Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

Do Consumers Avoid Watching

Over-the-Counter Drug Advertisements?


An Analysis of Cognitive and Affective Factors

That Prompt Advertising Avoidance

JISU HUH Do consumers avoid viewing over-the-counter (OTC) pharmaceutical advertisements? And,
University of Minnesota
if so, why? The authors of the current study tested their proposed “Ad Avoidance Model”
jhuh@umn.edu
by surveying a nationally-representative sample of U.S. adults about their reactions to
DENISE E. DELORME
advertisements for OTC drugs (specifically analgesics). The researchers focused on how
University of Central
Florida four advertising-reaction factors—two cognitive (perceived utility and skepticism) and two
denise.delorme@ucf.edu affective (irritation and attitude toward advertising)—influenced avoidance behaviors. The

LEONARD N. REID
results revealed that avoidance was directly linked to irritation and attitude, although
University of Georgia attitude also partially mediated the relationship between irritation and avoidance. Age and
Virginia Commonwealth
socioeconomic status also played roles in these relationships.
University
lnreid@uga.edu

INTRODUCTION psychological and other intervening variables, the


In most situations, advertising is created to influ­ primary goal of most advertising messaging is to
ence consumers by affecting a positive sequence of move consumers along a positive-oriented process­
communication events, known as the "hierarchy of ing path involving cognitive, affective, and behav­
effects" (DeLorme, Huh, and Reid, 2006; Menon, ioral reactions to advertisements.
Deshpande, Zinkhan, and Perri, 2004; Vakratsas Early examinations of consumers' general atti­
and Ambler, 1999). Although advertisers recognize tudes or reactions toward advertising have pro­
that the nature of advertisement processing (e.g ., duced mixed findings. But studies in 1995 and
low and high involvement) is affected by a host of 1998 comparing those earlier findings with their

• Consumer avoidance of over-the-counter (OTC) drug advertising is more directly and strongly
influenced by affective reactions (e.g., irritation) and attitude toward such advertising than
cognitive reactions (e.g., perceived utility or skepticism).

• As such, when consumers are more irritated by and feel less favorable toward OTC drug
advertisements, they are more likely to avoid them.

• High-income and younger consumers most likely will be irritated by (and avoid) OTC drug
advertisements, so diagnostic pretesting should consider these particular segments.

• Irritation and perceived utility are inversely linked, hence irritation could be mitigated by making
advertisements more informational and useful to the target consumers.

DOI: 10.2501/JAR-2015-022 December 2015 JOURnflL OF HDUERTiSMG RESEARCH 4 0 1


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

S o m e h a v e fo u n d escape encountered advertisements that • identified the interconnected relation­


reduce their exposure to and process­ ships among the cognitive, affective,
a d v e r tis in g ir r ita tio n ing of advertisement content (Speck and and behavioral components of advertis­
Elliott, 1997). Advertising avoidance may ing avoidance;
to b e a n e m o tio n a l be intentional or unintentional (Duff and • used a comprehensive model-testing
Faber, 2011) and involves both deliberate approach based on the hierarchy of
r e s p o n s e a s s o c ia te d decisions and habit-based behaviors (Baek effects (HOE) framework (Menon et al.,
and Morimoto, 2012), which tend to be 2004).
w ith d is p le a s u r e similar across different media (Prender-
gast, Cheung, and West, 2010). Research on advertising irritation and
a n d im p a tie n c e . Advertising avoidance has been concep­ advertising avoidance mainly has been
tualized in various ways in the literature. descriptive, and, to the current authors'
Common classifications are: knowledge, none of this work has been
conducted within the domain of OTC drug
own results revealed that attitudes toward • "cognitive avoidance" (e.g., intentionally advertising. Recent advertising-avoidance
advertising seem to be more favorable than ignoring advertisements); inquiry has been more theoretically driven,
previously suggested (e.g., O'Donohoe, • "mechanical avoidance" (e.g., using a and several conceptual models have
1995; Shavitt, Lowrey, and Haefner, 1998). DVR to skip television commercials); been proposed and tested (e.g., Baek and
These studies documented that many • "behavioral avoidance" (e.g., leaving the Morimoto, 2012; Cho and Cheon, 2004;
consumers like advertisements and that room during a commercial break; Cho Edwards, Li, and Lee, 2002).
their attitudes tend to vary by attitudinal and Cheon, 2004; Kelly, Kerr, and Dren- The authors of the current article believe,
dimensions and types of attitudes (e.g., nan, 2010; Speck and Elliott, 1997); however, that there is a need for more com­
Reid and Soley, 1982). • "active avoidance" versus "passive prehensive identification and explanation
Yet, consumers often react to advertising avoidance" (Duff and Faber, 2011). of the drivers of advertising avoidance and
in negative ways. Two of the many nega­ their underlying mechanisms. This espe­
tive reactions are irritation and avoidance. A more complete overview of advertising- cially is the case regarding the influence
Both are widespread, increasing, and con­ avoidance research is provided later in of irritation, other advertising-reaction
cerning as they can potentially damage this article. factors, and individual differences (Speck
effectiveness of individual advertisements, The current study analyzed key factors and Elliott, 1997). The authors furthermore
campaigns, and the industry overall (Duff influencing advertising avoidance. In the believe their research addresses this gap
and Faber, 2011). context of over-the-counter (OTC) drug and contributes benchmark evidence for
Some have found advertising irrita­ advertising—specifically for analgesics— future comparison.
tion to be an emotional response associ­ this study compared the relationships Advertising irritation and avoidance
ated with displeasure and impatience between advertising avoidance and four also are worthy of additional investigation
(Aaker and Bruzzone, 1985). Others have advertising-reaction factors: for practical purposes. In the United States,
described advertising irritation as feelings billions of dollars are spent on advertising
of annoyance that are less intense than • advertising irritation annually to generate positive responses
offensiveness (which stems from moral • utility to branded products. In 2014 advertising
concerns) (Li, Edwards, and Lee, 2002), • skepticism spending experienced its biggest increase
but more intense than dislike (Aaker and • attitude. in a decade at an estimated $180.12 bil­
Bruzzone, 1985). But the antecedents and lion, expected to reach $189.38 billion in
consequences of advertising irritation have Specifically, the authors: 2015 and $200 billion by 2016.1Thus, there
been the subject of substantial research is significant monetary value associated
(See "Literature Review" on pp. 279-280). • investigated how advertising avoidance 1 "Total US A d Spending to See Largest Increase Since
Advertising avoidance is a behavio­ differentially is related to key demo­ 2004." (2014, July 2). Retrieved July 22, 2015, from
eMarketer Press Release: http://www.emarketer.com/
ral outcome of advertising exposure. graphics and the above four advertising- A rticle,/T otal-U S-A d-Spending-See-Largest-Increase-
It includes all actions by consumers to reaction factors; Since-2004/1010982.

