Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

American Economic Review: Papers & Proceedings 2016, 106(5): 435–440

http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.p20161101

The Psychological Lives of the Poor  †

By Frank Schilbach, Heather Schofield, and Sendhil Mullainathan*

There is growing interest in understanding Psychologists often study this underlying


the psychology of the poor—biases that may resource by imposing “cognitive load” to tax
affect ­decision-making are of particular interest. bandwidth and measure the impact on judg-
The sheer diversity of potential biases—hyper- ments and decisions. The many ways to induce
bolic discounting, probabilistic, and judgmental load produce similar results on various band-
errors just to name a few—poses a key chal- width measures and consequences from reduced
lenge. These psychological biases cannot easily System 2 thinking. This insight is particularly
be put into a common unit such as money spent. useful because it implies that bandwidth is both
However, two insights from psychology make malleable and measurable. It also suggests a
this problem more tractable. unified approach of studying the psychology
First, a large body of work points toward a of poverty. We can understand factors in the
­two-system model of the brain.1 System 1 thinks lives of the poor, such as malnutrition, alco-
fast: it is intuitive, automatic, and effortless, and hol consumption, or sleep deprivation, by how
as a result, prone to biases and errors. System they affect bandwidth. And we can understand
2 is slow, effortful, deliberate, and costly, but important decisions made by the poor, such as
typically produces more unbiased and accurate technology adoption or savings, through the lens
results. of how they are affected by bandwidth. Clearly,
Second, when mentally taxed, people are bandwidth is not the only important aspect of the
less likely to engage their System 2 processes. psychological lives of the poor; no single metric
Put simply, one might think of having a (men- can take on this role. However, it provides a way
tal) reserve or capacity for the kind of effortful to at least partly understand a great many of the
thought required to use System 2. When bur- thought processes that drive ­decision-making by
dened, there is less of this resource available for the poor.
use in other judgments and decisions. Though
there is no commonly accepted name for this I. Bandwidth
capacity, we will refer to it in this article as
“bandwidth” (Mullainathan and Shafir 2013). Much like human capital is an abstraction of
a diverse set of skills with common elements,
bandwidth comprises a diverse set of psycho-
logical constructs with common elements. At an
* Schilbach: Department of Economics, Massachusetts intuitive level, bandwidth captures the brain’s
Institute of Technology, 50 Memorial Drive, Building ability to perform the basic functions that under-
E52 Room 560, Cambridge, MA 02139 (e-mail: fschilb@ lie ­higher-order behavior and d­ ecision-making.
mit.edu); Schofield: Perelman School of Medicine and
the Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, 423 Underlying this broad construct are two core
Guardian Dr., Blockley Hall 1102, Philadelphia, PA 19104 components, measures of which are typically
(e-mail: hschof@upenn.edu); Mullainathan: Department used to capture bandwidth.
of Economics, Harvard, 1805 Cambridge Street, Littauer The first component is cognitive capacity,
Center, M-18, Cambridge, MA 02138 (e-mail: mullain@fas.
harvard.edu). We thank our helpful discussants.
the psychological mechanisms that underlie our
† 
Go to http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.p20161101 to visit ability to solve problems, retain information,
the article page for additional materials and author disclo- engage in logical reasoning, and so on. The
sure statement(s).
1 
second is executive control, which underlies
This ­two-system model has direct relevance to many of the ability to manage our cognitive activities.
the heuristics and biases familiar to economists. Kahneman
and Frederick (2002) and more recently Kahneman (2011) Executive control oversees planning, attention
provide reviews. Fudenberg and Levine (2006) develop a allocation, initiating and inhibiting actions, and
model with two systems in the context of time discounting.  impulse control. It determines our ability to
435
436 AEA PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS MAY 2016

