Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy and Behavior


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / ye b e h

Review

The contribution of neuropsychology to diagnostic assessment in epilepsy


Marilyn Jones-Gotman a,⁎, Mary Lou Smith b, Gail L. Risse c, Michael Westerveld d, Sara J. Swanson e,
Anna Rita Giovagnoli f, Tatia Lee g, Maria Joana Mader-Joaquim h, Ada Piazzini i
a
Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
b
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
c
Minnesota Epilepsy Group, St. Paul, MN and Dept. of Neurology, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA
d
Medical Psychology Associates, Orlando, FL, USA; Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, USA; Neurosurgery, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven CT, USA
e
Department of Neurology, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
f
Laboratory of Neuropsychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Carlo Besta Neurological Institute, Milan, Italy
g
Laboratory of Neuropsychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
h
Epilepsy Surgery Program and Psychology Services, Clinical Hospital, Federal University of Paraná, Curitiba, Brazil
i
Regional Epilepsy Center, S. Paolo Hospital, University of Milan, Milan, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Neuropsychology plays a vital role in the treatment of epilepsy, providing information on the effects of
Received 21 December 2009 seizures on higher cortical functions through the measurement of behavioral abilities and disabilities. This is
Received in revised form 18 February 2010 accomplished through the design, administration and interpretation of neuropsychological tests, including
Accepted 20 February 2010
those used in functional neuroimaging or cortical mapping and in intracarotid anesthetic procedures. The
Available online 14 May 2010
objective of this paper is to define and summarize in some detail the role and methods of neuropsychologists
in specialized epilepsy centers. Included are information and recommendations regarding basic ingredients
Keywords:
Neuropsychology
of a thorough neuropsychological assessment in the epilepsy setting, as well as suggestions for an
Cognition abbreviated alternative exam when needed, with emphasis on functions associated with specific brain
Language regions. The paper is intended for novice and experienced neuropsychologists to enable them to develop or
Memory evaluate their current practices, and also for other clinicians, who seek a better understanding of the
Tests methodology underlying the neuropsychological input to their work.
Assessment © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Functional neuroimaging
Intracarotid anaesthetic procedure

1. Introduction and overview population of patients. The objective of this paper is to define and
summarize in some detail the role and methods of the neuropsychol-
Over the past half century, neuropsychology has played an ogist in a specialized epilepsy center. It is intended for novice and
increasingly vital role in the diagnostic assessment of epilepsy. experienced neuropsychologists to enable them to develop or
Advances in our understanding of brain-behavior relationships have evaluate their current practices; it is also intended for other clinicians,
mirrored the rapidly expanding technologies of diagnostic neuro- who seek a better understanding of the methodology underlying the
physiology, and structural and functional neuroimaging, resulting in a neuropsychological input to their work. The authors, an international
more accurate understanding of the effects of seizures on higher group of individuals working in the neuropsychology of epilepsy,
cortical functions. In addition to the design, administration and presented several workshops together on this field while members of
interpretation of a neuropsychological test battery, neuropsycholo- an International League Against Epilepsy subcommission. That earlier
gists have a primary role in the design and interpretation of collaboration led to the current production of this paper aimed at
intracarotid anesthetic procedures and functional neuroimaging or providing basic information that can be applied in various settings and
cortical mapping paradigms as alternative methods of assessing the geographical locations.
integrity of brain structures and networks. The integration of Neuropsychological data have long been shown to correlate with
neuropsychologists in comprehensive epilepsy programs has also focal areas of brain dysfunction in patients with epilepsy [1–12] and to
led to their involvement in assessing the cognitive side effects of predict cognitive outcome following epilepsy surgery, particularly when
antiepileptic drugs and in the psychosocial aspects unique to this combined with other variables such as structural brain pathology, age
of seizure onset, language lateralization or unilateral memory perfor-
⁎ Corresponding author. Montreal Neurological Institute, 3801 University Street,
mance during intracarotid anesthetic procedures [13–21]. The specific
Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2B4. Tel.: +1 514 398 8907; fax: +1 514 398 8540. contribution of neuropsychology is unique in that cognitive assessment
E-mail address: marilyn.jonesgotman@mcgill.ca (M. Jones-Gotman). describes and quantifies behavioral abilities and disabilities, whereas

1525-5050/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.02.019
4 M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

other diagnostic methods, such as EEG or brain imaging, assess ana- 2.2.1. IQ testing
tomical or neurophysiological abnormality. By localizing behavioral Neuropsychological examinations almost invariably include basic
dysfunction, neuropsychological findings can reinforce or question data intelligence quotient (IQ) testing. There is considerable evidence
from other sources about the site of seizure focus. Disagreement indicating that seizure disorders are associated with an increased
with results from other diagnostics leads to further investigation; for cognitive risk [e.g., 24]. Although superior IQs have been reported in
example, an unsuspected atypical representation of language can be individuals with epilepsy, the distribution of IQ scores tends to be
discovered owing to discrepant behavioral and anatomical findings. skewed toward lower values [25]. We measure intelligence in
Thus, neuropsychological assessment makes its own valuable contri- individuals with epilepsy because IQ scores are good predictors of
bution to diagnosis in epilepsy, and the integration of those findings educational, vocational and economic outcome, and because –owing
with the data from multiple sources provides the most comprehensive to standardization on large samples– they are highly reliable. In
picture for a given patient. addition, the high correlation between verbal IQ and school
In the paragraphs that follow, it is our intention to provide a performance allows us to use educational achievement to estimate
framework rather than a recipe for maximizing the contribution of premorbid IQ and to detect cognitive decline in patients. In contrast,
neuropsychology. We present information and recommendations on nonverbal intelligence is largely independent of education and thus
what we believe to be basic ingredients for a thorough neuropsycho- may provide additional balance in interpreting the neuropsycholog-
logical assessment in the epilepsy setting. Our emphasis will be on ical profile. As IQ scores are moderately correlated with scores on
functions associated with particular brain regions, with reference to other cognitive tasks, comparison of general level of ability as
some specific tests. Excellent descriptions and norms of tests that are reflected in IQ to performance on specialized tasks contributes to
commonly used in the field can be found in the books by Lezak, detection of specific deficits.
Howieson and Loring [22] and Strauss, Sherman and Spreen [23]. Although a variety of techniques are used to assess intelligence, the
most common measures used in epilepsy centers are the Wechsler
2. The neuropsychological assessment Scales. These tests provide only a basic overall measure of cognitive
level; they do not provide localizing or lateralizing information. There-
2.1. Indications fore, as administration of IQ tests is very time-consuming (2 hours, on
average, in patients with epilepsy), we recommend administering a
Neuropsychological assessment is indicated in all patients being short form. A number of short forms are available, and factors such as
considered for epilepsy surgery, to assist in localizing areas of dys- language considerations and the particular information to be derived
function and in counselling patients and their families regarding other than an IQ estimate will guide one's choice. The time thus saved
postoperative cognitive outcome. This should be followed up with a can be devoted to tasks that are sensitive to specific side and/or site of
postoperative assessment no earlier than three to six months dysfunction and to those that address other issues of interest. An ex-
following surgery because some deficits prior to this time may be ception to this recommendation is with assessment of children because
transient. Follow-up evaluations at one- to two-year intervals can be schools often require a full IQ test for provision of special services.
performed as indicated to track recovery or evolution of cognitive
abilities. 2.2.2. Attention
The evaluation is also appropriate when patients present with Attention is a complex cognitive construct that includes the ability to
cognitive complaints, such as poor memory or impaired concentra- respond to basic sensory stimulation, selectively attend to relevant
tion. In many cases their concerns are driven by practical problems stimuli while suppressing responses to irrelevant stimuli, maintain
such as educational and occupational limitations. The neuropsycho- focus on the environment and respond to changing demands. Attention
logical evaluation can help define the details of the problem and can also is subserved by a complex network involving frontal, parietal and
provide a baseline for later evaluations as indicated. A third group of subcortical regions. Disruption of attention is common among patients
seizure patients who should always be referred are those suspected of with epilepsy due to a variety of factors including seizures, side effects of
experiencing cognitive side effects of their antiepileptic medications. In medications and underlying neuropathology. Problems with attention,
these instances, brief screening procedures (see below) are more useful along with memory deficits, are among the most commonly cited
than a comprehensive test battery because they can be repeated easily cognitive symptoms for epilepsy patients of all ages.
to assess change in function associated with a change in medications. In Attention is a building block for other cognitive functions. There-
the broadest sense, epilepsy patients should be referred for neuropsy- fore, its disruption can interfere with cognitive performance in other
chological assessment whenever the patient or the treating neurologist domains, especially memory. Thus it is crucial to identify problems
would benefit from a more detailed cognitive profile, particularly as it with attention to understand better its impact on the interpretation
relates to the patient's seizure type and frequency. of results from these other measures, as they reflect independent
localizable deficits. For example, a patient may have severe memory
2.2. Content of test battery problems due to inability to attend to the stimuli adequately for en-
coding and storage. Without proper assessment of attention, the
A basic neuropsychological assessment should sample many memory impairment may create the misleading impression of signif-
different cognitive functions, thus testing the status of different icant temporal lobe dysfunction.
brain regions and networks. Such an assessment, if thorough, requires Measurement of attention can be accomplished at several levels,
six to eight hours of direct contact between patient and examiner, as and is driven by both neuroanatomical and theoretical models [26,27].
can be appreciated from the discussion (below) of tests to be included. Although clinical observations can play a valuable role in assessing
The information obtained provides a broad picture of a patient's attention, there are also direct methods and psychometric tests available
cognitive strengths and weaknesses as well as highly specific aspects for this purpose. Symptoms of inattention to sensory / environmental
of brain function. The scores generated by such an evaluation allow stimuli can be assessed through double simultaneous stimulation to
objective conclusions to be drawn, comparing an individual's scores to look for unilateral suppression. Hemispatial inattention for visual
norms and to those of other patients with known lesions, and they stimuli is typically assessed through visual search tasks [e.g., 28].
provide a solid baseline to which later assessments can be compared if Selective attention, sustained attention, and vigilance are most often
patients are re-evaluated. The formal tests should be complemented assessed using visual or auditory continuous performance tasks. More
by careful behavioral observations of the patient's attitude, effort and complex aspects of attention, such as shifting focus, are addressed later
strategies, thus providing a context for interpretation of test scores. in the section on executive functions.
M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12 5

