Sunteți pe pagina 1din 153

0
0-

In this major experimental study Peter Robinson casts new light on issues of
::s
VI
central concern to second language acquisition theory and pedagogy. His 0
::s
Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis relates such current issues in SLA theory as
the nature of implicit-explicit learning, the role of 'noticing,' and focus-on-form
versus meaning-only-processing, to recent work in mainstream cognitive
psychology, suggesting a reinterpretation of the familiar acquisition/learning
distinction.

"By applying a research design from experimental psychology to natural


language data, Robinson's important study sheds light on central theoretical
issues concerning the implicit, explicit, incidental, and intentional learning of
second and foreign languages. This book will be read with equal profit by those
in applied linguistics and general learning theory."
·Professor Richard Schmidt
University ofHawai'i at Mano-a/

"This book brings together views from cognitive psychology and SLA theory on
the implicit acquisition of grammar rules. The empirical part of this study is the
first attempt, to my knowledge, to put claims from two prominent figures in
these two disciplines (Reber and Krashen respectively) to the test in a single
design."
Professor Jan H Hulstijn
Free University, Amsterdam

Peter Robinson has held positions as lecturer in applied linguistics and second
language acquisition at the universities of Pittsburgh, Hawaii, and Queensland.
Currently he is Associate Professor of Linguistics and Second Language
Acquisitions at Aoyama Gakuin University, Shibuya, Tokyo. He completed his
M.A. in Language and Literature in Education at the University of London

THEORETICAL STUDIES
Institute of Education and his Ph.D. in Second Language Acquisition at the
University of Hawaii. He has published widely in the areas of SLA theory and

IN SECOND
SL pedagogy.

4 LANGUAGE ACQUISITION
LANG
Consciousness, Rules,
and Instructed Second Language
Acquisition
Theoretical Studies in Peter Robinson
Second Language Acquisition

Simon Belasco
General Editor
Consciousness, Rules,
Vol. 7
and Instructed Second Language
Acquisition

PETER LANG PETER LANG


New York• Washington, D.C./Baltimore New York• Washington, D.C./Baltimore
Rem• Frankfurt am Main• Berlin• Vienna• Paris Bern• Frankfurt am Main• Berlin• Vienna• Paris
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Robinson, Peter.
Consciousness, rules, and instructed second
language acquisition/ Peter Robinson. Table Of Contents
p. cm. - (Theoretical studies in second language acquisition; vol. 7)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
1. Second language acquisition. 2 . Language and
languages-Study and teaching. I. Title. II. Series. Acknowledgements. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xm
Pll8.2.R58 418--dc20 95-38411

Chapter 1: Consciousness, Implicit Learning


ISBN 0-8204-3040-4
ISSN 10 51-6670

And Second Language Acquisition . 1


1 .0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . 1
Die Deutsche Bibliothek-CIP-Einheitsaufnahme 1 . 1 Focus on form in L2 pedagogy 2
Robinson, Peter :
1 . 1 . 1 Amenability . . 3
Consciousness, rules, and instructed second language 1 . 1 .2 Generalizability 3
acquisition/ Peter Robinson.-New York; Washington, D.C./Baltimore; Bern; 1 . 1 .3 Accessibility . . 4
Frankfurt am Main; Berlin; Vienna; Paris: Lang. 1 . 1 .4 Entailability . . 4
(Theoretical studies in second language acquisition; Vol. 7) 1 . 1 .5 Dependent variables in effect of instruction studies 5
ISBN 0-8204-3040-4
1 .2 Consciousness in L2 learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
NE:GT
1 .2. 1 The acquisition/learning distinction . . . . . . . . 7
1 .2.2 Consciousness as awareness, intention and explicit
knowledge . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
1 .3 Renewed interest in consciousness in cognitive psychology 12
1 .4 The generalizability of research using artificial grammars . 14
1 .5 Experimental studies of the effects of structured stimuli on
L l learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
1 .6 Experimental studies of SLA under different conditions 15
1 .6. 1 Hulstijn ( 1 990). 16
1 .6.2 Doughty ( 1 988) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
1 .6.3 Ellis ( 1 993) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
The paper in this �ook meets the guidelines for permanence and durability 1 . 7 The need for further studies in a second language context. 19
of the Commtttee on Production Guidelines for Book Longevity 1 .8 The aims of the present study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20
of the Council of Library Resources.

Chapter 2: Rules, Complexity And Second


Language Knowledge . . . 23
© 1996 Peter Lang Publishing, Inc., New York
2.0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . 23
2 . 1 Pedagogic rules and linguistic rules 23
All rights reserved. 2. 1 . 1 The effectiveness of pedagogic rules 24
Reprint or reproduction, even partially, in all forms such as microfilm' 2. 1 .2 Noticing examples and understanding pedagogic rules 27
xerography, microfiche, microcard, and offset strictly prohibited.
2. 1 .3 Two dimensions of rule complexity . . . . . . . . 28
Printed in the United States of America.
2.2 Relating rule complexity, consciousness and learning . . 29
2.2. 1 Claims based on experimental evidence of implicit
learning studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
v1 Contents Contents vii

2.2.2 Pedagogic implications of the claims . . . . 30 3 .4.4 Attention, salience and grammatical sensitivity 78
2.2.3 Speculation about rule complexity in L2 pedagogy. . 31 3 .5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
2.3 Second language rule complexity . 33
2.3 .1 Perceptual salience . . . . . . . 33 Chapter 4: Implicit Learning, Memory And
2.3 .2 Lexical rules versus abstract rules 34 81
2.3.3 Size of context and degree of semantic opacity . 35 Knowledge . . . . . .
2.3.4 Processing load and rule complexity 35 4.0 Introduction . . . . . 81
2.3 .5 The consistency of expert judgments in problem 4. 1 Implicit and explicit learning 82
classification and problem solving 36 4. 1 . 1 Evidence of dissociations using artificial grammars . 84
2.4 Empirically motivating the easy/hard rule distinction . 37 4. 1 . 1 . l Implicit task conditions . 88
4. 1 . 1 .2 Rule-search conditions . . . 88
2.4. 1 Identifying unfamiliar structures . . . 38
42 4. 1 . 1 .3 Effects of providing explicit instruction. . 89
2.4.2 Rating scale surveys o f rule complexity . . .
2.4.3 The simple and complex rules identified for the study 44 4. 1 .2 Arguments against dissociations . . . . . . 89
4. 1 .2 . 1 Fragmented memory or abstract knowledge? 90
4 . 1 .2.2 Verbalizability of knowledge and awareness 91
Chapter 3: Implicit And Explicit Information 91
4 . 1 .2.3 Processing accounts of the evidence .
Processing . . . . . . . . . 51 4.2 Implicit and explicit memory . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
3. 0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . 51 4.2 . 1 Methodologies for studying dissociations . . . 94
3 . 1 Resources, informat ion processing and consciousness 51 4.2. 1 . 1 Recognition and recall . . . . 94
3 .2 Attention . . . . . . 53 4.2. 1 .2 Subliminal exposure and preference rating . 94
3 .2. 1 Filter theories of attention . . 54 4.2 . 1 .3 Priming. . . . . . . 95
3 .2.2 Capacity theories of attention . . 54 4.2. 1 .4 Word completion . . . . . . 95
3.2.3 Noticing and attentional theory . 57 4.2.2 Evidence for dissociations . . . 95
3.2.4 Alertness, orientation and detection . 58 4.2.3 Explanations, dissociations and task demands . 96
3.2.5 Attention, awareness and detection . 59 4.3 Implicit knowledge . . . . . . . . . . . 98
3 .2 .6 Attentional and non-attentional learning versus 4.3 . 1 The rule-based abstractionist position. . . 99
encoding specificity . . . . . . 61 4.3 . 1 . 1 Prototypes and abstraction . . 99
3 .2. 7 Summary of the role of attention 64 4.3 . 1 .2 Connectioni st models of categorizatio n 1 00
3 .3 Memory . . 64 4.3 . 1 .3 Natural language and categorizati on . 101
3 .3 . 1 Short-term and long-term memory 65 4.3 .2 The exemplar-based distributive position . . . 101
3 .3 .2 Automaticity and memory . . 66 4 .3 .2 . 1 Estes' s model of classification . . . . . 101
3.3.3 Access to long-term memory . 68 4.3 .2.2 Nosofsky's exemplar-based model of
3 .3 .3 . 1 Activation views . 69 classification. . . . . . . . . 1 02
3 .3 .3 .2 Processing views . 70 4.3 .2.3 Medin and Murphy's theory model of
3.3.3 .3 Multiple systems views . 71 classification . . . . 1 02
3.3 .4 Multiple systems and multiple explanations . 73 4.3 .3 Rules, instances and task demands . 1 02
3.3.5 Automaticity, retrieval and context instantiation . 74 4.3 .4 The Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis 1 03
3.3.6 Summary of the role of memory . . 75 4.3 .5 Summary . . . . 1 05
3 .4 Individual differences, resources and SLA . 75 4.4 The present study . . . . . . 1 05
3.4. 1 Individual differences and conscious learning . 75 4.4. 1 Summary of the research design. . 1 07
3 .4.2 Individual differences and unconscious acquisition 76 4.4.2 Research hypotheses . . . . . 1 07
3.4.3 Short-term memory capacity and learning . 77 4.4.3 Rationale for the research hypotheses . 1 08
viii Contents Contents ix

6.3 Variance data . . . . . . . . . 1 43


Chapter 5: Methodology For The Study. 1 13 144
6.3. 1 Analyses for Hypothesis 5
5 .0 Introduction . 1 13 1 44
6.3 . 1 . 1 Accuracy . . . .
5 . 1 Subjects : : : :
· 1 13 6.3 . 1 .2 Reaction time . . 145
� :: : : : : : : : :
. . . . . . . . . . . ·

5 .2 Data collection procedur s


. . .
1 14 6.3 . 1 .3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 5 . 1 45
5.2.1 Pret�sting and aptitude testing . . . . . 1 15 1 45
6.4 Aptitude data . . . . . . . . . . . .
5 .2.2 Assignment to experimental conditions . 1 16 6.4. 1 Analyses for Hypothesis 6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 49
5.2.3 The implicit learning condition . . 1 17 6.4. 1 . 1 Accuracy and aptitude . . . . . . . . . . . 1 49
5 .2.4 The incidental condition 1 17 6.4. 1 .2 Reaction time and aptitude . . . . . . . . 151
5.2.5 The explicit rule-search con i i n dt� : 1 18 6.4. 1 .3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 6. 151
5.2.6 The explicit instructed condition . 118 6.5 Summary of research hypotheses and results . 151
5.2.7 The transfer phase . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 19
5 .2.8 The post-experimental debriefing session . 1 19
5 .3 Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 20
Chapter 7: Further Analyses . . . 1 53
7 .0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 53
5 .3 . 1 Computer materials . . . . . 1 20
7 . 1 The effect of grammaticality and sentence type 1 54
5.3.2 Pretest and aptitude measures . 1 20
7. 1 . 1 Simple rule grammatical sentence types . 1 56
5.3.3 Rules . . . . . . . . . 121
7 . 1 . 1 . 1 Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . 1 56
5 .3 .4 The training set sentences 121
7 . 1 . 1 .2 Reaction time . . . . . . . . 158
5 .3 .5 The transfer set sentences 122
7 . 1 .2 Simple rule ungrammatical sentence types 158
5 .3 .6 Debriefing materials . . . 1 23
7 . 1 .2. 1 Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 59
5 .4 Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . 1 23
7 . 1 .2.2 Reaction time . . . . . . . . . . 1 59
7. 1 .3 Complex rule grammatical sentence types. 1 60
Chapter 6: Analyses And Results . 125
7 . 1 .3 . 1 Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 60
6.0 Introduction . . - 1 25 7 . 1 .3 .2 Reaction time . . . . . . . . . . 161
6.1 Accuracy and reac i n ti t � �� d t :
aa 125 7 . 1 .4 Complex rule ungrammatical sentence types 1 62
6. 1 . 1 Analyses for Hypothesis 1 1 27 7 . 1 .4. 1 Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 62
6. 1 . 1 . 1 Accuracy . . . . 1 27 7 . 1 .4.2 Reaction time . . . . . . . . . . . 1 63
6. 1 . 1 .2 Reaction time · . 1 29 7 . 1 .5 Summary of the effect of grammaticality and
h � d � � f ; H �� h � :
6. 1 . 1 .3 Summary of t e e i e c o y t e is 1 131 sentence type . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 64
6. 1 .2 Analyses for Hypothesis 2 . . . . . . 133 1 68
6. 1 .2.1 Complex rule accuracy . . . 7 .2 Simple and ,complex rule pretest structures . . . . . .
13J 1 70
6. 1 .2.2 Simple rule accuracy . . . . 7 .3 Awareness and aptitude . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
134
7.3 . 1 Measures of awareness and the effects of learning
6. 1 .2.3 Complex rule reaction time . 137 condition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 70
6.1 .2.4 Simple rule reaction time . . . . . . . . . 138 7 .3 . 1 . 1 Noticing across conditions . . . . 1 70
6. 1 .2.5 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 2. 139 7.3 . 1 .2 Looking for rules across conditions 171
6. 1 .3 Analyses for Hypothesis 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 7.3 . 1 .3 Verbalizability across conditions . 1 72

6 . 1 .3 . 1 S mple and complex rule accuracy . . . . . 140 7.3 .2 Measures of awareness and aptitude . . . . 1 73
6. 1 .3.2 Simple and complex rule reaction time . . . 1 40 1 73
7.3 .2. 1 Awareness and memory . . . . . .
6. 1 .3 .3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 3 . 1 41 1 74
7.3 .2.2 Awareness and grammatical sensitivity
6.2 Rule awareness data. . . 1 41 1 74
7 .3 .3 Summary of the findings for awareness .
6.2. 1 Analyses for Hyp t esi 4 . �h � . : : :: : : : : : : : 143
7.4 Summary of further analyses and results . . . . . . . .
1 77
6.2. 1 . 1 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 4. 143
Contents XI
x Contents

233
condition fam iliarization phase of training.
· · ·

Chapter 8: The Fundamental Similarity Of s . 239


Appendix G: The training set sentence
. . · · · ·
. . .

Implicit And Explicit L2 Learning . 1 79 s and follow up questions


Appendix H: Training set sentence
8.0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 79 for implicit, incidental, rule-search and 24 1
8 . 1 Awareness and task demands . . . . . . . . . . 1 79 instructed conditions . · · · · · · · · · ·

ion
8. 1 . 1 Varieties of awareness under different learning
Appendix I: Computer instructions for the familiarizat 249
conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 80
phase of the transfer task ·

8 . 1 .2 The sensitivity of measures of awareness . . . 1 82 25 1


8.2 Learning at different levels of the dependent variable. 1 84 Appendix J: The transfer set sentences
253
8.2.1 The level of rules . . . . . 1 85 AppendixK: Subject information
8.2.2 The level of grammaticality . . . . . . . . . 1 86 257
8.2.3 The level of type . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 87 Bibliography .
8.3 Explanations of the effects of grammaticality and type on 289
Index .....
learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 88
8.3 . l Learning in the instructed condition. . . . . . . 1 88
8.3 . 1 . 1 Confirmation bias and grammaticality . 191
8.3 .2 Learning in the implicit condition . . . 1 92
8.3.3 Leaming in the incidental condition. . 195
8.3 .4 Learning in the rule-search condition . 1 97
8 .4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 98

Chapter 9: Summary And Conclusions 1 99


9.0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 99
9.1 Implicit and explicit second language learning . . . . . 1 99
9. 1 . 1 Second language learning and rule complexity . 200
9 . 1 .2 Generalizing from Reber' s claims about implicit
learning to Krashen's claims about acquisition . 203
9.2 Limitations ofthe study and directions for future research 205
9 .3 Summary of findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 208

Appendix A: Grammaticality judgment test as input to the


rating scale survey of rule complexity. . 211
Appendix B: Rating scale survey of rule complexity . . 215
Appendix C: Computer instructions for the implicit
condition familiarization phase of training. 223
Appendix D: Computer instructions for the incidental
condition familiarization phase of training. 227
Appendix E: Computer instructions for the rule-search
condition familiarization phase of training. 23 1
Appendix F: Computer instructions for the instructed
XIV Acknowledgements

the main stud� . included resid ent .students at Tokai University at


.
Honolulu and VlSltors from Tokai Umversity at Tokyo, as well as others
from Kansai Gaidai Junior College and the Japan America Institute of
Management S cience, both in Hawa i ' i Kai, and students in the Chapter 1
University of Haw�i'i at Manoa's NICE program. I am also grateful to
the SEAMEO. Regional Language Centre in Singapore for awarding me
the �ell_?wsh�p that allo�ed me time to prepare this m anuscript for Consciousness, Implicit Learning And
pubhcat10n. Fmally a special thank you goes to Misook Kim for her help
and support throughout the busy time when this was written. Second Language Acquisition
I gratefull� acknowl �dge the permission of the following to
reproduce copyright m aterial: Language Learning for permission to
reproduce parts of Chapter 3 which originally appeared in 'Attention
memory and the 'noticing' hypothesis', Volume 45, no. 2, pp. 283-3 3 1
_ .
; 1.0 Introduction
Recent debate in second language acquisition (SLA) theory has
Studles m Second Language Acquisition for permission to reproduce
parts of Chapters 2 and 8 which originally appeared in 'Leaming simple focused on the role of consciousness in second language (L2)
and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule­ development. Schmidt ( 1 990, 1 993a, 1 993b, 1 994a, 1 995 ; Schmidt
& Frota 1 986) has claimed that conscious awareness of the form of
search and instructed conditions', Volume 1 8, no. 1 , pp. 27-67; Prentice
Hall Inc., Englewood Cliffs, NJ., for permission to reproduce the figure input at the level of 'noticing' is a necessary condition for language
'A capacity model of attention' from p. 1 0 of D. Kahneman, ( 1 973) development to occur. A number of other S�� r�searc�ers have
Attention and effort; Academic Press Inc., New York, NY., for
also argued for a role for 'consciousness-raismg (Elhs l 9 9 3 a,
1 993b; Fotos & Ellis 1 99 1 ; Rutherford 1 987; Sharwood S mi�h
permission to reproduce the figure 'Multiple resources in attention' from
1 98 1 ; Yip 1 994), 'input-enhancement' (Doughty � 99 1 ; Elhs
p. 69 of C. Wickens, ( 1 984) 'Processing resources in attention' in R.
1 994b, 1 995; Sharwood Smith 1 99 1 , 1 993; White, Spada,
Parasuraman and D. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention; and The
Lightbown & Ranta 1 99 1 ), 'focus on form' (Long 1 99 1 , 1 994;
American Psychological Association for permission to reproduce the .
Long & Crookes 1 992), 'priming' (Mellow & Cummmg 1 994) �d
figure 'A fmite-state grammar' from A. Reber 'Implicit learning and tacit 'analytic teaching' (Lyster 1 994) activities in L2 classrooms :which
knowledge' (1989) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, selectively direct learner attention to form as an �id to
2 1 9-235 . internalization of the L2 system, and some have based rat10nales
for form-focused instruction on models of the interface between
conscious and unconscious forms of L2 knowledge (Bialystok 1 97 8 ,
1 98 1 , 1 990; Ellis 1 993a, 1 994b, 1 995; Hulstijn 1 990b; cf. Mahle &
Raupach 1 989; Odlin 1 986; Robinson 1 99 3 a). . .
In part, the current interest in the role of consc10usness m L2
.
learning has been prompted by critical reaction to the Momtor
. _
Model of Krashen which proposes that two distmct proces�es
operate in language development, a conscious process of deduction
based on rule application which results in a learned syst�m, and an
unconscious process of induction which results in an acqmr�d system
(Krashen 1 978, 1 98 1 a, 1 982, 1985, 1 994; Krashen & Sehg�r 1 97 5 ;
Seliger 1 975). The l atter system is claimed to be responsi�le for
most language production since the conditions under which the
learned system can operate successfully seldom arise. In addition,
the development of the learned system is restricted to a relatively
2 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 3

small number of simple rule-governed domains. By implication, the generalizable to other related forms in the interest of increasing rate
development of the acquired system is a priority which of acquisition; (iii) accessibility, the need to focus on those forms
methodology based on Krashen's theory, e.g., the Natural Approach which lead to restructuring or resetting of parameters by making
(Krashen & Terrell 1983), seeks to foster by avoiding form-focused negative evidence accessible to the learner; and (iv) entailability,
instruction and the provision of target language rules. the need to focus on one of those forms which cluster together as a
In contrast, those who argue for a constitutive role for consequence of a particular parameter setting, and which entail each
consciousness specify that methodology should contrive the other, in the expectation of triggering the acquisition of the whole
opportunities for learners to focus on form and consciously notice cluster of properties.
features of the target language input and some recent studies have
examined the effects of design features of language teaching 1.1.1 Amenability
materials that attempt to manipulate the extent of noticing There have been claims that the number of rules that are explicitly
(Alanen 1 995; Doughty 1 988, 1 99 1 ; Ellis 1 995 ; Fotos 1 993 ; F otos teachable is small and limited to those describing structures that do
& Ellis 1 99 1 ; Hulstijn 1 990a; Jourdenais, Ota, Stauffer Boyson & not require complex processing operations. Some have argued that
Doughty 1 995; Leeman, Arteagoitia, Fridman & Doughty 1 99 5 ; the structures most amenable to explicit instruction are those which
Loschky & Bley-Vroman 1 993; Lyster & Ranta 1 995; Rankin have transparent form-function relationships, in which, for
1990; Schmidt 1 995; Shook 1 994; VanPatten & Cadierno 1 993 ; example, morphological additions or word order permutations have
Watanabe 1 992; Zalewski 1 993). The issues of what forms to focus a clearly identifiable semantic function (see Ellis 1 990:1 67 ; Krashen
on and of how to achieve this focusing of attention are currently 1 982:97; Pica 1 983, 1 985, 1 994:66). Some have also argued that
the subject of much debate. The present study addresses both o f amenability, and processing difficulty, are developmentally
these issues by examining claims arising within cognitive scheduled. For example, where complex permutations in word order
psychology and SLA research that: (i) complex stimulus domains are concerned, it has been claimed that explicit form-focused
are most effectively learned nonconsciously; and that (ii) the instruction cannot break the stage-wise order of acquisition
beneficial effects of instruction in rules, and of conscious rule­ consequent on the learner shedding grammatical processing
search, are limited to cases where the rules describe relatively simple strategies. However, if timed to focus on structures characterizing
patterns of structural covariance. This chapter first summarizes the learner's current, or next stage of development then form­
suggestions that have been made within SLA research about which focused instruction can be effective (Ellis 1 989, 1 990, 1 994b,
forms to focus on in L2 pedagogy. Following this, the debate over 1 994c; Larsen-Freeman & Long 1 99 1 ; Meisel, Clahsen &
the role of consciousness in L2 learning is described and related t o Pienemann 1 98 1 ; Pienemann 1 989, 1 992; Pienemann, Johnston &
the investigation o f similar issues i n mainstream experimental Brindley 1 988).
psychology, particularly those studies that have used artificial
grammars as a stimulus domain to be learned under implicit and 1.1.2 Generalizability
explicit conditions. Finally, those studies in the field of SLA that There have also been claims that markedness relations, based o n
have examined the implicit learning of natural L2s are reviewed. implicational universals (see Doughty 199 1 ; Eckman 1 977, 1 98 5 ,
1 99 1 ; Gass 1 979; Hawkins 1 988; Pavesi 1 986; Robinson 1 994b;
1.1 Focus on form in L2 pedagogy Rutherford 1 986, 1 987; Schachter 1 990; Towell & Hawkins 1 994;
Four concerns could be said to underlie suggestions that have been White 1 989b; Zobl 1 983, 1 985), hold potential for decisions about
made about which forms to focus conscious awareness on in sequencing the items to be focused on (Ellis 1 993a, 1 994c; Larsen­
classroom language learning: (i) amenability, the need to focus on Freeman & Long 1 99 1 :309-3 1 5; Long 1 988, 1 99 1 , 1 994; Long &
those forms which are best suited to, or most amenable to explicit Crookes 1 992) since instruction focused on some marked forms
presentation, leaving those less amenable to such presentation t o (e.g., relativization on objects of comparison) has been shown t o
incidental o r i mplicit learning processes; (ii) generalizability, the generalize to acquisition of less marked forms (e.g., relativization
need to focus on those forms where instruction is maximally on subjects) but not vice versa, i.e., instruction on less marked
4 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 5

forms does not generalize to more marked forms (Eckman, Bell & in this way will depend on the degree of configurational similarity
Nelson 1988). between the L l and L2 concerned. Entailability and generalizability
are similar to the extent that they are both ways of maximizing the
1.1.3 Accessibility reorganizational consequences of a focus on form. They differ in
Within the framework of Universal Grammar (Chomsky 1981, that entailability makes reference to a notion of inheritance of
1986; cf. Bley-Vroman, Felix & Ioup 1988; Cook 1993; Eubank properties specific to a particular linguistic theory (Chomsky 1981,
1991; Felix 1991; Flynn 1987a, 1987b; Towell & Hawkins 1994; 1986), while generalizability is based on implicational relations
White 1989a), it has been suggested that candidates for form­ derived from descriptions of language typology (Greenberg 1966).
focused instruction should include aspects of the target language
which are inaccessible to the learner from the evidence of positive 1.1.5 Dependent variables in effect ofinstruction studies
examples of grammatical L2 structures alone. Rather, negative A growing body of SLA research addresses the effect of a focus on
evidence of ungrammaticality needs to be made available to learners form on language acquisition in instructed settings, using structures
in order to disconfirm inappropriate hypotheses about L2 motivated by the accessibility and entailability arguments reviewed
parameter settings and subset, superset relations in the native and above as dependent variables (e.g., Felix & Weigl 1991; Schwartz &
target language that may arise when based only on positive Gubala-Ryzak 1992; Trahey & White 1993; White 1991, 1992;
evidence. That is, when the L 1 is a superset of the L2 with regard to White et al. 1991). Some research has also studied the effect of
the structures allowed by a particular parameter setting, positive instruction using typological markedness and the notion of
evidence of the L2 alone will not disconfirm a projected similarity generalizability to motivate the dependent variable (e.g., Doughty
between the L l and L2. In these cases access to negative evidence 1991; Eckman et al. 1988; Gass 1982; Pavesi 1986; Zobl 1985).
of what is not possible in the L2 is necessary to limit the scope of The procedure followed in these studies has usually been to contrast
the projection from the Ll (Trahey & White 1993; White 1992). the performance of one or more instructed groups with a control
Opponents of this view argue negative data cannot initiate group to identify differences in the effectiveness of the
reorganization of the L2 grammar since attending to it requires a instructional treatments (see Ellis 1994b: eh. 14; Larsen-Freeman &
conscious learning process different in kind from the unconscious Long 1991: ch.8; and Long 1983, 1988, and for summaries). While
process responsible for the acquisition of competence (Krashen such studies have demonstrated important effects supporting a role
1985; Schwartz 1986, 1992, 1993). They argue that, as in L l for form-focused instruction they have, however, largely been
development (Atkinson 1992; Goodluck 1991; O'Grady 1993), all conducted under classroom learning conditions. Consequently, given
the L2 learner needs access to is positive evidence of L2 structure. the lack of experimental control in such studies, it is unclear what
learning processes underlie the aggregate advantage of instructed
1.1.4 Entailability groups. Krashen (1985) has argued that this advantage is due not t o
A related motivation for a focus on form ansmg within the the content of the lesson, and the formal instructional treatment,
framework of Universal Grammar is that of entailability. While but to the fact that L2 classrooms provide acquisition-rich
accessibility, as I have described it above, has to do with the nature environments and a supply of comprehensible input which activates
of the evidence of structure that the learner must have access to in the unconscious acquisition process, and it is this that is responsible
order to fix appropriate parameters in the L2, entailability refers t o for the apparent advantages of instruction. Long (1988), arguing
the differential consequences of focusing on individual forms. It has against this position, cites studies that show positive effects for
been suggested that a focus on selected items can not only trigger instruction even in contexts where learners are exposed t o
the realization that a particular parameter applies in that language acquisition-rich environments outside the classroom. Recent studies
but also lead to the simultaneous acquisition of a cascade of related appear to support the value of a focus on form in facilitating rate of
features that are entailed by the parameter setting (Hilles 1986; learning, and the acquisition of communicatively redundant features
Lightfoot 1989, 1991; Schwartz 1993; Towell & Hawkins 1994; of the L2 which are not learned despite considerable exposure t o
White 1989a, 1991, 1992). How much new knowledge is triggered comprehensible input in acquisition-rich immersion environments
6 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 7

(Day & Shapson 1991; Harley 1989, 1993; Harley & Swain 1984; effects for learning the structure of complex stimulus domains
Lyster 1994; Spada & Lightbown 1993), but clearer support can without conscious awareness. However, these studies have
only come from controlled experimental studies showing superior predominantly made use of artificial grammars as the stimulus
performance by explicitly form-focused instructed groups relative domain (e.g., Reber 1967, 1969, 1976, 1989, 1993; Reber & Allen
to groups that process the dependent variables of interest implicitly 1978; Vokey & Brooks 1992, and see Chapter 4). The
or indirectly. generalizability of their findings to the domain of natural L2
The debate about the role of consciousness has proceeded, learning is something which must be demonstrated by SLA research.
however, without much empirical evidence from controlled Engaging in such research is likely to provide a clearer evidential
experimental studies of the differential effects of L2 learning under base for the speculations of SLA theorists regarding the extent t o
implicit, explicit and incidental conditions with regard to any of the which unconscious learning of L2s is, or is not, possible.
above described motivations for a focus on form. Where
experimentally controlled studies of the effect of explicit 1.2 Consciousness in L2 learning
instruction exist (e.g., Robinson & Ha 1993), they often lack Recent theoretical arguments supporting a role for consciousness
equivalent incidentally or implicitly exposed groups, making it and focus on form in L2 learning (Bialystok & Sharwood Smith
impossible to draw conclusions from these studies about the 1985·' Cohen Larsen-Freeman & Tarone 1991; Ellis 1990, .
1993a,
' & Pienem ann
differential effectiveness of explicit conscious attention to form 1994b, 1995; Harley & Swain 1984; Lightbown
versus incidental or implicit processing of the forms concerned. An 1993; Long 1991, 1994; Long & Crookes 1992; Rutherfo rd 1987;
exception is Doughty (1988, 1991), who studied the acquisition of Sharwood Smith 1981, 1992, 1993; Schmidt 1990, 1993a, 1993b,
relative clauses in the implicational order (the 'Noun Phrase 1994a, 1995), have been made, in part, in reaction to claims that
Accessibility Hierarchy') identified by Keenan and Comrie ( 1977) L2 development is a largely unconscious process. Kra�hen � 19 � 1a,
as a function of different conditions of exposure. Her study thus 1982, 1985, 1993, 1994), in particular, is associated with this view,
dealt with the generalizability issue of form-focused instruction (see maintaining that two processes operate in language learners. The
section 1.6.2). first process, which is consciousness dominated, he te�s 'le��� ng ­ :
Similar studies, motivated by the accessibility and entailability The second process, which is unconscious, he terms acqms1t10n .
issues and examining the extent of learning under different These two processes, he claims, result in the development of
conditions of exposure, are lacking in the field of SLA. There are distinct, non-interfaced language systems.
no experimental studies, to date examining the differential
effectiveness of exposure to positive and indirect negative evidence 1.2.1 The acquisition/learning distinction
of L2 parameter settings under nonconscious, implicit conditions, Krashen's distinction though defended by some (e.g., Schwartz
versus conscious instructed conditions. There is a similar lack of 1986, 1993; Zobl 1992, 1995) and assumed by others (e.g., Paradis
experimental studies using performance on supposedly amenable 1994), has been extensively criticized in the SLA literature (Gre�g
versus less amenable rules as the dependent variable under different 1984 1988· Larsen-Freeman & Long 1991: 245-249; McLaughlm
conditions of exposure. No doubt this is partly because distinctions :
1978 1987, 1990a, 1990b; Munsell & Carr 1981). In parti�ular,
between structures of different levels of complexity, which are McLaughlin (1978), in an early response to Krashen, has pomted
supposedly differentially amenable to explicit instruction, have been out that it is impossible to know with certainty whether L2 learners
difficult to operationalize in an uncontroversial way. Thus, claims are operating via consciously accessed 'rule' or via subcons�iou�ly
by Krashen (1982, 1985) and Ellis (1990) that simple structures determined intuitions or 'feel,' in making grammaticality
alone are learnable through explicit instruction (see Chapter 2) have judgments. Yet Krashen has claimed that this distinction reveals
gone unexamined. which system is being drawn on-conscious knowledge of . the. �le
Studies in mainstream psychology, in contrast, have addressed guiding judgments based on the learned system, but feel or mtmt10n,
the issue of the differential effectiveness of learning under explicit without conscious access to rules, guiding judgments based on the
and implicit conditions, and have been claimed to reveal significant acquired system (McLaughlin 1987:21). Further, McLaughlin
8 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 9

