Sunteți pe pagina 1din 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/270568302

Antecedents and Consequences of Salesperson Job Satisfaction: Meta-Analysis


and Assessment of Causal Effects

Article  in  Journal of Marketing Research · February 1993


DOI: 10.2307/3172514

CITATIONS READS

860 9,334

2 authors:

Steven P. Brown Robert A. Peterson


University of Houston University of Texas at Austin
35 PUBLICATIONS   6,338 CITATIONS    139 PUBLICATIONS   12,197 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Convenience samples of college students and Research Reproducibility View project

TYPE IV Error in Marketing Research: The Investigation of ANOVA Interactions View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Steven P. Brown on 07 January 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


STEVEN P. BROWN and ROBERT A. PETERSON*

A three-phase quantitative investigation of relatianships involving salesperson job


satisfaction was undertaken. First, the strength, valence, and consistency of pairwise
relationships were assessed by means of a meta-analysis. Second, methodological
characteristics coded as moderator variables were used to account for variability
in study effects. Finally, weighted mean correlations resulting from the analysis of
pairwise relationships were used to evaluate a causal model of antecedents and
consequences of job satisfaction. In general, relatianships involving job satisfaction
were robust across study contexts. Systematic moderating effects of type of sales-
force and operationalization of job satisfaction were found, Several summary con-
clusions about antecedents and consequences of salesperson job satisfaction are
drawn from the analyses.

Antecedents and Consequences of Salesperson


Job Satisfaction: Meta-Analysis and
Assessment of Causal Effects

Job satisfaction is one of the most widely studied con- individual study fmdings, and account for variability in
structs in salesforce research. Empirical research has in- study effects attributable to methodological decisions made
vestigated numerous variables in attempts to determine by researchers. It may also afford summary conclusions
how it is created and how it affects other work out- about nomological effects involving salesperson job sat-
comes. Despite this extensive research, consensus has isfaction.
not been established about several important relation- In particular, the objectives of our study are to (I)
ships. The strength, significance, and generality of re- assess the strength and consistency of pairwise relation-
lationships involving job satisfaction have not been es- ships involving salesperson job satisfaction, (2) account
tablished by integrating results across the research stream. for variance in these relationships by using study char-
Moreover, no summary assessment of how job satisfac- acteristics as moderator variables, and (3) evaluate a causal
tion is detemiined and how it influences other job atti- model of commonly studied effects involving job satis-
tudes and behaviors has been undertaken. faction to establish their generality.
Because of the current status of salesforce job satis-
faction research, a quantitative assessment of relation- THE RESEARCH DOMAIN
ships involving job satisfaction across a variety of sales The Job Satisfaction Construct
contexts appears worthwhile. Such an assessment may
settle unresolved questions, establish the generality of Locke (1976, p. 1300) defined job satisfaction as "a
pleasurable or positive emotional state resulting from the
appraisal of one's job or job experiences." He differ-
entiated between job satisfaction and morale by noting
that satisfaction has an individual rather than a group
"Sleven P. Brown is Assistani Professor of Marketing and Tcny
Research Fellow. Terry College of Business, Univcrsily of Georgia.
referent and a temporal orientation toward the past and
Robert A. Pelcrson holds (he John T Sluart 111 Centennial Chair in present rather than toward the future. Churchill. Ford,
Business Adniinisiraiion and ihe Charles Hurwit/ Fellowship at the and Walker (1974, p. 255) delineated the conceptual do-
University of Texas at Austin. main of the job satisfaction construct as "all character-
The authors appreciate the helpful suggestions of Tom Leigh. Bill istics of the job itself and the work environment which
Cron. Jeff Sagcr, Subhash Sharma. Sritiivas Reddy. the editor, and
three anonymous JMR reviewers. [industrial] salesmen find rewarding, fulfilling, and sat-
isfying, or frustrating and unsatisfying."

Journal of Marketing Research


Vol. XXX (February 1993), 63-77
64 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Operationally, job satisfaction has been investigated appears to favor the causal precedence of job satisfaction
as a salesperson's affective state t^lative to several job (Bartol 1979; Reichers 1985). Organizational commit-
facet.s. including the supervisor, the work itself, pay, ment and job satisfaction have consistently been strongly
promotion opportunities, and coworkers (Churchill. Ford, and ptisitively correlated in salesforce contexts as well
and Walker 1974; Smith. Kendall, and Hulin 1969). Many as in other organizational settings (Bateman and Strasser
researchers have considered the effects of these job fac- 1984; Johnston et al. 1990).
ets separately, whereas others have summed or averaged Propensity to leave and turnover. Propensity to leave
them into a global measure. Other widely used measures the organization, a behavioral intention that precedes ac-
of job satisfaction divide the construct into intrinsic and tual salesperson turnover (e.g., Futrell and Parasuratiian
extrinsic components (e.g.. Porter and Lawler 1968), 1984), has consistently been found to be related nega-
whereas still others merely consider the construct glob- tively to job satisfaction and organizational commitment
ally with no division into separate facets (e.g., Bagozzi (e.g.. Johnston et al, 1987, 1990). Empirical research
1980a.b; Hackman and Oldham 1975; Pearson, Barker, has consistently shown that propensity to leave is an ef-
and Elliott 1957). fective predictor of salesperson turnover (e.g.. Johnston
et al. 1990; Sager, Varadarajan, and Futrell 1988) but
has been inconsistent with respect to antecedent effects
Correlates of Jab Satisfaction
on propensity to leave. Some researchers (e.g.. Johnston
More than 30 constructs have been investigated em- et al. 1987; Fem, Avila, and Grewal 1989) have found
pirically as antecedents or consequences of job satisfac- that performance exerts a significant effect on propensity
tion. To systematically review these antecedents and to leave, whereas others (e.g., Futrell and Parasuratnan
consequences, we sttoicture our discussion by grouping 1984) have not. Similarly, some researchers (Sager and
specific pairwise relationships into four summary cate- Johnston 1989; Sager, Varadarajan. and Futrell 1988)
gories: (1) work outcomes, (2) individual differences. have found a direct effect of satisfaction on propensity
(3) role perceptions., and (4) two types of organizational to leave, whereas others (e.g., Johnston et al. 1990) have
variables, supervisory behaviors and job/task character- not.
istics. Each of these summary categories consists of spe-
A negative relationship between job satisfaction and
cific pairwise relationships. The following overview of
turnover has con.sistently been found (Johnston et al. 1990;
these relationships idenliftes inconsistencies and gaps in
Sager, Varadarajan, and Futrell 1988). Prevailing mixlels
prior research and provides a context for our investiga-
represent salesperson turnover as a direct result of pro-
tion.
pensity to leave (Futrell and Parasuraman 1984; John-
ston et al. 1990). Propensity to leave is generally be-
Work Outcomes lieved to mediate the effects of other antecedent variables
Much of the interest in salesperson job satisfaction stems on salesperson turnover.
from its theoretical and empirical relationships with job
outcomes such as pertbmiance. organizational commit- Individual Differences
ment, propensity to leave, and turnover. Each is dis- Individual differences include both demographic and
cussed briefly. dispositional variables. Demographic variables that have
Sales performance. An extensive research literature been investigated include age, education, sales experi-
has addressed the question of whether satisfaction causally ence, and organizational tenure. Research on career stages
precedes performance or vice versa (e.g., Bagozzi 1980a; has suggested that salespeople's perspectives on their jobs
Schwab and Cummings 1970). Considerable conceptual and profession evolve over titne. which in tum suggests
and empirical evidence appears to favor the conclusion that variables such as age and organizational tenure may
that performance is primarily antecedent to job satisfac- be related, perhaps nonlinearly, to job satisfaction (e.g.,
tion (e.g.. Bagozzi 1980a: Sheridan and Slocum 1975; Cron 1984; Cron and Slocum 1986).
Wanous 1974), though other evidence (e.g.. Behrman With the exception of work motivation and task-spe-
and Perreault 1984; Dubinsky and Hartley i986a) sug- cific and generalized self-esteem (e.g., Bagozzi !98()a,b;
gests that the relationship may be spurious and attrib- Hafer and McCuen 1985; Kohli 1985), relatively few
utable to common antecedent variables. Regardless of dispositional variables have been studied in relation to
which of these interpretations is tnore accurate, sates job satisfaction. In current models, the effects of these
performance and job satisfaction have shown a consis- variables on job satisfaction arc considered to be indi-
tent but very modest positive association. rect, mediated by sales performance. That is, salespeo-
Organizational commitment. The question of causal ple with higher motivation level and more prositive self-
ordering has also been debated for Ihe job satisfaction- concept are more likely to be high performers because
organizational commitment relationship. Some authors they are willing to exert the necessry effort and bring
(e.g., Bateman and Strasser 1984) have argued that or- greater self-confidence to the selling task (e.g.. Bagozzi
ganizational commitment precedes job satisfaction, 1980a,b). According to prevailing models, the resulting
whereas others have maintained that job satisfaction is positive effect on sales performance is conveyed indi-
the antecedent (e.g.. Bluedom 1982; Johnston et al. 1990). rectly to job satisfaction through its positive association
The preponderance of conceptual and empirical evidence with perfonnance (e.g., Bagozzi 1980a).
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION

Role Perceptions have been related to job satisfaction include task auton-
Salespeople's perceptions about their role a.s organi- omy, task significance, task variety, participation, influ-
zational boundary spanners (Pruden 1969; Pruden and ence over standards, innovativeness required, job in-
Reese 1972) have been frequently studied and found to volvement, value congruence, and pay (e.g., Becherer,
have important effects on job satisfaction. In particular, Morgan, and Richard 1982; Dubinsky and Skinner 1984;
effeets of three role perception constructs, role ambi- Teas 1981). In general, greater job satisfaction appears
guity, role conflict, and role clarity, have often been as- to be associated with more significant, challenging, and
sessed.' Both role conflict and role ambiguity have been varied tasks, with greater amounts of participation and
consistently negatively related to job satisfaction (Behr- involvement, with shared values, and with higher pay.
man and Perreault 1984; Jackson and Schuler 1985). Even Potential Moderator Variables
so, though the valence of these relationships has gen-
erally been negative, the magnitude of the effeets has As Weitz (1981) has observed, findings of "simple
varied considerably across studies (e.g., Bagozzi 1980b; and sovereign" relationships that hold across the great
Behrman and Perreault 1984; Ford, Walker, and Chur- variety of sales contexts are unlikely, and contingency
chill 1979). The effects of role ambiguity and role con- approaches are necessary to understand more fully how
flict on sales performance have been even less consistent outcomes of sales work are determined. Research on job
(cf. Bagozzi 1978; Hampton, Dubinsky, and Skinner satisfaction has been conducted in a variety of sales con-
1986). Role clarity has consistently been found to be re- texts with different types of salesforces and measure-
lated positively to job satisfaction (e.g., Teas, Wacker, ment instruments. In our investigation, several different
and Hughes 1979; Kohli 1985), though the magnitude study characteristics are coded as ptitential moderator
of the effect has varied across studies. variables to account for variance in effect sizes. These
characteristics include the type of salesforce investigated
Organizational Variables (industrial or nonindustrial), the type of customer served
(organizational or individual), whether a single-company
Factors related to job design, organizational structure
salesforce or multiple-company salesforces were stud-
and communication, supervisory behaviors, and com-
ied, and job satisfaction measure (global vs. "by facet"
pensation have consistently been found to have Impor-
tant effects on job satisfaction (e.g., Churchill, Ford, and measurement).'
I

Walker 1976). The many organizational variables that Type of Salesforce


have been studied can be partitioned Into two broad
subgroups, supervisory behaviors and job and task char- The duties and responsibilities of sales jobs vary widely
acteristics. across industries (Moncrief 1986). Various types of
Supervi.'iory behaviors. The nature and quality of the salesforces have been studied, and job type has been found
interaction sales managers maintain with salespeople sig- to moderate effects involving satisfaction in sales and
nificantly influence the latter's job satisfaction (e.g., other contexts (e.g., Ivancevicb and Donnelly 1974).
Churchill, Ford, and Walker 1976; Jaworski and Kohli Thus, it is possible that the relationships between role
1991). In general, studies have found that greater amounts perception variables and satisfaction may be stronger
of consideration (e.g.. Teas 1981), feedback (Teas 1981), among salespeople with more difficult and challenging
communication (Churchill, Ford, and Walker 1976), close boundary-spanning resptmsibilities because role stress and
supervision (Comer, Maehleit, and Lagace 1989), and the necessity of coping with it are more integral to such
contingent approving behavior (e.g., Kohli 1985) are as- jobs (e.g., Behrman and Perreault 1984; Pruden 1969),
sociated with greater job satisfaction. Arbitrary punish- Type of Customer Served
ing behavior, not surprisingly, has been related nega-
tively to job satisfaction (Kohli 1985; Schul, Remington, Type of customer served is closely related to type of
and Berl 1990). These findings have been consistent across sales job, but sufficiently distinct to warrant separate
studies. consideration (cf. Churchill et al. 1985). Churchill et al.
Job/task characteristics. Job/task characteristics that (1985) found modest mixlerating effects of customer type
on relationships between several types of predictor vari-
ables and sales performanee.
'Though the literature makes little conceptual distinction between Number of Company Salesforces Studied
role ambiguity and role clarity (aside from the fact thai one is ihe
reverse of the oiher), Ihc constmct.s are treated separately here lor Though the majority of studies involved only one
three reasons. First, ihey arc likely to have different antecedents (e.g., salesforce, this was often recognized as a potential lim-
managerial action.s that remove ihc sources of role ambiguity are likely
to improve role ctariiy). Secotid, effetts involving role clarity, on the itation on generalizability. Aggregating findings across
average, were slightly greater than those involving ntle ambiguity and
a greater number of outliers had to be removed to achieve homoge-
neity in the aggregated grouping than in the combination of the two
separale subgroups. Third, separate treatment of the effects better re- 'Too few available study effects and/or inadequate descriptions of
riecis the nature of the research being summarized and guards against sample characteristics precluded assessment of additional ptjtential
los.s of interpretive precision from aggregation into overly broad cat- moderator variables .such as company size or longitudinal versus cross-
egories (i.e.. the "apples and oranges problem"; Bangert-Downs .sectionaJ studies.
66 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

studies provides an opportunity to assess the substantive After the relevant studies were identified, eacb was
effect of this potential limitation by comparing results of coded for the potential moderator variables described
studies conducted on one salesforce with those of studies previously. The studies were coded independently by one
conducted on multiple salesforces. of the authors and a second coder. No discrepancies were
found.
Job Satisfaction Measure
The two most commonly used job satisfaction mea- Analysis of Pairwise Relationships
surement scales, the job descriptive index (JDI; Smith, The meta-analysis was conducted according to pro-
Kendall, and Hulin 1969) and INDSALES (Churchill. cedures suggested by Hedges and Olkin (1985). These
Ford, and Walker 1974), both measure satisfaction by procedures are statistically rigorous and their validity is
facet. Other measurement instruments (e.g.. Porter and widely accepted (e.g., Bangert-Downs 1986; Rao and
Lawler 1968; Hackman and Oldham 1975) measure sat- Monroe 1989). They emphasize homogeneity tests to as-
isfaction globally. The aggregation of findings across sess the significance of variation in study effects and
studies affords an opportunity to assess the magnitude of suggest approaches such as partitioning effects by means
nomological validity levels obtained by using the two of moderator variables and eliminating outliers until ho-
different approaches to measuring job satisfaction. mogeneity is achieved (Hedges and Olkin 1985).
The first stage of the meta-analysis entailed conduct-
METHOD
ing an analysis of each pairwise relationship involving
Collection and Coding of Studies job satisfaction for which multiple study effects were
available. Individual study effects were weighted by an
Studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis were iden- estimate of the inverse of the variance {N — 3) to give
tified by searching the ABI-Inform and Psychlit com- greater weight to more precise estimates (Hedges and
puterized databases and conducting issue-by-issue searches Olkin 1985, p. 227 ff.). Homogeneity tests were then
of eight journals and two series of proceedings vol- conducted according to formulas given by Hedges and
umes. References appearing in relevant publications were Olkin (1985, p. 235) to assess the significance of the
searched for additional studies that might be included. variance in effect sizes. Methodological details of the
Tbe effect size metric used in our study was r, the homogeneity tests are provided in the Appendix.
zero-order Pearson product-moment correlation coeffi-
For nonhomogeneous relationships, if elimination of
cient."" Fifty-nine studies were identified that could be
a single outlier did not result in homogeneity and at least
included in the analyses; they contained 254 usable study
10 study effects were available, moderator analyses w. e
effects.*
conducted to account for the variance in effect sizes. Study
effects were partitioned into subgroups on the basis of
values of the moderator variables and the weighted mean
Mssue-by issue searctie<> were conducled of the Journal of Market-
ing Re.seiirch, Journal of Marketing. Journal of the Academy of Mar- observed correlations of the subgroups compared. Sig-
keting Science, Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, nificance tests of differences in subgroup means were
Journal of Applied Psychology, Journal of Business Research. Acad- conducted according to tbe formula given by Rosenthal
emy of Management Journal. Administrative Science Quarterly, and (1984, p. 118). The study effects were then regressed
proceedings volumes published by the American Marketing Associ-
ation and Academy of Marketing Science. on the coded study characteristics to assess whether the
'"Partialed" effects from multiviiriate models, such as regression moderator variables, considered jointly, adequately ac-
or path coefficients, were not used in the analyses because such ef- counted for the variance in study effects.
fects are not comparable across studies unless the same variables are
included in the model in every study (Hunter and Schmidt 1990. p. Causal Model
272).
^A list of Ihe studies included in the meta-analysis is available from Hunter and Schmidt (1990, p. 4()) suggested that meta-
the authors. Some studies that investigated salesperson job satisfaction analysis can be employed in theory development by us-
could noi be included in the analyses for several reasons: (I) some ing aggregated study effects to test causal models. Ac-
were conceptual lathcr ihan empirical (e.g.. Walker, Churchill, and cordingly, we use weighted average effect sizes resulting
Ford 1977). (2) some could not be included because their results were
obviously based on the same data as those in other studies that were from the analyses just described to explore nomological
included (e.g., Kohli utih^ed the same data in his 1985 and 1989 relationships among some of the most frequently studied
studies). (3) some considered only one facet of job .satisfaction (e.g.. and substantively important antecedents and conse-
Motowidlo 1982 ftK'used exclusively on pay satisfaction). (4) some quences of job satisfaction.
mixed salespeople and other types of workers in (he same sample and
did not report results separately for the salespeople (e.g.. Singh and The hypothesized model is depicted in Figure 1. The
Rhodes 1991), (5) some reported results only in multivariate models, relationships represented in the model have all been in-
and these study effects could not be convened to r (e.g.. Teas 1981). vestigated in several empirical studies, but the research
(6) some investigated constructs ihat tlgure in our model but did not findings for these relationships are inconsistent in terms
include job satisfaction (e.g.. Hart. Moncrief, and Pamsuraman 1989), of their significance, magnitude, and, in some cases, va-
(7) some reported relationships that were unique to that study and thus
could not be integrated with those in other studies (e.g.. Puffer 1987). lence. Estimation of tbe mtxicl with study effects aggre-
In all, we identified 30 studies that could not be included in the anal- gated across the research stream is intended to address
ysis for one or more of the preceding reasons. questions about the nature, strength, and generality of
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION

Figure 1 previously, however, Behrman and Perieault (1984) have


HYPOTHESIZED MODEL contended that the frequently observed small ptisitive as-
sociation between performance and satisfaction may be
attributable to the relationship of both with common an-
tecedent variables. Results of several empirical studies
(e.g., Dubinsky and Hartley 1986; Hampton. Dubinsky,
and Skinner 1986) have supported the Behrman and Per-
reault contention.

Consequent Effects
Consistent with the best available conceptual and em-
pirical evidence (Bluedom 1982; Johnston et al. 1990;
Reiehers 1985), organizational commitment is modeled
as a consequence of job satisfaction. Consistent with the
findings of Johnston et al. (1990). the model posits a
direct negative effect of organizational commitment on
propensity to leave and no direct effect of job satisfac-
tion on propensity to leave. That is, the negative effect
of job satisfaction on propensity to leave is posited to be
indirect and mediated by organizational commitment.
the nomological relationships by using all available evi- Inasmuch as sales performance is likely to be partially
dence, This analysis also makes possible an integrative a function of company resources, such as product, rep-
quantitative assessment of the much-debated questions utation, standing in the industry, and office support of
of whether job satisfaction constitutes a cause or an ef- selling efforts (Weitz 1981), it is logical to posit a direct
fect of sales performance and organizational commit- positive effect of sales perfonnance on organizational
ment (cf., Bagozzi 1980a; Johnston et al. 1990). commitment. To the extent that salespeople attribute
For a construct to be included in the analysis, multiple performance outcomes to organizational support, their
study effects relating it to every other construct in the commitment to the organization is likely to increase.
model were required. Because of the limited number of
study effects available, it was not possible to include su- Procedure
pervisory behaviors, job/task variables, or personal The causal model analysis was conducted by con-
characteristics in the causal model. even though they have structing a matrix of weighted mean observed correla-
generally been found to be antecedents of salesperson tions of all of the pairwise relationships among con-
role perceptions and job satisfaction (e.g., Behrman and structs in the tnodel and using the matrix as input to a
Perreault 1984; Churchill, Ford, and Walker 1976; John- LISREL estimation program. In the model, paths con-
ston et al. 1990). The constructs included in the model necting the constructs to their observed measures were
were the ones most frequently studied in relation to job fixed to equal the square root of the sample-size-weighted
satisfaction and each was represented by at least 11 study tiiean Cronbach's alpha reliability coefficient computed
effects. Furthermore, despite virtual consensus that pro- across studies (U>ehlin 1987, p. 105).
pensity to leave exerts a strong effect on salesperson
turnover (e.g., Futrell and Parasuraman 1984; Johnston ., . RESULTS .
et al. 1990), multiple study effects relating actual turn-
over to other constructs in the model were not available. Meta-Analysis ,'
Therefore, propensity to leave was used as a proxy for Table 1 lists the number of study effects, cumulative
actual turnover. satnple size, weighted mean observed correlation and
weighted mean correlation corrected for attenuation from
Antecedent Effects measurement error, and 95% confidence intervals for each
The model posits negative effects of role ambiguity pairwise relationship represented by multiple study ef-
and role conflict on both sales performance and job sat- fects. Table 1 indicates the frequency with which various
isfaction. Empirical effects of these role constructs on constructs have been studied in relation to salesperson
job satisfaction have been primarily negative in valence, job satisfaction. Means for the summary categories were
but not consistently significant. They have had generally computed as simple means of the average effect sizes
weaker and more inconsistent effects on sales perfor- within categories. This approach minimized the potential
mance. biasing of results by weighting multiple effects from the
Sales performance is modeled as an antecedent of job same study by the sample size and thus overrepresenting
satisfaction, consistent with the best available evidence large-sample studies and underrepresenting smaller-sam-
on the causal priority of these constructs (Bagozzi 1980a; ple studies that reported a number of relationships (Ban-
Sheridan and Slocum 1975; Wanous 1974). As noted gen-Downs 1986).
68 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Table 1
ANALYSIS OF SUMAAARY CATEGORIES AND SPECIFIC PAIRWISE RELATIONSHIPS INVOLVING JOB SATISFACTION

95% confidence 95% confidence


Number of inter\'al interx'al
Relationship study effects Cumulative N Observed f Corrected f (observed^ t corrected)'
Wari oulfomrs 66 I4.I0K ..W ,37
Pc cfomiaiiL'e-&al li fai:! ion 2» 7.621 ,13 ,15 07 < p< . 19 .08 < p < .22
Pnipensiiy to leave- saiisfaciHin 19 3,992 -36 -.46 -.54 <p< -.18 -.66 < p < - . »
Sail s fac Hun • I urmiver 7 908 -.22 -.24 -.32 <p<; -.12 -.39 < p < -.13
Organ iuiional commitment-^aii.sfaction 11 1,387 50 .60 .24 < p< .76 .30 < p < .90
Prrional. haraaerisiift 38 11,575 ,14 .19
Orgiuii/atUinul IcfiurC'SaiisfocIJcm 13 3.762 -02 -.03 - . 0 9 < p < ,05 -.11 < p < .OS
Work tiiotivuion-salisfaclion 6 1.106 ,30 .41 .10 < p < .30 .14 t p< .68
EducMiim - sat isfiaKMi 6 2.143 - 09 -.10 --24 < p < ,06 - . 2 7 < p < .07
Age -satisr«.i ion 6 2.250 - 04 -.03 -.14 < p < .06 -.17 < p < .07
Specific self-esiccm-5»iisf»ction 5 1.836 22 .28 .02 < p < .42 .03 < p < .53
General i in] sclf-c<^Ki;m--uili^ljK:lion 2 458 .17 .23 .08 < p < .26 .07 < p < .43
Role percepiioni 38 6.225 36 .43
Roir amhigiiiiy'SaIi<ir»::H>r 15 2.431 -.36 -.45 -,62< p< - 10 -.75 < P-: -.13
Role ci)nt1ic|.\3ti.il>L'tion t7 2.641 - 33 -.42 - . 5 2 < p < -.04 -,80< p < -.04
Role clariiy.sjtisfKtJon 6 1.153 ,40 .49 .M < (>< .60 ,24 < p < .74
Organhaiional variables 30 9.633 30 .41
Supervisory behaviors 19 3.544 ,30 NA
Clmcnos of iiipcrvisJon-satisFaclKm 5 928 37 .43 ,25 < p < .49 .31 < p < .59
Leader t:on%idcralion-«Btisfaction 4 552 .37 .47 .17 < p < .57 ,27 < p < .67
Job feedback -lialisfoElion 4 581 .23 .38 .03 <; p < .43 03 < p < .53
Comingnl rewa^d^-^aIi.^faction 2 311 ,43 .49 .42 < p < .44 .46 < p < .52
Arbitrary punuhment-ialiiifaction 2 311 - 30 -.37 - . 5 2 < p < -.08 -.63 < p < -.09
Amount of commiSimn-i-alisfiti'lion 2 461 ,08 NA' - , l 4 < p < .30 NA
Job/las k variables 31 6.089 26 NA
Job invotvemcnt'saliiifacliiin 6 2.021 .27 .33 . 2 l < p < .33 .25 <: p <; .41
Influence over KlandardS'SiUisfactHHi 4 722 .40 .44 .23 < p < .33 .27 < p < .61
P»tiic i pal ion- saii sfa-tion 3 361 .33 .43 .06 < p < .64 .11 < p < .73
tnniivaiiveimt n:quiiEil-.!ia[isfac[itiii 3 688 .07 NA -.26 < p < .40 NA
Task vantly-saliafaclion 3 463 .24 .30 .11 < p < .37 15 < p < .43
Task sitEnificance-talisfacEiun 3 465 .24 .30 .18 < p < .30 .25 < p < .33
Task autonomy-salisfaclion 3 463 .21 .27 .15 < p < .27 1B< p < .36
Paj-sails faction 3 433 18 .26 -.11 < p < .47 - I2< p < .64
Value congTucnce-saiisfactiun 3 40 .57 .35 < p < ,45 32 < p < .82
'NA itidicates insufficient ttliability information available to correct study effects for measurement error.

