Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

Freshwater Biology (1995) 34, 21-28

The reaction time of leech and triclad species to


crushed prey and the significance of this for their
coexistence in British lakes
R . M . H . SEABY, A.J. MARTIN A N D J . O . Y O U N G *
Department of Environmental and Evolutionary Biology, University of Liverpool, PO Box 147, Liverpool L69 3BX. U.K.

^Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.

SUMMARY

1. Three species of leeches, Erpobdella octoculata, Glossiphonia complanata and Helobdella


stagnalis, and four species of triclads, Polycelis nigra, P. tenuis, Dugesia polychroa and
Dendrocoelum lacteum, commonly coexist on stony shores in productive British lakes. All
species are food limited and there is much overlap in their diet. For both leech and
triclad communities, coexistence of species is through the occurrence of food refuges.
Leeches are more successful than triclads at capturing live prey, whereas both groups
feed on damaged prey, comprising incapacitated, live or dead animals that are leaking
body fluids. If triclads are better than leeches at exploiting damaged prey, this could be
a mechanism for their coexistence.
2. Laboratory experiments investigated the comparative speeds al which leeches and
triclads responded to crushed prey. Young and adult predators were offered a crushed
specimen of the oligochaete Tubifex tubifex, the snail Lymnaea peregra, the crustacean
Asellus aquaticus or the chironomid Chironomus sp., and their reaction times recorded.
These four prey groups constitute the main diet of the predators in the field. Only D.
polychroa and D. lacteum showed a significantly different reaction time between young
and adults to crushed prey, and the reason for this is unclear. All predators, except H.
stagnalis and D. polychroa, showed a difference in reaction time to the four types of prey,
presumably a consequence of differences in both the 'quality' and 'concentration' of the
different prey fluids, and there were some differences between predators in their speed
of reaction to the same prey type. The following sequence, from fastest to slowest, in
general reaction time to prey was obtained: £. octoculata, D. polychroa, P. tenuis, D.
lacteum, P. nigra, H. stagnalis and G. complanata.
3. The location of the damaged food by the predators can be explained partly in terms
of their foraging behaviour, with E. octoculata, D. polychroa and P. tenuis exhibiting a
more seek-out strategy than other species which have a more sit-and-wait behaviour,
and partly on the leveJ of sophistication of their chemosensory system used to detect
leaked prey fluids. This system is highly developed in triclad species but poorly
developed in leeches.
4. In a second type of experiment in which prey, L. peregra, A. aquaticus or Chironomus
sp., were offered at different time intervals after crushing to H. stagnalis and P. tenuis,
few predators fed on food crushed for 24 h or longer, although a few leeches fed on
Chironomus crushed for up to 72 h.
5. It is concluded that coexistence of leech and triclad species on stony shores in lakes is
assisted by partitioning of food on a damaged or live basis.
© 1995 Blackwell Science Ltd 21
22 R.M.H. Seaby, A.J. Martin and J.O. Young

