Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2002, Vol. 82, No. 4, 675– 686 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.4.675

Victim and Offender Accounts of Interpersonal Conflict:


Autobiographical Narratives of Forgiveness and Unforgiveness
Jeanne S. Zechmeister and Catherine Romero
Loyola University of Chicago

Participants wrote 2 narratives that described an incident in which they angered or hurt someone
(offender) or in which someone angered or hurt them (victim) and the offense was forgiven or not
forgiven. Victims portrayed the offense as continuing (open), and offenders portrayed the offense as over
(closed). Forgiveness narratives portrayed offenses as closed and with positive outcomes; however, for
some victims, forgiveness coincided with continued anger, suggesting incomplete forgiveness. Disposi-
tional empathy was associated with more benign interpretations of offenses, and situational empathy
(e.g., for the offender) was associated with victims’ forgiveness. In contrast, offenders’ empathy for
victims was associated with less self-forgiveness. Thus, both victim or offender role and forgiveness must
be considered to understand narratives of interpersonal offenses.

During the past decade we have witnessed a growing interest in forgiveness” (e.g., Baumeister, Exline, & Sommer, 1999; Enright
the psychology of forgiveness, both in the public (e.g., Time & The Human Development Study Group, 1991, McCullough &
Magazine, 1999) and scientific domains (e.g., McCullough et al., Worthington, 1994; Vitz & Mango, 1997), an effort that has a long
1998; McCullough, Worthington, & Rachal, 1997; Scobie & Sco- history in philosophers’ attempts to understand forgiveness
bie, 1998; Worthington, 1999). Forgiveness is often operationally (Haber, 1991; Scobie & Scobie, 1998). For example, Baumeister et
defined in terms of behavioral, affective, and cognitive responses al. (1999) described two dimensions of forgiveness: intrapsychic
following an interpersonal offense (e.g., Enright & The Human and interpersonal. The former involves the emotional and cogni-
Development Study Group, 1991, 1996). Individuals are described tive aspects of forgiveness, and the latter involves social or be-
as forgiving if they inhibit retaliatory or destructive responses and havioral aspects. Whereas “total forgiveness” requires the presence
instead respond with conciliatory or constructive behaviors, affect, of both dimensions, “hollow forgiveness” (or pseudoforgiveness)
and cognition (McCullough, 2000; McCullough et al., 1997; is characterized as the interpersonal act in the absence of the
North, 1987; Rusbult, Verette, Whitney, Slovik, & Lipkus, 1991; intrapsychic state. For example, a victim may verbally express
Subkoviak et al., 1995; Worthington, 1999). For example, in forgiveness to the offender, yet may continue to harbor resentment
forgiving, an individual may recognize situational determinants and hurt. Such hollow forgiveness may be motivated by the vic-
that caused an offender’s actions (cognitive), feel sympathetic or tim’s desire to fulfill a socially prescribed role (Trainer, 1981; see
compassionately toward the offender (affective), and discuss pos- also Scobie & Scobie’s, 1998, description of responses to minor
sible solutions to problems or help the offender (behavior). For- offenses). Similarly, victims may “forgive” to feel morally supe-
giving responses reflect an underlying motivational change that rior or to gain power over the offender (e.g., Trainer’s “expedient
counteracts individuals’ more natural tendencies to withdraw forgiveness”). Baumeister et al. (1999) also described “silent for-
and/or retaliate following an interpersonal offense (McCullough et giveness,” in which the intrapsychic state is not accompanied by an
al., 1997, 1998; Rusbult et al., 1991). interpersonal act, such as reconciliation.
Recent theoretical development of the forgiveness construct has Several theorists have explicitly noted that forgiveness does not
attempted to differentiate “true” forgiveness and “false” or “pseudo- require reconciliation (e.g., Enright & Zell, 1989; Scobie & Sco-
bie, 1998; but see Power, 1994, for an exception). In these con-
ceptualizations, it is possible to forgive someone without recon-
Jeanne S. Zechmeister and Catherine Romero, Department of Psychol- ciling, and in fact reconciliation may at times be undesirable (Fow,
ogy, Loyola University of Chicago. 1996; Freedman, 1998). Hence, conciliatory behaviors need not
Portions of these data were presented at the 107th Annual Convention of include reunion of the two parties involved. For example, forgivers
the American Psychological Association, Boston, August 1999, and the may express kind words about the offender to others or pray for the
annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, well-being of the offender.
May 2000. We gratefully acknowledge the generosity of the Fetzer Insti- True forgiveness (or “intrinsic forgiveness,” Trainer, 1981) is
tute, Kalamazoo, Michigan, for their support of this research, in coopera- defined as a conscious choice in which individuals give up their
tion with the Templeton Foundation. We thank an anonymous reviewer,
legitimate claim for retaliation following an interpersonal offense
Roy Baumeister, Robert Emmons, and Eugene Zechmeister for their in-
and substitute conciliatory responses. A key feature of some the-
sightful comments and suggestions.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeanne S. orists’ definitions of true forgiveness is that it is offered uncondi-
Zechmeister, Department of Psychology, Loyola University of Chicago, tionally (e.g., Al-Mabuk, Enright, & Cardis, 1995). Thus, forgive-
6525 North Sheridan Road, Chicago, Illinois 60626. E-mail: jzechme@ ness is seen as a gift that is offered to an offender whose behaviors
luc.edu may justify retaliation or demands for restitution, but the forgiver

