattern of exploratory behavior called ‘risk assessment’
distribution (top versus bottom of the water column)29,32; ventila- or ‘antipredator apprehension’67.
tion rate49–52; and changes in the coloration of the animal13,14,47,53–55;
however, this final measure can be difficult to obtain when the Effects of rearing in ‘enriched’ environments
animal is in the black compartment. Rearing in ‘enriched’ environments has been used in studies assessing
Negative controls for this test can be made using all-white and anxiety behavior of rodents73–75. In the Laboratório de Neurociências
all-black tanks, with the same dimensions as the tank described e Comportamento, we compared the effects of rearing in an enriched
below8,12; if different animals are used for each tank in the with impoverished environment on the exploratory behavior of
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

validation procedure, then no statistically significant lateral zebrafish in the dark/light preference task; significant increases were
preference (i.e., time spent in each compartment) should be observed in the time spent in the white compartment (150.1 ± 15.3%,
observed, showing that the preference in the dark/light tank in relation to the impoverished environment) and decreases in the
is indeed for the dark side of the apparatus and not for some frequency of BCF transient swimming (78.7 ± 3.5%) and freezing
other spatial constraint. (67.3 ± 9.8%) in this compartment. Our ‘enriched’ tank had hiding
places, rocks, vegetation and natural substratum, whereas the ‘impov-
Face validity of scototaxis erished’ tank had only water quality, temperature, aeration and light-
The dark/light preference protocol has face validity (i.e., the ability ing controls. These observations support the hypothesis that rearing
of a given task to appear to measure what it is supposed to measure, in ‘enriched’ environments reduces anxiety-like behavior in fish, an
in that the given task ‘looks like’ the endophenotype56 that it is mod- effect that has been observed in rodents73–75—in which it has been
eling57). Bakshi and Kalin58 proposed that the main endophenotype suggested that environmental enrichment reduces emotionality and
of anxiety is an aberrant expression of defensive behavior. As an induces ‘facilitation’ of motor skills73.
example, the anxiety or fear of brightly lit spaces is measured in this
Multiple test sessions in the dark/light preference task
model; the white compartment is avoided, and animals (zebrafish)
Contrary to rodent models such as, e.g., the elevated plus-maze and
spend most time (71.3 ± 26.2%) in the black compartment12. Other
dark/light transitions test, the dark preference (at least in zebrafish)
anxiety-related behaviors, such as thigmotaxis, freezing and BCF
does not present the so-called ‘one-trial tolerance,’ an effect in
transient swimming, also occur more in the white than in the black
which animals exposed a second time to the same apparatus tend
compartment (unpublished observations).
to present sensitization76 (i.e., increased time spent in the ‘safe’
environment) and there is lack of effect of benzodiazepines77–80.
Predictive validity of scototaxis
In a 1-week experiment in which animals were tested daily (at the
The dark/light preference protocol has predictive validity, defined,
same photoperiod), no evidence of sensitization or habituation
based on a psychometric point of view59–61 as the extent to which
was observed in any parameter tested81 (time spent in each com-
the dependent measure predicts behavior on a related meas-
partment, number of transitions, latency for exploration) (average
ure62. Unpublished data from our laboratory (Laboratório de
measures intraclass correlation r  =  0.983, F(8,36)  =  58.095, P < 0.001,
Neurociências e Comportamento, UFPA) show that increased
in a one-way random effects model where subject effects are ran-
dark compartment activity occurs in zebrafish, which also dem-
dom, and H0 for the F-test is that the true value for the intraclass
onstrate increased central area entries in the open field (r2[gl = 13]  = 
correlation coefficients is 0).
0.89, P < 0.01). Predictive validity is also defined as the degree to
which a model can predict the effect of antianxiety compounds Developmental factors in the dark/light preference task
with proved clinical efficacy2,63–65. It has been reported66 that anxi- Some results indicate that the preference for dark environments
olytic compounds predictably alter the time that zebrafish spent in zebrafish are age-dependent82, which is probably an effect of
in the white compartment of the apparatus, and similar results the maturation of melanophores83,84. Thus, in larval zebrafish,
were obtained, using chlordiazepoxide (0.0, 0.02 and 0.2 mg kg − 1), which show no pigmentation, a preference for light environments
in the Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento (153.4 ± is observed14,85, whereas a preference for dark environments is
11.4% [0.02 mg kg − 1] and 121.7 ± 4.1% [0.2 mg kg − 1] in relation observed in adult zebrafish8,12. These developmental differences
to controls) (unpublished data). should be taken into account, given that melanophores are impor-
tant for the ontogeny of defensive behavior in fish45.
Construct validity of scototaxis
The construct validity of most anxiety models rely on linking them Comparison with other tests
to species-specific defense reactions67. In this model, a behavioral Currently, there are few behavioral tests of anxiety, temperament
co-adaptation to heightened dorsal distribution of melanophores or emotionality in fish. The open-field, a test in which the animal’s
(i.e., preference for dark substrata to enable crypsis) is central to the exploration of a novel environment is the dependent measure, has
construct in question. In animal models, the construct of anxiety been used to test dopaminergic and glutamatergic drugs14,23,26,29,
is sometimes defined in terms of opponent motivational processes whereas the vertical distribution test has been used to assess sub-
being ‘activated’ simultaneously, creating an approach–avoidance stances that act on the nicotinic receptor30,32. However, none of
conflict: at the same time when animals tend to explore novel these tests have been validated using antianxiety drugs, such as
environments, they also tend to express fear of those novel benzodiazepines and serotonergic agents. Currently, this validation