(1978), and Gregg (1984) both recount evidence, based on their own The pedagogic consequences of Krashen's theory have appeared
experiences, of something which was initially learned, and to be a reluctance to engage in formal grammar teaching or a focus
consciously and effortfully produced, eventually being produced on form in isolation from meaning-focused activities. The aim of
without conscious effort, thus contradicting Krashen's claim that the Natural Approach (Krashen & Terrell 1983) is to create the
what is learned cannot become part of the acquired system. conditions for facilitating unconscious 'acquisition' and minimize
Krashen's counterargument to claims of this type, that the acquired the time spent on conscious learning of formal properties of the
system had eventually caught up with the learned system· and was in language. Pedagogic priority is given to ensuring the provision of
effect responsible for the effortless production, is, as McLaughlin comprehensible input to learners, often in the form of reading
(1987) and Gregg (1984) both point out, unfalsifiable. (Elley 1991; Krashen 1993), and engaging them in activities which
Another problem with the acquisition/learning distinction, as lead them to process the input. It is argued that vocabulary is
formulated by Krashen, is the fact that it is impossible to reproduce acquired incidentally through engaging with comprehensible input
these two conditions experimentally in order to study the extent of during such activities (Dupuy & Krashen 1993; Elley 1991; Krashen
learning that takes place under them since the task conditions which 1989, 1993, 1994; Pitts, White & Krashen 1989). This process of
Krashen asserts must be met for 'acquisition,' or 'learning' to occur incidental acquisition is argued to be preferable to, and distinct from,
are impossible to manipulate experimentally. The main reason is that induced by conscious efforts to learn, following direct
that Krashen claims the 'affective filter' must be lowered in the vocabulary instruction (Dupuy & Krashen 1993). It is further argued
acquisition condition: that the incidental acquisition of vocabulary via meaning-focused
activities takes place as a result of focusing on key vocabulary items
The affective filter is a mental block that prevents acquirers from fully that are known, and inferring the meanings of unknown words on
utilizing the comprehensible input they receive for language acquisition. the basis of this knowledge together with knowledge of the global
When it is 'up' the acquirer may understand what he hears and reads, but
context (Krashen, Terrell, Ehrman & Herzog 1984). This
the input will not reach the LAD. This occurs when the acquirer i s
unmotivated, lacking i n self confidence o r anxious (Krashen 1985:3).
inferential process will lead not only to vocabulary development,
but will also trigger the acquisition of the formal properties of
While it is possible to manipulate variables like intention or syntax and grammar in the absence of conscious awareness of such
attention through task instructions, it is difficult both ethically and properties. The influence of Krashen's theory on the thinking of
within an experimental setting to manipulate anxiety and other Prabhu (1987) and the conceptualization which underlay the
components of the 'affect' construct, even if the subtle differences development of the Bangalore 'Procedural Syllabus,' particularly
Krashen assumes are causal, i.e., between 'low' and 'moderate' the acquisition/learning distinction and the characterization of
anxiety (1985:25), could be quantified. It is true that there are acquisition as a "subconscious" process, is obvious:
constructs for measuring anxiety, for example the Taylor Manifest
Task-based teaching operates with the concept that, while the conscious
Anxiety Scale (see Krashen 1981a: 30), but although Krashen mind is working out some o f the meaning-content, a subconscious part of
reports that moderate anxiety, as measured by this scale, was the mind perceives, or acquires (or recreates as a cognitive structure) some
positively correlated with achievement in Spanish as a second of the linguistic structuring embodied in those entities, as a step in the
language (in a study by Chastain, 1975), no particular point on the development of an internal system o f rules (Prabhu 1987 :69�70).

scale is identified by him as distinguishing low from moderate


anxiety. Also, as Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991:243) point out, 1.2.2 Consciousness as awareness, intention and explicit knowledge
it isn't clear to what extent other factors supposedly regulating the Schmidt (1990, 1993a, 1993b, 1994a, 1995), in contrast, questions
lowering and raising of the affective filter, i.e., high motivation and the extent to which unconscious processes can result in language
high esteem, can offset the problem of excessive anxiety, and vice learning and distinguishes between three different senses of the term
versa. 'conscious' as it is used in cognitive psychology and SLA theory.
The first of these identifies consciousness with the contents of
10 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 11

awareness, which can be at the levels of perception, noticing and (iii) Implicit knowledge as the result of implicit learning. In yet
understanding. Consciousness in the sense of awareness at the level another sense implicit knowledge refers to the knowledge base
of perception, in which attention is allocated to signal detection established by learning under implicit conditions, as in Reber (1989).
(see Tomlin & Villa 1994 and Chapter 3) is necessary for all In this sense implicit knowledge of a domain is the consequence of
learning. Perception is also necessary for noticing to occur, and processing information without conscious awareness of the rule­
noticing, which involves rehearsal in short-term memory (see governedness, or structure of the domain, and without an intention
section 3.2.5) Schmidt argues, is a necessary condition for language to learn the rules regulating the structure. This sense of implicit
learning. Consciousness, in the sense of awareness at the level of knowledge is different from the first sense above in that the
rule understanding, he argues, based in part on his own experience of knowledge that results from implicit learning need not invoke an
learning Portuguese (Schmidt & Frota 1986), usually accompanies, innately prespecified component to explain its nature or form,
and is strongly facilitative of, attempts to learn. The second sense "...there is, at this juncture, no reason to place any priority on
of consciousness that Schmidt identifies equates consciousness with particular biological determinants of a specific kind. All forms of
intention. In this sense consciousness is not necessary to language implicit knowledge are taken as essentially similar at their deepest
learning since incidental learning is possible. Finally Schmidt notes levels" (Reber 1989:219). This sense of implicit knowledge is also
that consciousness is also often equated with the contents of claimed to be different from the second sense in that the knowledge
explicit knowledge. However, this term is itself ambiguous (see which results from implicit learning generalizes to contexts beyond
Mohle & Raupach 1989; Odlin 1986; Robinson 1993a, 1994a, those represented by the specific stimuli experienced during
1995d) and its senses are perhaps best illustrated by examining the learning. In contrast, automatized knowledge develops as a function
meanings of the counterpart term, implicit knowledge. There are of practice in constant environments (Logan 1988a, 1988b;
three senses in which the term 'implicit' is used to describe Shiffrin & Schneider 1977; Strayer & Kramer 1990). Similarly,
knowledge, and so distinguish it from an explicit counterpart. Anderson's distinction between procedural and declarative
knowledge concerns skill acquisition, about which "implicit learning
(i) Implicit knowledge as innate. Implicit knowledge can refer t o says little or nothing" (Reber 1993:15). Nonetheless, despite
the innate constraints on a possible language which, some have making a distinction between implicit knowledge as the result of
proposed, learners are alert to in learning their first (e.g., Chomsky implicit learning, and implicit knowledge as automatized, Reber
1986; Lightfoot 1989, 1991; Pinker 1984, 1989, 1994), and concedes that the two display similar properties: "Automatic
possibly second languages (White 1989a, 1990), though the form of processes are classic examples of the actions of implicit systems, "
such constraints is claimed to be unavailable to consciousness and occurring outside of consciousness and conscious control, and
cannot, therefore, be articulated by language learners (Chomsky requiring minimal attentional effort (see Reber 1993:15).
1990). Schmidt's position regarding the constitutive role played by
conscious awareness, or noticing, of target language features during
(ii) Implicit knowledge as automatized. Implicit knowledge can L2 learning is supported by the introspective evidence of his own
also be used as a term to refer to automatized knowledge, meaning experiences (Schmidt & Frota 1986) and by claims of cognitive
knowledge which is accessed with relatively little attentional effort psychologists in the area of general learning theory (Brewer 1974;
either as a result of practice (McLaughlin 1987; Shiffrin & Ericsson & Simon 1980) that learning without awareness is not
Schneider 1977; Strayer & Kramer 1990), or as a result of the possible. Several other L2 researchers have argued that conscious
strength of individual representations in memory (Logan l 988a; learning is an important aspect of L2 development, albeit to a
Logan & Stadler 1991; Robinson & Ha 1993; Rothkopf & Dashen considerably lesser extent than Schmidt. In Bialystok's case (1990)
1995). In this sense procedural, in contrast to declarative knowledge implicit, unanalyzed knowledge is eventually transformed into
(Anderson 1983), could also be said to be implicit in that it is analyzed knowledge via a process that takes place below the level of
accessed with little attentional effort and lies beyond conscious awareness. This analyzed knowledge is subsequently available to be
control. drawn on in developing explicit conscious metalinguistic
12 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 13

representations. Such metalinguistic awareness is only useful, (see e.g., Broadbent 1954; Shannon 1948), and by progress in the
however, for the kind of language tasks that are common in formal field of neurocomputing (see e.g., Hebb 1949; Rosenblatt 1958), the
education, like grammar tests and academic writing. Unlike topic of consciousness and its relationship to models of information
Bialystok, who does not concede the possibility that conscious processing became respectable again (see e.g., Baars 1988;
metalinguistic knowledge can influence the development of Bickerton 1991; Bowers & Meichenbaum 1984; Crick 1995;
analyzed representations, Ellis (1993a, 1994b, 1994c, 1995) adopts Dennett 1991; Jackendoff 1987; Pekala 1991; Searle 1990, 1992;
a "weak interface position" in which explicit knowledge, at Velmans 1991). In Kihlstrom's words, "consciousness, in the form
developmentally sensitive points, is claimed to be capable of feeding of attention, perception, memory, imagery and thought, is once
into the implicit system causing changes in its representation. The again at the center of things-with the difference that mental
information processing model of SLA proposed by McLaughlin, contents and processes are approached with the same commitment
Rossman and McLeod (1983), based on the model of automaticity to publicly verifiable, quantifiable observation that characterized the
developed by Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), views language behaviorist paradigm of Watson and Skinner" (1984:150).
development as a shift from controlled to automatic processing. Along with the interest in specifying the role of consciousness
McLaughlin (1990a, 1990b) prefers to discuss this transition in during information processing and learning there was, necessarily,
terms of the concepts of attention and control rather than in terms renewed interest in evidence regarding the extent to which
of consciousness, since he sees many definitional problems with the information could be processed and learned in its absence. In a
use of the term. As Hulstijn ( l 990b) points out, in McLaughlin et historical overview of the "rediscovery" of the unconscious in
al.'s information processing framework the relationship of explicit psychology Reber observes that:
to implicit knowledge during development is not viewed as
consecutive, in either direction, since both knowledge sources can be ... during the 1970's it was becoming increasingly apparent that people d o
not always solve problems, make decisions, or reach conclusions using
drawn on in the development of automaticity, depending on "the the kinds of standard, conscious and rational processes that they were
subject matter concerned, ...environmental conditions, and ... more or less assumed to be using. Moreover, importantly, they often do not
individual capacities" (Hulstijn 1990b:34). seem to know what they knew nor what information it was that they had
based their problem-solving or decision-making on (Reber 1992:98).
1.3 Renewed interest in consciousness in cognitive psychology
The claims of such SLA theorists need to be placed in the context Research by Kahneman and Tversky (see Kahneman, Slovic &
of the growing literature in mainstream cognitive psychology that Tversky 1982), Nisbett and Wilson (1977) and Langer (1978),
examines the role of consciousness in learning and memorial amongst others, on the relationship between complex decision
processes (see Schmidt 1990, 1993a for a review of this literature making and awareness is representative of the renewed interest in
with special reference to SLA issues). Throughout the fifties and the influence of the unconscious. The strong claim of Reber, and
early sixties a number of social scientists had argued for the other psychologists who argue for the primacy of implicit,
importance of 'tacit' knowledge (e.g., Polanyi 1958; von Hayek nonconscious information processing in regulating our response t o
1962), but the issue of the nature, and possible limits, of the our physical environment, can be stated as follows: the acquisition
influence of conscious awareness on learning was a neglected topic of knowledge of complex regularities, either of our first language
for psychological research and theoretical speculation. This was no (Ll ), or of the world around us, proceeds by a process of induction
doubt a consequence of the influence of behaviorism on which is largely independent of conscious explicit efforts to learn.
psychological research which "removed consciousness (not t o The phenomenon of acquiring knowledge in the absence of
mention the unconscious) from the vocabulary of the science" conscious explicit attempts to learn has been termed implicit
(Kihlstrom 1984:159). However, as a consequence of the demise of learning, and it has been studied in a variety of domains e.g.,
behaviorism and the 'cognitive revolution' in psychology, which knowledge of facial characteristics and typical accompanying
was motivated in part by criticisms of Skinnerian learning theory by personality traits (Lewicki 1986), knowledge of the working of
Chomsky (1959), by the growth of information processing theory miniature economic systems (Broadbent, Fitzgerald & Broadbent
14 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 15

1986), and knowledge of the rules underlying the distribution of 1.5 Studies ofthe effects ofstructured stimuli on Ll learning
sequences of lights at different points on a computer screen Work in a research tradition , related to that established by Reber
(Howard, Mutter & Howard 1992). Most work has been done, (1967) has also made use of artificial grammars. In contrast, this
however, regarding the acquisition of knowledge of rules of artificial work has been motivated by information about the course and
grammars (e.g., Dulany, Carlson & Dewey 1984; McAndrews & conditions of language acquisition. Most of these studies address
Moskovitch 1985; Reber 1967, 1969, 1976, 1989, 1993; Turner & research questions arising from hypotheses about the nature of L 1
Fischler 1993; Vokey & Brooks 1992). development, in particular the question of whether features
Reber (1989, 1992, 1993) has claimed that implicit and explicit characteristic of motherese e.g., prosodic and morphological cues t o
learning are completely dissociable. Both involve the allocation of phrasal and constituent structure, help to make salient features in
attentional resources to input and result in memorial the input to learners and so aid induction of the underlying grammar
representations of the input. Critics of Reber (e.g., Dulany et al. of the child's developing system (see Snow 1977, and Bohannon &
1984; Perruchet, Gallego & Savy 1990; Shanks & St. John 1994; Stanowitz 1988 for arguments supporting the claim that motherese
Vokey & Brooks 1992) argue over his interpretation of the is instrumental in facilitating L l development, and Gordon 1990 for
representation that results from learning under these two a critique of their position). However, these studies (e.g., Braine,
conditions, particularly over his claim that the knowledge base Brody, Brooks, Sudhalter, Ross, Catalano and Fisch 1990; Morgan,
established by learning under implicit conditions is abstract, and Meier & Newport 1987, 1989; Morgan & Newport 1981), never
independent of the influence of memory for specific instances use children of the appropriate age as subjects. Braine et al. (1990)
encountered during learning. In Chapter 4 the representational is the single study using children, and they used children aged 7-10
issues involved in this debate are considered in some detail. In years, which is older by at least one year than the age at which
particular the relationship of attention to memory is examined in maturational constraints have been claimed to affect the language
the light of criticisms of Reber's ' abstractionist' claims about the learning mechanisms (see Long 1990, for a review of the evidence
representation that results from implicit learning. The specific for maturational constraints which concludes that such constraints
issues involved in measuring and interpreting the measurement of become operative at around the age of 6 years).
implicit learning, as well as the issues involved in measuring and Most of these studies, then, extrapolate from adult performance
interpreting the measurement of memory are also examined. to child performance, which is a dubious procedure since adults and
children differ cognitively in fundamental areas of likely influence
1.4 The generalizability of research using artificial grammars on the course of language acquisition (see e.g., Keil 1989, and
The tradition of research into implicit learning that has arisen out Wellman 1990 on conceptual development, and Long 1990;
of the claims and counterclaims referred to above has been Newport 1989; Patkowski 1980; Rosansky 1975 and Scovel 1988,
uninformed by information about the course or conditions of first on critical period effects in language learning ability). These L 1
or second language acquisition. The selection of language, and an studies are also much less concerned with the effect of the
artificial grammar at that, as the object to be learned is made simply conditions of learning manipulated by the researchers on implicit
because it represents a stimulus domain of a sufficient level of learning cited above.
complexity to study the general phenomenon of implicit learning.
Consequently the generalizability of claims made about the effects 1. 6 Experimental studies ofSLA under different conditions
of learning artificial grammars under different conditions needs t o A number of experimental studies have also been done in the area of
be demonstrated by both first and second language researchers (Carr SLA. In contrast to the L l motivated studies, these have used the
& Curran 1994; Schmidt 1994b). This will involve using natural population of interest, i.e., L2 learners, as subjects during
language as the stimulus domain. In the sections below I review the experimentation. These studies have used either artificial (DeGraaff
use of natural language in experimental studies of L1 and L2 1995; DeKeyser 1994, 1995; McLaughlin 1980; Nation &
acquisition. McLaughlin 1986), semi-artificial (Hulstij n 1989, 1990a) or natural
language as input to the learning activity (Doughty 1991; N. Ellis
16 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 17

1993; Hulstijn 1990a). Of interest to the present study are results meaning was better for meaning-oriented groups, an� recall of f? rm
fr ?m studies reviewed below that used natural language as the was better for form-oriented groups, with the attent10n-to-meanmg­
. and-form group performing most successfully. There �ere no
stimulus domam. However, as Schmidt (1994b) notes, these studies
have demonstrated few, if any, effects for learning under implicit differences between groups on the second measure of learnmg, that
conditions. of retention.
There are a number of design problems with this study. Firstly
1. 6.J Hulstijn (1990) there is the issue of the target structure selected. Hulstijn reports no
Hulstijn (1990a) studied the effects of two primary conditions, evidence to support the claim that it was unlikely to be known by
focus on form versus focus on meaning, on measures of recall and intermediate learners other than the assertion that it is "generally
retention of a structure of Dutch. This structure was a sentence found difficult" (1990a:55). Despite the length of the pretest it is
containing a subordinate clause and a passive modal auxiliary. This unclear whether this was an adequate measure of prior knowledge
structure was chosen because "intermediate learners of Dutch were since it involved a confound between knowledge of the target
unlikely to be familiar with all its grammatical properties" structure's grammaticality and the subjects' skill at writing it down
(1990a:55). The research questions were whether attention t o quickly. Secondly, eleven out of the total of twenty sentences used
meaning alone would incidentally facilitate the acquisition of formal in the pretest sentence-copying task contained the target structure,
knowledge, and whether the attention to meaning group would and one of the pretests actually used two stimulus sentences
outperform the attention to form group on measures of such formal presented during the treatment. This is likely to have alerted
knowledge as well as measures of knowledge of the content of target subjects in the incidental, meaning-oriented condition to the formal
characteristics of such sentences. Consequently there 1s . a strong
sentences. Two sentence-copying pretests of knowledge of the
target structur.e were given during which subjects were briefly shown possibility that these subjects were aware of, and consciously . trying
sentences which they had subsequently to copy. Since exposure t o to understand, the formal characteristics of the sentences while also
the sentences was brief, accurate copying was taken t o indicate the answering the meaning questions, thereby invalidating the
use of prior knowledge of the structure. The treatment for the form distinction between treatments. No debriefing measures were taken
gro�p was a series of sentence fragment ordering tasks, in which to identify what subjects were thinking about during the treatments,
sub3ects had to match eight sentence fragments with the order which could have been used to help confirm that separate
illustrated by a sentence appearing on a computer screen. In treatments were actually delivered. Thirdly, there is the problem of
contrast, the meaning-oriented group were asked to read the unequal length of necessary exposure which Hulstijn attempted to
sente�ces appearing on the computer screen and answer open-ended correct for by having control groups. Rather than balance the
q�esttons �b?ut the . content. These questions required subjects t o results of the thirty-second incidental exposure group with a ten­
second incidental group it would have been better to devise a form­
�Ive an opm10n which they then had to write down. The posttest
mvolved a cued recall task in which subjects were required t o oriented task that required the same amount of exposure time as the
remember all the target sentences presented during training. This original incidental exposure group. Finally �here i� the � robl�m of
was scored for accuracy of form and content. A second posttest possible learning effects in the posttests. It 1s poss1bl� , given (1)_ � he
measure followed this. This was a retention test, identical in form to length and nature of the cued recall task (remembermg and wntmg
the pretest sentence copying test. Due to unequal periods of down the nine target stimuli), and (ii) the fact that the format of
exposure required during the form and the meaning treatments (the the sentence-copying posttest was repeated from the pretes� phase,
anagram task required 30 seconds of exposure to the target while that learning could have occurred here and not dunng the
the opinion qu�stion needed only 10 seconds of exposure), both a treatments.
form-plus-meamng and a meaning-only group with much shorter
exposure time were added to the two primary conditions as controls. 1. 6.2 Doughty (1988)
s at
Subjects were adult learners of Dutch from a variety of non-Indo­ Doughty (1988, 1991) studied the acquisition of relative clause
Accessibility Hierar chy
Germanic native language backgrounds. Results showed that recall of different points on the Noun Phrase
18 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 19

(Keenan & Comrie 1977) by adult learners of English as a second 1. 6.3 Ellis (1993) . .
co�dit10ns on th e
language (ESL) as a function of learning under three conditions. In N. Ellis (1993) studied the effects of three
native speakers of
the first condition, a rule-oriented group were given rules for learning of rules of Welsh morphology by
(i) a r a dom gr��p, who �ere
relativization at different points along the hierarchy and examples. English . The three groups were: . . �mg-Elhs s oper a�� on­
In the second condition a meaning-oriented group were required t o exposed to instances alone durm g tra ��
answer comprehension questions on stimuli containing examples of alizat ion of an 'impl icit' learn ing cond ition, (1993:302); (11) a
relativization at different points on the hierarchy. These stimuli grammar group, who were taught the rules to a criterion of success
had been highlighted at structurally salient points. In the third before presentation of those instances seen by the random group;
condition subjects were simply exposed to examples of structures and (iii) a structured group, who were taugh t by a blend of rules and
.
mstan ces as the random
containing relative clauses without instructions to manipulate them examples before being exposed to the same
te ext nsi"'.e amou ts of
structurally or to attend to meaning. and grammar groups. However, despi � . lea �mg � y
nstra te impli cit
The meaning-oriented group outperformed both the others in training, Ellis' research failed to demo �
transfer comprehension tests. However, it is impossible to say what rando m group of the rule for soft muta tion in Welsh. Subjects m
the
.
caused this since in this condition there was a confound between two the structured group performed best in the test phase
(1993 ) study are the fact
possible causal variables: the presence of both attention-drawing Two things to be noted about Ellis's
erien ced langu age leam�rs,
devices and meaning-oriented instructions to answer comprehension that: (i) the subjects were relatively inexp
tiz ment s place d on notic e
questions on the stimuli. Performance resulting from this treatment, having simply been recruited via adver �
tion to be l�arn ed was
that is, may either have been the result of simply attending to the boards; and (ii) the rule of soft muta . iar to the
to be famil
meaning of the presented sentences containing relative clauses, or linguistically sophisticated and likely � .
the result of a conscious search for rules prompted by the saliency cts, both conc eptua lly and in term � of their expene�ce of
subje
age learmng, of
inducing highlighting of structural points in the examples. This solving problems in the context of foreign lan� .
confound is clear from the instructions to the meaning-oriented which they appear to have had very little . Studi es hke _those .of
group which require them to read sentences in order to understand Hulstijn (1990a) and Doughty (1991) using subje ct I? opula twns w:ith
t L2 and usmg rn · es relat mg
the meaning and the grammar (1988:85). greater experience in learning the targe synta x,
e.g., word order and
It is also likely that there was some contamination of treatment to a more familiar area of L2 structure,
groups resulting from informal contact between subjects over the seem more likely to produce clear er evide nce of an e � fect for
v�h. �ity, smce
. .
1cal
ten-day period of the treatment, when subjects in meaning-oriented implicit learning and also to have greater e_colog
maJo nty of L2
and rule-oriented treatments could easily have conferred with each this situation is more typical of those facmg the
other about the differences in the treatments. No debriefing sessions learners.
were used to identify the differential extent to which subjects were
searching for or aware of rules during the meaning-oriented 1. 7 The needfor further studies in a second language context
treatment. While the three studies reviewed above all demonst:ate the
The third condition, which consisted simply of exposure t o superiority of conditions that require attention to meamng over
examples, appears closest to the implicit condition manipulated by conditions that require attention to form alone, none � f t�e results
Reber, but this condition was the one that demonstrated least for conditions approximating Reber's implicit memonzat10n tas_ks
success on the transfer tasks. Consequently, as Schmidt (1994b:32) show significant evidence of learning. Further, � ach of these studies
comments, it is unclear whether any implicit learning occurred in contained design flaws that obscu: e th� potentl�l relevanc� of the
.
this experiment, since in both the unsuccessful exposure-only results. It is simply not clear that 1mphc1t learnmg, as described and
condition, and the successful meaning-oriented condition learners facilitated by Reber's experiments, actually occu: red.
could have actively been pursuing hypotheses about the formal Yet, despite this lack of research evidence, some L2
characteristics of stimuli sentences. methodologists, for a number of years now (Asher 1979; Krashe!l
198l a, 1985, 1993; Prabhu 1987; Terrell 1982), have based their
20 Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA Consciousness, implicit learning and SLA 21

prescriptions on assumptions that implicit learning is not only a speakers of another language. In particular, the present study
.
factor, but the most significant factor in L2 development. examines the issue of the relationship between the complexity of
Consequently, the Natural Approach (Krashen & Terrell 1983) the L2 rule to be learned, and the conditions under which learning
Total Physical Response (Asher 1979) and the Bangalore takes place. The generalizability of claims made by �eb�r (19 � 9,
Procedural Syllabus (Prabhu 1987; cf. Beretta 1989) advocate a set 1993) concerning the acquisition of the rules of an artificial fimte­
of methodological procedures that avoid an explicit focus on formal state grammar to SLA is examined by contrasting performance by
rules of grammar during instruction. Such prescriptions are worth non-native speakers of English on learning simple and complex
little without research demonstrating in detail the extent of the rules of English under four different conditions of exposure. T�� of
phenomenon in the domain of L2 learning and the extent to which these conditions, an implicit and a rule-search � onditl? n,
.
attention to formal features of the input to learners can be operationalize the distinction between implicit and explicit learnmg
manipulated so as to aid induction, independently of accompanying using experimental tasks familiar from the work of Reber (1969,
grammar explanations (Long 1991; Schmidt 1990, 1993a). 1976, 1989, 1993). Two other conditions, an inc� dental and an
. .
It is also premature to draw firm conclusions for syllabus design instructed condition, operationalize the distmct10n usmg
from the few studies of implicit L2 learning that have so far been experimental tasks familiar from the discussions of the difference
done (Ellis 1993a; Robinson 1993a, 1994a). Motivating decisions between acquisition and learning in SLA research and pedagogy (e.g.,
about the units and sequence of language instruction is necessarily Elley 1991; Ellis 1992, 1994b; Gregg 1984; Krashen 198l a, 1982,
speculative, and such work must integrate established research 1985 1994· Krashen & Terrell 1983; McLaughlin 1987; Terrell
findings from a variety of relevant areas (see e.g., the approach ; '
1982 Zobl 1992, 1995). The operationalization of the distinction
taken by Long and Crookes 1992 to motivating a task-based between a simple and complex rule is described in Chapter 2. The
approach to syllabus design based on the evidence of SLA research). four conditions are motivated by the discussion in Chapters 3 and 4
If research in the area of implicit learning is to have any and are described in Chapter 5. The aim of the research is t o
consequence for the design of structural syllabuses, as Ellis (l 993a) establish the degree of generalizability from the paradigm for
claims, the important question of exactly what areas of syntax are examining the extent of implicit learning developed by Reber
learnable implicitly and explicitly needs to be further addressed. (1969, 1976, 1989, 1993) to SLA research, and to address the issue
Both Reber (1989, 1993) and Krashen (198l a, 1982, 1985, 1994) of the extent to which structural knowledge of a second language
make similar claims about the differential effectiveness of can be acquired without consciousness (Schmidt 1990) in the sense
nonconscious, implicit conditions and conscious, explicit conditions of an awareness of rules, or a deliberate intention to learn the
in learning rules of different orders of complexity. Both claim that structure of the language.
complex rules are best left to implicit processes, whereas simple
rules can be learned explicitly. If this could be shown to be so for
SLA, then it would have important implications for L2 syllabus
design and methodology. However, it is not clear whether the results
from Reber's experimental studies of artificial grammar learning,
which have been used to support this conclusion, can be used t o
support similar arguments made by Krashen and others who claim
that acquisition of the formal characteristics of a second language is
necessarily a largely unconscious process.

1.8 The aims of the present study


There is thus a need for further studies that address the effects of
different conditions of learning on the acquisition of a natural
language by a representative sample of its potential acquirers, adult
Chapter 2

Rules, Complexity An d Second


Language Kn owledge

2. 0 Introduction
This chapter is concerned with motivating a distinction between the
simple and complex rules of English which serve as the dependent
variables for the present study. After a discussion of the distinction
between pedagogic rules and linguistic rules, a description is made of
claims arising from experimental studies of implicit learning (Reber
1 967, 1 969, 1 976, 1 989, 1 993), and within the field of SLA theory
(Krashen 1 982, 1 985, 1 994), that implicit and explicit learning
conditions are differentially sensitive to the complexity of the
domain to be learned. Next, a comparison is made of proposals by a
number of SLA researchers for distinguishing between the order of
difficulty of L2 rules. Finally the procedure adopted in the present
study for identifying simple and complex L2 rules is explained, and
the rules identified are related to the preceding discussion.

