Sunteți pe pagina 1din 17

Mindfulness

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-1066-1

ORIGINAL PAPER

Gender Differences in Self-Compassion: Examining the Role of Gender


Role Orientation
Lisa M. Yarnell 1 & Kristin D. Neff 2 & Oliver A. Davidson 2 & Michael Mullarkey 2

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Meta-analytic research suggests that women have slightly lower levels of self-compassion than men, but the contri-
bution of gender role orientation has not been carefully explored. The current study examines the joint associations
of self-identified gender and gender role orientation with self-compassion in undergraduate (N = 504) and community
adult (N = 968) samples, using two measures of gender role orientation. The Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) and
the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) were used to classify each participant into a single gender role orien-
tation category based on relative scores on the masculinity and femininity subscales, with respect to the sample, and
average scores for each continuous subscale were also retained. The categorical gender role orientation classifications
were used in mean comparisons of self-compassion across groups, and the average masculinity and femininity
subscale scores were used in regression modeling. Results replicated the small effect size for gender differences
in self-compassion for both samples, with self-identified men having significantly higher levels of self-compassion
than self-identified women. Results also consistently showed that the impact of self-identified gender on self-
compassion was smaller than the impact of masculine gender role orientation, suggesting that socialization plays a
strong role, and that those high in both femininity and masculinity tended to have the highest levels of self-
compassion. Effect sizes and specific findings differed by gender, sample, and gender role orientation measure.
Therefore, a nuanced understanding of differences in self-compassion based on gender and gender role orientation
is needed.

Keywords Self-compassion . Gender differences . Gender role orientation . Self-compassion training

Given the robust benefits of self-compassion suggested in we relate to ourselves in instances of perceived failure,
the empirical literature, it is important to clarify findings inadequacy, or personal suffering. As defined by Neff
from previous studies suggesting that men have higher (2003b), self-compassion entails three main elements,
levels of self-compassion than women in North America each of which has a positive and negative pole that rep-
by considering the role that gender role orientation might resents compassionate versus uncompassionate behavior:
play in these differences. Self-compassion refers to how self-kindness versus self-judgment, a sense of common
humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness versus over-
identification. Self-kindness entails being understanding,
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
warm, and supportive toward oneself. Rather than harshly
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-1066-1) contains supplementary judging oneself for personal inadequacies, the self is of-
material, which is available to authorized users. fered kindness and unconditional acceptance. It also in-
volves actively soothing and comforting oneself in times
* Lisa M. Yarnell of suffering. Common humanity involves recognizing our
lyarnell@air.org shared human experience, acknowledging that all humans
are imperfect and make mistakes, that all people face
1
American Institutes for Research, 1000 Thomas Jefferson St., NW, challenges in their lives. Rather than feeling isolated by
Washington, DC 20007-3835, USA one’s imperfection—feeling as if BI^ am the only one who
2
The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA is struggling—one takes a broader and more connected
Mindfulness

perspective. Mindfulness involves paying attention to in the social sciences, however, as even a small effect is
one’s present moment experience of suffering with clarity likely to result in differences in important life outcomes
and balance, without being lost in an exaggerated (Keith 2006). Additionally, the meta-analysis suggested
storyline about negative aspects of oneself or one’s life, that samples with a greater percentage of ethnic
a process called Bover-identification.^ While these ele- minority participants displayed larger gender differences
ments are separable and do not co-vary uniformly, they in self-compassion, such that cultural norms for
do mutually interact as a dynamic system to create a self- Bmasculinity^ and Bfemininity^ may be playing a role.
compassionate mindset. To date, however, there has been little examination of
It is important to note that self-compassion has both role of gender role orientation in interpreting these ap-
Byin^ and Byang^ qualities (Neff and Germer 2018). In parent differences in men and women’s levels of self-
traditional Chinese philosophy, yin and yang refer to the compassion.
seemingly opposite but interdependent qualities of male– The small effect size observed in the meta-analysis be-
female, hard–soft, and active–passive. Self-compassion tween men and women in their levels of self-compassion
has not only yin qualities that stem from soothing, suggests that most variance in self-compassion is not
comforting, and validating suffering but also yang quali- between but within gender groups (Hyde 2005). One im-
ties that stem from protecting, providing, and motivating portant source of individual variation may be orientation
action to alleviate suffering. Many people think of self- to Bmasculine^ and Bfeminine^ gender roles, which may
compassion more in terms of yin, such as a mother sooth- relate in distinct ways with self-compassion. While there
ing her crying child, or a father putting his arm around his is debate in the field over the precise meanings of sex
upset son and saying, BIt’s going to be okay.^ However, versus gender (Lips 2017), we use the term sex to refer
yang is equally essential to self-compassion, with a pro- to the anatomy of an individual’s reproductive system and
totypical image being a mother bear protecting her cubs secondary sex characteristics, which can include male,
from danger, or a father working two jobs to put food on female, and intersex persons (American Psychological
the table for his children. Thus, self-compassion cannot be Association [APA] and National Association of School
said to be essentially Bmasculine^ or Bfeminine,^ but in- Psychologists 2015; American United Nations Free and
stead transcends this duality in its focus on the alleviation Equal 2016). We use the term gender to refer one’s self-
of suffering. identity, which may or may not correspond to biological
The construct of self-compassion has received consid- sex (Prince 2005). While this typically refers to Bmen^
erable attention over the last several years. Research typ- and Bwomen^ in the current Western societal context, it
ically shows that self-compassion is positively associated also includes a variety of nonbinary and pangender self-
with psychological wellbeing (Barnard and Curry 2012; identifiers, such as Bbigender,^ Bnongendered,^ or being
Zessin et al. 2015). In fact, one meta-analysis found a gender BX^ (Richards et al. 2016). We use gender role to
large effect size when examining the negative association refer to a person’s psychological identification with typi-
between self-compassion and depression, anxiety, and cal societal gender roles. However, adherence to a partic-
stress in 20 studies (MacBeth and Gumley 2012). ular gender role can be placed along a continuum, with
Moreover, self-compassion is associated with psychologi- some persons more extremely or unvaryingly Bsex-typed^
cal strengths such as happiness, optimism, and life satis- than others (Bem and Lenney 1976). Placement along
faction (Hollis-Walker and Colosimo 2011; Neff et al. these continuums can be referred to as gender role
2007), as well as being linked to increased motivation, orientation. Biological sex, gender, and gender role ori-
health behaviors, positive body image, and resilient cop- entation are distinct constructs. For instance, two biolog-
ing (e.g., Albertson et al. 2015; Allen et al. 2012; Breines ical males who both self-identify as being a woman may
and Chen 2013; Sbarra et al. 2012). Although levels of have different degrees of identification with Bmasculine^
self-compassion have been found to vary cross-culturally, and Bfeminine^ gender roles.
self-compassion appears to be equally predictive of well- The majority of research conducted in the USA on
being across cultures (Neff et al. 2008). gender role orientation and its correlates has relied on
Recent meta-analytic work indicates that men and the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem 1974) and the
women in North America (USA and Canada) tend to Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence and
differ in self-compassion levels as measured by total Helmreich 1978). These measure self-reported associa-
scores on the SCS, with women reporting slightly less tions with items loosely reflecting agency/instrumentality,
self-compassion than men (Yarnell et al. 2015). While labeled Bmasculine^ traits (e.g., analytical, independent,
the difference suggested was robust in that it was iden- competitive), and communality/expressiveness, labeled
tified across 88 studies, it was small in absolute size Bfeminine^ traits (e.g., affectionate, gentle, understand-
(Cohen’s d = 0.18). An effect of this size is meaningful ing). In scale construction, these items were chosen from
Mindfulness