222 | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | nature protocols


protocol
is ongoing in our laboratory. Comparison between the results of as possible. If any injection procedure is necessary, animals should
these tests is difficult because different drugs and treatments have be anesthetized. For anesthesia, 100–200 mg liter − 1 tricaine meth-
been used in each model. In terms of ease of application, as well as anesulfonate (MS-222) solution can be used for induction, and
predictive, face and construct validity, the scototaxis test is at least 50–100 mg liter − 1 for maintenance; alternatively, menthol97 (100
as good as the other tests. However, the open-field is likely to suffer mg liter − 1) or clove oil98,99 (150 mg liter − 1) can be used. Recovery is
from difficulty in isolating locomotor and emotional effects (as is attained by returning the animals to fresh, well-aerated water100.
the case with rodents86), which should not happen in the proposed
dark/light test and in the vertical distribution test. Choice of material for the tank.  The tank should be constructed
of matte acrylic that is as non-reflective as possible. Zebrafish are
General background information relating to the procedure a naturally shoaling species101, and the use of reflective surfaces for
Timing of test.  Zebrafish present circadian rhythmicity in the tank can affect stimulus control, as the animals might display
locomotor behavior that is entrained by environmental markers social behavior in relation to their own reflections.
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

(‘zeitgebers’) such as feeding regimens and the dark–light cycle87;


this rhythmicity is probably related to diel vertical migrations, which Behavioral sampling procedures and session duration.  Although
impose a tradeoff between predation risk and the existence of anti- using what Dunbar102 calls ‘point sampling,’ in which the behavior
predator ‘windows’ for feeding in near-surface waters at dawn and state that the animal is performing at a predetermined ‘point’ in
dusk88–90, akin to the tradeoffs that exist in situations in which risk time and its duration are recorded, can be convenient for observers,
is temporally variable72. Consistent with these observations, we have this sampling method is not enough to establish preference103. In
observed in the Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento our experiments, we use ad libitum sampling for the whole 15-min
that the preference for dark environments is inverted when animals session, which not only results in better measures of preference104
are tested during the night (from 1800 to 0000 hours)(H[df  =  3]  =  but also enables observation of other behavioral acts.
10.787, P  =  0.013) (C.A.G.M. Dias, unpublished data). As such, it
is preferable that animals are tested during the day. Other potential uses.  An identical apparatus has also been used
to assess learning in goldfish105. In this procedure, the animal is
Handling of animals before testing.  In procedures such as the placed in the white compartment of the apparatus, and the time it
rodent-elevated plus-maze62 and the dark/light preference test1,5,62, takes to swim to the black compartment is the dependent measure;
‘it is important to ensure that in [such experiments], handling decreasing latencies over trials represent the acquisition of inhibi-
of rodents and any experience with prior stressors, particularly tory avoidance.
immediately before testing, is consistent across animals and treat- Another use of this protocol, although indirect, is the design of
ment groups’62. The stressful character of handling is prominent in ‘biased’ tanks for place preference conditioning106. The initial aver-
fish91–95, especially given that prolonged handling can rapidly lead to sion to the light compartment is overcome by the rewarding effect of a
hypoxia, a further source of stress in these animals96. The transfer drug, and drugs can be compared in terms of their reward potency by
of animals from housing to test tanks should therefore be as quick assessing the degree to which this aversion is supplanted across trials.

MATERIALS
REAGENT SETUP ideal levels for each species can be found in databases such as FishBase.
Fish species The choice of species for use in this experiment depends mainly Table 1 presents optimal values for these parameters for zebrafish, guppy
on a high density of dark melanophores in the dorsum of the animal. In the and goldfish. Other environmental parameters, such as light and noise
Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento, we demonstrated the levels, also need to be considered.
existence of this pattern in zebrafish, goldfish, fighting-fish Betta splendens For example, as schooling fish, zebrafish can be kept at fairly high
(Bruno Rodrigues dos Santos, unpublished data), Nile tilapia, Cardinal tetra, densities: ~20 fish can be kept in volumes ranging from 400 ml as young larvae
guppy, lambari and banded knifefish12. Each of these species is also being (1–5 d post-fertilization; dpf) to 3 liters as the fish approach juvenile stage
used as a model animal in other types of research, including speciation (3 months, defined as the beginning of reproductive age)100. For general activity
events, development, social behavior and sexual selection. Thus, choosing assessment, tanks should be made of transparent glass, Plexiglas (acrylic) or
species is more a matter of validating the preference for them and matching polycarbonate, enabling easy observation of the animals. A different tank, for
scototaxis with the more general questions being asked by the experimenters. breeding, should be smaller (24 cm × 12 cm × 12 cm), and present either artificial
Zebrafish was one of the first species to be tested in this paradigm8 because or natural glass in the bottom, as well as a bottom layer of glass marbles.
it has become one of the preferred laboratory model organisms in genetics Preparation of drugs and toxins  Administration routes for drugs and
and developmental biology107. toxins can vary; most used are intraperitoneal injection and waterborne
The description of the behavior of some of these species was made administration. The first procedure has the disadvantage of stressing the
elsewhere12; some species differences are of note (Fig. 1). Briefly, animal (even when the animal is anesthetized, as it should be in any
preference for dark environments was found in all these species; however, procedure involving injections); thus, identical handling and injection
the banded knifefish and the Nile tilapia failed to present enhanced locomo- procedures must be used for control and treated fish. The second procedure
tion in an all-white tank (a positive control for the validation of scototaxis; is used in most of the studies so far24,25,27,28,47,110,111 but has the disadvantage
see ‘Validation of test in laboratory’, under ‘Equipment Setup’). As such, of producing uncertainty in relation to the system bioavailability of the
use of species other than the ones presented here demand validation. drug in question; this limitation can be avoided if the researcher has access
Animal housing  Animals should be housed in accordance with guidelines to high-speed liquid chromatography and tandem mass spectrometry to
for the use of fish in research108,109, as well as in accordance with the more measure the presence and amount of the drug in the nervous system, plasma
general guidelines for the use of animals in research. Briefly, animals should or urine112. When using this latter route of administration, we recommend
be group housed in water conditions that are adequate for the species. dissolving the stock solutions in a 250-ml beaker containing 200 ml of
The main water quality parameters are temperature, oxygen saturation, de-chlorinated tank water, at a temperature of ~27 °C. Drug doses should be
nitrogen compounds, carbon dioxide, pH and salinity; information about calculated based on the weights of the salts. Stock solutions to be dissolved

nature protocols | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | 223


protocol
in de-chlorinated tank water should contain the Figure 1 | Different 1,000
drug of interest, plus associations. For diazepam, species of fish vary 800
in the intensity of