Introduction Material and methods

Experimental procedure
The most numerous invertebrate predators inhabiting
the stony littoral zone of British productive lakes Adults of the leeches E. octoculata (17-18 mm body
comprise a guild of leech and triclad species (Reynold- length at rest; 30-32 mg wet weight), G. complanata
son, 1966; Young & Ironmonger, 1981). The common (12-13 mm; 20-22 mg) and H. stagnalis (8-9 mm; 9-
species include the leeches Erpobdella octoculata (L.), 10 mg) and the triclads P. nigra (7-8 mm body length
Glossiphonia complanata (L.) and Helobdella stagnalis (L.), in gliding movement; 6-7 mg wet weight), P. tenuis
and the triclads Polycelis nigra (Mull), Polycelis tenuis (7-8 mm; 6-7 mg), D. polychroa (9-10 mm; 8-9 mg) and
Ijima, Dugesia polychroa (O. Schmidt) and Dendrocoelum D. lacteum (14-16 mm; 23-26 mg) were collected from
lacteum (Mull.). Populations of all these species are Crose Mere, an ion-rich, productive lake in Shropshire
food limited (Reynoldson, 1983; Martin, Seaby & (Nat. Grid Ref. SJ 430 305). Some specimens of each
Young, 1994a), and field studies have demonstrated species were starved for 10 days prior to their use in
much overlap in their diets, which include oligo- experiments. Others were cultured in the laboratory,
chaetes, molluscs, crustaceans and insects (Reynoldson at 14 °C using a natural photoperiod, to produce
& Davies, 1970; Reynoldson, 1975; Young, 1981; Young young which were used in experiments soon after
& Spelling, 1989). However, within each of the triclad hatching from cocoons (E. octoculata and triclad
and leech communities, each genus has a food refuge, species) or release from parents (glossiphoniid
that is, a prey taxon on which it feeds to a greater leeches).
extent than do the other genera (Reynoldson & Davies, Oligochaetes, molluscs, crustaceans and chironom-
1970), and this allows their coexistence (Reynoldson, ids constitute the major components in the diet of the
1966, 1983; Young & Spelling, 1989). The question predators in the field (Reynoldson & Davies, 1970;
arises as to how leech and triclad species can coexist, Young, 1981; Young & Spelling, 1989). Accordingly,
given the considerable overlap in their diets; for the four prey species used in experiments were the
example, both D. polychroa and G. complanata feed oligochaete, Tubifex tubifex (Mull.) (20-22 mm body
extensively on molluscs. length), the gastropod mollusc Lymnaea peregra (Mull.)
(10-12 mm), the isopod crustacean Asellus aquaticus
Laboratory studies have shown that leeches are far
(Linn.) (10-12 mm) and the chironomid Chironomus
more successful than triclads at feeding on intact live
sp. (10-12 mm). The first and last species were obtained
prey organisms, but that both feed on damaged prey from an aquarium shop in Liverpool, and the others
comprising incapacitated live or dead animals that from ponds on Merseyside. All experiments were
are leaking body fluids (Reynoldson & Young, 1963; conducted at 14 °C, an optimum temperature for all
Martin, Seaby & Young, 1994b). If triclads are more the predator species (Reynoldson, Young & Taylor,
successful in locating and exploiting damaged prey 1965; Young & Ironmonger, 1982), under natural light
than leeches, then coexistence might be permitted conditions.
through the division of the food niche in terms of Crystallizing dishes of 7 cm diameter and 200 ml
damaged or live prey. Unfortunately, field studies on capacity were three-quarters filled with lake water
diet only identify the type of prey eaten and not its from Crose Mere. A single predator was placed in
condition. each dish and allowed to acclimatize for 30 min.
Little is known about the comparative ability of Almost always, the predator came to rest at the edge
leeches and triclads to detect and react to damaged of the dish in the angle of the wall and the floor. If it
prey, and the present work was designed to answer did not, the dish was discarded. An immobilized prey
the following questions. Does the type of prey and individual, crushed by a single uniform squeeze using
age of the predator (leech or triclad species) affect the a forceps, was introduced gently to the centre of the
predator's reaction time to crushed food? Is there a floor of the dish, avoiding physical disturbcince of the
difference in reaction time to a particular food between water. The time taken for the predator to find and
different predator species? Does the length of time a start feeding on the prey was recorded in seconds.
prey organism has been dead have an effect on the Observations were continued for up to 1 h. For each
feeding behaviour of the predator? predator and prey combination, thirty replicates were
© 1995 Blackwell Science Ltd, Freshwater Biology, 34, 21-28
Leeches and triclads 23
Table 1 The mean reaction times (s), with standard deviations in parentheses, of adult and young leeches and triclads to various
crushed prey species, n = 30, except for Dendrocoelum lacleum for which n = 20. Because only two adull and two young D. lacteum
fed on Lymnaea peregra, mean times are not shown. Young Polycelis nigra were not used.
Eo = Erpobdella octoculata; Gc = Glossiphonia complanata; Hs = Helobdella stagnalis; Pn = Polycetis nigra; Pt = Polycelis tenuis; Dp =
Dugesia polychroa; Dl = Dendrocoelum lacteum; Tt = Tubifex tubifex; Lp = Lymnaea peregra; Aa = Asellus aquaticus; Ch = Chironomus
species