pools of characteristics that were piloted among US gender. For example, the qualities of nurturance and caring
undergraduate samples, selected as being more valued associated with feminine gender norms may facilitate self-
for one gender group than the other (Bmasculine^ for compassion. On the other hand, feminine norms of self-
men, and Bfeminine^ for women). While items are similar sacrifice (Baker-Miller 1986; Raffaelli and Ontai 2004;
between the measures, the PAQ was explicitly designed to Ruble and Martin 1998) may lead to lower levels of self-
measure instrumentality and expressiveness, rather than compassion among Bfeminine^ women, as the needs of the
Bmasculinity^ and Bfemininity^ per se, based on the posi- self are not given attention or validity. Women who are
tion that these characteristics are essentially what the Bandrogynous^ may exhibit higher levels of self-compassion,
BSRI measures (Spence and Helmreich 1978; Hoffman as research has shown that androgynous girls tend to have
and Borders 2001). In both measures, men and women higher levels of authenticity and are comfortable asserting
can associate themselves with either (Bmasculine^ or their voice (Harter et al. 1998). BAndrogynous^ women who
Bfeminine^), neither (Bundifferentiated^), or both are higher in self-compassion may thus be more able to remain
(Bandrogynous^) sets of characteristics, and be catego- relationally connected while tending to the needs of the self,
rized accordingly. While the scales are somewhat narrow resulting in greater self-compassion. If so, it may be that ob-
in their shaping gender role orientation only in terms of served gender differences in self-compassion between men
agency/instrumentality and communality/expressiveness, and women do not hold true for Bandrogynous^ women when
the scales have also been interpreted in other ways (e.g., compared to men in general.
as reflecting Bself-directedness^ and Bother-orientation,^ Research has also shown that women who adopt tradi-
Ballard-Reisch and Elton 1992; or reflecting multiple un- tional Bfeminine^ gender roles tend to experience stressful
derlying dimensions of instrumentality, Lippa 1985; events as more aversive and are less capable of bouncing
Pedhazur and Tetenbaum 1979). back from failure experiences, compared to women who
Despite varying interpretations, a number of studies identify with more flexible gender roles (Nevid and
suggest that the scales remain useful and valid for classi- Rathus 2016). This may be due to the tendency of
fying men and women in North America into the original Bandrogynous^ individuals to more sensitively distinguish
gender orientation role categories (e.g., Ahmed et al. 2016; the situational effectiveness of various coping strategies,
Holt and Ellis 1998; Oswald 2004), and they capture and deploy them accordingly, including changing the situation
constellations of characteristics shown to predict attitudes, when it is perceived as controllable (e.g., direct action), and
behaviors, and health (Davis 2009; Wood and Eagly 2009). changing the self when it is not (e.g., acceptance; Cheng
For example, individuals categorized as Bfeminine^ are 2005). These abilities may be associated with greater self-
more likely to show symptoms of internalizing disorders compassion, enabling Bandrogynous^ women to more effec-
such as depression, and less likely to report symptoms of tively cope with stress and failure.
externalizing disorders such as alcohol abuse; while Similarly, it may be that Bmasculine^ norms of being
individuals categorized as Bmasculine^ are more likely to strong, unemotional, pragmatic, and independent (Deaux
show symptoms of externalizing disorders such as aggres- and Kite 1993; Levant 2011; Pederson and Vogel 2007)
sion (Price et al. 2018; Taylor 2015). There is some sug- work against masculine men’s ability to show tenderness
gestion that both men and women who are classified as to themselves in times of need, resulting in lower levels of
Bandrogynous^ have better mental health outcomes, in- self-compassion than among men with a more
cluding decreased stress and anxiety (Lam and McBride- Bandrogynous^ orientation. For example, studies have
Chang 2007; Prakash et al. 2010). Men and women classi- shown that men who adhere to traditional masculine gen-
fied as Bundifferentiated^ appear to have worse mental der norms tend to avoid or inhibit vulnerable feelings and
health outcomes, including low self-esteem (Berthiaume intimacy with others (Levant and Pollack 1995; O’Neil
et al. 1996). However, the literature appears mixed on 2008); experience limited access to authentic feelings;
whether Bandrogyny^ or Bmasculinity^ alone appears to and show heightened psychological distress (Levant
be most strongly associated with psychological health for 2011; Pederson and Vogel 2007). In support of this prop-
both genders, with some US studies suggesting that instru- osition, one recent study of adult heterosexual men
mental (Bmasculine^) but not expressive (Bfeminine^) showed that conformity to Bmasculine^ norms is associat-
traits are predictive of healthy adjustment for both men ed with lower levels of self-compassion, although the de-
and women (e.g., Aube et al. 1995; Moscovitch et al. gree to which men associated with Bfeminine^ character-
2005; Whitley 1983). istics was not explored (Reilly et al. 2014).
Levels of self-compassion are likely to differ among men Yet particularly for men, the social stigma tied to
and women with various gender role orientations due to the gender-nonconforming displays may also be associated
characteristics thought to reflect Bmasculinity^ and with lower levels of self-compassion. Gender displays that
Bfemininity,^ and these associations may be unique for each are compatible with cultural expectations may be referred
Mindfulness

to as gender-normative, while displays incompatible with and a community sample), and employing two distinct mea-
these expectations constitute gender nonconformity (APA sures of gender orientation role (the BSRI and PAQ). While
and NASP 2015). In recent times, women have been ac- these measures differ in their precise operationalization, they
cepted, and even encouraged, to take on Bmasculine^ share the same basic conceptual understanding of gender role
traits and behaviors such as confidence in one’s abilities, orientation. We hoped that this approach would help establish
competitiveness, leadership roles, and participation in the robustness of findings and provide information as to how
sports; yet in some contexts it is less acceptable for men varying operationalization of gender role orientation may in-
to take on Bfeminine^ traits such as gentleness and kind- form apparent findings of gender differences. Because we are
ness, concern for relationships, and emotional expression not aware of prior research examining these questions in
(Priess et al. 2009). For example, numerous studies have North American populations, we considered the examination
found that parents and peers are more likely to disapprove of these associations to be exploratory.
of gender-role violations in boys than in girls (e.g., Kane
2006; Martin 1990; Sirin et al. 2004).
More systematic research is needed on the association Method
between self-compassion and gender role orientation, to
clarify the overly simplistic interpretation that men and Participants
women differ in self-compassion in a binary manner,
and to consider how gender role orientation in conjunc- Undergraduate Sample This sample was recruited from a sub-
tion with gender may shape levels of self-compassion and ject pool of undergraduate college students attending a public,
its associated health benefits. Given that self-compassion southwestern US university. After removing two cases from
entails both yin and yang elements which operate in a the original sample due to not completing any of the questions
dialectic, and that gender roles themselves operate in a on one of the three main questionnaires pertaining to gender
dialectic, it is likely that simplistic dichotomies are insuf- orientation role or self-compassion, the final sample size was
ficient to explain the association of self-compassion and N = 504, including 266 self-identified women and 238 self-
gender. For example, Tatum (2013) examined the associ- identified men. Range in age was 17 to 24 years (M = 20.79,
ation of self-compassion with Bmasculinity^ and SD = 1.24). The sample was 54% European American, 31%
Bfemininity^ scores using the PAQ in a combined sample Asian/Asian American, 9% Latino/Hispanic, 4% Multiethnic,
of US undergraduate and community participants, and 2% African American, and 0.2% Native American.
showed positive associations for both scales with self-
compassion; however, interactions between Bmasculinity^ Community Sample This sample was recruited from a US
and Bfemininity^ were not examined, and analyses were adult population via Mechanical Turk, an online survey re-
not conducted separately for each gender group. Also, search recruitment method that samples from the general pub-
Patzak et al. (2017) found among German undergraduates lic. After removing 16 cases from the original sample due to
that Bfeminine^ and Bundifferentiated^ men and women not completing any questions on one of the three main ques-
have lower self-compassion scores than Bmasculine^ and tionnaires, the final sample size was N = 968, including 616
Bandrogynous^ men and women using the BSRI, with no self-identified women and 352 self-identified men. Range in
apparent modifying effect of gender group. Of course, age was 18 to 76 years (M = 38.22, SD = 12.90), with no out-
gender roles may differ between Germany and North liers (all |z| < 2.93). The sample was 74% European American,
America. 8% Asian/Asian American, 6% Latino/Hispanic, 10% African
In order to elucidate these previous findings, the cur- American, 2% Native American, and < 1% Other. In terms of
rent study aimed to determine whether levels of self- education, 37% reported having a 4-year college degree, 22%
compassion differ according to gender role orientation in completed some college, 14% had a 2-year degree, 17% had
combination with gender, by examining levels of self- pursued graduate school, and 9% had a high school degree or
compassion both within and between gender groups in a less.
US sample. (Note that examining biological sex was out-
side the scope of this study.) Specifically, we were inter- Procedure
ested in whether the self-compassion levels of self-
identified men and women would differ according to their The undergraduate sample was invited to complete a survey
gender role orientation. We also were interested in wheth- questionnaire online, and participating students were given
er differences between men and women would still be course credit upon completion. The community sample was
apparent when taking gender orientation role into account. drawn from Mechanical Turk, which has been found to be
We approach this question using a multidimensional per- more nationally representative of the general population than
spective, including two different samples (an undergraduate college samples (Buhrmester et al. 2011). Participants were
Mindfulness

paid $0.30 for completing the survey questionnaires (see filler items thought to be gender-neutral (e.g., happy, friendly,
Buhrmester et al. 2011 for evidence of validity at low jealous). Participants respond according to the degree to
payment levels). Data collection was approved by an which the item reflects himself or herself, on a scale from 1
Institutional Review Board and followed standard procedures = Bnever or almost never^ to 7 = Balways or almost always
ensuring consent and privacy. true.^ Scores for the Masculinity and Femininity subscales are
calculated as the average of responses for items on each sub-
Measures scale. Participants are then classified into categories using a
median split for each subscale, calculated separately for men
Demographic Questionnaire A demographic questionnaire and women, into one of four gender role orientation catego-
asked participants to indicate their gender, current age, and ries: Undifferentiated (below the median for both subscales),
ethnic background. Feminine (at or above the median for femininity and below the
median for masculinity), Masculine (below the median for
Self-Compassion Scale Participants were administered the 26- femininity and at or above the median for masculinity), or
item Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff 2003a), which as- Androgynous (at or above the median for both femininity
sesses the six components of self-compassion: Self-Kindness and masculinity).
(e.g., BI try to be understanding and patient toward aspects of
my personality I don’t like^), Self-Judgment (e.g., BI’m Personal Attributes Questionnaire Participants were given the
disapproving and judgmental about my own flaws and Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence et al. 1974),
inadequacies^), Common Humanity (e.g., BI try to see my which contains eight items that reflect masculinity (instrumen-
failings as part of the human condition^), Isolation (e.g., tality/agency), and eight items that reflect femininity (expres-
BWhen I think about my inadequacies it tends to make me siveness/communality). Items are on bipolar scales anchored
feel more separate and cut off from the rest of the world^), at the upper end by a characteristic seen as more stereotypi-
Mindfulness (e.g., BWhen something painful happens I try to cally associated with and preferable for one gender than the
take a balanced view of the situation^), and Over-identification other, though socially desirable for both gender groups. For
(e.g., BWhen I’m feeling down I tend to obsess and fixate on example, upper anchors for the femininity subscale (e.g., very
everything that’s wrong^). Responses are given on a 5-point kind, very understanding of others) are thought to be desirable
scale ranging from 1 = BAlmost Never^ to 5 = BAlmost for both genders, but more strongly so for women, and more
Always.^ In order to calculate a total self-compassion score, typically exhibited by women; while upper anchors for the
a grand mean is taken of the six subscale means after items in masculinity subscale (e.g., independent, very self-confident)
the negative components are reverse coded. See Table 1 for are thought to be desirable for both genders, but more strongly
internal consistency for the SCS and other measures. so for men, and more typically exhibited by men (Spence and
Helmreich 1978). Participants respond on a scale of 1 to 5
Bem Sex Role Inventory Participants were administered the based on where they think they fall on the scale. Scores for
60-item Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem 1974), which the Masculinity and Femininity subscales are calculated as the
contains 20 items thought to reflect femininity (e.g., affection- average of responses for the items on each subscale.
ate, gentle, understanding); 20 items thought to reflect mascu- Participants are classified into the same groups as for the
linity (e.g., analytical, competitive, individualistic); and 20 BRSI, using a similar median split method, but with the