675
676 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

neither retaliates nor requires particular behaviors of the offender writing about their own actions as an offender (using a repeated
(e.g., apology, restitution). measures design), place the incident in an isolated context and
Such a conceptualization of forgiveness may be at odds with perceive their own actions and motivations as justified. Thus, the
victims’ experience of interpersonal offenses and forgiveness. That role one assumes, either victim or offender, influences perception
is, it is possible that victims may require an apology, an attempt by and/or recall of the event (see also Gonzales, Manning, & Haugen,
the offender to make amends, visual displays of the offender’s 1992). Baumeister et al. (1990) suggested that offenders are mo-
remorse, or some other conditions before they forgive. Thus, the tivated to perceive their actions as inoffensive and to perceive
actual experience of forgiveness may not conform to theoretical themselves as acceptable people. Victims, in contrast, are moti-
definitions of forgiveness. Little psychological research has ex- vated to portray the incident as harmful and themselves as deserv-
plored individuals’ concepts of forgiveness, and participants’ un- ing of sympathy or restitution; in so doing, victims may claim a
derstanding of what it means to forgive has been open to idio- higher moral standing. Thus, both victims and offenders seem to
graphic interpretation in most forgiveness research. Thus, an portray events in a self-serving manner. One purpose of the present
important step in forgiveness research is to describe what individ- study was to replicate these findings by having participants write
uals mean when they say they “forgive” or “do not forgive” and to about incidents involving anger or hurt in which they were the
compare these meanings to theoretical definitions of forgiveness. victim or the offender.
The narrative method (Baumeister & Newman, 1994; Gergen & A second purpose of the study was to examine the role of
Gergen, 1988) provides an excellent tool for studying forgiveness forgiveness following interpersonal offenses. A growing body of
as it occurs naturally, that is, in real-life situations and from the empirical and theoretical work on forgiveness suggests that several
perspective of the person doing the forgiving. variables influence whether forgiveness occurs following an of-
fense, including empathy, apology, attributions, severity of the
The Narrative Method offense, arousal, and relationship closeness (McCullough, 2000;
McCullough et al., 1998). A key factor in Baumeister et al.’s
Narratives provide subjective accounts of what happened from (1990) narratives of interpersonal conflict was closure. They ex-
one person’s (i.e., the narrator’s) point of view. Narratives indicate amined narratives for the presence or absence of apology to
the aspects of an incident a narrator regards as important and differentiate victims’ and offenders’ motives to seek closure re-
meaningful enough to be included in the story (Gergen & Gergen, garding the incident. They reasoned that offenders would be ex-
1988). The objective truth of the events is not as important as the pected to mention an apology as a way to bracket the offense as
narration itself, which involves motivated choices regarding which over. Social prescriptions indicate it is appropriate to forgive
details to emphasize. For example, if a narrator does not mention following an apology (Scobie & Scobie, 1998; Trainer, 1981);
an apology, we cannot assume an apology did not occur or that it thus, offenders may mention an apology to demonstrate the offense
did occur and the narrator denies its occurrence. Instead, we is over, or at least, should be over. In contrast, victims may be less
assume that the presence or absence of an apology in an incident likely to mention an apology if they are motivated to maintain their
was not critical to the meaning of the narrator’s personal story. victim status. Baumeister et al. observed that offenders mentioned
Baumeister and colleagues (e.g., Baumeister, Stillwell, & an apology more than victims, supporting their hypothesis that
Heatherton, 1995; Baumeister, Stillwell, & Wotman, 1990; Leith offenders were more motivated to seek closure for the past offense
& Baumeister, 1998; Schultz & Baumeister, 1999) advocated the compared with victims.
use of personal narratives as a tool for investigating the phenom- One way that offenders and victims can attain closure for an
enology of psychological constructs—that is, how people experi- offense is through forgiveness. Thus, perceptions of past interper-
ence anger, guilt, shame, and interpersonal conflicts. Narratives sonal offenses are likely to differ depending on whether the event
may be particularly useful for understanding people’s motivations is forgiven or not forgiven. For example, forgiven events may
as they describe important events in their lives. Baumeister and include more references to empathy and apologies than do unfor-
Newman (1994) proposed that people structure their experiences given events and may be perceived as time-limited with positive
in story-like form to make sense of these experiences. In narratives consequences, regardless of whether the writer is victim or of-
about unpleasant events such as interpersonal offenses, people may fender. In contrast, unforgiven events may be placed in a longer
seek to explain the purpose behind the offense and interpret and time frame with negative consequences when the writer is the
justify events in terms of their values. Narratives can also provide victim (Baumeister et al., 1990). Thus, in addition to writing as
a sense of efficacy and control as individuals attribute the causes victim or perpetrator of an offense, we asked participants to choose
of events to internal or external determinants. Finally, Baumeister events that were forgiven (other forgiveness or self-forgiveness,
and Newman suggested that individuals may construct narratives respectively), or not forgiven. Our aim was to extend Baumeister
so as to maximize their self-worth. et al.’s (1990) findings by examining the role of forgiveness in
Baumeister and his colleagues (1990, 1995) used narratives to victim and offender narratives.
examine individuals’ responses to interpersonal transgressions in- To the extent that forgiveness represents a motivational shift
volving anger and guilt. They demonstrated that how individuals from relationship-destructive tendencies to relationship-con-
perceive interpersonal conflicts depends on their role as victim or structive tendencies (McCullough et al., 1997, 1998; Rusbult et al.,
offender. For example, Baumeister et al. (1990) observed that 1991), victims’ narratives of forgiven offenses may reflect the
victims of interpersonal conflicts involving anger tend to place the same degree of closure as offenders’ narratives. However, forgive-
incident in a longer time frame, with continuing anger and rela- ness may also be used to enhance victims’ position of moral
tionship damage, and to perceive the offenders’ motivations as superiority (Trainer, 1981). Thus, hollow forgiveness (Baumeister
arbitrary or senseless. These same individuals, however, when et al., 1999) or incomplete forgiveness (Enright & The Human
FORGIVENESS NARRATIVES 677

Development Study Group, 1991) may be reflected in victims’ ings (order of narrative instructions was counterbalanced): of-
portrayals of forgiven offenses in which the offense continues to fender forgives/does not forgive, victim forgives/does not forgive,
remain open and has lasting negative consequences. Such a por- offender forgives/victim forgives, and offender does not forgive/
trayal would allow victims to maintain their victim status, despite victim does not forgive.1 Following Baumeister et al. (1990), we
their report of forgiveness. coded narratives for the presence or absence of variables relating
Previous accounts of forgiveness have emphasized forgiveness to offense severity, blameworthiness, self-threat, time frame and
of others rather than self-forgiveness. Baumeister et al. (1990) consequences of the event, motives and intentions, writer’s affec-
observed that offenders were motivated to portray their offense in tive response, victim’s response, and empathy for the other person.
the least negative light, thus preserving a positive self-concept.
This self-enhancing motivation may be greater in offenders who Method
have forgiven themselves and weakened in offenders who have not
forgiven themselves. Participants
Participants were friends, family members, and coworkers of students in
Dispositional Empathy and Forgiveness an advanced psychology research course at Loyola University Chicago.
Each student asked 8 individuals to write two narratives for a class project.
Using McAdams’s (1995, 1996) framework, Emmons (2000) The final sample (N ⫽ 122) wrote one or two usable narratives for a total
conceptualized personality not only as a set of dispositional traits, of 215 narratives. The sample was predominantly female (60.7%), Cauca-
but also as personal strivings or motives and more broadly as sian (72.1%), and Catholic (49.2%) and were predominantly full-time
integrative life stories. Thus, as snapshots of such life stories, students (58.2%; see Table 1 for demographics). The mean age was 27.7
narratives may reflect individual differences in personality. That (SD ⫽ 13.2, range ⫽ 18 – 68, Mdn ⫽ 21).
is, narrative features may be included more or less frequently
depending on the narrator’s personality, as measured with standard Materials
instruments. For example, individuals high in dispositional empa-
Narrative instructions. Instructions for each narrative asked partici-
thy may be more likely to include empathy features in their pants to describe an incident in which they were the victim or offender of
narratives relative to individuals low in dispositional empathy. an offense that was either forgiven or not forgiven. Participants were asked
This is important because, as noted above, narratives are shaped by to choose an especially important or memorable event and were encour-
a host of factors, only one set of which are the objective “facts” of aged to tell the whole story. The personality inventory followed the two
the incident described. Thus, by taking into account individual narrative pages.
differences in key dimensions such as empathy, it is possible to Empathy. The 28-item Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis,
determine the relative contributions of dispositional and situational 1983) comprises empathy-related statements that participants rate on a
determinants of narrative responses. scale from 1 (Doesn’t describe me well) to 5 (Describes me very well).
Coefficient alphas in the present sample for the relevant subscales were .77
We assessed other-focused empathy, which comprises per-
(Perspective-Taking) and .81 (Emotional Concern). We summed partici-
spective-taking and emotional concern for another (Davis, 1983,
pants’ responses for these two subscales to form an other-focused empathy
1996), because previous theoretical work considered these com- measure (␣ ⫽ .85) to assess dispositional empathy. The mean empathy
ponents critical for forgiveness (e.g., Enright & The Human De- score was 51.60 (SD ⫽ 9.43, range ⫽ 26 – 68).
velopment Study Group, 1991; McCullough, 2000; Worthington,
1998, 1999). Furthermore, empirical research has identified situ-
Procedure
ational empathy (e.g., empathy for a specific offender) as a key
mediator in the forgiveness process (e.g., McCullough et al., 1997, Students in an advanced psychology research-methods course asked
1998). It is possible that dispositional empathy may promote participants to write two narratives as part of a class project on forgiveness.
situational empathy, which may in turn promote forgiveness. Thus, Participants wrote about two interpersonal offenses and then completed the
we hypothesized that dispositional empathy would predict whether empathy measure. They then sealed their responses in an envelope, which
narrators included empathy-related features such as perspective was returned to the course instructor (Jeanne S. Zechmeister). Most par-
ticipants wrote two narratives that conformed to the instructions, although
taking in their narratives. In addition, one way in which disposi-
some participants wrote they could not think of an event that satisfied the
tional empathy may facilitate forgiveness is through narrators’ instructions (see Table 2). The average length of the narratives was 157.6
ability to identify benign causes for the offense, such as by words and 9.5 sentences. A wide range of events was described, including
recognizing that some offenses are due to external circumstances. sexual infidelities, drug- or alcohol-related deaths, and social humiliations.
Therefore, we also hypothesized that individuals high in disposi- Two coders evaluated the narratives for the presence or absence of vari-
tional empathy would be more likely to attribute the offense to ables associated with the offense severity, blameworthiness, self-threat,
benign causes. time frame, consequences, intentions, affective responses, empathy, and
forgiveness. These variables were selected to replicate previous research on
victim and offender portrayals of interpersonal offenses (see Baumeister et
Overview al., 1990). Dichotomous coding was used to increase the objectivity and
Similar to Baumeister et al. (1990), we asked participants to
write two narratives. Participants recalled incidents in which they 1
To avoid confounding the narrators’ perspective (victim, offender) and
angered or hurt someone (offender) or someone angered or hurt forgiveness instructions (forgive, do not forgive) within any one partici-
them (victim). Further, participants chose situations in which they pant’s two narratives, we did not use two pairings of narrative instructions:
forgave or did not forgive (either themselves or the other person). victim forgives– offender does not forgive and victim does not forgive–
We randomly assigned participants to one of four narrative pair- offender forgives.
678 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