2.1 Pedagogic rules and linguistic rules


Second language researchers frequently invoke the notion of rule t o
describe what is learned during the process of instructed ci r
naturalistic SLA . A s in L1 acquisition, L 2 learners engage in
processes of regularization and generalization (see Fodor & Crain
1 987; Larsen-Freeman & Long 1 99 1 ; Marcus, Pinker, Ullman,
Hollander, Rosen & Xu 1 992), and such observable behaviour is
often the basis on which cognitive correlates of rules are attributed
to learners (see Reese 1 989; Slobin 1 979) . However, there are two
senses of the term rule which need to be distinguished. In the first
sense, the use of the term rule implies a claim about the form in
which knowledge of language is represented in the mind of the
learner. However, linguistic theories differ with respect not only t o
the formalisms, but also to the nature o f the rules and cognitive
mechanisms they propose. In fact, the need to posit rules of the
symbol-processing type traditionally invoked by linguists as part o f
24 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 25

language representation is questioned by physicalist philosophers o f


mind (e.g., Crick 1 995; Searle 1992) and by production systems and Prabhu 1 987); (ii) the attention-focusing position (rules are useful
connectionist models of cognition, see Holland, Holyoak, Nisbett & as devices for focusing attention on selected aspects of the
Thagard 1 986; McClelland & Elman 1 986). For example the debate structures to be learned, which are then . learned as a consequenc� o f
.
between White (1 992) and Schwartz and Gubala-Ryzak ( 1 992) inductive processes that are not accessible to co�� c10usness: Se iger �
1 979; Sharwood Smith 1 99 1 , 1 993); a�d (111) the consc10us
regardin g the interpretation of data supposedly demonstrating the .
acquisition, or resetting, of the verb-movement parameter for understanding position (learning and applymg pedagogi� . rules can
English by French native-speaker learners invokes this linguistic lead to a conscious understanding of the structural regulanties of the
.
sense of rule. White argues that the data demonstrate that learners domain the rule applies to: Schmidt 1 990; S�hmidt & Frota 1 986).
have reset the parameter, or learned the rule. Schwartz and Gubala­ Examples of these theoretical positions are given below.
Ryzak argue against this interpretation, since the theory o f
Universal Grammar predicts that resettin g the parameter entails the (i) The non-isomorphy, non-interface position. Bialystok ( 1 98 8 )
acquisition of a cluster of properties which the learners show n o claims that the form o f pedagogic rules and the form of our
evidence o f having acquired. Pedagogic rules, in contrast, are linguistic competence, or implicit knowl� dge of language, are
traditionally presented as simplified versions of such linguistic rules �
different in kind, and consequently are not mterfac d, or mutually
which necessarily fall short of exhaustive treatment and avoid the accessible. Bialystok distinguishes between 'analyzed knowle ge and �
abstract theoretical characterization typical of linguistic rules 'articulated' knowledge. Language development is charactenzed y �
(Corder 1988; Faerch 1 986; Sharwood Smith 1 994; Westney 1 994). her as the accumulation of unanalyzed knowledge of language. This
Given their limited scope and level of detail how is it possible fo r accumulation takes place independently of awareness of the
pedagogic rules to be used to develop L 2 competence? structure of the knowledge, though increases in the amount o f
knowledge are accompanied by an increase in the exten . o f i s � �
2.1.J The effectiveness ofpedagogic rules
analysis. Analyzed knowledge "is represented as a proposi ion m !
There has, to date, been little empirical investigation of the which the formal structure and the relationship to meanmg are
effectiveness of different pedagogic rule formats on the apparent. Non-analyzed knowledge is assigned a mental
.
development of L2 knowledge. In contrast, a large empirically based representation in which the underlying formal constituents are n o t
necessarily identifiable... I n these terms, analyzed knowledge can be
literature exists on the effectiveness of pedagogic rule formats, rule
described as access to the propositional structure of � on-analyzed
explanations and their use during problem-solving in other areas o f
the curriculum. This includes studies o f the effectiveness o f physics

knowledge" ( 1 988:33). However, the process of analys! s ta es place
below the level of awareness and is thus different m kmd from
and mathematics explanations imposing differential 'cognitive load'
'verbal ' articulated knowledge: "Thus it is erroneous to equate
on the learner (Swelle r 1 988, 1 990); studies of the effectiveness o f
reminding learners o f earlier presented, analogous examples during

analyz d knowledge with articulated knowledge, or knowl�dge o f
rules" ( 1 988:40). Consequently for Bialystok it is not possible for
general problem-solving (Ross 1989; Ross & Kennedy 1 990) and
pedagogic rule-focused activity to contribute to the development o f
studies of problem classification systems used by expert and novice
the implicit system.
learners (Chi, Glaser & Rees 1 982; Voss, Perkins & Segal 1 99 1 ) .
Discussions of L2 pedagogic grammar have failed to draw on this

Prabhu ( 1 987) also argues that pedagogic rules cann_o be used t o
initiate language development. He claims that explicitly learn.ed
literature, and have revolved instead around the general question o f
whether rules (of any description) should be presented to learners.
grammar rules only " . . . 'make sense' when. they acco d with \
language samples arising from or conformmg . to �ne s own
The theoretical claims regarding the effectiveness of pedagogic
rules can be grouped as follows: (i) the non-isomorphy, non­ !
competence, and there is often a sense of satisfac 10n or . o f
discovery when that happens: what one has 'known' with�mt b�mg
interface position (implicit knowledge, or competence, and explicit
knowledge of rules are different in kind and non-interfaced: �
aware of it is now confirmed as being right (hence the sa isfact10n ?,
and is also seen to be rule-governed (hence the sense of dis� overy)
Bialystok 1 978, 1 988; Krashen 1 979, 1 9 8 1 a, 1 982, 1 985, 1 994;
( 1 987:77). For Prabhu the sense of satisfaction that can anse from
26 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 27

rule-focused a�tivity is only possible when "one has already Schmidt's account of his own learning of Portuguese (Schmidt &
developed an mternal system capable of yielding samples which Frota 1 986). Seliger ( 1 979), Bialystok ( 1 988) and Prabhu ( 1 9 8 7 )
conform to the .rule. When that is not the case, rules are just so consider awareness at the level of understanding, which may be
much complex mform�tion" ( 1 987:78). In other words, explicit made possible by exposure to a pedagogic rule, to be irrelevant t o
rules can sei:e a� ?evices for developing awareness of what has L2 development. Schmidt, in contrast, reports his own experience
already been implicitly learned. Contrary to Schmidt ( 1 990), Prabhu that "improvement in second language performance follow(ed) o n
asserts that development of the representational system proceeds the heels o f understanding" ( 1 990: 1 47 ) and that this understanding
. was sometimes accompanied by recollection of an explicitly taught
without awareness. Like Bialystok ( 1 9 8 1 ), he claims explicit rules
make sense �nly after the implicitly learned system has reached a rule. Seliger's position, while denying the potential contribution t o
. development that understanding a pedagogic rule can make, does
particular pomt of development.
consider such rules facilitative to the extent that they focus
(ii) The attention-focusing position. Seliger ( 1 979) observes that attention on aspects of the language to be learned, leading t o
teachers �resenting pedagogic rules to learners often "have as their 'noticing' in Schmidt's terms ( 1 990). Schmidt notes that what
goal causmg someone to P.roduce a language form, that is, getting a studies there are of the effectiveness of providing explicit rules t o
learner to perform consistently with regard to some aspect o f learners are inconclusive about whether "such effects are due t o
language behavior" (Seliger 1 979:360). However, as both Green and increased learner understanding as a result o f instruction, or
Hecht ( 1 992) an � � �liger ( 1 ?'. 9) have shown, learners can perform increased salience of forms leading to awareness only at the level o f
form-focuse � activities requmng article production (Seliger 1 979) or noticing" ( 1 990: 1 46).
error correction (Green & Hecht 1 992) that suggests that conscious
knowledge of such rules, as indicated by the extent to which a 2.1.2 Noticing examples and understanding pedagogic rules
learner is able to ver�alize them. after the task, is disjoint with the There are thus two ways in which explicitly presented pedagogic
knowledge .drawn on m pe�formmg the tasks. Seliger's conclusion, rules have been claimed to lead to language learning, and these are
therefore, �s that pedagogic rules are neither isomorphic with the associated with developing awareness of the language system at two
represe �tat10n of languag� knowledge in the learner' s internally of the different levels identified by Schmidt ( 1 990), noticing and
developm� system, no: devices drawn on in monitoring production, understanding. Attending to the rule may: (i) simply cause learners
but r�ther ,�re useful m language teaching in order to get learners t 0 to notice the structures which it is explaining, or (ii) attending t o
do thmgs with language in an efficient manner, to focus on those the rule may also lead to an understanding of the structural
asp:cts . of th� language phenomenon that must be acquired, and t o regularities upon which it is based.
avoid mefficient testing of hypotheses" ( 1 979:3 60). Pedagogic Complexity is likely to affect structural noticing, and rule
rules, h � concludes, are not used deductively to ' control output, ' understanding in the same way. Simply noticing the structures
ex�ept m an e.xtremely small number of cases (Seliger 1 979:3 67). presented as examples is unlikely to be facilitative of learning if the
Seh�er !hus differ� . from Krashen ( 1 982: 1 2 1 ), who attributes a structures themselves are too complex and the salient features of
mom!ormg and edi�mg function to pedagogic rules. For Seliger the the structures that the rule regulates are consequently not obvious.
function of pedagogic rules is to aid induction: Similarly, the more complex the explanation of a rule is, the less
likely it is to lead to understanding, and the more likely it is to be
!
. . .the� m ?st like y serve as a mechanism to facilitate the learner's focusing ignored in favor of simpler rules of thumb, as in the case o f

on cr�tenal at nbutes of the real language concept that must be induced. Schmidt's choice o f rules for learning the aspectual requirements of
That is, consc10us or pedagogical rules make the inductive hypoth e s i s
.
past tense Portuguese verbs: "I believed that I used two rules of
testmg process more efficient (Seliger 1 979:368).
thumb for aspectual choice-use imperfect for used to and for
making excuses-I was told some more complex rules for aspectual
(iii) The conscious understanding position. The positions choice in class, but I ignored them" ( 1 990: 1 4 7).
regarding the use of pedagogic rules reviewed above conflict with
28 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 29

2.1.3 Two dimensions ofpedagogic rule complexity


The procedure for identifying the simple and complex structures,
These two components of pedagogic rules must be well matched if
and the accompanying brief and detailed explanations of them,
the possibilities of noticing and understanding are to be maximized
which together make up the simple and complex rules presented t o
for the learner. The complexity of the structural regularities
learners in the instructed condition o f the present study are
captured by exemp lars must match the complexity of the
explained in section 2.4 of this chapter. The following section
accorr,ipanying explanation of them. Simple patterns of structural
_ examines claims that have been made within the field of cognitive
covar�ance �h1ch are present in examples may, in one scenario, be psychology and L2 pedagogy regarding the relation between
explame� m an overly complicated fashion. Conversely, the
consciousness, learning condition, and the complexity of the
�omplex1ty of the . structural covariance in the exemplars may be stimulus domain to be learned.
madequately descnbed, because oversimplified. Estimates of the
relative complexity of pedagogic rules have, therefore, to take both
2.2 Relating rule complexity, consciousness and learning
of these factors into consideration. The relationship between these
Reber ( 1 989), approaches the issue of the relation between rule
two dimensions of pedagogic rules is illustrated in Figure 1 .
complexity, consciousness and learning from a tradition of using
artificial grammars to study the effects of different degrees of rule
awareness on learning. Reber claims that where the stimulus domain
is non-complex, and where the rules underlying it are simple, then
Structure implicit processes will not be displayed. Implicit processes will,
+simple however, be displayed where the stimulus domain is complex, and

+.
will result in the unconscious abstraction of the corresponding
1 2 complex rules. In contrast, explicit efforts to learn complex rules
are much less likely to be successful. In the field of SLA theory
Explanation + brief + detailed Krashen ( 1 98 1 a, 1 982, 1 985) distinguishes between 'easy' and
'hard' rules of English for L2 learners, often using the terms
3 4 'simple' and 'easy' interchangeably to refer to the same rule (see
section 2.2.3). Like Reber, Krashen asserts that only easy rules are
+ complex consciously leamable; hard rules must be acquired via the
unconscious processes that develop the acquired system.

2.2.1 Claims based on experimental evidence of implicit learning


Figure 1. Elements Of Pedagogic Rules studies
Based on the research using artificial grammars referred to above,
Reber's claim is that the knowledge base that results from learning
Effective pedagogic rules, in which the level of detail of the under implicit conditions is abstract. Reber ( 1 976, 1 989, 1 992,
explanation matches the extent of the covariance of the structures 1 993) consistently refers to the notion of 'rules' to describe what is
regulated by the rule, would occupy quadrants 1 and 4. These learned during exposure under implicit conditions: " . . . the basic
correspond to effective simple and complex pedagogic rules procedure is to have an acquisition phase, during which subjects
respectively. Less effective rules would occupy quadrants 2 and 3 . acquire knowledge of the rules of the grammar" ( 1 989:220).
These correspond to rules which explain a relatively simple pattern Further, Reber claims that complex rules, corresponding to complex
of structural covariance in a complicated, overly detailed way non-salient covariations in the stimulus domain, are unlikely to be
(quadrant 2), �nd rules which describe a complex pattern o f learned, following a process of induction, under the condition t o
structural covariance in a brief oversimplified form (quadrant 3). explicitly search fo r rules. This i s not the case with learning such
rules under implicit conditions: " . . . explicit processing of complex
30 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 3 1

materials has a decided disadvantage in relation to implicit implicit and explicit conditions. Either such syllabuses would ne�d to
processing" ( 1 989:223). However, simple rules, corresponding t o restrict the inventory of forms to be learned to those that are hkely
non-complex covariations, can be induced and so learned under the to be learnable under explicit conditions (perhaps adopting criteria
explicit rule-search condition. This is of great potential interest to for sequencing such forms from developmental evidence, where
L2 research, since it makes a prediction, which it should be possible available), or the syllabus designer could incorporate infor� ation
to examine empirically, about the question of what is learnable about the appropriate learning condition into the specific�tton . o_f
under different conditions, and therefore of the effects of items, leaving the teacher to methodologically :veave the 1mphc1t
manipulating the conditions under which aspects of linguistic and explicit strands of the syllabus together durmg the process o f
structure are noticed. instruction.
It is clear from Reber' s discussion of these issues that he believes There are also likely to be consequences for the methodological
that the complexity of the covariance in the stimulus domain implementation of some an�lytic, Ty�e B, s)'.ll�buses that employ
affects saliency. Even if the learning condition is an implicit one, task as the unit of analysis, regardmg dec1s10ns about how t o
following instructions simply to memorize presented strings of the facilitate a ' focus on form' (Long 1 985, 1 99 1 ; Long & Crookes
artificial grammar, Reber claims that the presence of simple 1 992), since forms that vary in complexity may be differei;itially
patterns of covariance in the stimulus domain will become salient suited to options for focusing on form that range along a contmu�m
and trigger explicit search for rules, which are likely to be _
of explicitness e.g., from pedagogic rule yre� entat1on, to mak mg
subsequently discovered. Hence his statement that the rules to be _
input to a task salient through highhghtmg o� phonological
learned, and the stimulus domain which they regulate, must be emphasis, to simple repetition of examples of mput structure
complex if implicit learning is to be displayed: "If the system in use experienced on task (Jourdenais et al. 1 995 ; Leeman et al. 1 99 5 ;
is too simple, or if the code can be broken by conscious effort, then Sharwood Smith 1 993).
one will not see implicit processes" ( 1989:220). Consequently Reber
uses artificial finite-state grammars to generate the strings of letters 2.2.3 Speculation about rule complexity in L2 pedagogy
used as stimuli in his studies (see section 4. 1 ) because they are .
The relevance of the distinction between levels of complexity of
"sufficiently complex . . .that the underlying structure is not within the linguistic stimuli to be learned under explicit and implicit
the conscious memorial domain of the typical subject conditions has been acknowledged by L2 researchers but has
upon . . .entering the laboratory" ( 1 989:220). resulted, to date, in either post-hoe reasoning about results o� studies
The grammars of natural languages, of course, are considerably intended to address other research questions (see e.g., B ialystok
more complex than the artificial grammars used by Reber. However, 1 979; Green & Hecht 1 992; Lyster 1 994; Pica 1 983 ; Ramat 1 9 ? 2),
rules regulating subsystems of the total grammar may differ or unmotivated assertions that stimuli used in experiments differ
sufficiently in complexity to allow the claims made by Reber to be along an unspecified dimension of complexity. Hulstijn ( 1 990 a), ip
examined using L2 learners as subjects. particular, has claimed that the target structure presente� . m _ his
study of implicit learning was complex, but no . empmcal or
2.2.2 Pedagogic implication of the claims theoretical motivation for this claim was offered by him (see 1 .6 . 1 ) .
The issue of what is learnable under implicit versus explicit Without such empirical o r theoretical support claims for
conditions is of major interest to L2 research. If the effects o f complexity differences between rules often end up as simple matters
consciousness-ra1smg, or noticing features o f the L2, are of dispute between individual researchers. Tarone ( 1 985), for
differentially sensitive to features of the linguistic system to be example, in a study of the differential maste� of rules under
learned, then it follows that the structural criteria for grading and different task conditions (a narrative, a conversat10n and a formal
sequencing the units of the language presented to learners adopted grammar test) follows Krashen ( 1 982) in distinguishing th� simple
by many of the synthetic, Type A syllabuses described by White rule for third person ' s' from the complex rule for article use.
(1 988), Nunan ( 1 9 88), and Long and Crookes ( 1 992), must be Tarone also includes the rule for direct object pronoun occurrence
reevaluated in the light of evidence of what is learnable under along with third person 's' as an example of a simple rule. However,
32 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 33

Preston ( 1 989) disagrees: " . . . object pronoun occurrence and article proposed by Slobin. In other words, Krashen's candidates for easy
use are more subtle morpho-syntactic and semantic processes. versus hard rules, and Ellis' s too as described above, are descriptive
Neither can have its rule for use stated easily... " ( 1 989:259). rules, and as such are examples of what Chomsky ( 1 9 8 6 : 1 5-24)
Krashen ( 1 982:97-98) distinguishes between formal and terms grammars of E-language, i.e., theories of the characterization
functional dimensions of complexity in distinguishing 'easy' from of language independently of any concern for how it is
'hard' rules: "The rules that we can learn and carry around in our psychologically and biologically represented in the mind (cf.
heads for use as a Monitor are not those that are the earliest Chomsky 1 990; Searle 1 990, 1 992).
acquired, nor are they those that are important for communication. Schwartz ( 1 993) and Zobl ( 1 992, 1 995) both attempt to
Rather, they are the simple rules, rules that are easiest to describe reconcile Krashen's distinction between acquisition and learning
and remember" ( 1 982:97). Wh-question formation, requiring with recent work in Universal Grammar (Chomsky 1 986), and in
extensive permutations of word order, is claimed to be a formally particular relate the notion of rule of language used by Krashen to a
complex rule, in contrast to the suppliance of the morpheme for more appropriate grammar of I-language, a theory that purports t o
third person agreement, which is formally simple. The use of plural depict what one knows when one possesses language in the
forms is functionally simple, whereas choice between definite and Chomskyan sense of competence. To this extent they invoke the
indefinite articles is functionally complex. interaction between rule complexity (as defined by the descriptions
Ellis ( 1 990: 1 67) makes essentially the same distinction between of language in Universal Grammar), and implicit knowledge (in the
structures that are amenable to explicit instruction and those that innatist sense described in section 1 .2.2 above), to explain the
are not; structures that can be explicitly taught are those that are course of L2 development.
formally simple and are transparent with regard to the form­
function relationship the grammatical rule regulates. Examples of 2.3 Second language rule complexity
formally simple and functionally transparent structures, Ellis In addition to the formal and functional complexity distinction
suggests, are plural 's' and the copula 'be' ( 1 990: 1 67). Ellis notes, cited by Krashen ( 1 982) and Ellis ( 1 990), L2 researchers have
however, that formally complex structures can be explicitly taught appealed to a variety of criteria for distinguishing complex from
in some cases if the learner is developmentally ready. This, in less complex or simple rules. Some examples of these are given
contrast to Krashen, assumes an interface position between implicit below.
and explicit knowledge in which explicit knowledge can feed into
the implicit system at developmentally appropriate points and 2.3.1 Perceptual salience
cause restructuring (see Ellis 1 993a and Robinson 1 993a, 1 994a). In a study of the acquisition of wh-question formation rules by
The distinction between formal simplicity and semantic college level students of ESL, Bardovi-Harlig ( 1 987) cites salience
transparency, which both Krashen and Ellis use to distinguish simple as the criterion distinguishing preposition pied-piping in wh­
from complex rules, relates to a similar distinction made by Slobin, questions of the form 'To whom did John give the book?' from wh­
who distinguishes operating principles for the acquisition o f questions involving preposition stranding of the form 'Who did
language motivated by perceptual saliency from those motivated by John give the book to? ' . It is this criterion, she claims, which
degree of semantic transparency ( 1 979, 1 985). However, Krashen accounts for the fact that preposition stranding was acquired before
nowhere motivates his distinction by specifying the extent to which pied-piping. Her results run counter to the predictions of
the simple and complex structures they relate to differentially markedness theory which claims that the unmarked form,
affect processing mechanisms or make differential demands o n preposition pied-piping, will be learned before the marked form,
processing resources. In fact, as Gregg ( 1 98 8 : 73 ) points out, preposition stranding.
Krashen's distinction between easy and hard rules, and his notion o f By salience Bardovi-Harlig appears to mean frequency in the
rules i n general, is one based on pedagogic formulation o f the rules, input, since preposition stranding, though marked, is much more
and is not licensed either by a full theory of the mental frequent as a wh-question form than pied-piping. However, it is
representation of grammar or by operating principles of the type clear that salience could also be operationalized in perceptual terms
34 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 3 5

to explain her results, since the relationship between the wh-word passe compose, use the correct form of avoir or etre plus the past
and the preposition which is crucial to understanding this form of participle of the verb" ( 1 979: 1 0 1 ).
wh-question formation is visually and acoustically more salient in
preposition stranding than in pied-piping by virtue of the fact that 2.3.3 Size of context and degree of semantic opacity
they occupy positions at the beginning and at the end of the Green and Hecht ( 1 992), in a similar study to that of Bialystok
structures in question. In pied-piping structures the wh-word is less ( 1 979) and Seliger ( 1 979), required school age German speaking
salient by virtue of being sentence-internal, and is therefore less learners of English to both identify errors in sentences by making
easily 'noticed' . Learners, that is, may simply have been using an appropriate correction and to state the rule which had been
strategies for allocating attention by adopting what Slobin ( 1 9 8 5 ) broken. Where learners were able to do the former, but not the
calls the operating principle o f paying attention to the end o f words latter, they claimed they were displaying 'implicit' knowledge. The
or strings. This, rather than linguistically defined markedness, m ay rules that were most consistently correctly articulated they term
have led them to be more aware of the critical aspect of structure 'easy' rules. These include, "those that ( 1 ) referred to easily
regulating preposition stranding than pied-piping. Salience in this recognized categories; (2) could be applied mechanically; (3) were
perceptual sense may therefore relate to the ease with which not dependent on large contexts: for example the morphological
structural correlates of rules can be noticed and so learned. Similar dichotomies like a/an, who/which, straightforward cases o f
considerations underlie Meisel, Clahsen and Pienemann' s proposal some/any, and simple word order" ( 1 992: 1 79). Hard rules, which
( 19 8 1 ) that the acquisition of word order rules in German may be were rarely identified or articulated, involved permutations and
regulated by the use of processing strategies which lead learners first additions and deletions that were applied over larger structural
to attend to word order permutations at the beginning and ends o f contexts, and invoked semantically opaque principles. These
strings before attending to the details o f sentence-internal included:
permutations. However, while one of Slobin's operating principles
those that involve aspect, such as the use of the continuous form or the
can be invoked to support the claim that preposition stranding is
perfect tense . . . These are semantic distinctions that express a speaker' s
more salient than pied-piping, and therefore more easily noticed perspective on a situation . . . They do not allow of simple exhaustive
and so learned, it is also true that another of Slobin's principles descriptions and they are not always governed by features of the
'avoid discontinuities' makes the reverse prediction about immediate linguistic context (Green & Hecht 1 992: 1 80).
learnability. Preposition stranding, 'who-to' is a discontinuous
structure, in contrast to the continuous 'to whom' . 2.3.4 Processing load and rule complexity
The speculations about rule complexity reviewed immediately above
2.3.2 Lexical rules versus abstract rules are diverse in the factors they consider, but all invoke, to some
Bialystok ( 1 979) attempted to identify the extent to which degree, general information processing load as a determinant of rule
manipulating time allowed and level of learner in an L2 complexity. For example, degree of perceptual salience, size o f
grammaticality judgment task elicited implicit versus explicit context o f rule application and degree of semantic opacity have all
knowledge of the rules regulating the grammaticality of structures been claimed to affect the complexity of a rule, and all affect the
presented for judgment. The subjects, English speaking high school amount of attention and processing effort expended in learning and
learners of French, were required to identify errors and articulate remembering a rule (easy rules are supposedly those with
rules for stimulus sentences. She found that adjective errors were perceptually salient structural features, which are applied to small
easier to detect than pronoun errors, which in turn were easier than contexts, and which involve transparent meaning-to-form
verb errors. This relates to the difficulty of the rules with which the relationships). Only one of these proposals invokes a specific
errors were associated, "rules pertaining to single lexical items were linguistic theory to explain the complexity of the structures
easiest and those to general structures were most difficult" regulated by the rule (a version of markedness theory in the case o f
( 1 979 : 1 00). An example of the former is "Color adjectives always Bardovi-Harlig 1 987), and she does so only to observe that its
come before the noun." An example of the latter is "To form the
36 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 3 7

predictions fail to account for the difference in learning difficulty drawn o n to establish consistency o f judgment, and there is some
the structures pose to learners. evidence that the knowledge base established by experience is
As described in Figure 1 (see section 2. 1 .3) the complexity of a superior to that established by purely formal academic training. For
pedagogic rule has two dimensions, the complexity of the structure example, Voss ( 1 986) found that success in solving real world
which the rule attempts to explain, and the complexity of the economics problems correlated more with practical experience than
explanation itself. While one may select the structures upon which with formal training in economics. Ceci and Liker ( 1 986) studied
a rule is based according to the linguistically derived notions of the knowledge base underlying the expertise of expert horse racing
accessibility, generalizability and entailability described in Chapter handicappers and found their decisions to be based on "a complex
1 , the process of describing the structures in pedagogic terms creates internal calculus in which a number of variables are related to one
an additional set of processing demands over and above those another in multiplicative rather than additive ways" (Best
associated with attending to the structural features of the examples. 1 992:484). Experts were not necessarily more intelligent than non­
It is likely that such general information processing load experts (using measures obtained from the Wechsler Adult
considerations, along with the evidence provided by classroom Intelligence Scale), but experts did consistently outperform non­
contact with learners, are also influential in the decisions about the experts in handicapping ability.
relative complexity of rules made by experienced, practicing ESL These studies lend support to the decision to ask teachers of ESL
teachers. However, there is currently no agreed upon taxonomy o f with substantial experience to provide the estimate of complexity
these processing demands, or an accepted model of the way they used in identifying the dependent variables for the present study.
interact with the complexity of the structure a pedagogic rule The procedure was felt to have the following advantages: (i) it
describes which could be used as the basis of an objective, provided empirical support for an easy/ hard rule distinction; (ii) it
scientifically approved metric of rule complexity. is replicable; (iii) it has high face validity, since experienced ESL
teachers are the group most qualified to make expert decisions about
2.3.5 The consistency of expert judgment in problem classification the factors influencing the complexity of pedagogic rule formats;
and problem-solving and (iv) it was expected that such teachers would use predominantly
Given the lack of consensus over the nature of the objective criteria information processing load criteria similar in kind to those used by
to apply in distinguishing easy from hard rules described above how Krashen to make their decisions about rule complexity-it was the
are such rules to be identified? The solution adopted in the present comparability of claims by Krashen with those of Reber regarding
study was to use the expert judgment of experienced L2 teachers to the relationship of rule complexity to learning condition that the
identify the rules by seeking to establish consensus about rule study sought to address.
complexity based on the subjective criteria underlying their shared
expertise. The procedure of using informants to rate the 2.4 Empirically motivating the easy/hard rule distinction
complexity of problems, in the absence of any accepted a priori The following steps were followed in identifying the easy and hard
criteria that could be used to direct classification, is a standard one rules for the present study. (i) A grammaticality judgment test o f
in experimental studies of problem-solving ability (see e.g., Reed, structures that were judged to range in degree o f complexity and
Ackinclose & Voss 1 990). Second language teachers, in addition to potential familiarity was given to a group of subjects. These subjects
any formal training they may have in ESL or linguistics, also have were comparable to the intended subjects for the present study in
experience of teaching to draw on in making judgments about terms of their age, proficiency level, language background and
pedagogic rule complexity. Evidence exists from studies of expert length of exposure to English. (ii) T�ose items which the subjects
judgment in other areas of the curriculum that consistency of most consistently and incorrectly rated ungrammatical were selected
judgment in problem classification tasks can be achieved as a result as the basis for pedagogic rules. (iii) Pedagogic rules based on these
of formal training in a knowledge domain like Physics (see Chi, structures were written by the researcher by analyzing the elements
Glaser & Rees 1 982). However, experience in applying th at of the constructions. (iv) The resulting rules were given t o
knowledge domain to practical problems in everyday life can also be experienced ESL teachers to rate for complexity using a seven
38 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 39

point rating scale. (v) Subsequently, the same teachers were asked t o
rate the structures described by the rules for their complexity using a 1. * Who did John give the book to?
Q-sort of cards illustrating the structures. (vi) The results were 2. Who did you suggest I apply to?
examined to see if the teachers had distinguished between the 3. Who did you suggest that should apply?
complexity of candidate rules, and if their rating of the complexity 4. Who did you suggest should apply?
of the rules corresponded with the results of the Q-sort of the 5. Who did you suggest that I apply to ?
relevant structures. (vii) The rules identified in this way were then 6. * That there cow gives a lot Qf milk.
examined to see if a plausible explanation could be found confirming 7. That cow there gives a lot of milk.
the easy/hard rule distinction. These procedures are described ill 8. * To whom did John give the book?
detail below. 9. * What Mary does is eat cake.
1 O. * Where the cheese is is in the bag not in the
2.4.1 IdentiJYing unfamiliar structures basket.
For the purposes of the present study, the rules to be prese�ted � o 1 1. *John 's hitting him angered me.
_
learners, and the structures exemplifying them, had to be d1stmct m 1 2. There exists a copy of that book.
terms of their degree of complexity for the learner, and at the same 13. I sent to John the book that I read yesterday.
time unfamiliar to the learners. Unfamiliarity was necessary in order 14. Is what Mary does good?
to control for prior learning of the rules, which could have 15. *In the morning he jogs.
invalidated the claims for learning based on the treatments. 1 6. This cow here gives a lot of milk.
Although a number of criteria for distinguishing the com? l �xit� of 1 7. This here cow gives a lot of milk.
rules have been proposed it is clear that not all of these d1stmct1ons 1 8. It's Mary who eats cake.
apply to rules and structures that are also likely to be unfamiliar t o 19. Never have I seen him.
_
learners. That is, rules for third person ' s ' , o r wh-quest10n 20 . I saw the dog that barked.
formation cited by Krashen as easy and hard rules, for example, are 21. I saw the dog you fed.
likely to be known by most L2 learners of the intermediate level 22. I saw the dog that you fed.
targeted for the present study. 23 . I saw the dog barked.
..
To identify these rules a pilot study of rule fam1hanty was
.