Table 1 Means, standard deviations (SDs) and internal reliability statistics for self-compassion and gender role orientation scales, by sample and
gender group (between-gender comparisons)

Undergraduate sample Community sample

Women (N = 266) Men (N = 238) Women (N = 616) Men (N = 352)

M (SD) α M (SD) α M (SD) α M (SD) α

Self-Compassion 2.94a (0.58) 0.91 3.10b (0.58) 0.91 2.92a (0.81) 0.95 3.16b (0.78) 0.94
Femininity (BSRI) 5.10a (0.57) 0.80 4.67b (0.60) 0.81 4.77a (0.79) 0.85 4.33b (0.76) 0.85
Masculinity (BSRI) 4.64a (0.71) 0.87 5.05b (0.74) 0.89 4.22a (0.88) 0.88 4.64b (0.90) 0.89
Femininity (PAQ) 3.96a (0.51) 0.79 3.76b (0.58) 0.80 3.89a (0.65) 0.82 3.64b (0.66) 0.81
Masculinity (PAQ) 3.40a (0.51) 0.69 3.73b (0.61) 0.79 3.26a (0.70) 0.78 3.52b (0.70) 0.79

Means with different letters differ significantly across gender groups within each sample (p < 0.05 for undergraduate sample, p < 0.01 for community
sample)
Mindfulness

median calculated across men and women. However, when Welch test statistics are provided for cases not meeting this
the gender groups differ in sample size, as here, the average of assumption. For all results, we utilized a p value of α = 0.05,
the medians for each gender group is used (Spence and two-tailed, for the undergraduate sample, given the explorato-
Helmreich 1978). ry nature of this work; and α = 0.01, two-tailed, for the com-
Note that utilizing predefined or external norms for the munity sample given its larger size and increased possibility of
medians is another option for classification for the PAQ; how- type I error. We report effect sizes to inform on practical mag-
ever, the more commonly used method of determining nitude of effects. We used Cohen’s (1988) rules of thumb for
sample-specific cutoffs accommodates differences in the effect size of t tests d: 0.20 = small, 0.50 = medium, and
unique balance of masculinity and femininity that is meaning- 0.80 = large. We used Keith’s (2006) rules of thumb for effect
ful within each sample. Note also that alternative scoring size of ANOVA η2 (equivalent to R2): 0.01 = small, 0.10 =
methods were examined for the PAQ in its initial construction, medium, and 0.25 = large.
including use of continuous scores in regression modeling
(Spence and Helmreich 1978). In the current study, we Regression Modeling In addition to examining mean differ-
employed the traditional median split method, and also exam- ences in self-compassion based on gender role orientation
ined continuous scale scores in regression models, for both the category membership, we also considered the association of
BSRI and the PAQ. This enabled us to examine the potentially self-compassion scores with continuous measures of feminin-
unique information that may be revealed using either of these ity and masculinity. While the BSRI and PAQ typically cate-
methods. gorize participants into groups based on the median split
method (Spence and Helmreich 1978; Hoffman and Borders
Data Analyses 2001), the continuous variables allowed the full range of
scores to be utilized in considering covariation with self-com-
Analyses were conducted in Stata Version 13.1 and Mplus passion. This also addressed the concern that some partici-
Version 7.4 and were identical for each sample. pants with average scores close to median cutoffs may be
Nonresponse for entire scales was minimal, as reported above, arbitrarily classified using the median split method, and
and missingness of data at the item level was considered to be allowed us to consider the unique contribution of masculinity,
at random, and was treated by averaging scores for other items femininity, and their interaction to variation in self-
on the respective scale or subscale, within participant. This compassion (holding the other characteristics constant) by en-
scale-wise treatment of missing data, which may be referred tering each as a separate predictor in regression models.
to as Bipsative mean imputation,^ essentially imputes the scale Additionally, these models allowed for within-group and
mean for each respondent’s observed items into the missing between-group analyses of these associations.
value. This method avoids reduction in sample size that occurs We assessed unstandardized path coefficients of predictors
with listwise deletion, and avoids reduction of variability in for statistical significance and assessed effect size using stan-
scores across participants that tends to occur in sample-based dardized regression coefficients. We also examined R2 values
imputation (Schafer and Graham 2002). The majority of cases for each model, as a measure of the amount of variance ex-
(99 to 100%) had at most 1–2 missing responses per scale or plained by the predictors, for each measure and sample, using
subscale in both samples, with the remaining cases having Keith’s (2006) rules of thumb: 0.01 = small, 0.10 = medium,
responses for at least half of the items in each scale or 0.25 = large. Cohen’s (1988) rules of thumb for determining
subscale. the effect size of standardized regression coefficients are as
Analyses consisted of two main parts, each utilized to ex- follows: 0.10 = small, 0.30 = medium, 0.50 = large.
amine within- and between-gender group differences in self- To determine main and potential interaction effects, we ran
compassion: mean comparisons using analysis of variance simultaneous regression analyses using Mplus, first within,
methods, and regression modeling. Note that because we did then across gender groups, for each gender orientation role
not have a diverse enough sample to analyze results by eth- measure. The first step of each regression model examined
nicity, we did not include ethnicity as a variable in this study. main effects of masculinity, femininity, and in the cross-
gender model only, the variable of gender, on self-compas-
Gender and Gender Orientation Role Mean Comparisons To sion. A second step added an interaction term between mas-
compare mean self-compassion scores, we relied on analysis culinity and femininity. The within-gender models centered
of variance methods using Stata. For comparisons between the masculinity and femininity terms using gender group-
gender groups, we utilized t-tests (analogous to a two-group mean centering, while the cross-gender models centered mas-
ANOVA). For comparisons within gender groups (across gen- culinity and femininity terms based on the whole sample. The
der role orientation), we used ANOVAs followed by Tukey within-gender models allow masculinity and femininity scores
tests (Stevens 1999). Bartlett’s tests of the homogeneity of to vary and covary as they uniquely do within each gender
variance assumption were examined for all ANOVAs, and group, and the mean used for centering to be defined
Mindfulness

accordingly, while the between-gender models consider the prior meta-analysis (Yarnell et al. 2015), college men
variances, covariances, and grand means to all be defined displayed higher self-compassion than college women,
without regard to gender group. ΔR2 between the step 1 and with a small effect size observed, t(502) = 3.09, p =
step 2 models was used as a measure of effect size for the 0.002, d = 0.28. (Barlett’s tests indicated that the homoge-
interaction term using Keith’s (2006) rules of thumb for neity assumption was met for all gender group compari-
ΔR2: 0.02 = small, 0.13 = medium, 0.26 = large. To probe sig- sons.) In the community sample, men also displayed
nificant interactions, we plotted the association (slope) be- higher self-compassion compared to women, with a small
tween one of the gender role orientation subscale scores effect size observed, t(966) = 4.44, p < 0.001, d = 0.30.
(femininity) and self-compassion for lower and higher levels Note that self-compassion levels did not significantly dif-
of the other subscale (masculinity), defined by cutpoints of − 1 fer between undergraduate and community women (p =
SD and + 1 SD above or below the mean. 0.72), or between undergraduate and community men
We utilized these models to examine whether gender role (p = 0.31). Table 1 also shows that men and women in
orientation contributes to individual differences in self-com- each sample differed significantly in levels of masculinity
passion, accounting for gender, and conversely, whether and femininity for both measures, in the expected direc-
between-gender differences in self-compassion persist, ac- tion (p < 0.001).
counting for gender role orientation. We chose simultaneous Correlations between scores on the BSRI and PAQ in
regression in lieu of alternative models that posit stronger the college sample were r = 0.70 for femininity and r =
theory as to causality or that give precedence to any one of 0.78 for masculinity, and r = 0.70 for femininity and r =
the examined variables. 0.73 for masculinity in the community sample. These rel-
atively high correlations support the concurrent validity of
the measures in the each sample (Bohrnstedt 2010), while
Results also reflecting the uniqueness of each measure (i.e., strong
but not perfect correlations were found).
Comparison of Self-Compassion by Gender Next, for within-gender comparison of self-compassion
and Gender Role Orientation Group scores, between gender role orientation groups, Table 2 shows
the percentage of men and women in each sample categorized
For between-gender comparison of self-compassion into each gender role orientation group and their mean self-
scores, with all gender role orientation groups combined, compassion score. As noted, the BSRI uses median split
Table 1 shows mean self-compassion scores by gender for scores within gender to classify gender role orientation
the undergraduate and community samples. As in the groups. Median values for femininity and masculinity

Table 2 Percentage of participants in each gender role orientation category and mean self-compassion score, by gender and measure, in undergraduate
and community samples