402 J0URM1L OF HDUERTISMG RESEARCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

with understanding advertising irrita­ Gore, Madhavan, McClung, and Riley, specific domains in which advertising is
tion and avoidance in an effort to mitigate 1994; Kim, 2005; Ratchford, 1987). used. Furthermore, knowledge is believed
and reduce these two negative consumer best advanced by studying advertis­
responses to advertising for different types The current study conceptually was ing within the context of these domains
of products. Scholars, in fact, have urged grounded in the fundamental elements (Nyilasy and Reid, 2009).
both advertising theorists and practition­ of the "hierarchy of effects" (HOE) frame­ Thus, the current authors believe,
ers to pay attention to advertisements that work of how advertising works (e.g., product-specific, focused investigations:
are ignored and to identify why they are Vakratsas and Ambler, 1999).
ignored (Duff and Faber, 2011). Although some theories acknowledge • are less removed from actual consumer
that advertising may work differently experiences than nonproduct-specific
R esearch Product Focus: OTC Drugs under different product-based circum­ investigations, and
The authors of the current paper sought stances, HOE models are claimed—both • should be considered additive in the
a common field of consumption experi­ explicitly and implicitly—to have uni­ search for universal applicability of
ence for comparative purposes. Hence, versal applicability (e.g., Vakratsas and psychological concepts across different
they restricted their study to analgesics, Ambler, 1999). product advertising domains.
a widely experienced type of OTC drug. The HOE model posits that consumer
Such a narrow focus is in line with a tra­ responses to advertising occur sequen­ LITERATURE REVIEW
dition of product category research (e.g., tially, starting with a cognitive response, Advertising avoidance has received con­
Grow, Park, and Han, 2006; Kim and King, leading to an affective response and in turn siderable research attention (e.g., Duff
2009). Although a general, nonproduct- to a behavioral response. and Faber, 2011; Kelly et al, 2010; Speck
specific advertising-avoidance model The current authors built their and Elliott, 1997). Although advertising
could provide informative results, the cur­ "advertising-avoidance model" within avoidance has been examined across all
rent authors believed a test of a product- the HOE framework. The advertising- media (e.g., Baek and Morimoto, 2012; Cho
specific model would produce more useful avoidance model, the authors believe, can and Cheon, 2004; Prendergast et al., 2010;
results and provide greater certainty of be used to explain advertising effects and Rojas-Mendez and Davies, 2015; Speck
comparative uniformity of advertising- outcomes—including advertising irritation and Elliott, 1997), television has received
related consumer responses. and avoidance, respectively—for other the most avoidance attention. Many tele­
The authors believed their results would types of products that share many of the vision advertising studies have been
have broad theoretical implications to help same cognitive, attitudinal, and behavioral sought to determine the influences of new
explain other product advertising avoid­ responses to OTC drug advertisements. technologies on commercial viewing and
ance and effects beyond the OTC drug Theoretical ideas about how advertis­ avoidance—e.g., "zipping (fast-forwarding
advertising context. Indeed, scholars have ing works must be considered relative to commercials) and zapping (channel
considered OTC drugs as a: particular advertising domains (Nyilasy switching)"—(Cronin and Menelly, 1992;
and Reid, 2009). A domain represents a Heeter and Greenberg, 1985; Krugman,
• "think product" (a product for which boundary condition, which is believed Cameron, and White, 1995; Krugman and
the purchase decision is mainly logical to influence basic relationships between Johnson, 1991; Stout and Burda, 1989).
based mainly on functional facts); advertising factors and outcome variables. To the current authors' knowledge, pub­
• "search product" (a product dominated There are four known domains (Nyilasy lished research on advertising avoidance in
by attributes for which information can and Reid, 2009): print media is rare (Prendergast et al, 2010;
be obtained before purchase); Speck and Elliott, 1997). There has been
• "high-involvement product" (a prod­ • product category significant interest in investigating adver­
uct that requires extensive thinking and • strategic objective/intent tising avoidance in the online environ­
information-seeking for purchase deci­ • media form ment— e.g., pop-up advertisements, spam,
sion making); • historical time period. advertisements on social networking sites,
• a product with "high-risk trial" (a prod­ and online video advertising (Baek and
uct that involves a high level of risk if Accordingly, understanding how advertis­ Morimoto, 2012; Cho and Cheon, 2004;
a wrong product were purchased; e.g., ing works is viewed as dependent on the Duff and Faber, 2011; Edwards et al, 2002;

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 J0URM1L OF HDUERTISinG RESEARCH 4 0 3


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

Goodrich, Schiller, and Galletta, 2015; Kelly factors of advertising avoidance have been Results revealed privacy concerns,
et al, 2010; Morimoto and Chang, 2006). taken in the context of: advertising irritation, perceived person­
Much research on advertising avoidance alization, and skepticism toward per­
has studied the processes of avoidance and • online p o p-up ad v ertisin g —testing sonalized advertising were significant
the influence of different types of avoid­ the impact of various advertising mes­ antecedents of advertising avoidance.
ance on advertising effectiveness. A subset sage factors and consumer advertising-
of this literature, however, has explored reaction factors on avoidance of pop-up Applying a similar conceptual framework
potential antecedents of advertising avoid­ advertisem ents. The proposed model of advertising-avoidance antecedents as
ance in one or more media and the ways explained the interconnected role of per­ used in the latter two studies (Baek and
these factors operate. Antecedents exam­ ceived advertising entertainment, infor­ Morimoto, 2012; Cho and Cheon, 2004), the
ined include: mativeness, intrusiveness, and irritation authors of the current study focused on a
in shaping pop-up advertising avoid­ set of affective and cognitive advertising-
• dem ographics (e.g., Heeter and Green­ ance (Edwards et al, 2002). reaction factors as key antecedents of
berg, 1985; Speck and Elliott, 1997); advertising avoidance across different
• perceived advertising clutter (e.g., Cho • Internet advertising—a causal model of media and advertising forms.
and Cheon, 2004); general Internet advertising avoidance
• perceived irritation (e.g., Baek and Mori­ that encom passed three dim ensions HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT
moto, 2012; Edwards et al., 2002); (cognitive, affective, behavioral; Cho Affective Advertising-Reaction Factors:
• perceived intrusiveness (e.g., Edwards and Cheon, 2004). The model stemmed Irritation and Attitude
et al, 2002; Goodrich et al, 2015); from previous research findings that had In considerable prior research on adver­
• advertising skepticism (e.g., Baek and identified three significant antecedents tising in general and on pharmaceutical
Morimoto, 2012); of Internet advertising avoidance (per­ advertising in particular,
• advertising usefulness, informativeness, ceived advertising clutter online, per­
or utility (e.g., Edwards et al, 2002; Lee ceived goal impediment, negative prior • advertising irritation (e.g., Baek and
and Lumpkin, 1992; Speck and Elliott, experience with online advertising). M orimoto, 2012; De Pelsmacker and
1997); Perceived advertising clutter and Van den Bergh, 1998; Edw ards et al,
• goal im pedim ent (e.g., Cho and Cheon, goal im pedim ent were consistent with 2002; Fennis and Bakker, 2001; Pasadeos,
2004); prior findings on advertising avoidance 1990) and
• attitude tow ard advertising (e.g., Cho in traditional media (Speck and Elliott, • attitude toward advertising (e.g., Diehl,
and Cheon, 2004; Prendergast et al, 2010; 1997), whereas negative prior experi­ Mueller, and Terlutter, 2008; Menon
Rojas-Mendez and Davies, 2015; Speck ence (i.e., overall dissatisfaction and et al, 2004)
and Elliott, 1997). perceived lack of utility) was identified
as a new predictor for Internet advertis­ have been conceptualized and examined
A lthough some advertising-avoidance ing avoidance. as important affective consumer responses
antecedents ap p ear u niversal, others to advertisements.
depend on media context, individual dif­ • personalized advertising—a model of A dvertising irritation is considered a
ferences, and situational factors (Speck advertising avoidance for personal­ potentially im portant advertising-reaction
and Elliott, 1997). Overall, advertising ized advertisem ents (Baek and M ori­ factor that influences advertising avoid­
inform ativeness (utility) and a ttitu d e moto, 2012). The model was composed ance (De Pelsmacker and Van den Bergh,
tow ard advertising have been identified of two factors influencing personalized 1998; Edwards et al, 2002). Because of its
as rather consistent predictors of advertis­ advertising-avoidance behavior: potential importance, a num ber of stud­
ing avoidance (e.g., Cho and Cheon, 2004; ies have investigated the antecedents and
Edw ards et a l, 2002; Lee and Lum pkin, -v*cognitive (personalized advertising consequences of advertising irritation
1992; Prendergast et a l, 2010; Speck and skepticism) (e.g., Aaker and Bruzzone, 1985; De Pels­
Elliott, 1997). ■ ^affective (p riv a c y c o n c e rn s , macker and Van den Bergh, 1998; Fennis
Various theory-based, systematic m od­ a d v ertisin g irritatio n , perceived and Bakker, 2001; Greyser, 1973; Pasa­
eling approaches to examining influencing personalization). deos, 1990). However, little research has