24. Into the house John ran


conducted at the University of Hawai'i in the Spring semester o f 25 . *Into the house ran John.
1 993. Since the demands o f the training and transfer phase i n the
present study required more than basic �roficienc� in Eng�ish, it was * = items eventually selected as the basis of pedagogic
decided to target subjects of approximately mtermediate level. rules
Consequently, a comparable group of students was targeted for the
pilot study of rule unfamiliarity. Further, in o�der to ensure
. Figure 2 : The Targeted Items In The Pilot Study Of Rule
maximum generalizability the subjects for both studies had, ideally,
Familiarity
to share similar language learning backgrounds, and be o f
approximately the same age, with approximately the same length
and quality of exposure to English. It was decided to target Japanese
Accordingly, twenty students aged 1 9-20 years from a Japanese
learners of ESL who had completed school level education in
university, who were engaged in a two month intensive English
English, and who were either currently enrolled in or recently
program in Hawai' i, were used as subjects for the pilot study. The
graduated from a junior college or university, since these would be _
format adopted for the pilot study was a forty two item
representative of the great majority of subjects who would take part
grammaticality judgment test that required subjects to c �rcle
in the eventual study.
'grammatical,' 'ungrammatical,' or 'not sure' in response to wntten
Second language rule complexity 4 1
40 Second language rule complexity

Table 1 . Percentage Scores Of Correct, Incorrect And Not


sentences (see Appendix A). Subjects were told to base their decision
Sure Responses To Items On The Pilot StudyTest Of Rule
on grammaticality, not on what they thought might be errors in
Familia rity
punctuation or spelling. Twenty-five target structures were chosen
for inclusion, and subsequent analysis, based on some of the claims
for differences in complexity previously reviewed in this chapter. Item no. % % %
These target structures included a number of sentences illustrating Correct Incorrect Not sure
the notions of Subjacency and the Empty Category Principle from
Universal Grammar (Bley Vroman et al. 1 988; White 1 989a). A l .* 39 56 5
number of focus constructions were also selected, since they involve 2. 28 56 16
unusual and sometimes complex permutations of word order, and 3. 33 51 16
because many of them are rarely covered in ESL/EFL textbooks 4. 16 51 33
(Celce-Murcia & Larsen-Freeman 1 98 3 : 403-4 1 6). Eighteen, 5. 66 11 23
mostly ungrammatical, items were added as distractors. The twenty­ 6.* 5 90 5
five target items are listed in Figure 2. 7. 28 56 16
Items 1 and 8 are examples of question formation with 8.* 66 23 11
preposition stranding and pied-piping. Items 2, 3, 4 and 5, 1 3 , 20, 9.* 0 1 00 0
2 1 , 22 and 23 are examples relating to Subjacency and the 1 0. * 5 90 5
possibility of 'that' deletion. Items 6 and 7 are examples of the pre­ 1 1 .* 28 56 16
and post-subject use of emphatic 'there' . Items 9 and 1 0 are pseudo­ 1 2. 23 72 5
cleft constructions. Item 1 1 is an example of gerundivization. Item 13. 79 16 5
1 2 is an example of the use of existential 'there' to focus on a verb. 14. 33 56 11
Item 14 is an example of a pseudo-cleft question form. Items 1 5 1 5.* 56 39 5
and 24 are examples of adverbial fronting of time and location 16. 23 61 16
phrases. Items 1 6 and 1 7 are similar to items 6 and 7, but with 1 7. 11 78 11
different demonstrative pronouns and adverbs. Item 1 8 is a cleft 1 8. 78 22 0
construction. Item 1 9 is an example of adverbial of frequency 19. 33 56 11
fronting, with subject-verb inversion. Item 25 is an example of 20. 67 33 0
adverbial of location fronting with subject-verb inversion. 21. 45 39 16
The percentage correct, incorrect and not sure for responses t o 22 . 90 5 5
these items are given in Table 1 . Two items, 9 and 2 5 , received 23 . 33 67 0
1 00% incorrect responses, while item 1 0 received 90% incorrect 24. 28 56 16
responses. It appeared, then, that pseudo-cleft structures were 25.* 0 1 00 0
unfamiliar to this group, as was the structure illustrating subject-verb
inversion following fronted adverbials of location. Item 6, with pre­
subject emphatic 'there' was also rated ungrammatical by 90% o f * = items selected as the basis of pedagogic rules
the subjects, although accuracy was higher for the related item
number 7, with post-subject emphatic 'there' . These items, together The other items chosen as the basis of pedagogic rules were item
with other examples of the targeted items that seemed obviously t o 1 5, adverbial of time fronting, which seemed to illustrate an easy
differ in complexity, were consequently selected as the basis o f the rule, and item 1 1 , gerundivization, which seemed to illustrate a more
pedagogic rules written by the researcher for the present study. complex rule. Items 1 and 8 were also chosen as the basis for writing
rules of wh-question formation with pied-piping and preposition
42 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 43

stranding, since these structures had been claimed to differ in ordered and given to fifteen experienced teachers of ESL (mean
complexity for L2 learners (see Bardovi-Harlig 1 987 and section length of ESL teaching experience = 1 0.6 years, see Table 2).
2.3 . 1 above). However, such structures, and possibly therefore
versions of their associated pedagogic rules, were obviously familiar
to subjects, since the illustrative items, 1 and 8 , were judged Table 2. Details Of Teachers Who Took Part In The Rating
relatively accurately (only 55% and 20% inaccurate). Scale Survey Of Rule Complexity
It was hoped that rules based on the four clearly unfamiliar items
(6, 9, 1 0 and 25) would also be distinguished by the teachers in Teacher Ll ESL experience Qualifications
terms of their complexity. Rules based on the other four items, 1 , 8,
1 1 and 15 were primarily added as distractors to ensure an even
1. English 1 0 years B .A. American Literature,
distribution in terms of the relative complexity of rules offered as
M.A. ESL
choices. The rules were written by the researcher and subsequently
2. English 1 0 years B.A. Music, M.A.Linguistics,
presented to a group of 1 5 experienced teachers of ESL to rate fo r
M.Ed. Educational Foundations
complexity.
3. English 9 years B .A. Philosophy, M.A. Asian
It was decided that the format for identifying unfamiliar rules
Studies, M.A. ESL
adopted in the pilot study would also be used as the pretest of rule
4. English 20 years M.A. International Management,
knowledge for the present study. I have referred already to the
M.A. ESL
problems of using extensive pretests which consequently run the
5. English 1 5 years B.A. Asian Studies, M.A. ESL
risk of alerting subjects to the formal characteristics of the
6. English 6 years B .A. English Literature, M.A. ESL
structures to be presented during implicit, incidental and instructed
7. English 1 1 years B .A. Elementary Education,
conditions, and so invalidating claims about differences between the
M.A. ESL
conditions based on degree of rule awareness or consciousness of
8. English 3 years B.A. & M.A. American Literature,
form (see the review of studies by Hulstijn ( 1 990a) and Doughty
Ph.D English
( 1 988) in Chapter 1 ). The version of the pilot study used for the
9. Japanese 5 years B .A. American Literature,
pretest consisted of the first thirty items, to which subjects had t o
M.A. ESL
respond ' grammatical,' ' ungrammatical,' o r ' not sure ' . A s in the
1 0. English 8 years M.A. International Management
pilot study, during the pretest subjects were told to ignore what they
1 1. English 23 years B.A. English, M.A. ESL
thought may be punctuation or spelling errors and to focus on
12. English 1 4 years B .A. Speech & Drama,
grammaticality. ' Ungrammatical' responses to the two grammatical
R.S.A. Diploma T.E.F.L.
items subsequently identified as unfamiliar and differing in
13. English 9 years M.A. Japanese, M.A. ESL
complexity were used as the basis for selecting subjects for the
14. English 4 years M.B.A.
study. It was felt that responses to these items would be masked by
1 5. English 1 2 years M.A. American Literature
the task of making responses to other items in the pretest, and by
completing subsequent pretests of aptitude. Consequently it was felt
that this pretest would not alert subjects to the structures to be used
as exemplars of easy and hard rules.
The teachers were asked to rate each rule for complexity using a
2.4.2 Rating scale surveys of rule complexity scale from +3 for most complex, or hard, to -3 for least complex,
The rules written by the researcher, which were based on the eight or easy. Subsequently, after an interval of more than a week, the
items described above, are given in Appendix B. These rules, same teachers were asked to complete a Q-sort of the struct�res
together with instructions to rate each rule for the relative which were the focus of each of the eight rules. Each of the eight
complexity it would present to learners of ESL, were randomly structures was written on a separate card. The cards were randomly
44 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 45

ordered before being presented to each teacher. The teachers were


instructed to sort the cards in order of the difficulty they felt 7
learning each of the structures would pose a non-native speaker of 6
English, with the most difficult at the top, and the least difficult at
5
the bottom. The teachers were also told that they could indicate
equivalent difficulty by clipping cards together using paper clips. .5 4
This separate measure of the difficulty of the structures was then
compared with the difficulty ratings for each rule given using the
� 3
rating scale response. In this way the correlation of the judgments 2
of the difficulty elicited via the rating scale survey, and via the Q­ 1
sort was examined with a view to establishing the consistency of the -11- Eas
0 -1-!:::;:::::;==::r:==;::=..�r--.--.-�r---r-"""T"--ir--,
teachers' judgments. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 0 1 1 12 1 3 14 1 5
Teacher
2.4.3 The simple and complex rules identifiedfor the study
Following the procedure described above two rules were identified as
clearly distinct in complexity by teachers. These rules were also Figure 3. Results Of The Rating Scale Survey Of The
based on two of the items which the Japanese ESL learners had most Complexity Of Two Pedagogic Rules
consistently rated ungrammatical, item 1 0 (90% inaccurate) and
item 25 ( 1 00% inaccurate). The rules describing these structures
were therefore assumed to be potentially unfamiliar, and different
along a dimension of complexity. These rules are those for the 8
optional inversion of subject and verb following fronting cif
7
adverbials of direction (e.g., Into the house ran John or John ran),
and the more difficult rule for deriving pseudo-cleft sentences with a 6
focus on a prepositional phrase (e.g., Where the baby sleeps is in the 5
01!
bed not on the floor). =

Figure 3 gives the results of the rating scale survey of the � 4


complexity of these two rules, and Figure 4 gives the results of the � 3
Q-sort rating for the learning difficulty of the two sentences around 2
which the rules were based. As can be seen from Figure 3 , only one ___._ Hard structure
teacher failed to distinguish the two rules as differing in complexity. -II- Easy stncture
Scores for Figure 3 have been converted from the range -3 to +3 t o o ..i.--r--r--.--r---.---r---r--r--r--=;r==r=T=:;=T=,
a range from 1 to 7 , where 1 corresponds to -3, very easy, and 7 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 0 1 1 1 2 13 14 1 5
corresponds to +3, very hard. Teacher

Figure 4. Results Of The Q-Sort Of The Difficulty Of


Structures Described By The Pedagogic Rules
46 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 47

As can be seen from Figure 4, all teachers distinguish between the RULE 2
order of difficulty of the two structures described by the rules. Scores
for Figure 4 have been converted from the rank ordered Q-sort; a Some sentences tell about the locations of two things e.g.,
score of 8 indicates the structure was stacked on top and judged to Alice stands on the rif{ht and Judy stands on the lefi.
be the most difficult, and a score of 1 indicates it was stacked at the
bottom and judged to be the easiest. The formats for these rules We can change this sentence to focus on one of the locations e.g.,
presented to teachers to rate for complexity are given in Figures 5 Where Alice stands is on the rif{ht.
and 6 below, and the complete set of eight rule formats used in the
rating scale survey are given in Appendix B. To make sentences like these, first, choose the su�ject you want t o
emphasize, e.g., 'Alice', then place 'where' in front o f it:

RULE 1 Where Alice...

Some sentences contain a suqject a verb and a location phrase or a Next, follow the subject with the verb, e.g., ' stands' :
time phrase. Here are some examples:
Where Alice (su�iect) stands (verb) ...
Joan (subject) crashed (verb) into the wall. (location phrase)
Note that the verb cannot come before the subject:
John (subject) slept (verb) in the morninf{. (time phrase)
* Where stands (verb) Alice (su�ject) . . .

Both location and time phrases can be moved to the front of the * means ungrammatical
sentence:
Next add a singular form of the verb 'be' which agrees in tense
Into the ·wall Joan crashed. followed by the phrase describing the location of the subject:

On Tuesday morninf{ John slept. Where Alice stands is on the rif{ht.


Where Alice stood was on the rif{ht.
The verb can also come before the suqject, but only when location
phrases are at the front of the sentence: If the verbs do not agree in tense the sentence is ungrammatical:

Into the wall (location phrase) crashed (verb) Joan. (su�ject) * Where Alice stands was on the ri�ht.

* On Tuesday morninf{ (time phrase) slept (verb) John. (subject) For special emphasis, the second location can be contrasted with
* means ungrammatical the focus location by joining the locations using 'not'. This 'not' is
required. Without it the sentence is ungrammatical.

Figure 5. Format Of The Word Order Rule Of Focus Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.
Presented To Teachers To Rate For Complexity *Where Alice stands is on the right the left.

Figure 6. Format Of The Pseudo-Cleft Rule Of Focus


Presented To Teachers To Rate For Complexity
48 Second language rule complexity Second language rule complexity 49

in pseudo-clefts the canonical SVO word order is preserved (for


Into the house John ran. discussion of pseudo-cleft structures see Baker 1 9 8 9: 3 6 7-3 7 1 ;
Into the house ran John. Celce-Murcia & Larsen-Freeman 1 983 :404-408; Declerck 1 994;
Fletcher 1 980; Mccawley 1 988:59-6 1 ; Prince 1 978).
(- deletion - addition + permutation) The two rules can be distinguished, then, in that Rule 1 requires
/ knowledge of constraints on possible word order permutations, but
Figure 7. Elements Of The 'Easy' Word Order Rule Of Focus no knowledge of possible additions or deletions, while Rule 2
requires knowledge of constraints on additions and deletions, but n o
knowledge o f word order permutations. The word order
permutations described by Rule 1 are typical of those required at
The baby sleeps in the bed and the dog sleeps on the floor. Stage 3 (the final-to-initial movement of adverbials of time and
(Where) the baby sleeps (is) in the bed (not) on the floor. location), and Stage 4 (the optional internal-to-final movement o f
the subject to end position following fronting o f adverbials of
addition addition deletion addition location) of Pienemann, Johnston and Brindley's ( 1 98 8) proposed
+agreement +agreement -agreement taxonomy of word order stages for the acquisition of English. The
(location) (tense) constraints on addition of elements involved in Rule 2 requires
knowledge of agreement between the wh-word used and the focused
(+ addition + deletion - permutation) location phrase, knowledge of the contrasting location phrase which
is preceded by 'not', and knowledge of tense agreement between the
Figure 8. Elements Of The 'Hard' Pseudo-Cleft Rule Of two verbs used. The deletion constraint requires the learner t o
Focus identify and omit the subject and predicate o f the parallel, non­
focused clause (see Figure 8). These more complex sentence­
internal operations are characteristic of Pienemann' s et al. 's Stage
Once the easy and hard rules were identified, a number of post­ 5 structures (see Pienemann et al. 1 988 :224).
hoc comparisons and contrasts were made between them. Both of When asked to comment on why they had judged the two rules
these rules are rules of 'focus' . Celce-Murcia and Larsen-Freeman to differ in complexity, teachers frequently referred to the length of
( 1 983) note that pseudo-clefts in particular, like other rules o f the rules, (the easy rule was shorter), and the number of conditions
focus, chara�terize spoken rather than written language, "which on the rules, (the hard rule had a greater number of conditions). A
may explam _ number commented that it was possible to reduce the easy rule to a
m part why they have often been ignored in ESL/EFL
texts" 0 ? 83 :408). In the case of adverbial fronting (see Figure 7) 'rule of thumb,' (if place is at the front then subject and verb can
. switch positions, but if time is at the front they can't), whereas this
the rule mvolves constramts on permutation, or changes in word
order, of the type that allows a sentence-internal constituent (the was not possible with the hard rule. Many also commented that the
s�bj �ct, 'John' � to be optionally placed in sentence-final position. use of two verbs in the hard rule was unusual and possibly confusing,
S1m1lar adverbially conditioned constraints on subject-verb inversion as was the fact that the wh-word fronting the sentences had n o
have been observed in languages other than English (see Bresnan & interrogative function. The use o f these rules to motivate the
Kanerva 1 989). classification of sentence types presented in the training and
In contr�st, the pseu�o-cleft construction (see Figure 8) appears transfer sessions of the present study is described in detail in
to be specific to Enghsh. The rule involves constraints on the Chapter 5, sections 5 .3 .4-5 .
addition of items requiring sentence-internal agreement and
deletion of unnecessary items. However, in contrast to the r�le for
fronting adverbials of direction, which allows subject-verb inversion,
Chapter 3

Implicit And Explicit Information


Processing

3. 0 Introduction
The aim of this chapter is to address in greater detail the issue of
the relationship between noticing, consciousness and the operations
of implicit and explicit processes during language learning. I n
sections 3.2 and 3 .3 two causal variables in explanations o f
dissociations between implicit and explicit processes, the concepts
of attention and memory, are described and related to models o f
information processing and definitions o f consciousness i n
experimental psychology and SLA theory. I n particular the role o f
'noticing' i n filter theories and capacity theories o f attention is
described and related to three functional classifications of
attentional resources; 'alerting,' ' orienting' and 'detecting' . Various
type classifications of memory, 'episodic' versus ' semantic,'
'procedural' versus 'declarative,' and 'short-term' versus ' long­
term,' are then described, and the question of whether these are
indeed different systems, or simply functional distinctions, is
discussed. Following this a summary model of the relationship o f
attention to memory during implicit and explicit processing is
proposed. Finally, in section 3 .4 of this chapter the issue of
individual differences in language learning aptitude is considered ill
light of the proposed model.

3.1 Resources, information processing and consciousness


Schmidt's claim that awareness at the level of noticing is necessary
for converting L2 input to intake invokes, but does not explain in
detail, attentional mechanisms and their relationship to encoding
and retrieval from the various subsystems of memory that have
been proposed. Neither are these attentional mechanisms and
information processing relationships explained in detail in other
proposals which, following Schmidt, have attributed an important
role to noticing in L2 pedagogy (e.g., Ellis l 993a, 1 994b, 1 994c,
52 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 53

1 99?; Fotos 1 993 ; Fotos & Ellis 1 99 1 ; Long 1 994; Zalewski 1 99 3 ) . allocation, has rarely been invoked by SLA researchers, despite its
While th�re have b.een attempts to describe the relationship potential relevance to such current issues in SLA as task complexity
between mput and mtake at the neurophysiological level o f and grading in task-based approaches to syllabus design (Long &
explanation (Sato & Jacobs 1 992) there has been, to date, n o Crookes 1 992; Nunan 1 988, 1 989; Robinson 1 993b, 1 99 5 a;
attempt at a systems-level characterization of why attention is Robinson & Ross 1 996; Robinson, Ting & Urwin 1 995).
allocated to input under certain task conditions and not others. Part of the difficulty in motivating a theory of attentional
Attentional mechanisms, in Schmidt's view, are causal factors in mechanisms in SLA by drawing upon an accepted body of relevant
L2 learning, since they are responsible for allocating the cognitive findings from cognitive psychology research lies also in the recent
resou�ces . that lead to .noticing, and subsequent encoding in m emory. challenge to traditional information processing accounts o f
Schmidt is clear, as is Reber, about the necessity of attention to attention posed by more recent connectionist accounts.
input during both implicit and explicit learning. There are, though, Information processing models, such as that of Broadbent ( 1 9 5 8),
currently competing characterizations of the role of attention view attention as an executive process directing the serial passage of
during implicit information processing, with one recent explanation information between separate short-term and long-term memory
of implicit learning even claiming that the learning effects observed stores. In contrast, connectionist accounts dispute the modular
during a sequence learning task are due to a 'nonattentional' form metaphor for cognitive architecture that the information
of processing (Curran & Keele 1 993). processing views are based on, as well as the assumption of seriality,
Part of the difficulty in motivating an explanation of such arguing that executive attentional control is distributed throughout
mechanisms using current cognitive theory lies in the fact, as the entire processing system, in local patterns of neuronal
Baddeley ( 1 986:225) notes, that the study of attention has been excitation and inhibition, rather than in some central executive
d?minated by theories of the role of attention in perception and processor. Recent attempts to reconcile connectionist and control
visual processes, particularly signal detection and pattern architectures in the study of attention have been proposed (e.g.,
recognition. The role of attention in the control of memory and Schneider 1 993; Schneider & Detweiler 1 988) but are not yet widely
action, arguably areas of greater potential application to accepted.
descriptions of SLA processes, have, until recently, been less well
studied. Recent work on the memory/attention interface has 3.2 Attention
progressed considerably beyond the early multistore model of The concept of attention has three uses. It can be used to describe
m �m �ry and attentional control of Atkinson and Shiffrin ( 1 968), the processes involved in 'selecting' the information to be
reJectmg many of its fundamental assumptions (Rumelhart & processed and stored in memory. Dichotic listening tasks, for
McClelland 1 985), though as Cowan ( 1 988) notes, cognitive example, have been used to examine the fact that attention has a
psychology has yet to settle on an accepted view of the mutual variable focus, and can select information to be processed, to the
constraints imposed by memory and attention during information exclusion of other information (Cherry 1 953; Moray 1 959). It can
processing. Two established frameworks for describing skill be used to describe our ' capacity' for processing information.
development and performance, Shiffrin and Schneider's theory o f Studies of divided attention show that attention is capacity limited
automaticity ( 1 977) and Anderson's ACT* theory o f skill and that decrements in performance increase as the number of task
acquisition ( 1 983), have been heavily cited in the SLA literature dimensions, or components to be processed increase (Taylor,
(e.g., Ellis 1 993a, 1 993b; Faerch & Kasper 1 984; Kohonen 1 992; Lindsay & Forbes 1 967). Finally it can be used to describe the
McLaughlin 1 987; Mohle & Raupach 1 989; O'Malley & Chamot mental 'effort' involved in processing information. Pupillary
1 990; Robinson 1 989; Schmidt 1 992). However, more recent work dilation, for example, can be measured as a physical index of the
in the study of action (see Holding 1 989; Navon 1 984; Navon & degree of mental effort required in attending to increasingly
Gopher 1 ? 80; Schneider & Detweiler 1 988; Wickens 1 980, 1 9 84, complex tasks (Kahneman 1 973). Each of these uses has influenced
1 989) which has studied the role of attentional processes in skilled the development of theories of attention.
performance using the dual-task paradigm for examining attentional
54 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 5 5

3.2.1 Filter theories of attention concurrently are not excessive (see Figure 9). I n this respect
Early filter theories of attention were based on pipeline models o f capacity theories such as Kahneman's differ from filter theories,
information processing, in which information i s conveyed i n a fixed which characterize incoming stimuli as inevitably involved in
serial order from one storage structure to the next. In Broadbent's competition for limited resources.
( 1 95 8) 'bottleneck' model voluntary control of information
processing is exercised by a selective attention mechanism or filter
which selects information from a sensory register and relays it to a
Miscellaneous
detection device. Once past the selective filter, information is manifestalions
Arousal
analyzed for meaning, rather than for physical properties alone, of arousal

enters awareness, and is encoded in short-term memory. Treismah


( 1 964 ), based on evidence that subjects noticed their own name �
: Available :

when repeated in an unshadowed ear during dichotic listening tasks, ; capacity ;


Enduring .. ..
argued for a filter mechanism that was sensitive to semantic disposi tions

information, as well as sensory information, and against the


proposal that all information in an unselected channel is completely
tuned out, and unavailable for detection. Arguments against
Treisman's attenuated filter model are that the preattentive Momentary
---" ,
/
.
'
.----­
'

processing, or analysis before detection, it requires is too complete


I ' '
intentions ' I '

and resource demanding. Late selection theories (Norman 1 96 8 ;


Watanabe 1 980) propose that all information i s processed i n
01t�------���o
Possible activities
parallel, and enters working memory, where a decision is made about
the importance of the information. Information that is judged
important is elaborated or rehearsed; that which is judged
unimportant is not selected for elaboration and is forgotten. Responses

3.2.2 Capacity theories of attention (from Kahneman 1 973 : 1 0)


Underlying filter theories of attention and their associated
mechanisms of selective attention is the metaphor of a limited Figure 9. Kahneman's Capacity Model Of Attention
capacity channel, in which information competes for limited
attentional resources available to the passive processor. More
recent theories emphasize the voluntariness of the subject's control Wickens ( 1 980, 1 984, 1 989) has recently expanded Kahneman's
of attentional resources and the task specificity of decisions about view o f attentional resource allocation, arguing that rather than a
attentional allocation. The metaphor most suited to these theories single pool of resources, there are multiple pools. These pools
is that of attention as a spotlight with a variable focus, which can be occupy different points on three intersecting dimensions of resource
narrowed and intensified, or broadened and dissipated, as task systems. These are: (a) the dimension representing perceptual/
conditions demand. In Kahneman' s model ( 1 973) resources are cognitive activities versus response processes; (b) the dimension
allocated to incoming stimuli from a pool of cognitive resources representing processing codes required by analog/spatial activities
that varies as a function of the subject's state of arousal. Allocation versus verbal linguistic activities; and (c) the dimension representing
is divided between enduring predispositions (e.g., to recognize one ' s processing modalities e.g., auditory versus visual perception and
own name) and momentary intentions (e.g., to eavesdrop). Divided vocal versus manual responses (see Figure 1 0). Wickens ( 1 980,
attention does not necessarily lead to decrements in performance, 1 984, 1 989) argues that the attentional demands of tasks, and so
given sufficient arousal and that the demands of the tasks performed
56 Implicit and explicit information processing
Implicit and explicit information processing 57

their relative difficulty, will be increased when concurrently 3.2.3 Noticing and attentional theory
performed tasks draw simultaneously on the same pool of resources. Capacity theories, like those of Kahneman and Wickens, are
In the worst cases, like studying for two exams, interference effects properly seen as extensions of the late selection filter theory. I n
_
may m �ke time-sharing impossible and the attentional mechanism addition to mechanisms o f selective attention, they propose
of 'senal processing' will be adopted whereby separate task mechanisms for adjusting the deployment of attentional resources
compo!lents are completed in succession. In other cases where to suit the particular conditions of task demands. In this �ense more
'
there is less 'global similarity' between the tasks and so less recent attentional theory provides a framework for relatmg the act
'
resourc� competition, as in driving a car while talking, the of noticing to those task conditions which facilitate it. Noticing
m �chamsm o � parallel processing may be adopted. However, while following detection is more likely to occur in dual-task p erfor� an� e
this may avoid the need for time-swapping, degradations in the _
_ which draws on distinct, not identical resource pools, smce m this
quality of the attention allocated to both activities may lead t o case more attentional resources can be allocated to input processing.
poor performance. When tasks draw o n completely different pools It could be argued that Wickens' distinctions between the resource
of resources, or when one of the tasks is automatized then pools drawn on in task performance are still too co�rse grained t o
successf�l time-sharing and dual-task performance is possib le. I n allow a successful analysis of the workload and attentional demands
these ctrcu1!1stances the me�hanism of 'parallel processing' is of L2 tasks. Schneider and Detweiler ( 1 988) argue that mechanisms
alway� appl1ed .. However, Wickens notes, individuals may differ are insufficiently specified in Wickens' model, proposing a five
_
both m thetr time-sharing ability, and in their store of available phase model of skill acquisition which specifies the type of
resources : therefore individual differences, as well as task attention switching that occurs at each level.
_
charactenstics, may determine which of these two mechanisms of However, the dispute between cognitive theorists over the
attentional allocation is adopted. proper characterization of the mechanisms . used to . resolve
competing task demands is arguably one which is _ o ts1?e the

"""'"(11----
preserve of SLA theory construction per se, but ?ne which it m u�t
STAGE S
be informed by, and conceivably, in tum, contnbute to. What is
Central
necessary, I would suggest, at the current stage of theory
Encoding Processing Responding
' development in SLA, is a conceptual model for relating th� extent
_
t

Sootiol

R-------''<--..::....
Manual '?<"J',o
o�
to which subtle changes in task demands can affect the cond1t10ns of
implicit and explicit information processing. Th� ne�� for such a
i;! J'<"
model is prompted by the fact that the study of 1mp �1c1� proc�sses,
Visual Verbal Vocal

<t
0
J'� in laboratory settings like those of Reber ( 1 989), is mcreasmgly
0
acknowledging the fact that implicit processi?� is not a
:::<
I _
Auditory
homogeneous, undifferentiated alternative to exRl � c1t processm�,
but is itself susceptible to variance in task cond1t1ons, and their
�c 00
Spatial different processing consequences (Shanks & St. John 1 � 94;
� Whittlesea & Dorkin 1 993). It is prompted also by the observat10ns
J'�Vertiol
of Schmidt ( 1 990 : 1 43) that task demands are powerful determinants
of what gets noticed, and by speculation that certain L2 tasks may,
of their nature, facilitate noticing of different amounts, or aspects
(from Wickens 1 984: 69) of input (Ellis 1 995; Fotos & Ellis 1 99 1 ; Long 1 989; Loschky &
Bley-Vroman 1 993; Rankin 1 990; Robinson 1 995a; Robi�son et a !.
Figu re 1 0. Wic ken s' Mod el Of The
Stru ctur e Of Mul tiple 1 995). Wickens' model is at a sufficient level of generality that it
Reso urce s can allow the resource demands of both controlled laboratory tasks
and more complex classroom tasks to be categorized with a view to
58 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 59

identifying the extent to which such resource demands implicate, or in transfer effects in task performance. A trammg task making
create the conditions for implicit and explicit processes. similar demands to a transfer task will facilitate detection on the
latter, since the subject is oriented to the particular stimuli relevant
3.2.4 Alertness, orientation and detection to meet its demands, whereas differences in the processing demands
While theories of attention, then, have moved away from their of training and transfer tasks will lead to inhibition, or a relative
early preoccupation with problems of signal detection, and the delay in allocating attention to the detection of relevant stimuli.
question of 'where' selective attention occurs in relation to the
information processing sequence connecting the sensory register 3.2.5 Attention, awareness and detection
and short-term memory, there is still a need for a theory which It is clear that of the three functions of attention just described it is
identifies the mechanism whereby, and the point at which, selective detection that is most similar to what Schmidt terms ( 1 990)
attention takes place. Signal detection theory, acknowledging the noticing. Detection is responsible for encoding in memory, and
parallelism of input processing, has moved from unidimensional t o therefore is the attentional level at which Tomlin and Villa claim
multidimensional formats for statistically modeling this process (see ( 1 994: 1 92) learning must begin. However, Tomlin and Villa point
Cohen & Massaro 1 992; Kadlec & Townsend 1 992), though as out that detection does not necessarily· imply awareness. There can
Mandler ( 1 992) points out, purely connectionist accounts o f be detection without awareness (e.g., subliminally presented items
parallel input processing and representation are left without a can cause semantic priming, see Balota 1 983, 1 986; Cowan
mechanism for selectively directing attention to input, suggesting 1 988: 1 74). It appears to follow from Tomlin and Villa's definition
the need for "consciousness as an intervening limited serial of detection and awareness, then, that there can be learning without
process," functioning as a "gate between external information and awareness. This brings them, apparently, into conflict with Schmidt,
internal representations" ( 1 992 : 54). who claims conscious noticing is necessary for learning.
Tomlin and Villa ( 1 994), purposefully avoiding reference to These different positions can be reconciled if the concept o f
consciousness, argue that a fine grained analysis of the process of noticing i s defined to mean detection plus rehearsal i n short-term
detection that leads to noticing is also necessary for SLA theory, memory, prior to encoding in long-term memory, a view associated
and distinguish between the alerting, orienting and detection with a number of late selection theories of attentional allocation
functions of attention during the allocation of selective attention. (Cowan 1 988, 1 993; Norman 1 968). Although some have suggested
Alertness concerns an individual' s "general readiness to deal with that the contents of awareness and the contents of short-term
incoming stimuli or data" (Tomlin & Villa 1 994: 1 90). Warning storage are identical (e.g., Stem 1 985), the position illustrated in
signals, for example, can increase alertness or vigilance leading t o Figure 1 1 is that activation in short-term memory must exceed a
an increased likelihood o f signal detection. Orientation concerns the certain threshold before it becomes part of awareness (Cowan
allocation of resources based on expectations about the particular 1 988 : 1 65; Shiffrin 1 993 : 1 95), and further, that short-term memory
class of incoming sensory information, and involves activation of is the subset of long-term memory in a currently active state (see
some higher level schema or plan of action and events. During section 3 .3 . 1 ). In this way noticing can be identified with what is
detection a specific bit of information is engaged by attention. both detected and then further activated following the allocation of
Detection is more demanding of attentional resources and makes attentional resources from a central executive. Rehearsal (Baddeley
further processing of a stimulus possible at higher levels, such as 1 986:99) following detection would be a consequence of the
storage and rehearsal in short-term memory. allocation of resources to fulfill task demands, as, for example, in
Tomlin and Villa claim that alertness is of interest to SLA to the the capacity model of Wickens ( 1 989). The nature of rehearsal and
extent that learners must be ready to process information. elaboration would vary according to whether the task demanded
Orientation is of interest to the extent that prior experience m ay data-driven or conceptually driven processing (see Jacoby 1 9 8 3 ;
predispose learners to attend, for example, to form or meaning in Roediger, Weldon & Challis 1 9 89 :24, and section 3 .2.7). I n data­
processing a stimulus. In this sense orientation, and the related driven processing stimuli are encoded in "small pieces which are
processes of input facilitation and inhibition, are clearly implicated later assembled in working memory" (Best 1 992:76). In contrast,
60 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 6 1

conceptually driven processing involves more effortful integration claim, but such subliminal exposure effects are unlikely to have
of encoded stimuli within the context of surrounding stimuli, effects over intervals longer than a few hundred milliseconds, are
drawing on 'expectations or plans,' themselves the result of the rapidly lost from memory, and cannot in any useful sense be
activation of schemata in long-term memory. claimed to be evidence of learning (Bolender 1 986; Shanks & St.
These different types of processing are initiated in response t o John 1 994).
task demands, and affect the nature o f encoding. Roediger et al.
( 1 989) claim that the nature of the encoding required during study is (ii) A ttention. Both detection and noticing require attention.
a powerful predictor of performance during recognition tests, and Consequently, the view taken here is that there can be no learning,
that "memory tests benefit to the extent that the operations or encoding in memory, without attention. Although some have
required at test recapitulate or overlap the encoding operations described particular forms of learning as being non-attentional
performed during prior learning" ( 1 989: 1 6). They point out that (Curran & Keele 1 993), this description refers to the degree o f
effects supposedly demonstrating dissociations between attention directed at the stimulus, and such learning mechanisms are
performance on implicit and explicit memory tests can largely be properly considered less attention demanding, not attentionless.
explained by the fact that in implicit tests encoding and retrieval
demands are matched, whereas this is not the case in explicit tests (iii) Awareness. Awareness is critical to noticing, and
(see section 4.2 for further discussion). In the context of Figure 1 1 distinguishes it from simple detection. Noticing is a consequence o f
the roles of detection, attention and awareness in learning are now encoding in short-term memory, and is necessary for learning.
briefly summarized. What is noticed may be subsequently encoded in long-term episodic
memory. It is possible to briefly notice and forget, and to notice
and remember over time. More permanent encoding in long-term
memory is a consequence of the level of activation of information
Long termmemory in short-term memory, itself the result of rehearsal and elaboration.
.,,. ... - .... .... The nature of the rehearsal in short-term memory is a consequence
.. of the processing demands of particular tasks, and can involve data­
tection ,. '' , ,. noticing ' ' ,\ driven processing, and conceptually driven processing (see Jacoby