BSRI PAQ

Undiff Fem Masc Andro Undiff Fem Masc Andro

Undergraduate women (N = 266)


Gender role classification (percent) 26 24 25 24 27 37 14 23
Self-compassion mean 2.89a 2.77a 2.87a 3.22b 2.85a 2.74a 3.14b 3.23b
Undergraduate men (N = 238)
Gender role classification (percent) 24 26 23 27 24 13 32 32
Self-compassion mean 2.97 3.04 3.18 3.20 2.84a 2.81a 3.15b 3.35b
Community women (N = 616)
Gender role classification (percent) 23 25 25 26 25 25 18 31
Self-compassion mean 2.69a 2.76a 2.89a 3.30b 2.69a 2.50a 3.13b 3.33b
Community men (N = 352)
Gender role classification (percent) 30 22 20 27 25 12 34 29
Self-compassion mean 2.87a 3.16ab 3.22ab 3.42b 2.74a 2.92ab 3.28bc 3.46c

Means not sharing same letter differ significantly across gender role classification groups, within each sample and measure (p < 0.05 for undergraduate
sample, p < 0.01 for community sample). Percentages may not sum to 100 due to rounding. SD available from first author (omitted for parsimony)
Undiff undifferentiated, Fem feminine, Masc masculine, Andro androgynous
Mindfulness

(respectively) were as follows: college women (5.10 and differences based on the PAQ among college men was:
4.60), college men (4.65 and 5.15), community women Welch statistic (4, 123.70) = 10.91, p < 0.001, which sug-
(4.90 and 4.25), and community men (4.35 and 4.70). The gests an identical conclusion.)
PAQ classifies into gender role orientation groups based on Among community women, a one-way (BSRI gender
the average of subgroup medians that are found for each gen- role orientation) ANOVA showed significant gender role
der. Median values on the PAQ for femininity and masculinity orientation group differences in self-compassion, F(3,
(respectively) were as follows: undergraduates (3.88 and 3.57) 612) = 19.62, p < 0.001 (small to medium ES, R2 = 0.09).
and community adults (3.81 and 3.38). Similar to the undergraduate sample, post hoc Tukey tests
Note that the medians used for classification are slight- showed that androgynous women had higher self-
ly lower for all subscales and both genders in the com- compassion than women in each of the other gender role
munity sample, relative to the college sample; and squares orientation groups, who did not differ from each other.
of SD shown in Table 1 indicate that scores were consis- Results for community women based on the PAQ were
tently more variable for all subscales and both genders in also similar to college women, with significant gender
the community sample, as well. Note additionally that the orientation role group differences in self-compassion indi-
BSRI tended to classify participants more evenly across cated, F(3, 612) = 45.83, p < 0.001 (medium ES, R2 =
the gender role orientation groups than the PAQ, in terms 0.12). Post hoc Tukey tests showed that androgynous
of percentages. These statistics demonstrate that the dis- and masculine women (who did not differ) had higher
tributions of femininity and masculinity scores are unique self-compassion than undifferentiated and feminine wom-
for each sample, and highlight the importance of en (who did not differ). (The robust test result for gender
interpreting gender role orientation as defined in each role orientation group differences based on the BSRI
sample, accordingly. among community women was: Welch statistic (4,
As a test of whether differences in self-compassion 408.36) = 20.91, p < 0.001. Robust test result for gender
scores were significant, across gender orientation role role orientation group differences based on the PAQ
groups, within each gender, among college women, a among community women was as follows: Welch statistic
one-way (BSRI gender role orientation) ANOVA found (4, 391.99) = 42.92, p < 0.001. These tests suggest identi-
significant gender role orientation group differences in cal conclusions.)
self-compassion, F(3, 262) = 7.87, p < 0.001 (small to me- Among community men, a one-way (BSRI gender role
dium ES, R2 = 0.08). Post hoc Tukey tests showed that orientation) ANOVA showed significant gender role ori-
androgynous women had higher self-compassion than entation group differences in self-compassion, F(3,
women in each of the other gender role orientation 348) = 9.27, p < 0.001 (small ES, R 2 = 0.07). Post hoc
groups, who did not differ from each other. Results based Tukeys found that androgynous men had more self-
on the PAQ also indicated significant gender role orienta- compassion than undifferentiated men, but no other group
tion group differences in self-compassion, F(3, 262) = differences were found. For the PAQ, significant group
12.22, p < 0.001 (medium ES, R 2 = 0.12). Post hoc differences in self-compassion were also found, F(3,
Tukey tests showed that androgynous and masculine 348) = 18.40, p < 0.001 (medium ES, R 2 = 0.14), with
women (who did not differ from each other) had higher Tukey contrasts showing that androgynous men had
self-compassion than undifferentiated and feminine wom- higher self-compassion than feminine and undifferentiated
en (who did not differ from each other). (The robust test men. Masculine men also had more self-compassion than
result for gender role orientation group differences based undifferentiated men, but did not differ from androgynous
on the BSRI among college women was as follows: or feminine men. (The robust test result for gender role
Welch statistic (4, 172.32) = 9.00, p < 0.001, which sug- orientation group differences based on the BSRI among
gests an identical conclusion.) community men was as follows: Welch statistic (4,
Among college men, a one-way (BSRI gender role ori- 176.96) = 19.24, p < 0.001, which suggests an identical
entation) ANOVA found no significant gender role orien- conclusion.)
tation group differences in self-compassion, F(3, 234) =
2.17, p = 0.08 (R2 = 0.03). Results for college men based Within-Gender Associations of Self-Compassion
on the PAQ differed from those based on the BSRI, with with Continuous Gender Role Orientation Variables
significant gender role orientation group differences in
self-compassion, F(3, 234) = 12.41, p < 0.001 (medium To examine the associations of continuous measures of
ES, R2 = 0.14). Androgynous and masculine men (who masculinity, femininity, and their interaction within each
did not differ) had higher self-compassion levels than un- gender group, Table 3 shows results of the regression
differentiated and feminine men (who did not differ). (The models run within gender group, by sample. These
robust test result for gender role orientation group models permit the gender role orientation variables to
Mindfulness

vary and covary uniquely in each gender group, and they significant interactions found (BSRI for women, and
employ what is essentially group-mean centering. Main PAQ for men). Those high in both femininity and mascu-
effects remained similar in size and significance between linity had the highest levels of self-compassion.
the step 1 (main effects) and step 2 (main plus interaction
effects) models, so results are shown for step 2 models Community Sample Among community women, results
only for parsimony. were similar to those found in the undergraduate sample
based on the BSRI, with both femininity and masculinity
Undergraduate Sample Among undergraduate women, the showing significant, positive associations with self-com-
BSRI model showed that both femininity and masculinity passion, with a small ES for femininity and a medium ES
scores were significantly and positively associated with self- for masculinity (see standardized regression coefficients
compassion, with a small ES for femininity, and a medium ES in Table 3). Also, the interaction term was significantly
for masculinity (see standardized regression coefficients in associated with self-compassion beyond these main ef-
Table 3). The interaction term was also significant, with a fects, with a small ES. The PAQ model showed that mas-
small-to-medium ES. For the PAQ, the association of femi- culinity had a significant, positive association with self-
ninity with self-compassion scores was nonsignificant, while compassion, with a large ES indicated, but femininity did
masculinity scores were significantly associated, with a medi- not make a significant contribution. Unlike for undergrad-
um effect size. The interaction term was not significant (p = uate women, a significant interaction between femininity
0.29). Among undergraduate men, BSRI masculinity scores and masculinity scores was also found for community
were significantly and positively associated with self-compas- women, with a small ES indicated. As shown in Fig. S2,
sion, with a medium ES, while femininity and the interaction those high in both femininity and masculinity using each
term were not significant (p = 0.08 and p = 0.07, respectively). measure of gender orientation role had the highest levels
With the PAQ, however, both femininity and masculinity were of self-compassion.
significant, positive predictors, with a small ES for femininity Finally, among community men, results with the BRSI
and a medium ES for masculinity. The interaction term was model indicated significant, positive associations of both mas-
also significant, with a small ES. culinity and femininity with self-compassion, with a small-to-
Figure S1 of our online Supplementary Materials de- medium ES observed for femininity and a medium ES for
picts the association of femininity and self-compassion for masculinity, but no significant interaction. Results were simi-
undergraduates low versus high in masculinity for the lar among community men based on the PAQ, again with both

Table 3 Unstandardized (SE) and standardized regression coefficients and variance explained for regression models predicting self-compassion from
femininity and masculinity scores and their interaction (by gender, sample, and gender role orientation measure)

BSRI PAQ BSRI PAQ

b (SE) β b (SE) β b (SE) β b (SE) β

Undergraduate women (N = 266) Community women (N = 616)


Intercept 2.94 (0.03) – 2.93 (0.03) – 2.92 (0.03) – 2.91 (0.03) –
Femininity 0.13* (0.06) 0.13 0.00 (0.06) 0.00 0.14*** (0.04) 0.13 0.05 (0.04) 0.04
Masculinity 0.22*** (0.05) 0.27 0.46*** (0.06) 0.41 0.27*** (0.04) 0.29 0.60*** (0.04) 0.52
Interaction 0.26** (0.08) 0.18 0.13 (0.12) 0.06 0.15*** (0.04) 0.16 0.16*** (0.05) 0.10
R2 0.12 0.17 0.11 0.29
ΔR2 0.03 0.00 0.02 0.01
Undergraduate men (N = 238) Community men (N = 352)
Intercept 3.09 (0.04) – 3.08 (0.04) – 3.15 (0.04) – 3.15 (0.04) –
Femininity 0.11 (0.06) 0.11 0.14* (0.06) 0.14 0.19*** (0.05) 0.18 0.17** (0.06) 0.15
Masculinity 0.20*** (0.05) 0.25 0.36*** (0.06) 0.37 0.22*** (0.05) 0.25 0.51*** (0.05) 0.46
Interaction 0.13 (0.07) 0.12 0.19* (0.07) 0.15 0.02 (0.05) 0.02 0.03 (0.07) 0.02
R2 0.08 0.18 0.12 0.27
ΔR2 0.01 0.03 0.00 0.00