404 JO UR flfiL OF RDUERTIBIIIG RESEARCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? T H E A R F .O R G

Advertising irritation • advertising in particular media (e.g., Cho (advertising irritation and attitude) and
and Cheon, 2004; Cronin and Menelly, advertising avoidance:
is considered a 1992; Lee and Lumpkin, 1992; Speck and
Elliott, 1997); HI: A dvertising irritation will be
potentially important • attitude toward the advertisement (e.g., positively related to advertising
Stout and Burda, 1989). avoidance.
advertising-reaction
In research on advertising-avoidance H2: The relationship between adver­
factor that influences research in traditional and new media, tising irritation and advertising
attitude toward advertising in a particular avoidance will be m ediated by
advertising avoidance. medium is the most prominent advertising- attitude toward advertising.
avoidance antecedent. For example:
Cognitive Factors:
em pirically investigated the connection • Consumers who zapped television com­ Perceived U tility and S kepticism
betw een advertising irritation and adver­ mercials tended to have less positive In the literature, both perceived adver­
tising avoidance. attitudes tow ard the advertisem ents tising utility or inform ativeness (e.g.,
The authors believe that only two stud­ (Stout and Burda, 1989). Ducoffe, 1995) and advertising skepti­
ies explicitly have examined the relation­ • Greater zipping and zapping was asso­ cism (e.g., Baek and Morimoto, 2012; Fri-
ship betw een advertising irritation and ciated with negative attitudes toward estad and W right, 1994; Oberm iller and
avoidance (Baek and M orim oto, 2012; television advertising (Lee and Lum p­ Spangenberg, 2000) have been character­
Edwards et al, 2002). From an online sur­ kin, 1992). ized and treated as cognitive consum er
vey of a convenience sam ple of college • When potential predictors of advertising advertising-reaction factors.
students, advertising irritation was found avoidance were compared across four From existing research on advertising
to have a direct positive effect on respond­ traditional media including two broad­ avoidance, advertising utility (i.e., per­
ents' avoidance of personalized advertise­ cast (television, radio) and two print ceived usefulness or informativeness of
ments (Baek and Morimoto, 2012). And an (magazines, newspapers) media, atti­ advertisements) has emerged as a consist­
analysis of the relationships am ong p op­ tude toward advertising in the medium ently significant antecedent of advertis­
up advertising intrusiveness, irritation, was the strongest predictor of advertis­ ing avoidance (e.g., Cho and Cheon, 2004;
and avoidance found perceived intrusive­ ing avoidance for all media (Speck and Edwards et al, 2002; Lee and Lumpkin,
ness of online pop-up advertisements gen­ Elliott, 1997). 1992; Speck and Elliott, 1997).
erated irritation and advertising avoidance • Consumers likely avoided an advertise­ Some stu d ies on the relationship
(Edwards et al., 2002). m ent sim ply on awareness that it was between advertising utility or usefulness
Com pared w ith research on the rela­ an advertisem ent, not because of the and advertising avoidance have in d i­
tionship betw een advertising irritation advertising content, a finding that sug­ cated advertisem ents that are perceived
and avoidance, there are relatively more gests attitudes tow ard advertising in as useful and informative are less likely
empirical studies providing evidence of general can lead to advertising avoid­ to be avoided. For example, television-
attitude tow ard advertising influencing ance (Cronin and Menelly, 1992). com m ercial avoidance w as inversely
advertising avoidance. More positive atti­ related to belief of advertising-information
tudes tow ard advertising are associated To the current authors' knowledge, there usefulness (Lee and Lumpkin, 1992). Like­
with lower advertising avoidance, as dem ­ is no formal theory or empirical research wise, advertising avoiders considered
onstrated in studies that have examined sim ultaneously examining the relation­ advertisem ents less useful than did non
attitudes toward: ships among advertising irritation, adver­ avoiders for both broadcast and print
tising attitude, and advertising avoidance. media (Speck and Elliott, 1997).
• advertising in general (e.g., Prendergast On the basis of the works they have cited, Similar findings have been reported
et a l, 2010; Rojas-Mendez and Davies, however, they offer the following hypothe­ for Internet advertising. Pop-up adver­
2015; R ojas-M endez, D avies, and ses regarding the relationships between the tisements less likely w ould be avoided if
Madran, 2009); two affective advertising-reaction factors they were thought to provide information

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 JOURRAL OF RDUERTISIRG RESEARCH 4 05


