Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2015 American Psychological Association

2015, Vol. 20, No. 3, 289 –300 1076-8998/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038684

Stressful Work Environment and Wellbeing: What Comes First?

Marko Elovainio Tarja Heponiemi


National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland and National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
University of Helsinki

Markus Jokela and Christian Hakulinen Justin Presseau


University of Helsinki Newcastle University

Anna-Mari Aalto Mika Kivimäki


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland University College London
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The association between the psychosocial work environment, including job demands, job control, and
organizational justice, and employee wellbeing has been well established. However, the exposure to
adverse work environments is typically measured only using self-reported measures that are vulnerable
to reporting bias, and thus any associations found may be explained by reverse causality. Using linear
regression models and cross-lagged structural equation modeling (SEM), we tested the direction of the
association between established job stress models (job demand control and organizational justice models)
and 3 wellbeing indicators (psychological distress, sleeping problems, and job satisfaction) among 1524
physicians in a 4-year follow-up. Results from the longitudinal cross-lagged analyses showed that the
direction of the association was from low justice to decreasing wellbeing rather than the reverse.
Although the pattern was similar in job demands and job control, a reciprocal association was found
between job control and psychological distress.

Keywords: psychosocial factors, risk factors, sleeping problems, stress, working population

A large number of published studies support the risk status of reversed causality. The direction of the association is important in
the leading occupational stress models (Elovainio, Kivimaki, & making causal inferences and, therefore, in designing preventive
Vahtera, 2002; Kivimäki, Nyberg, Batty, Fransson, & Heikkilä, et interventions (Hemingway & Marmot, 1999). In this study, we
al., 2012). Recently, the direction of the association between specifically tested the direction of the association between work
work-related psychosocial factors and mental health problems has related psychosocial risks, such as high job demands, low job
been questioned (Ybema & van den Bos, 2010; Lang, Bliese, control, and organizational injustice, and wellbeing problems, in-
Lang, & Adler, 2011). Although the occupational stress models cluding sleeping problems, psychological distress, and job dissat-
suggest that environmental psychosocial factors affect wellbeing isfaction (Karasek & Theorell, 1990; Robbins, Ford & Tetrick,
and health, it is reasonable to assume that also health problems and 2012).
reduced wellbeing may affect the perception of environmental The two established occupational stress models used in epide-
demands and challenges. Using self-reported measures of both miological studies, the job demands– control model (Karasek &
environmental factors and potential outcomes creates a risk of Theorell, 1990) and the organizational justice model (Colquitt,
Greenberg, & Zapata-Phelan, 2005), focus on different environ-
mental factors as wellbeing and health risks. The job demands–
control (job-strain) model introduced by Karasek and Theorell
This article was published Online First February 23, 2015.
Marko Elovainio, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, (1990) includes two central components: high job demands (the
Finland and Department of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki; need to work quickly and hard), and lack of control over skill use,
Tarja Heponiemi, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland; time allocation, and organizational decisions. The theory suggests
Markus Jokela and Christian Hakulinen, Department of Behavioural Sci- that workers who have concurrent high demands and low job
ences, University of Helsinki; Justin Presseau, Institute of Health and control (experience job strain) cannot moderate the stress caused
Society, Newcastle University; Anna-Mari Aalto, National Institute for by the high demands through time management or learning new
Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland; Mika Kivimäki, Department of skills. Thus, they become subject to high stress at work, and if
Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London.
ongoing, are at increased risk of psychological distress and dis-
This study has been supported by the Finnish Work Environment Fund
and the Academy of Finland. ease.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marko A more recent organizational justice model (Colquitt, Green-
Elovainio, National Institute for Health and Welfare, P.O. Box 30, 00270 berg, & Zapata-Phelan, 2005) focuses on perceived unfairness in
Helsinki, Finland. E-mail: marko.elovainio@thl.fi resource distribution, decision-making principles, and treatment

289
290 ELOVAINIO ET AL.

practices within organizations (referred to as distributive, proce- Vermunt & Steensma, 2005; Tepper, 2001; Ybema & van den Bos,
dural and interactional justice; Lind & Tyler, 1988). Research on 2010).
organizational justice as an individual’s perception of fairness in Multiple mechanisms linking psychosocial factors and wellbe-
an organization, has mainly focused on two things: (a) what ing have been proposed and supported. It has been shown that a
individuals perceive as being fair in organizations and (b) what the negative psychosocial work environment provokes negative emo-
consequences of such perceptions might be. It has been shown that tional reactions, low self-esteem, social exclusion, and loss of
the perception of organizational justice is affected by a combina- physical resources, which in turn are closely related to problems in
tion of rules and norms related to decision-making principles psychological wellbeing (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Karasek & Theorell,
(procedural justice component) and treatment practices (interac- 1990; Elovainio et al., 2013). Men who experienced a high level of
tional justice component) that people experience as being fair or justice at work had a lower risk of incident metabolic syndrome
unfair. When people within an organization perceive that they get than employees with a low level of justice (Gimeno et al., 2010),
what they deserve (their input and what they get back from the and low organizational justice was associated with increased cir-
organization are in balance), that the rules treat them fairly (the culating inflammatory marker C-reactive protein during a 10-year
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

decisions made follow fair rules), and that other people, especially follow-up and increased interleukin-6 during a 5-year follow-up
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