Total time (s)


e.g., a 5 mg ml − 1 stock solution should contain
600
40% propylene glycol, 10% ethyl alcohol, 5% their preference for
sodium benzoate and 1.5% benzyl alcohol. This the dark environment, 400
stock solution can be either injected or and some species 200
dissolved in 200 m of de-chlorinated tank water. might not present a
0
EQUIPMENT  preference for either Guppy Lambari Zebrafish Tetra Knifefish Mosquitofish Swordtail
• Buckets or 2 L beakers for temporary housing dark (black bars) or
• Fish nets light (white bars) environments. This appears to be the case with mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki),
• Black and white acrylic tank (description in text) which, contrary to other poeciliids such as guppy and swordtail (Xiphophorus helleri), does not present
• Video camera a preference for dark environments. Other species of fish, however, spend more time in the dark than
• Automated (e.g., Ethovision, MouseTracker) or in the bright portion of the aquarium. Data for guppies, lambaris, zebrafish, cardinal tetras and banded
nonautomated (e.g., Etholog, JWatcher, X-Plo- knifefish taken from Maximino et al.12; data for mosquitofish and swordtail are unpublished. The
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

Rat) behavioral transcription software experiments were performed in compliance with the recommendations of SBNeC (Brazilian Society
• Discard tank, glass for Neuroscience and Behavior), which are based on National Institutes of Health’s Guide for Care and
EQUIPMENT SETUP Use of Laboratory Animals, and also complied to the Canadian Council on Animal Care’s Guidelines
Transport and temporary housing equipments  on the Care and Use of Fish in Research, Teaching and Testing.
Animals should be transferred from tempo­rary
housing tanks (bucket or beakers) by using fish nets.
Dark/light preference tank  Here we use the apparatus described else- using an adapted behavioral transcription software (X-Plo-Rat v1.1.0). Other
where8,12 and presented in Figure 2, although variations exist. An acrylic tank software are available for this type of data collection, following a similar
(15 cm × 10 cm × 45 cm height × width × length) is used that is divided rationale (i.e., recording the duration of ‘behavioral states’ such as entrance
equally into one-half black and one-half white. Walls and bottom are either in a given compartment, e.g., Ethovision, EthoLog and JWatcher). If compu-
black or white, so as to warrant uniform substrata for each compartment. terized transcription is not available, an observer can make hatchmarks on
The water column is kept at 10 cm, yielding a final volume of a data sheet for each compartment entry that the fish makes and use a timer
4.5 liters. Water should be de-chlorinated. The colored material chosen to determine the duration spent in either compartment. It is also possible to
should not be reflective, to avoid the tendency of those animals that present use automated video tracking software if the contrast between background
shoaling tendencies113,114 to behave in relation to their own reflection. The and the animal are altered; this can be done using GPL-licensed video editing
tank contains central sliding doors, colored with the same color of the software such as VirtualDub.
aquarium side, thereby defining an uncolored central compartment Equipment for validation of test in laboratory  Interspecific variation in this
measuring 15 cm × 10 cm × 10 cm. test has been observed12, and scototaxis should not be treated as a fixed trait in
The species were used in our laboratory are small, ornamental fish, meas- a given species. As such, cross-validation should be made in each laboratory, as
uring a maximum 5 cm long. For longer animals, the size of the tanks should to warrant that the population or species in question presents this trait. Nega-
be changed accordingly. For example, in our tests with the banded knifefish, tive controls for this test can be made using all-white and all-black tanks, with
which measures ~10 cm, we used a tank measuring 15 cm × 10 cm × 55 cm, the same dimensions as the tank described above8,12; if different animals are
resulting in a central compartment of 15 × 10 × 20 cm12. used for each tank in the validation procedure, then no statistically significant
Location and/or illumination  Tanks should be illuminated by environmental lateral preference (i.e., time spent in each compartment) should be observed,
light (75 W light bulb, located at 1.80 m above the aquarium top), which keeps showing that the preference in the dark/light tank is indeed for the dark side
illumination uniform and constant between trials. At this distance, environ­ of the apparatus, and not for some other spatial constraint. Also, the all-white
mental light tends to produce an average of 975 lux right above the tank. tank should produce increased locomotion, as a signal of increased anxiety-
Data collection apparatus  The primary method for data collection used in like behavior8,12; however, the opposite result (decreased locomotion in the all-
our laboratory is the manual transcription of video-recorded experiments white tank) can also be expected, as freezing can be an anxiety-like behavior.
Discard tank  After animals are tested, they should be removed carefully
from the test tank and transferred to a discard tank, with the same conditions
Table 1 | Water quality parameters for three species of fish. found in the housing tank (see ‘Animal housing’). Although multiple testing
does not affect the behavior of zebrafish (see ‘Multiple test sessions in the
Parameter Species dark/light preference task’), discarded animals should preferentially not be
used in another scototaxis session, especially if they were subjected to drug
Zebrafish Guppy Goldfish or toxicant treatments.
Danio Poecilia Carassius
rerio reticulata auratus
Temperature (°C) 18–24 18–28  < 41 5 cm

Dissolved oxygen 6.98–7.65 4.5 5.5–7.0


(mg liter − 1)
Nitrogen compounds NH3–N: NH3–N: NH3–N:
(mg liter − 1)  < 0.16 0.11–0.16  < 0.15
NO2–N: NO2–N: NO2–N: 15 cm 10 cm

 < 0.009 0.008–0.009  < 0.01


pH 6.0–8.0 7.0–8.0 6.0–8.0
22.5 cm
Salinity (%)  < 6  < 35  < 2 Figure 2 | Test apparatus for the proposed protocol. The apparatus should
Information for zebrafish and goldfish were included because these species are being increasingly be made of matte acrylic, as described in the text, and the size depicted
used in behavioral studies in neurosciences and pharmacology; information for guppy was included
because this species is widely used in behavioral ecology experiments. Information retrieved from is suitable for fish that have a maximum body length of 5 cm. Longer
FishBase. species require proportionally longer tanks.