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

(Edwards et al, 2002). And, perceived lack In the context of these findings and and affective advertising reactions. Empiri­
of utility, w hich was m easured as part applying the basic conceptual model of cal evidence in the advertising-irritation
of prior negative experience w ith Inter­ the hierarchy of effects (HOE) model, the literature, for example, indicates younger
net advertising, was positively related to authors of the current study proposed the consum ers exhibit higher advertising
Internet advertising avoidance (Cho and following hypotheses: irritation than older consumers (Greyser,
Cheon, 2004). 1973). And, higher socioeconomic status
"Advertising skepticism," a consumer H3: Perceived advertising utility will (SES; white-collar, higher income, college-
trait defined as a tendency to disbelieve have an indirect negative rela­ educated) consum ers had significantly
advertising claims (Obermiller and Span- tionship to advertising avoid­ higher advertising irritation than their
genberg, 1998), has been found to play a ance through its negative link to lower SES counterparts (Aaker and Bruz­
role in advertising responses, including advertising irritation. zone, 1985).
advertising avoidance (Obermiller and A dvertising skepticism also has been
Spangenberg, 2000). In addition, skeptical H4: A dvertising skepticism w ill associated with certain consumer dem o­
consumers tend to exhibit greater advertis­ have an indirect positive rela­ graphic characteristics, particularly age,
ing irritation and, thus, are more likely to tionship to advertising avoid­ education, and income (DeLorme, Huh,
avoid advertisements: ance through its positive link to and Reid, 2009; Obermiller and Spangen­
advertising irritation. berg, 1998). Advertising skepticism has
• Skepticism has been associated nega­ varied by age and education as well as by
tively with attitudes toward advertising C o n s u m e r D e m o g ra p h ic s an d experience and marketplace beliefs (Ober­
and positively w ith advertising avoid­ A d v e rtis in g A v o id a n c e miller and Spangenberg, 1998).
ance (Obermiller, Spangenberg, and Previous research {e.g., Heeter and Green­ Skepticism tow ard direct-to-consumer
MacLachlan, 2005). berg, 1985; K rugm an, Cam eron, and prescription drug (DTC) advertising has
• Advertising skepticism partially m edi­ White, 1995; Prendergast et al, 2010; Speck been positively related to education and
ated the relationship between advertis­ and Elliott, 1997) has explored advertis­ income, b u t w hen all exam ined dem o­
ing irritation and avoidance regarding ing avoidance among various consumer graphic and health-related characteristics
personalized advertising (Baek and demographics including: were considered concurrently using a
Morimoto, 2012). m ultivariate analysis m ethod, only edu­
• age cation rem ained a significant predictor
Despite the potential importance of adver­ • gender of DTC advertising skepticism (DeLorme
tising skepticism in shaping adv ertis­ • income et a l, 2009).
ing avoidance, em pirical investigation • education There is evidence that perceived adver­
of advertising skepticism has been rela­ • race tising utility or usefulness also varies by
tively limited compared w ith studies on • marital status. consumer demographic characteristics (e.g.,
advertising utility, to the current authors' DeLorme et al., 2010; Deshpande, Menon,
knowledge—a failing that calls for more Yet only a few variables, such as age and Perri, and Zinkhan, 2004; O'Keefe, Nash,
scholarly attention. income, significantly have been related and Liu, 1981; Soley and Reid, 1983). In this
Regarding the relationship betw een to a d v e rtisin g -av o id an ce beh av io rs stream of literature, significant differences
the cognitive and affective advertising- (Speck and Elliott, 1997). Evidence of in perceptions of advertising utility or infor­
reaction factors, research has demonstrated dem ographic antecedents of advertising mativeness were found for consumers':
an inverse relationship between advertis­ avoidance is inconsistent and weak when
ing usefulness and advertising irritation, consumer predispositions and advertising- • age {e.g., King, Reid, Tinkham, and
suggesting that advertisements perceived reaction factors are considered (Prender­ Pokrywczynski, 1987),
as useful {i.e., inform ative, relevant, of gast et al., 2010; Speck and Elliott, 1997), • race {e.g., DeLorme et al, 2010; Soley and
value) elicit less irritation than do other the current authors believe. Reid, 1983),
advertisements (e.g., Aaker and Bruzzone, Demographic characteristics more likely • socioeconomic background (education,
1985; Aaker and Norris, 1982; Ducoffe, are related to advertising avoidance indi­ income; e.g., O'Keefe et al, 1981; Soley
1995; Edwards et al., 2002; Pasadeos, 1990). rectly through their influence on cognitive and Reid, 1983).

406 JOURFIRL OF RDUERTISIRG RESEARCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

On the basis of these previous findings, the To investigate general Internet adver­ stim uli advertisem ents influenced p er­
following hypotheses predicted the role of tising avoidance, a model had been pro­ ceived advertising intrusiveness. And that
dem ographics in influencing advertising posed in which cognitive and affective perceived advertising intrusiveness, in
avoidance indirectly through their relation­ advertising-reaction factors influenced turn, influenced advertising irritation and
ships to advertising-reaction antecedents: advertising-avoidance behavior concur­ avoidance (Edwards et al, 2002).
rently with no particular sequential order In the latter experiment, the scholars
H5: Age, income, and education will (Cho and Cheon, 2004). That model was m anipulated three advertising message
be significantly related to the tested w ith a convenience-sample survey factors:
consum er advertising-reaction of college students; the results indicated
factors that are related to adver­ perceived goal im pedim ent, perceived • editorial-advertisement congruence,
tising avoidance. Specifically, advertisem ent clutter, and prior negative • duration of interruption, and
experience significantly predicted avoid­ • intensity of cognition
H5a: Age will be positively related ance of Internet advertising.
to advertising utility and nega­ In contrast, four advertising-reaction w hen faced w ith a pop-up advertise­
tively related to advertising constructs were examined in the context ment, measuring four advertising-reaction
skepticism. of personalized advertising (privacy con­ variables, and docum enting advertising-
cern, perceived personalization, advertis­ avoidance behavior through observation.
H5b: Income and education will be ing irritation, and advertising skepticism) The results supported the hypothesized
negatively related to advertising in a sequential model (Baek and Morimoto, model with slight modification by adding
utility and positively related to 2012), in which: a direct path from advertising entertain­
advertising skepticism. ment and advertising irritation.
• advertising irritation (affective fac­ The c u rren t a u th o rs' adv ertisin g -
A dvertising Avoidance M odel tor)—directly and indirectly through avoidance m odel was inform ed by the
Previous studies on advertising avoidance skepticism — influenced advertising literature on advertising avoidance and its
proposed conceptual tripartite m odels avoidance; antecedents as well as by the conceptual
of advertising avoidance (e.g., Baek and • advertising skepticism (cognitive fac­ tripartite m odels of advertising avoid­
M orimoto, 2012; Cho and Cheon, 2004), tor) operated as both a direct predictor ance (Baek and M orim oto, 2012; Cho
based on a three-com ponent structure of of advertising avoidance and a mediator and Cheon, 2004). Using this model, the
consumer response to advertising stimuli between other advertising-reaction fac­ authors predicted (See Figure 1):
(Vakratsas and Ambler, 1999): tors and advertising avoidance.
• a d v e rtis in g -a v o id a n c e b e h a v io rs
• cognition Still others proposed an empirically based directly w ould be influenced by con­
• affect model depicting how consumer reactions sum ers' negative affective reactions to
• behavior. (i.e., perceived advertising informativeness advertising {i.e., advertising irritation
and advertising entertainment) to different and advertising attitude);
Those studies ap p eared to have con­
structed and tested similar tripartite m od­
els because they included sim ilar types
of affective and cognitive advertising-
reaction factors influencing advertising-
avoidance behavior. Yet, the causal order
of the affective and cognitive advertising-
reaction factors differed across differ­
ent stu d ies, an d the m ethodological
approaches v aried d e p en d in g on the
research context and specific advertising-
reaction factors considered. Figure 1 Conceptual Model of Advertising Avoidance

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 J0UR1L OF RDUERTISinG RESERRCH 4 0 7