2.2.3. Nonverbal cognitive functions 2.2.4. Language


Nonverbal cognition encompasses a wide range of functions Language tasks measure the integrity of the dominant hemisphere
believed to be largely independent of linguistic processing, many of and, when compared to performance on visual spatial tasks, can be
which are mediated by the nondominant hemisphere. Nonverbal used to detect lateralized dysfunction. A typical language assessment
cognition has traditionally been evaluated almost exclusively with for epilepsy includes measures of naming, comprehension, generative
tasks of visual perception and visual spatial ability; assessment of fluency, single word reading, sentence repetition and reading [34].
auditory perception may also be important in certain cases. Perception When resection from the dominant temporal lobe is being
and spatial reasoning in any sensory modality represent higher level considered, visual object naming and auditory or responsive naming
cognitive processes that can only occur successfully if primary sensory [35,36] are the most important language tests because these patients
input and other supportive functions, such as sustained attention, are are at risk for postoperative dysnomia [37]. In patients undergoing
intact. Thus, when designing a battery of tests it is important to dominant posterior temporal or extratemporal resections, some
consider this functional hierarchy and, when necessary, to screen for additional specialized language tests may be added. Administration
more basic deficits such as visual disturbance, visual inattention or of a comprehensive aphasia examination is rarely necessary for
hemispatial neglect, which might preclude valid assessment of higher patients with epilepsy, who typically are not aphasic prior to or
level abilities. following surgery.
Dichotic listening tasks may be used as an index of hemispheric
2.2.3.1. Visual perception. Tasks include visual cancellation and visual dominance for language [38] when it is not feasible to conduct an
scanning [28], and drawing of simple symmetric objects such as a clock intracarotid anesthetic procedure, fMRI, or other cortical mapping
face, a daisy or a Greek cross. More complex asymmetric drawing tasks procedure.
such as the copy condition of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure [29,30]
may also be helpful, especially if the deficit is subtle. 2.2.5. Executive functions
Assuming that primary visual processing skills are adequate, The term “executive functions” includes the cognitive processes of
assessment of visual perception should include measures of visual initiation, planning, regulation of behavior, overcoming of habitual
recognition, as well as visual organization/ integration. Visual responses, attention, working memory, mental flexibility, planning
recognition involves the discrimination and interpretation of complex and reasoning that are necessary for the execution of goal directed
visual stimuli. Facial recognition is a relatively pure measure of this activity. These abilities are stressed most clearly when the individual
ability if no memory component is involved, as in the Benton Facial encounters novel situations that call for new forms of behavior based
Recognition test [31]. The test involves matching pictures of human on deliberation and choice.
faces presented in identical views, and in a more difficult condition, Examination of executive functions in epilepsy is useful for
matching front view with three-quarter view photographs. Visual detecting focal deficits associated with frontal lobe epilepsy where
recognition can also be assessed by tasks involving interpretation of a attention deficits, impulsivity, impaired motor coordination, and
pictorial scene in which the subject must identify what is missing or difficulty changing behavioral strategies are common. In contrast to
anomalous in a picture. Tests of visual organization/integration might patients with temporal lobe seizures, patients with frontal lobe
involve the ability to identify a figure from its disassembled or dis- epilepsy may show deficits in planning and behavioral adaptation in
organized parts as in the Hooper Visual Organization test [32] or to the context of intact memory [39]. Assessment of basic frontal lobe
perform visual closure based on degraded or fragmented drawings [33]. functions is necessary in any comprehensive neuropsychological
evaluation for epilepsy, but a more extensive assessment is recom-
mended for patients with frontal lobe seizures or those being
2.2.3.2. Spatial ability and constructional skills. The cognitive proces- considered for frontal topectomy.
sing of spatial relationships can be evaluated both with measures that
require only mental manipulation of stimuli and with procedures 2.2.5.1. Attention. Patients with frontal lobe disturbances are sensitive
requiring the actual construction of drawings or objects. Mental tests to distracting sensory information and have problems inhibiting,
of spatial ability overlap to some extent with measures of visual shifting and sustaining attentional focus. Inhibition may depend
perception, but typically require a spatial manipulation or judgment. critically upon prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex [40,41], and
An excellent example of this type of task is the Judgment of Line can be examined using Stroop tests [42; see 23]. Difficulties with
Orientation [31], which requires matching the visual angle of a line to sustained attention and inhibition can also be detected using
a sample. continuous performance tests [e.g., 43] and Go-No Go tasks. The
Constructional tasks of spatial ability involve a manual motor Trail Making Test [44] taps the ability to switch attention and may be
component. These vary from measures of graphomotor speed as in most sensitive to dorsolateral frontal damage [45].
Digit Symbol Coding, to copying or drawing tasks, to tests involving
two and three dimensional construction. 2.2.5.2. Fluency. Fluency tasks may be verbal (e.g., phonemic tasks
These measures of visual perceptual and spatial ability are believed requiring patients to produce words beginning with a specific letter,
to assess the integrity of left or right parietal lobe or the nondominant or semantic tasks requiring them to name items by category such as
temporal lobe. However, specific sensitivity of individual tests to focal animals or foods) or nonverbal. The Design Fluency test evaluates
cortical dysfunction has been difficult to demonstrate in many cases, nonverbal fluency in the nondominant hemisphere [46]. Patients are
particularly in populations of patients with focal seizure onset in the asked to invent abstract designs without drawing objects or any
absence of a structural lesion. Nonetheless, inclusion of a variety of nameable forms. Comparing performance on verbal with nonverbal
these measures in a comprehensive test battery can often result in the fluency tasks helps detect lateralized frontal lobe dysfunction in
identification of trends in performance across tasks that are ultimately patients with focal seizure disorders.
consistent with the area of epileptogenesis.
2.2.5.3. Memory. Performance of patients with frontal lobe dysfunction
2.2.3.3. Auditory perception. Tests of nonverbal auditory perception on learning and memory tasks may be compromised by impairments
may be helpful in patients with compromise of auditory association in working memory. Working memory tasks involve holding in mind
cortex in the nondominant hemisphere. Meaningful interpretation of and manipulating information, as in backward span or supraspan tests
these higher level auditory functions is possible only in the presence [e.g., 47]. In addition, frontal lobe patients may have difficulty
of appropriate auditory acuity. recollecting the source of information, such as the context or order
6 M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