Table 1 even though they did not express their anger overtly (25.2%
Demographic Characteristics of the Sample (N ⫽ 122) vs. 5.8%). Offenders, on the other hand, were more likely to
describe the victim’s response as an overreaction (9.8% vs. 2.7%).
Variable n % Lest offenders appear too callous, we should note that narrators
Gender who wrote as offenders were more likely than victim narrators to
Women 74 60.7 mention regret for the incident (60.6% vs. 5.4%) and to blame
Men 46 37.7 themselves (59.6% vs. 2.7%).
Missing 2 1.6 Baumeister et al. (1990) observed that offenders, compared with
Race/ethnicity
victims, were more likely to mention an apology in their narra-
White/Caucasian 88 72.1
Hispanic 12 9.8 tives, suggesting that offenders were motivated to describe the
Asian/Pacific Islander 9 7.4 offense as over and isolated. Similarly, offenders in our study were
African American 2 1.6 slightly more likely to mention an apology than were victims
Native American 1 0.8 (16.0% vs. 8.1%). The presence of an apology, however, was
Missing 10 8.2
Student status moderated by forgiveness.
Full-time student 71 58.2
Nonstudent 50 41.0 Narratives of Forgiveness and Unforgiveness
Missing 1 0.8
Religion These findings for victim and offender narratives suggest that
Catholic 60 49.2 narrators’ self-enhancing motives influenced the features they in-
Protestant/other Christian 13 10.7
Jewish 13 10.7 cluded in their stories of interpersonal offense (see also Baumeister
Agnostic/Atheist 6 4.9 et al., 1990; Draycott & Dabbs, 1998; Gonzales et al., 1992). We
“Other” 14 11.5 hypothesized that these motives may be reduced or eliminated
“None” 8 6.6 when an offense is forgiven, relative to when the offense is not
Missing 8 6.6
forgiven. However, if narrators describe hollow or incomplete
Note. Missing refers to the number of participants who left an item blank. forgiveness, their narratives may not differ in terms of closure
Questions for race/ethnicity and religion used a free-response format. (e.g., time span, consequences of the offense). We also examined
the narratives for clues regarding the forgiving process, including
narrators’ mentions of apologies, excuses, making amends, re-
reliability of the ratings. The mean agreement was 86.1% (SD ⫽ 11.2, venge, affect, and empathy. Results indicated that forgiveness,
range ⫽ 54%–100%). After coding the narratives independently, the two relative to unforgiveness, dramatically influenced the stories that
coders discussed discrepancies until consensus was reached. A series of people tell about interpersonal conflicts.
2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 2 chi-square analyses examined differences in narratives as a Narratives of forgiven offenses, whether written from the victim
function of victim versus offender role, forgiven versus not forgiven or offender perspective, were more likely than narratives of un-
events, and the presence or absence of coded features.
forgiven offenses to include features that indicated the offense was
closed. Narrators of forgiven offenses were more likely to describe
Results the offender’s apology (17.8% vs. 4.3%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 211) ⫽ 9.07,
One goal of this study was to replicate Baumeister et al.’s (1990) p ⫽ .003, ␾ ⫽ .21, attempts to make amends (18.6% vs. 4.3%),
findings for victim and offender narratives of interpersonal con- ␹2(1, N ⫽ 210) ⫽ 9.74, p ⫽ .002, ␾ ⫽ .22, positive consequences
flict. In general, we successfully replicated their findings (see (22.9% vs. 4.1%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 15.18, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ .27,
Table 3). Participants’ self-serving narration was most apparent happy endings (36.4% vs. 3.1%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 35.20, p ⬍
when they described the offender’s intentions and motives. When
writing as offenders, compared with victims, narrators more often
portrayed their actions as caused by external or mitigating circum- Table 2
stances (44.2% vs. 19.8%), explained their actions could not be Number and Percentage of Completed and Missing
helped (13.5% vs. 5.4%), and asserted their actions were justified Narratives in Each Condition
(61.5% vs. 11.7%). Offenders were more likely than victims to
implicate the victim in provoking the incident (22.1% vs. 1.8%) Forgiveness condition
and to state that the cause of the incident included the victim
Forgive Did not forgive
(63.5% vs. 33.3%).
In contrast, victims were more likely than offenders to state Writer and presence N % N %
explicitly that the offender’s motives were incoherent or senseless
(12.6% vs. 4.9%) and more often portrayed the offender’s motives Offender
Completed 55 87.3 49 86.0
as arbitrary (50.5% vs. 14.4%), inconsistent (23.4% vs. 11.5%), Missing 8 12.7 8 14.0
and immoral (14.4% vs. 1.9%). Victims’ accounts were more Victim
likely than offenders’ accounts to describe the relationship as Completed 63 95.5 48 85.3
damaged (38.7% vs. 18.4%) and to claim that they suffered from Missing 3 4.5 10 17.2
multiple provocations (38.7% vs. 12.7%). Victims’ accounts, com- Note. Narratives were counted as missing if the page was blank or if
pared with offenders’ accounts, described continuing anger (23.4% participants wrote comments such as “I can’t think of anything” or “I have
vs. 3.0%), and portrayed their anger as justified (85.6% vs. 54.9%) forgiven every individual.”
FORGIVENESS NARRATIVES 679

Table 3
Results of Content Coding for Offender and Victim Narratives (N ⫽ 215)

Offenders Victims
Item (%) (%) ␹2a ␾b

Offender’s intentions/motives
Described as incoherent or senseless (explicit statement) 4.9 12.6 3.98 .14*
Portrayed as arbitrary, contradictory, incoherent, or senseless
(rater’s judgment) 14.4 50.5 31.51 .38***
External or mitigating circumstances 44.2 19.8 14.80 ⫺.26***
Could not be helped 13.5 5.4 4.13 ⫺.14*
Justified or justifiable 61.5 11.7 57.99 ⫺.52***
Inconsistent 11.5 23.4 5.21 .16*
Immoral 1.9 14.4 10.92 .23**
Deliberately cruel or malicious 14.4 17.1 0.29 .04
Victim provoked incident (thus shares blame) 22.1 1.8 21.56 ⫺.32***
Cause of incident includes victim 63.5 33.3 19.53 ⫺.30***
Time frame and consequences
Long-term past events preceding incident 39.4 44.1 0.49 .05
Multiple or accumulated provocations 12.7 38.7 18.53 .30***
Positive consequences 16.3 12.6 0.61 ⫺.05
Happy endings 22.1 20.7 0.06 ⫺.02
Negative consequences at time of event 81.7 79.3 0.21 ⫺.03
Denial of lasting negative consequences 26.9 27.9 0.03 .01
Damage to relationship 18.4 38.7 10.69 .22***
Victim’s response
Victim still angry 3.0 23.4 18.51 .30***
Anger justified (rater’s judgment) 54.9 85.6 24.25 .34***
No overt expression 5.8 25.2 15.05 .26***
Portrayed as overreaction 9.8 2.7 4.68 ⫺.15*
Offender’s response
Offender regrets incident 60.6 5.4 74.99 ⫺.59***
Self-blame 59.6 2.7 82.45 ⫺.62***
Offender apologizes 16.0 8.1 3.14 ⫺.12†

Note. Percentages represent the proportion of narratives codable on that dimension that were coded as having
the feature present.
a
The N for each analysis was typically 215, although this varied slightly between 210 and 215. Some features
were not codable in all stories. For example, victims’ responses to the offense were not coded if the narrator
reported the victim had died as a result of the offense. b ␾, the Pearson product–moment correlation for
dichotomous variables, indicates the strength of the relationship between whether the feature was present or
absent and whether the narrative was written from the offender or victim perspective. Following Cohen (1988),
values of ␾ representing small, medium, and large effect sizes are .10, .30, and .50, respectively.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