;_
ttention : +attention & rehearsal -> ', - > elaboration 1 983). Data-driven processing involves simple maintenance
' :"<.. rehearsal of instances in memory. Conceptually driven processing
-•worene� • +owo�e� : / involves elaborative rehearsal and the activation of schemata or
higher order relations from long-term memory which are used t o

�-;;;J
organize instances into more abstract configurations and can be
induced, for example, by the instruction to search for rules
underlying a sequence of stimuli. Measures of awareness are difficult
to operationalize given that (i) the experience of noticing may be
fleeting and thus difficult to recall, and that (ii) one may be aware
of, yet unable to verbalize or otherwise articulate the nature of that
Figure 1 1 . Noticing As Detection With Awareness In Short­ which one is aware of.
Term Memory
3.2. 6 Attentional and non-attentional learning versus encoding
specificity
(i) Detection. Detection requires attention. There is evidence The position taken here is that all learning requires attention.
that detection can occur without awareness, as Tomlin and Villa Curran and Keele ( 1 993) claim that performance on a sequence
learning task using a dual-task methodology reveals effects for a
62 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 63

non-attentional form of learning. Their results can be explained by (ii) Attention and awareness. It is clear, though, from Curran and
invoking the notion of encoding specificity and the distinction Keele's discussion that both forms of learning require attention. It is
between data-driven and conceptually driven processing, both less clear that the two forms of learning they identify are
mediated by attention, illustrated in Figure 1 1 . differentially dependent on awareness. Awareness was assessed as the
ability to verbalize the rules of the sequence in answer to a question
(i) Single-task training and dual-task transfer. Curran and Keele about whether they noticed or could describe a pattern underlying
( 1 993) used a single-task training to dual-task transfer paradigm for the stimulus presentations. This is precisely the awareness induced
examining the influence of attention on the learning of sequences by the task instructions for the instructed group, and the
of stimuli (the letter X) displayed in numbered quadrants of a consequence of the interpretation of the task made by the aware
computer screen. One group were shown the rules regulating the non-instructed group. But awareness may also be of the frequency of
presentation of stimuli in these numbered quadrants, another group repetition of co-occurring stimuli. This may not qualify as a
were not. Subsequently, subjects in the non-instructed group were pattern, or rule, and so subjects may have failed to report it, but it
divided into aware and unaware groups on the basis of their ability to could still have contributed to performance, and such co­
correctly identify the nature of the - rules regulating the presented occurrences could have been consciously noticed. This type o f
sequences. The finding was that learning, as measured in decreases in awareness was not assessed by Curran and Keele, but it is precisely
reaction times, was better for instructed learners, and for aware non­ the type of awareness one could expect the learners in the non­
instructed learners than for unaware non-instructed learners in attentional mode to have had. Shanks and St. John ( 1 994) make the
single-task conditions. All groups showed evidence of learning, in same point: "If subjects have learned something other than rules,
the sense of improved reaction times over the period of single-task then asking them about rules may lead to erroneous conclusions. O n
training. However, when a secondary task was added, counting high the other hand, i f we ask the subjects questions about what they did
pitched tones occurring during each trial of the transfer session, the in fact learn, we may get reasonable answers" ( 1 994:427).
performances of the two groups were at the same level, though Awareness, that is, is a function of the interpretation of the nature
again the performances of all groups showed evidence of learning, in of the encoding and retrieval processes required by the task, and
the sense of relatively faster reaction times compared to initial both forms of awareness I have described are likely to have
practice. Curran and Keele argue that this is evidence of two forms accompanied performance by subjects in the experiment (cf.
of learning, an attentional form, displayed by the instructed and Schmidt 1 995; Stadler 1 995). The problem lies in the insensitivity
aware non-instructed learners under single task conditions, and non­ of Curran and Keele's measure of awareness.
attentional learning, displayed by the non-aware subjects under
single task conditions. The attentional form is degraded by the (iii) Encoding specificity and awareness. Rather than invoking
attentional demands of the secondary task, but the non-attentional two distinct, non-interfaced forms of learning, one could invoke the
form is not. Since the instructed and aware non-instructed subjects distinction between data-driven and conceptually driven processing,
showed some evidence of learning in the dual-task transfer phase, as a consequence of task demands, to account for Curran and
Curran and Keele argue that this is the result of a switch to the non­ Keele's results. Data-driven processing requires accumulation and
attentional form of learning that had been occurring in parallel with rehearsal of instances encountered in the input in memory, and m ay
the attentional form in the single-task training sessions. Curran and lead to the development of simple patterns of association, or the
Keele conclude that this is evidence for two distinct, non-interfaced, strength of dependency between co-occurring items. Conceptually
parallel forms of learning. They thus· differ from Hayes and driven processing requires the elaboration of input following
Broadbent ( 1 988) who claim that selective, attention demanding, activation of schemata for, in this case, sequence relations, and the
and unselective, less attention demanding forms of learning attendant rehearsal of more abstract patterns of hierarchical
compete with each other, and are applied serially. organization. Both require awareness, though the nature of what is
noticed during processing will be relative to the interpretation
placed on the task by the learner.
64 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 65

3.2. 7 Summary of the role of attention resonance imaging and evoked brain potential (Cotman & Lynch
Four . poi �ts can . now be made about the model illustrated in Figure 1 989; Squire & Zola-Morgan 1 99 1 ; Thompson 1 986), which appear
_ the consciousness hypothesis of Schmidt
1 1 . ( 1) It is consistent with to lend firmer support to previously speculative distinctions
( 1 990) which claims there is no learning without awareness at the between memory systems. Secondly, a rapidly expanding literature
level of noticing. (ii) It is consistent with one interpretation o f on dissociations between conscious awareness and performance o n
claims by Reber ( 1 989) and Krashen ( 1 98 l a, 1 982) that learning is 'implicit' versus ' explicit' memory tests has been interpreted as
!he result of both explicit and implicit information processing; this supporting the claim that the separate systems support functionally
IS th �t explici� and implicit information processing require differentiated forms of learning. This literature is critically reviewed
conscious attention to form at input, but that implicit information in Chapter 4. The remainder of this chapter is devoted to a review
processing is data-driven and results in the accumulation o f of the separate memory systems that have been proposed, the
in�tance � , whereas explicit information processing is conceptually extent of their supposed implication in implicit and explicit
driven, mvolvmg _ access to schemata in long-term memory. (iii) information processing, and a summary of the evidence for
The . n_iodel aJlows little, if any influence (outside of fleeting individual differences in L2 learning ability as a function of the
subhmmal exposure effects) of nonconscious information availability of cognitive resources like memory and attention.
processing on the accumulation of L2 knowledge. (iv) It is
consistent with the position that dissociations in the extent o f 3.3.1 Short-term and long-term memory
knowledge, and awareness o f knowledge arising during learning, are Most current models of memory assume the need for a functional
consequences of the particular encoding and retrieval operations distinction between short-term and long-term storage (Raaijmakers
requi�ed by particular tasks (Green & Shanks 1 993 ; Jacoby 1 98 3 ; & Shiffrin 1 992), although whether this is represented as a
Roediger et al. 1 989), which implicates the operation of a central distinction between separate systems and stores (Shiffrin 1 993), or
executive attentional mechanism allocating limited resources t o whether the separate storage functions take place within a single
satisfy known task demands (Whittlesea & Dorken 1 993), rather interconnected workspace (Baddeley & Hitch 1 993 ; Carlson, Khoo,
than the operation of two distinct, consciously and unconsciously Yaure & Schneider 1 990) is currently a matter of dispute.
accessed systems. Proceduralist views of memory incline to the latter position, and
are evolutions of Hebb's views ( 1 949) that one mechanism for the
3.3 Memory storage of memories "was the continued activity, or reverberation,
The debate about the role of consciousness in L2 learning has, to of the cells and cell assemblies recruited by a perceptual act"
date, involved considerable speculation about the role of attention (Crowder 1 993 : 1 42), and that should this reverberation continue
in information yrocessing, but little discussion of the memory long enough, a mechanism causing structural changes in the neural
systems underlymg such processing. Possibly this neglect is due t o units would lead to indefinite memory. Versions of proceduralist
the desire by contemporary theorists and practitioners alike t o positions include those of Cowan ( 1 993), that short-term memory
disso�iate t�emselves from the audio-lingual approach to language is the subset of long-term memory in a current state of activation,
teachmg which placed a heavy emphasis on memorization and rote and the connectionist/control architecture of Schneider ( 1 993 ).
learning of patterns. It may also be due to the sense, perhaps Even those who argue for a more modular approach (Shiffrin 1 99 3)
confirmed by the discussion so far, that descriptions of memory agree that the early modal model of Atkinson and Shiffrin ( 1 96 8),
should properly take place within the context of a theory o f which proposed distinct, serially connected, short and long-term
attention and its role in information processing. This may be true, stores, with separate phonological and semantic codes respectively,
but there are reasons for thinking that discussions of memory are is wrong. All are in agreement about three basic properties of short­
due to receive more prominent treatment in SLA theory than they term storage: (i) it involves temporary activation of neural
have to date. Firstly, more extensive evidence of the structures; (ii) it is the site of control processes such as directing
neu�ophysiological substrata ?f memory are currently becoming focal and peripheral attention, rehearsing current information and
available, as a result of experimental methodologies like magnetic coding new inputs; and (iii) it is capacity limited. As such short-term
66 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 67

memo� "serves as the interface between everything we know and initial performance is limited by lack of knowledge, not simply
everythmg we can see or do" (Cowan 1 993 : 1 66), and is traditionally availability of attentional resources in a capacity-limited short-term
thought of as the workspace where skill development begins memory, and that automaticity is the result of an increasing long­
(Anderson 1 983 ; Baddeley 1 986; Shiffrin & Schneider 1 977) and term knowledge base which is eventually drawn on as the basis for
where knowled�e is encoded into and retrieved from long-term automatic responses. Logan's ' instance' theory rests on three main
memory. For this rea�on meas�res of short-term memory capacity assumptions: (i) stimuli are obligatorily encoded in memory once
_
have been used as mdices of aptitude for skill learning and cognitive attended to; (ii) each instance together with whatever aspects of the
deve �opment in SLA (e.g., Carroll 1 993 ; Carroll & Sapon 1 95 9 ; context were attended to is stored separately; and (iii) retrieval o f
Harrmgton & Sawyer 1 992; Sasaki 1 993) and in a wide variety o f the memories i s an obligatory consequence o f attending to similar
other areas of educational research (e.g., Ackerman 1 988; Kyllonen stimuli. Together these assumptions imply a learning mechanism­
& Christal 1 990). the accumulation of instance representations. Memory becomes
T�o current areas of memory research, however, imply that the stronger not through strengthening (Cohen, Dunbar & McClelland
domam of the control processes in short-term memory may not be 1 990; Schneider & Detweiler 1 98 8), but because "each experience
as extensive as has traditionally been claimed. The first of these lays down a trace that may be recruited at the time of retrieval"
challenges the traditional role attributed to short-term memory in (Logan 1 988a:494 ). As memory accumulates so retrieval becomes
the development of automatic processing; the second challenges its faster, until the speed of decisions about task performance made o n
role in controlling access to long-term memory. the basis o f retrieval from memory exceeds the speed o f decisions
made by a general algorithm. At this point memory-based solutions
3.3.2 A utomaticity and memory become the basis of automatic responses.
Automatic processes are often cited as classic examples of Instance theory, therefore, raises questions about previous views
the
operatio?s of implicit systems (Reber 1 993 ). The properties of automaticity which placed the responsibility for developing
of
auto�atic processes are that they are fast, effortless, obliga automaticity on the speed up of processes under attentional
. tory,
consistent, unconscious and learned. Traditional resource supervision in short-term memory. These questions are raised by
based
theories of automaticity and skill acquisition, such as Ander three aspects of Logan's proposals: (i) the parallelism of memory­
son's
ACT* theor?' , or Shiffrin and Schneider's theory (see McLa based and algorithm-based processing; (ii) the obligatoriness o f
ughlin
1 987; Schmidt 1 992) locate the mechanisms which are respon encoding into memory; and (iii) the obligatoriness of retrieval from
sible
for the development of the learned responses drawn on in autom memory. Firstly, instance theory claims automatic, implicit
atic
skilled performance in short-term memory. In Shiffrin information processing does not follow consecutively from an
and
Schneider's theory ( 1 977; cf. McLaughlin et al. 1 983), contro explicit learning phase of controlled processing, but develops in
lled
processes activate sequential and temporary links in short-term parallel with it, and is to a large extent independent of it. Secondly,
memory. Following practice in consistent environment the assumption of obligatory encoding of attended stimuli appears
s
strengthening of corresponding links in long-term memory occurs
Eventually automatic processes, requiring little attent ional
'. to allow some role for memory without awareness in the
development of automaticity, since there can be detection o f
�upervisory contr� l, activate permanent links in long-term memory attended stimuli which operates outside o f awareness and which does
m parallel each time exposure to a particular pattern of inputs not involve noticing (see Figure 1 1 ) . However, it appears that such
occurs. memories would not contribute much to the knowledge base drawn
Logan ( 1 98 8a, 1 988b, 1 990) has argued that modal resource on in automatic responses, since the quality of attention allocated
based theories of automaticity merely describe the pheno mena at encoding is, for Logan, directly implicated in the speed o f
associated with autom aticity, but do not explain 'why' practice retrieval from memory: "Attention to an item will have some
in
constant � nvi:onments is necessary to its development, or 'why' impact on memory; it does not assume that all conditions o f
. _ by a gradua
automattcity is accompamed l withdrawal of attent ion. attention produce the same impact" ( 1 988a:494). Consequently,
Memory-based theories of automaticity like those of Logan claim noticing of instances appears to be necessary to establish a
68 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 69

knowledge base which can prompt retrieval with sufficient speed t o an unconsciously accessed implicit memory (Schacter 1 98 9 ;
outperform algorithm-based processing as the basis o f performance. Schacter & Tulving 1 982; Squire & Cohen 1 984). The argument for
Thirdly, the obligatoriness of retrieval also implies a process that dissociable subsystems of memory has recently been invoked by
develops in parallel with algorithm-based processing in the early Paradis ( 1 994) to explain aspects of L2 development, and it is
stages of skill development, but is not dependent on the attentional implied, if not specified in detail in the Monitor model of Krashen
control processes in short-term memory. ( 1 979, 1 9 8 l a, 1 982). The evidence for and against such
dissociations is reviewed in detail in Chapter 4. Here explanations of
3.3.3 Access to long-term memory dissociations that have been proposed and their implications for
Claims that memory without awareness may play a more significant models of the architecture of memory are briefly considered.
role in cognitive processes than has hitherto been acknowledged
have also come from studies of apparent dissociations between 3.3.3.1 Activation views
rary
perfo�mance . on recognition and recall tasks requiring direct, Activation views hold that priming effects are due to the tempo
entatio ns and are based on
conscious retneval of material presented during a study phase, and activation of pre-existing semantic repres
ying the develo pment of
indirect tasks which facilitate retrieval of the material presented at theories o f the processes underl
study, but without apparent conscious attempts to recall (Merickle automaticity.
& Reingold 1 989; Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork 1 988; Schacter
1 987, 1. 989; . schacte.r, . Chiu & Ochsner 1 993). Repetition priming (i) Auto-associative semantic activation. Graf and Mandler
effects m lexical decision tasks, for example, have been claimed t o ( 1 984) claim that implicit memory performance is due t o
provide evidence for the use o f implicit memory. I n these tasks temporary activation o f a representation that occurs automatically,
subjects are presented with letter strings and asked to judge if they but which carries with it no contextual information which could
are words or non-words. Implicit memory is inferred from the contribute to explicit remembering. Presentation of a word in a
reduced latency, or speed of responses to previously presented words study list activates its representation, thus making it more accessible
compared to novel words. The claim that implicit and explicit than non-studied words. However, this activation "is not sufficient
memory are dissociated is based on the observation that to determine recognition or recall operations, which are dependent
manipulating one variable during the study phase will affect on the relationships of the event to be retrieved and the
p�rformance on the later implicit and explicit test phases characteristics of the context in which this event first occurred"
differently. For example, during the study phase, task instructions (Besson, Boaz, Fischler & Raney 1 992:90). Activation theories like
may be varied to focus attention on the semantic aspects of the that of Graf and Mandler thus propose a mechanism, spreading
studied words, or on the physical features of the words, thereby activation, which is variably related to the processing demands � f
manipulating differences in the depth of processing (Craik & tasks, which can either create the conditions for such automatic
Lockhart 1 972). This manipulation is found to affect performance access, or lead to more attentionally demanding processing which
on explicit tests, which are facilitated by the greater depth o f impedes it (see the processing level explanations given below).
processing required by focusing on semantics, but not to affect
performance on implicit tests. In tests using amnesics, with damage (ii) Similarity based recruitment and activation. Logan ( 1 990)
to the hippocampus, it has been found that repetition priming also offers an automatic activation explanation of repetition
effects are preserved on implicit tests, relative to normal priming effects. In lexical decision tasks judgments about wor�s or
populations, while performance on tasks requiring explicit recall non-words are made initially on the basis of an algonthm.
from previously presented word lists is impaired (Vallar & Shallice Subsequently, when the same words are presented a second or third
1 990). time, decisions are made on the basis of single-step access t o
Such dissociations have been used by some to support the previous encounters with the stimulus which are activated i n
argument that two distinct subsystems are responsible for these memory. However, Logan is clear that instances are encoded i n
effects-an explicit memory, accessed by conscious retrieval, and memory together with whatever contextual coordinates happen to
70 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 7 1

be attached to them. Consequently, at least some episodic, personal draw on integrative processes whereas explicit tests draw o n
memories could be activated by obligatory retrieval of instances. elaborative processes.
The whole context of that which was attended is thus used as the
basis for computing the similarity of instances encoded during a 3.3.3.3 A-fulti]Jle systeltlS views
study phase to those attended during a test phase. Some researchers have attributed differences in performance o n
implicit and explicit tests to differences in the properties o f
3.3.3.2 Processing views underlying memory systems which are differentially activated by
Processing views argue that the nature of the match between the attempts at explicit, conscious recall. The difference between this
processing demands encouraged by the task instructions at study and type of explanation and the preceding explanations lies in the
at test contributes to differences in performance on tests. preference of researchers for associating functionally differentiated
Activation views and processing views are, to an extent, evidence of memory performance with system level
complementary, as I will argue in section 3 .3 .4 below. characterizations of the architecture of memory. Processing views
prefer to avoid this and explain functional differentiations at the
(i) Data-driven and conceptually driven processing. Jacoby level of task demands. Multiple systems views often refer to
( 1 983) has argued that conceptually driven processing, such as neurophysiological evidence of different patterns of brain activity,
organizing and reconstructing, which is 'subject initiated' and guided and neural architecture, to support their claims for different
by knowledge and expectations from long-term memory, can be subsystems. Taken to an extreme this approach ceases to explain
distinguished from data-driven processing, which is initiated and data, since each time performance on memory tasks appears to be
guided by data, for example the shapes of letters (see also Blaxton functionally differentiated, a new subsystem of memory is proposed
1 989; Hamann 1 990; Roediger & Blaxton 1 987; Roediger et al. to account for it (McKoon, Ratcliff & Dell 1 986; Richardson­
1 989). Study tasks encourage a blend of both forms of processing, Klavehn & Bjork 1 992; Tulving 1 985). Nonetheless, a number o f
but task instructions can bias the subject to performing more of one distinctions between subsystems of memory have attracted support
than the other. Task instructions in the test phase are also claimed among some memory researchers.
to bias subjects to draw on one or the other form of processing;
explicit tests draw primarily on conceptually driven processing, and (i) Procedural and declarative memory. Squire and Cohen
implicit tests draw primarily on data-driven processing. What ( 1 9 84) distinguish between consciously accessed declarative
distinguishes test performance is the transfer of appropriate memory, and unconscious procedural memory, the former
processing strategies initiated during the study phase of memory responsible for performance on explicit tests, the latter for
tests to the transfer test. Implicit test performance reflects study­ performance on implicit tests. This explanation is inspired by
test overlaps in data-driven processing; explicit test performance neurophysiological evidence of the relation of brain impairments t o
reflects study-test overlaps in conceptually driven processing. memory functions. Combining studies o f human amnesia with
Different memory systems are not necessarily involved. animal models of human amnesia, Squire and Zola-Morgan ( 1 99 1 )
have identified the medial temporal lobe as the site of memory
(ii) Integrative and elaborative memory processes. Graf and functions. The medial temporal lobe binds together distributed
Ryan ( 1 990) also take a processing perspective, and distinguish storage sites in the hippocampus and neocortex. The role of the
integrative from elaborative processing. Integrative processing medial temporal lobe in distributing information in neocortex is
"results from processing that bonds the features of a target into a only short-lived, and after time memories stored in neocortex
coordinated whole," whereas elaborative processing "associates a become independent of the medial temporal lobe. Squire and Zola­
target with other mental contents" ( 1 990:990). Following the same Morgan interpret this as evidence of the neurophysiological basis o f
reasoning as Schacter, they claim study task performance declarative memory for facts. The medial temporal lobe functions
encourages a blend of both types of memorization, but implicit tests as a short-term organizer of information in long-term storage in
neocortex. Lesions to the hippocampus and neocortex surrounding
72 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 73

the amygdala produce memory impairments, while lesions to the of knowledge compilation ( 1 983). However, Anderson's ACT*
amygdala do not, suggesting that the amygdala is responsible, in theory does not propose that episodic and semantic memory are
part, for the memories that are preserved in amnesics-the ability separable systems within declarative memory. Rather than motivate
to make associative links between stimuli and sensory modalities his procedural/declarative distinction with reference to
and �ther fu�ctions important to skill development and procedural neurophysiological evidence alone, or with reference to a model o f
learmng (Sqmre, Amaral & Press 1 990). At present, evidence from information processing during skill acquisition, Tulving, like Reber
neurophysiology is suggestive of, though far from clear evidence ( 1 993 ), has claimed that distinctions between memory systems are
for, the claim that different aspects of memory functioning are the result of evolutionary adaptive processes. Episodic memory is a
centralized in different sites. Impairments in performance o n late arrival, and phylogenetically derived from semantic memory.
explicit tests o f recognition and recall appear to b e linked t o Like Reber, Tulving claims that ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny,
impairments of .the hippocampal system; performance o n implicit thereby explaining why in early childhood children are able t o
memory tests 1s not affected by such impairments. Studies o f perform on indirect measures o f memory, which, he claims, access
differences in synaptic plasticity within anatomically distinct sites the semantic system, and only later are able to perform on direct
have also been identified and used to support claims for different measures which access the episodic system.
forms of memory (Gluck & Granger 1 993 :668).
3.3.4 Multiple systems and multiple explanations
(ii) Episodic and semantic memory. Tulving ( 1 983, 1 985, 1 986) As Schacter ( 1 987) points out, there are advantages and
has developed a multiple systems theory of memory which disadvantages to each of these explanations, and they are not all
distinguishes between episodic memory for personal involvement mutually exclusive. One major disadvantages of multiple systems
with an event, and semantic, decontextualised memory. This views, however, is that they are inconsistent with each other. Note,
distinction has been used in explanations of dissociations between for example, the inconsistency between the use of procedural and
implicit and explicit memory (Schacter 1 9 89). The episodic declarative to describe memory systems in the work of Squire and
memory system is argued to be responsible for performance o n Zola-Morgan ( 1 99 1 ), Tulving ( 1 986) and Anderson ( 1 983),
tasks that require explicit remembering o f recent events, while described above. Multiple explanations underlie the multiple systems
semantic memory is argued to be responsible for performance o n that have been proposed. If confusion is to be avoided with regard
tests that require access to pre-existing decontextualised semantic to the role of key variables like attention and consciousness, the use
representations. Episodic memory deals with, " unique, concrete, of the same pair of terms to indicate a contrast between memory
personal and temporally dated events, " while semantic memory mechanisms must be made with reference to the assumptions of the
involves "general, abstract, timeless knowledge that a person shares specific explanation they are based on. It is clear, for example, that
with others" ( 1 986:307). Tulving' s multiple memory model is explanations of forms of memory and their relationship to learning
hierarchical. Episodic memory is seen as a specialized subsystem o f differ with respect to the role of consciousness. For Anderson
semantic memory, accessed by autonoetic, intentionally ( 1 983 ), skill learning begins with conscious declarative knowledge
manipulated, consciousness. Both episodic and semantic systems which is later proceduralised. This is an interface position. For
deal with declarative knowledge. Declarative memory is viewed as a Cohen ( 1 9 84) and Squire and Zola-Morgan ( 1 99 1 ), consciously
separate system embedded within a procedural memory. This accessed memory and unconsciously accessed memory are
distinction, then, is in some ways similar to the dissociated and distinct forms which are responsible for different
declarative/procedural distinction made above. However, it differs kinds of learning.
from the accounts of Cohen ( 1 984) and Squire and Zola-Morgan Confusions may also arise through failure to consider the
( 1 99 1 ) by viewing declarative memory as nested within procedural different levels at which each explanation is pitched. Claims for
memory, rather than viewing them as separate co-existing systems distinctions between procedural and declarative memory based o n
functioning in parallel. . It is also similar to the neurophysiological evidence may be true at that level, but they are
procedural/declarative distinction made in Anderson's ACT* model interacting systems at the level of information processing during
74 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 75

task performance (Bunge 1 973 ; Robinson 1 993b). The question o f 3.3. 6 Summary of the role of memory
whether the function o f different memory systems can be isolated In summary, this review of the nature of the relationship of
in task performance by amnesics and monkeys with lesions to the memory to attention during information processing leads to the
hippocampus is, to a large extent, irrelevant to the question of how following conclusions. (i) There is minimal evidence of encoding
they interact and contribute to learning during task performance by into long-term memory without awareness-what evidence there is,
normal L2 learners (though see Robbins 1 992, and Paradis 1 994, reviewed in section 3 .2.6, is largely irrelevant to the debate over the
who base large claims for L2 learning and pedagogy on this nature of implicit memory. By far the larger part of this debate, and
evidence). Of interest to the information processing level o f the issue of the role of attentional control processes in short-term
explanation is a close examination o f the attentional demands o f memory, concerns evidence of memory retrieval without awareness.
tasks, and the extent to which manipulations o f these demands can (ii) Short-term memory should be distinguished from long-term
lead to differences in measures of learning and retention. memory, adopting the definition that short-term memory is that
subset of long-term memory in a currently activated state. (iii)
3.3. 5 A utomaticity, retrieval and context instantiation Retrieval from long-term memory can be a consequence o f
An alternative to inferring two distinct forms of memory, then, conceptually driven processing under attentional control in short­
would be to reconcile an explanation based on automatic activation term memory, as in explicit memory tasks. When this occurs the
of pre-existing representations, as in Logan's instance theory, with contents of retrieval are determined by the interaction o f
an explanation based on processing distinctions between conceptually driven processing and the specific nature o f task
conceptually driven and data-driven processes. In this account, demands. (iv) There can be automatic activation of previously
explicit instructions to remember are assumed to lead subjects t o attended information encoded into long-term memory. When this
associate each currently attended target with each previously occurs the contents of retrieval are determined by the interaction of
experienced target, and additionally to involve comparing the data-driven processing and the nature of the specific task demands.
currently attended context of recall with the whole of the (v) This position is inconsistent with the view that separate l � ng­
previously experienced situational context of study. This requires term memory systems are differentially responsible for consc10us
conceptually driven processing, and Graf and Ryan's closely related and unconscious information access during informatic . 1 <irocessing
elaborative memory strategy, rather than data-driven processing (Paradis 1 994; Schacter 1 989; Tulving 1 9 86). (vi) H is consistent
alone. However, while a currently attended target may find a with the view that level of attentional awareness dunng retrieval is a
perceptual match with a previously encoded target, the surrounding function of task demands and automatic processes, which jointly
situational context differs, and context is coded as a part of instance determine access to a single long-term memory store.
representation in Logan's theory (Logan & Etherton 1 994). For
this reason, single step access to a matching instance may be slower 3.4 Individual differences, coguitive resources, and L2 acquisition
in situations requiring explicit recall, than in situations which do The nature of the interaction between cognitive resources during
not, like implicit tests, since the currently attended contextual information processing and language learning is, as yet, little
coordinates of the stimuli will not find a satisfactory match. In understood. Nonetheless, a certain number of predictions follow
implicit tests recognition is data-driven, and differences in the from Schmidt's claim that noticing is necessary to language
study-test contexts are not invoked by task demands. Therefore learning, and from the description of the relationship of attention
access may be more automatic, since instances find a clearer match. to memory during implicit and explicit information processing
This hybrid 'task-based' position would have the virtue of which has been described above.
accommodating two sources of implicit memory which Richardson­
Klavehn and Bjork ( 1 988) argue need to be accounted for "ahistoric 3.4.1 Individual differences and conscious learning
traces that depend on pre-existing codified representations, and Firstly, short-term memory capacity should be important, since this
historic traces that incorporate contextual information" determines how much can be noticed, and subsequently rehearsed,
( 1 988:53 3), without the need to invoke separate systems. while performing a task. It should be related to differential
76 Implicit and explicit information processing Implicit and explicit information processing 77

performance on single tasks, when resources in the L2 are limited was therefore more directly transferable to L2 processing than long­
and skills are relatively non-automatic, and to a greater extent to term memory capacity. Cook ( 1979) extended the research to child
performance on dual-tasks which require time-sharing and attention L2 learners, finding greater ability on digit span measures of short­
switching. Success in workload management, using Wickens' model term memory than on recall of words, and greater ability in both o f
(see Figure 1 0), should be most clearly related to short-term these when the digit span and word list were presented i n the L 1 .
memory capacity when concurrently performed tasks draw on Short-term memory for sentences was tested by presenting subjects
similar resource pools. Secondly, when tasks require conceptually with L2 sentences of between four and sixteen words in length and
driven processing, the availability of knowledge schemas to organize then asking for recall. Performance was significantly worse than
perception, and direct attention to relevant aspects of the stimulus when the sentences were presented to adults in their L 1 .
domain to be learned, should also be important. This prediction, Papagno et al. ( 1 99 1 ) examined the influence of a proposed
then, claims that the extent to which relevant pre-existing subsystem of short-term working memory, the phonological store,
representations are available will determine the efficiency of on adult acquisition of L2 vocabulary. Phonological storage was
attentional allocation, which in turn will lead to more successful disrupted by articulatory suppression of subvocal rehearsal. They
task performance. contrasted performance on paired-associate tasks in the L I ,
English, and in the L2, using pairs of English and Italian, Russian
3.4.2 I,ndividual differences and unconscious acquisition and Finnish words, finding that this disrupted the learning o f
It should be noted, however, that those who claim SLA is a largely Finnish-English and Italian-English paired-associates, but not
unconscious process (Krashen 1 985), or who claim that learning Russian-English or English-English. They speculate that the formal
without awareness is a more robust and extensive form of learning similarity of word structure in English and Russian was greater than
than that which arises as a consequence of explicit instruction to for the other two languages and this allowed subjects to circumvent
look for rules or to apply consciously learned rules to examples of the use of rehearsal in phonological short-term memory in learning
relevant structures (Abrams & Reber 1 988; Reber 1 989, 1 993), the associates.
argue that ultimate attainment in second language, or artificial Harrington and Sawyer ( 1992) examined the effect of differences
grammar learning is likely to be unrelated to currently available in L2 reading skill and working memory capacity, using a measure
measures of cognitive abilities. These, it is argued, measure only the developed by Daneman and Carpenter ( 1 980) for assessing reading
abilities drawn on in explicit information processing, where span. They found that subjects with larger measures of working
individual differences can be expected. Implicit learning, or memory capacity scored higher on tests of reading skill. In contrast,
acquisition, which are independent of conscious efforts to learn, are reading skill did not appear to correlate highly with more passive
abilities which are relatively homogeneous in the population, and measures of short-term storage such as backward digit span or
unrelated to short-term memory capacity or the ability t o random word span.
consciously divide and maintain attention. These studies lend some support to the claim, consistent with
Schmidt's noticing hypothesis, that short-term memory capacity is
3.4.3 Short-term memory capacity and learning a predictor of success in performing L2 tasks. Carroll ( 1 97 1 , 1 99 3 ;
The relationship of short-term memory capacity to processing in Carroll & Sapon 1 959) and Sasaki ( 1 993) have also used measures
the first and second language has been discussed by Lado ( 1 965), of short-term memory to predict differences in aptitude for L2
Stevick ( 1 976), Cook ( 1 977, 1 979, 1 986), Papagno, Valentine and learning, finding significant correlations between scores on memory
Baddeley ( 1 99 1 ) and Harrington and Sawyer ( 1 992). Cook ( 1 977) components of aptitude tests and subsequent foreign language
assessed the extent to which adult subjects differed on identical tests learning achievement. It should be noted, however, as Carroll has
of short-term and long-term memory using the first and then the acknowledged, that the use of memory tests in foreign language
second language. He concluded that performance on short-term aptitude research has not drawn much on recent findings in memory
memory tests was more similar in the L 1 and L2 than performance research reviewed above. In particular no measures of implicit
on long-term memory tests, and that short-term memory capacity memory ability are currently used in aptitude tests. Given the claims
Implicit and explicit iriformation processing 79
78 Implicit and explicit iriformation processing

of Krashen and Reber that acquisition and implicit learning are of metalinguistic knowledge with the aim of developing such
likely to be insensitive to explicit measures of memory, it remains sensitivity is certainly possible (O'Malley & Chamot 1 990).
t� b� se�n "':hether implici� measures provide more homogeneous Nonetheless, Schmidt's noticing hypothesis predicts that efficiency
d1stnbutions m the test takmg populations than explicit measures, in directing attention to relevant areas of the structural domain t o
and whether they correlate more with language learning under be learned i s likely to lead to more effective learning, and eventual
conditions like those advocated by the Natural Approach (Krashen understanding, given the possible condition that such noticing and
& Tei:rell 1 983), which aim to develop unconscious acquisition, hypothesis testing takes place within an appropriate developmental
assummg, of course, that such a phenomenon could be empirically schedule (see e.g., Pienemann 1 989). It seems plausible to expect
de�onstrated and distinguished in a principled way from learning that such efficiency will be related to the availability o f
which proceeds by conscious noticing and hypothesis testing. metalinguistic knowledge and the ability to identify grammatical
analogies between sentences as revealed, for example in Carroll and
3.4.4 Attention, salience and grammatical sensitivity Sapon's ( 1 959) measure of grammatical sensitivity.
The ability to notice features of target language input should not
only be related to short-term memory capacity, but should also be 3. 5 Summary
affected by the existence of pre-existing knowledge representations In summary, Schmidt's claim that noticing is necessary to learning,
which can selectively guide the allocation of attentional resources. and that awareness at the level of understanding is strongly
Such representations would facilitate the act of selectively attending facilitative of subsequent learning can be related to measures o f
to �spects of the input for the purposes of conscious hypothesis cognitive resources that facilitate noticing-short-term memory
testmg about the target language, or may simply lead to effective capacity and grammatical sensitivity. The further claim is that
priming of attended features, making recall and use of such features these abilities are drawn on in explicit information processing, and
more automatic (cf. Mellow & Cumming 1 994 ). Hypothesis testing that since explicit information processing is the mode necessary for
may be a consequence of explicit instructions to search for, or t o adult L2 learning, then individual differences in these abilities will
apply rules o f the target language during form-focused tasks, or correlate with measures of L2 learning success. Possible differences
based on an individual impulse to do so during meaning-focused between adults and pre-critical period children in this respect are
activities. Noticing and hypothesis testing, that is, may be discussed in section 4.3 .4 of the following chapter.
constrained by task demands (Fotos 1 993), but are not limited by
them (Allwright 1 984; Slimani 1 992). General measures of field
independence and dependence have been used to address the issue of
individual differences in the ability to manage the deployment of
selective attention (Chapelle & Green 1 992; Chapelle & Roberts
1 986; Naiman, Frolich, Stern & Todesco 1 978), but there have
been doubts expressed about the usefulness of these measures in the
domain of L2 learning (see Skehan 1 989). More domain specific
schematic knowledge such as text schemata have been claimed to
facilitate L2 reading comprehension (Carrell 1 984; Swaffer 1 9 89),
and it seems likely that in the domain of grammatical knowledge,
familiarity with the basic metalinguistic principles for describing
stru�tural patterns and structural analogies would aid hypothesis
testmg. Carroll ( 1 97 1 , 1 993 ; Carroll & Sapon 1 959) has claimed
that grammatical sensitivity to structural patterns is a component
of aptitude for learning languages and not necessarily related to
previous foreign language study. However, explicit training in areas
Chapter 4

