Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Intrinsic Motivation in Museums:

Why Does One Want to Learn?

Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi
and
Kim Hemanson
The University of Chicago

One often meets successful adults, professionals, or survived i f we did not find the process of making
scientists w h o recall that their lifelong vocational sense of our environinent pleasurably rewarding. But
interest was first sparked by a visit to a museum. In this spontaneous propensity is often exunguiiilied as
these accounts the encounter with a real, concrete children's desire to learn is rechanneled in new direc-
object from a different world - an exotic animal, a tions by societal goals and expectations The abstract,
strange dress, a beautiful artifact - is the kernel from externally imposed tasks children confront in school
which a n entire career of learning grew For others undermine the motivation to learn for many, often
with an already developed curiosity about some field for the rest of life. Research indicates that the natural
such as zoology, anthropology, or art, the museum niotivation to learn can be rekindled by supportive
provided an essential link in the cultivation of knowl- environments (Deci et al. 1981: Deci 1992: McCombs
edge - a place where information lo'it its abstractness 1 9 9 1 ) , by meaningful activities ( M a e h r 1984;
and became concrete. In either case, many people McConihs 1991); by being freed of anxiety. fear, and
ascribe powerful motivation to a museum visit, claim- other negative mental states (Diener and
ing that their desire to learn more about some aspect 1980; McComhs 1991); and when the challe
of the world was directly caused by it. the task meet the person's skills (Csikszent
Granted that these accounts of "crystallizing expe- 1990a, 1990h).
riences" (Waiters a n d Gaidner 1986) attributed to The view of learning taken in this naper is broadei
museums might often be embellished and exaggerat- than mere knowledge acquisition, although that is
ed in retrospect, i t would be rash to dismiss them certainly an important part of it. In our view, learning
entirely, for the fascination of museums seems to be a involves an open process of interaction with the envi-
very real psychological phenomenon. The question ronment. This experiential process develops and
rather becomes. How do museums motivate viewers expands the self, allowing one to discover aspects of
to learn? Is there a unique, sui generis "museum expe- oneself that were previously unknown. Thus the
rience" that helps viewers start on the long journey of learning experience involves the whole person, not
learning? How do museums present information in a only the intellectual but the sensory and emotional
meaningful way, a way that deepens a person's expe- faculties as well. And when complex information is
rience a n d promotes further learning? To begin presented in a way that is enjoyable - intrinsically
answering these questions, it will be useful to review rewarding - the person will be motivated to pursue
what we know about human motivation in relation to further learning.
learning.
Children are born with a desire for knowledge, and
some of the most stupendous feats of learning - to Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivatio
walk, talk, get along with others, to take care of one- Human action is motivated by
self - are accomplished without seeming effort in the kinds of rewards: extrinsic a
first few years of life. It would be difficult to see how a extrinsically motivated when the anticipated re
species as dependent on learning as we are could have come from outside the activity. In this case. p
mance is simply a means to some other end -
obtain praise or to avoid punishment, to get a degree,
The authors wish to acknowledge the helpfulsuggestions of or to live up to societal expectations. A person acts for
the participants at the Annapolis conference, and especially the sake of intrinsic rewards when the performance
Margie Marino and Mark Harvey for their assistance with itself is worth doing for its own sake. even in the
the literature on museum visitors research. absence of external rewards. For example, most
I
sports, games, and artistic activities are intrinsically rely almost exclusively on intrinsic rewards. How,
motivated, because except for a few professionals, one then, can intrinsic rewards be made a part of the
gets n o rewards from performing them beyond the museum experience?
experience itself. Usually we are motivated by both
extrinsic and intrinsic rewards at the same time. I
might go to work every day primarily because, if 1 The Origins of Intrinsic Motivation
don't, I will get fired, and I need the job to pay my Psychologists began to write about intrinsic motiva-
bills. But if in addition I also enjoy my job, the quality tion in the late 1950s, when some researchers con-
of my life will improve, and I am likely to get better at cluded that the basic physiological needs for food and
what I am doing. security did not seem to explain why rats explored
This general principle holds for learning as well. new territory, were willing to work just to see novel
Most learning in schools is extrinsically motivated sights, and experimented with challenging tasks
(Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984). The acquisition (Csikszentmihalyi and Nakamura 1989). These find-
of knowledge is rarely enjoyed for its own sake, and ings suggested that the basic list of "drives" had to be
relatively few young people would continue to learn expanded by adding novelty, curiosity, and compe-
in schools in the absence of parental and societal pres- tence drives (Butler 1957; Harlow 1953; Montgomery
sures. Because of the stress on external incentives in 1954; White 1959). More recently, Deci (1992) stated
formal education, intrinsic motivation in schools has that the inherent psychological needs are competence,
been rarely studied. Classic examples are studies that self-determination, and relatedness (see also white
seek to find ways to make the task of learning subject 1959). In any case, the desire to learn for its own sake
matter and participating in classroom activities more appears to be a natural motive built into the central
intrinsically motivating (see, e.g., Benware & Deci nervous system. A species could not survive long if it
1984; Lepper and Cordova 1992). did not find pleasure in processing information
Learning is intrinsically motivated when it is spon- (Butler 1957; Hebb 1955; Miller 1983, p.111;
taneous. The most clear examples of intrinsic motiva- Montgomery 1954; Tiger 1992; Csikszentmihalyi
. tion may be found watching children at play. When 1993). As Miller wrote, "The mind survives by ingest-
playing, children pay attention because they want to, ing information."
because they find the information interesting and Clearly, however, not all information is equally
important in its own right. People are intrinsically attractive. Because a person cannot process more than
motivated when they are freely expressing themselves a limited amount of information at a time (Kahneman
by doing what interests them (decharms 1968; Deci 1973; Hasher and Zacks 1979; Csikszentmihalyi 1978,
and Ryan 1985; White 1959). Dweck (1986) and oth- 1993), environmental stimuli compete for attention
ers (Nicholls, Patashnick, and Nolen 1985; Heyman with each other. Attention is a scarce resource - per-
and Dweck 1992) describe students who are intrinsi- haps the most precious scarce resource there is
cally motivated as having "learning goals," while stu- (Simon 1969, 1978). Even though we are surrounded
dents who are extrinsically motivated have "perfor- by exponentially increasing waves of information, the
mance goals." amount of it that any person actually notices and
Students who are intrinsically motivated tend to then retains in memory may be less than it was in the
have higher achievement scores (Hidi 1990; Lepper days of our cave-dwelling ancestors, and it certainly
and Cordova 1992; Gottfried 1985), and they develop cannot be much more. Therefore what information
their aptitudes further over time (Csikszentmihalyi, we select to attend to, and how intently, is still the
Rathunde, and .Whalen 1993). Intrinsic enjoyment of most important question about learning.
learning appears to be associated with higher creativi-
ty as well (Amabile 1983, 1985). Under certain condi- Curiosity and Interest
tions, external rewards appear to undermine intrinsic In the first instance, we choose what information to
motivation and to decrease performance (see, e.g., attend to in terms of curiosity and interest. Curiosity
Deci 1971, 1972; Lepper and Greene 1978; McGraw refers to individual differences in the likelihood of
1978). When one's mind becomes focused on meeting investing psychic energy i n novel stimuli. For
an external goal or requirement, attention or "psychic instance, if we say that Mary is curious we mean that
energy" is split and no longer fully focused on the compared to other persons she will devote more effort
task at hand. to find out things she does not know (or is not sup-
Schools can afford to ignore intrinsic rewards to a posed to know). Of course we are all curious to a cer-
certain extent, because they have strong external tain degree, in that our attention is attracted by novel
incentives~grades,truant officers-to enforce learn- or unexplained stimuli - a loud noise, a sudden
ing. Of course, such extrinsically motivated learning is bustling activity, a strange animal, or a mysterious
very wasteful and inefficient. But museums, without object. It is by appealing to this universal propensity
external means to compel1 a visitor's attention, must that museums can attract the psychic energy of a visi-