focus, to shift attention, to work with informa- by limited time to respond, the correlations
tion in our memory, and to ­self-monitor. between judgments of risks and benefits were
These components are rich in nuance, but they significantly more pronounced than when given
share the common feature that both are scarce more time to ponder a response. The same affec-
resources, the taxing of which causes negative tive evaluation apparently serves as a heuristic
spillovers to other aspects of cognitive function- attribute for assessments of both benefits and
ing. In this sense, while the detailed distinctions risks when resources are limited.
between different brain capacities are central to Economists have applied these ideas to more
any psychological investigation, they are less standard economic tasks, such as s­mall-stakes
central to those interested in the underlying risk aversion or monetary discounting, typically
determinants or downstream consequences of finding an impact of diminished bandwidth
these capacities. (Deck and Jahedi 2015). Similar results have
One important feature of bandwidth is that it been found in many other decisions that rely on
can be readily measured, both in the lab and in cognitive capacity and executive control, such
field settings. One example you may be familiar as food choice. Shiv and Fedorikhin (1999)
with, the Raven’s matrices test, measures indi- is a canonical example in which participants
viduals’ capacity to think logically and solve chose between slices of cake and fruit salad
problems in novel situations, independent of under varied levels of load, manipulated through
acquired knowledge. This task is a nearly uni- digit rehearsal. Those whose minds were busy
versally accepted measure of fluid intelligence rehearsing a ­seven-digit number chose the cake,
and a common component of IQ tests (Raven the impulsive choice, 50 percent more often than
1936). The online Appendix describes three those who were rehearsing a ­two-digit number.
other such measures of bandwidth and three Not all replications have produced the same
common features of these tasks: (i) ease of results, and the magnitudes of the original effects
administration; (ii) broad applicability; and (iii) appear likely to be an aberration. However, the
ease of instruction. idea that occupying mental bandwidth dimin-
The basic premise of the tests used to study ishes capacity for ­self-discipline seems to be
bandwidth is that it is possible to “load up” cog- more generally supported by the data.
nitive resources, and to use this additional load to
examine how bandwidth, behaviors, and choices Productivity.—In contrast to the rich body
change. These cognitive load studies have been of evidence on the link between bandwidth and
conducted for over 70 years and are in the canon ­decision-making, evidence on the relationship
of experimental psychology, reliably replicating between bandwidth and productivity is much
in many contexts. As a result, by studying the more limited. There is good reason to believe
effects of cognitive load, we have experimental that this link exists: impaired cognitive func-
evidence of the impact of diminished bandwidth tion, judgment, and ­decision-making likely have
on a wide variety of aspects of mental function. consequences for one’s performance in the labor
market, especially in work that relies heavily
­Decision-Making.—Prospective memory, or on cognitive capacities such as attention, perse-
the ability to remember to execute tasks in verance, or memory. For instance, a rag picker
the future, and executive control are particu- trying to find valuable items among mountains
larly affected by cognitive load (Marsh and of garbage may be particularly affected by
Hicks 1998). For instance, dieters exhibit less reductions in bandwidth. Although these argu-
­self-control in the eating arena and people dis- ments are intuitive, it would be presumptuous
count delayed rewards at significantly higher to believe that these effects must exist, and the
rates when under load (Ward and Mann 2000; magnitude of effects may vary widely with con-
Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney 2003). text. This is an area of research ripe for further
These shifts in underlying cognition mani- investigation.
fest in myriad contexts and for w ­ ide-ranging
outcomes. For example, Finucane et al. (2000) Utility.—All economists would agree that pov-
asked respondents to judge the risks and bene- erty lowers utility by decreasing consumption.
fits of various products and technologies (e.g., However, it may lower utility through an addi-
nuclear power). When bandwidth was taxed tional channel: individuals with low b­ andwidth
VOL. 106 NO. 5 The Psychological Lives of the Poor 437