of events. This may be tested by exposing patients to two different sets indices of hand proficiency are grip strength (using a hand dynamom-
of stimuli (e.g., lists of words) and later asking them to differentiate eter), speed (finger tapping), and dexterity (measured with pegboard
between the two. tasks). The preferred hand is not necessarily the most proficient, and
large discrepancies between the hands are not unusual in normal
2.2.5.4. Planning. Planning requires the ability to look ahead, conceive subjects. Despite the possibility of these discrepancies, highly deviant
of alternatives, weigh choices (think strategically), follow rules, performance across several motor tasks is rare in normal subjects [23].
inhibit impulses, and sustain attention. Planning can be assessed Therefore, conclusions about laterality of cerebral dysfunction are best
with simple measures, such as the Porteus mazes [48], or with more based on results derived from several motor tasks.
difficult ones such as tower tasks (London, Hanoi, and Toronto; e.g.,
[49]). Also, a patient's approach to copying a complex figure (e.g., the 2.2.7. Memory
Rey Complex Figure) may provide insight into planning ability.
2.2.7.1. Components of memory and anatomic correlates. The profound
2.2.5.5. Concept formation. The most widely-used test of concept memory impairments known to result from bilateral lesions in the
formation is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; [50]), which medial temporal lobe are rare. In contrast, the more restricted,
requires that the patient derive a principle, maintain it for a period of material-specific memory deficits of patients with unilateral temporal
time and then switch to another one with minimal feedback from the lobe dysfunction occur commonly. Therefore, a thorough memory
examiner. Thus the WCST relies on working memory, the ability to assessment should address each hemisphere with tasks appropriate
make inferences and deductions, to switch an ongoing action or adapt to its specialization– verbal learning and memory tasks to evaluate
to circumstances, and the ability to benefit from feedback. Although the dominant temporal lobe, and visuospatial or visuoperceptual
this test is used to assess frontal lobe damage [51], some patients with learning and memory tasks to evaluate the nondominant temporal
temporal lobe epilepsy also have difficulty performing the task [52], lobe. Verbal tasks use names, word lists, stories or number sequences
likely due to metabolic disruption of frontostriatal pathways in these as stimuli, while nonverbal tasks use faces, places, music or abstract
patients [53]. In addition, the WCST is useful for examining the designs. It is best to use tasks that are as purely verbal, or purely
processes used to solve complex problems. Conceptual reasoning can nonverbal, as possible to maximize differences between the hemi-
also be assessed with WAIS subtests such as Matrix Reasoning and spheres and to increase the probability that the tasks challenge
Similarities or with a measure of complex problem solving and primarily one temporal lobe [61].
hypothesis testing such as the Booklet Category Test [54].
2.2.7.2. Memory batteries. Many neuropsychologists working in
2.2.5.6. Social behavior and personality. Lesions of the orbital surface of epilepsy programs use memory tests from published batteries
the frontal lobes may cause affective and personality changes [55] because these are convenient and are usually standardized on large
including poor social judgment, disinhibition or social inappropriate- samples. The most common is the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS); the
ness, a tendency to place immediate gratification over long-term con- most recent evidence with respect to its efficacy in epilepsy has been
sequences of behavior, or excessive behavioral rigidity. A number of for the WMS-III, published in 1997 [62]. Subtests are divided into
scales have been developed to assess behavior change due to pathology verbal and visual modalities in that version of the WMS, but normative
in the frontal systems [56]; examples are the BRIEF [57] and the Frontal data specific to focal lesion populations were not reported and only a
Systems Behavior Scale [58]. In addition, the clinical interview and few studies over the past decade have examined the sensitivity of
observations assist in assessing these aspects of behavior. WMS-III subtests to left or right temporal lobe pathology. In a meta-
analysis of memory studies specific to epilepsy, WMS subtests did not
2.2.6. Somatosensory and motor functions differentiate left from right temporal lobe groups [63]. However, one
Somatosensory and motor testing is conducted to assess the recent study has reported material specific differences in performance
functions of the primary somatosensory cortex (postcentral gyrus) of WMS-III subtests by temporal lobectomy patients, with relative
and the primary motor cortex (precentral gyrus), and to provide impairment on verbal memory measures associated with left temporal
lateralizing and localizing information for patients being evaluated as lobectomy and relative impairment of visual memory in cases of right
surgical candidates. For some nonsurgical patients, the results provide temporal lobectomy [64].
information about hand function that may impact on the patient's One reason for the limited predictive value of the WMS is that the
daily living skills or vocational options. stimuli used in most subtests contain both verbal and nonverbal
Testing of somatosensory and motor function is typically conducted elements rather than reflecting primarily the functions of one hemi-
on the hands. There are a number of quantitative tests of somatosensory sphere or the other. Another difficulty with standardized test batteries
function available [22,23,59], including but not limited to the following: in general is that although the subtests attempt to tap different aspects
1) two-point discrimination, the ability to detect separation of two of memory, the verbal and nonverbal tasks are dissimilar and thus
stimuli presented simultaneously; 2) point localization; the ability to cannot provide a direct comparison of the hemispheres. It remains to
judge whether two successive stimuli were at the same or different be seen whether the WMS-IV, published in 2009, will demonstrate
locations; 3) position sense in the distal joints; 4) stereognosis, or improved sensitivity to material-specific memory functions.
recognition of stimuli by touch; 5) double-simultaneous stimulation,
a measure of tactile attention; 6) graphesthesia, tactile identification 2.2.7.3. Key factors in selecting and designing effective memory
of symbols traced on the skin. Two-point discrimination provides the measures. An important factor in assessing memory functions in the
greatest sensitivity and specificity [59]. In administering somatosensory two hemispheres is using tasks that are as similar as possible in
tests, the patient's attentional state should be monitored; interpretation structure and procedure, differing only in being either highly verbal or
of these data should take into account sex differences in sensory dis- highly nonverbal. This allows one to compare the efficacy of one
crimination [23]. temporal lobe to the other, even within individual patients. A growing
Assessment of motor function involves two aspects of hand function: number of tasks paired in this way are now being used by
preference (handedness) and proficiency (strength, speed and dexter- neuropsychologists working in epilepsy programs, and some exam-
ity). Handedness is often associated with the hand used for writing, but ples will be given below.
that activity is not always concordant with hand choice for other Another important variable is whether a task involves a single
unimanual tasks; therefore, it is best to use a questionnaire [e.g., 60] that exposure to the material to be learned followed by a retention test, or
assesses a variety of manual activities. The three most commonly used whether it is a learning task involving several learning trials and a
M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12 7

later retention test. We recommend against single-exposure tasks. and age. This subtest was not included in the recent WMS-IV revision.
They yield results that can vary considerably owing to effects of The ideal face memory task would incorporate a learning rather than a
attention, comprehension, and individual strategies, which are likely single exposure paradigm [80], and would expose the faces one at a
to play a bigger role on an only trial (or on a first trial), and these time as opposed to showing them in an array as is the case in many
effects can confound memory findings. Learning tasks provide the face memory tasks.
opportunity to improve with additional exposure to the material, Although experimental work has shown the importance of the
increasing the difference between people with a true learning deficit right medial temporal lobe structures in spatial memory, these
and those who do poorly after a single exposure for other reasons. findings have not yet been translated into standardized tests for
Using tasks with several learning trials permits observation of how a clinical use. Some experimental tasks show more promise than others.
patient would remember material after a single exposure, because the An analog of the radial arm maze revealed selective impairments in
first trial provides that information. spatial memory in both operated and unoperated patients with right
temporal lobe epilepsy compared with left temporal lobe groups and
2.2.7.4. Matched verbal vs. nonverbal learning and memory tests. controls [81]. However, on a test of memory for the spatial locations of
Several matched memory tasks have been developed in recent years, objects in a scene, differences in accuracy between unoperated
many of which arise from the addition of a nonverbal analog to a temporal lobe epilepsy patients and controls were not found [82].
previously-existing verbal test. Thus, there are now nonverbal analogs Further work on the development of clinically useful spatial memory
to the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; [22,65]; nonverbal: tests is needed.
[66]), and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT; [67];
nonverbal: [68]). In contrast, in one instance a verbal task was 2.2.7.6. Semantic memory. Semantic memory, that component of
added to an existing nonverbal one [69]. These tests vary in how memory reflecting knowledge of the world acquired from education,
closely matched the verbal and nonverbal versions are to each other, experience and repeated exposure to stimuli, can be impaired in
and although encouraging results in presurgical epilepsy patients people with temporal lobe epilepsy. Given its explicit verbalized
have been published [70], many of these tests await further empirical nature, semantic memory overlaps with the construct of language.
confirmation. Semantic memory impairment may be observable in verbal deficits
The Warrington Recognition Memory test (WRM) is a matched involving naming and single-word comprehension, as well as in visual
pair of tasks using words and faces. It has been reported to be sensitive knowledge and association deficits, while phonological, syntactic, and
to the effects of temporal lobe lesions in patients with neoplasms and perceptual abilities are preserved. In temporal lobe epilepsy, deficits
infarctions [71], but its clinical utility in distinguishing left from right in naming, semantic organization, classification, encoding and judg-
presurgical epilepsy patients is very limited [e.g., 72]. The WRM uses a ment have been described frequently, relating to lateral or medial
single-exposure paradigm, and its poor diagnostic efficiency in focal temporal lobe damage and reflecting loss of knowledge and/or im-
epilepsy may result from that, and/or from the fact that memory is paired access to the semantic store [83]. A number of tests have been
tested immediately after presentation of the stimuli. To best evaluate reported to provide a valid assessment of semantic memory in
medial temporal lobe functions, memory tests should incorporate a patients with epilepsy [83] including, for example, measures of object
delayed recall or recognition trial. naming and association of verbal and visual stimuli.