.001, ␾ ⫽ .40, and feelings of peace following the incident (31.4% events or whether social desirability concerns prevented partici-
vs. 1.0%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 33.65, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ .40. In contrast, pants from mentioning revenge.
when describing unforgiven offenses, compared with forgiven For the remaining coded variables, narrators’ descriptions de-
offenses, narrators were more likely to describe negative conse- pended on whether they wrote as a victim or offender and whether
quences of the offense (91.8% vs. 71.2%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ the offense was forgiven or unforgiven. To explicate these effects,
215) ⫽ 14.33, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ ⫺.26, refer to present circumstances we describe forgiven and unforgiven narratives first from the
(76.3% vs. 44.9%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 21.67, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ ⫺.32, victims’ perspective and then from the offender’s perspective.
and claim that anger was justified (85.3% vs. 59.3%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ Victim narratives. Of the 111 victim narratives, 63 were writ-
213) ⫽ 17.16, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ ⫺.28. Thus, just as many of these ten in the offender-forgiven condition and 48 were written in the
features distinguished victims’ and offenders’ narratives in offender-not-forgiven condition. Consistent with the manipulation,
Baumeister et al.’s (1990) study, individuals’ narratives of inter- victims were more likely to state explicitly they had forgiven the
personal conflict were also differentiated on the basis of the offender in the forgiven condition (84.1%) than in the not-forgiven
presence of forgiveness or unforgiveness. condition (0%). Moreover, 25% of victims who wrote about un-
A central aspect of forgiveness definitions is that victims re- forgiveness stated they would “never forgive” the offender (see
nounce their desire for revenge. We coded narrators’ desire for Table 4, Victim narratives columns).
revenge and whether revenge occurred. Only 19 (8.9%) narrators The time frame and consequences of the offense differed de-
mentioned revenge, and this was unrelated to whether narrators pending on whether victims forgave the offender or did not for-
described a forgiven or unforgiven offense and whether the nar- give. Compared with forgiving narrators, victims who wrote about
rator was an offender or victim. It is not clear from these data unforgiven offenses were more likely to refer to long-term events
whether revenge was not important in narrators’ accounts of the preceding the event (33.3% vs. 58.3%) and relationship damage
680 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

Table 4
Results of Content Coding for Victim (N ⫽ 111) and Offender (N ⫽ 104) Narratives
in Which the Offense Is Not Forgiven or Is Forgiven

Victim narratives Offender narratives

Offender Offender Self Self


is not is is not is
forgiven forgiven forgiven forgiven
Item (%) (%) ␹2 ␾ (%) (%) ␹2 ␾

Victim explicitly forgives 0.0 84.1 77.28 .83*** 15.6 16.4 0.01 .01
Victim “will never forgive” 25.0 1.6 14.44 ⫺.36*** 0.0 0.0
Offender’s intentions/motives
Described as incoherent or senseless (explicit statement) 8.3 15.9 1.40 .11 6.1 3.7 0.32 ⫺.06
Portrayed as arbitrary, contradictory, incoherent, or
senseless (rater’s judgment) 58.3 44.4 2.10 ⫺.14 22.4 7.3 4.84 ⫺.22*
External or mitigating circumstances 16.7 22.2 0.53 .07 46.9 41.8 0.28 ⫺.05
Could not be helped 0.0 9.5 4.83 .21* 12.2 14.5 0.12 .03
Justified or justifiable 8.3 14.3 0.93 .09 53.1 69.1 2.81 .16†
Inconsistent 18.8 27.0 1.03 .10 14.3 9.1 0.68 ⫺.08
Immoral 27.1 4.8 11.00 ⫺.32** 2.0 1.8 0.01 ⫺.01
Deliberately cruel or malicious 29.2 7.9 8.66 ⫺.28** 16.3 12.7 0.27 ⫺.05
Victim provoked incident (thus shares blame) 2.1 1.6 0.04 ⫺.02 12.2 30.9 5.24 .22*
Cause of incident includes victim 31.2 34.9 0.16 .04 59.2 67.3 0.73 .08
Time frame and consequences
Long-term past events preceding incident 58.3 33.3 6.91 ⫺.25** 46.9 58.3 1.26 .11
Multiple or accumulated provocations 47.9 31.7 3.00 ⫺.16† 17.0 9.1 1.43 ⫺.12
Refers to present circumstances 70.8 49.2 5.25 ⫺.22* 81.6 40.0 18.66 ⫺.42***
Positive consequences 2.1 20.6 8.51 .28** 6.1 25.5 7.08 .26**
Happy endings 0.0 36.5 22.10 .45*** 6.1 36.4 13.76 .36***
Improved relationship 0.0 4.8 2.35 .14 4.2 16.4 4.00 .20*
Negative consequences at time of event 91.7 69.8 7.90 ⫺.27** 91.8 72.7 6.34 ⫺.25*
Denial of lasting negative consequences 0.0 49.2 32.77 .54*** 8.2 43.6 16.57 .40***
Damage to relationship 72.9 12.7 41.63 ⫺.61*** 22.9 14.5 1.19 ⫺.11
Victim’s response
Victim still angry 39.6 11.1 12.31 ⫺.33*** 4.4 1.8 0.59 ⫺.08
Anger justified (rater’s judgment) 97.9 76.2 10.42 ⫺.31** 72.3 40.0 10.70 ⫺.32**
No overt expression 18.8 30.2 1.88 .13 6.2 5.5 0.03 ⫺.02
Portrayed as overreaction 4.2 1.6 0.69 ⫺.08 0.0 18.2 9.47 .30**
Revenge 10.2 9.7 0.01 ⫺.01 8.5 7.3 0.05 ⫺.02
Victim makes amends 2.1 14.3 4.95 .21* 6.7 1.8 1.52 ⫺.12
Offender’s response
Offender regrets incident 2.1 7.9 1.88 .13 89.8 34.5 33.12 ⫺.56***
Self-blame 2.1 3.2 0.12 .03 89.8 32.7 35.06 ⫺.58***
Offender explicitly apologizes 2.1 12.7 4.12 .19* 6.7 23.6 5.30 .23*
Offender makes excuses 20.8 7.9 3.88 ⫺.19* 0.0 3.6 1.67 .13
Offender makes amends 2.1 17.5 6.68 .24** 6.8 20.0 3.50 .10†
Narrator’s affect
Guilt 0.0 1.6 0.77 .08 73.5 40.0 11.77 ⫺.34**
Anger 45.8 50.8 0.27 .05 4.1 25.5 9.09 .30**
Peaceful 2.1 27.0 12.43 .34*** 0.0 36.4 22.06 .46***
Love 0.0 7.9 3.99 .19* 2.0 5.5 0.28 .09
Empathy
Fantasy 8.3 1.6 2.88 ⫺.16† 8.2 3.6 0.98 ⫺.10
Personal distress 0.0 1.6 0.77 .08 30.6 9.1 7.73 ⫺.27**
Perspective taking 10.4 39.7 11.83 .33*** 28.6 21.8 0.63 ⫺.08
Emotional concern 2.1 11.1 3.32 .17† 69.4 36.4 11.32 ⫺.33**
Self-focus 95.8 71.4 10.98 ⫺.32*** 71.4 92.7 8.21 .28**
Other focus 0.0 30.2 17.47 .40*** 53.1 29.1 6.18 ⫺.24*

Note. Percentages represent the proportion of narratives codable on that dimension that were coded as having the feature present.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