their supervisors, treat them fairly (supervisors can be trusted and (Elovainio, Ferrie, et al., 2010).
are respectful), they quite often think that organizational justice is Although evidence supports the idea of psychosocial factors as
high (for a review see Cropanzano et al., 2001). The justice causal factors predicting deteriorating health and wellbeing, alter-
researchers have long been interested in the consequences arising native explanations are also plausible. It may be that health and
from organizational functioning, (Heponiemi et al., 2007; McFar- wellbeing problems contribute to negative perceptions of the psy-
lin & Sweeney, 1992; Moorman, 1991; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997), chosocial environment. This would be especially probable in jus-
but only during the last two decades has the scope of research on tice perceptions that are dependent on interpretations of social and
organizational justice been expanded to employee wellbeing and relational factors, but may also affect perceptions of job demands
health outcomes (Elovainio et al., 2001; Elovainio et al., 2002; and even job control. Ybema and van den Bos (2010) tested the
Hausknecht et al., 2011; Schmitt & Dorfel, 1999; Tepper, 2001). reversed associations between depression, sickness absenteeism,
The imbalance between job demands and control and low per- and organizational justice dimensions in a representative sample of
ceived justice are suggested to undermine wellbeing by generating 1519 employees. According to their results, stronger evidence
psychological stress. The appraisal process is a basic assumption supported the role of justice dimensions as a predictor of depres-
of classic and contemporary stress theories, suggesting that when sive symptoms and absenteeism than the reverse.
people evaluate their capacity to cope with environmental chal- Jessica Lang and her coworkers (2011), however, found that
lenges to be insufficient, they experienced psychological stress among military personnel, depressive symptoms led to subsequent
which leads to reduced wellbeing and health problems (Lazarus & lower organizational justice perceptions. Furthermore, they found
Folkman, 1984). Psychological stress may affect wellbeing and no evidence supporting the hypothesis that organizational justice
health outcomes through direct activation of physiological neu- perceptions would predict depressive symptoms. The authors con-
roendocrine responses to stressors, or more indirectly through cluded, in accordance with the assumption of affective perception,
unhealthy behaviors such as smoking, lack of exercise, or exces- that depressed individuals perceive events more negatively. They
sive alcohol consumption performed to cope with perceived stres- also suggested that depressed individuals are also treated differ-
sors, which increase the risk of deleterious effect on wellbeing and ently in reaction to their depressed state.
health (Hemingway & Marmot, 1999; Hemingway et al., 2003). Severe mental health problems, such as depression, inevitably
One of the main axes of neuroendocrine stress responses is the affect the way an individual perceives and experiences his or her
autonomic nervous system (ANS). Stress may also affect dysregu- environment (Gotlib & Joormann, 2010). However, little is known
lation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, which is asso- about the effects of less severe stress-induced psychological states,
ciated with disturbances in the circadian rhythm of cortisol and the such as psychological distress, sleeping problems, and dissatisfac-
development of the metabolic outcomes. tion. According to the equity theory (Adams, 1963), being stressed
A large body of evidence suggests that the psychosocial work should motivate people to select less stressful tasks and contexts
characteristics defined by the job demand– control are associated and in this way try to make their environment less demanding.
with wellbeing and health indicators, such as low heart rate vari- However, this option is not often possible in real life. It is reason-
ability (Chandola et al., 2008), progression of atherosclerosis able to assume that when feeling stressed, people are less able to
(Rosenström et al., 2011), sickness absences (Zapf et al., 1996), use adaptive coping, such as actively rearranging or prioritizing
poorer cognitive performance (Elovainio et al., 2012), metabolic tasks. Thus, stress may reduce the sense of or actual resources
syndrome (Chandola et al., 2006), and incident obesity (Brunner, people have to cope with the demands at work and they may
Chandola, & Marmot, 2007). Similarly, it has been shown that experience their environment as more strenuous and demanding
there is a link between low organizational justice and experienced than it otherwise would be. This may be attributable to perception
stress, psychological distress, depressive symptoms, sickness ab- or be the result of less capacity to undertake work tasks because of
sence, lower cognitive performance, sleeping problems, and even reduced physical or cognitive capacity. For instance, sleeping
cardiovascular disease and death (Elovainio et al., 2009; Elovainio, problems are often associated with tiredness that in turn may affect
Kivimaki, & Helkama, 2001; Elovainio et al., 2002; Elovainio, actual abilities to cope with the cognitive or physical demands at
Leino-Arjas, Vahtera, & Kivimaki, 2006; Elovainio et al., 2012; work (Kristof-Brown et al., 2005).
Ferrie et al., 2006; Kivimaki, Elovainio, Vahtera, & Ferrie, 2003; Thus, we formed three hypotheses: (a) negative work-related
Kivimaki et al., 2003, 2006; Ndjaboue, Brisson, & Vezina, 2012; psychosocial factors (low organizational justice, high demands and
STRESSFUL WORK ENVIRONMENT 291

low control) predict decreasing wellbeing; (b) problems in well- Measures


being (psychological distress, sleeping problems and job dissatis-
Job demands were measured by 5 items (␣ ⫽ .85 in 2006 and
faction) predict increasingly negative perceptions of work-related
0.90 in 2010) derived from Harris’ (1989) stress index. The items
psychosocial factors; or (c) there are reciprocal associations be-
were I have to work very hard, My job involves an excessive
tween negative work-related factors and wellbeing. The current
amount of work, I don’t have enough time to get my work done
study expands on previous evidence by testing the associations
properly, We do not have enough resources to do our job, and We
with longer follow-up times (latency period) than previous studies.
do not have enough personnel to do our job. All items used a
Previous studies have also tested the reciprocal associations using
5-point Likert-type response format ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
only depression or depressive symptoms as the wellbeing indicator
agree) to 5 (strongly agree). These items were used because they
(Lang et al., 2011; Ybema & van den Bos, 2010). We tested the
were very similar to those in the original Job Contents Question-
direction of the association between less severe wellbeing prob- naire used by Karasek (1979) when the concept was first devel-
lems, such as sleeping problems, psychological distress, and job oped.
dissatisfaction, which have previously been suggested as outcomes Job control was measured by 3 items (␣ ⫽ .77 in 2006 and 0.76
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of a strenuous work environment (Karasek & Theorell, 1990;


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in 2010) derived from Karasek’s Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ)