(say, due to physical pain) may find consump- II.  Poverty and Bandwidth
tion of other goods less enjoyable. That is, the
utility from a given basket of goods may be While it may seem odd that a person’s fun-
reduced by low bandwidth. There is suggestive damental “capacity” can be easily affected in
evidence of such a link. In a study to determine many basic dimensions, that oddity is precisely
how best to rehabilitate prisoners of war from the point. We have traditionally viewed cogni-
malnourishment, 32 volunteers s­emi-starved tive capacity as fixed, but in fact it can change
themselves for six months and then followed with circumstances. More specifically, we will
varied rehabilitation diets (Keys et al. 1950). now discuss how bandwidth can be influenced
The changes to the participants’ physiological, by poverty. Our discussion includes some of the
physical, cognitive, and psychological func- factors which have already been shown to influ-
tions were closely tracked. Unsurprisingly, ence bandwidth, and others for which evidence
interest in food increased as the starvation is limited but suggestive, warranting additional
period progressed. Perhaps more surprisingly investigation.
though, hunger substantially impacted individ-
uals’ interest in sex and other activities, gen- Nutrition.—Economists understand nutrition
erating a ­one standard deviation decline from both as consumption—consuming food pro-
baseline levels. While not conclusive, this evi- vides pleasure—and as an investment—nutrition
dence is fascinating as it suggests an entirely can also affect physical productivity. However,
new channel of the link between income and too little food may also affect mental function:
happiness. thoughts may become lethargic, attention diffi-
cult to sustain, and temptations harder to resist.
Old Questions Revisited.—Studying band- Hunger may be more than unpleasantness or a
width also opens up a new approach to many cause of physical weakness; it might also dimin-
traditional topics in development economics. In ish bandwidth.
studying technology adoption, for instance, we Schofield (2014) tests this idea using an
typically consider learning by doing, credit con- experiment to examine the impact of provid-
straints, or even psychological phenomena such ing calories on measures of bandwidth among
as learning through noticing. However, every ­low-BMI ­cycle-rickshaw drivers in India. One
phase of technology adoption clearly relies on task had subjects search through a grid of sym-
bandwidth. For example, bandwidth is neces- bols for a specific set of symbols and cross them
sary to an understanding of how to use the tech- out. This tedious task requires mental stamina,
nology and adapt it to local circumstances, and making it a natural measure of bandwidth.
to having the ­self-control and advance-planning Individuals with higher caloric intake showed
to save up capital to acquire the new technology. an almost immediate 12 percent improvement
Studying bandwidth, and the factors influencing in performance on such tasks, a gain that was
it, can allow us to better understand technology sustained at endline.2
adoption. Of course, such effects are particularly inter-
Many topics traditionally studied by esting to economists if they also affect economic
development economists have this feature. decisions. Schofield (2014) finds some evi-
Consider, for instance, the impact of literacy dence of this in a r­eal-world effort discounting
on ­decision-making. Of course, one enormous task in which participants were given a choice
benefit of literacy is that it allows individuals to provide no labor and earn nothing, or take a
to access knowledge through books or news- journey with a lighter load today or a heavier
papers. However, literacy may also substan- load tomorrow, with both trips earning the same
tially unburden individuals’ bandwidth as they
are not forced to keep all relevant information
stored in their minds—exactly as in the load 2 
Treated individuals received a portion of their com-
experiments—since they can simply take notes. pensation ­in-kind, as food, resulting in somewhat greater
Imagine the load imposed on an illiterate person attrition among ­high-earning treated individuals at endline.
While likely to work against finding an effect on labor mar-
who receives detailed instructions on how to use ket outcomes, its impact on cognitive function tests is less
fertilizer in the next season! clear. A second iteration of the study addressing this concern
is ongoing. 
438 AEA PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS MAY 2016