(12.7% vs. 72.9%) and to state that they are still angry (11.1% state that the offender could not help his or her actions (9.5% vs.
vs. 39.6%). In general, forgiving and unforgiving victims de- 0%). In contrast, unforgiving victims were more likely than for-
scribed their offender’s motives similarly. Three important differ- giving victims to portray the offenders’ actions as immoral (27.1%
ences did emerge in victims’ descriptions of motives, however. vs. 4.8%) and deliberately cruel or harmful (29.2% vs. 7.9%),
Victims who forgave were more likely than unforgiving victims to suggesting that a potential stumbling block to forgiveness is vic-
FORGIVENESS NARRATIVES 681

tims’ perception that the offender behaved immorally or unjustly. regret (89.8%), self-blame (89.8%), and guilt (73.5%) than did
Unforgiveness, therefore, may be related to a moral stance that self-forgiving offenders (34.5%, 32.7%, and 40.0%, respectively).
justice should prevail over forgiveness. In contrast, offenders who forgave themselves were more likely to
Victims who forgave their offender were more likely than report an improved relationship with their victim (4.2%
unforgiving victims to mention the offender’s apology (12.7% vs. 16.4%). In addition, offenders who had forgiven themselves
vs. 2.1%) and attempts to make amends (17.5% vs. 2.1%), their were more likely to portray the victim’s response as an overreac-
own attempts to make amends (14.3% vs. 2.1%), and feelings of tion (0% vs. 18.2%), report that the victim provoked the offense
love (7.9% vs. 0%). In contrast, unforgiving victims were more (12.2% vs. 30.9%), and describe feelings of anger about the
likely than forgiving victims to describe the offender as making offense (4.1% vs. 25.5%). In this way, offenders who forgave
excuses for the offense (20.8% vs. 7.9%). themselves implicated the victim in sharing the blame for the
To the extent that apologies and making amends can be viewed offense.
as signs that offenders and victims achieve closure regarding an Similar to victims’ accounts, offenders who forgave themselves
offense, victims who forgave admitted more closure than victims were more likely to mention an apology (23.6%) and making
who did not forgive. Similarly, forgiving victims were less likely amends (20.0%) than were offenders who did not forgive them-
than unforgiving victims to place the offense in a longer time selves (6.7% and 6.8%, respectively), indicating offenders’ at-
frame with lasting negative consequences. In fact, forgiveness, tempts to close the incident. Indeed, whereas offenders in
relative to unforgiveness, was associated with positive conse- Baumeister et al.’s (1990) study described offenses with a limited
quences and victims’ denial of lasting negative consequences. time frame and consequences, this tended to be true only for
Note, however, that even when offenders were forgiven, a large offenders who had forgiven themselves in our study. Thus, for-
proportion of victims referred to long-term events with the of- giving themselves may have been a way for offenders to diminish
fender (33.3%) and multiple provocations (31.7%), their present the gravity of their offense and experience closure regarding the
circumstances (49.2%), and angry affect (50.8%) and portrayed offense.
their anger as justified (76.2%). These findings suggest that some In the previous section we noted that victims’ narratives of
victims who forgive do not fully relinquish their victim status but forgiveness, compared with unforgiveness, were more likely to
instead are careful to describe both their victim status and their include empathy features such as perspective taking and emotional
forgiveness of the offender. concern for their offender. When narrators wrote as offenders,
Finally, we coded empathy using Davis’s (1983) description of however, the relationship between forgiveness and empathy fea-
four types of empathy: fantasy, personal distress, perspective tak- tures was reversed. Offenders who had not forgiven themselves
ing, and emotional concern. Victims who forgave their offender demonstrated more emotional concern for their victims (69.4%)
were more likely to take the offender’s perspective (39.7%) and to and other focus (53.1%) than did offenders who had forgiven
express emotional concern for their offender (11.1%) compared themselves (36.4% and 29.1%, respectively). Unforgiving offend-
with victims who wrote about unforgiven offenses (10.4% ers also experienced more personal distress as a result of thinking
and 2.1%, respectively). Coders also judged narratives for the about their victims than did offenders who forgave themselves
extent of self- and other focus using two dichotomous, independent (30.6% vs. 9.1%). These findings suggest that empathy for their
criteria (e.g., self-focused vs. minimal self-focus). Narratives of victim’s experience may make offenders’ self-forgiveness more
unforgiven offenses evidenced more self-focus than did forgive- difficult. In contrast, offenders who forgave themselves demon-
ness narratives (95.8% vs. 71.4%), and coders more often judged strated more self-focus (92.7%) than did offenders who did not
forgiveness narratives as other focused than they did unforgiveness forgive themselves (71.4%).
narratives (30.2% vs. 0%). These results are consistent with pre- Descriptions of offenses. Across all narratives, most of the
vious research on the relationship between forgiveness and empa- offenses involved friends (34.4%), family (34.0%), and intimates
thy (e.g., McCullough et al., 1997), in which forgiveness was (19.1%), rather than strangers (2.3%) or acquaintances (10.2%).
associated with victims’ ability to understand the offender’s Victims and offenders differed in the offenses they selected to
perspective. describe. When writing as victims, narrators were more likely to
Offender narratives. Of the 104 offender narratives, 55 de- select offenses involving threats to self-esteem or worth (63.1%)
scribed an offense in which the narrator had forgiven him- or compared with offenders (11.5%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 60.42, p ⬍
herself, and 49 described an offense in which the narrator had not .001, ␾ ⫽ .53; this effect was stronger for unforgiven (␾ ⫽ .61)
forgiven him- or herself. These offender narratives differed dra- than forgiven (␾ ⫽ .46) offenses. In addition, coders evaluated
matically from the victim stories (see Table 4, Offender narratives offenses according to blameworthiness (accidental/negligent, in-
columns). Of the 41 coded items comparing forgiven and not- tentional). When writing as victims, narrators portrayed offenses
forgiven narratives shown in Table 3, only 9 (22.0%) of the coded as intentional (61.3%) rather than accidental or negligent (38.7%);
items had similar results for victim and offender narratives. however, when writing as offenders, narrators’ offenses were
It is interesting that whether offenders forgave themselves or did equally intentional (49.0%) or accidental/negligent (51.0%), ␹2(1,
not forgive themselves was unrelated to reports of their victim’s N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 3.25, p ⫽ .072, ␾ ⫽ .12.
forgiveness (16.4% vs. 15.6%, ns). The motives and intentions Forgiveness instructions were more directly related to narrators’
identified by offenders differed as a function of whether they descriptions of the severity of harm following the offense. Regard-
forgave themselves. Narrators who did not forgive themselves less of victim versus offender instructions, offenses written in
were more likely to portray their offense as arbitrary or senseless response to no-forgiveness instructions were more often coded as
(22.4%) compared with narrators who had forgiven themselves moderate or severe harm (79.6%) rather than mild harm (20.4%),
(7.3%). Offenders who did not forgive themselves reported more whereas offenses written in response to forgiveness instructions
682 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