Implicit Learning, Memory And


Knowledge

4. 0 Introduction
This chapter reviews the experimental evidence for a distinction
between implicit and explicit processes at three different levels of
description, the levels of learning, memory and knowledge.
Following this review, the objectives of the present study of implicit
and explicit L2 learning are described and a rationale for the
research hypotheses is presented. Research into implicit learning
and memory has made use of different methodologies, and it is
important to distinguish the two (Ellis 1 994a; Reber 1 99 3 ;
Robinson 1 993a, 1 995d). Leaming i s the process whereby new
knowledge is acquired as a result of exposure, in this case t o
examples of language, under particular task conditions. Section 4 . 1
reviews the evidence of differences in learning under conditions that
manipulate degree of awareness of the rules describing the synthetic
grammars used in experiments by Reber ( 1 989) and others.
Describing learning must of necessity invoke memory and
knowledge, since knowledge is the end result of learning, and
memory is where knowledge is stored. However, dissociations
between implicit and explicit memory, as opposed to learning, are
properly studied under conditions in which the knowledge accessed
by memory pre-exists the experimental task conditions. Of course,
this is rarely the case in studies of implicit memory, since these
employ a study-test format in which stimuli are presented at study
as the basis for later measures of accessibility (see section 4.2). T o
this extent, then, the study phase of implicit memory tests involves
learning, in the sense of encoding items into memory, and the test
phase measures the extent of their implicit activation by the test
instructions. However, this is not the sense in which implicit
learning is used by Reber who argues that abstraction of rules
regulating the presentation of letter strings takes place during
artificial grammar learning experiments, following the instruction
82
Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 83
Implicit learning, memory and knowledge

grammatical/ungrammatical significantly better than chance, but t o


to simply memorize, and that this learned, item-independent
be unable to articulate the rules o f the grammar that produced the
knowledge is generalizable to novel letter strings, forming the basis
of grammaticality judgments made in the later transfer tests. acceptable strings. Based on this evidence Reber makes claims about
Knowledge is thus implicated in any study of learning, or the product and process of implicit learning: (i) the product is an
mem? ry, though t�e:� is a temporal dimension in the study o f abstract knowledge of the rules of the grammar; and (ii) the process
learnmg (the acqms1t10n o f new, or the modification o f old whereby this knowledge is accumulated is inaccessible to conscious
knowledge) that is absent in the study of memory (access of existin � awareness. Reber has also claimed that implicit learning is superior
to explicit learning. This claim is based on comparisons of the
knowledge). The representational issue addressed in section 4.3 thus
takes its bearings from the study of implicit learning (how does a transfer phase performance of subjects trained under the
knowledge base accumulate over time?), though it clearly must also memorization condition with that of subjects following the
account for evidence arising from the study of implicit memory instruction to search for rules. Subjects trained under the
(what knowledge can be accessed, and under what conditions?). The memorization condition perform better than those trained under
the rule-search condition.
central representational issue in the study of implicit learning
concerns the abstractness of the accumulated knowledge base. Is it
abstract, and independent of memory for specific instances
en�ountered duri�g learning, as Reber ( 1 989, 1 993) has consistently
_ s
claimed, or 1s 1t the result of access to specific examples or
fragments of examples presented during learning? Section 4 . 3 .4
summarizes the review of implicit learning, memory and knowledge,
s
proposing that implicit and explicit processes at each of these levels
T
are 'fundamentally similar,' while section 4.4 describes the
objectives and research hypotheses for the present study.

4.1 Implicit and explicit learning


Reber's research ( 1 969, 1 976, 1 9 89, 1 993) is based on two
meth? dologies for studying implicit learning, synthetic grammar v
learn1�g and probability learning. The synthetic grammar learning
expenments consist of an acquisition, or training phase, and a
T
testing phase. In the training phase subjects are presented with
strings of letters generated by an artificial, Markovian, left-to-right
finite-state grammar. A typical example of the grammar used is
sho�n in Figure 1 2. In the testing, or transfer phase of experiments, (from Reber 1 989:222)
subjects are presented with new examples of strings generated by the
rules of the grammar and asked to judge which of them are well Figure 12. The Finite-State ' Reber' Grammar
formed. Subjects have been found to perform differently as a
function of the different conditions under which training takes
place. Specific details of these training conditions, and the evidence In the probability learning experiments subjects are exposed to
of differential success in learning are reviewed below. rapidly presented sequences of events for three minutes, at a rate of
Reber claims that these experiments produce evidence of implicit two per second. In the transfer phase they are asked to make
learning, which is the result of following instructions to memorize predictions about the probability of a future event. In Reber and
the strings presented during training. Subjects trained under the Millward ( 1 97 1 ) the probability of an event occurring in a sequence
memorization condition have been found to classify the strings into
84 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 85

was adjusted to steadily increase over fifty presentations, then (i) The complexity of artificial grammars . Reber has argued that
systematically decrease over the next fifty, then increase again over the complexity of the synthetic grammar used to generate strings
the following fifty, etc. Reber's claim, based on the subjects' viewed during training is a necessary prerequisite for the observation
patterns of correct responses, is that they had learned the deep or occurrence of implicit learning, since if the rules are simple
organizing principle, or structure, of increasing and decreasing enough to be analyzed by conscious effort, then subjects in the
probabilities over periods of fifty trials, and not merely the specific implicit learning condition will become aware of, and begin t o
probabilities of occurrence of individual events. Other researchers search for and find, the rules of the grammar. Figure 1 2 represents
have made use of matrix scanning methodologies for studying the grammar used in Reber ( 1 967, 1 969 and 1976), and in studies by
implicit learning, in which the occurrence of sequences of numbers others (e.g., Servan-Schreiber & Anderson 1 990). Variants of this
(Lewicki, Hill & Bizot 1 988) or lights (Nissen & Bullemer 1 987) in grammar have also been used in experiments by Reber (Reber &
quadrants of a computer screen have been the target of learning. Allen 1 978) and others (e.g., Knowlton & Squire 1 994; Turner &
However, Reber has commented that the nature of the repeating Fischler 1 993 ; Vokey & Brooks 1 992; Whittlesea & Dorken 1 99 3 ),
sequences used derives from rules which are less 'abstract' than with the aim of varying the complexity of permissible strings, or o f
those induced during synthetic grammar learning experiments seeing whether subjects can distinguish between strings generated by
( 1 993 :44). Consequently, in the following sections my discussion of different grammars (Whittlesea & Dorken 1 993 ). Nonetheless,
the experimental evidence for implicit learning is restricted to those although they are complex, it is clear that such grammars are
studies which have employed synthetic grammars, since, of the considerably less complex than those of natural languages, and differ
methodologies briefly surveyed here, they involve the use of a in their fundamental properties by lacking, for example, a semantic
stimulus domain most representative of the level of complexity o f and a phonological component, and many of the structure­
the target o f learning during SLA. dependent characteristics of natural languages which allow syntactic
relations to occur over discontiguous elements (Baker 1 989; Braine
4.1.1 Evidence of dissociations using artificial grammars 1 965; Chomsky 1 959, 1 965; Mccawley 1 988; Pinker 1 994).
There are several reasons for caution in assuming that claims about
implicit learning using artificial grammars could be generalizable t o (ii) Lack of consistency across studies. It is clear, also, that few
SLA. (i) First, there is reason to doubt that learning artificial features of implicit learning studies, apart from the use of finite­
grammars and learning natural languages involve comparable state grammars to generate test strings, are standardized from study
processes given the difference in complexity of the two stimulus to study. Sometimes differences in treatments are motivated by
domains. (ii) Secondly, there is the issue of comparability across the manipulations of variables of interest, such as the degree o f
studies that have so far been done since there is a lack of similarity or complexity o f test strings (McAndrews & Moscovitch
consistency in many studies regarding the operationalization of 1 985; Vokey & Brooks 1 992), or by the limitations of the
instructional variables. This inconsistency gives cause to doubt that experimental setting and the nature of the subjects (for example the
a single homogeneous process could properly be inferred from the amnesics studied by Knowlton & Squire 1 994). However, at other
different instructional treatments that are consequently delivered. times, key moderator variables which could be expected to affect
(iii) Thirdly, there is the issue of the slender effect sizes used t o the nature and extent of learning, like length of stimulus
support claims for the occurrence of implicit learning. Though presentation time, form of presentation and the nature o f
demonstrating that performance above the level of chance is made instructions t o subjects, differ across studies for n o apparent reason.
possible by learning under implicit conditions, the studies so far The variation of length of stimulus string, presentation time,
have failed to show a robust level of success. (iv) Finally, there is number of runs or repetitions of the string, and manner o f
the issue of the consistency of the findings themselves, since there presentation during training across nine implicit learning studies is
have been a number of failures to replicate results obtained on early presented in Table 3 , together with descriptions of instructions t o
implicit learning studies. subjects in the implicit condition of each study.
86 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 87

Table 3. Instructional Variables In Nine Implicit Learning (iii) Effect size. The effect size of correct classification of novel
Stud ies grammatical sentences as a function of training under implicit
conditions is typically small, though statistically distinguishable
Study Length of Presentation Repetitions Format Instructions from performance under rule-search and instructed conditions in a
Stimulus Time to Subjects number of studies. Comparisons of results across conditions are
summarized below in sections 4. 1 . 1-3 . Here it is sufficient to
Reber 4-8 letters subject-paced 1 run, printed memorize illustrate the magnitude of the effect size used to support claims for
( 1967) (20 strings) 4 strings card for a test the occurrence of implicit learning.
on 5 cards

Reber 3-6 letters 1 0 seconds 3 runs, printed pay the


Table 4. Effect Sizes For Performance By Ten Implicit
& Allen (20 strings) per card 1 string card utmost
attention
Learning Groups
( 1976) per card

Matthews 5-1 0 letters subject-paced 1 run, printed memorize Study Effect Size
et al. (20 strings) 20 strings list for a test
( 1989) on one list Reber ( 1 967) .69
Reber ( 1 976) .66
McAndrews 3-8 letters about 24 3 runs, printed choose the
Reber & Allen ( 1 978) .73
& (24 strings) minutes 8 pairs card best name
per card to market
Reber et al. ( 1 980) .5 1
Moscovitch
( 1985) a computer Knowlton & Squire ( 1 994) normal = .59 amnesic = .55
Whittlesea & Dorken ( 1 993) .64
Vokey & 3-7 letters subject-paced 1 run, printed study then Turner & Fischler ( 1 993) . 62
Brooks ( 1 6 strings) 4 strings card recall and Servan-Schreiber & Anderson ( 1 990) . 6 8
( 1 992) on 4 cards write down McAndrews & Moscovitch ( 1 985) .53

Knowlton 3-7 letters 3 seconds 1 run, printed read then


& Squire (23 strings) per card 1 card card recall and
( 1994) per string write down
(3 tries)
Table 4 illustrates effect sizes for implicit learning of
unstructured stimulus displays in Reber ( 1 967, 1 976), Reber and
Turner & 4-8 letters subject-paced 1 run, computer memonze Allen ( 1 978), and Reber, Kassin, Cantor and Lewis ( 1 980). These
Fischler (2 1 strings) 3 strings screen as much results are based on Reber ( 1 989), in which Reber summarizes his
( 1993) in 7 sets as you can own research findings, using percentage scores for correct
classification of examples of strings occurring twice during transfer
Whittlesea 7 letters subject-paced 2 runs computer pronounce grammaticality judgment tests. Also included in Table 4 are results
& Dorken (80 strings) for each screen on the first from Servan-Schreiber and Anderson ( 1 990) (a weighted average o f
(1 993) string run then spell percentage correct to unstructured stimuli in the transfer
on the next grammaticality judgment session); Turner and Fischler ( 1 994)
(percentage correct across all items in a transfer grammaticality
Servan- 6-8 letters 5 seconds 1 run, computer read then
4 strings screen recall and judgment session); Whittlesea and Dorken ( 1 993) (the probability
Schreiber (20 strings)
& Anderson in 5 sets write out of judging grammatical items correctly in the transfer session);
( 1990) Knowlton and Squire ( 1 994) (percentage correct across all items o n
88 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 89

transfer sessions by normal subjects and amnesics ); and McAndrews ( 1 993 :49). This is possible in simple stimulus domains, but
and Moscovitch ( 1 985) (percentage correct across all items in a otherwise will result in the formation of correlated grammars, or
transfer grammaticality judgment test). partial and idiosyncratic rule systems that are not isomorphic with
These data, then, while obscuring the effects of manipulating the underlying structure of the grammar.
variables of interest within individual studies, like degree o f
similarity o f items (McAndrews & Moscovitch 1 985 ), or the effect 4.1. 1.3 Effects ofproviding explicit instruction
of response bias (Whittlesea & Dorken 1 993), do give an The effects of providing explicit instruction in the rules have been
approximation of the effect sizes observed across studies for examined by Reber et al. ( 1 980). In this study instruction took the
performance on transfer grammaticality judgment tests following form of showing subjects the schematic grammar illustrated in
training under implicit conditions, where performance at 50 per Figure 1 2, and giving them a seven-minute course on how it could be
cent accuracy would represent chance. used to generate letter strings. Their basic finding was that providing
explicit instruction, and structuring the presentation of stimuli t o
iv) Replicability. Finally it should be noted that there have been a conform to this instruction, will produce effective learning i f the
number of failures to replicate the finding that implicit learning instruction is provided early in the training process. If the stimuli
occurs and is superior to explicit attempts to search for rules using are unstructured, or if the instruction occurs late, then this
artificial grammars, e.g., Dulany et al. ( 1 984) and Abrams ( 1 987), condition will also be relatively ineffective compared to the implicit
as well as a recent failure to replicate findings regarding the condition. It should be noted that a similar caveat applies here
effectiveness of implicit learning following Hayes and Broadbent's regarding the complexity of the rule taught, as applies t o
methodology ( 1 988) by Green and Shanks ( 1 993). instructions to search fo r rules: i f the rule is too complex, Reber
claims, then this condition will also be ineffective relative to
4.1.1. 1 Implicit task conditions implicit learning, regardless of the point at which instruction is
Given these caveats Reber ( 1 989, 1 993) has noted that provided. In other words, instruction helps if the stimulus domain is
performance under implicit conditions in artificial grammar learning simple, and structured to conform to the rule taught, and if training
typically exceeds performance under rule-search conditions. is provided early. However, Reber et al. 's ( 1 980) results show that
Implicit conditions are operationalized following the instruction t o even when these conditions are met, instructed subjects perform
remember, or pay the utmost attention to the stimuli presented, little better than implicit subjects (. 7 1 for percentage correct o n
without informing subjects of their rule-governed nature. Reber's transfer items following explicit instruction, compared to .69 for
studies, it should be noted, have no measure of whether this implicit learners in the same study). By implication, following
condition is delivered successfully, whereas other studies (e.g., Reber's claims, one might expect instructed learners to perform at
Servan-Schrieber & Anderson 1 990) have added a recall test with about the same level as implicit learners where the stimulus domain
feedback to the implicit condition and imposed a threshold o f is at the level of complexity of artificial grammars, but to fare
correct recall o n responses before allowing subjects to proceed t o much worse when faced with a considerably more complex stimulus
viewing the next stimuli. domain such as natural language.

4.1.1.2 Rule-search conditions 4.1.2 Arguments against dissociations


In rule-search conditions subjects are told that rules underlie the Arguments have been put forward disputing Reber's claims, a) t o
stimuli presented and are asked to search for them during training. have demonstrated that implicit learning o f abstract rules underlying
The typical low performance of this group is due, Reber claims, t o the stimulus display takes place, and b) that the accumulation o f
the fact that the stimulus domain i s complex. Where the stimulus knowledge during implicit learning takes place in the absence of
domain is simple, and the structural correlates of rules are made awareness, and that, to this extent, such learning is unconscious and
salient, then Reber claims this condition can produce effective dissociable from conscious explicit learning. This dispute, then,
learning-"Looking for rules will work if you can find them" concerns both the product and the process of implicit learning. The
90 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 91

counterarguments to Reber all involve the claim that memory is the the product of memory-based strategies, citing as evidence analyses
basis for later performance, not abstract rule knowledge, and that of verbal reports given by implicitly trained subjects who were
such memories are accessible to awareness given appropriate and subsequently asked to search for rules. The verbal report data
sensitive measures. showed them to be drawing on a knowledge base that was both item
specific and rule-based. Importantly, too, for the implicitly trained
4.1.2.1 Fragmented memory or abstract knowledge? group, such rules were accessible to consciousness, and were
There are three versions of the memory-based counterarguments t o successfully communicated to a yoked group of subjects using an
Reber's claim about the abstract product o f implicit learning. Vokey unknown letter set. This was evidenced by the yoked group's better
and Brooks ( 1 992) claim performance in the transfer than chance performance on string discrimination tasks requiring
grammaticality judgment test is based on similarity of instances them to identify which of five strings was grammatical. However,
encountered during training to those encountered at test, and report the yoked groups never did as well as their implicitly trained
findings that degree of similarity of training and transfer instances informants, suggesting that the item specific contribution to their
affected transfer test judgment more than rule conformity (see also knowledge base had been affected by the letter change.
McAndrews & Moscovitch 1 985; Turner & Fischler 1 993). Vokey
and Brooks suggest that dual knowledge, rule and instance, models of 4.1.2.2 Verbalizability of knowledge and awareness
representation (e.g., Logan 1 988a) as well as distributed memory The claim that memory-based mechanisms are, at least in part,
models (e.g., Hintzman 1 986; Rumelhart & McClelland 1 9 8 5 ), responsible for the knowledge base that develops during implicit
could explain their findings (see section 4.3). Perruchet and Pacteau learning is linked to the claim that measures of awareness used t o
( 1 990) argue that whole string similarity can be further analyzed assess the extent o f subjects' knowledge o f the rules are inadequate
into substring knowledge of memory for bigrams, or letter pairings. since they require subjects to verbalize knowledge that is disj oint
They found that transfer test strings that preserved frequently with the knowledge they gain by consciously attending to the
presented bigrams tended to be accepted as grammatical, even when stimulus domain (Perruchet & Pacteau 1 990; Shanks & St. John
these strings violated the rules of the grammar. Servan-Schrieber 1 994). Shanks and St. John ( 1 994) have argued that the
and Anderson ( 1 990) found that inducing chunking by presenting retrospective protocols typically used by Reber to measure the
visually spaced groupings of bigrams and trigrams in the training extent of subjects' awareness of the abstract rules of the grammar
session affected transfer test performance, since ungrammatical thus fail to meet the "information criterion" of measuring the
strings that contained chunks tended to be wrongly judged precise nature of information they have gained by consciously
grammatical whereas, of the ungrammatical sentences, those that attending. Like Perruchet and Pacteau ( 1 990) and Brody ( 1 9 89)
violated chunks were rejected most often. Shanks and St. John also argue that the use of retrospectiv_e
None of these findings, however, can explain the apparent protocols is relatively insensitive as a measure of awareness and
stimulus independent nature of the knowledge gained during implicit should be accompanied by measures that meet the "sensitivity
learning illustrated in Reber ( 1 969) and Matthews et al. ( 1 989). criterion" like concurrent think-aloud protocols and recognition
Reber ( 1 969) found that changing the letters used to produce tests, rather than explicit recall alone. Similar claims are made by
stimulus strings presented in a transfer phase had no effect o n Perruchet and Amorim ( 1 992) and Perruchet and Gallego ( 1 993)
accuracy, whereas changing the structure o f the grammar that about the sensitivity of measures of awareness used in sequence
generated the strings caused a significant drop in accuracy. learning experiments (Nissen & Bullemer 1 987), which claim t o
Matthews et al. ( 1 989) also found that changing the stimuli used produce evidence o f implicit rule learning in the absence of
throughout the course of a four-week implicit learning experiment awareness.
had no adverse effect on performance by one group, relative t o
another group who used the same letters throughout the 4. 1.2.3 Processing accounts of the evidence
experiment. However, Matthews et al. conclude that implicit Claims for the memory-based nature of representations, and the
learning is not a homogeneous process of rule abstraction, but is also consequent insensitivity of measures of 'rule' awareness, are also
92 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 93

invoked by processing accounts of implicit learning experiments. As to explicit learning. In implicit memory studies, in contrast, the
with the implicit memory experiments reviewed in section 3 . 3 , stimulus domain does not contain new knowledge, but rather is
processing explanations explain dissociations in terms o f the limited to the encoding, and subsequent retrieval of previously
transfer of processing strategies adopted during encoding to test known information, usually in the form of words or pictures. The
performance. To this extent, implicit learning conditions result in a dissociation between explicit and implicit memory retrieval
distinct kind of processing, not necessarily a dissociable form o f processes has been studied experimentally by contrasting
unconscious rule learning, which is a consequence o f the instruction performance on tasks that do not require conscious recollection o f
to memorize. Nonetheless, such processing is a conscious operation, experiences at the time o f encoding, with performance o n tasks
involving noticing and maintenance rehearsal in short-term that do require conscious reflection on experiences at the time o f
memory, prior to long-term storage. Subsequent long-term memory encoding (Merickle & Reingold 1 989; Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork
for associations between items presented during training is a 1 988; Roediger et al. 1 989; Schacter 1 987, 1 989; Schacter et al.
consequence of this type of rehearsal, and retrieval is facilitated if 1 993). The distinction between implicit and explicit memory,
study-test processing operations are matched. Whittlesea and deriving as it does from differences in performance on such tasks,
Darken ( 1 993 ) have recently pointed to the importance of thus relates, as Schacter points out ( 1 987:50 1 ), to the subject's
encoding specificity, and of matching the nature of the processing psychological experience at the time of retrieval from memory; it
encouraged at study and at test. In their study subjects were found to does not necessarily imply the existence of two separate memory
improve in grammaticality judgments when the study-test stores. It is also true that conscious awareness of, or reflection on,
processing strategies were the same. Subjects performed better o n experiences at the time of encoding may not simply be a
items which were encoded and tested in the same modality, either by consequence of task demands, under voluntary control, but m ay
spelling or by pronouncing the letter strings. sometimes occur involuntarily.
In summary, this review of studies of implicit learning leads to a
similar conclusion to that drawn by Shanks and St. John ( 1 994). (i)
There is evidence that performance under implicit task conditions, Test type Form of memory
following the instruction to memorize or simply attend to stimuli, Explicit Explicit Implicit
can result in item-specific knowledge, as well as the induction of an intentional involuntary
abstract, item-independent representation of the stimulus domain. Direct 1 2 3
(ii) It is likely that both forms of knowledge are drawn on in Indirect 4 5 6
transfer grammaticality judgment tests. (iii) Both forms of
knowledge are the result of fully attended explicit information
processing. (iv) Subjects are conscious of processing strategies Figure 13. Measures Of Memory And Forms Of Memory
regulating the development of both knowledge bases and can
demonstrate awareness given sufficiently sensitive measures of that
awareness. (v) The studies reviewed above do not support the claim For these reasons Richardson-Klavehn and Bjork ( 1988) propose
that two dissociable forms of learning, differentially susceptible to the model illustrated in Figure 1 3 for distinguishing between
awareness, result from exposure under implicit, versus rule-search measures of memory and forms of memory. Direct and indirect
and instructed conditions tests relate to the presence of task instructions to consciously
remember a prior episode (direct tests) or to indirectly facilitate it
4.2 Implicit and explicit memory (indirect tests). Explicit and implicit memory refer to those mental
A prerequisite for studies of implicit learning is exposure to a states that involve conscious access of prior events, and
stimulus domain which is unknown prior to the experimental nonconscious activation of memory for prior events, respectively.
situation. Additionally Reber has proposed that the stimulus domain Explicit remembering can be voluntary, or involuntary. Studies of
must be complex to create the conditions for implicit, as opposed
94 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 95

dissociations between implicit and explicit forms of memory have shapes. The same dissociation was observed by Lewicki ( 1 985) with
been based on a comparison of cells 1 and 6 in Figure 1 3 . respect to explicit memory for subliminally presented noun
adjective pairs, which was at chance, and preference for the
4.2.1 Methodologies for studying dissociations subliminally presented adjectives in sentence completion tasks.
Those who argue for the existence of two distinct capacities or
systems (Cohen 1 984; Squire 1 987, 1 992; Squire & Zola-Morgan 4.2. 1.3 Priming
1 99 1 ; Tulving & Schacter 1 990) claim that direct tests, such as There are a number of different types of test of the relationship
recognition and recall measures, draw on explicit memory; indirect between priming and memory. In lexical decision tasks a list o f
tests, such as word completion and preference rating, are presumed words is presented, with n o instructions other than to attend.
to access implicit memory. These measures are described below. Subsequently subjects are shown another list containing some words
from the old list plus some new words. In some cases the
4.2.1.1 Recognition and recall instructions accompanying the presentation of the second list
In recall tasks the subject is told to recall something from memory require the subject to identify ' acceptable' words of English.
that formed part of an earlier target event, for example words that Implicit memory is then inferred from the decreased latency in the
appeared on a previously presented list. Recall can be uncued or making of a decision about the legality or acceptability of a
cued. In cued recall subjects are presented with part of the previously presented word, when compared to words not previously
previously attended event to aid conscious recall (e.g., the first presented or 'primed. ' Priming can also be contrived prior to
letter of a word). Such cues can also be items that are semantically, decisions about 'preferences' for words on the second list, rather
phonemically or graphemically related to, but not a part of the than acceptability judgments.
earlier event (e.g., a semantically associated word-a synonym or
superordinate term, see Nelson & McEvoy 1 979). In recognition 4.2.1.4 Word completion
tests the subject is asked to identify which of a series of presented In direct measures of explicit memory, like the word identification
words or objects were previously presented and which are novel. In tests of recall and recognition cited above, the metric for assessing
both recall and recognition tests the need to draw on memory to the extent of access to memory is the accuracy of responses when
complete the task is thus made explicit in the task instructions. subjects are asked to identify previously presented words. In
contrast, indirect measures of implicit memory like word
4.2. 1.2 Subliminal exposure and preference rating completion tests require subjects to complete a partially presented
In indirect tests, which are presumed to access implicit memory, the letter string with the most appropriate word. The partial
subject is asked to perform a task using materials only some of representation can be a word fragment, in which letters have been
which have been presented before. If the subject shows greater systematically deleted from a word, or provision of the word stem.
facility using the previously presented materials than the novel Memory for previously presented words is inferred from the
materials, then this is inferred to be an effect due to the use of frequency with which they are used to complete the letter string.
implicit memory. Subliminal exposure, for example, can be used t o
demonstrate the dissociation o f implicit and explicit memory. 4.2.2 Evidence for dissociations
Kunst-Wilson and Zajonc ( 1 980) exposed subjects to geometric Four types of evidence are used to support the claim for
shapes for periods too brief, they claimed, to permit consci�us dissociations in normal populations. (i) Explicit memory, using
perception. Explicit memory, as accessed by a forced-ch01ce recognition or recall measures, is sensitive to the type of processing
recognition test in which subjects had to indicate which of a new set required during study. Jacoby and Dallas ( 1 9 8 1 ) required subjects to
of shapes they had already seen, was at chance. However, when answer questions about the meaning of words presented at study
asked to indicate a preference for one of a series of paired shapes (elaborative processing) or identify whether they contained
(consisting of one previously presented and one new shape), the particular letters (non-elaborative processing). They found
subjects showed a significant preference for the previously presented subsequent recognition and recall was better for the group who
96 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 97

received the study treatment requmng elaborative processing. require that the precise nature of memory task demands be
However, performance on implicit tests was insensitive to the identified so that they can be manipulated with a view to examining
different study treatments. (ii) Indirect tests of implicit memory are the processing consequences for performance on implicit and
sensitive to shifts in the modality of study and test, whereas direct explicit tests.
tests of explicit memory are not. Jacoby and Dallas ( 1 9 8 1 ) found Like Richardson-Klavehn and Bjork ( 1 988), Merickle and
that auditory study followed by visual test conditions impaired Reingold ( 1 99 1 ) use the terms 'direct' and ' indirect' measures t o
implicit memory performance but had little or no effect on explicit distinguish the types o f experimental tasks described above, which
recall and recognition. (iii) Priming effects on word fragment are presumed to access implicit and explicit memory. Since direct
completion and word stem completion tests have also been found t o measures (like the recognition task in the Kunst-Wilson & Zaj onc
lead to retention over much longer periods o f time than recognition 1 980 experiment) and indirect measures (like the preference rating
memory for explicitly remembered material (Tulving, Schacter & task) of memory are quite dissimilar Merickle and Reingold n ote
Stark 1 982). (iv) Performance on implicit and explicit tests has that it is possible the observed dissociations may simply reflect the
been found to be statistically independent, implying that different different task conditions used. This means that attempts to make
abilities are drawn on in these tests (Schacter 1 987: 508). claims about the dissociability of explicit and implicit memory need
Other evidence for dissociations comes from the performance o f to control for as many of the following as possible across the two
amnesics who are impaired in performance on explicit tests o f types of measures.
recognition and recall, but are not impaired i n performance o n
implicit tests relative to a normal population (e.g., Squire 1 9 8 7 ; (i) Retrieval or test cues. There is a need to control for the use o f
Squire & McKee 1 993). These findings are relatively particular cues across recognition and recall experiments, and
uncontroversial; however, there is disagreement about the extent t o preference and word completion measures. Nelson, Canas, Bajo and
which a dissociation can be inferred o n the basis o f contrasting Keelean ( 1 987) have shown that in cued recall of word lists, using
young and elderly adult performance on implicit memory tests. word stems, response accuracy can vary with the lexical set size o f
Light and Singh ( 1 987) and Graf ( 1 990) claim elderly adults the cue (cues with uncommon o r small set sizes like 'ood' are m ore
perform poorly on explicit memory tests relative to young adults, likely to cue recall than cues with larger set sizes like ' ine' . Recall
showing a 5 0% decline in performance, compared to only a 4% has also been shown to vary depending on the semantic set size o f
decline in performance on implicit tests. Russo and Parkin ( 1 993 ) the target. Words from a large semantic set like fruit, e.g., apple,
claim steeper declines in implicit memory performance are are less likely to be recalled accurately than words from a smaller
apparent when memory is tested for visual, rather than verbal, semantic set (Nelson 1 987; Nelson et al. 1 987; Snodgrass 1 98 9 ) .
material, and further that elderly subjects' reduced implicit memory Finally, recall has been shown to vary depending on the nature o f
was caused by the decline in explicit memory, which was therefore the relation o f the cue to the target. 'Intralist' cues, where the cue
unavailable to facilitate implicit retrieval, suggesting age related forms a physical part of the initially presented target list, result in
differences in the interaction of processing modes rather than more successful recall than 'extralist' cues, where the cue is
differences in the relative capacities of separate systems. semantically, phonologically or graphemically related to, but not a
physical part of the initially presented list (Nelson 1 987; Nelson &
4.2.3 Explanations, dissociations and task demands McEvoy 1 979).
Explanations for dissociations can be divided into those which
propose that different memory systems underlie performance o n (ii) Response bias. Response bias can be assessed on direct
direct and indirect tests, and that these two systems are measures like recognition tasks, for example, by comparing
differentially regulated by conscious awareness, and explanations recognition performance requiring responses in different modalities,
that attribute differences in performance on the tests to differences whereas this cannot be assessed on indirect measures like word
in the task demands of study and test formats, which encourage completion tests. As a result it is not possible to tell if dissociations
different types of processing (3 .3 .3). Both types of explanation are a result of a difference in response bias on the two tasks.
98 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 99