p­ayment tomorrow. Treated participants were The second study uses ­quasi-experimental vari-
25 percent more likely to take the journey today ation in actual wealth. Indian sugar cane farm-
rather than delay until tomorrow at the cost of ers receive income annually at harvest time, and
a more difficult trip, suggesting a meaningful find it hard to smooth their consumption. As a
reduction in discount rates for effort in their pro- result, they experience cycles of poverty—poor
fessional activities. before harvest and richer after—generating
the opportunity to compare cognitive capacity
Alcohol.—Excessive alcohol consumption across states and within person.
has long been associated with poverty (Fisher Both studies show large and direct impacts of
1926), but its economic consequences are poverty on bandwidth, which tells us something
poorly understood. Steele and Joseph’s (1990) about poverty’s mental consequences; when you
“alcohol myopia” theory offers insights into the are poor, economic challenges are more than
effects of alcohol on human behavior. This the- just economic, they are also cognitive. These
ory posits that the narrowing effect on attention difficult decisions tax scarce cognitive resources
is a defining feature of alcohol, which in turn even further.
causes individuals to focus on simple, present,
and salient cues. Viewed through the lens of this Other Factors.—Many other correlates of
paper, alcohol lowers bandwidth. poverty may impact bandwidth, including
Schilbach (2015) conducted a t­hree-week physical pain, sleep deprivation, or noise pol-
field experiment to investigate whether such lution. While lab evidence, described in the
cognitive effects can translate into economi- online Appendix, suggests that these factors
cally meaningful ­real-world consequences. In can severely impede many aspects of cognitive
his study, reducing daytime drinking among function, field evidence on economic outcomes
­low-income workers in Chennai via financial is much more limited.
incentives increased individuals’ daily savings Other factors do not fit as well. First, stress or
at a study office by 60 percent compared to a allostatic load fits only imperfectly. Some com-
control group that received similar average study ponents of stress—what we commonly refer to
payments independent of their alcohol consump- as worries or having something on your mind—
tion. A simple calibration exercise suggests that fit the concept of bandwidth well. Stress also
these effects are not purely mechanical, i.e., entails a biological element though, which has
individuals do not just save more as a conse- long-term physical and mental consequences.
­
quence of increased earnings. This argument is For instance, chronic stress can have cardiovas-
further supported by the fact that sobriety incen- cular consequences or may lead to depression.
tives and the commitment savings feature were Such effects extend beyond the notion of band-
substitutes in terms of their effects on savings. width described above.
Second, while depression is an important
Monetary Concerns.—Being poor means understudied aspect of the lives of the poor, it
having less money to buy things, but it also does not fit well under the umbrella of band-
means having to spend more of one’s bandwidth width. Some of the symptoms of depression,
managing that money. The poor must manage such as sleep deprivation or appetite loss, may
sporadic income, juggle expenses, and make produce effects on bandwidth as described
difficult t­rade-offs. Even when the poor are not above. However, depression entails a plethora of
actually making financial decisions, these preoc- other symptoms, such as hopelessness, helpless-
cupations can be distracting. Thinking and fret- ness, sadness, or even suicidal tendencies, that
ting about money can effectively tax bandwidth. go well beyond our concept of bandwidth.
To establish a causal relationship between
poverty and mental function, Mani et al. (2013) Why Focus on Poverty?—Everyone has lim-
use two distinct but complementary designs. ited bandwidth, and many of the factors listed
First, they experimentally induce rich and poor above—hunger, pain, or sleep deprivation—can
participants to think about everyday financial impact anyone. And the psychological stud-
demands. For the rich, these financial snags are ies described in this paper were conducted on
of little consequence. For the poor, however, they a wide spectrum of people. Some factors such
can trigger persistent and distracting c­ oncerns. as monetary concerns do seem to single out the
VOL. 106 NO. 5 The Psychological Lives of the Poor 439