68 I Pubtie tnstitutimfor Personal Learning


2

tor long enough so that a more extensive interaction, Biit because situational interest "tends to be evoked
lerhaps leading to learning, can later take place. ¥iiiddenlby something in the environment, it
Interest refers to a differential likelihood of investing has only a short-term effect and marginal infl
psychic energy i n o n e set of stimuli rather t h a n on the subject's knowledge and reference system"
mother. To say that Mary is interested in horses (Krapp, Hidl and Renninger 1992, p. 6). Thus situa-
neans that she is likely to talk about horses, to seek tional interest may not affect one's motivation to
t 3
out information about them, to think about them, learn more. In contrast, individual interest is defined
and to wish that she could feed, groom, and ride hors- as a relatively enduring preference for certain topics,
1 :s more than she does these things in relation to, say, subject areas, or activities (Hidi 1990). The pursuit of
1 dogs, cats, elephants, or gerbils. If we had n o interests, individual interests a r e usually associated w i t h
the sensory world would be completely confusing, increased knowledge, positive emotions, and t h e
cause we would literally not know where to turn. intrinsic desire to learn m o r e (Krapp, Hidi 6
s William James ([I8901 1950, p. 402) remarked Renninger 1992).
ver a hundred years ago: In Interest and Effort in Education (1913) J o h n
The moment one thinks of the matter, one Dewey described t h e importance of individual
sees how false a notion of experience that is interest. Students who are not genuinely interested in
1 which would make it tantamount to the mere learning a particular subject do not identify with the
presence t o the senses of a n outward order. material and only put out temporary, marginal effort.
Millions of items in the outward order are pre- Dewey described this type of learning as forced and
, sent to my senses which never properly enter coercive. He believed it resulted in mechanical knowl-
into my experience. Why? Because they have edge and did not effect a qualitative change in the
no interest for me. My experience is what 1 agree to individual (see also Schiefele 1991, p. 300). On the
attend to. Only those items which I notice shape other hand, individual interests are intrinsically moti-
' my mind-without selective interest, experience vating, propelling an individual to pursue further
is a n utter chaos. (James 1890,402). learning opportunities. While interests tend to be
Interests are partly universal, partly the result of individually unique, they are broadly characterized as
dividual experiences and one's idiosyncratic person- having high personal meaning (Dewey 1913; Maehr
history. Most people are interested in food when 1984; Schiefele 1991). But an activity need not be
ungry, in the opposite sex, in whatever gives them already meaningful to a person in order for it to pro-
e r or acclaim, in babies and pets. But beyond vide intrinsic rewards. For instance, John may reluc-
se few common targets, interest soon becomes tantly agree to join his friends in a game of bridge,
npredictable. Some people a r e attracted t o car expecting it to be a waste of time. Yet after a few
ngines, others to ancient Mesopotamian toothpicks, hands the stimulation provided by the game turns out
orne to maps, and others to baseball cards. to be so enjoyable that John can hardly leave the
Most researchers regard interest as a phenomenon table.
that emerges from a n individual's interaction with the Museum visitors may at first attend to an exhibit
environment, and they distinguish between situation- because of curiosity and interest. But unless the inter-
r >
a1 interest and individual interest (Krapp, Hidi, and action with the exhibit becomes intrinsically reward-
enninger 1992). Situational interest occurs when ing, visitors' attention will not focus o n it long
ne encounters tasks or environments with a degree enough for positive intellectual or emotional changes
f uncertainty, challenge, or novelty. These environ- to occur. Therefore it is important to consider what
ments nourish our built-in propensities for curiosity makes an experience rewarding in and of itself, so as
and exploration. According to Berlyne (1960, 1974), to understand what may motivate a person to look
f 7
certain structural stimulus characteristics, such as and think about an exhibit for "no good reason"-
novelty, surprisingness, complexity, and ambiguity, that is, in the absence of external rewards.
ead to motivational states that result in curiosity and
xploratory behavior. The Flow Experience
Contextual characteristics that evoke situational Studies conducted in a great variety of settings by dif-
interest-or curiosity-tend to be similar between ferent investigators have shown that a common expe-
individuals. These contextual stimuli provide the riential state characterizes situations in which people
" h o o k " for museums to capture visitor attention. are willing to invest psychic energy in tasks for which
Without such situational interest, viewers may not extrinsic rewards are absent. Chess players, rock
attend to an exhibit at all. Hence unobtrusive obser- climbers, dancers, painters, and musicians describe
vation of how visitors allocate attention is one of the the attraction of the activities they do in very similar
''most videly used techniques for assessing the effec- terms, stressing t h e fact t h a t w h a t keeps t h e m
tiveness of m u s e u m s (Loomis 1987; Serrell and involved in these demanding activities is the quality
Ralphling 1993). of the experience that ensues. Many activities that are
b 7

Inlriwic Moliva~ionin Museums I 69


also well rewarded with money and prestige, such as incompetent), individuals are natural learners and