(iii) Response metric. The response metrics on word completion, & Anderson 1 990) in the letter strings requmng well-formedness
recognition and recall tests are different. Merikle and Reingold judgments. Consequently Matthews et al. ( 1 989) and McAndrews
suggest that consequently "dissociations between direct and indirect and Moscovitz ( 1 985) argue both rule-based and exemplar-based
memory tasks may simply reflect differences in measurement scales representations are implied by transfer task performance, while
and not.. .underlying processes" ( 1 99 1 :225). Merickle and Reingold Shanks and St. John ( 1 994) argue that implicit learning is no
suggest, therefore, that task-specific differences relating to the different from any other form of general learning, or from learning
three dimensions described above must first be controlled for before under the rule-search and instructed conditions, since these also
making claims about differences in underlying processes based o n result from a blend of these two forms of classification. Reber has
task performance. However, this was clearly not done i n earlier also acknowledged that memorization plays a large part in implicit
implicit memory research which simply associated different stu�y learning, but his insistence on the abstractness of the resulting
test formats with different systems (Schacter 1 987). More vahd representation is no doubt motivated by the need to give substance
procedures for theorizing memory performance would be to identify to his larger theoretical position, that implicit learning is "primary"
"format-specific memory effects," as in Graf and Ryan ( 1 990), and in the sense of ontogenetically and phylogenetically earlier evolved,
then propose processing explanations based on t�em, or to com�are and consequently deeper and more robust, than later evolved
performance on tasks which controlled for the differences descn?ed conscious forms of learning (Abrams & Reber 1 988). In the sections
above while differing only in task instructions, and then base claims below rule-based and exemplar-based positions on the representation
'
about implicit and explicit memory on the observed differences (if of implicit knowledge are described and then related to seemingly
any) in performance (for a related discussion see Robinson 1 995c). complementary models of categorization and rule-based reasoning.

4.3 Implicit knowledge 4.3.1 The rule-based abstractionist position


Knowledge is what is learned over time, following exposure t o Reber defines implicit learning as, "an unconscious process . . .
stimuli presented under the different task conditions described i n yielding abstract knowledge. Implicit knowledge results from the
section 4. 1 , and what i s activated by the direct and indirect memory induction of an abstract representation of the structure that the
tests described in 4.2. Current discussions of the representation of stimulus environment displays, and this knowledge is acquired in the
implicit knowledge have tended to side either with the claim that it absence of conscious, reflective strategies to learn" ( 1 989:2 1 9 ) .
is rule-based, and abstract, or with the claim that it is the result of The unconscious procedural knowledge resulting from implicit
access to memory for instances, or fragments of instai:ices learning, Reber claims, both anticipates, and is always in excess of,
encountered during training. There is evidence that both views conscious declarative knowledge of the stimulus domain represented
capture important aspects of the knowledge drawn on in trans�er by the finite-state grammar. Reber also maintains a clear distinction
task performance during implicit learning studies (and also durmg between implicit learning and memory ( 1 989: 2 1 9). The former is a
SLA, see e.g., Ellis 1 993a; Schmidt 1 992 for discussion). On the one process of induction which results in an abstract representation of
hand some process of abstraction seems necessary to account for the rules regulating allowable concatenations of elements in the
findings by Reber ( 1 967), McAndrews and Moscovitch ( 1 985), and stimulus domain. Access to this knowledge base in making
Matthews et al. ( 1989), that transfer of knowledge took place from grammaticality judgments is independent of access to memory for
the grammar and exemplars experienced during the training phase specific items or trained exemplars. His position is thus
of their experiments, to new letter strings in transfer test characterized by Vokey and Brooks ( 1 992) as ' abstractionist.'
performance, a finding which poses pro?lems for an a�count based
purely on the development of item-specific representat10ns . . C?n � he 4.3.1.1 Prototypes and abstraction
other hand, significant effects have been found for the facihtat10n Abstraction of typical characteristics of category exemplars by a
of transfer task performance on items which preserved the process of feature counting or averaging is basic to prototype and
similarity of whole instances (Vokey & Brooks 1 992), or preser�ed schema theory (Malt 1 989; Medin & Smith 1 984; Rosch 1 973). I n
bigrams and trigrams (Perruchet & Pacteau 1 990; Servan-Schreiber this sense a schematic representation of the grammar could be
1 00 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 101

derived by counting each legitimate bigram pairing as a feature, then 4.3. 1.3 Natural language categorization
summing their distributional preferences to provide an abstract Connectionist models have been developed in the domain of
representation of the mutual constraints on their positional natural language learning (e.g., Gasser 1 990; Henning 1 993 ; Lee &
relations. This representation of grammatical dependency and Gasser 1 992; Robinson 1 994c; Rumelhart & McClelland 1 986;
constituency relations (Robinson 1 986, 1 990), would be stored in Shirai 1 992), and a claim that the result of implicit L2 learning is
semantic memory, independently of knowledge of individual abstract could also be interpreted within this framework.
experiences of bigram pairings, which would be stored in episodic Alternatively, however, current linguistic theory offers another
memory (Tulving 1 9 83). However, the assumptions of prototype interpretation of the claim that implicit L2 learning is abstract.
theory-that categorization is based on abstraction of defining This is to identify implicit learning conditions as those which are
features from examples of a stimulus encountered during a period o f necessary for access to Universal Grammar (Chomsky 1 986; cf.
exposure, and that this requires 'analytic' thinking skills (Malt Pinker 1 989, 1 994; White 1 989a), and the rule-based
1989)-have been disputed by exemplar-based theories of configurational constraints on parameterization and the shape of
classification and recognition (e.g., Hintzman 1 986; Nosofsky possible languages. Schwartz ( 1 992, 1 993) and Zobl ( 1 992, 1 99 5 )
1 992). These are more closely related to the memory-based have recently made this claim with regard to the conditions Krashen
representational position which Reber rejects. identifies as necessary for the process of language acquisition. They
claim unconscious acquisition processes, but not conscious learning
4.3.1.2 Connectionist models of categorization processes, can access Universal Grammar and facilitate L2
An alternative interpretation of the abstractionist claim could be representational change and development by triggering parameter
based, as Reber has himself suggested, on connectionist models of resetting and recategorization of the target language grammar.
categorization (see for discussion Adams, Aizawa & Fuller 1 992;
Dienes 1 992; Grossberg 1 980, 1 988; Kirsch 1 99 1 ; Lachter & Bever 4.3.2 The exemplar-based distributive position.
1 988; MacWhinney, Leinbach, Taraban & McDonald 1 989; Pinker Vokey and Brooks ( 1 992), along with many critics of Reber's
& Prince 1 988; Smolensky 1 988) and would follow from the act of abstractionist position, propose a 'distributive' view of the
noticing frequently co-occurring bigram pairings. Schmidt, for knowledge resulting from the experiments Reber describes in which
example, has claimed that implicit learning is probably best "experience with the distributional properties of the domain (e.g.,
characterized as "the gradual accumulation of associations between frequency, characteristic features, privilege of occurrence) can
frequently co-occurring features," as in connectionist architectures, remain distributed in the knowledge base and can be exploited by a
rather than the "unconscious induction of abstract rule systems" process of forming immediate local models or analogies as the need
( 1990 : 1 49). Schmidt seems to have in mind here rule systems of the arises" ( 1 992:328). Similarly, McLaughlin ( 1 980: cf. McLaughlin et
type proposed by formal linguists (e.g., Chomsky 1 986; Pinker al. 1 983) distinguishes implicit learning, of the purely abstract type
1 99 1 ). However connectionist models have also been shown to be identified by Reber, from 'analogic' learning, of a type similar to
capable of representing abstract rule-like knowledge, in the sense of that proposed by Ross ( 1 989) and Skousen ( 1 990). The claim that
learning categories like noun, verb, animate and inanimate object implicit learning may derive from exemplar-based learning and
(represented as clusters of hidden units), as a consequence of analogic reasoning derives support from a number of recent models
learning to predict the sequence of words in simple letter strings fed of classification.
to a network during training (Elman 1 988, 1 990). To this extent
connectionist networks are also capable of modeling the rule 4.3.2.1 Estes 's model of classification
abstraction process that Reber claims is responsible for implicit Estes' s ( 1 986, 1 992) model was proposed to account for the kind of
learning, and, in his own words, "may be able to provide a formal learning that occurs when, for example, a person is involved in
foundation for examining the underlying processes that we have classifying plants into classes of fruits and vegetables. It is assumed
long felt lies at the core of implicit learning" ( 1 993 : 1 06). that experience results in specific memories for objects called fruits
or vegetables, and that when called on to classify a new plant, its
1 02 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 1 03

overall similarity to exemplars of each of these classes is computed or to search �or the1!1. There is an extensive literature investigating
and the person then assigns it to a class to which it seems more the hypothesis testmg and reasoning procedures subjects adopt in
similar based on a probability related to the ratio of similarities. One rule discovery tasks (see Evans, Newstead & Byrne 1 993), which
problem with applying this model to the learning process that takes have demonstrated consistent findings with regard to problem­
place during artificial grammar learning experiments is that such solving ability and reasoning. One such finding is that o f
experiments typically contain only grammatical examples during conf�rmation bias in rule discovery experiments. Subjects are
the training phase. There are no examples of ungrammatical strings consiste.ntly observed to test hypotheses formed during rule-search
which could serve as the basis for forming two distinct categories. by seekmg to confirm predictions the hypothetical rule makes about
t�e structur.e of the pr?blem domain, rather than by seeking t o
4.3.2.2 Nosofsky 's exemplar-based classification model disconfirm it. Overlookmg negative, disconfirming evidence often
Nosofsky also assumes that categorization is made on the basis o f leads subjects to overly narrow hypotheses based on positive
computing the similarity o f an experienced event to previously evidence alone (Klayman & Ha 1 989; Wason 1 960; cf. Bley­
experienced events. Since the context in which events are Vroman 1 983). There is also evidence that instructions to apply
experienced introduces a certain amount of contextual variance into rules can preempt the effects of exemplar-based learning. Nosofsky,
instance encoding Nosofsky adopts a multidimensional scaling Clark and Shin ( 1 989) trained subjects by exposing them to multiple
approach to representing context effects on similarity assessment. examples of visual stimuli, then asked them to classify stimuli as old
"Exemplars are represented as points in a multidimensional ?r n�w . in a transfer task. Additionally they were given rules for
psychological space, with similarity assumed to be a decreasing identifymg old versus new stimuli which contradicted the evidence
function of distance in the space" ( 1 992:3 64). Selective attention of their previous experience. Nosofsky et al. report that subjects
can be used to "shrink the space along its component dimensions," overwhelmingly based their classification decisions on the rules· the
thus facilitating the act of matching similar instances and so impact of the earlier experiences with the stimuli were forgotten.
categorizing newly experienced events. In this approach the As Shanks and St. John ( 1 994) point out, the fact that subjects in
positive evidence of grammatical examples presented during this experiment were following instructions to apply rules does not
training in artificial grammar experiments would result in a faster mean that the impulse to test and follow a rule cannot be self­
and clearer judgment about category membership in transfer well­ initia�ed unde� task conditions like those in the artificial grammar
formedness tests, than previously unexperienced ungrammatical le�rnmg expenmen�s of Reber. Shanks and St. John, after reviewing
.
examples. evide�ce for the ab1hty to apply and follow rules in problem-solving
exper�ments, and relating this to artificial grammar learning
4.3.2.3 Murphy and Medin 's theory model of classification expent?ents, conc h-1;de, "t.here are in fact two separate learning
A different approach to identifying the process of computing strategies ... The rule mductton strategy is characterized by conscious
similarity is taken by Murphy and Medin ( 1 985). They also argue effort to develop and evaluate hypotheses which are often
that the features or units used to compute similarity are not �nrepresentative of the actual grammar... The instance strategy is
invariant across contexts, but change as a function of a person's mvoked by instructions simply to memorize or observe the training
needs, interests and goals. Thus the judgment of similarity, and strings" ( 1 994:427).
decisions about the relevant attributes to use in computing
similarity, depends on a person's individual, task dependent, theory 4.3.4 The Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis
for relating them. The evidence of implicit and explicit learning and information
processing reviewed in this and the preceding chapter lend support
.
4.3.3 Rules, instances, and task demands to the claim that they are the result of fundamentally similar
The models of exemplar-based classification given above, it should processes. I refer to this as the 'Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis'
be stressed, cannot be used to predict how subjects will perform o n (see. also Robinson 1 995b). Like Shanks and St. John ( 1 994), and
classification tasks when they are given instructions to follow rules Whittlesea and Darken ( 1 993), I have taken the position that there
1 04 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 1 05

is no evidence that what is learned during implicit learning distinguishing child and adult learning of languages, but no longer
conditions is unconscious. Information is processed explicitly, clearly dissociable processes once maturational constraints (Long
following the procedure of attending, noticing and rehearsal in 1 990) make access to ontogenetically earlier evolved implicit
memory described in Chapter 3 . However, different learning learning mechanisms problematic for the adult, and once domain­
conditions are likely to lead to differential reliance on strategies for independent problem-solving procedures become established in the
accumulating knowledge of the stimulus domain. The tasks to be adult.
used in this study exemplify such differences. The task instructions
for the implicit memorization condition facilitate the development 4.3. 5 Summary
of a knowledge base in which conscious memory for strings and In summary, the Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis is based o n
substrings plays a large part. Task instructions for the incidental three claims which have been motivated by the discussion o f
condition require subjects to pay greater attention to coding and implicit and explicit learning in the preceding chapters. (i) I t
remembering propositional meaning than instructions for other incorporates Schmidt's noticing hypothesis by claiming that
conditions. In contrast, the task instructions for the rule-search and noticing, under any condition, is necessary to learning (see section
instructed conditions facilitate the development of a knowledge base 3 .2. 7). (ii) It assumes that learning under any condition results in
in which the inductive search for, or deductive application of, the development of a knowledge base in which rule-governed
abstract configurational relations between syntactic categories plays abstract knowledge, as well as the accumulation of memories for
a large part. Each of these conditions, however, is likely to promote specific instances, plays a part (see section 4 . 1 .2.3). (iii) It does not
a blend of the available means for constructing representations, just assume that different memory systems responsible for the contents
as different memory tasks involve a blend of data-driven and of implicitly and explicitly learned knowledge, or that different
conceptually driven processing, though facilitating and emphasizing systems play fundamentally different roles in conscious and
one or the other through their task instructions. There is no reason unconscious learning and memory retrieval (see section 3 .3 .6). This
to believe that data-driven and conceptually driven processing, or hypothesis, of course, has been largely based on the evidence_ o f
the processing requirements of the different tasks used in the controlled experimental research into artificial grammar learmng,
present study, lead to the development of distinct knowledge and studies of implicit memory for words and visual objects. There
sources either, or that they are related to distinct processing is a lack of equivalently controlled experimental research findings in
systems differentially regulated by awareness. Rather, they are the domain of natural L2 learning which could be used to either
mutually compensatory strategies for fulfilling task demands and are support or refute this hypothesis. With this in mind the present
coordinated by the attentional mechanisms described in Chapter 3 . study, described in the following section, examines the extent t o
The Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis, it must be noted, is which Reber's methodology for studying implicit learning is
based on evidence of adult learning. It may be, of course, that generalizable to the context of SLA.
implicit and explicit learning are more clearly dissociated in child
learning, though there has been little experimental research into 4.4 The present study
implicit learning processes in precritical period child language The present study addresses the issue of implicit learning within th·e
learners, as noted in Chapter 1 , which could be used to confirm or context of the induction of rules of a natural language by adult L2
refute this speculation. The Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis is learners. The aim of the study was to examine the extent to which
thus a logical complement of B ley-Vroman's Fundamental evidence of learning is similar under Reber's and Krashen' s
Difference Hypothesis ( 1 989) which holds that child and adult conditions, and therefore the extent to which it is possible t o
language learning are distinct, the one allowing implicit access t o generalize conclusions about the role o f consciousness i n learning
Universal Grammar and associated domain specific mechanisms for using Reber' s experimental methodology, which involves learning
language learning, the other mediated by conscious problem-solving the rules regulating letter strings generated by an artificial finite­
using general, domain independent, cognitive mechanisms. In this state grammar, to more ecologically valid L2 learning conditions.
acconnt_ imnlicit and exnlicit 1earnin2: would be wavs of
1 06 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 1 07

Krashen ( 1 982, 1 985) and Reber ( 1 989, 1 993) make similar trammg set exemplars to the new transfer set exemplars was
claims about the extent of learning possible under these conditions. therefore assumed to demonstrate the extent of learning. The
Complex rules are more likely to be learned under the implicit dependent measures for performance on the transfer set were speed
condition for Reber, and under the incidental condition for Krashen, and accuracy of response, with fast accurate responses to a
than under explicit instructed or rule-search conditions. Both Reber grammaticality judgment test in the transfer phase being taken t o
and Krashen agree that simple rules can be learned under explicit indicate successful learning o f the rules.
conditions if the structural correlates of the rules are made salient. The issue of the influence of language learning aptitude on the
To examine these claims 1 04 adult learners of ESL were exposed t o ability to induce rules under all conditions is also addressed in the
structures conforming to simple and complex rules of English under present study. Reber has claimed that implicit learning is insensitive
four experimental training conditions. These conditions were: (i) an to individual differences in cognitive ability ( 1 989, 1 992; Reber,
implicit condition in which subjects were instructed simply t o Walkenfield & Hernstadt 1 99 1 ), whereas explicit learning is
remember presented sentences; (ii) an incidental condition in which sensitive to the influence of such abilities. Similarly Krashen
subjects were instructed to answer comprehension questions based on ( 1 98 l b, 1 982) has claimed that measures of aptitude access those
the sentences; (iii) an explicit rule-search condition in which cognitive faculties responsible for learning but not for acquisition.
subjects were instructed to search for the rules underlying the The present study used scores on two subtests of the Modern
sentences; and (iv) an explicit instructed condition in which subjects Language Aptitude Test (MLAT)(Carroll & Sapon 1 959): paired­
were shown rules, then asked to relate them to the sentences. associate learning, a measure of explicit memory, and words-in­
Two of these conditions, the implicit condition and the explicit sentences, a measure of grammatical sensitivity, as a test of Reber's
rule-search condition, replicated conditions used by Reber ( 1 969, and Krashen' s claims. These were administered prior to the
1 976, 1 989) in research into the implicit learning of artificial computer training sessions.
finite-state grammars. These were matched by two parallel Finally, written reports of the extent to which subjects were
conditions, the incidental condition and the explicit instructed aware of or looking for rules in the four experimental conditions
condition, which were judged to have greater ecological validity for were elicited from subjects, in their own language, in a debriefing
L2 learners. It is these latter two conditions that are assumed in session. The materials used, and the procedures followed in
most of the discussion of the role of consciousness in SLA. In administering them, are described in sections 5 .2 and 5.3 of th_e
particular Krashen ( 1 98 l a, 1 982, 1 985, 1 994) has claimed that following chapter. Sections 4.4. 1 and 4.4.2 below summarize the
language 'acquisition' takes place incidentally as a result o f research design and the research hypotheses for the study.
processing input during meaning-oriented tasks with n o conscious
focus on form. In contrast, Krashen's 'learning' process was 4.4.1 Summary of the research design
simulated by the explicit instructed condition in which subjects were The following factors are included in the present study:
required to monitor input using consciously learned rules. (a) Learning condition, with four levels: (i) implicit condition; (ii)
The rules presented during training in the explicit instructed incidental condition; (iii) explicit rule-search condition; and (iv)
condition were of different orders of complexity. Sentence tokens explicit instructed condition, a between subjects factor.
corresponding to these rules were presented during training in all (b) Rules to be learned, with two levels of difficulty: simple versus
conditions, exactly matched across conditions for order o f complex, a within-subject factor.
presentation. Only the instructional treatments differed. The rules (c) Two measures of language learning aptitude: paired-associate
from which the sentence tokens were derived, and which were learning and grammatical sensitivity.
themselves presented in the explicit instructed condition, were
identified following the procedures described in 2.4. A transfer 4. 4. 2 Research hypotheses
session followed the training session during which new sentences, o f Hypothesis I. The implicit and incidental conditions will pattern
the same type as those presented i n the training sessions, were together in all comparisons and the explicit rule-search and explicit
presented. The ability to generalize from instruction on the specific instructed conditions will pattern together in all comparisons.
1 08 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 1 09

Hypothesis 2. Performance on complex structures in the transfer Reber have claimed that learning conditions that involve a
session will be faster and more accurate for subjects trained under conscious focus on form will produce inferior learning effects,
implicit and incidental conditions than for subjects trained under relative to an implicit condition (in the case of Reber) and an
explicit rule-search and explicit instructed conditions. incidental condition (in the case of Krashen) where there is no such
Hypothesis 3. Performance on simple rules will be superior t o conscious focus on form. This difference will be most pronounced
performance on complex rules under all conditions using measures where the stimulus domain is complex. However, Reber claims that
of speed and accuracy of response. even in the case of a simple stimulus domain, where explicit
Hypothesis 4. In all conditions subjects will attest to being more instruction can be effective, implicit learning will be at least equally
aware of simple rules than of complex rules during the debriefing effective. If implicit learning and incidental learning display similar
sess10n. effects then both conditions should produce evidence of learning
Hypothesis 5. The variance in performance under implicit and superior to that of explicit conditions on complex rules, and should
incidental conditions will be equivalent and less than the variance in pattern together in performance on simple rules.
performance on rule-search and instructed conditions, which will
also be equivalent. Hypothesis 2: Performance on complex structures. The second
Hypothesis 6. Measures of language learning aptitude will predict hypothesis was that implicit and incidental conditions would be
differences in performance under the explicit rule-search and clearly superior to the two explicit conditions in the case o f
explicit instructed conditions but not under the implicit or learning complex rules. This derives from Reber ( 1 989:223):
incidental conditions. "implicit processing of complex materials has an advantage over
explicit processing," and from Krashen ( 1 982:98): "only 'easy'
4.4.3 Rationale for the research hypotheses rules are learnable".

Hypothesis 1: The relationship of implicit, incidental, rule­ Hypothesis 3: Performance on simple structures. The third
search and instructed conditions. The first hypothesis relates hypothesis was that performance on simple rules would be superior
Reber's implicit and explicit rule-search conditions to the incidental to performance on complex rules under all conditions, as
and explicit instructed conditions described by SLA theorists and demonstrated by measures of speed and accuracy of performance.
researchers. The implicit and incidental conditions are alike in not This follows from the classification of the rules and structures by
requiring a conscious focus on the grammatical form of the stimuli experienced teachers described in 2.4.
presented during training. In the implicit condition the task
instructions are simply to remember co-occurring words, whereas in Hypothesis 4: The relationship between rule complexity,
the incidental condition the task instruction is to read for meaning. learning condition and awareness. Hypothesis 4 predicts that in all
Paradis ( 1 994:395) suggests that the implicit and incidental learning conditions subjects would be more likely to attest to awareness of
conditions result in identical knowledge bases: "Implicit competence rules regulating simple structures than rules regulating complex
refers to the knowledge inferred from individuals' performance . . . [it structures. Verbalizability is a way of operationalising awareness and
is] . .. acquired incidentally . . . [and] . . . stored implicitly". To examine of identifying the objects of awareness. It is clear, though, that
the similarity of learning under implicit and incidental conditions it awareness need not imply the ability to verbalize. One can be aware
was hypothesized that the two conditions would pattern together in of something, without being able to put it into words. In the present
all comparisons. study, without time pressure, and using their L 1 , subjects were asked
In contrast to the above two conditions, both the rule-search and to describe any rules that they had been aware of during training.
instructed conditions involve a conscious focus on form. In the Verbalizability is thus used as a measure of awareness, and as
latter this involves applying a previously learned rule during evidence for claims made about the relationship of awareness to rule
training, and in the former it involves a search for the rules complexity. Reber ( 1 989:220) claims that even in the implicit
underlying sentences presented during training. Both Krashen and condition subjects will become aware of rules if they are salient, and
1 10 Implicit learning, memory and knowledge Implicit learning, memory and knowledge 111

that saliency will be induced by the simplicity of the stimulus Hypothesis 6: The relationship between learning condition and
domain: "A rich and complex stimulus domain is a prerequisite for aptitude. Reber, Walkenfield and Hernstadt ( 1 99 1 ) have also
the occurrence of implicit learning. If the system in use is too claimed that the degree of success achieved under implicit and
simple, or if the code can be broken by conscious effort, then one explicit conditions is differentially sensitive to differences in
will not see implicit processes". Similarly, in the rule-search relevant cognitive abilities, with measures of individual differences
condition Reber claims that subjects will only become aware of in such abilities predicting performance under explicit conditions,
simple rules. Complex rules can only be learned implicitly and will but not affecting performance under implicit conditions: "Implicit
not be accessible to awareness. Although there is no specific functions should operate largely independently of standard measures
motivation for the remaining conditions, it seems obvious that of cognitive capability" ( 1 99 1 :888). Similarly Krashen ( 19 8 1 b) has
subjects will be more likely to be aware of simple rules than o f claimed that aptitude batteries measure the abilities that are drawn
complex rules in the incidental condition. O f course, in the explicit on in ' learning,' but not those that are activated during
instructed condition subjects have been taught both rules, but they 'acquisition' . To examine this claim the present study used
are more likely to forget, or have an imperfect recollection of the performance on two subtests of the MLAT(Carroll & Sapon 1 95 9),
complex rule than of the simple rule. the words-in-sentences test (a measure of grammatical sensitivity)
and the paired-associates test (a measure of memory for foreign
Hypothesis 5: The relationship between learning condition and vocabulary). The final hypothesis was that performance on these
variance in performance. The fifth hypothesis concerns the two subtests would correlate significantly with differences in
relationship of variance in performance to learning under all performance under rule-search and instructed conditions, but n ot
conditions. The hypothesis is that there will be greater population with differences in performance under implicit and incidental
variance when learning takes place under the rule-search and conditions.
instructed conditions, compared to the implicit and incidental
conditions. This hypothesis is motivated by Reber (Reber,
Walkenfield & Hernstadt 1 99 1 ) and Zobl ( 1 992). Reber et al.
( 1 99 1 :888) claim that, "fewer individual differences and smaller
population variances are to be expected when comparing the covert
and unconscious with the overt and conscious functions". Likewise,
Zobl ( 1 992), in a meta-analysis of standard deviations of responses
to items on a variety of grammaticality judgment tests, claims that
performance that engages unconscious, tacit linguistic knowledge
will be more uniform, and display smaller population variances
across the species, than performance which engages learned
encyclopedic knowledge. Zobl's claim that scores will be more
variable where learning is taking place and more uniform when
learners are drawing on the tacit, acquired system, is apparently
corroborated by evidence from VanPatten ( 1 988) regarding the
higher standard deviations for the performance of a number of rule­
focused instructional groups than for meaning-focused instructional
groups on post treatment grammaticality judgment tests. Zobl
( 1 992, 1 995) argues, like Krashen ( 1 9 8 1 b, 1 982), that this evidence
of uniformity of performance is the result of unconscious
acquisition processes facilitated by the meaning-focused treatments.
Chapter 5

Methodology For The Present Study

5. 0 Introduction
The present chapter describes the subjects for the study, the data
collection procedures followed, the pretest materials and computer
training and testing programs used, and details of the analyses
performed to address the research hypotheses listed in section 4.4.2.