224 | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | nature protocols


protocol

PROCEDURE
Setup ● TIMING 0.5–1 h per cohort of experimental animals
1| Ensure the apparatus is filled with de-chlorinated water before use (water column  =  10 cm). Fill out data sheets with
subject number of animal, date, coded condition and experimenter initials before testing.

2| Bring the animal in an individual temporary transport recipient (e.g., bucket or beaker) into the behavioral test room.

3| Transfer the fish to the central compartment of the test tank with a net.
 CRITICAL STEP Ensure that all animals are handled in a consistent manner and that each animal is placed in the central
compartment in the same position.
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

Testing ● TIMING 15 min per animal


4| Following a 5-min habituation period, remove the central sliding doors, allowing the fish to swim freely in the apparatus.

5| Record the locomotor activity for 15 min.


 CRITICAL STEP During the habituation period, take precautions to begin data collection as soon as the central sliding
doors are removed. If sessions are video-recorded, this can be done by beginning records during the habituation period.
 CRITICAL STEP Entry into either compartment is recorded when approximately two-thirds of the body length of the animal
has crossed the midline.
 CRITICAL STEP Observers must avoid unnecessary movements and noise. If possible, sessions should be recorded with
an overhead camera, so that behavioral recording sessions take place in an isolated room. If this is not possible, the use of
physical barriers to block vision to the outside of the aquarium is useful103.
? TROUBLESHOOTING

Cleanup ● TIMING 1–2 min per animal


6| Transfer the animal from the test apparatus to a discard tank with a net.

7| Discard the water used for test, wash the tank thoroughly with tap water and refill it with de-chlorinated water before
testing with another animal.

Data analysis
8| Perform preferred data analysis. In a previous study12, we used non-parametric (Kruskal–Wallis) analyses of variance to
assess the effects of treatment on the following measures: latency for first choice of environment; duration of entries in each
compartment (Fig. 1); and number of midline crossings. However, further unpublished study revealed that other variables,
such as latency to begin exploration after an environment was first chosen, frequency of entries in each compartment and
mean duration of entries, are sensitive to manipulations. In addition, non-parametric analyses of variance are not the only
way to assess the effects of independent variables on dependent variables; however, normality of the data should be
assessed before parametric tests are used. For more information, refer to standard statistics texts.

● TIMING
Steps 1–3, setup: for cohort of experimental animals, 0.5–1 h
Steps 4 and 5, testing: 15 min per animal.
Steps 6 and 7, cleanup: 1–2 min per animal.
? TROUBLESHOOTING

Step 5: Fish jumps out of the aquarium


Infrequently (this happened twice in a block of 60 animals in our laboratory, under the conditions described above), the ani-
mal ‘jumps’ out of the test tank and falls off the apparatus. When this occurs, the experimenter must rapidly pick up the ani-
mal and discard it in the discard tank. Behavioral data from an animal that does this should be excluded from the analyses.

Step 5: Fish freezes on a given compartment after first choice


Occasionally (this happened only once in our laboratory, probably owing to an accident that happened in the testing room),
animals might freeze after they choose a compartment and no longer explore the apparatus for the whole 15-min session.
This is especially true with very stressful manipulations, or if there is noise or unnecessary movements in the experimenta-
tion room. If this happens, data from this animal should not be discarded from the analyses; however, the experimenter
should keep track of the freezing fish, and freezing behavior itself should be analyzed (either by recording its frequency in
individual fish or recording the number of fish which froze).
nature protocols | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | 225
protocol
Step 5: Fish demonstrates excessive thigmotaxis
If the animal spends too much time (~30% of the total test time) clinging to a particular wall (or to walls in general) of the
apparatus (maximum distance of ~2 cm from the wall), this might be a sign that the fish is behaving in response to its own
reflection. In this case, changing the material of which the apparatus is made should be considered, to avoid confounding
variables. If the material used is absolutely non-reflective, however, this could represent a pattern of exploratory behavior 22.
The experimenter should keep track of the thigmotactic fish, and thigmotaxis itself should be analyzed (either by recording
its frequency in individual fish or by recording the number of fish that displayed it).

ANTICIPATED RESULTS
The following results exemplify data that we have obtained using the dark/light preference protocol to investigate the an-
tianxiety effects of rearing in an enriched environment in goldfish and zebrafish115. The results are robust and replicable, and
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

show generalization across both species. This type of manipulation yields consistent antianxiety effects in rodent models of
anxiety73–75; an experiment conducted in our laboratory (Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento), using the scoto-
taxis test, is described below.
In this study, adult zebrafish and goldfish were divided in four groups, depending on species (zebrafish versus goldfish)
and rearing environment (enriched versus impoverished, as described above). Animals were kept in their respective rearing
environment for 2 consecutive months before testing. One hour before testing in the dark/light preference tank, animals
were drawn randomly from the rearing tanks and transported to the behavioral observation room. An observer, blind to the
experimental condition of the experimental animal, recorded the behavioral data using the protocol described above. Data
were analyzed using two-way analyses of variance, followed by Tukey’s HSD as post-hoc tests. Results showed that being
reared in an enriched environment for 2 months increases the time that both species of fish spent in the white compartment
of the test tank, compared with those reared in an impoverished environment. Thus, we can conclude that environmental
enrichment can decrease anxiety-like behavior in this test, perhaps by reducing emotionality, as seems to be the case with
rodents73–75.