surgery or computer programming, also seem to offer enjoy learning. . . . Insecurities and other forms of
intrinsic rewards in addition to the extrinsic ones; and negative cognitive conditioning interfere with o r
these are similar to the ones that artists and athletes block the emergence of individuals' natural motiva-
mention. We have called this common experiential tion to continually learn, grow, and develop in posi-
state the flow experience, because it is generally tive and self-determining ways." Negative mental
described as a state of mind that is spontaneous, states such as self-consciousness, depression, anxiety,
almost automatic, like the flow of a strong current loneliness, or anger also disrupt the flow experience
(Csikszentmihalyi 1975, 1990a). If a museum visit can (Csikszentmihalyi 1985). Dweck and her colleagues'
produce this experience, it is likely that the initial (Dweck 1975; Diener and Dweck 1980) research on
curiosity and interest will grow into a more extensive 'learned helplessness" highlights the serious effects of
learning interaction. low self-esteem and anxiety on learning achievement.
A general characteristic of activities that produce The intrinsically motivated learning state is character-
flow is that they have clear goals and appropriate ized by unselfconsciousness, joy, serenity, involve-
rules. In a game of tennis, or of chess, one knows ment, and happiness (Csikszentmihalyi 1985).
every second what one would like to accomplish. When goals are clear, feedback is unambiguous,
Playing a musical instrument, one knows what challenges and skills are well matched, then all of
sounds one wishes to produce. .A surgeon has clear one's mind and body become completely involved in
intentions during an operation, and it is this clarity of the activity. Attention is focused and concentration is
purpose that allows people to become so thoroughly so intense that there is no attention left over to think
involved with what they are doing. Conflicting goals about anything irrelevant or to worry about prob-
or unclear expectations divert our attention from the lems. In the flow state, a person is unaware of fatigue
task at hand. In addition to clear goals, flow activities and the passing of time; hours pass by in what seems
usually provide immediate and unambiguous feed- like minutes. This depth of involvement is enjoyable
back. One always knows whether one is doing well or and intrinsically rewarding. In many cases, individuals
not. Musicians find out immediately if they hit a describe the experience as becoming "one" with the
wrong note, tennis players if they hit the ball badly, environment - the painting, the music, the team.
and a surgeon knows right away if he has made a People often mention a sense of self-transcendence, as
mistake. This constant accountability for one's actions when chess players feel their moves becoming pan of
is another reason one gets so completely immersed in a universal field of forces or when dancers feel the
a flow activity. rhythm that moves them as part of the "harmony of
Another universally mentioned characteristic of the spheres."
flow experiences is that they tend to occur when the Flow activities lead to personal growth because, in
opportunities for action in a situation are in balance order to sustain the flow state, skills must increase
with the person's abilities. In other words, the chal- along with the increased challenges. Flow involves
lenges of the activity must match the skills of the indi- the person's entire being and full capacity. Since flow
vidual. If challenges are greater than skills, anxiety is inherently enjoyable, one is constantly seeking to
results; if skills are greater than challenges, the result return to that state, and this need inevitably involves
is boredom. This equation holds for the broadest pos- seeking greater challenges. In the process, flow activi-
sible range of skills: for instance, it includes physical, ties provide a sense of discovery; we discover things
mental, artistic, and musical talents. I will be frustrat- about ourselves as well as about the environment.
ed reading a book that was "above my head" and Flow activities, whether they involve competition,
bored when reading a book that is too easy and pre- chance, or any other dimension of experience, pro-
dictable. As skills increase, the challenges of the activ- vide a sense of discovery, a creative feeling of being
ity must also increase to continue the state of flow. transported into a new reality. They push us to higher
The skills involved are those perceived by the individ- levels of performance and lead to previously unexpe-
ual, however, and not necessarily the actual ones. If rienced states of consciousness. In short, they trans-
one thinks of himself as an incompetent football play- form the self by making it more complex. In this
er, this perceived incompetence will affect perfor- growth of the self lies the key to flow activities. One
mance regardless of its validity. Even if o n e is cannot enjoy doing the same thing at the same level
involved in an activity that typically induces flow, for long. We grow either bored or frustrated; and then
flow cannot be attained if he or she is worried about the desire to enjoy ourselves again pushes us to
performance or if other negative mental states prevail. stretch our skills or to discover new opportunities for
Research has substantiated the importance of a using them.
positive state of mind for learning. McCombs (1991, If these conditions are present, it is possible for
p. 119-20) writes that "in the absence of insecurity individuals to be in flow in any activity, be it conver-
(e.g., feeling afraid, being self-conscious, feeling sation, solving differential equations, or driving a car.