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

• negative affective reactions would raffles or sweepstakes with cash rewards • perceived health (measured by a
be shaped by cognitive reactions (i.e., and other prizes. 7-point scale, ranging from 1 [extremely
advertising utility and advertising Respondents completed the survey in unhealthy] to 7 [extremely healthy]);
skepticism); about 14 minutes on average. Of the 418
• demographics (age was measured using
• cognitive reactions would be affected by completed surveys, 17 cases were elimi­
an open-ended question, and gender,
consumer demographics, particularly nated because of an unusually short time
education, race, and income were meas­
age, income, and education. for survey completion (between one min­
ured through closed-ended questions).
ute and five minutes) and patterns of low-
METHODOLOGY quality responses. After this data cleaning
An online survey was conducted with a process, a poststratification procedure The questionnaire first was pretested with
nationally-representative sample of U.S. (i.e., application of weighting) was used to a small sample of adults to diagnose any
adults (age 18 years or older). The study adjust for any survey nonresponse, non­ problems or issues with survey instruc­
used OTC analgesic advertising as an coverage, or under- and oversampling tions, question wording, and question
advertising context for more focused and resulting from the sample design. order. A second small pretest was con­
better contextualized data collection. Thus, ducted after development of the online
the sampling procedure used an inclusion Questionnaire and Measurements survey site to verify the survey length and
criterion of "currently using" or "having The 27-item questionnaire collected infor­ proper operation of the online data collec­
ever used any OTC analgesics in the past mation on: tion system.
six months."
The survey sample was purchased from • advertising avoidance (measured by RESULTS
the online survey firm, Knowledge Net­ seven 7-point scales; Speck and Elliott, The age of the respondents ranged from
works, which provides nationally rep­ 1997; See Table 1); 18 to 87 years, and the average age was
resentative samples for online surveys. 51 (SD = 17.1). The respondents were pre­
• advertising utility (measured by five
Knowledge Networks panel members are dominantly White (79.3 percent), and the
items adapted from three earlier studies;
recruited through probability-based ran­ sample included slightly more females
Deshpande et al., 2004; Obermiller and
dom digit dialing (RDD) sampling, and (55.4 percent) than males (44.6 percent).
Spangenberg, 1998; and Shavitt et al.,
households are provided with Internet About one-third (32.9 percent) of the
1998);
access and a computer, if needed. respondents had at least a college degree.
From this preestablished survey panel, a • advertising skepticism (measured using Median household income was $60,000-
sample of 909 adults was drawn randomly a nine-item 7-point Likert scale; Ober­ under $75,000.
and 553 completed the screener, result­ miller and Spangenberg, 1998; see also Of the 418 respondents, 157 (39.2 per­
ing in a 60.8 percent survey response rate. DeLorme et al., 2009; Huh, DeLorme, cent) reported being regular users of OTC
After filtering through the screening ques­ and Reid, 2012); analgesics. The number of OTC analge­
tion (OTC analgesic use), 418 qualified and • advertising irritation (measured by four sics used on a regular basis ranged from
completed the survey. The qualification items; used by Fennis and Bakker, 2001); 0 to 6. However, a large proportion of
rate was 75.6 percent. respondents reported using only one drug
• advertising attitude (measured by eight
All sample members received a notifica­ (44.4 percent) on a regular basis and the
7-point Likert scales adapted from
tion e-mail containing a link to the survey mean score was 1.18 (SD = 0.99).
research on global attitude toward adver­
site informing them that a new survey was
tising in general; Pollay and Mittal, 1993);
available for them to complete. After three Descriptive Statistics
days, e-mail reminders were sent to all • OTC analgesic use: Before testing the hypotheses, the data
non-responders. Telephone reminder calls ■4*OTC analgesic use on a regular were carefully examined and edited,
were also implemented three to four days basis (measured by a single yes-no and preliminary univariate and bivari­
after the e-mail reminder to generate a suf­ question); ate analyses were performed for statisti­
ficient response. To enhance survey coop­ number of OTC analgesics taken on a cal assumption checking. The descriptive
eration, various financial incentives were regular basis (measured by an open- statistic of summated advertising irrita­
offered to respondents, including special ended question). tion (Cronbach's a = 0.95) revealed the

408 J O U IH L OF HOUERTISIFIG RESEARCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

TABLE1 theoretical considerations were given to the


relationships among advertising irritation,
Over-the-Counter (OTC) Drug Advertising-Avoidance Behaviors
advertising attitude, and advertising avoid­
Behavior Mean (SD) N/A (%) ance, based on the previous literature (Baek
Rip out or discard advertising inserts for OTC analgesics and Morimoto, 2012; Edwards et al., 2002).
2 .9 0 (2.22) 89 (22.7)
before reading a magazine or a newspaper From this process, two competing modi­
Flip past or skip over ads fo r OTC analgesics in a magazine fied models were identified with w hat the
5 .4 3 (1.60) 61 (15.4)
or a newspaper current authors believed was a better fit
than the original proposed model in all fit
Ignore ads fo r OTC analgesics 5 .3 0 (1 .5 5 ) 30 (7.5)
indices (See Table 2).
Switch the TV channel during com m ercials fo r OTC analgesics 3.91 (1.80) 43 (10.9)
The two modified m odels included an
Tune out TV com m ercials fo r OTC analgesics 4 .7 0 (1 .8 1 ) 39 (9.9) added latent exogenous variable, socio­
Switch radio stations during com m ercials fo r OTC analgesics 3 .7 7 (1 .9 1 ) 101 (25.8) economic status (SES). That variable was
observed by two indicators, income and
Tune out radio com m ercials for OTC analgesics 4.47 (1.89) 99 (25.4)
education, which are considered the most
common indicators of consum ers' SES
respondents w ere not particularly irri­ affective advertising-reaction factors, and (e.g., Aaker and Bruzzone, 1985; Schiff-
tated by OTC analgesic advertising in advertising-avoidance behavior. All vari­ m an and Kanuk, 2010). Also, two direct
general (M = 3.99, SD = 1.28). However, ables and intervariable relationships in paths were added:
about a quarter of the sam ple (24.4%) had the hypotheses were incorporated in this
sum m ated advertising irritation scores of conceptual model. To test the hypotheses • between the two cognitive advertising-
>5 on a 7-point scale. sim ultaneously and to test this study's reaction factors (advertising utility and
All respondents reported taking some proposed m odel, the authors conducted advertising skepticism) and advertising
advertising-avoidance actions. The most structural equation modeling (SEM) analy­ attitude;
common advertising-avoidance behaviors sis with LISREL 9.1. Model estimation was • betw een ad v ertisin g irritatio n and
were: performed using the maximum likelihood advertising avoidance, based on previ­
estimation method. ous em pirical studies (e.g., Baek and
• "flipping past or skipping over adver­ The initial output testing the original Morimoto, 2012; Edwards et al., 2002).
tisem ents in a m agazine or a new s­ advertising-avoidance model exhibited a
paper" (M = 5.43, on a 7-point scale); poor fit betw een the original model and The two m odified m odels exhibited a
• "ignoring advertisements" (M = 5.30); the data, y} = 925.19; d f = 128, p = 0.00; reasonably good fit for explaining the
• "tuning out television commercials" (M goodness-of-fit index (GFI) = 0.80; com­ advertising-reaction factors and the pro­
= 4.70); parative fit index (CFI) = 0.91; norm ed cess of their operation leading to advertis­
• "tuning out radio commercials" (M = fit index (NFI) = 0.90; root-mean-square ing avoidance. According to the authors, a
4.47). error of approxim ation (RMSEA) = 0.13 "reasonably good fit" is acceptable under
(See Table 2). Subsequently, modified com­ the SEM analysis approach, which is not
"R ipping out or discarding ad v ertis­ peting models were developed based on a statistical testing and, as a result, often
ing inserts before reading a magazine or theoretical considerations and previous involves researchers' subjective judgments.
a new spaper" (M = 2.90) was the least empirical studies by adding or removing Between the two m odified m odels,
reported advertising-avoidance behavior paths between variables. the second m odified m odel included
(See Table 1). To improve the model, the authors used one additional path: residual covariance
m ultiple criteria for adding and delet­ between advertising utility and advertis­
Model Testing and Improvement ing estimated parameters from the origi­ ing skepticism, which slightly im proved
The c u rre n t p ro p o se d ad v e rtisin g - nal model as recommended by the SEM the model fit.
a v o id a n c e m o d el (See F ig u re 1) method literature (Kline, 2011), including On the basis of the model modification
predicted sequential links am ong cog­ theoretical/empirical justifications, residu­ and fitting results, the current study pro­
n itiv e a d v e rtis in g -re a c tio n facto rs, als, and modification indices. Particular posed the modified model with individual

December 201 5 JDURnRL OF RDUERTISinG RESEARCH 4 0 9


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

TABLE 2
Goodness-of-Fit Indices of Original and Modified Models
Model Model Diagram Fit Stats
Original X 2 = 9 2 5.19
d f = 128
GFI = 0.80
CFI = 0.91
NFI = 0.90
RMSEA = 0.1 25

M o d ifie d l X 2 = 357.02
d f = 125
GFI = 0.91
CFI = 0.97
NFI = 0.9 6
RMSEA = 0.071

M odified2 X 2 = 3 2 6 .5 4
d f = 124
GFI = 0.92
CFI = 0.9 8
NFI = 0.96
RMSEA = 0 .0 6 6

Note: Minimum-fit function yf (nonsignificant value indicates a good model-data fit); GFI = goodness-of-fit index (values close to 0.90 or 0.95 indicate a good fit); CFI = comparativefit index
(values close to 1 indicate a goodfit); NFI = normedfit index (values greater than 0.95 are typically considered a good fit); RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation (values of 0.05 to
0.08 indicate close fit, the smaller the better).