Femininity and Masculinity subscale scores centered for each measure, within each gender group and sample, prior to cross-multiplication. Results are
for step 2 (main and interaction effect) models, with ΔR2 indicated relative to step 1 (main effects only) models. Significance and standardized
coefficient omitted for intercepts
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
Mindfulness

femininity and masculinity scores showing significant, posi- social sciences), though a statistically significant amount of
tive associations with self-compassion, with a small ES ob- variance remained unaccounted for (p < 0.001), as indicated
served for femininity and a large ES for masculinity, but no by the residual variance parameter (as found for all models
significant interaction. presented here). This suggests that both gender and gender role
orientation (as measured by the BSRI) contribute meaningfully
Across-Gender Associations of Self-Compassion to individual differences in self-compassion, accounting for
with Continuous Gender Role Orientation Variables each other—that is, gender differences in self-compassion
persist even accounting for gender role orientation, and vice
Finally, to examine the associations of continuous mea- versa. However, the significant residual variance indicates that
sures of masculinity, femininity, and their interaction other sources of individual differences exist.
across gender groups, Table 4 shows results of the regres- In contrast, results based on the PAQ showed that mas-
sion models run for men and women combined, by sam- culinity scores were positively and significantly associat-
ple. These models directly address the question of wheth- ed with self-compassion, with a medium effect size, ac-
er gender group differences in self-compassion persist, counting for the other predictors, while gender and femi-
when taking gender role orientation into account. By def- ninity scores were not (p = 0.61 and p = 0.17, respective-
inition, the gender role orientation variables vary and co- ly). This suggests that after accounting for gender role
vary in these models as they do in each sample as a orientation, gender differences between men and women
whole, and for consistency, grand sample-mean centering in self-compassion are no longer significant. Additionally,
was applied. As main effects remained similar in size and a statistically significant, small-sized interaction was indi-
significance between the step 1 (main effects) and step 2 cated. The predictors in this model explained about 19%
(main plus interaction effects) models, results are again of variance in self-compassion.
shown for step 2 models only for parsimony. Notably, in both the BSRI and PAQ models, gender
and femininity had roughly equivalent association with
Undergraduate Sample For undergraduates, results based on self-compassion, accounting for other predictors in the
the BSRI showed small, statistically significant, positive asso- model, while the association for masculinity was stronger,
ciations of gender (coded B0^ for women and B1^ for men) and the interaction between masculinity and femininity
and femininity scores with self-compassion; and a medium- was meaningful. The association for gender depended on
sized, statistically significant, positive association of mascu- the gender role orientation measure. Figure S3 depicts the
linity with self-compassion (see standardized regression association of femininity and self-compassion for under-
coefficients in Table 4). Additionally, the interaction term graduates low versus high in masculinity for the signifi-
showed a significant and small effect, above and beyond these cant interactions found for each measure. Those high in
main effects. These predictors explained 11% of variance in both femininity and masculinity had the highest levels of
self-compassion (a medium-sized amount of variance in the self-compassion.

Table 4 Unstandardized (SE) and standardized regression coefficients and variance explained for regression models predicting self-compassion from
gender, femininity, masculinity, and femininity by masculinity interaction terms (by sample and gender role orientation measure)

Undergraduate sample (N = 504) Community sample (N = 968)

BSRI PAQ BSRI PAQ

b (SE) β b (SE) β b (SE) β b (SE) β

Intercept 2.96 (0.04) – 2.99 (0.04) – 2.93 (0.03) – 2.96 (0.03) –


Gender 0.12* (0.06) 0.10 0.03 (0.06) 0.02 0.19*** (0.05) 0.11 0.11 (0.05) 0.06
Femininity 0.12** (0.05) 0.13 0.07 (0.05) 0.06 0.15*** (0.03) 0.15 0.09 (0.04) 0.08
Masculinity 0.21*** (0.04) 0.27 0.41*** (0.04) 0.41 0.25*** (0.03) 0.29 0.58*** (0.03) 0.51
Interaction 0.15*** (0.04) 0.14 0.19** (0.07) 0.13 0.10*** (0.03) 0.11 0.12** (0.05) 0.08
R2 0.11 0.19 0.13 0.29
ΔR2 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.00

Gender groups coded as 0 = women, 1 = men. Femininity and Masculinity subscale scores centered for each measure, based on the whole sample, prior to
cross-multiplication. Results are for step 2 (main and interaction effect) models, with ΔR2 indicated relative to step 1 (main effects only) models.
Significance and standardized coefficient omitted for intercepts
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
Mindfulness

Community Sample In the community sample, results similar- may also be important, especially for those who identify
ly showed that the strongest predictor of self-compassion was as women.
masculinity (see Table 4). Specifically, similar to results for
the college sample, the BSRI model showed statistically sig-
nificant, positive associations of all main effect predictors with Contributions of Gender and Gender Role Orientation
self-compassion: small effects for gender and femininity, and
a medium effect for masculinity. Additionally, a statistically This study replicated past meta-analytic findings that self-
significant, small-sized interaction was indicated. Similar to identified men tend to have slightly higher levels of self-
the college sample, these predictors explained about 13% of compassion than self-identified women (Yarnell et al. 2015),
variance in self-compassion scores. to the magnitude of a small effect size. This difference was
Results for the communuity PAQ model were also sim- found for both college and community samples. Gender dif-
ilar to those for the PAQ in the college sample. ferences in self-compassion are consistent with research indi-
Specifically, statistical significance (p < 0.01) was not cating that women tend to be more self-critical (Cheng and
reached for gender or femininity, but a large, statistically Furnham 2004), and to brood and ruminate (Johnson and
significant, positive effect was observed for masculinity. Whisman 2013; Nolen-Hoeksema et al. 1999), and have lower
Additionally, a statistically significant, small-sized inter- levels of mindfulness than men on average (Bergomi et al.
action was indicated. These variables accounted for 29% 2012; Feldman et al. 2007; Lilja et al. 2011; Dundas et al.
of variance in self-compassion, considered a large amount 2013; Gilbert and Waltz 2010). This finding may appear sur-
of variance explained according to rules of thumb, though prising in light of literature indicating that women tend to
as for all other models, a significant amount of variance in display more empathic concern and compassion for others
scores remained unexplained (p < 0.001). than men do (Mestre et al. 2009; Sprecher and Fehr 2005;
As in the college sample, overall, the models showed a Van der Graaff et al. 2014). It appears that this tendency does
relatively stronger association of masculinity with self- not generalize to how women treat themselves, however. This
compassion scores than other predictors, and suggested also implies that there is a greater discrepancy between the
that the interaction between masculinity and femininity degree of compassion women show to themselves versus
is meaningful. Similarly, the association between gender others, compared to men.
and self-compassion, accounting for other variables, When analyzing the contribution of gender role orientation,
depended on the gender role orientation measure. however, results were more complex. Our findings suggest
Figure S4 depicts the association of femininity and self- that gender role orientation plays an important role in individ-
compassion for community sample members low versus ual self-compassion levels. In general, androgynous and mas-
high in masculinity for the significant interactions found culine men and women tended to have the highest levels of
for each measure. Those high in both femininity and mas- self-compassion. First, among both undergraduate and com-
culinity had the highest levels of self-compassion. munity women, comparisons using gender orientation role
categories based on the BSRI showed that androgynous wom-
en had more self-compassion than women in each of the other
Discussion gender orientation role groups (who did not differ from each
other). Using the PAQ, undergraduate and community women
Findings from this study provide greater nuance to prior who were classified as androgynous and masculine (who did
meta-analytic findings of lower levels of self-compassion not differ from each other) had more self-compassion than
among women relative to men in North America (Yarnell undifferentiated and feminine women (who did not differ from
et al. 2015). While replicating this finding, current results each other).
also consistently showed that the impact of self-identified Among undergraduate men, there were no differences in
gender on self-compassion appears to be smaller than the self-compassion according to gender orientation role category
impact of having a masculine gender role orientation, sug- based on the BSRI. Using the PAQ, androgynous and mascu-
gesting that socialization plays a strong role in the tenden- line men (who did not differ from each other) had higher self-
cy to be compassionate to oneself. Also, those high in compassion levels than undifferentiated and feminine men
both communal and expressive traits (associated with (who did not differ from each other), as was found for under-
Bfemininity^) and agentic and instrumental traits (associ- graduate and community women. When examining the self-
ated with Bmasculinity^) appear to have the highest levels compassion levels of community men, androgynous men had
of self-compassion. Additionally, a key finding was that more self-compassion than undifferentiated men using the
masculinity’s association with self-compassion was stron- BSRI, but no other group differences were found. Using the
ger than the association for femininity, yet interactions PAQ, androgynous men had more self-compassion than fem-
between levels of levels of masculinity and femininity inine and undifferentiated men. Masculine men also had more
Mindfulness