2.2.7.5. Other verbal and nonverbal learning and memory tests. A 2.2.7.7. Summary. Although some studies have reported that tradi-
number of single tasks have been reported to be sensitive to memory tional memory tests such as the Rey Complex Figure test or certain
deficits in unoperated epilepsy patients. In the verbal domain these subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scales can be sensitive to
include the RAVLT, the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; [73]), laterality of temporal lobe focus, others show that those tasks are not
and the SRT. All of these use lists of single words that are presented for sensitive, particularly in presurgical evaluation. Nonverbal memory
learning over several trials, with retention tested again after a delay deficits have been especially difficult to demonstrate with traditional
interval. Although we recommend using these tests with their tasks, but newer tests have been shown to be sensitive to non-
nonverbal analogs, published reports indicate good results also dominant temporal lobe dysfunction in unoperated patients. We
when used alone, especially on delayed retention scores. recommend the use of learning tasks over those with a single-
To sample memory in a way that is closer to the demands of exposure paradigm and the use of “matched” verbal and nonverbal
everyday life, it is valuable to include a test using meaningful prose, as in tasks, as these provide the best lateralizing and localizing data. For
a short story. Several such tests are available, including in the WMS. Our more detailed information about specific memory tests, see Djordjevic
recommendation is to choose a test of story learning rather than a test and Jones-Gotman [74] and Jones-Gotman and Djordjevic [84].
requiring recall of a story that has been presented only once [74,75].
There are many reports in the literature stating that “nonverbal” 2.2.8. Abbreviated test battery
memory measures had little success in detecting right temporal lobe Although we believe that an ideal evaluation should assess brain
damage, but until relatively recently those tests were essentially all function as thoroughly as our current tools allow, in some settings time
single-trial tasks. The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test [29,30] is and resources do not allow such comprehensive testing. If time is
used by most neuropsychologists in epilepsy programs, but this limited, we recommend sacrificing some portion of the IQ testing, even
supposedly nonverbal test does not reliably differentiate left from beyond the previously recommended short forms. A minimum estimate
right temporal lobe dysfunction [76]. It is an example of a task with a of overall ability could be based on a vocabulary measure together with
single exposure and dually-encodable material, and its usefulness as a one constructional task such as the Block Design subtest or another
memory test for individual cases is very limited. The copy phase of this measure of nonverbal intelligence. The inclusion of a short naming test
task is useful, however, for assessment of parietal lobe dysfunction. would enhance the assessment of verbal skills, adding little time to the
Using visuoperceptual tasks involving learning over several trials, overall assessment. An abbreviated memory evaluation should include
deficits in patients with nondominant temporal lobe dysfunction have matched (verbal and nonverbal) experimental learning measures
been documented for learning such things as lists of abstract designs similar to those described above, even if extensive normative data are
[77], arrays of dots [78] or arrangements of matchsticks [79]. Memory unavailable. It is important that the tasks chosen include a learning
for faces is believed to be a relatively pure nonverbal task, although component, avoiding the pitfalls associated with one-exposure memory
this is not true of the Faces subtest of the WMS-III, which does not paradigms. Executive function testing should focus on material-specific
control for several potentially verbal factors including gender, race, measures with appropriate counterparts such as verbal and nonverbal
8 M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

fluency, and should include a task requiring creative problem solving. or who have neuropsychological findings suggesting atypical lateral-
Psychomotor and sensory testing and measures of higher order visual ization. Patients whose seizure focus is clearly lateralized according to
perceptual and language processing can be considered optional, but all clinical tests (EEG, neuroimaging and neuropsychological), or in
should be selected when specific circumstances dictate the need. As noted whom there is no reason to suspect atypical organization of language,
below, severely shortened test batteries may be appropriate to screen for do not need to undergo an IAP. In many centers functional
side effects of antiepileptic medications or other highly selective neuroimaging studies are being performed for language lateralization.
assessments (e.g. return to baseline following a cluster of seizures), but When the results of these studies are unambiguous, an IAP for
those procedures should not be substituted for a multifaceted diagnostic language is unnecessary. With respect to memory, an IAP is necessary
evaluation. Thus, when time or resources are limited and an abbreviated in patients who are candidates for resection from a temporal lobe if
battery is needed, we recommend using one verbal and one “nonverbal” they are suspected of having bitemporal dysfunction, based on
subtest as a rough IQ measure, a naming task, one verbal and one neuropsychological, EEG or neuroimaging findings, and may be
visuoperceptual learning and retention task, a verbal and nonverbal desirable to help predict the risk to material specific (especially
fluency test, and a creative problem-solving task. This shortened verbal) memory decline postoperatively.
evaluation can be administered in approximately two hours and would
allow a retention interval of about two hours for the memory tests. 3.3. Recommendations for language assessment

3. Intracarotid anesthetic procedures (IAP) There is no standardized protocol available for determining
language dominance. Centers have typically developed their own
The intracarotid amobarbital procedure was introduced by Wada procedures, which are, however, based on broad consensus, and are
and Rasmussen in the 1950s to determine hemispheric language frequently quite similar.
dominance prior to epilepsy surgery, and was expanded a few years
1) Protocols should sample multiple language modalities including
later to include evaluation of memory functions [85–87]. This technique
automatic or rote speech, naming, reading, repetition and auditory
involves the injection of a short-acting anesthetic directly into the
comprehension. Stimulus items used in naming can double as
internal carotid artery, resulting in unilateral inactivation of a single
memory items and should consist of real objects or realistic
hemisphere while the opposite hemisphere is tested. Despite the
colored pictures to maximize correct perception of the items.
invasive nature of the procedure, it was long considered the “gold
2) Testing for language should be conducted during the period of
standard” for obtaining this critical information in preoperative patients.
maximum drug effect, with relatively greater weight given to
More recently, drugs other than amobarbital have also been used
responses occurring prior to the onset of motor recovery if the
in the IAP. These include propofol [88], the shorter acting methohex-
protocol involves a single injection of anesthesia.
ital [89], and most recently etomidate [90–92]. The methohexital test
3) The diagnosis of left hemisphere language dominance should be based
protocol differs from the basic original, as the very short drug effect
on evidence of appropriate language responses following the right
necessitates injecting additional drug during the procedure. The
injection combined with the presence of a global aphasia following
etomidate procedure introduced the modification of continuous
the left injection, and paraphasic errors during the recovery period.
infusion of the drug after the initial bolus injection, thereby extending
4) The diagnosis of reversed language dominance or bilateral
the period of hemianesthesia to ensure completion of the memory
language representation should be based on production of correct
protocol during maximum drug effect.
language responses following the left injection as well as language
Noninvasive neuroimaging techniques such as PET, functional MRI,
errors following the right injection [96].
and Magnetic Source Imaging are used increasingly to lateralize and
localize language cortex. To date, the clinical validity of these methods
3.4. Recommendations for memory assessment
for assessing risk to memory functions after surgery has not been
demonstrated. Therefore, we recommend the continued use of the IAP
In general, results of memory testing in the IAP have been less
in the presurgical diagnostic evaluation of surgical candidates, with
reliable and more difficult to interpret than language data. While
careful consideration of the guidelines listed below.
some concern has arisen over the incomplete irrigation of the hip-
pocampus with internal carotid injection, it has been demonstrated
3.1. General recommendations
that the hippocampus appears to be disabled or disconnected [97].
The duration of drug effect is a critical factor in obtaining reliable
When an IAP is performed, we strongly recommend that both
memory data because language testing may take place first, resulting
hemispheres be tested. The possibility of bilateral representation of
in memory items being shown during recovery from the drug. This
language can be determined only by testing both hemispheres, and
problem can be avoided using the etomidate version of IAP, in which
different information about memory function is obtained from each
following the initial bolus injection the level of anesthesia is
hemisphere. Furthermore, comparing memory results obtained from
maintained until all memory items have been presented [91,92].
the two hemispheres helps determine the side of seizure onset [93,94].
The dose should be held constant over the two injections. 1) Memory stimulus items should consist primarily of real objects
If there is an option of testing the two hemispheres on separate that can be encoded as verbal or visual information to maximize
days, this is desirable, as it has been shown that there is a lingering their “memorability.” Some protocols have successfully included
drug effect from the first injection that affects the second [95]. When modality-specific memory stimuli such as words or abstract
testing on different days is not possible there should be a minimum of designs, but these should not be substituted for “general memory”
30 minutes between injections. The hemisphere of the proposed items. A minimum of eight memory items is recommended for
surgery should be injected first in these cases, as the first injection is presentation during the drug condition.
believed to yield more accurate data. 2) Memory should be tested in a recognition paradigm presented
after full recovery from the drug effect.
3.2. Patient selection 3) Interpretation of memory results must take into account the
“memory dominance” effect in which injection into the nondom-
Determination of cerebral dominance for language is needed in inant hemisphere elicits less memory deficit than injection into the
people who are non-right handers or have a family history of left dominant hemisphere. This trend is exaggerated when the seizure
handedness, who have suffered early trauma to the left hemisphere, focus is on the nondominant side.
M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12 9