4 1 0 J0URI1HL OFHDUERTISinG RESEARCH December 2015


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

♦All path coefficients, except for the one between age and ad skepticism, are significant at p < 0.05.

Figure 2 A d ve rtisin g -A vo id a n ce M o d e l—Final M odel

path statistics shown as its final improved • SES was found to substantially impact Results revealed:
model (See Figure 2). both advertising utility and advertising
The paths of the modified model pro­ skepticism, indicating that consumers • Respondents were not strongly irritated
vided support for this study's individual with higher SES tend to have higher by OTC drug advertising, given that the
hypotheses as well as the overall concep­ advertising skepticism and lower per­ average score fell around the middle
tual model: ceived advertising utility (support­ point of the measurement scale.
ing H5). The role of age in this model • Nonetheless, all reported taking some
• All (but one) path coefficients were sig­ seemed to be relatively weak, suggest­ advertising-avoidance actions.
nificant in the hypothesized direction. ing just a weak positive relationship to • "Flipping past or skipping over adver­
advertising utility. tisements in a magazine or a news­
• The findings indicated the significant paper" was the most common behavior,
influence of advertising irritation on DISCUSSION followed by "ignoring ads," "tuning out
advertising avoidance both directly and The current study examined advertising television commercials," and "tuning
indirectly through mediation by adver­ avoidance as a negative outcome of adver­ out radio commercials."
tising attitude (supporting HI and H2). tising and how consumer advertising- • The least reported advertising-avoidance
reaction factors influenced the outcome, behavior was "ripping out or discard­
• As hypothesized, advertising utility with focus on advertising irritation. Specif­ ing advertising inserts before reading a
and advertising skepticism (cognitive ically, the study investigated how advertis­ magazine or a newspaper."
advertising-reaction factors) were sig­ ing avoidance was differentially related to
nificantly related to advertising irritation the factors of perceived utility, skepticism, SEM analysis testing the proposed
and advertising attitude and seemed to irritation, and attitude. It also examined advertising-avoidance model and sub­
exert indirect influence on advertising these factors' relationships in the context sequent model modification procedures
avoidance through these relationships of OTC drug advertising, using a compre­ generated results that supported this
(supporting H3 and H4). hensive model testing approach. study's overall conceptual model as well

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 J0UR1L OF HDUERTISII1G RESERRCH 411


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

W h e n c o n s u m e rs a r e m o re ir r it a te d by a n d f e e l Moreover, the final paths of the cur­


rent authors' modified model supported
le s s f a v o r a b le to w a rd a d v e r tis e m e n ts , th e y this study's conceptual model of adver­
tising avoidance. The researchers found
m o re lik e ly w ill a v o id th e m , r e g a r d le s s o f t h e ir substantial direct influence from affective
advertising-reaction factors and indirect
d e m o g r a p h ic a n d p s y c h o lo g ic a l c h a r a c te r is tic s . influence of cognitive advertising-reaction
factors through their relation to affective
advertising-reaction factors.
Between the cognitive and affective
advertising-reaction variables an interest­
as individual hypotheses. The final m odi­ affective factors and advertising avoid­ ing pattern emerged: The path coefficients
fied model showed: ance suggested when consumers are more suggest advertising irritation seems more
irritated by and feel less favorable toward strongly related to advertising skepti­
• relatively older consumers w ith lower advertisements, they more likely will avoid cism, whereas advertising attitude is more
SES likely will perceive higher advertis­ them, regardless of their demographic and strongly related to advertising utility. And,
ing utility and lower skepticism; psychological characteristics. although attitude toward advertising was
• perceived advertising utility is nega­ found to have a significant and direct
tively linked to advertising irritation and Im p lic a tio n s fo r A d v e rtis in g R e s e a rc h impact on advertising avoidance, the influ­
positively to advertising attitude; Of the many potential demographic, pre- ence of advertising irritation seems also as
• advertising skepticism is positively dispositional, and advertising-reaction prominent and direct.
linked to advertising irritation and nega­ influencers of advertising irritation—per­ The authors believe that this finding
tively to advertising attitude; ceived utility and skepticism seem the calls for more attention to advertising irri­
• advertising irritation is, in turn, posi­ most prominent. This indicates the demo­ tation in advertising-effect theory building.
tively linked to advertising avoidance graphic variables significantly related to Specifically, they encourage further empiri­
both directly and indirectly through its advertising irritation in prior research cal research on the relationship between
relation to advertising attitude; and might actually be: advertising irritation and advertising
• advertising attitude is directly and nega­ avoidance and other types of behavioral
tively linked to advertising avoidance. • operating through their relationships responses to advertisements.
to interm ediate perceptual reactions
The findings indicated significant direct linked to advertising irritation (e.g., per­ P r a c tic a l Im p lic a tio n s
influence of both advertising irritation ceived advertising utility or inform a­ The results of the current study, its authors
and advertising attitude (affective factors) tiveness and advertising skepticism), believe, send a clear, straightforward mes­
on advertising avoidance, w hile adver­ rather than... sage to advertising practitioners involved
tising irritation also operated through • ... influencing advertising irritation w ith product categories similar to OTC
its link to advertising attitude. The two directly. drugs:
cognitive factors (perceived utility and
skepticism) seemed to exert indirect influ­ In other words, the authors of the current • Because advertising irritation leads
ence on advertising avoidance through study found that the mixed or conflicting directly to advertising avoidance, adver­
their relationship to advertising irritation evidence of the influence of various demo­ tisers should strive to minimize irrita­
and attitude. graphics on advertising irritation (dem­ tion with their advertisements.
Among demographics, SES emerged as onstrated in prior studies) may have been
the most significant factor exerting influ­ due to the rather fragmented approaches This finding is particularly true for the
ence on advertising responses: Higher SES to examining the role of demographics as types of products sharing the characteristics
was linked to higher advertising skep­ antecedents of advertising irritation, with­ of OTC advertising, such as rational, high-
ticism and lower perceived utility. The out considering more directly linked psy­ involvement, and search products. Other
prom inent direct link betw een the two chological and advertising-reaction factors. types of product advertising also might