70 I Public Institutions for Personal Learning


A participant in one of our studies was in flow while that "attention is an act of connection." When we
watching the Chicago Bulls play basketball on televi- fully attend to something, we connect with life and
sion. He knew in detail each of the players' strengths, thus fulfill the basic human need for relatedness. "The
weaknesses, and definitive plays. "I can totally shut flow experience . . . is symbolic because it brings
.
away everything else. . . I played basketball when I together the psychic processes of the person and
was in high school. I think it's the most talent- unites them with a set of objective stimuli in the envi-
demanding of professional sports. . . . You get to know ronment. This is opposite from the state of alienation,
what people do, and you get sucked into their tech- in which one feels separated from oneself and from
niques because you can practically predict [their next the elements of one's life" (Csikszentmihalyi and
move]." However, when the New York Knicks game Rochberg-Halton 1981, p. 247). This process of con-
came on, he became distracted and bored, as he was nection could be referred to as integration. Moore
not as familiar with the play patterns of this team. A (1992, p. 261) says that "when we allow the great
person who becomes interested in hockey will feel possibilities of life to enter into us, and when we
that it is the "most talent-demanding of professional embrace them, then we are most individual." When
sports," and the same holds for soccer or baseball; in this integration occurs, an activity becomes meaning-
other words, what we invest a great deal of attention ful, and we become both more connected and more
in is bound to become ever more interesting and differentiated.
salient. Meaningful experiences are those that are both dif-
It is often assumed that for learning cognitive ferentiated and integrated. This dialectical process of
processes are more important than affective processes. integration and differentiation is necessary for psy-
But as Schiefele (199 1) points out, it is likely that chological development and personal growth (Damon
affective processes are at least as important for evok- 1983; Fowler 1981; Kohlberg 1984; Loevinger 1976;
ing broader conceptual understanding rather than Maslow 1968). For example, the psychiatrist H. F.
simple fact retention. Because emotional factors may Searles (1960, p.30) states this dialectic as follows:
influence learning only indirectly by stimulating cog- The human being is engaged, throughout his life
nitive processes, their importance is easily underesti- span, in an unceasing struggle to differentiate
mated (Schiefele 1991, p. 3 16; Isen, Daubman, and himself increasingly fully, not only from his
Gorgoglione 1987; Pekrun 1990). human, but also from his nonhuman environ-
ment, while developing, in proportion as he suc-
From Flow to Enduring Meaning ceeds in these differentiations, an increasingly
When a person is in flow, or fully enjoying an intrin- meaningful relatedness with the latter environ-
sically motivated activity, he or she usually describes ment as well as with his fellow human beings.
two dialectically related characteristics. On the one
hand, when involved in the activity, the individual This dialectic between integration and differentia-
fully expresses the self. In the process, he or she dis- tion is the process by which we learn and grow. On
covers previously unknown and unrealized potentials the one hand, we must discover the limits of our
and skills. Following Aristotle's views on the purpose being by expressing the purposes and potentials
of life, Dante wrote: "In every action . . . the main inherent in our biological organism. Only through
intention of the agent is to express his own image. . . . self-control, through shaping events to our intentions,
In action the doer unfolds his being" (quoted in can we learn who we are and what we are capable of.
Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981, p. 48). On the other hand, we must find ways to expandour
The statement: "It is like designing, discovering some- limited selves by forging ties with other human and
thing new," is the one most strongly endorsed by peo- nonhuman systems. Motivational research has high-
ple as being similar to the phenomenology of the flow lighted the importance of both individual autonomy
experience (Csikszentmihalyi 1975). One recent study and connection for facilitating intrinsic learning.
participant told us that she finds "learning often
comes as a surprise." This process of discovery and
learning about who we are could be thought of as dif- Implications of Intrinsic Motivation for Museums
ferentiation -the process of developing a unique self. How do these general principles apply to the kind of
On the other hand, people in flow tend to feel learning that can take place in museums? A schematic
connected with other entities, such as nature, a team, representation of the process of intrinsic motivation at
the family, or the broader community. Or in the case work in museums is presented in figure 1. Following
of many solitary pursuits, the activity connects one the steps of this process, it will be easier to see the
with a system of thoughts or beliefs. A rock climber concrete implications of motivational theory. Of
may declare that climbing is for him a form of "self- course, such a schematic approach cannot deal with
communication" (Csikszcntmihalyi and all the practical problems museums face. One of the
Csikszentmihalyi 1988). Cameron ( 1 992, p. 5 3) says major obstacles to an easy movement from theory to