self-compassion than undifferentiated men, but did not differ orientation giving themselves permission to be kind to them-
from androgynous or feminine men. selves. Masculine traits emphasizing autonomy and self-asser-
Results from our regression models examining gender role tion, on the other hand, may facilitate caring for the self with-
orientation as continuous predictors of self-compassion within out being in conflict with norms of self-sacrifice.
each gender and sample yielded similar results. Overall, mas- The interaction between masculinity and femininity also
culinity was the most consistent positive predictor of self- significantly predicted self-compassion, especially for those
compassion, for both men and women and with both gender identifying as women. Note that three of the four within-
role orientation measures, with a medium effect size typically gender regression models found that the interaction between
observed. Femininity was a significant positive predictor masculinity and femininity significantly predicted self-
using the BSRI for undergraduate and community women, compassion for women, but only one was found for men.
as well as community men; and was a positive significant This may suggest that women experience more robust benefits
predictor using the PAQ for undergraduate and community in self-compassion when they embrace both masculine and
men (but not women), with a small effect size. Moreover, feminine qualities. This is in accord with research suggesting
the interaction of masculinity and femininity was a significant that androgynous women experience more authentic assertion
predictor in three of the four models examined among women of their needs, perceive higher levels of support, and evaluate
(except for the PAQ model for undergraduate women), with a themselves more positively (Harter et al. 1998). It is also con-
small effect size. In contrast, the interaction was only signifi- sistent with research showing that androgynous women have
cant in one of the four models examined among men (the PAQ greater perceived self-efficacy and mental health (Rath and
model for undergraduate men). Mishra 2013; Thornton and Leo 1992) and are better able to
The finding that masculinity was the strongest and most deal with stress and bounce back from failure (Nevid and
consistent predictor of self-compassion may be surprising giv- Rathus 2016). If Bmasculine^ norms tend to emphasize auton-
en that masculine gender norms demand display of Bhard^ omy and independence and Bfeminine^ norms caring and nur-
rather than Bsoft^ emotions in the face of adversity (Mahalik turing, then it may be that having both helps women to fully
et al. 2003). Yet, self-compassion has both Byin^ and Byang^ apply self-compassion to themselves. Still, it is likely that
aspects, and can take the form of protecting, providing, and balancing yin and yang qualities is most conductive to the
motivating the self, qualities that are part of more masculine development of self-compassion for persons of all genders.
gender role norms (Neff and Germer 2018). A common mis- Further research should explore this issue further.
perception about self-compassion that stands in the way of An important question concerns the degree to which
people adopting the stance is that it is Bweak^ (Robinson gender differences in self-compassion are due to self-
et al. 2016). However, self-compassion is a strength in times identified gender versus gender role orientation. First of
of struggle, helping people cope in a more powerful manner all, note that self-identified men and self-identified wom-
with stress (Allen and Leary 2010), chronic illness (Sirois en differed in terms of their levels of masculinity and
et al. 2015), divorce (Sbarra et al. 2012), and even combat femininity for both samples, and according to both gender
trauma (Hiraoka et al. 2015). Also, masculine gender role role orientation measures—suggesting that gender differ-
norms value self-assertion and independence, which may ences in self-compassion may be due in part to gender
translate into a greater willingness to take one’s own need differences in gender role orientation. When entering gen-
seriously and give oneself compassion in times of distress. der and gender role orientation as simultaneous predictors
It should be noted that femininity also significantly predict- of self-compassion, results differed by measure. Results of
ed self-compassion in the positive direction, even though find- the cross-gender regression models based on the BSRI
ings were less consistent and effect sizes were typically small. suggested that differences in self-compassion between
Thus, our results suggest that higher levels of the traits asso- men and women remain even accounting for gender role
ciated with femininity are not a liability in terms of self- orientation; while results based on the PAQ indicated that
compassion and help buttress levels of the construct, just not the association of gender with self-compassion was no
as strongly as the traits associated with masculinity. Feminine longer significant, after masculinity, femininity, and their
traits such as being affectionate, gentle, understanding toward interaction were accounted for. The results also suggested
others correspond to the Byin^ self-compassion qualities of that gender role orientation predicts meaningful differ-
soothing, comforting, and validating, and appear to generalize ences in self-compassion, accounting for gender.
at least somewhat toward the self. However, feminine norms Additionally, all four cross-gender models suggested that
of self-sacrifice (which are themselves influenced by power levels of masculinity have the greatest impact on self-
inequality; Neff and Harter 2002) may somewhat attenuate the compassion among the three examined variables, and that
strength of this association. The common misperception that the interaction between masculinity and femininity con-
self-compassion is Bselfish^ (Robinson et al. 2016) may also tributes additionally to these levels. Last, results consis-
be standing in the way of people with a feminine gender role tently showed that the impact of gender on self-
Mindfulness

compassion was smaller than the impact of masculine between masculinity and femininity contributes meaningfully
gender role orientation, suggesting that socialization plays to individual differences in self-compassion to some extent.
a strong role. The fact that these patterns were obtained across analytic ap-
proaches, measures, and samples adds support to our findings.
Multiplistic Approach to Gender, Gender Role
Orientation, and Self-Compassion Limitations and Future Research Directions

As emphasized in this discussion, our findings were shaped by While providing insight on the role of gender role orientation
the gender role orientation measure used, as well as the sample in interpreting gender differences in self-compassion, this in-
being investigated. The BSRI and PAQ take the same general vestigation had several limitations. First, sizable variation in
approach to measuring gender role orientation (direct assess- self-compassion remained unaccounted for, above and be-
ment of self-identification with two independent trait qualities, yond the gender and gender role orientation variables. This
with item pools based on original US undergraduate student highlights that there are a host of additional sources of varia-
samples). Yet item pools and scales for the BSRI and PAQ tion in self-compassion besides gender and gender role orien-
differ (e.g., the latter employs bipolar endorsement subscales, tation that were not examined. Variables such as attachment
and focusing more directly on instrumentality and expressive- style (Pepping et al. 2015; Wei et al. 2011), parental criticism
ness). Our two samples were also distinct in terms of impor- (Neff and McGehee 2010), and trauma history (Tanaka et al.
tant social characteristics including age and level of education. 2011) have all been shown to have a strong influence on self-
Accordingly, both main and interaction effects differed to compassion, and it is likely that individual life history plays an
some degree depending on the model at hand. even more important role than gender or gender role orienta-
The fact that the PAQ models consistently explained more tion in influencing self-compassion. It may also be that sexual
variance in self-compassion scores than the BSRI models may orientation interacts with gender and gender role orientation in
suggest that instrumentality and expressiveness (as directly terms of predicting self-compassion, and this possibility
measured in the PAQ) have stronger associations with self- should be explored in future research.
compassion than Bmasculinity^ and Bfemininity^ as repre- Importantly, the roles of social stigma and hegemony in
sented by BSRI. While the BSRI has been suggested to largely these processes should be examined. Research has suggested
measure instrumentality and expressiveness as well (Spence that cross-gender role orientation may be accompanied by
and Helmreich 1978), it has also been argued that these are perceived stigma, perhaps particularly so among those who
only aspects of what the BSRI measures (Bem 1981; also see identify as men (Kane 2006; Martin 1990; Sirin et al. 2004),
Spence and Buckner 2000). For example, the BSRI contains suggesting a system in which traditional gender roles are re-
some items not reflective of instrumentality and expressive- inforced. The fact that the Bmasculine^ items in the BSRI and
ness per se, instead more directly reflecting qualities that have PAQ were thought in original samples to be more desirable for
traditionally been more desirable for one gender group (e.g., men than for women highlights systems of power in which it
Bathletic^ among the masculinity items, and Bshy^ and Bsoft- is more desirable for men than for women to be instrumental
spoken^ among the femininity items). The BSRI scores or agentic. Subsequent research has suggested that many items
showed strong internal consistency for both samples and both in the BRSI scales can be viewed as equally desirable among
genders in our study. Nonetheless, reliability does not imply women and men, as gender-neutral, or as being mixed in in-
validity, i.e., that a construct has been properly named. terpretation (Ballard-Reisch and Elton 1992; Hoffman and
However, correlations between the measures within each sam- Borders 2001). This suggests that the scales, while reliable,
ple and gender indicated a great deal of overlap, as reported, may be assessing somewhat different constructs than intended
supporting the construct validity of the measures (i.e., the in original design. Therefore, interpretations of our findings
BSRI and PAQ measure largely the same construct). More should be made cautiously, and further consideration of the
research should be done on the precise characteristics in- constructs underlying the associations uncovered here is war-
volved in the associations uncovered here, and other methods ranted. Another major limitation is that we examined US sam-
of measuring gender role orientation should be explored (e.g., ples only and were not able to examine findings separately by
priming tasks). ethnic group. Gender role orientation has been found to differ
While these nuances add richness to our findings, the same in meaning across U.S. ethnic groups (Abrams et al. 2016), so
overarching findings were obtained: self-identified gender and it will be important to explore these findings in other popula-
gender role orientation each contribute meaningfully to indi- tions to examine the robustness of effects across ethnic and
vidual and group differences in self-compassion (with all ef- cultural groups.
fects consistently in a positive direction); both masculinity and Also, our findings were limited to understanding gender
femininity are positively associated with self-compassion role orientation as measured by the BSRI and PAQ. As em-
(though masculinity to a greater degree); and the interaction phasized, both of these measures have received some criticism
Mindfulness