Of the three categories of antecedent variables, role to achieve homogeneity and for which at least 10 study
perceptions had the highest average correlation with job effects were available. Table 2 presents the results of
satisfaction (r = .36), followed by the two types of or- moderator analyses comparing weighted subgroup means.
ganizational variables, supervisory behaviors (r = .30) Two of the moderator variables (type of .salesforce and
and job/task variables (r = .26), and individual differ- job satisfaction measure) had significant effects on two
ences (r ~ .14). Job satisfaction had an average corre- pairwise relationships.
lation of .30 with other work outcomes. The effects of role ambiguity and role conflict on job
satisfaction were significantly stronger for Industrial
Homogeneity Tests salespeople than for nonindustrial salespeople. Thus, role
Homogeneity tests revealed overall consistency of re- perception variables appear to have a greater impact on
sults for most of the relationships considered. Only in job satisfaction for industrial salespeople than they do
the case of 11 pairwise relationships was deletion of more for other types of salespeople.
than a single study effect necessary to achieve overall Use of a global measure of job satisfaction versus
homogeneity, In only a single case (role conflict-satis- measurement by facets had a significant moderating ef-
faction) was deletion of more than two study effects nec- fect on relationships between role atnbiguity and role
essary. This finding indicates considerable robustness and conflict and job satisfaction. The effects of these role
generalizability of job satisfaction effects across rela- constructs on job satisfaction were greater for studies that
tionships and study contexts. measured satisfaction by facet than for those that mea-
sured it globally. Though we can only speculate about
Moderator Analyses the reason for this finding, it is possible that the much
Moderator analyses were conducted on relationships greater number of items in "by facet" measures (such as
that required deletion of more than a single study effect JDI and INDSALES) tap the domain of the satisfaction
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION 69

Table 2
WEIGHTED MEAN OBSERVED CORRELATIONS BY LEVELS OF MODERATOR VARIABLES

Relationship
Organizational
Role ambiguity- Role conflict- commitment-
Moderator satisfaction satisfaction satisfaction
Industrial vs. nonindustrial -.39 vs. -.32' - . 3 6 vs. -.25" .50 vs. .48
salesforcc (8 vs. 6) (7 vs. 8) (1 vs. 10)
Institutional vs. -.38 vs. -.30 - . 3 1 vs. -.29 .49 vs. .49
individual customer (11 vs. 3) (II vs. 4) (6 vs. 5)
Global vs, "by facet" -.27 vs. -.41" - . 2 5 vs. -.33^ .45 vs. .50
satisfaction mea.sure (5 vs. 9) (5 vs. 9) (4 vs. 7)
Single vs. multiple -.33 vs. -.41 - . 2 8 vs. -.39 .49 vs. .50
company salesforce (10 vs. 4) (12 vs. 4) (10 vs. 1)
'Sigtiitlcant at p < .05.
'Significant at p < .01.

construct more completely with greater resultant con- regression analysis revealed that significant variation re-
struct (including nomological) validity than global mea- mained unexplained by the moderator variables for the
sures. relationship between role conflict and satisfaction, sug-
The ability of moderator variables to account jointly gesting that other, unidentified moderators of this rela-
for the variance in study effects was assessed by re- tionship may be present.
gressing Fischer's 2-transforms of individual study cor-
relations on the coded study characteristics (Hedges and Causal Model Estimation
Olkin 1985, p. 240).*" Analysis of residuals (reported in Prior to estimation of the causal model, a test was con-
Table 3) indicated that the moderator variables, consid- ducted to assess the impact of using all available study
ered jointly, adequately accounted for the variance in all effects versus using only the homogeneous sets of study
of the relationships considered except the role conflict- effects. (The rationale and procedure for this test are de-
satisfaction relationship. The residual variance was mar- scribed in the Appendix.) The test indicated that the
ginally significant for the role ambiguity-satisfaction and complete and homogeneous sets of average correlations
satisfaction-organizational commitment relationships, but were equivalent overall. On the basis of this test, the
in both cases elimination of a single outlier resulted in complete set of study effects was used in the causal anal-
nonsignificance. Inspection of the residuals from the ysis so as to utilize all the available data. Thus, the anal-
ysis involved assessment of effects across the entire re-
search stream. Both matrices of average intercorreiations
'"The sample-size-weighted sum of squared residuals is distributed are given in Table 4 for comparison.
approximately as chi square, permitting a significance test of the ad- Fitting the mtxlel represented in Figure I to the ag-
equacy of model specification. Significance of the sum of squared gregated study effects resulted in a significant chi square
residuals suggests that the model is not adequately specified to ac-
count for all of the variance in effect sizes and that other, unidentified fit statistic of 123.00 with 8 degrees of freedom {p <
sources of variation in the study effects are present. Nonsignitlcance .(X)l), with goodness-of-fit index of .968 and root mean
of sum of squared residuals suggests that no residual variance remains square residual of .055.' These indices indicate a poor
unexplained. fit of the model to the aggregated study effects.
Path coefficients for the basic model are presented in
Table 5. Two of the hypothesized paths (role conflict-
perfonnance, performance-satisfaction) were not statis-
Table 3
tically significant. To gain a more accurate understanding
HOMOGENEITY TESTS OF RESIDUALS FROM MODERATOR of nomological effects, the basic model was modified to
VARIABLE REGRESSION MODELS better fit the aggregated data.
Ri-littioiiship Q d.f.
Role ambiguity-satisfaction 22.79' 13 'In the causal modei analysi.s. the irttidian sample size from the meta-
Role conflict-satisfaction 80.01" 13 analysis of relationships among constructs in the model {N = 1251)
Satisfaction-organizational commitment 23.57- 10 was used. This sample size was larger than that of any primary em-
'Significant at p < .05, but elimination of a single outlier results pirical study (median N = 175) and thus provided a very sensitive
in homogeneity. test of the goodness of model fit and the significance of structural
"Significant at p < .001. model paths.
7D JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Table 4
AVERAGE INTERCORRELATION A M O N G CONSTRUCTS IN THE MODEL"

Construct
Construct RA RC PERF SATIS OC PTL
Role ambiguity (A4) [79] .20 -.31 -.33 -.35 .36
Standard deviation .08 .10 .08 .13 .12
Number of studies 5 6 13 5 4
Cumulative sample size 842 826 2166 534 414
Role conflict [RC) .28 [.77] -.07 -.26 -.40 .28
Standard deviation .18 .10 .09 .12 .10
Number of studies 9 1 1 8 12 6 3
Cumulative sample size 1245 12S1 2042 712 337
Performance [PERF) -.24 -.07 [.88] .13 15 -.12
Standard deviation .15 .10 .07 .10 .09
Number of studies 7 8 29 7 9
Cumulative sample size 1204 1251 7021 863 1571
Satisfaction {SATIS) -.36 -.33 .13 1-851 .54 -.35
Standard deviation .13 .16 .07 .10 .07
Number of studies 15 17 29 9 18
Cumulative sample size 2431 2641 7621 1355 3890
Organizational commitment (OC) -.28 -.34 .IS .50 L-85] -.70
Standard deviation .1g .16 .10 .13 .06
Number of studies 6 7 7 11 4
Cumulative sample size 654 915 863 1587 423
Propensity to leave (PTL) .36 .28 -.12 -.36 -.70 [-78)
Standard deviation .12 .10 .09 .09 06
Number of studies 4 3 9 19 4
Cumulative sample size 414 357 1571 3992 423
'Entries below ihe diagonal represent the complete set of study effects. Those above the diagonal represent the reduced set of homogeneous
effects. Entries in the diagonal [ J are weighted mean Cronbach alpha coefficients.