Summary
The scototaxis protocol is a novel behavioral assay of anxiety-like behavior in fish. It is easy to use, low-cost and valid
results can be obtained in a short, 15-min testing period. The patterns of results obtained using this protocol are replicable
across fish species, studies and laboratories.

Acknowledgments Part of this research was supported by grants from CAPES 8. Serra, E.L., Medalha, C.C. & Mattioli, R. Natural preference of zebrafish (Danio
to C.A.G.d.M.D. and T.M.d.B. The authors thank the Dark/light Preference Team rerio) for a dark environment. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 32, 1551–1553 (1999).
at the defunct Laboratório de Psicobiologia e Psicopatologia Experimental from 9. Yoshida, M., Nagamine, M. & Uematsu, K. Comparison of behavioral
Unesp/Bauru for support with data collection. responses to a novel environment between three teleosts, bluegill Lepomis
macrochirus, crucian carp Carassius langsdorfii, and goldfish Carassius auratus.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS  C.M. and T.M.d.B. conceived and collected data for Fish. Sci. 71, 314–319 (2005).
most experiments regarding preference for dark environments in the Laboratório 10. Gazolla, R.A. Preference for dark substrates in C. auratus: influence of
de Neurociências e Comportamento and the Laboratório de Comportamento lighting conditions in housing environment. MSc thesis, 25 pp. (Universidade
Exploratório, and wrote this paper; C.A.G.d.M.D. conceived the experiments Estadual Paulista, Bauru/SP, Brazil, 2008).
regarding photoperiod and contributed to that section; A.G. and S.M. contributed 11. Gouveia, A. Jr. et al. Preference of goldfish (Carassius auratus) for dark
to sections regarding validity and to the theoretical background related to places. Rev. Etol. 7, 63–66 (2005).
scototaxis, and also wrote this paper. 12. Maximino, C. et al. A comparative analysis of the preference for dark
environments in five teleosts. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 20, 351–367 (2007).
Published online at http://www.natureprotocols.com. 13. Gerlai, R., Lahav, M., Guo, S. & Rosenthal, A. Drinks like a fish: zebra fish
Reprints and permissions information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/ (Danio rerio) as a behavior genetic model to study alcohol effects.
reprintsandpermissions. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 67, 773–782 (2000).
14. Bjerke, S. Developing behavioral assays to study dopamine-related disorders
1. Bourin, M. & Hascöett, M. The mouse light/dark box test. Eur. J. Pharmacol. in zebrafish (Daniorerio). PhD thesis, 104 pp. (The University of Oslo, Oslo,
463, 55–65 (2003). Sweden, 2002).
2. Belzung, C. & Griebel, R. Measuring normal and pathological anxiety-like 15. Budaev, S.V. ‘Personality’ in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): a correlational
behaviour in mice: a review. Behav. Brain Res. 138, 200–209 (2001). study of exploratory behavior and social tendency. J. Comp. Psychol. 111,
3. Green, S. & Hodges, H. Animal models of anxiety. In Behavioral Models in 399–411 (1997).
Psychopathology: Theoretical, Industrial and Clinical Perspectives (ed. Willner, P.) 16. McCartt, A.L., Lynch, W.E. Jr. & Johnson, D.L. How light, a predator, and
21–49 (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1991). experience influence bluegill use of shade and schooling. Environ. Biol.
4. Prut, L. & Belzung, C. The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of Fishes 49, 79–87 (1997).
drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 463, 3–33 (2003). 17. Bleakley, B.H., Martell, C.M. & Brodie, E.D. III. Variation in anti-predator
5. Hascöett, M., Bourin, M. & Dhonnchadha, B.A.N. The mouse light–dark behavior among five strains of inbred guppies, Poecilia reticulata.
paradigm: a review. Progr. NeuroPsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 25, Behav. Genet. 36, 783–791 (2006).
141–166 (2001). 18. Kleerekoper, H. et al. An analysis of locomotor behaviour of goldfish
6. Thorndike, E.L. A note on the psychology of fishes. Am. Nat. 33, 923 (1911). (Carassius auratus). Anim. Behav. 18, 317–330 (1970).
7. Satake, N. & Morton, B.E. Scotophobin A causes dark avoidance in goldfish 19. Crawshaw, L.I. Twenty-four hour records of body temperature and activity
by elevating pineal N-acetylserotonin. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 10, in bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) and brown bullheads (Ictalurus
449–456 (1979). nebulosus). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 51, 11–14 (1975).