L Intrinsic Motiv11tio11in Museums I 71


Figure 1 . The development o f learning through intrinsic motivation in museum settings

A. The "hook"

Curiosity ^Ã Interest
(Contextual stimuli that attract attention-i.e., sounds, (Stimuli that appeal to prior personal interest-
colors, kinetic displays, items with common cultural or domain-specificappeal; astronomy, archaeology, biology,

Sensory Intellectual Emotional


visual rational empathy
aural scientific self-reflection
kinesthetic historical

Challenges = Skills
(Opportunitiesfor actions in various dimensions of involve- (Provisionsfor developing skills at gradually increasing lev-
ment at gradually increasing levels of difficulty) els of competence, e.g, "zones of proximal development")

D. Growth of Complexity in Consciousness


( I f involvement is intrinsically rewarding, visitors wish to maintain the flow experience. This requires increasing challenges to
avoid boredom, and increasing skills to avoid frustration. The consequence of this dynamic involvement is a growth of sensory,
intellectual and emotional complexity.)

practice is the fact that visitors come with such a termined: both dinosaurs and mummies are ancient
broad range of interests and backgrounds that no sin- and therefore mysterious; both invoke awe and a
gle recipe for motivating them could possibly apply thrill of fear without actual danger. These seem to be
across the board. Nevertheless, these broad outlines universal reasons for people to want to pay attention.
can be quite helpful if one takes the trouble of adapt- Others are pleasing displays with bright colors, inter-
ing them to particular specific conditions. active exhibits, large size, and other stimuli that pro-
vide the situational interest necessary to attract atten-
The "Hook" tion.
The first step in the process of intrinsically motivated Museum researchers have already demonstrated
learning suggests that the museum exhibit must cap- that visitors remember better displays to which they
ture the visitors' curiosity. Michael Spock, as experi- have paid more attention (Falk 1991; Koran, Foster,
enced a professional as they come, says that dinosaurs and Koran 1989). However, we still are far from
and mummies are the surest exibits to attract atten- knowing what the fundamental dimensions of situa-
tion. Probably the reason for this attraction is overde- tional interest are. Until we collect systematic knowl-

72 / Public Institutions for Personal Learning


edge on this topic, we shall liiivc I Ã ‡ p n n m l l ~ yni.il ln-ii~?; ~ . t - i ~ ~ . i Ii -i \
~ à ‡ t çi I t 1 5 . * f r 4
< Q I ~ ( Ã ‡ ~ H .1 i~ r , t <~i l . t -

and error, finding out which coi~ipoiiriiiso l ,111 t0\l1il~- ~ ) i i t l ~ . i l ~ i l ~-.i.iitS