Results of each step of the model modification pro- The revised model is presented in Figure 2. Estimation
cedure are reported in Table 6. First, the two nonsig- of this model resulted in a much improved fit to the ag-
nificant paths were removed from the model. Next, paths gregated study effects (x^ ^ 35.69 with 8 d.f., p < .01,
were added sequentially, on the basis of the criteria of goodness of fit index = .991. RMSR = .021). These
substantive interpretability and expected gain in model indices suggest a very good fit of the model to the ag-
fit (e.g.. Kaplan 1990; McCallum 1986). The first pa- gregated data. The significant chi square value for over-
rameter freed was the path linking role conflict to or- all model fit is largely attributable to the large sample
ganizational commitment. Several empirical studies found size used in the analysis (A' = 1,251).'^ and the small
a significant negative pairwise correlation between these
constructs {e.g.. Darden, Hampton, and Howell 1989;
Dubinsky and Hartley 1986; Dubinsky and Skinner 1984; "Marsh. Balla. and McDonald (1988. p. 391) warn that "the prob-
Johnston et al. 1990). and LISREL modification indices ability of detecting a fal.sc model increases wiih N even when Ihe
suggested that job satisfaction did not cotnpletely me- model is trivially false." Our analy.sis. using a sample size much grcaler
than thai typical of studies in the research stream, fumi.shes a case in
diate tbe relationship as posited in the basic model. point.
The modification indices suggested a direct effect of The modification indices suggested that a still better fit could t>e
role ambiguity on propensity to leave in addition to the obtained by adding a direct job satisfaction-propensity to leave path.
indirect effect mediated by job satisfaction. This path at Some previous empirical studies (e.g.. Johnston et al. 1988) had found
a significant direct relationship, whereas others (e.g., Johnston ct al
first appeared nonintuitive, but reflection suggested suf- 1990) had not. Addition of the direct job sat is fact ion-propensity to
ficient rationale to add it. Some salespeople may expe- leave path resulted in an improved fit of x" O d.f.) = 8.07, p = ,327.
rience role ambiguity less as a result of tack of guidance Though this improvement in Tit was significant and resulted in an
or clarification from managers than as a result of a lack extremely well-fitting model, an anomalous positively valenced coef-
ficient for the direct satisfaction-propensity to leave path (.19) and
of confidence in tbeir ability to make the many routine inflation of the standard enors of the other paths antecedent to pro-
decisions required of them as organizational boundary pensity to leave resulted. This result was contrary to theory, common
spanners. Thus, they may feel misplaced in the role of sense, and the cumulative findings of empirical research pertaining to
salesperson and begin the process of thinking about leav- this relationship. Thus, though the aggregated data suggested a sig-
nificant direct satisfaction-pmpensity to leave relationship, adding the
ing the sales job for one that suits them better. This ra- path to the model adversely affected the precision and interpretability
tionale would account for a direct relationship between of the path estimates Hence, the estimates presented in Figure 2 and
role ambiguity and propensity to leave that is not me- Table 5 are for the mixlel that does not include the direct satisfaction-
diated by organizational commitment. propensity to leave path.
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION 71

Table 5 Figure 2
STANDARDIZED PATH COEFFICIENTS AND REVISED MODEL
f-VALUES FOR HYPOTHESIZED A N D REVISED MODELS

Standardized
Relanonship coefficient (- vatue
Hypothesized model
RA -» RC .36 1L!9
RA -» PERF -.29 -8.12
RC — PKRF .02 .52
RA — SATIS -.33 -9.37
RC -» SATIS -.31 -8.99
PE-RF -» SATIS .03 .86
PKRF — OC .09 3.12
SATIS -* OC .58 20.06
O C ^ PTL -.85 -33.52
X' = 123.00
d.f. = 8
p < .001
GFI = .968
RMSR = .055
Revised model
RA -> R C .36 11.22
RA - PERF -.28 -8.77
RA -* SATIS -.33 -9.77 ing reciprocal causation between job satisfaction and or-
RC -* SATIS -.29 -8.28 ganizational commitment and examining the valences and
RC ^ OC -.22 -6.53
RA — PTL .18 6.75 statistical significance of the resulting estimates. In this
PERF -» OC .08 2.78 case, the model estimated was identical to the revised
SATIS - • OC .47 14.58 model depicted in Figure 2, but with the addition of a
O C - * PTL -.78 29.42 reciprocal causal path fnini organizational commitment
X' = 35.69
d.f. = 8 to job satisfaction. The standardized path ct^fficients.
/) < .01 standard errors, and /-values for the reciprocal paths be-
GFI = .991 tween job satisfaction and organizational commitment
RMSR = .021 follow.
Path
Relationship coefficient Sh t-value
RX)t mean square residual indicates the model's ability Satisfaction-organ izalional
to accurately reproduce the correlations among the con- commitment .59 .097 6.06
Organizational commitment-
structs. satisfaction -.17 .140 -1.21
Antecedence of Job Satisfaction-Organizational The Standardized coefficient of the satisfaction-or-
Commitment ganizational commitment path had the predicted positive
A test used by Bagozzi (1980a) to evaluate the causa! valence, whereas that of the organizational commit-
priority of sales performance with respect to job satis- ment-satisfaction path was negative and counter to the
faction was used to assess whether job satisfaction is pri- prediction. Moreover, the satisfaction-organizational
marily antecedent to organizational commitment or vice commitment path was statistically significant, but the or-
versa. The test involved estimating a model incorporat- ganizational commitment-satis fact ion path was not.

Table 6
MODEL FIT INDICES

McDonatd' s
Gain d.f. absolute
Model d.f. (los.s) gain/loss ftt index
Hypothesized mtxiel 123.00 8 .955
Delete RC -» PERF 123.96 10 (.96) 2 .955
and PERF — SATIS
Add RC -» OC 79.22 9 44.74 I .972
Add RA -» PTL 35.69 8 43.53 I .989
Add SATIS - • PTL 8.32 7 27.37 I .9995
72 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Though the aggregated data were correlational rather ihan single outlier. Only one of the 28 relationships had sig-
experimental and primarily cross-sectional rather than nificant variance remaining after deletion of two outly-
longitudinal, this analysis suggests that the primary di- ing effects. These results, based on data collected from
rection of causation is from job satisfaction to organi- several different types of salespeople in widely varying
zational commitment rather than the reverse. It provides study contexts, suggest that salesperson job satisfaction
additional support for previous theoretical arguments and is consistently related to most of the constructs investi-
empirical evidence favoring the causal precedence of job gated (though important exceptions, such as role con-
satisfaction (Bartol 1979; Reichers 1985). flict, are noted).
A similar test of the causal ordering of sales perfor- A second insight relates to the three general categories
mance and job satisfaction was planned, but estimation of antecedent variables that have been related to sales-
of the basic model indicated that the path coefficient person job satisfaction. Of these categories, role percep-
linking the two constructs was not significantly different tions have the highest average correlation with satisfac-
from zero, suggesting the absence of any causal effect. tion, followed by organizational variables and individual
This finding appears to support the contention of Behr- differences. These findings suggest that when salesper-
man and Perreault (1984) that the often-observed small son job satisfaction is an important organizational goal,
correlation between these two constructs may be spu- steps to reduce role ambiguity and conflict may have the
rious rather than causal. greatest positive impact.
The two types of organizational variables, supervisory
Summary of Eindings behaviors and job/task variables, had important effects
The modifications resulting in the improved fit of the on job satisfaction. The fmdings suggest that managerial
revised model depicted in Figure 2 suggest several sum- focus on supervisory behaviors (e.g., encouraging sales
mary points about causal effects. For example, role con- managers to show greater consideration, initiate greater
flict does not have the predicted significant negative ef- structure, provide greater recognition of achievements,
fect on sales performance. Also, when effects of common and communicate more frequently) may also be effective
antecedents are controlled, performance and job satis- in improving salesperson satisfaction. They also suggest
faction are not directly causally related, and perfonnance that though job/task design characterislies are generally
does not have significant direct or indirect causal effects relatively less effective than role perceptions and super-
on propensity to leave. The effect of performance on or- visory behaviors in influencing salesperson job satisfac-
ganizational commitment, though statistically significant tion, they too can have important effects.
given the large sample size used in the analysis, is weak Analysis of causal effects involving job satisfaction
and lacking in substantive significance. The revised model across the available studies also prtxluced several in-
also indicates that job satisfaction only partially mediates sights. Both role ambiguity and role conflict were found
the effect of role conflict on organizational commitment to be consistently and negatively related to job satisfac-
and that role conflict exerts a significant negative direct tion. Role ambiguity and conflict, however, had differ-
effect on organizational commitment. Role ambiguity is ential effects on other work outcomes. Role ambiguity
not significantly related to organizational commitment, had a significant negative impact on sales performance
but it does have a significant direct negative effect on and propensity to leave, whereas role conflict did not.
propensity to leave. The model also indicates that, as Role conflict had a significant direct negative effect on
expected, both role ambiguity and role conflict have sig- organizational commitment, whereas role ambiguity did
nificant direct negative effects on job satisfaction and not. The effects of the role perception constructs on per-
that organizational commitment is primarily a conse- formance suggest that uncertainty about what the job en-
quence, rather than an antecedent, of job satisfaction. tails inhibits salespeople's ability to perform adequately,
but that conflict over incompatible demands of role part-
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS ners does not. The direct effect of role ambiguity on pro-
A meta-analysis can provide quantitative evidence of pensity to leave suggests that uncertainty over role re-
the generalizability of effects across studies and research quirements can be an important contributing factor to
contexts—or it can disconfirm the generality of phenom- intentions to leave the organization. This finding may
ena thought to be consensual. Moreover, a meta-analysis reflect some salespeople's inability to cope with the un-
can provide fresh insights into a research area by some- certainties inherent in the their boundary-spanning roles.
times pnxlucing surprising and unanticipated results. Our Though role conflict did not have a significant direct
meta-analysis and assessment of nomological effects ac- effect on performance or propensity to leave, it did have
complished all three. a significant negative direct effect on organizational
Our quantitative summary of research on the anteced- commitment. This finding suggests that incompatible de-
ents and consequences of salesperson job satisfaction mands of customers and clients can reduce the salesper-
provides several insights. One is that relationships in- son's attachment to the company he or she represents.
volving salesperson job satisfaction have been relatively Uncertainty about role requirements (ambiguity) did not
consistent acn>ss studies and research contexts. Only three have a similar effect on organizational commitment. Thus,
of the 28 relationships investigated had significant amounts sales management strategies oriented toward reducing role
of variance in study effects after the elimination of a ambiguity and role conflict appear likely to have differ-
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION 73

