Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

Society for Music Theory

Essay: Two Ways in Which Music Relates to the World


Author(s): FRED LERDAHL
Source: Music Theory Spectrum , Vol. 25, No. 2 (Fall 2003), pp. 367-373
Published by: Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Music Theory
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mts.2003.25.2.367

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Oxford University Press and Society for Music Theory are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Music Theory Spectrum

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 367

two ways in which music relates to the world 367

Essay: infant interplay, and expressive gestural communication.


These causes are not mutually exclusive.
Two Ways in Which Music Relates One suggestive idea in this volume, by Steven Brown, is
to the World that music first emerged together with language in a “musi-
language” that eventually split into the two modalities that
fred lerdahl we recognize today. The notion that music and language
have the same source goes back at least to Jean-Jacques
Rousseau, who wrote:
With the first voices came the first articulations or sounds formed ac-
cording to the respective passions that dictated them . . . Thus verse,
Of all the arts, music possesses the most technical vocabu- singing, and speech have a common origin. The first discourses were
lary. This state of affairs gives music theorists the ability to the first songs. The periodic recurrences and measures of rhythm, the
speak and write about music with enviable precision, but it melodious modulations of accents, gave birth to poetry and music along
also isolates us. Technical training in music theory is a spe- with language.2
cialized endeavor. Nonmusicians, and even musicians who Brown’s evolutionary argument is very general, but it can
are not theoretically inclined, do not easily understand us. be supported by two lines of contemporary evidence. The
From our isolation and their incomprehension comes the first comes from the brain sciences. The neuropsychologist
tendency to regard music as existing in a bubble, unrelated to Isabelle Peretz has reached some telling conclusions based
anything else in the world. This view is surely mistaken. on patterns of behavioral deficits in patients with brain
Here, I shall discuss two respects in which music relates to lesions.3 First, musical processing divides into two broad
the world beyond itself: its common origin and shared struc- components, rhythm and pitch. Second, musical and linguis-
tures with language, and its projection of intuitions of ten- tic processing share certain deficits but not others. On one
sion, attraction, and agency through the internalization of hand, rhythmic processing takes place in the same areas of
motion. Both aspects are fundamental to musical emotion. the brain for both language and music. On the other, lexical
Music exists in complex form only in the human species, retrieval and syntax in language and pitch processing in
and it appears in all human societies. How did it arise? Early music are activated in different areas of the brain. Contour
ethnomusicologists were concerned with this question, but recognition appears to take place in a different brain area
in recent decades the issue has largely been neglected. A sign than interval recognition and to precede it in processing, so
of recent reengagement is a rather speculative book, The that tone-deaf people are usually able to speak with normal
Origins of Music, in which biologists, paleontologists, evo- contour but contour-deaf people are necessarily tone-deaf.
lutionary psychologists, and anthropologists propose that These conclusions are supported in part by new imaging
music-making conferred an evolutionary advantage upon our techniques that track local brain activation.
distant ancestors.1 The hypothesized causes for the musical The second line of evidence comes from theoretical ac-
capacity include Darwinian sexual selection, synchronized counts of linguistic and musical cognitive capacities. The
group behavior, social bonding during grooming, mother-

2 Rousseau [1760] 1966, 50.


1 Wallin, et al., 2001. 3 Peretz 1993 and Patel & Peretz 1997.

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 368

368 music theory spectrum 25 (2003)