- . i i t ( 1 1 I ~ I I I in
I ~ ~
\\ Jt t i n i n $ t t \ È;(^.Ã

it are most attractive, for whom, under what coiuli- i i ~ i g l ~lie t lcd I < )I I I I ( ~ I I I ~ :l i l l ~ - ~. t~i i i i< ~~i i )({ t-i 4- t ~ i~i!v
tions. In other words, museum work will continue to lholtght simil,ir .uiil ~ i i i i i l . i ~ i t i.innnq; ~-. il~iin!<~ flli~<i~(~t
be an art rather than a science. Although this is not differem" ( I ~ n g c ri v t ) 1, p. . I - Ã1. l . \ l ~ i l ~ rI t. ~ . n1.1' IIII.~I~-
necessarily bad, a larger contribution of scientific mindfulness display infoim,i~ion it) Ãmic\ I .mi! pi 4 -
knowledge would surely help. sent various viewpoints. For example, l . . m p ~( l t ~ *\j
After the individual's curiosity is aroused; the p.47) contrasts the statement "The three main reasons
exhibit must engage sustained interest in order for for the Civil War were . . ." with the statement "From
learning to take place. While individuals vary in what the perspective of the white male living in the twenti-
they are interested in - astronomy, sports, mechan- eth century, the main reasons for the Civil War were
ics, archeology, biology and so on - some general . . . (p. 47). The latter approach calls for thoughtful
guidelines could be proposed. Most important, the comparisons. For example, How did women feel dur-
link between the museum and the visitor's life needs ing the Civil War? the old? the old from the North?
to be made clear. To inspire intrinsic motivation, the the black male today? and so on.
objects one finds and the experiences one enjoys, Information that is presented as true without alter-
while possibly inspiring awe and a sense of discovery, native perspectives discourages the motivation to
should not feel disconnected from one's own life. explore and learn more. Langer (1993, p. 45) terms
Moore (1992, p. 285) asserts that "when a r t is this situation a "premature cognitive commitment,"
removed as the province of professional artists, a dan- denoted by rigid beliefs mindlessly accepted as true
gerous gulf develops between the fine arts and the (see also Langer and Imber 1979). In sum, intrinsical-
everyday arts. The fine arts are elevated and set apart ly motivated learning is a n open process involving
from life, becoming too precious and therefore irrele- uncertainty and the discovery of new possibilities. A
vant. Having banished art to the museum, we fail to fixed presentation of the material thwarts such fur-
give it a place in ordinary life." What Moore says ther exploration. It is only through the conscious
about art could be said of museums in general. It is to choice of various possibilities that one can learn who
be hoped that the museum experience will inspire one is, what one's interests and beliefs are, and where
visitors to see the relationship between the exhibits one's unique talents lie (Csikszentmihalyi and
and their own concerns and perhaps be stimulated to Rochberg-Halton 198 1; Csikszentmihalyi and
create art, pursue science, and so on, after leaving the Robinson, 1990).
museum. But when we are intrinsically motivated to learn,
For example, an exhibit on rock formations may be emotions a n d feelings a r e involved a s well as
informative and pleasantly arranged. In addition, the thoughts. For example, our wish to know about peo-
exhibit may have features that are challenging and ples in faraway places includes not only the desire for
allow one to explore and develop skills. But the fea- intellectual understanding but the desire to feel emo-
tures of the exhibit that will induce the motivation to tionally connected to them as well. We are often
learn more are the deeper sense of meaning it pro- drawn to exhibits containing diaries and personal let-
vides. How does this exhibit pertain to me? How does ters because they connect us with another's feelings.
knowing about these rock formations link me to other As Moore (1992, p. 208) states:
people and times, the larger cosmos? Education We have spiritual longing for community and
should "speak to the soul as well as the mindu (Moore relatedness and for a cosmic vision, but we go
1992, p. 36). How does knowledge of rock formations after them with literal hardware instead of with
provide a "soulful" connection? Unless we make sensitivity of heart. . . . Our many studies of
progress in answering such questions, the information world cultures are soulless, replacing the com-
provided in the display is likely to disappear from the mon bonding of humanity and its shared wis-
visitor's consciousness without leaving any trace in dom with bites of information that have no way
memory. of getting into us deeply, of nourishing and
transforming our sense of ourselves. Soul, of
Opportunites for Involvement course, has been extracted from the beginning
Learning involves the use of sensory and emotional because we conceive of education to be about
faculties, as well as intellectual ones, and this connec- skills and information, not about depth of feel-
tion leads us to the third step in the process. To ing and imagination.
engage intellectual faculties, the exhibit should Museum researchers have become increasingly
encourage what Langer (1993; see also 1989) has aware that it is not enough to attract the fleeting
termed "mindfulness." Mindfulness is the "state of attention and interest of visitors; to be effective,
mind that results from drawing novel distinctions, museums must provide opportunities for the kind of
examining information from new perspectives, and deep absorption that leads to learning (Harvey,

Intrinsic Motivation in Museums I 73


Birjulin, and Loomis, forthcoming; Thompson 1993). physical distractions include crowds, noise, intimidat-
This is what Bitgood (1990, p. 1) calls "simulated ing guards, hunger, bladder pressure, and fatigue. In
immersion," or "the degree to which an exhibit effec- addition, preconceived notions may provide internal
tively involves, absorbs, engrosses, or creates for visi- distractions. Concentration can be hindered by rigid
tors the experience of a particular time and place." expectations - either one's own or someone else's.
For example, as Falk and Dierking (1992, p. 54) point
Conditionsfor Flow out, museum visitors often have the expectation that
When the visitor is interested in a n exhibit and they "should see the entire museum." Such prior
engaged through sensory, intellectual, and emotional expectations decrease the openness necessary for a
faculties, he or she should be ready to experience an meaningful learning experience by causing fixation
intrinsically rewarding, optimal experience. But for on an external goal and anxiety over the ability to
this experience to occur, the conditions for flow must meet that goal. Another example frequently occurs in
be present. In the previous section, we have seen that art museums when patrons feel they "should" be get-
one of the main requirements for flow is to have clear ting something out of the experience
'