were equally mild (48.7%) or moderate to severe harm (51.3%), Discussion


␹2(1, N ⫽ 215) ⫽ 18.59, p ⬍ .001, ␾ ⫽ ⫺.29. Two possibilities
exist: Either moderate to severe offenses are more difficult to The results of this study demonstrate that individuals’ percep-
forgive, or, once forgiven, offenses that may have been severe tions of interpersonal offenses depend both on their role as victim
were portrayed as mild in their harm. One clue to resolving this or offender and on whether they have forgiven or not forgiven the
time– order confounding is found in narrators’ descriptions of their offense. Previous narrative research demonstrated that offenders
emotional arousal at the time of the offense (none/mild vs. mod- are motivated to portray their offenses as over and isolated, with no
erate/severe). Victims’ arousal was more likely to be coded as lingering negative consequences, and to perceive their motives as
moderate or severe (76.4%) compared with offenders’ arousal justifiable (Baumeister et al., 1990). Victims, in contrast, portray
(61.5%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ 214) ⫽ 5.51, p ⫽ .019, ␾ ⫽ .16. This effect offenses as open and continuing, perhaps as a way to maintain their
was largely due to the difference between victims’ and offenders’ victim status. In general, our results replicated these findings and
arousal in stories of forgiveness: Forgiving victims were more extended them by exploring the role of forgiveness in people’s
likely to describe moderate to severe arousal at the time of the understanding of their interpersonal conflicts. A central question of
offense (75.8%) than offenders were (56.4%), ␹2(1, N ⫽ this study was whether forgiveness was related to differences in
117) ⫽ 4.96, p ⫽ .026, ␾ ⫽ .21. This finding suggests that offenders’ and victims’ narratives of past offenses. In addition, we
forgiveness may lead victims to portray offenses initially experi- also analyzed narratives to reveal whether participants portrayed
enced as moderately or severely arousing as ultimately mild in forgiveness as conditional (i.e., dependent on apologies, restitu-
their harm. tion) and whether narrators’ conceptualizations of forgiveness
could be characterized as total or hollow (Baumeister et al., 1999).
Dispositional Empathy and Narrative Features Clearly, forgiveness narratives were associated with narrators’
descriptions of more positive outcomes and affect, regardless of
To increase the reliability of measuring the relationship between whether the narrator wrote as a victim or an offender. Forgiveness
dispositional empathy and narrative features, we aggregated nar- narratives were more likely to include indications that the offender
rative features to create two continuous variables measuring and victim had achieved closure about the offense, as evidenced by
empathy-related constructs. Other-focused empathy comprised apologies, descriptions of happy endings, and feelings of peace. In
coded narrative features of perspective taking and emotional con- contrast, narrators who described offenses that had not been for-
cern, and the benign-attributions aggregate comprised statements given were more likely to describe negative consequences, refer to
indicating that the offender’s actions could not be helped, were present circumstances, and describe continuing anger that was
externally caused, and were justified. There was a trend for high justified. Thus, in general, narratives of forgiveness had the ap-
dispositional-empathy (⫹1 SD) participants to include more other- pearance of closure; the offense was over and in the past. Narra-
focused empathy in their narratives (M ⫽ 0.63) than did low tives of unforgiven offenses continued to remain open, with neg-
dispositional-empathy (⫺1 SD) participants (M ⫽ 0.47), b ⫽ .01,
ative consequences and affect lingering to influence narrators’
SE ⫽ .005, t(213) ⫽ 1.94, p ⫽ .053, ␤ ⫽ .12. Furthermore, high
present circumstances. In addition, 25% of victims extended the
dispositional-empathy participants made more benign attributions
influence of the offense into the future, reporting they “would
for the offense (M ⫽ 0.90) relative to low dispositional-empathy
never forgive” their offender.
participants (M ⫽ 0.64), b ⫽ .01, SE ⫽ .005, t(211) ⫽ 2.83, p ⫽
A central question in the psychology of forgiveness is whether
.005, ␤ ⫽ .17.
forgiveness is conditional; that is, does forgiveness follow only
However, the relationship between dispositional empathy and
after the offender expresses regret, apologizes, or makes amends?
benign attributions was moderated by forgiveness and narrator
Although forgiveness narratives were more likely to describe the
role. High dispositional-empathy victims who had forgiven their
offender’s apology and attempts to make amends than unforgive-
offender made more benign attributions (M ⫽ 0.72) relative to low
ness narratives were, these features were present in fewer than
dispositional-empathy victims who had forgiven (M ⫽ 0.16), b ⫽
20% of the forgiveness narratives. Thus, apologies and restitution
.03, SE ⫽ .01, t(211) ⫽ 2.70, p ⫽ .007, ␤ ⫽ .34. Thus, disposi-
tionally empathic victims showed an ability to consider benign did not appear to be critical features of the forgiveness process for
motives for the forgiven offense. Surprisingly, we observed a many narrators. Similarly, victims rarely described the offenders’
similar effect of dispositional empathy on the attributions of of- regret for the offense, regardless of whether the offense was
fenders who had not forgiven themselves. High dispositional- forgiven or not forgiven. Furthermore, when offenders experi-
empathy offenders were more likely to make benign attributions enced regret for their actions, it was associated with lack of
for their behavior— despite their failure to forgive themselves— self-forgiveness rather than forgiveness. These findings suggest
relative to low dispositional-empathy offenders (M ⫽ 1.33 that although apologies and restitution are more frequently asso-
and 0.95, respectively), b ⫽ .02, SE ⫽ .01, t(211) ⫽ 2.20, p ⫽ ciated with forgiveness than unforgiveness, these conditions may
.029, ␤ ⫽ .23. There was no effect of dispositional empathy on the not be necessary for forgiveness to occur.2
attributions of unforgiving victims and self-forgiving offenders. In
sum, dispositional empathy was directly related to the presence of 2
R. Baumeister (personal communication, September 17, 2001) offered
empathy in narratives, and this relationship was not moderated by an interesting interpretation of these data: Assuming that narrators attempt
narrators’ role or forgiveness. In contrast, the relationship between to present themselves favorably and seek moral credit for forgiving, one
dispositional empathy and benign attributions for the offense de- would not expect victims to portray apologies and restitution from the
pended on narrators’ role and whether the offense was forgiven or offender in their narratives. To the extent that victims feel obligated to
not forgiven. forgive when these conditions are present, their moral standing is not
FORGIVENESS NARRATIVES 683

These conclusions are consistent with recent findings for a Group, 1991; Enright, Eastin, Golden, Sarinopoulos, & Freedman,
largely Christian student sample who were asked to indicate their 1992). Thus, rather than conceptualizing forgiveness as either total
agreement with several statements about forgiveness (Kanz, 2000). or hollow, a process approach characterizes individuals’ progress
Two-thirds of students in that sample believed that an apology is in stages of forgiveness. Given that our instructions asked narrators
not necessary for forgiveness. Together, these findings support a to describe forgiven offenses, however, we assumed that victims
conceptualization of forgiveness as a choice or act on the part of believed they had forgiven their offender (indeed, 84.1% explicitly
the forgiver that does not necessarily depend on the offender’s stated they had forgiven their offender). Our analysis of narratives
actions or repentance (e.g., Enright, Gassin, & Wu, 1992; Hope, using dichotomous coding for the presence or absence of features
1987). In contrast, empirical studies have indicated that victims of related to interpersonal conflict and forgiveness precluded a se-
interpersonal offenses feel more favorably toward offenders and quential analysis of the process from conflict to forgiveness. Ad-
are less likely to retaliate against them when the offenders offer ditional research that asks narrators to describe their process of
apologies (e.g., Ohbuchi, Kameda, & Agarie, 1989). Thus, what forgiveness and codes features based on if–then contingencies
individuals say about apologies, in narratives or in response to (e.g., if offender apologizes, then victim’s anger is eliminated)
statements about forgiveness, may not correspond well to how they would help to identify processes related to forgiveness (e.g., Fehr,
actually respond to apologies. Additional research that compares Baldwin, Collins, Patterson, & Benditt, 1999).
individuals’ beliefs about the role of apologies and restitution to Finally, additional theoretical and empirical work on victims’
their actual responses will help to clarify the extent to which forgiveness must address the extent to which it is desirable for
forgiveness is conditional on certain behaviors of the offender. victims to exclude anger and details of the offense from their
personal narratives of the forgiven conflict and relationship with
Victims’ Forgiveness of Offenders the offender. Kanz’s (2000) study of Christian students revealed
that three fourths of the students believed it is possible to be
The narratives of interpersonal offenses were more complex simultaneously angry and forgiving, and two thirds believed that it
than an open/closed analysis would indicate. Victims’ narratives is not necessary to forget the hurt when an offense is forgiven.
indicated aspects of closure as well as self-enhancing motives. Similarly, our victims may not have been motivated to maintain
Victims who forgave, for example, often described and justified their victim status in relation to their offender but, instead, were
their anger and mentioned multiple provocations and the negative motivated to remember that they are vulnerable to hurt. Continued
consequences of the offense. Similar to unforgiving victims, for- anger at the offense, rather than the offender, may protect individ-
givers were more likely than offenders to describe the offender’s uals from additional harm. Several theorists have argued that
motives as arbitrary or senseless rather than justified. Thus, some forgiveness must not be confused with forgetting (e.g., Enright &
victims in our study may have sought to enhance their moral Coyle, 1998; Parsons, 1988; Worthington, 1999); indeed, some
standing by maintaining their victim status while proclaiming researchers have argued that forgetting the past harm may place
forgiveness. Such forgiveness, however, may be characterized as individuals at risk (e.g., Fow, 1996; Freedman, 1998; Katz, Street,
expedient forgiveness (Trainer, 1981). & Arias, 1997).
It is important to note that participants were asked to describe
their whole story; therefore, victims who forgave may have felt
required to describe all events that led to their anger and the Offenders’ Self-Forgiveness
negative consequence of the offense and to justify their anger, even Although offenders who forgave themselves often expressed
if these features were not important in their current thinking about regret and self-blame, they also implicated their victim in causing
the forgiven offense. Thus, rather than being motivated to maintain the offense more than did offenders who did not forgive them-
their victim status, while at the same time portraying themselves as selves. Self-forgiving offenders also described their own anger and
forgivers, some victims in our study may have tried to justify that tended to justify their actions more than offenders who did not
the offense was serious enough to warrant their effort at forgive themselves. In general, narratives of offenders who for-
forgiveness. gave themselves were self-focused and portrayed victims as de-
Slightly more than 10% of the victims who forgave their of- serving what they got. These offenders seemed to achieve self-
fender described that they were still angry. Definitions of forgive- forgiveness relatively easily. Similar to Baumeister et al.’s (1990)
ness frequently refer to victims’ “letting go” of anger and hurt in findings, offenders in our study often tried to usurp the victim
favor of conciliatory responses (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1999; status by portraying their offense as relatively minor and not
Enright and The Human Development Study Group, 1991; Mc- deserving of the victim’s angry reaction.
Cullough & Worthington, 1994; Scobie & Scobie, 1998). Thus, we Perhaps the narrators who expressed the most difficulty in our
might characterize these victims’ forgiveness as hollow because of study were those who could not forgive themselves. Many offend-
their continued anger toward the offender (Baumeister et al., ers expressed regret and guilt and blamed themselves; some re-
1999). Another interpretation is that these victims have made the ported they would never forgive themselves despite their efforts to
commitment to forgive, but may be at only the beginning of the apologize or make amends for their wrong, even when their victim
forgiveness process (Enright & The Human Development Study had forgiven them. These offenders seemed at a loss for how to let
go of their past offense and move toward forgiveness. Previous
narrative research revealed that guilt can strengthen interpersonal
enhanced greatly when they forgive. However, if apologies and restitution relationships if it leads to behavior change following a transgres-
are not readily apparent, the victims’ moral standing is enhanced when they sion (Baumeister et al., 1995; Leith & Baumeister, 1998). In
describe their forgiveness. contrast, shame—in which negative affect and evaluations focus
684 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