Predator species

Prey
species Eo Gc Hs Pn Pt Dp Dl

Adults
Tt 156.67 404.53 ,195.00 191.13 177,57 163.47 185.10
(65.16) (274.99) (275.90) (104,23) (103,39) (115-10) (111.27)
Lp 315.07 331.73 379,33 612,63 741.13 158.83 -
(143.74) (195.13) (226-20) (899.67) (1056.52) (98.78) -
Aa 175.77 499.43 443.70 183.07 164.27 169,23 166.80
(72.64) (336-90) (384.93) (106.90) (102.22) (122.75) (104.83)
Ch 141.73 460.53 431.73 186.73 167.10 187.10 228.80
(68.66) (297-02) (269.19) (162.60) (94.92) (123.96) (85.79)

Young
Tt 161.77 435.10 435.57 - 194,97 212.13 223.30
(69.18) (319.05) (349.51) - (100.78) (135.35) (109.54)
Lp 359.63 399.77 438.60 - 883.30 182.40 -
(147.29) (260.86) (285.86) (1198.95) (102.13) -
M 193.17 552.67 507.10 - 196.47 203.37 218.15
(67-88) (374.69) (407.47) - (110.10) (120.09) (117.54)
150,40 510.10 505.38 - 221.40 211.40 292.85
(59,60) (346.13) (335,18) - (128.68) (138.69) (79.27)

used with the exception of D. lacteum where there to age of predator and prey type. When a significant
were only twenty. Young P. nigra could not be tested difference was obtained a Tukey least significant differ-
as insufficient numbers were obtained. ence test was performed (Zar, 1984). To test for differ-
In experiments which investigated the attraction of ences between predator species in reaction times to
decaying crushed food, a single crushed prey was each of the four prey types, a one-way ANOVA was
introduced to the centre of the floor of each of 105 carried out for each food type followed by a Tukey
dishes of 7 cm diameter and 200 ml capacity and three- test where results were significantly different. Data for
quarters filled with lake water. The body fluids of the young and adult predators were analysed separately.
crushed prey had diffused throughout the experi- Results from the experiment investigating the effect
mental arenas in less than 30 min, as witnessed by the of decaying food on predator response were analysed
leaking of red haemoglobin from Chironomus. The using the chi-squared test. For each food type, compar-
dishes were divided into seven groups of fifteen. Each ison between the two predatory species was made at
group was left for either 30 min or 3, 5, 16, 24, 48 or each time interval.
72 h before a single predator was introduced, using a
spatula, into each dish at its edge in the angle of the
Results
wall and the floor. The predator was then observed
for 30 min to see if it fed. The experiment was repeated V^itJtin-species reaction times
for each of the prey types L. peregra, A. aquaticus and
Chironomus sp., and each of the predators H. stagnalis Erpobdella octoculata reacted significantly more slowly
and P. tenuis. Only adult predators were used. to L. peregra than to the other prey types which did
not differ from each other {Tables 1 and 2). There was
no difference between ages. For G. comptanata, there
Analysis of data
was no significant difference between ages. Its
A two-way ANOVA was used to test for differences response times to L. peregra (fast) and A. aquaticus
in reaction time within a predator species with regard (slow) were significantly different, with intermediate
© 1995 Blackweil Science Ltd, Freshwater Biology, 34, 21-28
24 R.M.H. Seaby, A.]. Martin and J.O. Young
Table 2 Results of two-way ANOVAs
Predator Age of predator Prey type to test for differences in reaction times
Significance level Significance level Tukey test prey groupings within a predator species to four
different prey types, with regard to age
Eo NS < 0.001 Tt Aa Ch of predator and prey type. Tukey test
Ge results also show significantly different
m <0.05
NS
Tl Iv Ch TtAaCh
groupings of prey types. Young
m

m < 0.001 Tt Aa Ch Polycelis nigra were not used and so
Pt
-NS < 0.001 Tt Aa Ch few Lymnaea peregra were eaten by
<0.05 NS Dendrocoelum lacteum that data are not
Dl <0.01 <0.01 TtAa Ch included in calculations. Predator and
prey abbreviations as in Table 1