412 JOURdHL OF RDUERTISIOG RESERRCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

benefit from this study's findings. However, irritating advertisem ents are perceived that are more emotional, low involvement,
more empirical studies are needed in con­ as possessing low information utility, it and experiential.
texts of different product types. is reasonable to assume irritation could Since prior research has established
As noted earlier, extant research has be m itigated by making advertisem ents product category as a significant influenc­
established that advertising irritation is more informational and useful to the tar­ ing factor on advertising avoidance, the
associated with: get consumers. proposed advertising-avoidance model
should be tested further in other advertis­
• product category; LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH ing contexts, especially for product catego­
• consum er c h a r a c te r is tic s and Like most research, this study has method­ ries vastly different from OTC drugs (e.g.,
perceptions; ological limitations. Thus, the findings and snack foods, financial services, gardening
• level of advertising repetition; suggestions should be interpreted with equipment, etc.).
• message and tactical elements of adver­ caution. Although this study proposed and Finally, although a path analysis using
tisements (De Pelsmacker and Van den tested a comprehensive conceptual model LISREL is considered a useful statisti­
Bergh, 1998; Fennis and Bakker, 2001; explaining advertising avoidance and cal tool for inferring causal relationships
Pasadeos, 1990). influencing factors, not all potential factors among variables, establishing the temporal
were included nor did it address all ques­ sequences among the variables could not
The advertiser has no control over two of tions regarding advertising avoidance. be tested empirically but presumed based
these factors: A dvertising clutter and advertising on theories and previous literature. Thus,
intrusiveness, for exam ple, w ere not caution is advised in interpreting the find­
• the product is the product; measured. ings as true evidence of causal links among
• the consumer controls messaging effects Future research on the role of these the variables.
as an active interpreter, not a passive factors in connection to those investi­ Advertising avoidance is a serious prob­
reactor. gated in this article w ould offer valu ­ lem for advertisers. Despite the aforemen­
able additional insight. In addition, due tioned limitations, the current study offers
Advertising content and repetition, how ­ to the natu re of a survey m ethod, this valuable insight for future research and
ever, can be m anipulated by advertisers to study could capture only w hat respond­ advertising campaign developm ent deci­
mitigate the direct im pact of advertising ents recalled, rather than collecting real­ sions for OTC medicines specifically and
irritation on advertising avoidance. time data. With technological advances in other similar products generally.
In the creation of advertisem ents for social-scientific m ethods, future research Given the financial resources allocated to
products such as OTC m edicines, the may be able to capture data on consumer product advertising, it is hoped the results
potential of advertisem ents to irritate irritatio n and avoidance of advertise­ of this study will stimulate more research
should be assessed diagnostically d u r­ m ents at the m om ent such responses on advertising irritation and advertising
ing the production process (e.g., pretested occur, which should help advance scien­ avoidance across other types of products
for irritation at various stages of creation tists' understanding of the phenomenon. to better understand and improve adver­
before m edia placem ent). Such p retest­ Another limitation is this study meas­ tising effectiveness.
ing will help advertisers avoid message ured and analyzed advertising avoidance
and executional elem ents, w hich trig ­ in general and did not examine situational
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
ger advertising irritation and avoidance. factors such as media content, mood, emo­
Irritability pretesting m ay be especially tions, and time. This limitation calls for Jisu H uh is associate professor a t the School of

im portant for high income and younger future research to include these potentially Journalism and Mass Communication a t the University of

consumers. As suggested by this study's im portant factors in examining advertising Minnesota. She specializes in research about advertising

results, these consumer types m ost likely irritation and avoidance. effects, particularly in the area of direct-to-consumer

w ill avoid advertisem en ts because of The a u th o rs also a c k n o w led g e (DTC) prescription drug advertising. Her other specialties

advertising irritation. th at—although there is value in con­ include the effects of indirect advertising as well as

Another technique for reducing adver­ d u c tin g p ro d u c t-c a te g o ry -s p e c ific credibility and tru st in interactive advertising. Her work

tising irritation pertains to the inverse link research—the findings might not apply to has been published in such journals as Communication

betw een irritation and utility. Given that advertising avoidance for product types Research, International Journal o f Advertising,

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 JOURflflL OF OOUERTISinG RESEARCH 4 1 3


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS?

Journal o f Advertising, Journal o f Advertising Research, Commercials." Journal of Advertising 21,2 (1992): and Consequences of the Perceived Intrusive­
Journal o f Consumer Affairs, and Journal o f Health 1-7. ness of Pop-Up Ads." Journal of Advertising 31,
Communication. 3 (2002): 83-95.
D eL orme, D. E., J. H uh , and L. N. R eid . "Age
D enise E. D eL orme is professor of advertising at the Differences in How Consumers Behave Follow­
F ennis, B. M., and A. B. Barker. "'Stay Tuned—
University of Central Florida. Her research interests ing Exposure to DTC Advertising." Health Com­
We Will Be Back Right After These Messages':
include pharmaceutical advertising and consumer munication 20, 3 (2006): 255-265.
Need to Evaluate Moderates the Transfer of Irri­
health information-seeking behavior. Her work can
tation in Advertising." Journal of Advertising 30,
be found in the Journal o f Advertising, International D eL orme, D. E., J. H uh , and L. N. R eid. "DTC
3 (2001): 15-25.
Journal o f Advertising, Journal o f Current Issues and Advertising Skepticism and the Use and Per­
Research in Advertising, Communication Research, ceived Usefulness of Prescription Drug Infor­
F riestad, M., and P. W right . "The Persuasion
Journal o f Health Communication, Health Marketing mation Sources." Health Marketing Quarterly 26
Knowledge Model: How People Cope With Per­
Quarterly, among other publications. (2009): 293-314.
suasion Attempts." Journal of Consumer Research
21,1 (1994): 1-31.
L eonard N. R eid is professor emeritus of advertising D eLorme, D. E., J. H uh , and L. N. R eid. "Eval­

a t the University o f Georgia and affiliate professor uation, Use, and Usefulness of Prescription
G oodrich , K., S. Z. S chiller , and D. G al-
of mass com munication at Virginia Commonwealth Drug Information Sources among Anglo- and
Hispanic-Americans." Journal of Health Commu­ letta. "Consumer Reactions to Intrusiveness
University. His research on pharmaceutical
nication 15,1 (2010): 18-38. of Online-Video Advertisements: Do Length,
advertising and drug information-seeking behavior
Informativeness, and Humor Help (or Hinder)
has appeared in the Journal o f Advertising Research
D e P elsmacker, P., and J. V an den Bergh . Marketing Outcomes?" Journal of Advertising
and other advertising, marketing, and communication
"Advertising Content and Irritation: A Study Research 55,1 (2015): 37-50.
journals. Reid is a Fellow of the American Academy

of Advertising and a form er editor o f the Journal of


of 226 TV Commercials." Journal of International
Consumer Marketing 10, 4 (1998): 5-27. G ore, P., S. M adhavan, G. M c C lung, and D.
Advertising.
R iley. "Consumer Involvement in Nonprescrip­
D eshpande, A., A. M enon, M. P erri III, and G. tion Medicine Purchase Decisions." Journal of
M. Z inkhan . "Direct-to-Consumer Advertising Health Care Marketing 14, 2 (1994): 16-23.
REFERENCES
and Its Utility in Health Care Decision Making:
A Consumer Perspective." Journal of Health Com­ G reyser, S. "Irritation in Advertising." Journal of
A aker, D. A., and D. E. Bruzzone. "Causes of
munication 9, 6 (2004): 499-513. Advertising Research 13,1 (1973): 3-7.
Irritation in Advertising." journal of Marketing
49, 2 (1985): 47-57.
D iehl, S., B. M ueller, and R. T erlutter. "Con­
G row, J. M., J. S. P ark, and X. H an . "Your Life
sumer Responses Towards Non-Prescription
A aker, D. A., and D. N orris. "Characteristics in Waiting: Symbolic Meanings in Direct-to-
and Prescription Drug Advertising in the US
of TV Commercials Perceived as Informative." Consumer Antidepressant Advertising." Journal
and Germany." International Journal of Advertis­
Journal of Advertising Research 22, 2 (1982): 61-70. of Communication Inquiry 30,2 (2006): 1630-1688.
ing 27,1 (2008): 99-131.