entiul ct'iects on work outcomes, depending on whether is. sales performance is. at best, very weakly related to
they primarily inlluence ambiguity or conflict. Strategies other important work outcomes such as job satisfaction,
that ptnmarily reduce role ambiguity are likely to have organizational commitment, and propensity to leave. The
positive effects on sales performance and reducing iin- implication is that managerial actions intended to in-
teniion to leave the organization, as well as on job sat- crease sales performance are not likely to affect these
isfaction, Strategies that primarily reduce role conflict other work outcomes indirectly through the mediation of
are likely to affect salespeople'.s organizational commit- their effects on performance. Some antecedent variables,
ment and job satisfaction, but not performance or inten- such as role ambiguity, however, may have simulta-
tion to leave the organization. neous direct effects on sales performance and other work
Another important finding of the nomological analysis outcomes.
is that, though a small positive correlation between sales Unlike sales performance, job satisfaction appears to
performance and job satisfaction is highly consistent across mediate the effects of role perception constructs on other
a large number of empirical studies, the variables do not work outcomes. Our study provides evidence useful in
appear to be causally related. This finding corroborates resolving inconsistencies present in prior research. For
previous empirical findings (e.g.. Behrman and Per- example, previous research has been divided on whether
reault 19K4; Dubinsky and Hartley 1986; Hampton, Du- job satisfaction directly affects prop>ensity to leave or
binsky, and Skinner 1986) and suggests that the corre- whether this effect is indirect and mediated by organi-
lation between performance and job satisfaction may be zational commitment. Our analysis, using the aggregated
spurious anil attributable to relationships with C{>mmon study effects, shows that the predominant effect is in-
antecedent variables. Hence, it appears that the large direct and mediated by organizational commitment, though
volume of research devoted to the question of whether the possible presence of a direct effect is suggested. This
perfonnance causally precedes satisfaction or vice versa finding suggests that the effect of job satisfaction on re-
may have been misdirected and thai greater attention ducing turnover occurs primarily through a pnx:ess of
should be paid to identifying common antecedent vari- increasing Investment in and attachment to the organi-
ables and more accurately specifying the true causal ef- zation.
fects. Another important finding of the causal analysis is
A similar pattem of performance-satis fact ion research evidence that job satisfaction primarily exerts a direct
results has been reported in nonsaies work contexts. For causal effect on organizational commilment rather than
example, laffaldano and Muchinsky (1985) reviewed 217 vice versa. This issue has long been debated, though the
studies of the performance-satisfaction relationship and preponderance of conceptual and empirical evidence has
found a weighted mean correlation of . 15 with only eight generally favored the interpretation of job satisfaction as
correlations exceeding .44. (Only one study included in antecedent to commitment (Johnston et al. 1990). Sup-
our analysis found an effect of this magnitude.) Their pod for this conclusion from effects generalized across
conclusion that "few other empirical relations have em- the research stream provides additional evidence of the
braced the nuil hypothesis so often yet continued to fos- precedence of job satisfaction in this relationship.
ter additional research" (p. 269) appears to hold true also The results of the meta-analysis closely parallel results
of salesperson research. laffaldano and Muchinsky spec- of simiJar meta-analyses conducted in primarily nonsaies
ulated that the importance of the two constructs may have contexts. For example. Fisher and Gitelson (1983) and
fostered the large volume of research despite unimpres- Jackson and Schuler (1985) conducted meta-analyses of
sive empirical effects. They also speculated that the vol- the relationships between role confiict, role ambiguity,
ume of null results may reflect dogmatic belief in the and their correlates. A comparison of the results of our
assertion that "a productive worker is a happy worker" study and those of the other meta-analyses is presented
(or vice versa), despite the accumulation of^ results in- in Table 7. The average correlations between the role
dicating a modest positive relationship at best. perception constructs and job satisfaction in those stud-
Previous re.search has been inconsistent with respect ies are all within one standard deviation of the mean cor-
to the effects of sales performance on organizational relation observed in our study. Other common relation-
commitment and propensity to leave. For example, Fu- ships studied in our meta-analysis and others have average
trell and Parasuraman (1984) did not find a significant correlations of similar magnitude.
relationship between performance and propensity to leave, Two coded .study characteristics (type of salesforce and
whereas Johnston et al. (1988) did observe such a re- job satisfaction measure) appeared to have systematic
lationship. Our analysis supports the conclusion of no moderating effects on observed effect sizes. The effects
direct effect of performance on propensity to leave. of role perception constructs (particularly role conflict
Though the effect of pcribmiance on organizational and role clarity) on job satisfaction were greater in stud-
commitment in the nomological analysis was statistically ies that used industrial salespeople than in studies that
significant, it accounted for less than \% of the variance used other types of salespeople. Though we can only
in organizational commitment and hence was lacking in speculate as to the reason for this finding, industrial mar-
substantive significance. ket environments may present greater boundary-span-
Our analysis suggests that sales performance is a "ter- ning challenges to salespeople than retail or trade-ser-
minal value" (Cherrington 1980), or an end in itself. That vicing environments and this in tum may strengthen the
74 JOURNAL OF AAARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Table 7
COMPARISON OF RESULTS WITH THOSE OF PREVIOUS META-ANALYSES

Present Fisher and Jackson and tqffatdano and Loher et Churchill


Relationship study Gilelson (1983) Schuler (1985) Muchinsky (1985) al. (1985) et ul. (1985)
Role ambiguity-satisfaction -.36 -.25 -.30
Role ambiguity-perfonnaiKe -.24 -.14 -.12
Role ambiguity-organizational commitment -.28 -.34 -.27
Role ambiguity-PTL .36 .32 .18
Role ambiguity-role conflict .28 .37 .27
Role conflict-satisfaction -.33 -.35 -.31
Role conflict-perfonnance -.07 -.10 -.05
Role connict-organizational commitment -.34 -.25 -.24
Role conflict-PTL .28 .29 .21
Performance-satisfaction .13 .15
Task significance-satisfaction .24 .25
Autonomy-satisfaction .21 .34
Feedback-satisfaction .23 .29
Role perceptions-performance .14 .29

relationship between role perceptions and job satisfac- tions of sales performance. The nomological analysis.
tion. however, indicates that sales performance is a "terminal
Measurement of job satisfaction by facets produced value" (i.e., it is an important end in itself, but has little
larger effects of role ambiguity and role conflict on job or no effect on other work outcomes). This finding sug-
satisfaction than did global measurement of satisfaction. gests that a reevaluation of the impact of sales perfor-
Though again we can only speculate as to the reason for mance on other work outcomes should be undertaken.
this fmding, instruments that provide for the exhaustive From these results, sales performance appears to be much
measurement of job satisfaction by facet may tap the do- less important in determining work attitudes than the re-
main of the satisfaction construct more comprehensively search literature has suggested. Though previous studies
and hence be less subject to attenuation of effects than have found thai sales performance has no causal effect
the shorter global measurement instruments. on job satisfaction, researchers have heen slow to dis-
count the significance of the relationship. Iaffaldano and
Potential Limitations Muchinsky (1985) noted a similar tendency in the or-
Several limitations of our research should be noted. ganizational behavior literature. Hence, mtxlels that note
Though the literature on salesperson job satisfaction was and explain the apparent nonrelationship of sales per-
surveyed comprehensively, we could noi include all formance and work attitudes should be conceptualized
studies in the analysis because in some the effects were and empirically tested. Theory and empirical research in
not reported in usable form and could not be converted intrinsic motivation (e.g.. Czikszentmihalyi 1990; Deci
lo usable form. However, there is no basis for positing and Ryan 1985; Sujan 1986) may provide a foundation
any systematic differences between the studies included for such models.
and those not included. The essentially null relationships between sales per-
Furthermore., small cell sizes in analyses of moderator formance and other work outcomes raise interesting and
variables limited power to reject the null hypothesis. important questions about turnover functionality, that is,
Hence, caution in interpreting these results is warranted. retaining high performers as low performers leave the
As in any meta-analysis, the results are constrained by organization (e.g., Hollenbeck and Williams 1986). If
the methcxls used in the primary empirical studies. The performance is not related directly or indirectly to sat-
statistical tests used in ihe meta-anaiyses. however, in- isfaction or propensity to leave and related only very
volve no assumptions that are not likely to hold with weakly to organizational commitment, what guidance can
small sample sizes (Rosenthal 1984. p. 118), so that lim- be given to sales managers for maximizing turnover
ited ptiwer to detect significant moderating effects is the functionality? Performance itself does not appear to con-
only potential problem. Notwithstanding this potential tribute significantly to salespeople's intentions to leave
problem, several statistically significant moderating ef- the organization. Hence, future research suggesting the
fects were observed. most effective ways for managers to improve turnover
functionality could make a useful contribution.
Directions for Future Research Our study indicates thai role ambiguity and role con-
Prevailing models (e.g., expectancy theory models) flict have differential effects on sales work outcomes.
represent outcomes of sales work (e.g., satisfaction, or- Research has provided little insight into the reasons un-
ganizational commitment, propensity to leave) as func- derlying these differential effects. Hence, research prob-
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION 75