Table 3 Tukey test results showing significantly different of the four prey types, T. tuhifex, A. aquaticus and
groupings of predators for each of four prey types. A, adult
predators; Y, young predators. Predator and prey Chironomus sp. (Tables 1 and 3). Glossiphonia complanata
abbreviations as for Table 1 and H. stagnalis were significantly slower to find these
food types than were the other predators between
Prey
Species Predator species which there were no significant differences. Dugesia
polychroa reacted faster to L. peregra and was signific-
Tt A Eo Pn Pt DpDl GcHs antly different to both P. nigra and P. tenuis, which
Y Eo Pt Dv Dl GcHs
Aa A Eo Pn Pt DvDl Gc Hs were the slowest, and to an intermediate group com-
Y Eo Pt Dp Dl GcHs prising the three leech species. Dendrocoelum lacteum,
Ch A Eo Pn Pt DpDl GcHs which fed sparingly on L peregra, was not included
Y Eo Pt Dv Dl GcHs
Lp A in the analysis.
Eo Gc Hs PnPt
Y Eo Gc Hs Pt Bearing in mind that young P. nigra were not used
in experiments, the results for the young predators
were similar to those for the adults. No young D. lac-
reaction times to Chironomus and T. tubifex, which teum fed on L. peregra in the experiments.
were inseparable from either L. peregra or A. aquaticus. When the predators were ranked in order of reaction
HeJobdella stagnalis showed no significant differences
time to each prey type and then these ranks summed
between reaction times for age or food type. The
for each predator species, the following sequence of
response time of P. nigra to L peregra was significantly
predators, in terms of increasing reaction time, for
slower than to other prey types between which there
both adults and young, was obtained: £. octoculata, D.
was no difference. For P. tenuis there was no significant
polychroa, P. tenuis, D. lacteum, P. nigra, H. stagnalis and
difference with respect to age. Reaction time to L
G. complanata (Table 4).
peregra was significantly slower than to the other food
types which showed similar reaction times. Dugesia
polychroa showeci no difference in reaction time to any
Response to decaying food
prey type but young were sigruficantiy slower than
adults to feed. Young D. lacteum reacted significantly In experiments investigating predator response to
slower than adults to prey. For both age-groups, decaying crushed prey, the number of predators, for
response times to Chironomus were slower than to T. both H. stagnalis and P. tenuis, which fed within a 30-
tubifex and A. aquaticus. So few L. peregra were attacked min period on A. aquaticus that had been crushed and
that they were not included in the analysis. had decayed for various time intervals, remained fairly
similar up to 16 h after crushing (Fig, 1). At 24 h,
numbers declined dramatically, with none feeding at
Between-species reaction times
48 h. At none of the time intervals was there a signific-
For both young and adults, significantly different ant difference between the numbers of H. stagnalis
reaction times between predator species to each of the and P. tenuis feeding. When exposed to L peregra, the
four prey types were obtained, and so, in the interest numbers of leeches and triclads which had fed
of space, orJy Tukey test results are shown in Table 3. declined gradually with only a single P. tenuis feeding
Adult reaction times were very similar for three out on L peregra which had been crushed 48 h earlier.
© 1995 Blackweil Science Ltd, Freshwater Biology, 34, 21-28
Leeches and triclads 25
Table 4 The ranks of the leech and triclad reaction times (cf.
15
Table 1) and the sums of the ranks for all prey types, for adult
and young predators. Young Polycelis nigra were not tested. (a)
Predator and prey abbreviations as for Table 1
10
Predator species