(see Hoffman and Borders 2001), so future research might Developers of the MSC program are trying to develop ways to
fruitfully employ other ways to measure gender orientation attract more men to their program, in part by using language
role such as priming tasks (e.g., van Well et al. 2007), such as Binner strength training^ rather than Bself-compassion
Implicit Association Tasks (e.g., Greenwald and Banaji training.^ Pilot testing suggests that reframing language helps
1995), or gender diagnosticity measures (e.g., Lippa 1991). men be open to self-compassion, especially to the Byin^ qual-
However, the BRSI and the PAQ offer a Bstandard^ definition ities that may feel vulnerable at first. People are increasingly
of what has been thought to constitute Bmasculinity^ and recognizing that social prescriptions against vulnerability for
Bfemininity^ in North America via their set item pools; yet men contribute to a culture of violence, and there appears to be
the traditional scoring methods for these measures define the greater willingness on the part of some men to confront these
relative balance of Bmasculine^ and Bfeminine^ characteris- prescriptions (Jewkes et al. 2015). Feminine gender roles
tics used for gender role classification to be derived internally, seem not to interfere as strongly with the willingness to learn
based on the sample at hand, in acknowledging the Bfluid^ self-compassion, although there is also a great desire among
definition of gender roles with respect to sample characteris- women to explore the Byang^ aspects, in part due to societal
tics. Nonetheless, future research should corroborate findings events highlighting the harmful consequences of patriarchy
with observer reports, in light of the bias potentially intro- (O'Neil et al. 2018). Thus, the developers of MSC are starting
duced by these self-report measures. Future research might to explicitly discuss the interwoven nature of yin and yang in
also fruitfully employ qualitative methods to better understand self-compassion and teach practices cultivating both, which
why self-compassion was higher among androgynous and will hopefully help all persons to be more self-
masculine persons, and the ways in which distinct groups compassionate in daily life.
manifest these characteristics. Given the robust benefits of self-compassion, it will be
The cross-sectional nature of this research means that the important to address how gender socialization norms may be
directionality of effects cannot be established. It is possible putting some persons at a disadvantage in terms of adopting
that having a particular gender role orientation impacts level this adaptive way of coping with difficult emotions and life
of self-compassion, but it is equally possible that having a events. One strategy may be for initiatives to focus on encour-
certain level of self-compassion influences the embrace of aging the development of self-compassion among all people,
characteristics in the gender role orientation measures—for regardless of identity affirmed or direction of predominant
example, being willing to take (healthy) risks (an indicator gender role orientation.
of Bmasculinity^); or being sensitive to the needs of others
(an indicator of Bfemininity^). Future research may employ Authors’ Contributions LY: designed and executed the study, collected
the college student data, conducted all submitted analyses, and wrote
longitudinal designs to try to better understand these associa-
large portions of the paper. KN: collaborated with the design and writing
tions, and path modeling can be used to explore how variation of the study, including choice of statistical models and theoretical align-
in gender orientation role and self-compassion explain gender ment. OD: collected the community sample data. MM: assisted with data
differences for specific outcomes. analysis, literature search, and writing. All authors approved the final
version of the manuscript for submission.
Although our research found that women tend to have
slightly lower levels of self-compassion than men, self-
compassion is a skill that can be learned by all people. The Compliance with Ethical Standards
Mindful Self-Compassion program (Neff and Germer 2013),
Ethics Statement No funding was received for this study. All proce-
for instance, and its adaptation for adolescents called Making dures performed involving human participants were in accordance with
Friends with Yourself (Bluth et al. 2016), have both been the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research commit-
shown to be effective in raising participants’ self-compassion tee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or
levels, in addition to improving other aspects of wellbeing comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies
with animals performed by any of the authors. Informed consent was
such as lowering depression and increasing life satisfaction. obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Both parts
Interestingly, based on the personal observations of the second of the data collection (undergraduate and community) were reviewed and
author, programs such as MSC tend to disproportionately at- approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of
tract women (about 85% of the audience). Even though men Texas at Austin.
tend to be slightly more self-compassionate than women, they
Conflict of Interest Lisa M. Yarnell declares that she has no conflicts of
may have stronger misgivings about self-compassion interest. Kristin D. Neff is co-creator of the Mindful Self-Compassion
(Robinson et al. 2016), though this has yet to be researched. program, described in this manuscript. Oliver A. Davidson declares that
The fear that self-compassion is a weakness might mean that he has no conflicts of interest. Michael Mullarkey declares that he has no
men are less likely to sign up for a self-compassion course. conflicts of interest.
There are many men who lack self-compassion who could
greatly benefit from developing this skill, but ironically, mas- Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
culine gender role norms may prevent them from doing so. dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Mindfulness

References Cheng, C. (2005). Processes underlying gender-role flexibility: Do an-


drogynous individuals know more or know how to cope? Journal of
Personality, 73, 645–673.
Abrams, J. A., Javier, S. J., Maxwell, M. L., Belgrave, F. Z., & Nguyen,
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2004). Perceived parental rearing style, self-
A. B. (2016). Distant but relative: Similarities and differences in
esteem and self-criticism as predictors of happiness. Journal of
gender role beliefs among African American and Vietnamese
Happiness Studies, 5(1), 1–21.
American women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
Psychology, 22(2), 256–267. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ahmed, T., Vafaei, A., Belanger, E., Phillips, S., & Zunzunegui, M.
Davis, S. N. (2009). Bem sex role inventory. In J. O’Brien (Ed.),
(2016). Bem sex role inventory validation in the international mo-
Encyclopedia of gender and society (pp. 58–60). Thousand Oaks,
bility in aging study. Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue
CA: Sage.
Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 35(3), 348–360.
Deaux, K., & Kite, M. E. (1993). Gender stereotypes. In F. Denmark &
Albertson, E. R., Neff, K. D., & Dill-Shackleford, K. E. (2015). Self-
M. Paludi (Eds.), Handbook on the psychology of women (pp. 107–
compassion and body dissatisfaction in women: A randomized con-
139). Westport, CT:Greenwood Press.
trolled trial of a brief meditation intervention. Mindfulness, 6, 444–
Dundas, I., Vøllestad, J., Binder, P. E., & Sivertsen, B. (2013). The five
454.
factor mindfulness questionnaire in Norway. Scandinavian Journal
Allen, A., & Leary, M. R. (2010). Self-compassion, stress, and coping.
of Psychology, 54, 250–260.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(2), 107–118.
Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.-P.
Allen, A. B., Goldwasser, E. R., & Leary, M. R. (2012). Self-compassion (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development
and well-being among older adults. Self and Identity, 11, 428–453. and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
American Psychological Association & National Association of School Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and
Psychologists (APA & NASP). (2015). Resolution on gender and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 177–190.
sexual orientation diversity in children and adolescents in schools. Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors.
Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/about/policy/orientation- Mindfulness, 1, 227–234.
diversity.aspx. Accessed 15 July 2018 Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition:
Aube, J., Norcliffe, H., Craig, J., & Koestner, R. (1995). Gender charac- Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102,
teristics and adjustment-related outcomes: Questioning the mascu- 4–27.
linity model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 284– Harter, S., Waters, P., Whitesell, N. R., & Kastelic, D. (1998). Level of
295. voice among high school women and men: Relational context, sup-
Baker-Miller, J. (1986). Toward a new psychology of women. New York: port, and gender orientation. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1–10.
McGraw Hill. Hiraoka, R., Meyer, E. C., Kimbrel, N. A., DeBeer, B. B., Gulliver, S. B.,
Ballard-Reisch, D., & Elton, M. (1992). Gender orientation and the Bem & Morissette, S. B. (2015). Self-compassion as a prospective pre-
Sex Role Inventory: A psychological construct revisited. Sex Roles, dictor of PTSD symptom severity among trauma-exposed U.S. Iraq
27, 291–306. and Afghanistan war veterans. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28, 1–7.
Barnard, L. K., & Curry, J. F. (2012). The relationship of clergy burnout Hoffman, R. M., & Borders, L. D. (2001). Twenty-five years after the
to self-compassion and other personality dimensions. Pastoral Bem Sex-Role Inventory: A reassessment and new issues regarding
Psychology, 61, 149–163. classification variability. Measurement and Evaluation in
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Counseling and Development, 34, 39–55.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. Hollis-Walker, L., & Colosimo, K. (2011). Mindfulness, self-compassion,
Bem, S. L. (1981). The BSRI and gender schema theory: A reply to and happiness in non-meditators: A theoretical and empirical exam-
Spence and Helmreich. Psychological Review, 88, 369–371. ination. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 222–227.
Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex typing and the avoidance of cross- Holt, C. L., & Ellis, J. B. (1998). Assessing the current validity of the
sex behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 48– Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 39, 929–941.
54. Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American
Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2012). Measuring mindful- Psychologist, 60, 581–592.
ness: First steps towards the development of a comprehensive mind- Jewkes, R., Flood, M., & Lang, J. (2015). From work with men and boys
fulness scale. Mindfulness, 4, 18–32. to changes of social norms and reduction of inequities in gender
Berthiaume, M., David, H., Saucier, J.-F., & Borgeat, F. (1996). relations: A conceptual shift in prevention of violence against wom-
Correlates of gender role orientation during pregnancy and the post- en and girls. The Lancet, 385(9977), 1580–1589.
partum. Sex Roles, 35, 781–800. Johnson, D. P., & Whisman, M. A. (2013). Gender differences in rumi-
Bluth, K., Gaylord, S. A., Campo, R. A., Mullarkey, M. C., & Hobbs, L. nation: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences,
(2016). Making friends with yourself: A mixed methods pilot study 55(4), 367–374.
of a mindful self-compassion program for adolescents. Mindfulness, Kane, E. W. (2006). BNo way my boys are going to be like that!^:
7, 479–492. Parents’ responses to children’s gender nonconformity. Gender &
Bohrnstedt, G. W. (2010). Measurement models for survey research. In P. Society, 20, 149–176.
V. Marsden & J. D. Wright (Eds.), Handbook of survey research Keith, T. Z. (2006). Multiple regression and beyond. Boston, MA: Allyn
(2nd ed., pp. 347–404). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing and Bacon.
Limited. Lam, C. B., & McBride-Chang, C. A. (2007). Resilience in young adult-
Breines, J. G., & Chen, S. (2013). Activating the inner caregiver: The role hood: The moderating influences of gender-related personality traits
of support-giving schemas in increasing state self-compassion. and coping flexibility. Sex Roles, 56, 159–172.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 58–64. Levant, R. F. (2011). Research in the psychology of men and masculinity
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s using the gender role strain paradigm as a framework. American
Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, Psychologist, 66, 765–776.
data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. Levant, R. F., & Pollack, W. (Eds.). (1995). A new psychology of men.
New York: Basic Books.
Mindfulness