Robbins, Ford & Tetrick, 2012). (Karasek, 1979; Karasek, Baker, Marxer, Ahlbom, & Theorell,
Both job strain and organizational justice have been associated 1981), which measured the freedom to make independent deci-
with sleeping problems (de Jonge et al., 2000; Elovainio et al., sions and possibilities to choose how to perform work (e.g., I have
2011), psychological distress measured by the GHQ (Stansfeld, a lot of say about what happens in my job). The items were rated
North, White, & Marmot, 1995; Elovainio, Kivimäki, & Vahtera, on a 5-point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5
2002), and job satisfaction (de Jonge et al., 2000; Colquitt, 2001). (totally agree).
As for many other psychosocial factors at work, a plausible mech- Organizational justice was assessed using a short 8-item version
anism through which perceived job strain and organizational in- (Elovainio, Heponiemi, Kuusio, et al., 2010) of Colquitt’s organi-
justice may affect mental health, dissatisfaction, and sleeping zational justice measure (Colquitt, 2001). This shorter version has
problems is prolonged stress. Both job strain and perceived injus- shown satisfactory psychometric properties (internal consistency
tice has been shown to be associated with experienced stress and a good model fit to the data) and criterion validity (Elovainio,
reactions/distress (Elovainio, Kivimäki, & Helkama, 2001; Stans- Heponiemi, Kuusio, et al., 2010). The items were rated on a
feld et al., 1995). Disturbed sleep is another marker of prolonged 5-point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally
stress, which is considered a common indicator of prolonged agree). Examples of the items include: Have you been able to
negative emotional states and related physiological changes (Es- express your views and feelings during procedures used to
pie, 2002). arrive at your outcome?, Has the authority figure who enacted the
procedure treated you with dignity?, and Does your outcome
reflect the effort you have put into your work? With respect to the
Method internal consistency, the alpha coefficient for this sample was 0.84
in 2006 and 0.86 in 2010. The complete justice scale was divided
The present study was part of the Finnish Health Care Pro-
into three dimensions: procedural justice (three items, ␣ ⫽ .70 in
fessionals Study, in which a random sample of 5000 physicians
2006 and 0.74 in 2010), interpersonal justice (three items, ␣ ⫽ .79
in Finland (30% of the whole physician population) was drawn
in 2006 and 0.91 in 2010), and distributive justice (two items, ␣ ⫽
from the 2006 database of physicians maintained by the Finnish
.94 in 2006 and 0.94 in 2010).
Medical Association. The register covers all licensed physicians
Psychological distress was measured with 4 items (␣ ⫽ .84 in
in Finland. Phase 1 data were gathered using postal question-
2006 and 0.79 in 2010) from the GHQ-12 (Goldberg, 1972), which
naires in 2006. Responses were received from 2841 physicians
measured anxiety/depression, as suggested by Graetz (1991).
(response rate 57%). The representativeness of the sample (in
Graetz’s three-factor structure has been suggested as the most
the first data collection phase) compared with the eligible preferable factor model for the GHQ-12 (Penninkilampi-Kerola,
population was tested and reported in a previous study Miettunen, & Ebeling, 2006). The respondents rated how much
(Elovainio et al., 2007), suggesting that the sample was repre- they were affected by each of the 12 listed symptoms of psycho-
sentative in terms of age, gender, and employment sector. There logical distress over the previous few weeks (1 ⫽ not at all, 2 ⫽
were no statistically significant differences between those who the same as usual, 3 ⫽ rather more than usual, or 4 ⫽ much more
participated to the follow-up study and those who participated to than usual). GHQ score (range 1– 4) was the mean of all items. In
the baseline study. In phase 2 (four years later in 2010), the data this study, the scale was used as a continuous variable.
were gathered by using either a web-based or a traditional postal Sleeping problems were measured with 4 items (␣ ⫽ .77 in 2006
survey. In phase 1, the consent to receive follow-up surveys was and 0.77 in 2010) derived from the Jenkins scale (Jenkins, Stanton,
obtained from 2206 participants. Those who had died or who had Niemcryk, & Rose, 1988). Respondents were asked how often
incorrect address information were excluded (n ⫽ 37); thus, at during the last four weeks they had: trouble falling asleep, woken
phase 2 the survey was sent to 2169 physicians. The total number up several times per night, trouble staying asleep including waking
of respondents was 1705 (response rate 79%). Of these, 181 had up too early, and felt tired after the usual amount of sleep. The
incomplete data and were excluded and so the final study sample scale ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (every night).
includes 1524 physicians (61% women) aged 24 – 69 (M ⫽ 49.7) Job satisfaction was assessed with the mean of 3 items (␣ ⫽ .66
years (2010). in 2006 and 0.86 in 2010) derived from Hackman and Oldham’s
292 ELOVAINIO ET AL.

(1975) Job Diagnostic Survey on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 Table 1


(totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree), with items such as I am Characteristics of the Study Sample (n ⫽ 1370 –1524)
generally satisfied with my work. (In 2006 job satisfaction was
measured using the original 7-point scale, and it was rescaled to be Characteristic n %
comparable with the 5-point scale by dividing the items by 7 and Gender
multiplying by 5.) Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for this study was Women 914 60.2
0.66 at phase 1 and 0.88 at phase 2. Men 605 39.8
Covariates included gender, age, and employment sector (hos- Employment sector
Hospital 696 45.7
pital, primary care, private, and the category ‘other’) and were Primary care 327 21.5
measured in 2010. Private 256 16.7
Other 245 16.1
Statistical Analyses
Mean SD
To estimate two-way antecedent– consequence relationships be-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Age 2010 (24–67) 49.7 9.47


tween work-related psychosocial factors and wellbeing simultane-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Procedural justice T1 3.31 0.75


ously, we fitted cross-lagged structural equation models (SEM). Procedural justice T2 3.30 0.76
The goodness-of-fit of the models was assessed by (a) the ␹2 test Interactional justice T1 3.57 0.96
Interactional justice T2 3.51 1.00
where the higher the p value, the better the fit of the data, and (b) Distributive justice T1 2.93 1.02
the standardized root means square residual (SRMR). We also Distributive justice T2 3.06 1.05
used fit indexes less sensitive to number of observations/parame- Job control T1 3.92 0.80
ters, such as CFI and TLI (Byrne, 2011). In comparing alternative Job control T2 3.79 0.83
models, a statistically significant improvement in the ␹2 value Job demands T1 3.58 0.96
Job demands T2 3.01 0.95
indicated a better fit of the model. All models were tested sepa- Distress T1 (1–4) 1.91 0.61
rately for organizational justice, job demands, and job control. The Distress T2 (1–4) 1.83 0.66
contribution of the potential confounding factors at baseline to the Sleeping problems T1 (1–6) 2.30 1.00
relationships between work-related factors and wellbeing indica- Sleeping problems T2 (1–6) 2.35 1.05
Job satisfaction T1 (1–5) 3.49 0.52
tors was taken into account by using adjusted values (residual
Job satisfaction T2 (1–5) 3.04 0.53
values) for the work-related psychosocial factors and wellbeing
outcomes in the SEM models. The contribution of the potential
confounding factors at baseline to the relationships between work-
levels of distress, job demands, and job control slightly decreased
related factors and wellbeing indicators was taken into account by
during the study period, but other average changes were relatively
using adjusted values (residual values) for the work-related psy-
small. Table 2 shows the correlations between the study variables.
chosocial factors and wellbeing outcomes in the SEM models. The
All of the outcomes were at least modestly associated with all of
adjusted values were predicted using linear regression models
the work-related psychosocial factors.
performed separately for each work related factors and wellbeing
The cross-lagged structural equation modeling in organiza-
indicators including all potential confounders (gender, age, and
tional justice and wellbeing outcomes were fitted with justice as
employment sector). We controlled the models for gender and age,
a latent variable (see Figure 1). The model incorporating the
because it has been show that women and younger, less experi-
path from earlier justice to later distress, ␹2(15) ⫽ 100,35,
enced physicians report more problems in wellbeing and also
SRMR ⫽ 0.039, CFI ⫽ .96, TLI ⫽ 0.92, sleeping problems,
experience their work as more strenuous than males and more
␹2(15) ⫽ 53.05, SRMR ⫽ 0.026, CFI ⫽ .98, TLI ⫽ 0.97, and
experienced physicians (Elovainio, Heponiemi, Vänskä, et al.,
job satisfaction, ␹2(15) ⫽ 83.28, SRMR ⫽ 0.039, CFI ⫽ .96,
2007). Furthermore, GPs in Finland have reported more work-
TLI ⫽ 0.93, all showed a reasonably good fit. The association
related problems and wellbeing problems than other physicians,
between justice and distress (coefficients ⫺.11, z ⫽ ⫺3.75, p ⬍
and thus we adjusted models for employment sector (Heponiemi et
.001) and between justice and job satisfaction (coefficients .22,
al., 2013). The measurement invariance (Schmitt & Kuljanin,
z ⫽ 6.31, p ⬍ .001) were significant. None of the models that
2008) for the organizational justice latent variable over the study
tested the paths also from earlier wellbeing indicators produced
phases was tested using strict invariance (loadings, intercepts &
better fits (range of ⌬␹2(1) from 0.22 to 1.2; see Figure 1).
residuals are invariant) and the test showed a good fit for the
None of the associations from earlier wellbeing indicators to
measurement model, ␹2(12) ⫽ 39.04, SRMR ⫽ 0.022, CFI ⫽ .98,
later justice evaluations (coefficients 0.02, z ⫽ 0.71; 0.02, z ⫽
TLI ⫽ 0.98). Composite scores on the three justice measures
0.40; ⫺0.01, z ⫽ ⫺0.46) were significant.
served as indicators of the latent justice constructs whereas indi-
The model fit was reasonable for the model testing the
vidual items served as indicators for job demands and job control.
associations from earlier job demands with later distress,
The wellbeing indicators were added as composite scores in the
␹22(15) ⫽ 157.39, SRMR ⫽ 0.060, CFI ⫽ .97, TLI ⫽ 0.94, and
SEM models. The analyses were performed using STATA/SE
the association between demands and distress was significant
v.12.0® [StataCorp, 2005].
(coefficient 0.12, z ⫽ 4.21, p ⬍ .001). The model testing the
reciprocal effects was better, ␹2(14) ⫽ 152.03, SRMR ⫽ 0.054,
Results
CFI ⫽ .97, TLI ⫽ 0.94, and the effect of earlier distress on later
Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study sample. Most of job demands was significant (coefficient 0.07, z ⫽ 2.53. p ⫽
the participants were women and worked at hospitals. The mean .011). The model incorporating the path from earlier job de-
STRESSFUL WORK ENVIRONMENT 293