Prey species Eo Dp Pt Dl Pn Hs Gc

Adults
Tt 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Lp 2 1 6 7 5 4 3
Aa 4 3 1 2 5 6 7 15
Ch 1 4 2 5 3 6 7
Sum 8 10 12 18 18 22 24 (b)

Young
Tt 1 3 2 4 - 6 5
Lp 2 1 5 6 - 4 3
Aa 1 3 2 4 - 5 6
Ch 1 2 3 4 - 5 6
Sum 5 9 12 18 - 20 20

Again, there were no significant differences between 15


the number of H. stagnalis and P. tenuis feeding at any (C)
of the time intervals.
When exposed to Chironomus, H. stagnalis fed on 10

food which had been left for a longer period of time


after crushing than did P. tenuis which ceased feeding
on prey after it had been crushed longer than 24 h.
One leech fed when exposed to a chironomid which
had been crushed for 72 h. At 24 h, and subsequent 72
time intervals, significantly more H. stagnalis fed than
did P. tenuis. Fig. 1 The number of predators, adult Polycelis tenuis {Pt) or
Helobdella stagnalis (Hs), which had fed within a 30-min period
on crushed prey, Asellus aquaticus (a), Lymnaea peregra (b) or
Discussion Chironomus sp. (c), which had been dead for various lengths of
time. « = 15 for each predator-prey combination.
In the present experiments, for both young and adult
animals, £. octoculata and all the triclad species reacted Martin & Seaby, 1993). Of the flatworm species, P.
relatively more quickly to the presence of crushed tenuis and D. polychroa have been shown to be more
food than did the two glossiphoniid leeches. The active predators than D. lacteum and P. nigra (Reynold-
location of food by the predators will depend on son & Young, 1963; Bellamy & Reynoldson, 1974;
their foraging strategy and their ability to detect Calow & Wooilhead, 1977; Calow, 1980; Calow & Riley,
the presence of prey. Schoener (1971) suggested two 1980; Reynoldson, Gilliam & Jaques, 1981; Herrmann,
extreme types of foraging behaviour, namely, 'sit- 1984). Thus, differences in feeding strategy may con-
and-wait' and 'search-out'. In common with other tribute in part to an explanation of the present findings.
erpobdellid leeches, £. octoculata is an active, although The ability to detect the presence of prey will depend
clumsy, hunter, and the only predator tested in the on the level of sophistication of the predator's sensory
current experiments which is capable of swimming system. Distance of predator from food source, an
(Greene, 1974; Davies et al, 1982; Anholt & Davies, important component of detection, was standardized
1986; Yoimg & Spelling, 1989), whereas the glossi- in the present experiments. However, when actively
phoniid leeches, G. compJanata more so than H. stag- searching for food E. octoculata can greatly extend its
nalis, have a relatively more sit-and-wait strategy resting body length, and its large size, relative to the
(Wrona & Calow, 1988; Young & Spelling, 1989; Young, other predators, may have resulted in artificially fast
© 1995 Blackweil Science Ltd, Freshwater Biotogy, 34, 21-28
26 R.M.H. Seaby, A.j. Martin and j.O. Young