goals. Unfortunately, one of the complaints visitors (Csikszentmihalyi and Robinson 1990, p. 144). When
most often voice is that they do not know what to do visitors feel intimidated or fearful, or when they try to
when they enter a museum. Helping visitors set man- sustain some rigid self-concept or achieve some pre-
ageable goals, both for the entire visit and for each defined result, they also lose the openness necessary .
stop at an exhibit, is one way to make the experience for an enjoyable learning experience. The physical
more enjoyable. Without feedback, however, involve- sources of distraction can be remedied by physical
ment is unlikely to be sustained. Successful displays means - reducing crowds, providing better facilities
tend to be those that ask visitors to commit them- - while the psychological causes must be addressed
selves to make guesses, to evaluate, to respond - and through information and education.
then provide information by which the visitors can In addition to negative expectations a person may
compare their responses to some other standard bring to the museum, negative mental states can be
(Bitgood 1990). caused by the social context (Deci et al. 1981;
Another feature of successful exhibits is that they McCombs 1991). Thus, the museum environment can
offer opportunities for involvement that can be either facilitate or hinder flow. Anxiety, embarrass-
matched with a broad range of visitor skills. This ment, or self-consciousness usually vary depending
notion was made familiar to the museum community on where we are and whom we are with. Social envi-
through the "social design movement," one of whose ronments that facilitate intrinsically motivated Icarn-
principal aims is to increase the fit between people ing support personal autonomy and responsibility
and their environment (Sommer 1972) or between rather than trying to control behavior. These support-
visitors and informal learning environments (Screven ive environments provide people with choices
1976). (Zuckerrnan et al. 1978) and acknowledge their per-
In this regard, Vygotsky's (1978) zone of proximal spectives or feelings (Kocstner et al. 1984). We
development provides a framework for understanding express who we are through our conscious choice of
how to moderate challenges so that they are at the actions. Thus it is important to allow individuals to
right level. The zone of proximal development is "the choose whenever possible. People are more open to
distance between the actual developmental level as learning when they feel supported, when they are ina
determined by independent problem solving and the place where they can express themselves and explore
level of potential development as determined through their interests without fear of embarrassment or criti-
problem solving under adult guidance or in collabora- cism, and when there are no predefined expectations
tion with more capable peers" (Vygotsky 1978, p. 86). constraining their behavior. Support, security, and
When individuals are assisted in particular tasks, they trust are critical for allowing openness to discovery
can learn at different levels. With assistance, one and intrinsically motivated learning. For personal
third-grade student may learn at a fifth-grade pace, growth and development, one must become less
while another learns at a fourth-grade pace. In devel- dependent on, or constrained by outside guidance, so
oping exhibits, it is useful to ask: How much assis- that spontaneous motivation will have a chance to
tance is available for visitors with different levels of awaken.
knowledge and ability? Do exhibits present gradually
increasing levels of difficulty? And are there provi- Growth of Complexity in Consciousness
sions for developing skills at gradually increasing lev- If a museum exhibit induces the flow state, the expe-
els of competence? rience will be intrinsically rewarding. The visitor will
In addition to a balance of challenge and skill, the be motivated to explore, and as he or she learns more,
visitor must be able to concentrate and devote full skills will increase. The consequence of this dynamic
attention to the given exhibit or activity. Well-known involvement is a growth of sensory, intellectual, and