Lilja, J. L., Frodi-Lundgren, A., Hanse, J. J., Josefsson, T., Lundh, L. G., Oswald, P. A. (2004). An examination of the current usefulness of the
Skold, C., et al. (2011). Five Facets Mindfulness Questionnaire— Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Psychological Reports, 94(3 Pt 2), 1331–
Reliability and factor structure: A Swedish version. Cognitive 1336.
Behavior Therapy, 40, 291–303. Patzak, A., Kollmayer, M., & Schober, B. (2017). Buffering impostor
Lippa, L. (1985). Review of Bem sex-role inventory. In J. V. Mitchell feelings with kindness: The mediating role of self-compassion be-
(Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (Vol. 1, pp. 176– tween gender-role orientation and the impostor phenomenon.
178). Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1289.
Lippa, R. (1991). Some psychometric characteristics of gender Pederson, E. L., & Vogel, D. L. (2007). Men’s gender role conflict and
dignosticity measures: Reliability, validity, consistency across do- their willingness to seek counseling: A mediation model. Journal of
mains, and relationship to the Big Five. Journal of Personality Counseling Psychology, 54, 373–384.
and Social Psychology, 74, 1000–1011. Pedhazur, E. J., & Tetenbaum, T. J. (1979). Bem Sex Role Inventory: A
Lips, H. M. (2017). Sex and gender: An introduction (6th ed.). Long theoretical and methodological critique. Journal of Personality and
Grove, IL: Waveland Press, Inc. Social Psychology, 37, 996–1016.
MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: A meta- Pepping, C. A., Davis, P. J., O'Donovan, A., & Pal, J. (2015). Individual
analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopa- differences in self-compassion: The role of attachment and experi-
thology. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 545–552. ences of parenting in childhood. Self and Identity, 14, 104–117.
Mahalik, J. R., Locke, B., Ludlow, L., Diemer, M., Scott, R. P. J., Prakash, J., Kotwal, A. S., Ryali, V., Srivastava, K., Bhat, P. S., &
Gottfried, M., & Freitas, G. (2003). Development of the conformity Shashikumar, R. (2010). Does androgyny have psychoprotective
to masculine norms inventory. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 4, attributes? A cross-sectional community-based study. Industrial
3–25. Psychiatry Journal, 19, 119–124.
Martin, C. L. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with Price, E. C., Gregg, J. J., Smith, M. D., & Fiske, A. (2018). Masculine
nontraditional and traditional gender roles. Sex Roles, 22, 151–165. traits and depressive symptoms in older and younger men and wom-
Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Frías, M. D., & Tur, A. M. (2009). Are women en. American Journal of Men’s Health, 12, 19–29.
more empathetic than men? A longitudinal study in adolescence. Priess, H. A., Lindberg, S. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2009). Adolescent gender-
The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12(1), 76–83. role identity and mental health: Gender intensification revisited.
Moscovitch, D. A., Hofmann, S. G., & Litz, B. T. (2005). The impact of Child Development, 80(5), 1531–1544.
self-construals on social anxiety: A gender-specific interaction. Prince, V. (2005). Sex vs. gender. International Journal of
Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 659–672. Transgenderism, 8, 29–32.
Neff, K. D. (2003a). The development and validation of a scale to mea- Raffaelli, M., & Ontai, L. L. (2004). Gender socialization in Latino/a
sure self-compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250. families: Results from two retrospective studies. Sex Roles, 50,
287–299.
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization
Rath, S., & Mishra, A. (2013). Self-efficacy of androgynous and sex-
of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–102.
typed employed and unemployed women. Journal of Social
Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. K. (2013). A pilot study and randomized
Sciences, 2(3), 139–145.
controlled trial of the Mindful Self-Compassion Program. Journal
Reilly, E. D., Rochlen, A. B., & Awad, G. H. (2014). Men’s self-
of Clinical Psychology, 69, 28–44.
compassion and self-esteem: The moderating roles of shame and
Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. K. (2018). The Mindful Self-Compassion masculine norm adherence. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 15,
workbook: A proven way to accept yourself, find inner strength, 22–28.
and thrive. New York: Guilford Press.
Richards, C., Bouman, W. P., Seal, L., Barker, M.-J., Nieder, T. O., &
Neff, K. D., & Harter, S. (2002). The role of power and authenticity in T’Sjoen, G. (2016). Non-binary or genderqueer genders.
relationship styles emphasizing autonomy, connectedness, or mutu- International Review of Psychiatry, 28, 95–102.
ality among adult couples. Journal of Social and Personal
Robinson, K. J., Mayer, S., Allen, A. B., Terry, M., Chilton, A., & Leary,
Relationships, 19, 827–849.
M. R. (2016). Resisting self-compassion: Why are some people
Neff, K. D., & McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological opposed to being kind to themselves? Self and Identity, 15(5),
resilience among adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9, 505–524.
225–240. Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon
Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2007). An examination of (series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol-
self-compassion in relation to positive psychological functioning ogy: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed.,
and personality traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 908– pp. 933–1016). New York: Wiley.
916. Sbarra, D. A., Smith, H. L., & Mehl, M. R. (2012). When leaving your ex,
Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hseih, Y. (2008). Self-compassion love yourself: Observational ratings of self-compassion predict the
and self-construal in the United States, Thailand, and Taiwan. course of emotional recovery following marital separation.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 267–285. Psychological Science, 23(3), 261–269.
Nevid, J. S., & Rathus, S. A. (2016). Psychology and the challenges of life Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the
(13th ed.). New York: Wiley. state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the Sirin, S. R., McCreary, D. R., & Mahalik, J. R. (2004). Differential reac-
gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality tions to men and women’s gender role transgressions: Perceptions of
and Social Psychology, 77, 1061–1072. social status, sexual orientation, and value dissimilarity. The Journal
O’Neil, J. (2008). Summarizing 25 years of research on men’s gender role of Men’s Studies, 12, 119–132.
conflict using the Gender Role Conflict Scale: New research para- Sirois, F. M., Molnar, D. S., & Hirsch, J. K. (2015). Self-compassion,
digms and clinical implications. The Counseling Psychologist, 36, stress, and coping in the context of chronic illness. Self and Identity,
358–445. 14, 334–347.
O'Neil, A., Sojo, V., Fileborn, B., Scovelle, A. J., & Milner, A. (2018). Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. E. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits,
The #MeToo movement: An opportunity in public health? The trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify?
Lancet, 391(10140), 2587–2589. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 44–62.
Mindfulness

Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: United Nations Free & Equal (2016). U.N. Free & equal fact sheet:
Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Intersex. Office of the High Commissioner for Human Rights
Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. (OHCHR). Retrieved from https://unfe.org/system/unfe-65-
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal Intersex_Factsheet_ENGLISH.pdf
Attributes Questionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and Van der Graaff, J., Branje, S., De Wied, M., Hawk, S., Van Lier, P., &
masculinity and femininity. JSAS: Catalog of Selected Documents Meeus, W. (2014). Perspective taking and empathic concern in ad-
in Psychology, 4, 43–44. olescence: Gender differences in developmental changes.
Sprecher, S., & Fehr, B. (2005). Compassionate love for close others and Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 881.
humanity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), Wei, M., Liao, K., Ku, T., & Shaffer, P. A. (2011). Attachment, self-
629–651. compassion, empathy, and subjective well-being among college stu-
Stevens, J. P. (1999). Intermediate statistics: A modern approach (2nd dents and community adults. Journal of Personality, 79, 191–221.
ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. van Well, S., Kolk, A. M., & Oci, N. Y. L. (2007). Direct and indirect
Tanaka, M., Wekerle, C., Schmuck, M. L., Paglia-Boak, A., & Research assessment of gender role identification. Sex Roles, 56, 617–628.
Team, M. A. P. (2011). The linkages among childhood maltreat- Whitley, B. E. (1983). Sex role orientation and self-esteem: A critical
ment, adolescent mental health, and self-compassion in child wel- meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social
fare adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35, 887–898. Psychology, 44, 765–778.
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2009). Gender identity. In M. R. Leary & R.
Tatum, K. J. (2013). Adherence to gender roles as a predictor of compas-
H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social
sion and self-compassion in women and men. Ph.D. thesis, Baylor
behavior (pp. 109–128). New York: Guilford Press.
University, Waco, TX.
Yarnell, L. M., Stafford, R., Neff, K. D., Reilly, E., Knox, M. C., &
Taylor, J. (2015). Gender orientation and the cost of caring for others. Mullarkey, M. (2015). Meta-analysis of gender differences in self-
Society and Mental Health, 5, 49–65. compassion. Self and Identity, 14, 499–520.
Thornton, B., & Leo, R. (1992). Gender typing, importance of multiple Zessin, U., Dickhauser, O., & Garbade, S. (2015). The relationship be-
roles, and mental health consequences for women. Sex Roles, 27(5), tween self-compassion and well-being: A meta-analysis. Applied
307–317. Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 7, 340–364.

S-ar putea să vă placă și