4) Memory performance should be at least 60% accurate to indicate epilepsy surgery candidates [106–108]. Magnetic Source Imaging
adequate memory in a given hemisphere. has the advantage of measuring neuronal activity in real time,
providing excellent temporal resolution in relation to stimulus
3.5. The IAP with pediatric patients onset, while the spatial resolution of the signal is also considered
equal to or better than fMRI [109]. The development of paradigms to
Because of the invasive nature and high level of cooperation activate the medial temporal structures is also underway [110].
needed for this test, not all children who are candidates for epilepsy
surgery may be able to successfully complete the procedure, 5. Pediatric issues
particularly the memory assessment; factors such as age and IQ may
help identify children in whom the IAP may be more difficult [98,99]. Assessment of children requires a different conceptual approach.
IAP memory testing in children, as in adults, has been used to predict With children, one has to consider the effects of dysfunction within a
seizure and memory outcome [100,101]. The IAP can be successfully developing nervous system, ongoing maturational changes, behav-
used in children as young as 6 years if they are cooperative. Many ioral and structural plasticity, and the impact of environmental and
centers now routinely use fMRI for language localization in pediatric social factors on development [111]. Moreover, the long-term impact
patients [102], resulting in less use of the IAP in children. of surgical treatment is likely to be very different in a child compared
Pre-IAP preparation is an important factor in maximizing a with adult patients, and prediction of risk of cognitive morbidity vs.
successful and valid procedure. Preparation includes pre-testing the benefit is more complex. Developmental factors such as these
child to determine the appropriate developmental level for language indicate that the brain-behavior relationships derived from the
assessment. In addition, a pre-IAP visit to the angiography suite to orient study of adults do not apply to children and necessitate a sub-
the child to the testing environment is recommended to reduce the stantially different approach to the assessment and interpretation of
child's anxiety and introduce key personnel. This is particularly results in children.
important when the angiography procedure is performed under general There is a large and well-established literature on the lateralizing
anesthesia and the patient may awaken confused and disoriented. and localizing signs seen in adults with focal epilepsy, but these may
or may not be present in children with epilepsy for a variety of
4. Functional neuroimaging/cortical mapping reasons. The underlying pathological substrate is frequently different
in children compared with adults. For example, Porter and colleagues
In contrast to traditional neurocognitive assessment, which can be found that 64% of pediatric patients with refractory temporal lobe
used to detect focal, lateralized or diffuse brain dysfunction based on epilepsy who underwent temporal lobe resection had cortical
profiles of test scores, fMRI is used to localize cerebral function dysplasia [112]. The majority of intractable seizures in children are
through task-correlated blood flow changes. Unlike the IAP, where due to disorders of cortical development, which results in more
language and memory are lateralized to a hemisphere using an widespread anatomical and functional disturbance [113].
inactivation procedure (hemianesthesia), fMRI employs an activation Another difference is the degree to which cognitive skills are
procedure to not only lateralize but also localize cognitive function dissociable in early childhood. In addition to the theoretical
prior to epilepsy surgery. Applications of fMRI in epilepsy include complexity of dealing with the developing nervous system and the
determining hemispheric representation of language and memory small body of published findings on preoperative assessments with
functions, studying functional reorganization and predicting cognitive pediatric candidates for epilepsy surgery, the neuropsychologist is
outcome after temporal lobe resection. faced with other challenges in working in pediatric programs. A wide
fMRI mapping techniques are widely available for language, but range of assessment skills and experience are necessary to assess
fMRI for clinical evaluation of memory in individual patients requires children from infancy to adolescence, with a wide range of etiologies
further task development and validation. Examination of fMRI that may result in patterns of atypical development or generally
activation in specific regions of interest and with carefully designed, normal development with only specific deficits [114]. The type of
well-controlled testing protocols offers a powerful method for assessment and the choice of procedures will depend on the age and
functional localization and for predicting language and memory developmental level of the child.
outcome [103]. At present, however, it is not clear whether activation Based on these factors, the recommended approach to assessment
maps from language protocols can be used to guide the resection of children should encompass the following basic principles:
boundaries as it has not been demonstrated that resection of activated
voxels is correlated with language decline. 5.1. Theoretically based developmental assessment
While considerable progress has been made in the design and
interpretation of fMRI studies of language and memory, it is not the time Assessment should employ techniques that are sensitive to
to abandon IAP [104]. IAP has the methodological advantage of being an developmental differences in ability that are theoretically grounded
inactivation procedure and the practical advantages of not being limited and based on sound developmental principles in emergence of the
by claustrophobia. On the other hand, limitations of the IAP include the skill being tested. For example, language assessment should take into
relatively short time period of drug activation in which testing can be account the expected developmental emergence of symbolic repre-
done, and its invasive aspects. While fMRI provides detailed information sentation, grammatical structure, and word knowledge for the child's
about functional localization, it is unclear to what extent activated chronological age. Differences in pragmatic use of language, compre-
voxels represent networks critical for the performance of a cognitive hension and language-based academic achievement are also impor-
activity or represent tertiary associated or non-task specific functions. tant. Simple language tasks that encompass word finding and verbal
Finally, fMRI task protocols differ and not all have the same validity, fluency will be insufficient to provide information about functional
particularly where careful attention has not been paid to control or integrity of language substrate in the dominant hemisphere for
contrast tasks. For example, fMRI language activation based on passive purposes of seizure localization or prediction of language outcome in
listening, silent repeating or categorizing, reading or naming contrasted the event of surgical intervention.
with rest or visual fixation is not concordant with language dominance
from IAP (see [105] for a review). 5.2. Use of tests with strong normative and psychometric backgrounds
Application of Magnetoencephalography/Magnetic Source Imag-
ing in mapping cortical functions is rapidly gaining credibility as an The use of clinical signs will always be an important component
alternative noninvasive method of localizing language cortex in of comprehensive neuropsychological testing, however normal
10 M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

development encompasses a wide range of ages at which develop- 7. Quality of life