on the self rather than the offensive behavior—limits individuals’ An unexpected finding was that high-empathy offenders ap-
efforts to deal effectively with their offense and the consequences peared to justify their behavior by offering benign attributions
for their relationship (Leith & Baumeister, 1998; Lewis, 1987; more frequently (relative to low-empathy offenders) when they
Tangney, Wagner, Fletcher, & Gramzow, 1992). Leith and had not forgiven themselves. As noted above, narratives of offend-
Baumeister observed that shame was associated with personal- ers who forgave themselves were associated with less empathy for
distress empathy, in which individuals focus on their own anxiety the victim. Dispositionally empathic offenders may find it difficult
and distress in response to their empathy for their victims’ suffer- to forgive themselves if they see self-forgiveness as being at odds
ing. In our study, nearly one third of offenders who did not forgive with their usual empathic stance. What may be portrayed in these
themselves wrote about their own distress in response to their offenders’ narratives is their effort to obtain closure while remain-
empathy for their victims, suggesting that one obstacle to self- ing empathic toward the victim. In contrast to their victim role
forgiveness is feelings of distress and shame about oneself, rather when forgiving others, where their benign attributions may repre-
than guilt for the offending actions. sent a more advanced stage of forgiveness, offenders’ benign
attributions for their behavior may represent an early stage of true
self-forgiveness. That is, perhaps high dispositional-empathy of-
Empathy and Forgiveness fenders who have not yet forgiven themselves grapple with the
dilemma of choosing between the two competing prosocial re-
Previous investigations of forgiveness demonstrated that for- sponses of forgiveness and empathy and attempt to resolve their
giveness is more likely when victims empathize with the offender, conundrum by considering mitigating factors that would permit
through either taking the offender’s perspective or feeling emo- them to forgive themselves while acknowledging their culpability
tional concern for the victim (e.g., McCullough et al., 1997, 1998; and maintaining a high level of empathy for their victim. The
Worthington, 1998, 1999). The results of this investigation sup- relationships among dispositional empathy, situational empathy,
ported the association between situational empathy and victims’ attributions, and forgiveness are obviously complex and merit
forgiveness of offenders. Victims who forgave were more likely to further investigation.
demonstrate perspective taking and emotional concern for their
offender than were victims who did not forgive. However, the
opposite occurred when narrators wrote as offenders: Self- Limitations and Future Directions
forgiveness was associated with less empathy for the victim and The sample for this investigation was predominantly students
often was associated with righteous self-focus and victim blaming. from a private, Catholic university, but we also included narratives
It is easy to wonder whether their victims forgave these offenders from members of the surrounding community to increase the
as easily as they seemed to forgive themselves. diversity of the sample and offenses. We sampled a range of
To the extent that both empathy for others and forgiveness are interpersonal offenses, including betrayals of trust, infidelities,
desirable prosocial responses following interpersonal offenses, a drug- and alcohol-related deaths, and social humiliations. Our
dilemma exists when offenders seek to forgive themselves. Em- reading of these narratives suggest that experiences of forgiveness
pathy that results in personal distress and shame is associated with (and unforgiveness) likely differ across the life span. Our limited
less self-forgiveness, which may prevent the offender from expe- sampling across the life span precluded analyses that compared
riencing closure following the offense. Thus, interventions aimed narratives according to age, victim or offender role, and forgive-
at helping offenders to forgive themselves should focus on empa- ness versus no-forgiveness instructions. Additional research that
thy for the victim and guilt for the particular behaviors associated examines developmental aspects of the forgiveness process, such
with the offense, but help offenders to prevent personal distress as the frequency of forgiveness, changes in the types of offenses
and shame in response to their actions. that require forgiveness, self-forgiveness, and changes in people’s
Although the empathy evident in the narratives appears to have conceptualizations of what it means to forgive, will enhance our
been more situational than dispositional, we observed some sup- understanding of forgiveness processes.
port for dispositional empathy as an important moderator in the Clearly, victims and offenders differed in the offenses they
forgiveness process. Overall, the narratives reflected individual chose to describe in our study. In general, victims tended to select
differences in empathy, as high dispositional-empathy individuals offenses that were self-threatening and intentional. Thus, it is
tended to evidence more narrative empathy, regardless of their role difficult to determine whether victim and offender narratives dif-
(victim vs. offender), and whether or not forgiveness had occurred. fered because of the role of the narrator or because of the different
Dispositional empathy was also associated with closure-promoting offenses described. In addition, coders rated narratives of forgive-
attributional processes such as asserting benign motives for the ness—regardless of whether they represented victim or offender
offender’s behavior; however, this association was context depen- perspectives—as less severe in their consequences. Features asso-
dent. As expected, there was a strong link between dispositional ciated with forgiveness, such as positive outcomes and affect, may
empathy and the presence of benign attributions in victims’ nar- have been portrayed more frequently because the offenses were
ratives depicting forgiveness of an offender: Empathic victims less severe, rather than as a result of the forgiveness process
more frequently identified nonmaleficent causes of the offender’s (although three quarters of forgiving victims did portray their
behavior. Thus, in this context, dispositional empathy may pro- emotional arousal at the time of the offense as moderate or severe).
mote successful closure and may be predictive of true forgiveness In addition, it is likely that the forgiveness process differs depend-
or a more advanced stage of the forgiveness process than that ing on the severity of the offense (Scobie & Scobie, 1998). Future
achieved by the low dispositionally empathic victims who express research that removes the confounding between the type of of-
forgiveness. fense, narrator role, and forgiveness will help to clarify how
FORGIVENESS NARRATIVES 685