Table 2
Correlations (Pearson R) Between Study Variables (n ⫽ 1369 –1524)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

1. Procedural justice T1 1
2. Procedural justice T2 .309 1
3. Interactional justice T1 .493 .257 1
4. Interactional justice T2 .233 .565 .309 1
5. Distributive justice T1 .357 .177 .220 .139 1
6. Distributive justice T2 .177 .299 .140 .321 .358 1
7. Job control T1 .413 .202 .223 .109 .263 .116 1
8. Job control T2 .191 .382 .119 .295 .125 .258 .363 1
9. Job demands T1 ⫺.203 ⫺.053 ⫺.097 ⫺.029 ⫺.209 ⫺.094 ⫺.117 ⫺.055 1
10. Job demands T2 ⫺.124 ⫺.123 ⫺.068 ⫺.135 ⫺.092 ⫺.233 ⫺.136 ⫺.246 .372 1
11. Distress T1 ⫺.211 ⫺.073 ⫺.134 ⫺.085 ⫺.154 ⫺.095 ⫺.190 ⫺.131 .298 .218 1
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

12. Distress T2 ⫺.168 ⫺.194 ⫺.082 ⫺.205 ⫺.073 ⫺.172 ⫺.145 ⫺.196 .205 .37 .42 1
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

13. Sleeping prob. T1 ⫺.146 ⫺.062 ⫺.088 ⫺.051 ⫺.125 ⫺.072 ⫺.166 ⫺.112 .203 .125 .526 .328 1
14. Sleeping prob. T2 ⫺.122 ⫺.127 ⫺.054 ⫺.093 ⫺.069 ⫺.107 ⫺.143 ⫺.156 .146 .191 .352 .544 .59 1
15. Job satisfaction T1 .288 .145 .209 .125 .165 .112 .223 .174 ⫺.235 ⫺.117 ⫺.210 ⫺.126 ⫺.162 ⫺.157 1
16. Job satisfaction T2 .173 .243 .130 .195 .107 .186 .157 .298 ⫺.092 ⫺.212 ⫺.148 ⫺.207 ⫺.143 ⫺.203 .217 1
Note. All correlations above 0.05 statistically significant.