reaction times being obtained for this leech, as it of reaction to different prey Also, there were some
would, by chance alone, be more likely to make contact differences between predators in their speed of reac-
with a food item in the small experimental arenas tion to the same prey type, which is of greater relevance
used. Certainly, this should be borne in mind in the to the question of coexistence. The speed of arousal
interpretation of the results. to prey by triclads, and discrimination between prey
Triclad species are well endowed with chemorecep- by both leeches and triclads would be governed by
tors by means of which food emitting body fluids both the 'quality' and 'concentration' of the prey
are readily detected (Hyman, 1951). These, although fluids. Both of these attributes are difficult to measure,
scattered over the entire body, are particularly concen- and this was not attempted in the present study.
trated in the anterior auricular grooves. The presence Lymnaea peregra was least 'attractive' to the predators,
of intact live prey is detected either by direct contact with the exception of G. complanata, H. stagnalis and
with chemoreceptors or by the disturbances created D. polychroa. Interestingly, these predators, particulariy
in the water by the prey. Ciliary tactile receptors G. complanata and D. polychroa, have a large component
are distributed over the entire body with particular of snails in their diet in the field (Reynoldson &
concentrations on the body margins and auricles. In Davies, 1970; Young, 1981; Young & Proctor, 1986;
contrast to triclads, chemosensory cues play a much Young & Spelling, 1989). Because the snails were
lesser role in prey detection by leeches, with chemosen- crushed in the present experiments, the presence of
sory ability mainly restricted to situations when the a shell and htehavioural defences such as shaking
animal is in physical contact with its prey (Davies (Townsend & McCarthy, 1980; Bronmark & Malmquist,
et al, 1982; Sawyer, 1986). Erpobdelk montezuma Davies, 1986; Brown & Strouse, 1988) could not have been
Singhal & Davies, a predaceous leech that undergoes involved in deterring attacks by the other predators.
a twilight migration into the surface waters of However, it is possible that mucous production,
Montezuma Well, Arizona, uses mechanoreception to evident during the crushing process, may have been
forage on pelagic amphipods (Blinn, Wagner & Grin, implicated. The present findings are in broad agree-
1986; Blinn, Pinney & Wagner, 1988). However, most ment with those of Bellamy & Reynoldson (1974)
erpobdellid leeches appear to encounter prey by who observed the response of triclads to prey juices
chance but once contact has been made tactile and/ dropped into experimental arenas; the fluid of the
or chemoreceptors are important in the selection and snail Potamopyrgus jenkinsi (Smith) was least attractive
handling of the prey (Greene, 1974; Davies etal, 1982; to the triclads.
Blinn & Davies, 1989). Thus, differences in the sensory
The concentration of leaked fluids from a crushed
systems used to detect prey between leeches and
prey organism will diminish with time and the stimu-
triclads would also contribute to an explanation of the
lus for predation using chemosensory cues will be
present experimental findings. Because the offered
reduced. Also, the process of tissue decay will com-
food comprised recently crushed prey which leaked
mence on death and the palatability of the food may
fluids, the triclads would be at an advantage, locating
change. Certainly, in the present experiments, the
prey in advance of leeches. However, as stated above,
number of leeches and triclads feeding on crushed
the fast response time of the erpobdellid was most
prey declined with time, with few feeding on prey
likely due to a combination of its active hunting
which had been killed for 24 h or longer. Exceptionally,
strategy and large size relative to the size of the
H- stagnalis fed on chironomids which had been
experimental containers used.
crushed for up to 72 h, and reasons for this can only
For each predator species, reaction times of young be speculative.
and adult animals were similar, with the exceptions It is concluded that differences in ability to capture
of D. polychroa and D, lacteum for which young were live prey, foraging strategies, and in the detecfion of
sigruficantiy slower. The reason for this is unclear, but damaged prey will contribute to the coexistence of
the presence of fewer sensory cells in and/or the lake-dwelling leeches and triclads. Leeches are more
reduced locomotory ability of the smaller animals may efficient than triclads at catching live prey due to the
be implicated. possession of suckers and in the case of £. octoculata
For each predator, with the exception of H. stagnalis to its macrophagous feeding habit (Reynoldson &