ing more deeply into the differences between these role ing the correlations computed on all studies of each relationship
perception constructs and into the processes that cause and one including the correlaiions computed only on the re-
them to affect work outcomes differently would make a duced sets of homogeneous study effects.
worthwhile contribution. Such research could have a The equivalence of these two matrices was assessed by means
substantial practical impact in suggesting improved of a test of overall equality of correlation matrices described
methods of training and feedback/control. by Werts et al. (1976). This test was conducted to assess the
The meta-analysis indicates several consistent rela- impact of including study effects from studies that reported
tionships involving correlates of job satisfaction and points some but not all of the relationships that we analyzed in the
out several important inconsistent relationships. For the basic model. The homogeneous sets of effects essentially rep-
role conflict-satisfaction relationship, coded study char- resent studies that replicate one another and hence "agree" on
acteristics did not adequately account for observed vari- the value of r for a particular relationship. We wanted to de-
ability in the study effects. Further study of this rela- termine whether using the more complete sets of effects in-
tionship is necessary to identify additional sources of volved significant departures from these "agreed upon" values
variation. of the correlations.
In brief, our research addresses several issues and in- The test consisted of specifying a two-group conftrmatory
consistencies in the literature on salesperson job satis- factor analysis model such that parameter estimates were in-
faction and related constructs, resolving sotne and noting variant across the two groups. The modei was specified so that
areas where further work is needed. Though many cor- correlations among the latent factors (i.e.. phi paths) were equal
relates of job satisfaction have been studied and many to the correlations among the observed variables (i.e., lambda
insights provided, improved conceptual tnodels appear X paths were all fixed to unity). The sample sizes used in ihe
necessary to explain the causal processes linking sales- analysis were the median .sample sizes from the iwo correlation
person job satisfaction with its antecedents and conse- matrices [N = 1251 and N = 842, respectively). The nonsig-
quences. nificant chi square statistic resulting trom this analysis (x' =
APPENDIX 28.83 with 21 d.f., p > .10) indicated that one set of param-
eter estimates fit the data for both groups, showing the overall
This appendix provides details of the methods used in the equality of the two correlation matrices. On the basis ol" this
meia-analysis and causal tnodcl estimation and modification. result, we used the correlations based on all studies in sub-
Homogeneity Tests sequent causal model analyses. Thus, these analyses involved
assessment of effects across the entire research stream.
After mean correlations and variances were computed for
each pairwise relationship, homogeneity tests were conducted REFERENCES
to assess ihe significance of the variances (Hedges and Ofkin Bagozzi. Richard P. (1978). "Salesforce Perfonnance and Sat-
1985). The tests were conducted for each pairwise relationship isfaction as a Function of Individual Difference, Interper-
according to the formula sonal, and Situational Factors," Journal of Marketing Re-
search, 15 (November), 517-31.
(1980a), "Performance and Satisfaction in an Indus-
trial Sales Force: An Examination of Their Antecedents and
Simultaneity," Journal of Marketing, 44 (Spring), 65-7.
The lest statistic Q is distributed approximately as chi square, (1980b), "The Nature and Causes of Self-Esteem. Per-
and ihc hypothe.sis of effect homogeneity is rejected when Q formance, and Satisfaction in the Sales Force: A Structural
exceeds the critical value of chi square for A: — 1 degrees of Equation Approach," Journal of Business, 53 (July). 315-
freedom, where k is the number of studies of the relationship. 31.
Nonhotiiogeneity of study effects suggests the potential pres- Bangert-Downs. Robert L. (1986), "Review of Developments
ence of mixlerator variables. in Meta-Analyiic Method." Psychological Bulletin, 99 (May),
388-99.
To arrive at a reduced set of homogeneous studies of a re-
Bartol, Kathryn M. (1979), "Professionalism as a Predictor of
lationship, we eliminated the study with the largest weighted Organizational Commitment, Role Stress and Turnover: A
deviation from the mean effect size (i.e., the largest chi square Multidimensional Approach," Academy of Management
deviate), recalculated the mean effect size, and repeated the Journal. 22 (December). 815-21.
homogeneity test. This procedure was repealed until the lest Bateman. Thomas S. and Stephen Strasser (1984), "A Lon-
statistic was nonsignificant, indicating a homogeneous set of gitudinal Analysis of the Antecedents of Organizational
study effects. Moderator variable analyses were conducted for Commitnienl." Academy of Management Journal, 27
relationships that required deletion of more than a single out- (March). 95-112.
lier to achieve homogeneity and for which at least 10 study Becherer, Richard C . Fred W. Morgan, and Lawrence M.
effects were available. Richard (1982). "The Job Characteristics of Industrial
Salespersons: Relationship to Motivation and Satisfaction."
Causal Model Analysis Journal of Marketing, 46 (Fall), 125-35.
Behrman. Douglas N. and William D. Perreault (1984), "A
The first step in testing Ihe basic model of nomotogieal ef- Role Stress Model of the Pertbrmancc and Satisfaction of
fects was to develop two matrices of weighted mean intercor- Industrial Salespersons," Journal of Marketing, 48 (Fall),
relalions among constructs included in the model, one includ- 9-21.
76 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 1993