linguistic capacity has three broad components: semantics, in the musical case. Syllables are placed not only to match
syntax, and phonology. Music does not, except peripherally, stress and grid but also to maximize, through relative dis-
have semantics in a linguistic sense, which includes lexical tances between attack points, the perceptual projection of
items as well as concepts such as reference and entailment. the constituents of the prosodic hierarchy. In this way long
Nor does music have a specifically linguistic syntax, which and short durations are assigned to syllables. It may be ob-
includes parts of speech, labeled phrase structures, negation, jected that language and even metered poetry are not spoken
anaphora, and so forth. Rather, the linguistic component with periodicity between metrical accents. However, limer-
that most resembles music is phonology, which, like music, icks and many short verses are recited with great metrical
concerns the organization of sound in time. The sounds of regularity,6 and music is never played by human performers
sentences break up into units of phrases and words; these with complete isochrony.7 The difference is one of degree.
units decompose into patterns of stressed and unstressed Periodic meter is an idealized mental construct for both
sounds and of long and short sounds, and they form rising music and poetry.
and falling contours. The derivation of contour follows largely from the stress
All of these phonological features have musical counter- grid, since the perception of relative stress is primarily a re-
parts. In a recent article I develop these parallels through a sult of relative pitch height, not of intensity, as one might
treatment of the sounds of a short poem, “Nothing Gold suppose.8 Following intonational theory and data,9 which
Can Stay” by Robert Frost, entirely as if they were musical establish focal pitches usually near the onset of syllables even
sounds, ignoring their meaning and syntax.4 The sounds of though pitch height continuously modulates, the model
the poem are put through the grouping, metrical, and reduc- posits four levels of tone height, with glides assumed be-
tional components of Lerdahl & Jackendoff 1983 (hereafter tween levels. In other languages the treatment of pitch
GTTM), and through a newly devised method for the deri- height might vary. Within the four-level framework, pitch
vation of contour. The analytic procedure relies on aspects of height is assigned via the stress grid from global to local
generative phonological theory, specifically the prosodic hi- levels, guided by a few paradigmatic shapes.10
erarchy, stress theory, and contour theory.5 Briefly, the The addition of contour to the metrical and durational
prosodic hierarchy describes the grouping of speech sounds assignments yields the normative realization of the poem in
into the levels of the syllable, word or clitic group, phonolog- musical notation shown in Example 1. Contained within
ical phrase, intonational phrase, and utterance. The stress this seemingly transparent notation are the structures of the
theory uses a notation similar to GTTM’s metrical grid and prosodic hierarchy, phonological stress, the metrical grid,
represents hierarchical patterns of syllabic stress. Stresses are duration, and pitch height.11
assigned cyclically over the prosodic groupings.
After these structures are established, the model assigns 6 Oehrle 1989.
metrical structure by finding the optimal match between a 7 Gabrielsson 1999.
permissible metrical grid and the stress pattern, essentially as 8 Handel 1989.
9 Reviewed in Ladd 1996.
10 This method bears comparison to the pitch-contour tradition in music
4 Lerdahl 2001a. theory, in particular the contour reduction algorithm in Morris 1993.
5 For prosodic hierarchy, see Hayes 1989; for stress theory, Liberman & 11 The phonologist William Idsardi recently apprised me of Frost’s read-
Prince 1977; and for countour theory, Pierrehumbert 1980 and Ladd ing of this poem, recorded in Paschen and Mosby 2001. Frost’s rendi-
1996. tion is extremely close to that represented in Example 1.

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 369

two ways in which music relates to the world 369

Ł Ł ý  Ł Ł Ł ¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ý
2

¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł ¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł
4

⁄Ł Ł Ł Ł
2 2
Ł Łý
Na-ture’s first green is gold, Her hard- est hue to hold. Her ear- ly leaf’s a flow- er; But on- ly so an hour.

        
⁄ ¼ ý ¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ý ¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ý ¹ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ý ¼ ý Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ý ¼ ý
Then leaf sub-sides to leaf. So E- den sank to grief. So dawn goes down to day. Noth-ing gold can stay.

example 1. Metrical, durational, and contour realization of “Nothing Gold Can Stay,” by Robert Frost.

From a musical perspective there is another step to take. proach can contribute in turn to the study of text setting. It
Traditional poetic analysis treats verbal recurrences as simple should also be remarked that this treatment extends to the
rhyming patterns: aabb, abab, and so forth. Music theory, in analysis of nonpoetic speech, with the proviso that in ordi-
contrast, has a highly developed approach to recurrence in nary speech there is little regularity in phonological stress
the form of prolongational structure. As in my theory of and syllabic repetition, so that its metrical and prolonga-
timbral prolongations,12 prosodic prolongational structure is tional structures are attenuated.
derived from global to local time-span reductional levels of To summarize the preceding, music and the the sounds of
syllabic prominence. Example 2 illustrates this for the first language share more organization than has commonly been
couplet. Noteheads signify relative structural importance. In recognized. The subcomponents of this organization corre-
an adaptation of GTTM’s threefold classification, dashed spond to the pattern of neuropsychological evidence men-
slurs represent the strong prolongation of rhyme, dotted slurs tioned earlier. Example 3 gives the hypothesized overall pic-
the weak prolongation of alliteration or assonance, and solid ture: those brain modules that process rhythm, contour, and
slurs the progression of nonrepetition. The graph shows not timbral relationships are the same in music and language,
isolated instances of alliteration and rhyme, as in standard while those that process purely pitch-intervallic structures
poetic analysis, but the richer relationship of partial repeti- and purely linguistic syntax and semantics occupy different
tions nested within rhymes—“green” is to “gold” as “hue” is parts of the brain. The convergence between cognitive the-
to “hold.” Note that it is timbral similarity rather than pitch ory and neuroscientific evidence calls for further investigation.
that is connected prolongationally, for “gold” is in the highest The most plausible explanation for this convergence is
pitch category while “hold” is in the lowest. This approach that music and language share the same evolutionary roots,
can be extended to the poem as a whole. in the form of pre-musical and pre-linguistic communicative
Incidentally, text setting is a rich source of evidence for and expressive auditory gestures involving shapes of group-
the interface between music and poetry.13 The present ap- ing, stress, duration, contour, and timbre. We still communi-
cate with infants and higher mammals in this manner. These
12 Lerdahl 1987. elementary shapes appear to lie at the basis of expressive
13 Ruwet 1972, Jackendoff 1989, Halle & Lerdahl 1994, and Hayes & utterance in language and of musical expression. With evo-
Kaun 1996. lution came specialization. Music and language diverged in