and D. polychroa, there were differences in the speed Young, 1963; Martin, Seaby & Young, 1994b). Triclads
© 1995 Blackwell Science Ltd, Freshwater Biology, 34, 21-28
Leeches and triclads 27
lack suckers and find live prey, with the excepfion of through mechanoreception. Canadian Journal of Zoology,
less vigorous, soft-bodied prey such as oligochaetes, 66, 427-430.
difficult to catch. Exceptionally, D. lacteum with its Blinn D.W., Wagner W.T & Grin J.N. (1986) Surface
anterior 'pseudosucker' and through the production sensilla on the predaceous leech Erpobdella montezuma:
of mucous traps is able to capture live prey such as possible importance in feeding. Transactions of the Amer-
Asellus (Reynoldson & Young, 1963; de Silva, 1976). ican Microscopical Society, 105, 21-30.
Bronmark C. & Malmqvist B. (1986) Interactions between
The glossiphoniid leeches, parficularly G. complanata,
the leech Glossiphonia complanata and its gastropod
and 0. lacteum exhibit a more sit-and-wait feeding
prey Oecologia, 69, 268-276.
strategy than E. octoculata and the other triclad species,
Brown K.M. & Strouse B.H, (1988) Relative vulnerability
capturing active prey which arrive in their vicinity
of six freshwater gastropods to the leech Nephelopsis
and presumably reducing the energy costs of active
obscura (Verrill), Freshwater Biology, 19,157-165.
foraging.
Calow P. (1980) Feeding in freshwater triclads -
Triclads can exploit damaged prey, including incapa- adaptational aspects. Nutrition in the Lower Metazoa
citated, live or recently dead animals that are leaking (eds D.C. Smith and Y. Tiffon), pp. 15-31. Pergamon
body fluids, better than leeches. The fast reaction times Press, Oxford.
of triclads to damaged prey can be explained in terms Calow P. & Riley H. (1980) Leeches: an adaptational
of a highly developed chemosensory system and their approach to their ecology and physiology. Journal of
Biological Education, 14, 279-289.
active foraging strategy, parficularly with regard to
Calow P. & Wooilhead A.S. (1977) Locomotory strategies
the dugesiid and planariid species. Leeches have a
in freshwater triclads and their effects on the energetics
poorly developed chemosensory system and this, of degrowth. Oecologia, 27, 353-362.
together with the more sit-and-wait feeding strategy Davies R.W., Linton L.R., Parsons W. & Edgington E.S.
of the glossiphoniids, results in a lesser ability to (1982) Chemosensory detection of prey by Nephelopsis
utilize damaged prey. The fast reacfion time of £. obscura (Hirudinoidea: Erpobdellidae) Hydrobiologia, 9,
octoculata in the present experiments was due to its 157-161.
acfive, although random, foraging strategy. Greene K.L. (1974) Experiments and observations on the
feeding behaviour of the freshwater leech Erpobdella
octoculata (L-) (Hirudinea: Erpobdellidae). Archiv fur
Acknowledgments Hydrobiologie, 74, 87-99.
Herrmann J. (1984) Prey choice and its impact on the
Thanks are expressed to the Natural Environment reproductive output in Dendrocoelum lacteum
Research Council for financing this work, to Professor (Turbellaria). Archiv fur Hydrobiologie, 100, 355-370.
T. B. Reynoldson and an anonymous referee for helpful Hyman L,H. (1951) The Invertebrates: Platyhelminthes and
comments on the text, and to the owners of Crose Rhynchocoela, the Acoelomate Bilateria, Vol. 2. McGraw
Mere and English Nature, Shrewsbury for permission Hill, New York.
to sample leeches and triclads. MarHn A.J., Seaby R.M.H. & Young J.O. (1994a) Food
limitation in lake-dwelling leeches: field experiments.
journal of Animal Ecology, 63, 93-100.
References Martin A.J., Seaby R.M.H. & Young J.O. (1994b) Does body
size difference in the leeches Glossiphonia complanata (L.)
Anholt B.R. & Davies R.W. (1986) Effect of hunger level and Helobdella stagnalis (L.) contribute to coexistence?
on the activity of the predatory leech Nephlopsis obscura Hydrobiologia, 273, 67-75.
Verrill (Hirudinoidea: Erpobdellidae) American Midland Reynoldson T.B. (1966) The distribution and abundance
Naturalist, 117, 307-311. of lake-dwelling triclad s-towards a hypothesis.
Bellamy L-S. & Reynotdson TB. (1974) Behaviour in Advances in Ecological Research, 3, 1-71.
competition for food amongst lake-dwelling triclads. Reynoldson T.B. (1975) Food overlap of lake-dwelling
Oikos, 25, 356-364. triclads in the field, journal of Animal Ecology, 44,
Blinn D.W. & Davies R.W. (1989) The evolutionary 245-250.
importance of mechanoreception in three erpobdellid Reynoldson T.B. (1983) The population biology of
leech species. Oecologia, 79, 6-9. Turbellaria with special reference to the freshwater
Blinn D.W., Pinney C- & Wagner V.T (1988) Intraspecific triclads of the British Isles. Advances in Ecological
discrimination of amphipod prey by a freshwater leech Research. 13, 235-326.