individuals forgive themselves and others for different types of narratives about guilt: Role in action control and interpersonal relation-
offenses. ships. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 173–198.
Although personal narratives may help to reveal individuals’ Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim and
motives and beliefs about events in their lives, they do not allow perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narra-
researchers to examine cognitive processes associated with the tives about anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
994 –1005.
encoding and retrieval of these events. However, our goal in
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
examining participants’ narratives of forgiveness was to describe (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
their understanding of forgiveness and to assess how well this Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evi-
understanding matches theoretical and empirical work on forgive- dence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and
ness to date. Subjective, personal accounts of interpersonal con- Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
flicts do not necessarily reveal the truth about what happened, but Davis, M. H. (1996). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Boulder,
do offer valuable information about what people think forgiveness CO: Westview Press.
is and how they think it occurs. An additional limitation of narra- Draycott, S., & Dabbs, A. (1998). Cognitive dissonance: I. An overview of
tive data is that we are unable to identify causal mechanisms the literature and its integration into theory and practice of clinical
underlying the forgiveness process. Despite this limitation, how- psychology. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 341–353.
ever, we gain external validity in our descriptions of forgiveness Emmons, R. A. (2000). Personality and forgiveness. In M. E. McCullough,
K. I. Pargament, & C. E. Thoresen (Eds.), Forgiveness: Theory, re-
by examining participants’ stories of the important conflicts in
search, and practice (pp. 156 –175). New York: Guilford.
their lives. Narrative research, in conjunction with experimental
Enright, R. D., & Coyle, C. T. (1998). Researching the process of forgive-
work, will improve our understanding of the forgiveness process. ness within psychological interventions. In E. L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.),
One goal of this research was to compare narrators’ conceptu- Law of life symposia series: Vol. 1. Dimensions of forgiveness: Psycho-
alizations of forgiveness and unforgiveness to current theories of logical research and theological perspectives (pp. 139 –161). Radnor,
forgiveness using narrators’ own words, on the basis of their own PA: Templeton Foundation Press.
experiences of interpersonal conflict. We successfully demon- Enright, R. D., Eastin, D. L., Golden, S., Sarinopoulos, I., & Freedman, S.
strated that personal narratives of forgiveness often do not include (1992). Interpersonal forgiveness within the helping professions: An
apologies and making amends and that forgiveness stories are attempt to resolve differences of opinion. Counseling and Values, 36,
more likely to portray the offense as over and isolated, and with 84 –103.
positive consequences. However, forgiveness stories also de- Enright, R. D., Gassin, E. A., & Wu, C. (1992). Forgiveness: A develop-
scribed anger and negative consequences. Future research that mental view. Journal of Moral Education, 21, 99 –114.
Enright, R. D., & The Human Development Study Group. (1991). The
examines the extent to which negative and positive cognition,
moral development of forgiveness. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz
affect, and behavior coexist as part of forgiveness will help to
(Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Vol. 1. Theory
clarify this complex process. (pp. 123–152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Enright, R. D., & The Human Development Study Group. (1996). Coun-
seling within the forgiveness triad: On forgiving, receiving forgiveness,
Conclusion and self-forgiveness. Counseling and Values, 40, 107–126.
Enright, R. D., & Zell, R. L. (1989). Problems encountered when we
Accumulating evidence suggests that how individuals portray
forgive one another. Journal of Psychology & Christianity, 8, 52– 60.
interpersonal conflicts depends on their role as victim or offender. Fehr, B., Baldwin, M., Collins, L., Patterson, S., & Benditt, R. (1999).
The findings of this investigation demonstrated that whether of- Anger in close relationships: An interpersonal script analysis. Person-
fenses are forgiven or not forgiven was also strongly related to ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 299 –312.
participants’ inclusion of narrative features and moderated the Fow, N. R. (1996). The phenomenology of forgiveness and reconciliation.
influence of narrators’ role. Personal narratives offer a rich source Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 27, 219 –233.
of information about how people conceptualize and experience Freedman, S. (1998). Forgiveness and reconciliation: The importance of
forgiveness in their everyday lives and should help to guide understanding how they differ. Counseling and Values, 42, 200 –216.
theoretical developments in the growing area of empirical forgive- Gergen, K. J., & Gergen, M. M. (1988). Narrative and the self as relation-
ness research. ship. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 21, pp. 17–56). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Gonzales, M. H., Manning, D. J., & Haugen, J. A. (1992). Explaining our
References sins: Factors influencing offender accounts and anticipated victim re-
sponses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 958 –971.
Al-Mabuk, R. H., Enright, R. D., & Cardis, P. A. (1995). Forgiveness Haber, J. G. (1991). Forgiveness. Savage, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
education with parentally love-deprived late adolescents. Journal of Hope, D. (1987). The healing paradox of forgiveness. Psychotherapy, 24,
Moral Education, 24, 427– 444. 240 –244.
Baumeister, R. F., Exline, J. J., & Sommer, K. L. (1999). The victim role, Kanz, J. E. (2000). How do people conceptualize and use forgiveness? The
grudge theory, and two dimensions of forgiveness. In E. L. Worthington, Forgiveness Attitudes Questionnaire. Counseling and Values, 44, 174 –
Jr. (Ed.), Dimensions of forgiveness: Psychological research and theo- 188.
logical perspectives (pp. 79 –104). Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Katz, J., Street, A., & Arias, I. (1997). Individual differences in self-
Press. appraisals and responses to dating violence scenarios. Violence and
Baumeister, R. F., & Newman, L. S. (1994). How stories make sense of Victims, 12, 265–276.
personal experiences: Motives that shape autobiographical narratives. Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 676 – 690. narratives of interpersonal conflict: Guilt-prone people are better at
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1995). Personal perspective-taking. Journal of Personality, 66, 1–37.
686 ZECHMEISTER AND ROMERO

Lewis, H. B. (1987). The superego experience: Shame and guilt. In H. B. preliminary empirical evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Lewis (Ed.), Sex and the superego: Psychic war in men and women (pp. chology, 60, 53–78.
185–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schultz, A., & Baumeister, R. F. (1999). The language of defense: Lin-
McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? guistic patterns in narratives of transgressions. Journal of Language and
Journal of Personality, 63, 365–396. Social Psychology, 18, 269 –286.
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A Scobie, E. D., & Scobie, G. E. W. (1998). Damaging events: The perceived
contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, need for forgiveness. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 28,
295–321. 373– 401.
McCullough, M. E. (2000). Forgiveness as human strength: Theory, mea- Subkoviak, M. J., Enright, R. D., Wu, C., Gassin, E. A., Freedman, S.,
surement, and links to well-being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy- Olson, L. M., & Sarinopoulos, I. (1995). Measuring interpersonal for-
chology, 19, 43–55. giveness in late adolescence and middle adulthood. Journal of Adoles-
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Jr., cence, 18, 641– 655.
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed
Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. A. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close
into anger? The relation of shame and guilt to anger and self-reported
relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal of
aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 669 – 675.
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586 –1603.
Time Magazine. (1999, April 5). Should all be forgiven? pp. 55–58.
McCullough, M. E., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. (1994). Models of interper-
Trainer, M. F. (1981). Forgiveness: Intrinsic, role-expected, expedient, in
sonal forgiveness and their application to counseling: Review and cri-
the context of divorce. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston Uni-
tique. Counseling and Values, 39, 2–14. versity, Boston, MA.
McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Rachal, K. C. (1997). Vitz, P. C., & Mango, P. (1997). Kernbergian psychodynamics and reli-
Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of Personality and gious aspects of the forgiveness process. Journal of Psychology and
Social Psychology, 73, 321–336. Theology, 25, 72– 80.
North, J. (1987). Wrongdoing and forgiveness. Philosophy, 62, 499 –508. Worthington, E. L., Jr. (1998). An empathy– humility– commitment model
Ohbuchi, K., Kameda, M., & Agarie, N. (1989). Apology as aggression of forgiveness applied to family dyads. Journal of Family Therapy, 20,
control: Its role in mediating appraisal of and response to harm. Journal 59 –76.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 219 –227. Worthington, E. L., Jr. (Ed.). (1999). Dimensions of forgiveness: Psycho-
Parsons, R. D. (1988). Forgiving—not forgetting. Psychotherapy Pa- logical research and theological perspectives. Philadelphia: Templeton
tient, 5, 259 –273. Foundation Press.
Power, F. C. (1994). Piaget on the moral development of forgiveness:
Identity or reciprocity? Commentary. Human Development, 37, 81– 85. Received July 11, 2001
Rusbult, C. E., Verette, J., Whitney, G. A., Slovik, L. F., & Lipkus, I. Revision received October 4, 2001
(1991). Accommodation processes in close relationships: Theory and Accepted October 17, 2001 䡲

S-ar putea să vă placă și