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 370

370 music theory spectrum 25 (2003)

  ð 
⁄ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
 
Na- ture’s first green is gold Her hard- est hue to hold

example 2. Prolongational structure of the first couplet of “Nothing Gold Can Stay,” by Robert Frost.

exclusively musical structures common structures exclusively linguistic structures


pitches & intervals durational patterns syntactic categories & relations
scales grouping (prosodic hierarchy) word meaning (lexicon)
harmony & counterpoint stress (contextual salience) semantic structures (reference, truth
tonality metrical grids conditions . . .)
pitch prolongations contour phonological distinctive features
tonal tension & attraction timbral prolongations (etc.)

example 3. Hypothesized brain organization of musical and linguistic structures.

their most characteristic features: pitch organization in I have reached a similar conclusion about music from a
music, and word and sentence meaning in language. Poetry different line of reasoning. In the early 1980s, it was estab-
straddles this evolutionary divergence by projecting, through lished empirically that music listeners of varied training and
the addition to ordinary speech of metrical and timbral pat- background make essentially identical judgments about per-
terning, its common heritage with music. ceived distances of pitches, chords, and regions from a given
Let us now consider a second way in which music relates tonic.15 This striking discovery motivated me both to seek a
to the world beyond itself. Drawing on philosophical and theoretical explanation for regular patterns in the data and to
linguistic work by Lakoff & Johnson (1980), Jackendoff develop a model that would quantify the qualitatively stated
(1982), and others, the cognitive scientist Steven Pinker stability conditions in GTTM’s conception of prolonga-
writes: tional analysis. This research agenda culminated in Tonal
Location in space is one of the two fundamental metaphors in language
Pitch Space,16 which presents an algebraic model of the per-
. . . The other is force, agency, and causation . . . Many cognitive scien- ceived distances of pitches, chords, and regions from one an-
tists have concluded from their research that a handful of concepts other. These distances are mapped onto multidimensional
about places, paths, motions, agency, and causation underlie the literal geometries that have precedent in the music-theoretic litera-
or figurative meanings of tens of thousands of words and constructions, ture. In addition to providing an explanatory framework
not only in English but in every other language that has been studied for the data, the model is used to trace event locations and
. . . These concepts and relations appear to be the vocabulary and syntax
paths at multiple prolongational levels, thereby conveying in
of mentalese, the language of thought.14