226 | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | nature protocols


protocol
20. Warren, E.W. & Callaghan, S. Individual differences in response to an open 48. Webb, P.W. Body form, locomotion and foraging in aquatic vertebrates.
field test by the guppy Poecilia reticulata (Peters). J. Fish Biol. 7, 105–113 Am. Zool. 24, 107–120 (1984).
(1976). 49. Altimiras, J. & Larsen, E. Non-invasive recording of heart rate and
21. Gervai, J. & Csányi, V. Behavior-genetic analysis of the paradise fish, ventilation rate in rainbow trout during rest and swimming. Fish go
Macropodus opercularis. I. Characterization of the behavioral responses of wireless! J. Fish Biol. 57, 197–209 (2000).
inbred strains in novel environments: a factor analysis. Behav. Genet. 15, 50. Barreto, R.E. & Volpato, G. Caution for using ventilatory frequency as an
503–519 (1985). indicator of stress in fish. Behav. Processes 66, 43–51 (2004).
22. Mikheev, V.N. & Andreev, O.A. Two-phase exploration of a novel environment 51. Barreto, R.E., Luchiari, A.C. & Marcondes, A.L. Ventilatory frequency
in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. J. Fish Biol. 42, 375–383 (1993). indicates visual recognition of an allopatric predator in naïve Nile tilapia.
23. Mok, E.Y. & Munro, A.D. Effects of dopaminergic drugs on locomotor activity Behav. Processes 60, 235–239 (2003).
in teleost fish of the genus Oreochromis (Cichlidae): involvement of the 52. Sager, D.R., Hocutt, C.H. & Staufer, J.R. Jr. Base and stressed ventilation
telencephalon. Physiol. Behav. 64, 227–234 (1998). rates for Leiostomus xanthurus Lacépède and Morone americana Gmelin
24. Giacomini, N.J., Rose, B., Kobayashi, K. & Guo, S. Antipsychotics produce exposed to strobe lights. J. Appl. Ichtyol. 16, 89–97 (2000).
locomotor impairment in larval zebrafish. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 28, 245–250 53. Fujii, R. The regulation of motile activity in fish chromatophores. Pigment Cell
(2006). Res. 13, 300–319 (2000).
25. Baraban, S.C., Taylor, M.R., Castro, P.A. & Baier, H. Pentylenetetrazole 54. Kawauchi, H., Kawazoe, I., Tsubokawa, M., Kishida, M. & Baker, B.I.
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

induced changes in zebrafish behavior, neural activity and c-fos expression. Characterization of melanin-concentrating hormone in chum salmon
Neuroscience 131, 759–768 (2005). pituitaries. Nature 305, 321–323 (1983).
26. Anichtchik, O.V., Kaslin, J., Peitsaro, N., Scheinin, M. & Panula, P. 55. Hoglund, E., Balm, P.H. & Winberg, S. Skin darkening, a potential social
Neurochemical and behavioural changes in zebrafish Danio rerio after signal in subordinate arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus): the regulatory role of
systemic administration of 6-hydroxydopamine and 1-methyl-4-phenyl- brain monoamines and pro-opiomelanocortin-derived peptides. J. Exp. Biol.
1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine. J. Neurochem. 88, 443–453 (2004). 203, 1711–1721 (2000).
27. Aihart, M.J. et al. Movement disorders and neurochemical changes in 56. Gottesman, I.I. & Gould, T.D. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry:
zebrafish larvae after bath exposure to fluoxetine (PROZAC). Neurotoxicol. etymology and strategic intentions. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 636–645 (2003).
Teratol. 29, 652–664 (2007). 57. Gould, T.D. & Gottesman, I.I. Psychiatric endophenotypes and the
28. Swain, H.A., Sigstad, C. & Scalzo, F.M. Effects of dizocilpine (MK-801) on development of valid animal models. Genes Brain Behav. 5, 113–119 (2006).
circling behavior, swimming activity, and place preference in zebrafish 58. Bakshi, V.P. & Kalin, N.H. Animal models and endophenotypes of anxiety and
(Danio rerio). Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 26, 725–729 (2004). stress disorders. In Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of
29. Echevarria, D.J., Hammack, C.M., Pratt, D.W. & Hosemann, J.D. A novel test Progress (eds. Davis, K.L., Charney, D., Coyle, J.T. & Nemeroff, C.) 885–900
battery to assess global drug effects using the zebrafish. Int. J. Comp. (American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Nashville, Tennessee,
Psychol. 21, 19–34 (2008). 2002).
30. Bencan, Z. & Levin, E.D. The role of α7 and α4β2 nicotinic receptors in the 59. Messick, S. Validity of psychological assessment: validation of inferences
nicotine-induced anxiolytic effect in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 95, 408–412 from persons’ responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score
(2008). meaning. Am. Psychol. 50, 741–749 (1995).
31. López-Patiño, M.A., Yu, L., Cabral, H. & Zhdanova, I.V. Anxiogenic effects 60. Trout, J.D. Measurement. In A Companion to the Philosophy of Science
of cocaine withdrawal in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 93, 160–171 (2008). (ed. Newton-Smith, W. H.) 265–276 (Blackwell Publishing, Oxford, 1999).
32. Levin, E.D., Bencan, Z. & Cerutti, D.T. Anxiolytic effects of nicotine in 61. Li, H. The resolution of some paradoxes related to reliability and validity.
zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 90, 54–58 (2007). J. Edu. Behav. Stat. 28, 89–95 (2003).
33. Wilson, D.S., Coleman, K., Clark, A.B. & Biederman, L. Shy-bold continuum 62. Walf, A.A. & Frye, C.A. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of
in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): an ecological study of a anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat. Protoc. 3, 322–328 (2007).
psychological trait. J. Comp. Psychol. 107, 250–260 (1993). 63. Willner, P. Behavioural models in psychopathology. In Behavioural Models
34. Budaev, S.V. Alternative styles in the European wrasse, Symphodus ocellatus: in Psychopathology: Theoretical, Industrial and Clinical Perspectives
boldness-related schooling tendency. Environ. Biol. Fishes 49, 71–78 (1997). (ed. Willner, P.) 3–18 (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1991).
35. Budaev, S.V. How many dimensions are needed to describe temperament in 64. Segal, D.S. & Geyer, M.A. Animal models of psychopathology. In
animals: a factor reanalysis of two data sets. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 11, 17–29 Psychobiological Foundations of Clinical Psychiatry (eds. Judd, L.L. & Groves,
(1998). P.M.) 1–14 (J.B. Lippincott, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, 1985).
36. Brown, C., Jones, F. & Braithwaite, V. In situ examination of boldness– 65. Matthews, K. & Reid, I. Animal models for depression: the anhedonic rat —
shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachyraphis episcopi. Anim. Behav. theory and practice. In New Models for Depression (eds. Ebert, D. & Ebmeier,
70, 1003–1009 (2005). K.P.) 49–71 (Karger, Basel, 1998).
37. Moretz, J.A., Martins, E.P. & Robison, B.D. Behavioral syndromes and the 66. Guo, S. Linking genes to brain, behavior and neurological diseases: what
evolution of correlated behavior in zebrafish. Behav. Ecol. 18, 556–562 can we learn from zebrafish? Genes Brain Behav. 3, 63–74 (2000).
(2007). 67. Kavaliers, M. & Choleris, E. Antipredator responses and defensive behavior:
38. Montgomery, K.C. The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation ecological and ethological approaches for the neurosciences. Neurosci.
and exploratory behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 48, 254–260 (1955). Biobehav. Rev. 25, 577–586 (2001).
39. Craig, W. Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Proc. Natl. 68. Gray, J.A. & McNaughton, N. Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the
Acad. Sci. USA 3, 685–688 (1917). Functions of the Septo-hippocampal System (Oxford University Press, Oxford,
40. McNaughton, N. & Corr, P.J. A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: 2000).
fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 285–305 69. Toth, M. & Zupan, B. Neurobiology of anxiety. In Handbook of Contemporary
(2004). Neuropharmacology Vol. 2 (eds. Sibley, D.R., Hanin, I., Kuhar, M. & Skolnick, P.)
41. Rodgers, R.J., Cao, B-J. & Holmes, A. Animal models of anxiety: an 3–58 (John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey, 2007).
ethological perspective. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 30, 289–304 (1997). 70. Abrams, P.A. Should prey overestimate the risk of predation? Am. Nat. 144,
42. Rodgers, R.J. Animal models of ‘anxiety’: where next? Behav. Pharmacol. 8, 317–328 (1994).
477–496 (1997). 71. Sih, A. Prey uncertainty and the balancing of antipredator and feeding
43. Risner, M.L., Lemerise, E., Vukmanic, E.V. & Moore, A. Behavioral spectral needs. Am. Nat. 139, 1052–1069 (1990).
sensitivity of the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Vision Res. 46, 2625–2635 (2006). 72. Lima, S.L. & Bednekoff, P.A. Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator
44. Yager, D. Behavioural measures of the spectral sensitivity of dark-adapted behavior: the predation risk hypothesis. Am. Nat. 153, 649–659 (1999).
goldfish. Nature 220, 1052–1053 (1968). 73. Prior, H. & Sachser, N. Effects of enriched housing environment on the
45. Fuiman, L.A. & Magurran, A.E. Development of predator defenses in fishes. behaviour of young male and female mice in four exploratory tasks.
Rev. Fish Biol. Fish. 4, 145–183 (1994). J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 37, 57–68 (1994).
46. Peitsaro, N., Kaslin, J., Anichtchik, O.V. & Panula, P. Modulation of the 74. Chapillon, P., Mannechpe, C., Belzung, C. & Caston, J. Rearing environmental
histaminergic system and behaviour by a-fluoromethylhistidine in zebrafish. enrichment in two inbred strains of mice: 1. Effects on emotional reactivity.
J. Neurochem. 86, 432–441 (2003). Behav. Genet. 29, 41–46 (1999).
47. Lockwood, B., Bjerke, S., Kobayashi, K. & Guo, S. Acute effects of alcohol on 75. Roy, V., Belzung, C., Delarue, C. & Chapillon, P. Environmental enrichment in
larval zebrafish: a genetic system for large-scale screening. Pharmacol. BALB/c mice: effects in classical tests of anxiety and exposure to a predatory
Biochem. Behav. 77, 647–654 (2004). odor. Physiol. Behav. 74, 313–320 (2001).