1995 Blackwell Science Ltd, Freshivater Biology, 34, 21-28


28 R.M.H. Seaby, A.j. Martin and j.O. Young
Reynoldson T.B. & Davies R.W. (1970) Food niche and Young J.O. (1981) A comparative study of the food niches
coexistence in lake-dwelling triclads. journal of Animal of lake-dwelling triclads and leeches. Developments in
Ecology, 39, 599-617. Hydrobiology, 6, 91-102.
Reynoldson TB., Gilliam J.R & Jaques R.M. (1981) Young J.O. & Ironmonger J.W. (1981) A quantitafive study
Competitive exclusion and co- existence in natural of the comparative distribution of non-parasitic leeches
populations of Polycelis nigra and P. tenuis (Tricladida, and triclads in the stony littoral of British lakes.
Turbellaria). Archiv fiir Hydrobiologie, 92, 71-113. Internationale Revue der Gesampten Hydrobiologie, 66,
Reynoldson T-B. & Young J.O. (1963) The food of four 847-862.
species of lake-dwelling triclads. journal of Animal Young J.O. & Ironmonger J.W. (1982) The influence of
Ecology, yi, 175-191. temperature on the life cycle and occurrence of three
Reynoldson T.B., Young J.O. & Taylor M.C. (1965) The species of lake-dwelling leeches (Annelida: Hirudinea)
effect of temperature on the life-cycle of four species Journal of Zoology, London, 196, 519-543.
of lake-dwelling triclads. Journal of Animal Ecology, 34, Young J.O., Martin A.J. & Seaby R.M.H. (1993)
23-43. Competitive interactions between the lake-dwelling
Sawyer R-T. (1986) Leech Biology and Behaviour, Vols 1 and leeches Glossiphonia complanata and Helobdella stagnalis:
2. Clarendon Press, Oxford. an experimental investigafion of a food refuge.
Schoener T.W. (1971) Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Oecologia, 93, 156-161.
Review of Ecology and Systematics, 2, 369-404. Young J.O. & Proctor R.M. (1986) Are the lake-dwelling
Silva de P.K. (1976) The importance of mucus of leeches, Glossiphonia complanata (L-) and Helobdella
Dendrocoelum lacteum (Muller) (Turbellaria, Tricladida) stagnalis (L.), opportunistic predators on molluscs and
in community existence. Archiv fur Hydrobiologie, 78, do they partition this food resource? Freshwater Biology,
363-374. 16, 465- 477.
Townsend C.R. & McCarthy T.K. (1980) On the defence Young J.O. & Spelling S.M. (1989) Food utilizafion and
strategy of Physa fontanalis (L.), a freshwater snail. niche overlap in three species of lake-dwelling leeches
Oecologia, 46, 75-79. (Hirudinea). journal of Zoology, London, 219, 231-243.
Wrona. FJ. & Calow P (1988) Optimal feeding in a Zar J.H. (1984) Biostatistical Analysis. PrenHce-Hall, Inc.,
sit and wait predator Albogiossiphonia heteroclita (L.) New Jersey.
(Hirudinoidea: Glossiphoniidae). Functional Ecology, 2,
171-175. (Manuscript accepted 15 January 1995)

1995 Blackwell Science Ltd, Freshwater Biology, 34, 21-28

S-ar putea să vă placă și