15 Krumhansl 1990.
14 Pinker 1997, 354–5. 16 Lerdahl 2001b.

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 371

two ways in which music relates to the world 371

a specific manner the otherwise vague intuition that listening cause of the visceral sense of the ebb and flow of musical
to a piece of music is like taking a journey. When allied to tension.20
words, pitch-space paths take on a narrative dimension as Recall Pinker’s statement: “Location in space is one of the
well. two fundamental metaphors in language. The other is force,
The pitch-space theory also enables the prediction of pat- agency, and causation.” The theory of tonal attraction brings
terns of tension and relaxation as events unfold. Four condi- force into the picture of musical space and motion. Like a
tions are needed to make valid predictions. First, there must spaceship moving among the moons of Jupiter, a melody or
be a component that derives and represents hierarchical chord progression moves in a certain direction but is affected
event structure, since tension is judged hierarchically more in its velocity and direction by the relative gravitational or
than sequentially.17 This goal is accomplished by an im- attractive force of other pitches and chords. A neighboring
proved version of GTTM’s prolongational analysis. Second, ornament may have little effect on its motion, but a tonic has
there must be a calculation of the perceived distance between considerable mass and may bring the tonal spaceship to rest.
any two chords, something the model does with great accu- But what of agency and causation? Pinker refers to a clas-
racy. Third, there must be a treatment of surface or sensory sic experiment by Heider & Simmel (1944), in which they
dissonance. Although this topic has been studied extensively made a cartoon film using three dots that were perceived by
by psychoacousticians, its behavior in musical contexts is subjects as moving not as inanimate objects but as animate
complex, and here the theory settles for an approximate agents. Pinker writes:
implementation. Fourth, there must be a model of melodic Agents are recognized by their ability to violate intuitive physics by
and harmonic attractions. The theory succeeds in this goal, starting, stopping, swerving, or speeding up without an external nudge,
subject to computational fine-tuning from experimental evi- especially when they persistently approach or avoid some other object.
dence that is only beginning to become available.18 Carol The agents are thought to have an internal and renewable source of
Krumhansl and I have undertaken an ongoing empirical energy, force, impetus, or oomph, which they use to propel themselves,
study of the predictions of the tension model over a wide usually in the service of a goal.21
range of diatonic and chromatic music. The correlations be- Similarly, a melody or chord progression does not simply
tween predictions and data are generally very high, and they follow the inertial path of least resistance. It would be dull
permit detailed and illuminating interpretations about lis- and would quickly come to a stop unless enlivened by mo-
teners’ responses.19 tion away from places that pull it toward rest. Such motion
According to this theoretical and empirical perspective, works against inertia and seems to be caused by an animate
then, not only the linguistic but also the musical capacity agent. Furthermore, such motion causes an emotional re-
employs space and motion in a constitutive way. This em- sponse. Echoing Pinker, the neurologist Antonio Damasio
ployment is not just cognitive in a disembodied sense but is a writes:
You can find the basic configurations of emotions in simple organisms,
17 This conclusion is sustained by empirical data on hierarchical and se- even in unicellular organism . . . You can do the same thing with a
quential predictions, as reported in Lerdahl, et al., 2000. simple chip moving about on a computer screen. Some jagged fast
18 See also Larson 2002 and Margulis 2003. movements will appear “angry,” harmonious but explosive jumps will
19 A preliminary version of this research appears in Lerdahl and Krum-
hansl 2003. For a historical review of music theories of tonal motion, 20 See Brower 2000.
tension, and attraction, see Rothfarb 2002. 21 Pinker 1997, 322.

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 372

372 music theory spectrum 25 (2003)

look “joyous,” recoiling motions will look “fearful.” A video that depicts Gabrielsson, A. 1999. “The Performance of Music.” In The
several geometric shapes moving about at different rates and holding Psychology of Music. Edited by D. Deutsch. Second edi-
varied relationships reliably elicits attributions of emotional state from tion. New York: Academic.
normal adults and even children. The reason why you can anthro-
pomorphize the chip or an animal so effectively is simple: emotion, as
Halle, J., and F. Lerdahl. 1994. “A Generative Textsetting
the word indicates, is about movement, about externalized behavior, Model.” Current Musicology 55: 3–26.
about certain orchestrations of reactions to a given cause, within a given Handel, S. 1989. Listening. Cambridge: MIT Press.
environment.22 Hayes, B. 1989. “The Prosodic Hierarchy in Poetry.” In
Phonetics and Phonology: Rhythm and Meter. Edited by
Here is a central source of musical emotion. We internal-
P. Kiparsky and G. Youmans. New York: Academic.
ize the motion of pitches and chords in reaction to contex-
Hayes, B., and A. Kaun. 1996. “The Role of Phonological
tual tonal forces in musical space. We attribute agency and
Phrasing in Sung and Chanted Verse.” The Linguistic
causation to musical motions that violate intuitive physics
Review 13: 243–303.
and inevitability to motions that yield to musical inertia and
Heider, F., and M. Simmel. 1944. “An Experimental Study
force. The character of the musical motions, which is shaped
of Apparent Behavior.” American Journal of Psychology 57:
also by their temporal realization, mirrors equivalent mo-
243–59.
tions in the “real” physical world. We map specific musical
Jackendoff, R. 1982. Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge:
motions onto specific emotional qualities, again in reflection
MIT Press.
of real-world equivalences.
———. 1989. “Rhythmic Structures in Music and Language.”
This argument about musical space, motion, force,
In Phonetics and Phonology: Rhythm and Meter. Edited by
agency, and emotion rejoins the earlier discussion about the
P. Kiparsky and G. Youmans. New York: Academic.
origin of “musilanguage” in expressive auditory gestures. But
Krumhansl, C. L. 1990. Cognitive Foundations of Musical
language lacks pitch structure except in the most rudimen-
Pitch. New York: Oxford University Press.
tary sense. Perhaps music is the quintessentially emotional
Ladd, D. R. 1996. Intonational Phonology. Cambridge: Cam-
art because its elaborate pitch structures so richly and pre-
bridge University Press.
cisely reflect motion, force, and agency, and therefore emo-
Lakoff, G., and M. Johnson 1980. Metaphors We Live By.
tions, in the outer world.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Larson, S. 2002. “Musical Forces, Melodic Expectation, and
references
Jazz Melody.” Music Perception 19: 351–85.
Lerdahl, F. 1987. “Timbral Hierarchies.” Contemporary
Brower, C. 2000. “A Cognitive Theory of Musical
Music Review 1: 135–60.
Meaning.” Journal of Music Theory 44: 323–79.
———. 2001a. “The Sounds of Poetry Viewed as Music.” In
Brown, S. 2001. “The ‘Musilanguage’ Model of Music.” In
The Biological Foundations of Music. Edited by R. J.
The Origins of Music. Edited by N. L. Wallin, B. Merker,
Zatorre and I. Peretz. Annals of the New York Academy
and S. Brown. Cambridge: MIT Press.
of Sciences 930: 337–54.
Damasio, A. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and
———. 2001b. Tonal Pitch Space. New York: Oxford Uni-
Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Har-
versity Press.
court Brace.
Lerdahl, F., and R. Jackendoff. 1983. A Generative Theory of
22 Damasio 1999, 70. Tonal Music. Cambridge: MIT Press.