mental milestones can be met and still be considered “normal”.
Normative references are helpful in establishing not only when Quality of life (QOL) has been defined by the World Health
emergence of a skill is delayed, but how frequently such a delay occurs Organization (1995) as a subjective, multidimensional and dynamic
in the non-referred population. This is a critical element in entity that encompasses physical, cognitive, psychological, social, and
determining the clinical significance of patterns of test performance spiritual aspects, resulting from an interaction between expectations
in children. and actual experiences. Satisfactory QOL is an important goal of
Establish baseline level of function as early as possible following epilepsy treatment, representing, together with seizure control, an
diagnosis. It is understood that by the time a seizure disorder is outcome of pharmacological trials and surgery, and a goal of cognitive
diagnosed, cognitive symptoms may already be present [115]. rehabilitation and psychosocial interventions.
However, changes in cognitive function are critical for determining The International League against Epilepsy's Commission on Outcome
disease progression, distinguishing iatrogenic effects from disease- Measurement in Epilepsy suggested that a standard QOL battery should
related variables, and identifying postoperative changes in patients include both epilepsy-specific and generic scales [120]. Epilepsy-related
who undergo surgery. factors account for only about 5% of the variance in QOL, much of the
remaining being attributable to psychological and social factors such as
5.3. Re-assessment mood, cognitive impairment, self-image, subjective perception of
epilepsy severity and disability, attitudes towards epilepsy and chronic
Developmental change may result in the emergence of different medication, and fear of stigma [121–123]. A comprehensive assessment
patterns of competencies and deficits over time. Developmental of quality of life should include consideration of these factors. Specific
change may also be impacted by fluctuations in seizure status or QOL instruments have been reviewed by Cramer [120].
alterations in medications to produce changes in the child's cognitive
or behavioral competencies. These different patterns may interact 8. Cross-cultural issues
with increasing educational demands as the child progresses through
the school system, and re-assessment will be helpful in providing Cultural diversity presents a challenge to professionals who wish to
recommendations to enhance educational progress. apply the methods discussed in the preceding sections to their local
populations. Accurate measurement of neuropsychological function is
5.4. Comprehensive multidisciplinary assessment culture-dependent [124]. Not only is there concern about whether the
functional domains are assessed accurately by the procedures described,
This principle is shared with adult neuropsychological assess- but whether cognitive functions operate in the same manner across
ments in that cognitive domains, psychosocial function, and emo- ethnic/cultural backgrounds (i.e., the concept of “functional equiva-
tional development are all inter-related. However, the pediatric lence”; [125]). Potential sources of bias include test content, the
patient may be more affected and have fewer internal resources and standardization sample, language and socioeconomic factors.
coping mechanisms compared to adults. Identifying patients at risk
for learning problems, emotional disturbance, and poor psychosocial 8.1. Content
development at an early stage will facilitate treatment and long-term
favorable outcomes. Development of standardized tests tends to rely on the experiences of
the dominant culture for which the test is primarily intended to be used.
6. Cognitive effects of antiepileptic drugs An example of this is the commonly used Wechsler Scales for assessment
of general intelligence. Cultural references abound in both the verbal and
An exhaustive report of antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) and their nonverbal tasks of this battery. The Wechsler scales have been adapted for
potential cognitive side effects is beyond the scope of this report. several different languages, with revisions to content also based on
However, it can be said that the effects of AEDs on cognitive function culture. However changes to the content may introduce a different source
are linked to their efficacy controlling seizures. Adverse side effects of interpretive ambiguity, specifically whether the constructs being
are the greatest factor in non-compliance, and cognitive complaints measured have been altered by the content changes.
are a common reason for discontinuing medication [116].
Assessing the potential side effects of AEDs can be challenging, as it 8.2. Standardization
may be difficult to disentangle such effects from those of the seizures
and the underlying neural abnormality. If possible, test-retest on and During standardization, publishers make every effort to represent
off the AED can be helpful, as long as alternate test forms are available. ethnic diversity to the degree that a group is represented in the
As testing twice poses a burden on the examiner and the patient, population. However, this may not be adequate, in that groups with
an alternative approach when feasible is to wean the patient off the small representation in the population may not be represented
AED before performing that patient's neuropsychological assessment, adequately in the normative sample, rendering the norm-referenced
carrying out the testing several days after the last dose. Alternatively, standard scores marginal for these under-represented groups.
when AED toxicity is suspected, administration of a brief screening Furthermore, even groups that are represented often comprise a
battery may identify individuals in whom a full neuropsychological relatively small part of the complete standardization sample, watering
battery should be deferred. Screening measures should include tests down the validity across populations. Minority groups that are
of working memory, and psychomotor and mental processing speed – included may also represent a select subsample of the larger group.
tasks generally not subject to significant practice effects.
Special individual factors (such as age, presence of intellectual 8.3. Language
disabilities or use of other prescription medications) should be con-
sidered when educational, quality of life and communication issues Effective communication between the clinician and the patient is
must be balanced with other clinical factors [117,118]. Finally, the critical. Even subtle differences in dialect can create barriers to clarity
subjective experience of cognitive decline cannot be discounted in the of interpretation of individual responses, resulting in lower scores due
clinical setting. Indeed the perception of decline can be as important to language differences rather than differences in verbal cognitive
as formal neuropsychological test results in a patient's/physician's abilities. In addition, the difficulty level for items on language tasks is
decision to continue or discontinue a given treatment [119]. not necessarily the same across different languages and cultures.
M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12 11

8.4. Socioeconomic [16] Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Haltiner A, Wyler AR. Relationship of age at onset,
chronological age, and adequacy of preoperative performance to verbal memory change
after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia 1995;36:137–45.
There are often educational differences (both level and quality) [17] Kneebone AC, Chelune GJ, Dinner DS, Naugle RI, Awad IA. Intracarotid amobarbital
associated with socioeconomic differences, even within a particular procedure as a predictor of material-specific memory change after anterior temporal
lobectomy. Epilepsia 1995;36:857–65.
geographic region. Attention to socioeconomic factors that may affect [18] Sawrie SM, Chelune GJ, Naugle RI, Lüders H. Empirical methods for assessing
performance on cognitive tasks is important for valid inferences meaningful neuropsychological change following epilepsy surgery. J Int Neuropsychol
regarding performance on tests as a measure of brain function. Soc 1996;2:556–64.
[19] Martin RC, Sawrie SM, Roth DL, et al. Individual memory change after anterior
Differences within as well as across geographic regions can be temporal lobectomy: a base rate analysis using regression-based outcome
profound, and can interact with cultural and ethnic differences. methodology. Epilepsia 1998;39(10):1075–82.
[20] Chiaravalloti ND, Glosser G. Material-specific memory changes after anterior temporal
lobectomy as predicted by the intracarotid amobarbital test. Epilepsia 2001;42:902–11.
8.5. Recommendations