74 / Public Institutionsfor Personal Learning


emotional complexity. This growth is especially -.
process, can we ir,uii wIi,if i v d t l i ~, i i i t ! i ~ i i . i i ,im nui
nportant to realize now that we live in an informa- (Screven 1976) riyiii!: 11111 ililri-imt ili\pl,i\-i, iiillcii~i~t
l aon society, when multimedia technology and com- signage, (.hllcir~tiw.iys nt involving visifois- t v l i i i i
puters are rapidly changing how we learn and how makiiig S L I I C i l i i i t only ,I single varitible is cliiiii~i-ii.it .I
ve are entertained. By pressing a button, in the com- timfÑitiu thcii ii~c.lsi~iiit~ilie re'sulis will yield usc'lnl
o n of our own home, we can learn about the entire rest-ills. 11 is impoittiiit 10 remain flexible' in one's poli-
history of a n , complete with detailed images on the ties, w [hat imstakes - can be corrected swiftly and Sllc-
" c r e e n ; w e do n o t need to e n t e r 3 museum. But
nuseums offer the opportunity to interact with a real
r environment, o n e in w h i c h t h e objects a r e still
cesses can be built on, These are the main features of
the experimental method on which all of science is Ii
,-
based. But then, as the philosopher Karl Popper said,
imbued with the blood, the tears, the sweat of their science is but common sense writ large. There is no iL
kers. Does this contact with the facticity of the his-
cal object actually matter? Or will virtual reality
reason museums could not use more common sense
and develop the habit of writing it large. We would all
^
Ãt
erienced in the communications room of one's benefit from it, and museums could go on performing
me give a n even more vivid learning experience their educational function with a clearer purpose and
!
n museums now provide? a renewed sense of self-confidence.
The jury is still out o n these questions. In o n e
respect, however, museums seem to have a distinct
advantage over solitary media-induced experiences.
ey provide information in a public space, where Acknowledgments
ere is a potential to develop the integrative dimen- The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful suggei-
n of personal growth. We learn about connected- lions of the participants at the Annapolis Conference,
ness through rituals - such as ceremonies o r rock and especially Margie Marino and Mark Harvey for
-
concerts and whenever we are exposed to an event their assistance with the literature on museum visitors
that is shared with others that feeling of connected- research.
ness is reaffirmed and strengthened. In modem soci-
'ety, however, there are fewer and fewer venues to
experience such shared events. Perhaps one of the
major underdeveloped functions of museums is to References
,provide opportunities for individually meaningful Amahiie, R. M 1983 The social psycliolofl of crcativnv New
experiences that also connect with the experiences of York- Springer-Verlag.
- 1985 Motivation and creativity: Effects of inotiva-
tional orientanun on crcaiivc ivriter'i Jounialcif
It is essential t o realize, however, that current
Personality und Social Piycholoyy 48- 393-97.
nowledge is insufficient to provide a basis for a thor- Benware. C.. and Deci. E L 1984 The quality nl Irarnine
informed museum practice. While we are get- with an active versus passive mot!vati<~nal set. Aiiii.'iicail
understand general principles of motivation Educational Research Journal 2 1: 755-65.
ly well, the necessary details are still largely Berlyne, D E. 1960 Conflict,arousaland citriosny New Yolk:
acking. For instance, w e have n o table where we Grove
could look u p t h e elements that will attract t h e -.I974 Novelty, complexity, and inteteslingnfss In
curiosity of different types of visitors; w e cannot Studies ;>I new experimental aesthetics, eii D E. Bcilyile, lip
anticipate the interests of the audience; we have only 175-80 New York: Wiley.
a rudimentary understanding as to how to balance Bitgood, S. 1990 The role 01 simulated inuner~ionin t x h i -
I
the challenges of the exhibit with the visitors' skills; bition Report 90-20. Jacksonville, Ala.: Cvnler lor Social
, , we are not sure how to nurture the growth of com- Design
Butler, R A 1957. The effect 01 deprivatioi~of visual inct-'Il-
ity in the visitors' consciousness after the first lives on visual exploration inotivat~on..liwrnal of
ks are struck. Many of these issues will take Comparatw and Physiolog~calP'ychology SO. 177-79
ecades of basic research to resolve. Cameron, J . 1992. Theartist's way. A spir~tualpathlo hi911er
In t h e meantime, however, it seems that each creativity New York: Putnam.
museum could generate knowledge about these press- Csikszentmthaiyi, M. 1975. Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. Son
ing q u e s t i o n s b y t a k i n g a m o r e e x p e r i m e n t a l Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
1 approach, by becoming a more active learning institu-
à . 1978 Aitention and the wholistic appriiacll lo
tion. If even 10 percent of museum space and staff behavior. In The stream of consciousness, cd. K. 5. Pope 6 J.
effons were devoted to collecting systematic informa- L. Singer. New York. Plenum, pp. 335-58.
' tion about how visitors are affected by the visit, we - 1985. Emergent motivation and the evoliitinn of lhe
oon have a much better idea of what learning self. Advancesin Motivation and Achievement 4: 93-1 19.
ace within the walls. Only by experimenting
- 1990a. Flow t he psychology of'optimalexpcriri~~i'. New
York: Harper Coilins
ne alternative after the other, in an iterative
Contents

Foreword The Influence of Culture on Learning and Behavior


John U . Ogbu, University of California
Acknowledgements. at Berkeley

Introduction Evidence of Development from People's Participation in


Communities of Learners
- - Eugene Matusov and Barbara Rogoff,
Parti University of California at Santa Cruz
Defining Outcomes and Research Questions - 15
Human-Factor Considerations i n the Design of
What Do We Think People Learn In Museums? Museums to Optimize Their Impact on Learning
John H. Falk, Lynn D. Dierking Alan Hedge, Cornell University
and Dana G. Holland 17
Learning and the Physical Environment
Gary W. Evans, Cornell University
How Should We Investigate Learning In Museums?
Research Questions and Project Designs
John H. Falk, Lynn D. Dierking Part III
and Dana G. Holland 23 Further Reflections About Long-Term Learning
Research in Museums
Establishing A Long-Term Learning Research 127
Agenda For Museums
John H. Falk and Lynn D. Dierking 31 Mucking Around in Museum Research
Deborah Edward, Austin Children's Museum 129

Part I1 The Need for Learning Research in Museums


Background papers Linda Downs, National Gallery of Art 133

Learning in Interactive Environments: Creating an Academic Home for Informal Science Education
Prior Knowledge and New Experience Alan J. Friedman, New York Hall of Science 135
Jeremy Roschell, University of Massachusetts,
Dartmouth
Notes on Contributors
Museum Memory
Douglas Herman, National Center for Health Conference Participants
Statistics and Dana Plude, Department
of Pyschology, University of Maryland

Intrinsic Motivation in Museums:


Why Does One Want to Learn?
Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi and Kim Hermanson,
The University of Chicago

S-ar putea să vă placă și