nature protocols | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | 227


protocol
76. Bertoglio, L.J. & Carobrez, A.P. Previous maze experience required to   96. van Raaij, M.T.M., Pit, D.S.S., Balm, P.H., Steffens, A.B. & van der Thillart,
increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze G.E.E. Behavioral strategy and the physiological stress response in rainbow
model of anxiety. Behav. Brain Res. 108, 197–203 (2000). trout exposed to severe hypoxia. Horm. Behav. 30, 85–92 (1996).
77. File, S.E. One trial tolerance to the anxiolytic effects of chlordiazepoxide in   97. Façanha, M.F. & Gomes, L.d.C. Efficacy of menthol as an anesthetic for
the plus-maze. Psychopharmacology 100, 281–282 (1990). tambaqui (Colossoma macropomum, Characiformes: Characidae). Acta
78. File, S.E. & Zangrossi, H. Jr. ‘One trial tolerance’ to the anxiolytic actions of Amazon. 35, 71–75 (2005).
benzodiazepine in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic   98. Keene, J.L., Noakes, D.L.G., Moccia, R.D. & Soto, C.G. The efficacy of clove
state? Psychopharmacology 110, 240–244 (1993). oil as an anaesthetic for rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss (Walbaum).
79. Rodgers, R.J. & Shepherd, J.K. Influence of prior maze experience on Aquacult. Res. 29, 89–101 (1998).
behaviour and responses to diazepam in the elevated plus-maze in male mice   99. Holloway, A.C., Keene, J.L., Noakes, D.L.G. & Moccia, R.D. Effects of clove
depends upon treatment regimen and prior maze experience. oil and MS-222 on blood hormone profiles in rainbow trout Oncorhynchus
Psychopharmacology 106, 102–110 (1993). mykiss, Walbaum. Aquacult. Res 35, 1025–1030 (2004).
80. Holmes, A. & Rodgers, R.J. Responses of Swiss-Webster mice to repeated 100. Westerfield, M. The Zebrafish Book: A Guide for the Laboratory Use of
plus-maze experience: further evidence for qualitative shift in emotional Zebrafish Danio (Brachydanio) rerio. (University of Oregon Press, Eugene,
state? Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 60, 473–488 (1998). Oregon, 2000).
81. Maximino, C. et al. Reliability of dark preference in zebrafish: test-retest 101. Spence, R., Gerlach, G., Lawrence, C. & Smith, C. The behaviour and ecology
© 2010 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols

correlations, differing black:white proportions of the apparatus, and inter- of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol. Rev. 83, 13–34 (2008).
laboratory replicability. Behav. Processes (in press). 102. Dunbar, R.I.M. Some aspects of research design and their implications in
82. Miklósi, Á. & Andews, R.J. The zebrafish as a model for behavioral studies. the observational study of behaviour. Behaviour 58, 78–98 (1976).
Zebrafish 3, 227–234 (2006). 103. Noakes, D.L.G. & Baylis, J.R. Behavior. In Methods for Fish Biology
83. Langsdale, J.R.M. Developmental changes in the opacity of larval herring, (eds. Schreck, C. B. & Moyle, P. B.) 555–574 (American Fisheries Society,
Clupea harengus, and their implications for vulnerability to predation. Bethesda, Maryland, 1990).
J. Mar. Biol. Assays 73, 225–232 (1993). 104. Volpato, G. Considerações metodológicas sobre os testes de preferência na
84. McClure, M. Development and evolution of melanophore patterns in avaliação do bem-estar em peixes [Methodological considerations about
fishes of the genus Danio (Teleostei: Cyprinidae). J. Morphol. 241, 83–105 preference tests for the evaluation of well-being in fish]. Rev. Brasil.
(1999). Zootecnia 36, 53–61 (2007).
85. Watkins, J., Miklósi, Á. & Andrew, R.J. Early asymmetries in the behavior of 105. Faganello, F.R. & Mattioli, R. Anxiolytic-like effect of chlorpheniramine
zebrafish larvae. Behav. Brain Res. 151, 177–183 (2004). in inhibitory avoidance in goldfish submitted to telencephalic ablation.
86. Walsh, R.N. & Cummins, R.A. The open-field test: a critical review. Progr. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 269–274 (2007).
Psychol. Bull. 83, 482–504 (1976). 106. Ninkovic, J. & Bally-Cuif, L. The zebrafish as a model system for assessing
87. Hurd, M.W., Debruyne, J., Straume, M. & Cahill, G.M. Circadian rhythms of the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. Methods 39, 262–274 (2006).
locomotor activity in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 65, 465–472 (1998). 107. Grunwald, D.J. & Eisen, J.S. Headwaters of the zebrafish—emergence of a
88. Clark, C.W. & Levy, D.A. Diel vertical migrations by juvenile sockeye salmon new model vertebrate. Nat. Rev. Genet. 3, 717–724 (2002).
and the antipredation window. Am. Nat. 131, 271–290 (1988). 108. Johansen, R., Needham, J.R., Colquhoun, D.J., Poppe, T.T. & Smith, A.J.
89. Levy, D.A. Sensory mechanisms and selective advantage for diel vertical Guidelines for health and welfare monitoring of fish used in research.
migration in juvenile sockeye salmon, Oncorhynchus nerka. Can. J. Fish. Lab. Anim. 40, 323–340 (2006).
Aquat. Sci. 47, 1796–1802 (1990). 109. American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, American Fisheries
90. Neilson, J.D. & Perry, R.J. Diel vertical migrations of marine fishes: an Society & American Institute of Fisheries Research Biologists. Guidelines for
obligate or facultative process? Adv. Mar. Biol. 26, 115–167 (1990). use of fishes in field research. Fisheries 13, 16–23 (1988).
91. Wedemeyer, G.A., Barton, B.A. & McLeay, D.J. Stress and acclimation. In 110. Zhdanova, I.V., Wang, S.Y., Leclair, O.U. & Danilova, N.P. Melatonin
Methods for Fish Biology (eds. Schreck, C.B. & Moyle, P.B.) 451–489 promotes sleep-like state in zebrafish. Brain Res. 903, 263–268 (2001).
(American Fisheries Society, Bethesda, Maryland, 1990). 111. Magalhães, D.d.P., Cunha, R.A.d., Santos, J.A.A.d., Buss, D.F. & Baptista,
92. Barton, B.A. Salmonid fishes differ in their cortisol and glucose responses D.F. Behavioral response of zebrafish Danio rerio Hamilton 1822 to
to handling and transport stress. North Am. J. Aquacult. 62, 12–18 sublethal stress by sodium hypochlorite: ecotoxicological assay using an
(2000). image analysis biomonitoring system. Ecotoxicology 16, 417–422 (2007).
93. Barton, B.A. Physiological and condition-related indicators of environmental 112. Covey, T.R., Lee, E.D. & Henion, J.D. High-speed liquid chromatography/
stress in fish. In Biological Indicators of Aquatic Ecosystem Health (ed. Adams, tandem mass spectrometry for the determination of drugs in biological
S.M.) 111–148 (American Fisheries Society, Bethesda, Maryland, 2002). samples. Analyt. Chem. 58, 2453–2460 (1986).
94. Frisch, A.J. & Anderson, T.A. The response of coral trout (Plectropomus 113. Bloom, H.D. & Perlmutter, A. A sexual aggregating pheromone system in
leopardus) to capture, handling, transport and shallow water stress. Fish the zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio. J. Exp. Zool. 199, 215–226 (1977).
Physiol. Biochem. 23, 23–34 (2000). 114. Breder, C.M. Jr. & Halpern, F. Innate and acquired behavior affecting the
95. Kuwada, H. et al. Effect of fish size, handling stresses and training procedure aggregation of fishes. Physiol. Zool. 19, 154–190 (1946).
on the swimming behavior of hatchery-reared striped jack: implications for 115. Maximino, C. et al. Tank enrichment alters exploratory behavior of zebrafish
stock enhancement. Aquaculture 185, 245–256 (2000). and goldfish. J. Exp. Anim. Sci. (in press).

228 | VOL.5 NO.2 | 2010 | nature protocols

S-ar putea să vă placă și