[21] Baxendale S, Thompson P. Defining meaningful postoperative change in epilepsy
surgery patients: measuring the unmeasurable? Epilepsy Behav 2005;6:2007–11.
While it may not be possible to create the literally thousands of [22] Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D. Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York:
versions of tests needed to address all of these concerns, attention to Oxford University Press; 2004.
[23] Strauss E, Sherman E, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: admin-
these issues and their influence on assessment of nonverbal as well as istration, norms and commentary. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006.
verbal abilities is important to further develop our understanding of [24] Aldenkamp AP, Alpherts WC, Dekker MJ, Overweg J. Neuropsychological aspects of
the effects of epilepsy on cognitive function. We strongly encourage learning disabilities in epilepsy. Epilepsia 1990;31(Suppl 4):S9–S20.
[25] Holmes GL. The long-term effects of seizures on the developing brain: clinical and
development of culturally relevant norms to facilitate reliable and laboratory issues. Brain Dev 1991;13:393–409.
valid inferences that may be drawn about brain-behavior relation- [26] Mirsky AF, Anthony BJ, Duncan CC, Ahearn MB, Kellam SG. Analysis of the elements of
ships from the results of neuropsychological examination. attention: a neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychol Rev 1991;2:109–45.
[27] Posner MI, Peterson SE. The attention systems of the human brain. Ann Rev Neurosci
1990;13:25–42.
9. Conclusion [28] Mesulam M. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology. New York: Oxford
University, New York; 2000.
[29] Osterrieth P. Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Arch Psychologie 1944;30:
Neuropsychology occupies a unique position among diagnostic 206–356.
methods in epilepsy because it is the sole technique for understanding [30] Rey A. L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. Arch
the functional impact of epilepsy and its treatment. Cognitive and Psychologie 1942;28:286–340.
[31] Benton AL, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological
behavioral strengths and weaknesses identified by neuropsychological
assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983.
assessments contribute to a number of important decisions in medical [32] Hooper HE. Hooper Visual Organization Test (VOT). Los Angeles: Western Psychological
management, such as the identification of candidates for surgery, Services; 1983.
potential risk, benefits and efficacy of treatments, and rehabilitation. [33] James M, Warrington EK. Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury St. Edmunds,
Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Co; 1991.
Neuropsychology is an important part of any comprehensive care [34] Jones-Gotman M, Smith ML, Zatorre RJ. Neuropsychological testing for localizing and
program for persons with epilepsy. lateralizing the epileptogenic region. In: Engel Jr J, editor. Surgical treatment of the
epilepsies. 2nd ed. New York: Raven Press; 1993. p. 245–61.
References [35] Bell BD, Seidenberg M, Hermann BP, Douville K. Visual and auditory naming in patients
with left or bilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2003;55:29–37.
[1] Kimura D. Right temporal-lobe damage: perception of unfamiliar stimuli after [36] Hamberger MJ, Seidel WT, McKhann 2nd GM, Perrine K, Goodman RR. Brain stimulation
damage. Arch Neurol 1963;8:264–71. reveals critical auditory naming cortex. Brain 2005;128:2742–9.
[2] Milner B. Brain mechanisms suggested by studies of temporal lobes. In: Millikan [37] Hermann BP, Perrine K, Chelune GJ, et al. Visual confrontation naming following left
CH, Darley FL, editors. Brain mechanisms underlying speech and language. New anterior temporal lobectomy: a comparison of surgical approaches. Neuropsychology
York: Grune & Stratton; 1967. p. 122–45. 1999;13:3–9.
[3] Jones MK. Imagery as a mnemonic aid after left temporal lobectomy: contrast between [38] Zatorre RJ. Perceptual asymmetry on the dichotic fused words test and cerebral speech
material-specific and generalized memory disorders. Neuropsychologia 1974;12:21–30. lateralization determined by the carotid sodium amytal test. Neuropsychologia
[4] Milner B. Advances in neurology. In: Purpura DP, Penry JK, Walter RD, editors. 1989;27:1207–19.
Psychological aspects of focal epilepsy and its neurosurgical management. New York: [39] Culhane-Shelburne K, Chapieski L, Hiscock M, Glaze D. Executive functions in children
Raven Press; 1975. p. 299–301. with frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:623–32.
[5] Jones-Gotman M, Milner B. Right temporal-lobe contribution to image-mediated verbal [40] Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate
learning. Neuropsychologia 1978;16(1):61–71. conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 2004;303:1023–6.
[6] Sass KJ, Spencer DD, Kim JH, Westerveld M, Novelly RA, Lencz T. Verbal memory [41] Stuss DT, Floden D, Alexander MP, Levine B, Katz D. Stroop performance in focal lesion
impairment correlates with hippocampal pyramidal cell density. Neurology 1990;40: patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia
1694–7. 2001;39:771–86.
[7] Lencz T, McCarthy G, Bronen RA, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging in [42] Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:
temporal lobe epilepsy: relationship to neuropathology and neuropsychological 643–62.
function. Ann Neurol 1992;31:629–37. [43] Connors CK. Conners' Continuous Performance Test-II. Toronto: Multi Health Systems;
[8] Hermann BP, Wyler AR, Somes G, Berry III AD, Dohan Jr FC. Pathological status of the 2001.
mesial temporal lobe predicts memory outcome from left anterior temporal lobectomy. [44] Army Individual Test Battery. Manual of directions and scoring. Washington, DC: War
Neurosurgery 1992;31:652–6. Department, Adjutant General's Office; 1944.
[9] Saling MM, Berkovic SF, O'Shea MF, Dalnins RM, Darby DG, Bladin PF. Lateralization of [45] Stuss DT, Bisschop SM, Alexander MP, Levine B, Katz D, Izukawa D. The Trail Making
verbal memory and unilateral hippocampal sclerosis; evidence of task-specific effects. J Test: A study in focal lesion patients. Psychol Assess 2001;13:230–9.
Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1993;15:608–18. [46] Jones-Gotman M. Localization of lesions by psychological testing. Epilepsia 1991;32
[10] Baxendale SA, Van Paesschen W, Thompson PJ, et al. The relationship between (Suppl 5):41–52.
quantitative MRI and neuropsychological functioning in temporal lobe epilepsy. [47] Wagner DD, Sziklas V, Garver KE, Jones-Gotman M. Material-specific lateralization of
Epilepsia 1998;39:158–66. working memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:112–22.
[11] Baxendale SA, Sisodiya SM, Thompson PJ, et al. Disproportion in the distribution of gray [48] Porteus SD. Porteus Maze Test. Fifty years' application. New York: The Psychological
and white matter: neuropsychological correlates. Neurology 1999;52:248–52. Corporation; 1965.
[12] Wendel JD, Trenerry MR, Xu YC, et al. The relationship between quantitative T2 [49] Goel V, Grafman J. Are the frontal lobes implicated in “planning” functions? Interpreting
relaxometry and memory in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 2001;42: data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia 1995;35:623–42.
863–8. [50] Grant DA, Berg EA. A behavioral analysis of degreee of impairment and ease of shifting
[13] Loring DW, Lee GP, Meador KJ, et al. The intracarotid amobarbital preocedure as a to new responses in a Weigle-type card sorting problem. J Exp Psychol 1948;39:404–11.
predictor of memory failure following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neurology [51] Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. The role of the frontal lobes.
1990;40:605–10. Arch Neurol 1963;9:90–100.
[14] Wyllie E, Naugle R, Awad I, et al. Intracarotid amobarbital procedure: I. prediction [52] Hermann BP, Wyler AR, Richey ET. Wisconsin card sorting test performance in patients
of decreased modality-specific memory scores after temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia with complex partial seizures of temporal-lobe origin. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1988;10:
1991;32:857–64. 467–76.
[15] Chelune GJ, Naugle RI, Luders H, Awad IA. Prediction of cognitive change as a function of [53] Martin RC, Sawrie SM, Gilliam FG, et al. Wisconsin card sorting performance in patients
preoperative ability status among temporal lobectomy patients seen at 6-month follow with temporal lobe epilepsy: Clinical and neuroanatomical correlates. Epilepsia
up. Neurology 1991;41:399–404. 2000;41:1626–32.
12 M. Jones-Gotman et al. / Epilepsy and Behavior 18 (2010) 3–12