Bluedom. Allen C. (1982). "A Unified Model of Turnover Hampton, Ron, Alan J. Dubinsky. and Steven J. Skinner (1986),
From Organizations," Human Relations, 35 (February), 135- "A Model of Sales Supervisor Leadership Behavior and Re-
53. tail Salespeople's Job-Related Outcotnes," Journal of the
Cherrington, David J. (1980). The Work Ethic: Working Val- Academy of Marketing Science. 14 (Summer), 33-43.
ues and Values That Work. New York: Amacom. Hart, Sandra Hile, William C. Moncrief. and A. Parasuraman
Churchill, Gilbert A.. Jr.. Neil M. Ford. Steven W. Hartley, (1989). "An Empirical Investigation of Salespeople's Per-
and Orville C. Walker. Jr. (1985), "The Determinants of formance. Effort, and Selling Methixl During a Sales Con-
Salesperson Performance: A Meta-Analysis," Journal of test," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 17
Marketing Research, 22 (May). 103-18. (Winter), 29-39.
, . and Orville C. Walker. Jr. (1974). "Mea- Hedges. Lawrence V. and Ingram Olkin (1985). Statistical
suring the Job Satisfaction of Industrial Salesmen," Journal Methods for Meta-Analysis. Orlando. FL: Academic Press,
of Marketing Research. 11 (August), 254-60. Inc.
-, and (1976). "Organizational Climate Hollenbeck, John R. and Charles R. Williams (1986). "Turn-
and Job Satisfaction in the Salesforce." Journal of Market- over Functionality Versus Turnover Frequency: A Note on
ing Research. 13 (November). 323-32. Work Attitudes and Organizational Effectiveness." Journal
Comer. James M.. Karen A. Machleit, and Rosemary R. La- of Applied Psychology, 71 (August), 606-11.
gace (1989), "Psychometric Assessment of a Reduced Ver- Hunter. John E. and Frank L. Schmidt (1990). Methods of
sion of INDSALES." Journal of Business Research, 18 Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Re.search
(June). 291-332. Findings. Newbury Park. CA: Sage Publications. Inc.
Cron, William L. (1984), "Industrial Salesperson Develop- Iaffaldano, Michelle T. and Paul M. Muchinsky (1985), "Job
ment: A Career Stages Perspective," Journal of Marketing, Satisfaction and Performance: A Meta-Analysis," Psycho-
48 (Fall), 41-52. logical Bulletin, 97 (March). 251-73.
and John W. Slocum. Jr. (1986), "The Influence of Ivancevich, John M. and James H. Donnelly. Jr. (1974), "A
Career Stages on Salespeople's Job Attitudes. Work Per- Study of Role Clarity and Need for Role Clarity for Three
ceptions, and Performance," Journal of Marketing Re- Occupational Groups," Academy of Management Journal,
search, 23 (May), 119-29. 17 (March). 28-36.
Czikszentmihaiyi, Mihaly (1990), Flow: The Psychology of Jackson. Susan E. and Randall S. Schuler (1985), "A Meta-
Optimal Experience. New York: Haiper & Row Publishers, Analysis and Conceptual Critique of Research on Role Am-
Inc. biguity and Role Conflict in Work Settings," Organizational
Darden. William R., Ronald Hampton, and Roy D. Howell Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 36(August), 16-
(1989), "Career Versus Organizational Commitment: An- 78.
tecedents and Consequences of Retail Salespeoples' Com- Jaworski. Bernard J. and Ajay K. Kohli (1991). "Supervisory
mitment," Journal of Retailing, 65 (Spring). 80-106. Feedback: Alternative Types and Their Impact on Salespjeo-
Deci, Edward L. and Richard M. Ryan (1985), Intrinsic Mo- ple's Performance and Satisfaction." Jottrnal of Marketing
tivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New Research, 28 (May), 190-201.
York: Plenum Press. Johnston. Mark W., Charles M. Futrell. A. Parasuraman, and
Dubinsky, Alan J. and Steven W. Hartley (1986). "A Path- Jeffrey Sager (1988), "Performance and Job Satisfaction Ef-
Analytic Study of a Model of Salesperson Performance." fects on Salesperson Turnover: A Replication and Exten-
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 14 (Spring). sion." Journal of Business Research. 16 (March). 67-83.
36-46. , A. Parasuraman. Charles M. Futreil. and William C.
and Steven J. Skinner (1984), "Impact of Job Char- Black (1990). "A Longitudinal Assessment of the Impact of
acteristics on Retail Salespeople's Reactions to Their Jobs," Selected Organizational Influences on Salespeople's Orga-
Jottrnal of Retaiiitig, 60 (Summer), 35-62. nizational Commitment During Early Employment." Jour-
Fem, Edward F,, Ramon A. Avila, and Dhruv Grewal (1989), nal of Marketing Research, 27 (August), 333—44.
"Salesforce Turnover: Those Who Left and Those Who , P. Rajan Varadarajan, Charles M. Futrell, and Jeffrey
Stayed," Industrial Marketing Management, 18 (February), Sager (1987). "The Relationship Between Organizational
1-9. Commitment, Job Satisfaction, and Turnover Among New
Fisher, Cynthia and Richard Gitel.son (1983). "A Meta-Anal- Salespeople." Jourtial of Personal Selling and Sales Man-
ysis of the Correlates of Role Confiict and Ambiguity," agement. 7 (November). 29-38.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 68 (May). 320-33. Kaplan. David (1990). "Evaluating and Mtxiifying Covariance
Ford. Neil M., Orville C. Walker. Jr.. and Gilbert A. Chur- Structure Models: A Review and Recommendation." Mtd-
chill, Jr. (1979). "Organizational Climate and Salesforce tivariate Behavioral Research, 25 (April). L37-55.
Satisfaction: A Replication." '\n Educators' Conference Pro- Kohli. Ajay K. (1985). "Some Unexplored Supervisory Be-
ceedings, Neil Beckwith et al., eds. Chicago; American haviors and Their Influence on Salespeople's Role Clarity,
Markeling Association, 411-6. Specific Self-Esteem, Job Satisfaction, and Motivation,"
Futrell. Charles M. and A. Parasuraman (1984), "The Rela- Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (November), 424-33.
tionship of Satisfaction and Performance to Salesforce Turn- (1989). "Effects of Supervisory Behavior: The Role
over," Journal of Marketing, 48 (Fall), 33-40. of Individual Differences Among Salespeople," Journal of
Hackman. J. Richard and G. R. Oldham (1975), "Develop- Marketing, 53 (October). 63-75.
ment of the Job Diagnostic Survey." Journal of Applied Locke, Edwin A. (1976). "The Nature and Causes of Job Sat-
Psychology, 60 (April). 159-70. isfaction," in Handbook of Industrial and Organizational
Hafer. John and Barbara A. McCuen (1985), "Antecedents of Psychology, Marvin Dunnette, ed. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Performance and Satisfaction in a Service Sales Force as 1297-1350.
Compared to an Industrial Sales Force." Journal of Per- Loehlin. John C. (1987). Latent Variable Models. Hillsdale,
sonal Selling and Sales Management, 5 (November), 7-17, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates.
SALESPERSON JOB SATISFACTION n
Loher. Brian T.. Raymond A. Noe. Nancy L. Moeller, and of Mobley's Turnover Process Model," Journal of Personal
Michael P. Fitzgerald (1985). "A Meta-Analysis of the Re- Selling and Sales Management. 8 (May). 20-35.
lation of Job Characteristics to Job Satisfaction." Journal of Schul, Patrick L., Steven Remington, and Robert L. Berl (1990),
Applied Psychology. 70 (April), 280-9. "Assessing Gender Differences in Relationships Between
Marsh. Herbert W., John R. Balla. and Roderick P. Mc- Supervisory Behaviors and Job-Related Outcomes in the In-
Donald (1988). "Goodness-of-Fit Indexes in Confirmatory dustrial Sales Force," Journal of Personal Selling and Sales
Factory Analysis: The Effect of Sample Size," P.s\cholog- Management. 10 (Summer), 1-16.
ical Bulletin. 103 (May), 391-410. Schwab. Donald P. and Larry L. Cummings (1970), "Theories
McCallum. Robert C. (1986), "Specification Searches in Co- of Perfonnance and Satisfaction: A Review," Industrial Re-
variance Structure Modeling," Psychological Bulletin, 100 lations, 6 (October), 408-30.
(January). 107-20. Sheridan, John E. and John W. Slocum. Jr. (1975), "The Di-
Moncrief, William C . Ill (1986). "Selling Activity and Sales rection of the Causal Relationship Between Job Satisfaction
Position Taxonomies for Industrial Salesforces," Journal of and Work Performance," Organizational Behavior and Hu-
Marketing Research. 23 (August). 261-70. man Performance, 14 (April). 159-72.
Motowidlo. Stephan J. (1982), "Relationship Between Self- Singh, Jagdip and Gary K. Rhoads (1991), "Boundary Role
Rated Performance and Pay Satisfaction Among Sales Rep- Ambiguity in Marketing-Oriented Positions: A Multidimen-
resentatives," Journal of Applied Psychology, 67 (April). .sional, Multifaceted Opertionalization." Journal of Market-
209-13. ing Research, 29 (August), 328-38.
Pearson, Judson B.. Gordon H. Barker, and Rodney D. Elliott Smith. P. C , L. M. Kendall, and C. L. Huiin (1969). The
(1957), "Sales Success and Job Satisfaction." American So- Mea.surement of Satisfaction in Work and Retirement. Chi-
ciological Review. 22 (August). 424-7. cago: Rand McNally, Inc.
Porter. Lyman W. and Edward E. Lawler, III (1968), "Man- Sujan, Harish (1986). "Smarter Versus Harder: An Explora-
agerial Attitudes and Performance. Homewood, IL: Richard tory Attributional Analysis of Salespeople's Motivation,"
D. Irwin. Inc. Journal of Marketing Research. 23 (February). 41-9.
Pruden, Henry O. (1969). "Interorganizational Conflict. Link- Teas, R. Kenneth (1980). "An Empirical Test of Linkages
age, and Exchange: A Study of Industrial Salesmen." Acad- Proposed in the Walker. Churchill, and Ford Mode! of
emy of Management Jounwl, 12 (September). 339-50. Salesforcc Motivation and Performance," Journal of the
and R. M. Reese (1972). "Interorganizational Role- Academy of Marketing Science, 8 (Winter), 58-72.
Set Relations and the Performance and Satisfaction of In- (1981). "A Test of a Model of Department Store
dustrial Salesmen," Administrative Science Quarterly, 17 Salespeople's Job Satisfaction," Journal of Retailing, 57
(December). 601-9. (Spring), 3-25.
Puffer, Sheila M. (1987). "Prosocial Behavior. Noncompliant , John G. Wacker. and R. Eugene Hughes (1979), "A
Behavior, and Work Performance Among Commis.sion Path Analysis of Causes and Consequences of Salespeople's
Salespeople," Journal of Applied Psychology, 72 (August). Perceptions of Role Clarity," Jotimal of Marketing Re-
615-21. search, 16 (August), 355-69.
Rao, Akshay R. and Kent B. Monroe (1989). "The Effect of Walker, Ot^ille C , Jr.. Gilbert A. Churchill, Jr.. and Neil M.
Price. Brand Name, and Store Name on Buyers' Perceptions Ford (1977), "Motivation and Performance in Industrial
of Product Quality: An Integrative Review," Journal of Selling: Present Knowledge and Needed Research," Jottrnal
Marketing Research, 26 (August), 351-7. of Marketing Research. 14 (May). 156-68.
Reichers. Amon B. (1985). "A Review and Reconceptuali- Wanous, John P. (1974), "A Causal-Correlational Analysis of
zation of Organizational Commitment," Academy of Man- the Job Satisfaction and Performance Relationship." Journal
agement Review. 10 (July), 465-76. of Applied Psychology, 59 (February), 139-44.
Rosenthal. Robert (1984). Meta-Anatytic Procedures for So- Weitz. Barton A. (1981), "Effectiveness in Sales Interactions:
cial Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Inc. A Contingency Framework," Journal of Marketing. 45
Sager, Jeffrey K. and Mark W. Johnston (1989). "Antecedents (Winter). 85-103.
and Outcomes of Organizational Commitment: A Study of Werts, C. B.. D. A. Rock, R. L. Linn, and K. G. Joreskog
Salespeople," Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Man- (1976). "Comparison of Correlations, Variances, Covari-
agement. 9 (Spring). 30-41. ances. and Regression Weights With or Without Measure-
, P. Rajan Varadarajan, and Charles M. Futrell (1988), ment Error," Psychological Bulletin, 83 (November). 1007-
"Understanding Salesperson Turnover: A Partial Evaluation 13.

Rcprini No JMR301 lOS


View publication stats

S-ar putea să vă placă și