Baek, T. H ., and M. M orimoto . "Stay Away H eeter, C., and B. S. G reenberg. "Profiling the
D ucoffe, R. H . "H ow Consumers Assess the
From Me: Examining the Determinants of Con­ Zappers." Journal of Advertising Research 25, 2
Value of Advertising." Journal of Current Issues
sumer Avoidance of Personalized Advertising." (1985): 15-19.
and Research in Advertising 17,1 (1995): 1-18.
Journal of Advertising 41,1 (2012): 59-76.
Duff, B. R. L., and R. J. F aber. "Missing the H uh , J., D. E. D eL orme, and L. N. R eid. "Scepti­
C ho , C., and H. J. C heon . "Why Do People Mark: Advertising Avoidance and Distractor cism towards DTC Advertising: A Comparative
Avoid Advertising on the Internet." Journal of Devaluation." Journal of Advertising 40, 2 (2011): Study of Korean and White Americans." Interna­
Advertising 33,4 (2004): 89-97. 51-62. tional Journal of Advertising 31,1 (2012): 147-168.

C ronin , J. J., and N. E. M enelly. "Discrimi­ E dwards, S. M., H. L i, and J. L ee. "Forced Expo­ K elly, L., G. K err, and J. D rennan . "Avoid­
nation vs. Avoidance: 'Zipping' of Television sure and Psychological Reactance: Antecedents ance of Advertising in Social Networking Sites:

414 JOURRRL OF RDUERTISIRG RESEARCH D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5


DO CONSUMERS AVOID WATCHING OVER-THE-COUNTER DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS? THEARF.ORG

The Teenage Perspective." journal of Interactive Advertising: Integration of Concepts and Meas­ P rendergast, G., W. C heung , and D. W est.
Advertising 10,2 (2010): 16-27. ures From Marketing and Healthcare." Interna­ "Antecedents to Advertising Avoidance in
tional Journal of Advertising 23 (2004): 91-118. China." Journal of Current Issues and Research in
K im, W. An Exploratory Investigation of Consum­ Advertising 32, 2 (2010): 87-100.
ers' Pre-Purchase External Information Search for M orimoto, M., and S. C hang . "Consumer Atti­
Prescription and Non-Prescription Drugs. (Unpub­ tudes Toward Unsolicited Commercial E-mail R atchford, B. T. "New Insights About the FCB

lished doctoral dissertation), Athens, GA: Uni­ and Postal Direct Mail Marketing Methods: Grid." Journal of Advertising Research 27,4 (1987):

versity of Georgia, 2005. Intrusiveness, Perceived Loss of Control, and 24-38.


Irritation." Journal of Interactive Advertising 7, 1
R eid, L. N., and L. C. Soley. "Generalized and
K im, W. J., and K. W. K ing . "Product Category (2006): 8-20.
Personalized Attitudes Toward Advertising's
Effects on External Search for Prescription and
Social and Economic Effects." Journal of Adver­
Non-Prescription Drugs." Journal of Advertising N yilasy, G., and L. N . R eid . "Agency Practi­
tising 11, 3 (1982): 3-7.
38,1 (2009): 5-19. tioner Theories of How Advertising Works."
Journal of Advertising 38,3 (2009): 81-96.
R ojas-M endez , J. I., and G. D avies. "Avoid­
K ing , K. W., L. N. R eid, S. F. T inkham , and J.
ing Television Advertising: Some Explanations
P okrywczynski. "The Perceived Informativeness O bermiller, C., and E. R. S pangenberg. "Devel­
From Time Allocation Theory." Journal of Adver­
of National and Retail Advertising." Current opment of a Scale to Measure Consumer Skepti­
tising Research 45, 1 (2005): 34-48.
Issues and Research in Advertising 10, 1-2 (1987): cism Toward Advertising." Journal of Consumer
173-197. Psychology 7,2 (1998): 159-186. R ojas-M endez, J. I., G. D avies, and C. M adran .
"Universal Differences in Advertising Avoid­
K line, R. B. Principles and Practice of Structural O bermiller, C., and E. R. S pangenberg. "On the ance Behavior: A Cross-Cultural Study." Journal
Equation Modeling, 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guil­ Origin and Distinctness of Skepticism Toward of Business Research 62 (2009): 947-954.
ford Press, 2011. Advertising." Marketing Letters 11, 4 (2000):
311-322. Schiffman , L. G., and L. L. K anuk . Consumer
K rugman, D. M., and K. F. Johnson . "Difference Behavior, 10th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pren­
in the Consumption of Traditional Broadcast O bermiller, C., E. R. S pangenberg , and D. L. tice Hall, 2010.
and VCR Movie Rental." journal of Broadcasting M ac L achlan . "Ad Skepticism: The Conse­
and Electronic Media 35, 2 (1991): 213-232. quences of Disbelief." Journal of Advertising 34, S havitt, S., P. Lowrey, and J. H aefner. "Public

3 (2005): 7-17. Attitudes Toward Advertising: More Favorable

K rugman , D. M., G. T. C ameron , and C. M. Than You Might Think." Journal of Advertising

W hite. "Visual Attention to Programming and O 'D o nohoe , S. "Attitudes to Advertising: Research 38,4 (1998): 7-22.

Commercials: The Use of In-Home Observa­ Review of British and American Research."
Soley, L. C., and L. N. R eid . "Satisfaction With
tions." Journal of Advertising 24,1 (1995): 1-12. International Journal of Advertising 14, 3 (1995):
the Informational Value of Magazine and Tele­
245-261.
vision Advertising." Journal of Advertising 12, 3
Lee, S., and J. R. L umpkin . "Differences in Atti­
(1983): 27-31.
tude Towards TV Advertising: VCR Usage as a O 'K eefe, G. J., K. N ash, and J. Liu. "The Per­
Moderator." International Journal of Advertising ceived Utility of Advertising." Journalism Quar­
S peck, P. S., and M. T. E lliott. "Predictors of
11, 4 (1992): 333-342. terly 58,4 (1981): 535-542.
Advertising Avoidance in Print and Broadcast
Media." Journal of Advertising 26,3 (1997): 61-76.
Li, H., S. M. E dwards, and J. Lee. "Measur­ P asadeos, Y. "Perceived Informativeness of and
ing the Intrusiveness of Advertisements: Scale Irritation With Local Advertising." Journalism Stout, P. A., and B. A. Burda . "Zipped Com­
Development and Validation." Journal of Adver­ Quarterly 67,1 (1990): 35-39. mercials: Are They Effective?" Journal of Adver­
tising 31,2 (2002): 37-47. tising 18, 4 (1989): 23-32.
P ollay, R. W., and B. M ittal. "Here's the Beef:
M en o n , A. M.; A. D. D eshpande , G. M. Factors, Determinants, and Segments in Con­ V akratsas, D., and T. A mbler. "H ow Advertis­
Z inkhan , and M. P erri. "A Model Assess­ sumer Criticism of Advertising." Journal of Mar­ ing Works: What Do We Really Know?" Journal
ing the Effectiveness of Direct-to-Consumer keting 57, 3 (1993): 99-114. of Marketing 63,1 (1999): 26-43.

D e ce m b e r 2 0 1 5 JDURF1RL OF RDUERTISIRG RESEARCH 415


Copyright of Journal of Advertising Research is the property of Warc LTD and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

S-ar putea să vă placă și