mands to later sleeping problems showed a reasonable fit, Discussion


␹2(15) ⫽ 42.44 SRMR ⫽ 0.028, CFI ⫽ .99, TLI ⫽ 0.98, but the
This study addresses the following hypotheses: (a) negative
association between demands and sleeping problems was not
work-related psychosocial factors defined by the organizational
statistically significant (coefficient 0.025, z ⫽ 1.07, p ⫽ .284).
justice and job strain models (low organizational justice, high
The model testing the reciprocal effects was only slightly
demands and low control) predict decreasing wellbeing and (b)
better, ␹2(14) ⫽ 41.21, SRMR ⫽ 0.025, CFI ⫽ .99, TLI ⫽ 0.99,
problems in wellbeing (psychological distress, sleeping problems
and the effect of earlier sleeping problems on later job demands
and job dissatisfaction) predict increasingly negative perceptions
was not significant (coefficients 0.029, z ⫽ 1.10, p ⫽ .272).
of psychosocial work environment. We found more evidence sup-
Earlier job demands predicted later job satisfaction (coeffi-
porting the former hypothesis than the latter. In organizational
cient ⫺0.13, z ⫽ ⫺4.52. p ⬍ .001) and fit of the model was
justice the direction of the association was consistently from
good, ␹2(15) ⫽ 72.10, SRMR ⫽ 0.039, CFI ⫽ .99, TLI ⫽ 0.97,
justice perceptions to later wellbeing. In job demands the associ-
and slightly worse than the model testing the reciprocal asso-
ations were less clear. Psychological distress seemed to predict
ciations, ⌬␹2(1) 0.74). The association between earlier job
lower job demands in the future, although the association was
satisfaction and later job demands was not significant (p ⫽ reciprocal. This may reflect, of course, real changes in job de-
.494; see Figure 2). mands or rather perceptions. Distress and subsequent reduced
The models including the path from earlier job control to psychological resources may increase the perception of increasing
later psychological distress, ␹2(15) ⫽ 80.69, SRMR ⫽ 0.036, demands or abilities to cope with the existing level of demands.
CFI ⫽ .98, TLI ⫽ 0.96, and sleeping problems, ␹2(15) ⫽ 49.01, Our results contradict those of Lang and colleagues (2011),
SRMR ⫽ 0.027, CFI ⫽ .99, TLI ⫽ 0.98, showed a reasonable which suggested a reversed causation (depressive symptoms pre-
fit and the association between job control and distress (coef- dicted justice evaluations), and are more in line with those of
ficients ⫺0.058, z ⫽ ⫺2.10, p ⫽ .036) was significant. The Ybema and van den Bos (2010), which offered support for the
models testing the reciprocal effects gave only slightly better causal role of justice in predicting later depressive symptoms. The
fits, ␹2(14) ⫽ 77.96, SRMR ⫽ 0.033, CFI ⫽ .98, TLI ⫽ follow-up time in the Lang et al. study (2011) was considerably
0.96/␹2(14) ⫽ 46.78, SRMR ⫽ 0.025, CFI ⫽ .99, TLI ⫽ 0.98. shorter (less than a year) than the one in the current study (four
The association between earlier distress (coefficients ⫺.044, years), and that may explain some of the differences. It is possible
z ⫽ ⫺1.65, p ⫽ .099) and later job control, or between earlier that perceived injustice affects wellbeing only when the exposure
sleeping problems and later job control (coefficients ⫺0.042, is long-lasting and the reversed association masks the causal effect
z ⫽ 1.52, p ⫽ .056) were not significant. The model testing the of the perceived psychosocial environment. This reasoning is
association between job control and later job satisfaction show supported by the fact that the follow-up time of the Ybema and van
a reasonably good fit, ␹2(15) ⫽ 112.20, SRMR ⫽ 0.049, CFI ⫽ den Bos study (2010) was also relatively long. The follow-up time
.97, TLI ⫽ 0.95, and testing the reciprocal associations did not of our study was 4 years, which is relatively long, but there are
much improve the fit, ␹2(14) ⫽ 110.85, SRMR ⫽ 0.048, CFI ⫽ many studies showing effects of, for instance, organizational in-
.97, TLI ⫽ 0.93. The association from earlier job control with justice and sleeping problems (Elovainio et al., 2010) and psycho-
later job satisfaction was significant (coefficient 0.15, z ⫽ 5.10, logical distress (Ferrie et al., 2006) that have used longer follow-up
p ⬍ .001), whereas the association to opposite direction was not times. It is probable, in fact, that relatively long exposure is needed
(coefficient 0.036, z ⫽ 1.20, p ⫽ .230; see Figure 3). for severe forms of distress and sleeping problems to develop.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

294

Figure 1.
ELOVAINIO ET AL.

Cross-lagged models of the associations between organizational justice and wellbeing indicators.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Figure 2.
STRESSFUL WORK ENVIRONMENT

Cross-lagged models of the associations between job demands and wellbeing indicators.
295
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

296

Figure 3.
ELOVAINIO ET AL.

Cross-lagged models of the associations between job control and wellbeing indicators.
STRESSFUL WORK ENVIRONMENT 297

There may be a problem when testing the direction of the associ- studies have demonstrated cross-sectional associations between
ation between two factors if one factor is more stable than the organizational justice and prevalent depressive symptoms
other. However, our cross-lagged models suggested a relatively (Elovainio et al., 2002), and longitudinal associations between low
high test-retest stability for both justice/strain measures and sleep- justice at baseline and psychiatric morbidity two years later (Ki-
ing problems/GHQ. In job satisfaction, this may be a problem vimäki et al., 2003). Previous findings from the Whitehall II study
because of temporal instability. One explanation for the differ- cohort showed that those who reported at baseline that they were
ences could be that different occupational groups were studied. It treated unfairly by their supervisors had a higher risk of incident
may be easier for a physician to change job than for someone psychiatric morbidity three years and six years later (Ferrie et al.,
working in the military. The most obvious reason is, however, the 2006).
wellbeing indicator used. Depression changes the way the envi- Our results suggest that the predictive effects of justice and job
ronment is perceived almost by definition, but the effects of less control on wellbeing are apparent also even when accounting for
severe psychological problems are less evident. The previous age, sex, and working sector. Especially sex but also age has been
studies used solely depressive symptoms or depression as the associated with all the wellbeing outcomes used in this study
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

wellbeing indicator, whereas we measured psychological distress, (Hemingway & Marmot, 1999). These adjustments are not, of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sleeping problems, and job satisfaction. However, the version of course, sufficient to rule out the possibility of residual confound-
the distress measure used was the one that has been shown to ing. Although the GHQ scale used in this study has been shown to
overlap with measures of depressive symptoms quite well (Aalto et have good predictive validity in previous studies (Goldberg, 1972),
al., 2012). Thus, future studies should test the direction of the use of clinical interviews or more established measures of mental
association between psychosocial work environment and wellbe- health problems, including distress, would have been preferable.
ing by using a wide range of wellbeing indicators, so as to offer a In interpreting the present results, it is important to note some
more complete theoretical frame for understanding the dynamic additional limitations. The main limitation of this study is that all
nature of the associations between psychosocial work environment the participants were physicians and our findings may not apply to
and employee wellbeing. wider populations. Thus, the results should be further tested using
Our results can be interpreted as supporting the general stress other occupational groups. Physicians are, however, a good focal
model, suggesting that environmental stimuli that are appraised as occupation for several reasons. It is useful sometimes to test your
a threat to oneself cause stress (Lazarus, 1993; Lazarus & Folk- hypothesis in one relatively homogeneous occupational group to
man, 1984) and that exposure to such threats may lead to health avoid differences in health and wellbeing problems that are related
problems and reduced wellbeing (McEwen, 1998, 2000). Most of to socioeconomic differences (Elovainio et al., 2011). Physicians
the leading occupational stress theories suggest that the fundamen- are considered a high socioeconomic group with a strenuous work
tal stress factor, and in fact the most important effect modifier of environment. Physician work is cognitively and mentally demand-
any other stress factors, is control over the things affecting one’s ing, involving long work hours, on-call work, and high responsi-
work process and environment (Karasek & Theorell, 1990). Sim- bility and thus it is no surprise that physicians have been shown to
ilarly, the uncertainty management model of organizational justice have more wellbeing problems than people working in other high
is based on the idea that a fundamental dilemma is caused by socioeconomic occupations (Virtanen et al., 2007).
giving up autonomy, in other words the ability to fully control the It is also possible that personality traits, such as neuroticism or
environment and decisions concerning factors affecting oneself trait anxiety, may act as predisposing factors for disturbed sleep
(see, e.g., van den Bos, 2001; van den Bos & Lind, 2002) and the (Elovainio, Kivimäki, Vahtera, Virtanen, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen,
self-threatening condition that that lack of control creates (see, 2003), distress, or job satisfaction. Such dispositions also affect the
e.g., Miedema, van den Bos, & Vermunt, 2006). way people experience their (psychosocial work) environment as a
Our findings are also in line with previous studies that have source of negative emotions and the way they respond with mood
suggested that psychological distress, problems with sleep, and changes following stressors in their daily life. However, earlier
low job satisfaction are common after exposure to high job strain research has shown that incident mental disorders can be predicted
or low justice (Elovainio et al., 2001; Elovainio et al., 2002; by organizational justice measured not just with individual self-
Elovainio, Kivimaki, Vahtera, Keltikangas-Jarvinen, & Virtanen, reports but also with work-unit mean scores (Kivimaki et al.,
2003; Elovainio, Leino-Arjas, et al., 2006a; Ferrie et al., 2006; 2003). A further potential limitation is common method variance,
Karasek & Theorell, 1990; Kivimäki et al., 2006; Kivimäki et al., a potential source of inflated associations. Unfortunately, objective
2012). By characterizing exposure to job strain and organizational measures of, for instance, sleep problems, such as actigraphy and
justice and the wellbeing outcomes with measures obtained at two polysomnography, are very difficult to implement in large epide-
time points, we were able to conduct a prospective study of the miological studies such as this.
long-term effects of psychosocial risks. The job strain model is Besides the two job stress models detailed above, there are
probably the most widely tested model of psychosocial risks in the numerous other conceptualizations, including the effort–reward
area of occupational health psychology (for reviews see Kivimäki imbalance (ERI) model, which suggests that people get stressed
et al., 2012), and the evidence supporting the model is strong. All when they are placed in demanding situations without being re-
previous organizational justice studies that have used a single warded adequately. ERI is widely tested and supported psychos-
measure of exposure have shown an association between low ocial health risk (Siegrist, 1996) that has been shown to predict
organizational justice and a greater number of sleeping problems emotional exhaustion (de Jonge et al., 2000) and cardiovascular
in two samples of women (Elovainio, Kivimaki, et al., 2003) and heart disease (Kuper et al., 2002). From the justice model perspec-
between organizational justice and distress (Kivimaki, Elovainio, tive, ERI is basically one of the organizational justice dimensions
Vahtera, & Ferrie, 2003; Ylipaavalniemi et al., 2005). Previous often called distributive justice. Thus, ERI is basically included
298 ELOVAINIO ET AL.