poor, but perhaps other factors, such as concern classify and identify those likely to experience
for status, may disproportionately impact the decrements in bandwidth. It is also a first step
rich. However, these observations do not invali- toward finding ways to limit the impact of band-
date bandwidth as a lens for studying poverty for width reductions among the poor.
several reasons. Third, evidence relating to bandwidth occurs
First, take the analogy to human capital, in two, typically distinct, parts: (i) direct evi-
another concept that applies across the income dence that some factor affects bandwidth; and
spectrum. Understanding the lives of the (ii) often indirect evidence that these changes in
poor through this lens has proven invaluable. bandwidth are likely to affect many downstream
Similarly, understanding various correlates behaviors. We have little evidence showing the
of poverty through the lens of bandwidth can whole chain from factors impacting bandwidth
be equally insightful by drawing attention to to changes in ­real-world choices with serious
impacts we might not traditionally consider, or consequences, such as mortgage financing or
to the potential for feedback loops. The univer- the choice of medical care, operating through
sality of the concept increases its usefulness, bandwidth. This dearth of evidence is particu-
allowing us to apply it in many contexts around larly acute outside of the lab. Crisper evidence is
the world, knowing that we are relying on a needed to fully map out these relationships and
basic feature of human psychology. understand the scope of these impacts in field
Second, there are reasons to believe that the settings.
effects of diminished bandwidth are larger for the Finally, benchmarking the magnitude of deci-
poor. Individuals in poverty are more likely to be sion errors against other commonly accepted
exposed to many of these factors (e.g., malnu- metrics such as dollar values would help to cal-
trition, pain, heat) and to experience them more ibrate the importance of bandwidth in the lives
extensively. Further, the poor are less likely to of the poor.
have coping mechanisms, such as direct deposits
or automatic enrollments, available to reduce the References
negative effects of limited bandwidth. Not only
is their exposure greater, but the “same mistake” Deck, Cary, and Salar Jahedi. 2015. “The Effect
is likely to be more costly for the poor than for of Cognitive Load on Economic Decision
the rich. Finally, money is a potential substitute Making: A Survey and New Experiments.”
for bandwidth. It is often possible to buy your- European Economic Review 78: 97–119.
self the extra slack you need—hiring someone Finucane, Melissa L., Ali Alhakami, Paul Slovic,
to cook and clean—or to reduce the factors and Stephen M. Johnson. 2000. “The Affect
which lead to lower bandwidth—purchasing a Heuristic in Judgments of Risks and Benefits.”
comfortable bed in a quiet neighborhood. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 13 (1):
1–17.
III.  Research Directions Forward Fisher, Irving. 1926. Prohibition at Its Worst. New
York: MacMillan.
For all of its promise, the study of bandwidth Fudenberg, Drew, and David K. Levine. 2006. “A
in development is young, so we conclude with Dual-Self Model of Impulse Control.” Ameri-
future research directions. can Economic Review 96 (5): 1449–76.
First, much as we often examine outcomes Hinson, John M., Tina L. Jameson, and Paul Whit-
such as health or income, bandwidth should ney. 2003. “Impulsive Decision Making and
become more commonly measured. It is rela- Working Memory.” Journal of Experimental
tively easy to integrate measures of bandwidth Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
into RCTs, and doing so would allow us to sum- tion 29 (2): 298–306.
marize many dispersed effects using a metric Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking, Fast and
with ­well-known downstream consequences. Slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux.
Second, specific to poverty itself, additional Kahneman, Daniel, and Shane Frederick. 2002.
work to clarify what it means to “feel poor,” “Representativeness Revisited: Attribute Sub-
and the mechanisms leading to these percep- stitution in Intuitive Judgment.” In Heuristics
tions, would move the field forward signifi- and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judg-
cantly. Understanding these perceptions helps to ment, edited by Thomas Gilovich, Dale G­ riffin,
440 AEA PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS MAY 2016

and Daniel Kahneman, 49–81. Cambridge, Mainly Reproductive.” Master’s thesis, Uni-
UK: Cambridge University Press. versity of London.
Keys, Ancel, Josef Brožek, Austin Henschel, Olaf Schilbach, Frank. 2015. “Alcohol and
Mickelsen, and Henry L. Taylor. 1950. The ­Self-Control: A Field Experiment in India.”
Biology of Human Starvation. Minneapolis: Unpublished.
University of Minnesota Press. Schofield, Heather. 2014. “The Economic Costs
Mani, Anandi, Sendhil Mullainathan, Eldar Sha- of Low Caloric Intake: Evidence from India.”
fir, and Jiaying Zhao. 2013. “Poverty Impedes Unpublished.
Cognitive Function.” Science 341 (6149): Shiv, Baba, and Alexander Fedorikhin. 1999.
976–80. “Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of
Marsh, Richard L., and Jason L. Hicks. 1998. Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision
“Event-Based Prospective Memory and Exec- Making.” Journal of Consumer Research 26
utive Control of Working Memory.” Journal of (3): 278–92.
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Steele, Claude M., and Robert A. Josephs. 1990.
and Cognition 24 (2): 336–49. “Alcohol Myopia: Its Prized and Dangerous
Mullainathan, Sendhil, and Eldar Shafir. 2013. Effects.” American Psychologist 45 (8): 921–
Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So 33.
Much. New York: Henry Holt & Company. Ward, Andrew, and Traci Mann. 2000. “Don’t
Raven, John C. 1936. “Mental Tests Used in Mind If I Do: Disinhibited Eating under Cog-
Genetic Studies: The Performance of Related nitive Load.” Journal of Personality and Social
Individuals on Tests Mainly Educative and Psychology 78 (4): 753–63.

S-ar putea să vă placă și