[54] DeFilippis NA, McCampbell E. Manual for the Booklet Category Test. Odessa, FL: [90] Andelman F, Kipervasser S, Maimon S, Neufeld M, Kramer U, Fried I. Are WADA test
Psychological Assessment Resources; 1991. results a function of the drug used? Epilepsia 2008;49(Suppl 7):155.
[55] Damasio AR, Anderson SW. (2003) The frontal lobes. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, [91] Jones-Gotman M, Sziklas V, Djordjevic J, et al. Etomidate speech and memory test
editors. Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. p. 404–46. (eSAM): A new drug and improved intracarotid procedure. Neurol 2005;65:1723–9.
[56] Malloy P, Grace J. A review of rating scales for measuring behavior change due to frontal [92] Jones-Gotman M, Sziklas V, Djordjevic J. Intracarotid amobarbital procedure and
systems damage. Cog Behav Neurol 2005;18:18–27. etomidate speech and memory test. Can J Neurol Sci 2009;36(Suppl 2):S51–4.
[57] Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive [93] Loring DW, Murr AM, Meador KJ, et al. Wada memory testing and hippocampal volume
Function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000. measurements in the evaluation for temporal lobectomy. Neurol 1993;43:1789–93.
[58] Grace J, Malloy PF. Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe): Professional manual. Lutz, [94] Perrine K, Westerveld M, Sass KJ, et al. Wada memory disparities predict seizure
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001. laterality and postoperative seizure control. Epilepsia 1995;36:851–6.
[59] Corkin S, Milner B, Rasmussen T. Somatosensory thresholds. Arch Neurol 1970;23: [95] Bengner T, Haettig H, Merschhemke M, Dehnicke C, Meencke HJ. Memory assessment
41–58. during the intracarotid amobarbital procedure: Influence of injection order. Neurol
[60] Annett M. A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br J Psychol 2003;61:1582–7.
1970;61:303–21. [96] Risse GL, Gates JR, Fangman MD. A reconsideration of bilateral language repre-
[61] Jones-Gotman M. Neuropsychological techniques in the identification of epileptic foci. sentation based on the intracarotid amobarbital procedure. Brain Cogn 1997;33:
Epilepsy Res 1992:87–94 Suppl. 5. 118–32.
[62] Wechsler D. WMS-III. Administration and Scoring Manual. The Psychological [97] Gotman J, Bouwer MS, Jones-Gotman M. Intracranial EEG study of brain structures
Corporation. San Antonio: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1997. affected by internal carotid injection of sodium amobarbital. Neurol 1992;42:2136–43.
[63] Lee TMC, Yip JTH, Jones-Gotman M. Memory deficits after resection from left or right [98] Westerveld M, Zawacki T, Sass KJ, Spencer SS, Novelly RA, Spencer DD. Intracarotid
anterior temporal lobe in humans: A meta-analytic review. Epilepsia 2002;43: amytal procedure evaluation of hemispheric speech and memory function in children
283–91. and adolescents. J Epilepsy 1994;7:295–302.
[64] Doss RC, Chelune GJ, Naugle RI. WMS-III performance in epilepsy patients following [99] Hamer HM, Wyllie E, Stanford L, Mascha E, Kotagal P, Wolgamuth B. Risk factors for
temporal lobectomy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:173–9. unsuccessful testing during the intracarotid amobarbital procedure in preadolescent
[65] Rey A. L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaire de France, Paris; children. Epilepsia 2000;41:554–63.
1964. [100] Lee GP, Park YD, Westerveld M, Hempel L, Blackburn LB, Loring DW. Wada memory
[66] Majdan A, Sziklas V, Jones-Gotman M. Performance of healthy subjects and patients performance predicts seizure outcome after epilepsy surgery in children. Epilepsia
with resection from the anterior temporal lobe on matched tests of verbal and 2003;44:936–43.
visuoperceptual learning. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1996;18:416–30. [101] Lee GP, Westerveld M, Blackburn LB, Park YD, Loring DW. Prediction of verbal memory
[67] Buschke H, Fuld PA. Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory decline after epilepsy surgery in children: Effectiveness of Wada memory asymmetries.
and learning. Neurol 1974;24:1019–25. Epilepsia 2005;46:97–103.
[68] Fletcher JM. Memory for verbal and nonverbal stimuli in learning disability subgroups: [102] Sachs BC, Gaillard WD. Organization of language networks in children: functional
Analysis by selective reminding. J Exp Child Psychol 1985;40:244–59. magnetic resonance imaging studies. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2003;3:157–62.
[69] Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Olivier A, et al. Learning and retention of words and [103] Binder JR, Sabsevitz DS, Swanson SJ, Hammeke TA, Raghavan M, Mueller WM. Use of
designs following excision from medial or lateral temporal-lobe structures. Neuropsy- preoperative functional MRI to predict verbal memory decline after temporal lobe
chologia 1997;35:963–73. epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia 2008;49:1377–94.
[70] Jones-Gotman M. Psychological evaluation for epilepsy surgery. In: Shorvon S., Dreifuss [104] Binder JR. fMRI could, in principle, replace the Wada Test. In: Miller JW, Silbergeld DL,
F.D., Fish D., Thomas D., editors. The treatment of epilepsy. Oxford: Blackwell Science editors. Controversies in epilepsy surgery. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc; 2006.
Ltd.; 1996. p. 621-30. p. 342–3.
[71] Warrington EK. Recognition memory test. Berkshire: NFER-Nelson; 1994. [105] Binder JR, Achten E, Constable RT, et al. Functional MRI in epilepsy. Epilepsia 2002;43
[72] Hermann BP, Connell B, Barr WB, Wyler A. The utility of the Warrington Recognition (Suppl 1):S51–63.
Memory Test for temporal lobe epilepsy: Pre- and postoperative results. J Epilepsy [106] Papanicolaou AC, Simos PG, Breier JI, et al. Magnetoencephalographic mapping of the
1995;8:139–45. language-specific cortex. J Neurosurg 1999;90:85–93.
[73] Delis DC, Kramer J, Ober BA, Kaplan E. The California Verbal Learning Test. New York: [107] Papanicolaou AC, Simos PG, Castillo EM, et al. Magnetoencephalography: a noninvasive
Life Science Associates; 1993. alternative to the Wada procedure. J Neurosurg 2004;70:867–76.
[74] Djordjevic J, Jones-Gotman M. Psychological testing in presurgical evaluation of [108] Doss RC, Zhang W, Risse GL, Dickens DL. Lateralizing language with magnetic source
epilepsy. In: Shorvon S, Perucca E, Fish D, Dodson E, editors. The treatment of epilepsy. imaging: Validation based on the Wada test. Epilepsia 2009;50:2242–8.
2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell Science Ltd.; 2004. p. 699–715. [109] Hamalainen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Magnetoencephalo-
[75] Frisk V, Milner B. The role of the left hippocampal region in the acquisition and retention graphy: theory, instrumentation, and applications to non-invasive studies of the
of story content. Neuropsychologia 1990;28:349–59. working human brain. Rev Mod Phys 1993;65:413–97.
[76] Barr WB, Chelune GJ, Hermann BP, et al. The use of figural reproduction tests and [110] Ver Hoef LW, Sawrie S, Killen J, Knowlton RC. Left mesial temporal sclerosis and verbal
measures of nonverbal memory in epilepsy surgery candidates. J Int Neuropsychol Soc memory: A magnetoencephalography study. J Clin Neurophysiol 2008;25:1–6.
1997;3:435–43. [111] Fletcher JM, Taylor HG. Neuropsychological approaches to children: towards a
[77] Jones-Gotman M. Right hippocampal excision impairs learning and recall of a list of developmental neuropsychology. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1984;6:39–56.
abstract designs. Neuropsychologia 1986;24:659–70. [112] Porter BE, Judkins AR, Clancy RR, Duhaime A, Dlugos DJ, Golden JA. Dysplasia: A
[78] Risse GL, Doss RC, Hempel AM. Penovich, PE The performance of temporal lobe epilepsy common finding in intractable pediatric temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurol 2003;61:365–8.
(TLE) patients on verbal and nonverbal selective reminding procedures: pre and [113] Duchowny MS. Developmental lesions in epilepsy. ASIM 2001;1:147–52.
postoperative comparisons. Epilepsia 2006;47(Suppl 4):102. [114] Smith ML. Presurgical neuropsychological assessment. In: Lassonde M, Jambaque I,
[79] Helmstaedter C, Pohl C, Hufnagel A, Elger CE. Visual learning deficits in nonresected Dulac O, editors. The neuropsychology of childhood epilepsy. New York: Plenum
patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex 1991;27:547–55. Press; 2001. p. 207–14.
[80] Dade LA, Jones-Gotman M. Face learning and memory: the twins test. Neuropsychology [115] Fastenau PS, Johnson CS, Perkins SM, et al. Neuropsychological status at seizure onset in
2001;15:525–34. children: risk factors for early cognitive deficits. Neurology 2009;18(73):526–34.
[81] Abrahams S, Pickering A, Polkey CE, Morris RG. Spatial memory deficits in patients with [116] Tatum W, French JA, Faught E, et al. Post marketing experience with topiramate and
unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuro-psychologia 1997;35: cognition. Epilepsia 2001;42:1134–40.
11–24. [117] Baker GA, Goldstein LH. The dos and don'ts of neuropsychological assessment in
[82] Baxendale SA, Thompson PJ, Van Paesschen W. A test of spatial memory and its clinical epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2004;5(Suppl 1):S77–80.
utility in the pre-surgical investigation of temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Neuropsy- [118] Motamedi GK, Meador KJ. Antiepileptic drugs and memory. Epilepsy Behav 2004;5:
chologia 1998;36:591–602. 435–9.
[83] Giovagnoli AR, Erbetta A, Villani F, Avanzini G. Semantic memory in partial epilepsy: [119] Aldenkamp AP, Baker GA. Systematic review of the effects of lamotrigine on cognitive
verbal and non-verbal deficits and neuroanatomical relationships. Neuropsychologia function and quality of life. Epilepsy Behav 2001;2:85–91.
2005;43:1482–92. [120] Cramer JA. ILAE Subcommission on Outcome Measurement in Epilepsy (Camfield C,
[84] Jones-Gotman M, Djordjevic J. Neuropsychological testing in presurgical evaluation. In: Carpay H, Helmstaedter C, Langfitt J, Malmgren K, Wiebe S). Principles of health-
Shorvon S, Perucca E, Engel J, editors. The treatment of epilepsy. 3rd ed. Oxford: related quality of life: assessment in clinical trials. Epilepsia 2002;43:1084–95.
Blackwell Science Ltd; 2009. p. 851–63. [121] Giovagnoli A, Avanzini G. Quality of life and memory performance in patients with
[85] Branch C, Milner B, Rasmussen T. Intracarotid sodium Amytal for the lateralization of temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;101:295–300.
cerebral speech dominance: Observations in 123 patients. J Neurosurg 1964;21: [122] Perrine K, Hermann BP, Meador KJ, et al. The relationship of neuropsychological
399–405. functioning to quality of life in epilepsy. Arch Neurol 1995;52:997–1003.
[86] Milner B, Branch C, Rasmussen T. Study of short-term memory after intracarotid [123] Surmeijer TP, Reuvekamp MF, Aldenkamp BP. Social functioning, psychological
injection of sodium Amytal. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 1962;87:224–6. functioning and quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsia 2001;42:1160–8.
[87] Wada J, Rasmussen T. Intracarotid injection of sodium Amytal for the lateralization of [124] Wong T.M, Strickland T.L, Fletcher-Janzen E.F, Ardila A, Reynolds C.R. Theoretical and
cerebral speech dominance: Experimental and clinical observations. J Neurosurg practical issues in the neuropsychological assessment and treatment of culturally
1960;17:226–82. dissimilar patients. In Fletcher-Janzen E.F, Strickland T.L, and Reynolds CR, editors.
[88] Takayama M, Miyamoto S, Ikeda A, et al. Intracarotid propofol test for speech and Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology. New York: Kluwer Academic, Plenum
memory dominance in man. Neurol 2004;63:510–5. Publishers; 2000.
[89] Buchtel HA, Passaro EA, Selwa LM, Deveikis J, Gomez-Hassan D. Sodium methohexital [125] Pérez-Arce P. The influence of culture on cognition. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:
(Brevital) as an anesthetic in the Wada test. Epilepsia 2002;43:1056–61. 581–92.

S-ar putea să vă placă și