theoretically in the organizational justice model even in the narrow perception may be a strong candidate for being a work-related
definitions (Greenberg & Colquitt, 2005). psychosocial factor that may have causal effects on employee
This study has the benefit of using longitudinal data. All the wellbeing, although there is a subjective component in justice
analyses were conducted using participants with complete data on perception that seems not to account for the real wellbeing effects.
the measured variables. This meant that 30% of the original Our results supported similar conclusions concerning job control.
population was lost to follow-up because of missing data. How- However, perception of job demands may be more vulnerable to
ever, any differences in baseline characteristics between those subjective bias and thus the association between job demands and
included and excluded were small, suggesting that a major bias is wellbeing may be partly explained by reversed causality. It may be
unlikely and thus the results probably were not attributable to that reduced wellbeing (sleeping problems and distress) in fact
selection effect. We do not have information on whether some of means that people do not have cognitive or physical resources
the participants had changed their jobs during the follow up. It may needed to cope with the demands at work and thus they perceive
be that those who changed jobs and still perceived similar levels of their job demands higher. It is less probable that people with
injustice would be more negatively affected compared with those reduced wellbeing would be systematically selected to more de-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

who stayed where they were and experienced similar conditions, manding jobs. These results basically suggest that interventions
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

but it is difficult to evaluate whether this has any effect on the should be targeted to fair managerial and decision making proce-
detected cross-lagged associations. We tested the associations be- dures, increasing job control and reducing job demands. In addi-
tween job characteristics and wellbeing indicators using cross- tion, if the aim is specifically to reduce job demands only then it
lagged structural equation modeling, which is basically the only may be useful to find risk groups already experiencing wellbeing
method that tests both directions of the associations simultane- problems, such as poor sleep of psychological distress.
ously. We were thereby able to model the reciprocal associations
between the job characteristics and wellbeing indicators. We had a References
fixed follow-up time for each participant and thus there was no
need for exact discrete modeling, which is capable of taking into Aalto, A. M., Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., Uutela, A., & Pirkola, S.
(2012). The Beck Depression Inventory and General Health Question-
account the various follow-up times between longitudinal mea-
naire as measures of depression in the general population: A validation
sures. Over repeated follow-ups, autoregressive effects can only study using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview as the gold
decline and the cross-lagged effects would exhibit a peak at a standard. Psychiatry Research, 197, 163–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
certain time window and thus using different time frames in j.psychres.2011.09.008
discrete modeling may show arbitrarily different associations. The Adams, J. S. (1963). Towards an understanding of inequity. The Journal of
availability of long-term repeated data from only two occasions Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 422– 436. http://dx.doi.org/
prevented us from using other, more complicated models, such as 10.1037/h0040968
latent growth curve models. More follow-up measurement points Brunner, E. J., Chandola, T., & Marmot, M. G. (2007). Prospective effect
would have helped to eliminate the possibility that differences in of job strain on general and central obesity in the Whitehall II Study.
temporal stability between job characteristics and wellbeing would American Journal of Epidemiology, 165, 828 – 837. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1093/aje/kwk058
have biased the observed direction of the associations between the
Byrne, B. M. (2011). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic
two (if in the long run job characteristics would be more stable concepts, applications, and programming. London, UK: Routledge.
than wellbeing over time). Chandola, T., Britton, A., Brunner, E., Hemingway, H., Malik, M., Ku-
Although the present study suggests that the direction of the mari, M., . . . Marmot, M. (2008). Work stress and coronary heart
association between adverse psychosocial environments and em- disease: What are the mechanisms? European Heart Journal, 29, 640 –
ployee wellbeing is rather in the direction from adverse psychos- 648. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehm584
ocial environments toward wellbeing outcomes than the reverse, Chandola, T., Brunner, E., & Marmot, M. (2006). Chronic stress at work
some evidence supporting strain-to-stressor effects was also found. and the metabolic syndrome: Prospective study. BMJ: British Medical
Thus, the idea best supported by current evidence is that where a Journal (Clinical Research Ed.), 332, 521–525. http://dx.doi.org/
stressful environment causes reduced wellbeing, these two may 10.1136/bmj.38693.435301.80
Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A
start to reinforce each other in a vicious circle. Our finding
construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,
suggests, however, that the fundamental perceptions of work re- 386 – 400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.386
lated negative factors in our environment may not be understood as Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2005). What is
an outcome of the psychological states of employees. These find- organizational justice? A historical overview. In J. Greenberg & J. A.
ings suggest that the work related psychosocial factors even when Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 3–56). Mah-
measured using self-reported indicators rather precede wellbeing wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
than are outcomes of psychological states. It has been suggested Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D. R. (2001). Moral
that especially organizational justice perception would be affected virtues, fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organi-
by psychological factors, such as depressive symptoms (Lang et zational justice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 164 –209. http://dx
al., 2011). This reversed causality may be attributable to more .doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001.1791
de Jonge, J., Bosma, H., Peter, R., & Siegrist, J. (2000). Job strain,
negative perception of environmental factors, such as organiza-
effort-reward imbalance and employee well-being: A large-scale cross-
tional decision-making procedures among depressed people, or sectional study. Social Science & Medicine, 50, 1317–1327. http://dx
actual differences in treatment of depressed people in organiza- .doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00388-3
tions. Our results do not support the reversed causality of justice Elovainio, M., Ferrie, J. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Gimeno, D., De Vogli, R.,
perception and either of these explanations. Reduced wellbeing Shipley, M. J., . . . Kivimäki, M. (2009). Cumulative exposure to
seems not to affect justice perception, and this means that justice high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function: The
STRESSFUL WORK ENVIRONMENT 299

Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 66, syndrome: The Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environmental
32–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/oem.2008.039305 Medicine, 67, 256 –262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/oem.2009.047324
Elovainio, M., Ferrie, J. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Gimeno, D., De Vogli, R., Goldberg, D. P. (1972). The detection of psychiatric illness by question-
Shipley, M., . . . Kivimäki, M. (2010). Organisational justice and naire: A technique for the identification and assessment of non-psychotic
markers of inflammation: The Whitehall II study. Occupational and psychiatric illness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Environmental Medicine, 67, 78 – 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/oem Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current
.2008.044917 status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6,
Elovainio, M., Ferrie, J. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Shipley, M., Batty, G. D., 285–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305
Head, J., . . . Kivimäki, M. (2011). Socioeconomic differences in Graetz, B. (1991). Multidimensional properties of the General Health
cardiometabolic factors: Social causation or health-related selection? Questionnaire. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 26,
Evidence from the Whitehall II Cohort Study, 1991–2004. American 132–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00782952
Journal of Epidemiology, 174, 779 –789. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/ Greenberg, J., & Colquitt, J. A. (2005). Handbook of organizational
kwr149 justice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Elovainio, M., Heponiemi, T., Kuusio, H., Sinervo, T., Hintsa, T., & Aalto, Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1975). Development of the Job Diag-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

A. M. (2010). Developing a short measure of organizational justice: A nostic Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 159 –170. http://dx
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

multisample health professionals study. Journal of Occupational and .doi.org/10.1037/h0076546


Environmental Medicine, 52, 1068 –1074. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ Harris, P. E. (1989). The nurse stress index. Work & Stress, 3, 335–346.
JOM.0b013e3181f8447c00043764-201011000-00003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02678378908256952
Elovainio, M., Heponiemi, T., Vänskä, J., Sinervo, T., Kujala, S., Laakso, Hausknecht, J. P., Sturman, M. C., & Roberson, Q. M. (2011). Justice as
E., . . . Halila, H. (2007). [How well are Finnish physicians in the 21st a dynamic construct: Effects of individual trajectories on distal work
century?] Kuinka suomalainen lääkäri voi 2000-luvulla? Suomen Laa- outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 872– 880. http://dx.doi
karilehti. Finlands Lakartidning, 20 –21, 2071–2076. .org/10.1037/a0022991
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., & Helkama, K. (2001). Organization justice Hemingway, H., & Marmot, M. (1999). Evidence based cardiology: Psy-
evaluations, job control, and occupational strain. Journal of Applied chosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart
disease. Systematic review of prospective cohort studies. British Medi-
Psychology, 86, 418 – 424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3
cal Journal, 318, 1460 –1467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.318.7196
.418
.1460
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., & Vahtera, J. (2002). Organizational justice:
Hemingway, H., Shipley, M., Mullen, M. J., Kumari, M., Brunner, E.,
Evidence of a new psychosocial predictor of health. American Journal of
Taylor, M., . . . Marmot, M. (2003). Social and psychosocial influences
Public Health, 92, 105–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.92.1.105
on inflammatory markers and vascular function in civil servants (the
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., Vahtera, J., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., &
Whitehall II study). The American Journal of Cardiology, 92, 984 –987.
Virtanen, M. (2003). Sleeping problems and health behaviors as medi-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-9149(03)00985-8
ators between organizational justice and health. Health Psychology, 22,
Heponiemi, T., Elovainio, M., Laine, J., Pekkarinen, L., Eccles, M., Noro,
287–293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.3.287
A., . . . Sinervo, T. (2007). Productivity and employees’ organizational
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., Vahtera, J., Virtanen, M., & Keltikangas-
justice perceptions in long-term care for the elderly. Research in Nursing
Jarvinen, L. (2003). Personality as a moderator in the relations between
& Health, 30, 498 –507. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/nur.20205
perceptions of organizational justice and sickness absence. Journal of
Heponiemi, T., Kouvonen, A., Aalto, A.-M., & Elovainio, M. (2013).
Vocational Behavior, 63, 379 –395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-
Psychosocial factors in GP work: The effects of taking a GP position or
8791(02)00058-1
leaving GP work. European Journal of Public Health, 23, 361–366.
Elovainio, M., Leino-Arjas, P., Vahtera, J., & Kivimäki, M. (2006). Justice http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/cks112
at work and cardiovascular mortality: A prospective cohort study. Jour- Jenkins, C. D., Stanton, B. A., Niemcryk, S. J., & Rose, R. M. (1988). A
nal of Psychosomatic Research, 61, 271–274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. Journal of
j.jpsychores.2006.02.018 Clinical Epidemiology, 41, 313–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0895-
Elovainio, M., Linna, A., Virtanen, M., Oksanen, T., Kivimäki, M., Pentti, 4356(88)90138-2
J., & Vahtera, J. (2013). Perceived organizational justice as a predictor Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental
of long-term sickness absence due to diagnosed mental disorders: Re- strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly,
sults from the prospective longitudinal Finnish Public Sector Study. 24, 285–295. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392498
Social Science & Medicine, 91, 39 – 47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j Karasek, R., Baker, D., Marxer, F., Ahlbom, A., & Theorell, T. (1981). Job
.socscimed.2013.05.008 decision latitude, job demands, and cardiovascular disease: A prospec-
Elovainio, M., Singh-Manoux, A., Ferrie, J. E., Shipley, M., Gimeno, D., tive study of Swedish men. American Journal of Public Health, 71,
De Vogli, R., . . . Kivimäki, M. (2012). Organisational justice and 694 –705. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.71.7.694
cognitive function in middle-aged employees: The Whitehall II study. Karasek, R. A., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work: Stress, productivity
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 66, 552–556. http:// and the reconstruction of working life. New York, NY: Basic Books.
dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2010.113407 Kivimaki, M., Elovainio, M., Vahtera, J., & Ferrie, J. E. (2003). Organi-
Espie, C. A. (2002). Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, sational justice and health of employees: Prospective cohort study.
persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annual Review of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60, 27–33; discussion 33–
Psychology, 53, 215–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53 34.
.100901.135243 Kivimäki, M., Elovainio, M., Vahtera, J., Virtanen, M., & Stansfeld, S. A.
Ferrie, J. E., Head, J., Shipley, M. J., Vahtera, J., Marmot, M. G., & (2003). Association between organizational inequity and incidence of
Kivimaki, M. (2006). Injustice at work and incidence of psychiatric psychiatric disorders in female employees. Psychological Medicine, 33,
morbidity: The Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environmental 319 –326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702006591
Medicine, 63, 443– 450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/oem.2005.022269 Kivimäki, M., Nyberg, S. T., Batty, G. D., Fransson, E. I., Heikkilä, K.,
Gimeno, D., Tabák, A. G., Ferrie, J. E., Shipley, M. J., De Vogli, R., Alfredsson, L., . . . Theorell, T., & the IPD-Work Consortium. (2012).
Elovainio, M., . . . Kivimäki, M. (2010). Justice at work and metabolic Job strain as a risk factor for coronary heart disease: A collaborative
300 ELOVAINIO ET AL.