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
679-125.MTS.Lerdahl_pp367-374 8/28/03 2:25 PM Page 373

two ways in which music relates to the world 373

Lerdahl, F., and C. L. Krumhansl. 2003. “La teoría de la ten- and A. Gode. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
sión tonal y sus consecuencias para la investigación musi- 1966.
cal.” In Los últimos diez años en la investigación musical. Ruwet, N. 1972. Langage, musique, poésie. Paris: Seuil.
Edited by J. Martín Galán & C. Villar-Taboada. Valla- Wallin, N. L., B. Merker, and S. Brown, eds. 2001. The Ori-
dolid: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de gins of Music. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Valladolid.
Lerdahl, F., C. L. Krumhansl, J. Fineberg, and E. Hannon.
2000. “Modeling Tonal Tension and Attraction.” Paper
given at Toronto 2000: Musical Intersections.
Liberman, M., and A. Prince. 1977. “On Stress and Lin-
guistic Rhythm.” Linguistic Inquiry 8: 249–336.
Margulis, E. H. 2003. “Melodic Expectation: A Discussion
and Model.” Ph.D. dissertation, Columbia University.
Morris, R. D. 1993. “New Directions in the Theory and
Analysis of Musical Contour.” Music Theory Spectrum 15:
205–28.
Oehrle, R. T. 1989. “Temporal Structures in Verse Design.”
In Phonetics and Phonology: Rhythm and Meter. Edited by
P. Kiparsky and G. Youmans. New York: Academic.
Paschen, E., and R. P. Mosby, eds. 2001. Poetry Speaks.
Naperville, IL: Sourcebooks.
Patel, A. D., and I. Peretz. 1997. “Is Music Autonomous
from Language? A Neuropsychological Appraisal.” In
Perception and Cognition of Music. Edited by I. Deliège
and J. A. Sloboda. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Peretz, I. 1993. “Auditory Agnosia: A Functional Analysis.”
In Thinking in Sound: The Cognitive Psychology of Human
Audition. Edited by S. McAdams and E. Bigand. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Pierrehumbert, J. 1980. “The Phonology and Phonetics of
English Intonation.” Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts
Institute of Technology.
Pinker, S. 1997. How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.
Rothfarb, L. 2002. “Energetics.” In The Cambridge History of
Music Theory. Edited by T. Christensen. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Rousseau, J.-J. [1760] 1966. Essai sur l’origine des langues.
In On the Origin of Language. Translated by J. H. Moran

This content downloaded from


187.232.204.58 on Sun, 20 Jan 2019 22:42:17 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

S-ar putea să vă placă și