meta-analysis of individual participant data. The Lancet, 380, 1491– and progression of atherosclerosis: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young
1497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60994-5 Finns study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16, 139 –150.
Kivimäki, M., Virtanen, M., Elovainio, M., Kouvonen, A., Väänänen, A., http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021752
& Vahtera, J. (2006). Work stress in the etiology of coronary heart Schmitt, M., & Dörfel, M. (1999). Procedural injustice at work, justice
disease—A meta-analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment sensitivity, job satisfaction and psychosomatic well-being. European
& Health, 32, 431– 442. http://dx.doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.1049 Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 443– 453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). (SICI)1099-0992(199906)29:4⬍443::AID-EJSP935⬎3.0.CO;2-C
Consequences of individuals’ fit at work: Meta-analysis of person-job, Schmitt, N., & Kuljanin, G. (2008). Measurement invariance: Review of
person-organization, person-group, and person-supervisor fit. Personnel practice and implications. Human Resource Management Review, 18,
Psychology, 58, 281–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2005 210 –222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2008.03.003
.00672.x Siegrist, J. (1996). Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward con-
Kuper, H., Marmot, M., & Hemingway, H. (2002). Systematic review of ditions. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 27– 41. http://
prospective cohort studies of psychosocial factors in the etiology and dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.27
prognosis of coronary heart disease. Seminars in Vascular Medicine, 2, Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

267–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2002-35401 roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of


Lang, J., Bliese, P. D., Lang, J. W. B., & Adler, A. B. (2011). Work gets Applied Psychology, 82, 434 – 443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

unfair for the depressed: Cross-lagged relations between organizational .82.3.434


justice perceptions and depressive symptoms. Journal of Applied Psy- Stansfeld, S. A., North, F. M., White, I., & Marmot, M. G. (1995). Work
chology, 96, 602– 618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022463 characteristics and psychiatric disorder in civil servants in London.
Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: A history Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 49, 48 –53. http://dx
of changing outlooks. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 1–22. http://dx .doi.org/10.1136/jech.49.1.48
.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245 Tepper, B. J. (2001). Health consequences of organizational justice: Tests
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New of main and interactive effects. Organizational Behavior and Human
York, NY: Springer. Decision Processes, 86, 197–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2001
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural .2951
justice. New York, NY: Plenum Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1- van den Bos, K. (2001). Uncertainty management: The influence of un-
4899-2115-4 certainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress media- of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 931–941. http://dx.doi.org/
tors. The New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 171–179. http://dx.doi 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.931
.org/10.1056/NEJM199801153380307 van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis, allostatic load, and the aging nervous of fairness judgements. In Z. M. P. In (Ed.), Advances in experimental
system: Role of excitatory amino acids and excitotoxicity. Neurochem- social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 1– 60). Boston, MA: Elsevier. http://dx
ical Research, 25, 1219 –1231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: .doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80003-X
1007687911139 Vermunt, R., & Steensma, H. (2005). How can justice be used to manage
McFarlin, D., & Sweeney, P. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as stress in organizations? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Hand-
predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. book of organizational justice (pp. 383– 410). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Academy of Management Journal, 35, 626 – 637. http://dx.doi.org/ Virtanen, M., Vahtera, J., Pentti, J., Honkonen, T., Elovainio, M., &
10.2307/256489 Kivimäki, M. (2007). Job strain and psychologic distress influence on
Miedema, J., van den Bos, K., & Vermunt, R. (2006). The influence of sickness absence among Finnish employees. American Journal of Pre-
self-threats on fairness judgments and affective measures. Social Justice ventive Medicine, 33, 182–187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007
Research, 19, 228 –253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11211-006-0007-7 .05.003
Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and Ybema, J. F., & van den Bos, K. (2010). Effects of organizational justice
organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence on depressive symptoms and sickness absence: A longitudinal perspec-
employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845– 855. tive. Social Science & Medicine, 70, 1609 –1617. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.76.6.845 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.027
Ndjaboué, R., Brisson, C., & Vézina, M. (2012). Organisational justice and Ylipaavalniemi, J., Kivimäki, M., Elovainio, M., Virtanen, M.,
mental health: A systematic review of prospective studies. Occupational Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., & Vahtera, J. (2005). Psychosocial work char-
and Environmental Medicine, 69, 694 –700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/ acteristics and incidence of newly diagnosed depression: A prospective
oemed-2011-100595 cohort study of three different models. Social Science & Medicine, 61,
Penninkilampi-Kerola, V., Miettunen, J., & Ebeling, H. (2006). A com- 111–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.038
parative assessment of the factor structures and psychometric properties Zapf, D., Dormann, C., & Frese, M. (1996). Longitudinal studies in
of the GHQ-12 and the GHQ-20 based on data from a Finnish organizational stress research: A review of the literature with reference
population-based sample. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 431– to methodological issues. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
440. 1, 145–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.2.145
Robbins, J. M., Ford, M. T., & Tetrick, L. E. (2012). Perceived unfairness
and employee health: A meta-analytic integration. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 97, 235–272. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025408 Received October 16, 2013
Rosenström, T., Hintsanen, M., Kivimäki, M., Jokela, M., Juonala, M., Revision received October 10, 2014
Viikari, J. S., . . . Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2011). Change in job strain Accepted November 3, 2014 䡲

S-ar putea să vă placă și