5.1 Subjects
Subjects for the study were 1 04 non-native speakers of English,
enrolled in English language programs at four institutions in Hawai' i
during the period from October 1 993 to February 1 994. Subjects
were predominantly native speakers of Japanese. The programs
were the STEP-UP program at Tokai University at Honolulu, the
NICE program at the University of Hawai'i, and the English
language programs of the Japan America Institute of Management
Science (JAIMS) and Kansai Gaidai Junior College, both located in
Hawai'i Kai on the outskirts of Honolulu. These institutions were
selected as subject sources because of the relative homogeneity o f
their student populations. Tokai University and Kansai Gaidai are
both Japanese owned junior colleges which offer credit courses in
English and English language programs for those students needing t o
improve basic English ability before entering the credit programs.
Students at Tokai University tend to be from Japan, in their late
teens and early twenties. The Japanese students have all completed
school level education in Japan, including seven years of formal
classroom instruction in English. Students at Kansai Gaidai are all
native speakers of Japanese, and the group who participated in the
present study were all aged between eighteen and nineteen. Like the
students at Tokai they have all completed school level education in
Japan, including seven years of formal classroom instruction in
English. The students from JAIMS who participated in the present
study were all from a group enrolled in a management training
course. They were graduates from universities or colleges in Japan in
1 14 Methodology for the study Methodology for the study 1 15

their early to mid twenties. Students from the NICE program were classes were used during any period of data collection, and no more
predominantly native speakers of Japanese, in their early to mid than three subjects completed the computer training sessions at any
twenties, who had come to Hawai'i to improve their conversational one time, since the individual attention of the researcher was needed
English. Initial analyses showed subjects from all source institutions for each subject during the early familiarization phase of the
to be performing similarly under all conditions. training sessions to ensure that the task directions were fully
A small number of non-Japanese native speakers recruited from understood.
Tokai University and the NICE program were also included in the
subject pool (five Chinese and five Korean) and randomly assigned 5.2.1 Pretesting and aptitude testing
to the experimental conditions. These subjects also reported Subjects first completed a grammaticality judgment pretest. This
themselves to have received between six and eight years of formal consisted of the first thirty items of the grammaticality judgment
schooling in English before coming to Hawai'i to enroll in English test used in the pilot study to identify unfamiliar structures described
programs. Since no hypotheses were motivated by claims about in Chapter 2, section 2.4 (Appendix A). Subjects were told to circle
specific differences in proficiency levels, no attempt was made to either 'grammatical,' 'ungrammatical,' or ' not sure' beneath each
control for proficiency other than by selecting students from low t o sentence. They were told to make only one answer and to ignore
high intermediate level classes within the source institutions. The what they thought might be punctuation or spelling errors in
homogeneity of the subject pool was maintained to the extent that making their decision. The procedure was illustrated for them using
no beginning or extremely advanced subjects were used. What an example sentence. If subjects indicated uncertainty about the
differences there might have been between the proficiency levels o f meaning of any of the terms after this example had been completed
subjects included in the study were assumed not to have unduly the researcher attempted to further clarify them. Subjects then
affected performance on any particular condition, since all subjects completed the test. They were given as much time as they needed
were randomly assigned. Details of subjects included in the study, and generally took about five minutes. If subjects circled
including age, gender, native country, L l , and condition assigned are 'ungrammatical' for the sentences corresponding to the simple and
given in Appendix K. complex rules, they were selected for inclusion in the study. These
Subjects were recrnited during visits to classes arranged by the were 'Into the house ran John' and 'Where the cheese is is in the
Directors of the source institutions. During these visits the nature of basket not in the bag' . More extensive testing of their knowledge o f
the research was briefly described as research into the use of these rules was avoided since drawing the subjects' attention at the
computers to help language learners. The demands in terms of time, pretest stage to the structures to be learned during the treatments
pre-tests, treatment and post-tests were explained. Subjects were would have threatened the validity of the study, since both the
offered $ 1 0 in return for their participation. Subjects were implicit and incidental conditions required that the subjects not be
subsequently selected for inclusion in the study from amongst those aware of or be looking for grammatical rules in the training sessions.
who volunteered on the basis of the results of a pretest showing It was felt that the other distractor items in the pretest would mask
them to be unfamiliar with the rules to be presented in the study. the relevant items and divert attention from them, and that the
following aptitude tests would further disguise the structures that
5.2 Data collection procedures were the focus of the treatments. Taken together with the evidence
The data collection took place on site at Tokai University, JAlMS of the pilot study reported in section 2.4. 1 , which confirmed these
and Kansai Gaidai College. At Tokai University data collection took structures and their associated rules to be unfamiliar to a comparable
place in a computer laboratory equipped with Macintosh computers. group of subjects, it was felt that this pretest evidence of rule
At JAIMS and Kansai Gaidai the researcher brought a Macintosh knowledge alone was sufficient for subject selection.
computer and set it up in a quiet, convenient location. The Following the pretest the subjects selected for the experimental
researcher met students from the NICE program at an appointed treatment were asked to complete two aptitude tests. The memory
time and place on the University of Hawai' i campus and took them test consisted of a list of twenty English and Kurdish paired­
to an office equipped with two Macintosh computers. No intact associates. It was explained to the subjects that the Kurdish word
1 16 Methodology for the study Methodology for the study 1 17

meant the same as the paired English word in another language and replaced by the follow up question. This remained on screen until
they were instructed to try and memorize the pairings for a the response keystroke, after which the feedback appeared o n
subsequent memory test. To aid them in this task a number of words screen. This remained on screen until the 'press any key t o
were given on a practice sheet and they were instructed to search continue' prompt was followed, whereupon the next stimulus
the two columns of pairs to match each word with its associate. sentence appeared for ten seconds. The follow up questions differed
They were told that when they finished this they were to continue according to the treatment that was being delivered. The stimulus
studying the vocabulary and memorizing the pairings. They were presentation, follow up question, and feedback sequences for each
given two minutes to memorize the vocabulary, and then asked to condition are described below in sections 5 .2.3-5.2.6. The complete
complete the twenty four item multiple-choice test. In this test a training set of sentence stimuli and follow up questions for each
Kurdish word was given together with a choice of five English condition are given in Appendix H. In each condition the stimuli
words. Subjects had to choose the correct English equivalent for were the same matched sequence of forty sentences, twenty
each of the twenty four Kurdish items. The time limit was four generated by a simple rule and twenty generated by a complex rule.
minutes and the material used was from the short form of the
MLAT (Carroll & Sapon 1 959). 5.2.3 The implicit learning condition
The remaining aptitude test was the words-in-sentences test of In the training phase for the implicit learning condition, subjects
grammatical sensitivity, also taken from the short form of the were first given an opportunity to familiarize themselves with the
MLAT. This is a test of the ability to identify analogies between demands of the task by reading an explanation of the procedure
the grammatical function of underlined words in two sentences. The they were to follow on the computer screen (see Appendix C). A t
procedure for answering these questions was explained to each this point the researcher observed and answered questions about the
student by the researcher using the examples on the cover page of procedure. When the task procedure had been explained to their
the test. When the subjects indicated they understood the task they satisfaction the familiarization session ended and the first of two
were allowed to turn the page and complete the test. Subjects were training trials began on the computer. In all conditions the second
told they could take up to forty minutes to complete this test, trial was a repetition of the first. During each trial subjects were
though they generally took much less time than this. presented with sentences conforming to simple or complex rules
and instructed to remember them. After presentation of each
5.2.2 Assignment to experimental conditions stimulus sentence subjects were required to respond yes or no to a
After completing the pretest and the aptitude tests subjects were question asking them if a particular sequence of two words had been
randomly assigned to one of the four conditions referred t o used in the previously presented stimulus sentence. They were
previously: an implicit condition, an incidental condition, an instructed to respond using the keys 'c' for correct, or yes, and ' m '
explicit rule-search condition and an explicit instructed condition. for mistake, or no. They were given feedback in the form o f a
All conditions consisted of two phases, a training and a transfer correct/ incorrect message after each response.
phase. The training phase for each condition consisted of an initial
task familiarization phase, in which the demands of the training 5.2.4 The incidental learning condition
task were explained to the subject on the computer screen. At this In the training phase for the incidental learning condition subjects
point the researcher was available to help clarify procedures and were also first given an opportunity to familiarize themselves with
answer questions. After familiarizing themselves with the task t o the demands of the task by reading an explanation of the procedure
their own and the researcher's satisfaction, subjects began the first they were to follow on the computer screen (Appendix D). At this
of the two training trials. Each trial consisted of a sequence of forty point the researcher observed and answered questions about the
timed stimulus presentations, together with follow up questions t o procedure. When the task procedure had been explained to their
each stimulus requiring a yes/ n o response, and feedback to the satisfaction, the familiarization session ended and the first training
response. Stimulus sentences were timed to remain on screen for ten trial began on the computer. After presentation of each stimulus
seconds, after which they disappeared from the screen and were sentence subjects were presented with a question about the
1 18 Methodology for the study Methodology for the study 1 19

propositional content of the sentence they had just seen, to which comp_uter �creen disp�ays presented during the familiarization phase
they had to respond by typing yes or no by using the 'c', for correct a�e given m Appendix F. As can be seen in Appendix F, (screen
and 'm' for mistake keys, as in the implicit condition. As in the displays 2 and 6) the familiarization phase drew attention to four
implicit condition they were also given feedback in the form of a key points regarding each of the rules. These related to violations
correct/ incorrect message after each response. of the grammaticality of sentences which were constitutive of the
ungrammatical types presented during the transfer phase (see 5 . 3 .4
5.2.5 The explicit rule-search condition and _s . � .5). After subjects in this condition completed the
. _ _
As in the above conditions, in the training phase for the rule-search famihanzatton exercise the training set began. Subjects were told
condition subjects were first given an opportunity to familiarize that they could consult the written versions of these rules in
themselves with the demands of the task by reading an explanation answer�ng ques�io_ns duri�g the computerized training session. During
of the procedure they were to follow on the computer screen e�ch tnal of trammg, as m the other conditions, subjects first saw a
(Appendix E). The researcher observed and answered questions stimulus sentence then were asked a question requiring a yes/ n o
about the procedure until the task procedure had been explained t o response. These questions were metalinguistic and related the
their satisfaction. The familiarization session then ended and the stimulus sentence to the explanation of the rule given in the
first of the two training trials began on the computer. In the familiarization phase, e.g., 'Did the verbs used agree in tense?' 'Did
familiarization phase for the explicit rule-search condition subjects the �ubject of the sentence come after the verb?' Each question
were told that the sentences they were about to see exemplified reqmred a yes/no response. As in the implicit and incidental
rules of English. They were instructed to try and identify the rules conditions subjects were given feedback to each response in the
underlying the sentences and told that they would be asked t o form of a correct/ incorrect message which remained on screen until
describe the rules they had found at the end of the training session. they responded to the 'press any key to continue' prompt.
After presentation of each stimulus sentence during training they
were asked questions about the rules they were searching for. These 5.2. 7 The transfer phase
questions were phrased so as to require a yes/ no response (e.g., The transfer pha� e was the same for all conditions and immediately
.
Have you identified the rules yet? Are you still looking for the followed the trammg phase. After finishing training subjects were
rules? Do you know the rules for sentences like this?) which they told that they were to complete a grammaticality judgment test o n
were to make by using the keys 'c' for yes, or correct, and 'm' for the co�puter and instructed to respond as quickly as they could t o
no or mistake. Since these questions were open ended, unlike the each stimulus by pressing the ' c ' key if the sentence was a
questions in the other conditions, no feedback was given after each grammatically acceptable one and 'm' if it was a grammatically
yes/ no response, and the next stimulus appeared on screen unacceptable one. Subjects first completed a familiarization task on
immediately following the response keystroke. the co�puter before seeing the randomized set of twenty
grammatical and twenty ungrammatical sentences (Appendix I).
5.2. 6 The explicit instructed learning condition Accuracy for each subject's responses, together with the time taken
The familiarization phase for the explicit instructed condition was to make each response, measured in milliseconds, was recorded by
similar in form to that for the other conditions. Subjects were first the software used to write the training and transfer set programs.
asked to read through the two rules that were the focus of the study
(see Figures 5 and 6 in section 2.4). The researcher explained t o 5.2.8 The post-experimental debriefing session
subjects i n this condition that they would be asked t o learn the two After completing the transfer phase subjects were given a set of five
rules, and would be able to practice them on the computer. Subjects written questions and instructed to answer them in the space
were then asked to read the rules as the researcher explained them . provided using their L l . Answers to three of the questions were used
The key points of the explanations of these rules were rehearse.cl �o �ode the a�areness of subjects for the present study, these were;
during the familiarization phase for the explicit instructed Did you notice any rules of grammar when you did the first two
condition. The explanation, examples, and the sequence o f exercises on the computer?' , ' Were you looking for grammar rules
1 20 Methodology for the study Methodology for the study 121

when did the first two exercises on the computer?', and 'Can you o f the MLAT (Carroll & Sapon 1 959). For each o f these tests,
say what the rules are?' On the basis of these responses subjects subjects responded by circling answers on the test materials.
were coded as having noticed, or not; as having looked for rules, or
not; and as able to verbalize rules, or not. Answers were later 5.3.3 Rules
translated back into English for the purposes of data analysis. During the instructed condition, subjects were shown written
Despite the researcher's instructions, some subjects chose t o explanations of the simple and complex rules which they were asked
respond using English. The questions were written in Japanese for to read and consult during the training sessions (see Figures 5 and 6).
Japanese subjects. The few non-Japanese subjects were given English
versions of the questions. In each case the researcher explained the 5.3.4 Training set sentences
questions in English to the subject. If asked for clarification the The training set sentences consisted of twenty grammatical
researcher responded by giving necessary examples, or by reminding structures illustrating the simple rule and twenty grammatical
subjects of the experimental procedures they had just followed in structures illustrating the complex rule (Appendix G). These were
the particular condition to which they had been assigned. randomly ordered for presentation in the four conditions.

5.3 Materials (i) Simple rule training set sentences. The twenty sentences
illustrating the simple rule were made up of four tokens of five
5.3.1 Computer materials types. These types were the same as the types used in the
All training and transfer sessions were completed at Macintosh explanation of the rule given to teachers to rate for complexity and
computer terminals using standard qwerty keyboards. The given to subjects to study in the instructed condition. Type 1 were
Macintosh computers used were SE, SEII, and Hsi models. The sentences in the order, subject, verb, location phrase (e.g., Pau/
stimulus sentences were presented in black, on a white background, drove across the city). Type 2 were sentences in the order location
and written in size 1 2 Geneva font. The training and transfer phrase, verb subject (e.g., Up the road came Eric). Type 3
programs which recorded response time and accuracy were written sentences were in the order location phrase, subject, verb (e.g.,
using Mindlab software, version 2 . 1 (Meike 1 988). This software Round the track Bill raced). Type 4 sentences were in the order
allows the researcher to construct programs which consist of a series subject, verb, time phrase (e.g., Lori ate at five o 'clock), and Type 5
of stimuli, in the form of a pictures, numerals, words, or sentences. sentences were in the order time phrase, subject, verb (e.g., In the
Each stimulus can be timed to remain on screen from any period afternoon Elaine arrived).
between 1 /60th of a second and 1 8 seconds, measured in units o f
1 /60th o f a second. Presentation time for the stimuli presented (ii) Complex rule training set sentences. Similarly, the twenty
during training in the present study was fixed at 1 0 seconds. In the grammatical sentences illustrating the complex rule were also made
transfer session stimuli remained on screen until the response up of four tokens of five types. These types were also motivated by
keystroke. The time taken for each subject's responses during the description of the complex rule presented to teachers to rate for
training and transfer sessions was recorded in milliseconds, together complexity. Type 1 were sentences containing two conjoined
with accuracy for each response, which was recorded as 0, for clauses which contrasted the locations of two things (e.g., The girl
incorrect, and 1, for correct. Additionally the subject number, trial plays in the park and the boy plays in the yard). Type 2 were
number, and the key used to make each response were also recorded. sentences with singular subjects, and two forms of the verb 'to be'
(e.g., Where Sue is is in the car not on the boat). Type 3 were
5.3.2 Pretest and aptitude measures sentences with plural subjects, requiring agreement with a plural
The pretest materials consisted of responses to the first thirty items form of the main verb 'be' (e.g., Where Jim and Amy are is in the
used in the grammaticality judgment pilot test of rule familiarity kitchen not in the garden). Type 4 were sentences with singular
(Appendix A). The materials for the associative memory and the subjects and lexical main verbs (e.g., Where the President lives is in
words-in-sentences subtests were photocopied from the short form Washington not in Chicago), and Type 5 sentences contained plural
/
1 22 Methodology for the study Methodology for the study 1 23

subjects and lexical main verbs (e.g., Where Steve and Mark swim is the condition that the form of the verb 'be', following the m ain
in the pool not in the ocean). Additionally two of the four tokens verb must always be singular, and need not agree in number with the
illustrating the complex Type 2, 3, 4 and 5 sentences consisted of subject (e.g., Where Carl writes are at a desk not on the floor.I Where
the optional phrase beginning with 'not', described at the end of the the soldiers fight are in Europe not in Hawaii). The third
complex rule. In writing tokens of each of these types, only simple ungrammatical type violates the condition that the two verbs used
vocabulary and common English names were used. No location must agree in tense (e.g., Where the bird is was in the sky not in the
phrases or lexical verbs were repeated in either simple or complex sea). The fourth ungrammatical type violates the condition that
sentence tokens. sentences of this kind must begin with ' Where' (e.g., What the King
lives is in a palace not in an apartment). The final ungrammatical
5.3. 5 Transfer set sentences type violates the condition that when the second location is added
The transfer set sentences were made up of twenty grammatical and to contrast with the focused location, it must be introduced by
twenty ungrammatical sentences (Appendix J). The grammatical 'not'(e.g., Where Mark works is in America in Japan). There were
sentences were two new tokens of each of the ten types presented two tokens of each of these five ungrammatical types making a
during the training sessions. The ungrammatical sentences were total of ten ungrammatical sentences. All sentences in the transfer
systematic violations of these grammatical types, as described in the session were therefore either new examples of the previously
rules presented to teachers to rate for complexity, and to subjects to encountered grammatical sentence types, or new examples o f
study in the instructed condition. violations o f those sentence types with regard to specific conditions
described above. As with the training session, only common verbs,
(i) Simple rule transfer set sentences. Two new tokens of each of nouns and names were used, and no verbs, nouns, or location phrases
the five simple rule sentence types presented during training were were repeated across the forty sentences presented in the transfer
presented in the transfer set. In addition to these ten grammatical session.
sentences, there were ten ungrammatical sentences which violated
conditions on the rule. These were divided into two types, sentences 5.3. 6 Debriefing materials
in which subject-verb inversion occurs following a time phrase (e.g., The debriefing questions were written on two pieces of paper, in
On Saturday night danced Charlie), and sentences in which subject­ Japanese, by a native speaker of Japanese, and in English by the
verb inversion occurs preceding time and location phrases (e.g., researcher. Enough space was left beneath each question for subjects
Crawled Mick through the tunnel). These are violations of the to complete their answers, though if they felt they needed to they
simple rule which states that subject-verb inversion can only occur were told to use the blank pages on the reverse of the questionnaire
when location phrases are fronted. There were five tokens of each to complete their answers.
of these two types of violation, making a total of ten
ungrammatical sentences. 5.4 Analyses
Data was analysed using the statistical packages ' Statview SE +
(ii) Complex rule transfer set sentences. As with the simple rule, Graphics,' and 'SuperANOVA' available for use on Macintosh
two new examples of each of the five grammatical sentence types computers. A Macintosh Hsi was used for the data analysis.
illustrating the complex rule were presented during the transfer To address Hypothesis 1 , that performance on the implicit
session. In addition to these ten grammatical sentences, ten condition and incidental condition would pattern together, and that
ungrammatical sentences were presented. These ten ungrammatical performance on the rule-search condition and the instructed
sentences were of five types, corresponding to violations of the condition would pattern together, two repeated measure ANOVA o f
complex rule that were explained in the rule. The first accuracy and reaction time o f responses to all transfer set sentences
ungrammatical type violates the condition that subject-verb were performed. Planned comparisons between the implicit and
inversion is not possible following ' Where' (e.g., Where is Bill is in incidental conditions, and between the rule-search and instructed
the country not in the city). The second ungrammatical type violates conditions were made. A third planned comparison between the
/
124 Methodology for the study

joint performance of the implicit and incidental conditions versus


the rule-search and instructed conditions was also performed. The
planned comparisons were followed by post-hoe comparisons of all
training conditions. Chapter 6
To address Hypothesis 2, that performance on complex rules
would be superior for those trained in the implicit and incidental
condition, compared to performance by those trained in the rule­ Analyses And Results
search and instructed conditions, the factor Rule was removed from
the two repeated measure ANOVA. The ANOVA for accuracy and
reaction time on easy and hard rules were then followed by the same
series of planned and post-hoe comparisons used to address 6. 0 Introduction
Hypothesis 1 . This chapter describes the analyses performed to address the six
To address Hypothesis 3 , that performance on easy rules would hypotheses for the present study which were listed at the end o f
be superior to performance on hard rules for all conditions, the Chapter 4 . The presentation o f results is organized around the four
factor Condition was removed from the two repeated measure different types of data used to address these hypotheses. The
ANOVA for accuracy and reaction time. The ANOVA for each accuracy and reaction time data for performance on the transfer
condition was then performed separately to identify whether the grammaticality judgment task are first described in section 6. 1 . and
factor Rule was significant. are used to address Hypotheses 1 , 2 and 3 in sections 6. 1 . 1-3 . Next,
To address Hypothesis 4, that subjects in all conditions would be the rule awareness data are described in section 6.2. These data are
more likely to attest to being aware of simple rules than of complex used to address Hypothesis 4 in section 6.2. 1 . Thirdly, the variance
rules during the postexperimental debriefing session, a Chi-square data are described in section 6.3, and are used to address Hypothesis
analysis of subjects' responses to the debriefing questionnaire was 5 in section 6.3 . 1 . Finally the aptitude data are described in section
performed. 6.4 and are used to address Hypothesis 6 in section 6.4. 1 . A number
To address Hypothesis 5, that variance in performance under of issues arising from the analyses, which are not integral to the
implicit and incidental conditions will be equal and less than the research hypotheses addressed here, are examined in more detail in
variance in performance under rule-search and instructed conditions, the following chapter, for example the effects of grammaticality
which will also be equal, six paired F-tests were performed and sentence type on reaction time and accuracy in the transfer
comparing variance in accuracy and reaction time in the transfer task, and the relationship of the awareness data to the aptitude data.
grammaticality judgment task.
To address Hypothesis 6, that measures of language learning 6.1 Accuracy and reaction time data
aptitude will be related to performance under rule-search and Accuracy and reaction time for grammaticality judgments on 4 O
instructed conditions, but not to performance under implicit or sentences presented during the transfer session were recorded.
incidental conditions, accuracy and reaction time scores for the Correct responses were scored as 1 and incorrect responses were
transfer task were correlated with scores on the aptitude tests. scored as 0. Raw scores in milliseconds were entered for response
Pearson's r was reported, together with the significance level of reaction time. Twenty six subjects in each condition made responses
each correlation. to 20 grammatical items, and 20 ungrammatical items, forming a
Given the large number of comparisons made between groups in total of 40 responses to sentence tokens. The accuracy data are
the present study the alpha level for testing the six hypotheses was thus based on correct judgments of ' grammatical' for grammatical
set at the .0 1 level of significance. sentences, and correct judgments of 'ungrammatical' for
ungrammatical sentences. For the population of 1 04 subjects the
expected frequencies for chance performance on all sentences in
training are thus 520 responses in each cell of an accurate/
126 Analyses and results Analyses and results 1 27

inaccurate two cell matrix for grammatical sentences. The same is rule is lower for all conditions than mean reaction time for
also true for ungrammatical sentences. Using the Chi-square statistic performance on the hard rule. Reaction time for both easy and hard
to calculate probabilities of accurate and inaccurate responses, a rules is faster for those trained in the rule-search condition than for
ratio of 562 accurate to 478 inaccurate responses, (JO (562) -fE those trained in the implicit, incidental or instructed conditions.
(520) = 32*32/520 = 3 .39*2 = 6.7) or an accuracy level of 5 4%, These data are also normally distributed.
(562/ 1 040) would be significantly above chance at the p<.0 1 level
of significance. Considering the easy rule and hard rule sentences
separately, a ratio of 288 accurate to 232 inaccurate responses, or Table 6. Descriptive Statistics For Transfer Test Reaction
an accuracy level of 56%, would be above chance at the p<.0 1 level. Time On Easy And Hard Rules Under Each Condition
The descriptive statistics for response accuracy to sentences
conforming to easy and hard rules, split by condition, are given in Condition/Rule Mean Reaction Time SD SE
Table 5. Mean accuracy on the easy rule is higher for all conditions
than mean accuracy on the hard rule. Accuracy on both easy and Implicit
hard rules is higher for those trained in the instructed condition than Easy rule 8 148.838 303 1 .08 5 94.444
for those in the implicit, incidental or rule-search conditions. Mean Hard rule 9958.427 3325.64 1 652.2 12
Incidental
accuracy by all conditions on easy and hard rules is above 5 6% and
Easy rule 86 1 7.8 1 7 2705.89 530.669
is therefore significantly above the level of chance. The data are Hard rule 1 0679.067 27 1 9.573 533.352
normally distributed with no excessive kurtosis or skewedness. Rule-search
Easy rule 671 6.496 2704.865 53 0.468
Hard rule 8 1 6 1 .8 8 1 2720. 1 7 533 .469
Table 5. Descriptive Statistics For Transfer Test Accuracy O n Instructed
Easy And Hard Rules Under Each Condition Easy rule 84 1 8.063 2 1 07.83 1 4 13.38
Hard rule 9980.9 2902.382 569.204
Condition/Rule Mean % Accurate SD SE

Implicit 6.1.1 Analyses for Hypothesis 1


Easy rule 66.346 1 5 .202 2.98 1 The first hypothesis of the present study is that the implicit and
Hard rule 6 1 .346 1 2.927 2.535 incidental conditions will pattern together in all comparisons, and
Incidental the rule-search and instructed conditions will 'pattern together in all
Easy rule 72.308 1 4.576 2.859 comparisons. To address this hypothesis two repeated measure
Hard rule 58.654 I 0 .729 2 . 1 04
ANOVA, for accuracy and reaction time, were carried out. Three
Rule-search
1 5 .679 3 .075
planned comparisons of the results of each ANOVA were also made.
Easy rule 69. 6 1 5
Hard rule 56.923 1 2.576 2.466 These were comparisons of the results for implicit and incidental
Instructed conditions, the rule-search and instructed conditions, and a
Easy rule 84 .6 1 5 1 3 .923 2 .73 comparison of the results for the combined implicit/incidental
Hard rule 66.923 1 3 .044 2.558 conditions versus the combined rule-search/instructed conditions.

6.1.1.1 Accuracy
The descriptive statistics for reaction time in milliseconds to all The results of the repeated measure ANOVA for accuracy are given
sentences conforming to easy and hard rules, split by condition, are in Table 7. There is a significant main effect for Condition (F
given in Table 6. Mean reaction time for performance on the easy (3/ 1 00) = 6.799, p<.O l ) and for Rule (F ( l/ 1 00) = 70.566, p<. 0 1 ).
1 28 Analyses and results Analyses and results 1 29

There is no significant main effect for Grammaticality. The means


for performance on easy and hard rules across conditions are 0 Hard rule
illustrated in Figure 1 4 . There is no significant interaction between • Easy rule
Rule and Condition. There is an almost significant interaction of
.9
Grammaticality and Condition (F (3/ 1 00) = 3 .953, p = . 0 1 04), and
a significant interaction of Rule and Grammaticality (F ( 1/ 1 00) = .85
9.253, p<.0 1 ). These interactions are discussed in section 6. 1 .2 and
in more detail in Chapter 7, since they are not integral to the >.
u .8
predictions of the main hypotheses addressed in this chapter � u
.75
Table 7. ANOVA Table For Accuracy To Transfer Set �
.......

Sentences
0 .7
"'
!a
tU
.65
Source df Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value �
......
Squares Square Q) .6
u
c 3 10.7 1 6 3 .572 6 .799 .0003
Subject(Group) 1 00 52.539 .525 .55
R 1 1 5 .63 1 1 5 .63 1 70.566 .000 1 Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
R * C 3 2. 193 .73 1 3.300 .0235
R * Subject(Group) 1 00 22 . 1 5 1 .222 condition
G 1 3 .877 3 . 877 5 .232 .0243
G * C 3 8.789 2 .930 3 .953 . 0 1 04 Figure 14. Mean Accuracy For Performance On Easy And
G * Subject(Group) 1 00 74. 1 09 .74 1 Hard Rules Under Each Condition
s 9 2 1 .925 2.436 1 4.706 .000 1
S * C 27 5 .964 .22 1 1 .334 . 1 200
S * Subject(Group) 900 149.086 . 1 66
1 4.779 4.779 9.253 .0030
The result of the planned comparison of the implicit and
R* G
R* G * C 3 1 .945 .648 1 .255 .2940 incidental conditions shows no significant difference in accuracy
R * G * Subject(Group) 1 00 5 1 .65 1 .5 1 7 between these groups. However there is a significant difference
R * S 9 12.583 1 .398 8 .077 .000 1 between the rule-search and instructed conditions (F ( 1 /5 0) =
R* S * C 27 4.4 1 0 . 1 63 .944 . 5480 1 5 .465, p<.0 1 ). There is no significant difference the combined
R * S * Subject(Group) 900 1 5 5 .782 . 1 73 implicit/incidental conditions and rule-search/instructed conditions .
G * S 9 9.462 1 .05 1 6.91 1 .000 1 As Figure 1 4 shows, the difference between the rule-search and the
G * S * C 27 1 0 .6 1 2 .393 2. 584 .000 1 instructed condition is due to the greater accuracy of the instructed
G * S * Subject(Group) 900 1 36.90 1 . 1 52 subjects in performance on both rules. Post-hoe Scheffe
R* G * S 9 1 3 .444 1 .494 9.308 .000 1 comparisons also show the instructed condition to be significantly
R * G * S * C 27 5 .495 .204 1 .268 . 1 64 1
superior in accuracy to the implicit and the rule-search conditions
R * G * S * Subject{Grou£2 900 144.436 . 1 60
(p<.0 1 ), and an effect approaching significance for a difference
Dependent: Accuracy
between the instructed and incidental conditions (p = . 0 1 85).
Key: C Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed)
6.1.1.2 Reaction time
=

R Rule (Easy rule, Hard rule)


The results of the repeated measure ANOVA for reaction time are
=

G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical)


S = Sentence token given in Table 8. The main effect for Condition approaches
130 Analyses and results Analyses and results 131

significance (F (3/1 00) = 3 . 5 02, p = . 0 1 82) and there is a significant Table 8. ANOVA Table For Reaction Time To Transfer Set
main effect for Rule (F ( 1 /1 00) = 84.053, p<. 0 1 ). Mean reaction S e n te n c es
time for the rule-search condition is lower than for all other
conditions and mean performance on the easy rule is lower than Source df Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value
performance on the hard rule (see Figure 1 5). There is n o Squares Square
significant main effect o f Grammaticality o n reaction time, and n o c 3 2.902E9 9.673E8 3 .502 . 0 1 82
significant interactions o f Rule, Condition and Grammaticality. Subject(Group) 1 00 2.762E 1 0 2.762E8
The result of the planned comparison of reaction time for R 1 3 . 074E9 3 .074E9 84.053 .000 1
R*C 3 58301 1 26.7 l .943E7 .53 1 .66 1 8
implicit and incidentally trained subjects shows no significant
R * Subject(Group) 1 00 3 .657E9 3 .657E7
difference between these groups. However there is an effect G 1 5062 1 744.3 5 .062E7 1 .995 . 1 609
approaching an effect approaching significance for a difference G * C 3 39228742.5 l .3 08E7 .515 .6727
between these the faster rule-search condition and the instructed G * Subject(Group) 1 00 2.53 8E9 2.53 8E7
condition (F ( 1 150) = 5 . 83 5, p = . 0 1 75). There is no significant s 9 4.785E9 5.3 1 6E8 22. 1 7 1 .000 1
difference between the combined implicit/incidental versus rule­ S * C 27 77549565 1 .8 2.872E7 1 . 198 .2243
search/instructed conditions. Post-hoe Scheffe comparisons show no S * Subject(Group) 900 2 . 1 58E10 2.398E7
significant differences between any of the groups at the p<.0 1 level. R*G 1 7453 8 120.5 7.454E7 3 . 142 .0793
R*G*C 3 1 7 1 750 1 8 1 57250060.3 2.4 1 3 .07 1 1
R * G * Subject(Group) 1 00 2.372E9 2.372E7
0 Hard rule R * S 9 2.36E9 2.623E8 1 1 .986 .000 1
• Easy rule R * S * C 27 866 1 3 1 574.3 3 .208E7 1 .466 .0598
R * S * Subject(Group) 900 l .969E 1 0 2. 1 88E7
1 1 000 G * S 9 3 .592E9 3 .992E8 1 7.3 1 9 .000 1
1 0500 G * S * C 27 636080850.9 2.356E7 1 .022 .4340
<I)
G * S * Subject(Group) 900 2.074E 1 0 2.305E7
.§ 1 0000 R* G * S 9 l .952E9 2 . 1 69E8 9.58 1 .000 1
......
s:: 9500 R* G * S * C 27 3 9 1 44 1 9 8 1 .5 l .45E7 .640 .92 1 8
.9
...... R * G * S * Subject�GrouE} 900 2.038E l 0 2.264E7
<.)
ro 9000 Dependent: Reaction time

<+-< �500
0
"' Key: C = Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed)
§<I) 8000 R = Rule (Easy rule, Hard rule)
� 7500 G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical)
-
S Sentence token
Q)
=

u 7000
6500
Instructed
6.1.1.3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 1
Implicit Incidental Rule-search
The results of the analyses of accuracy and reaction time data to all
condition sentences in the transfer task shows no significant difference
between the implicit and incidental conditions. To this extent they
Figure 15. Mean Reaction Time For Performance On Easy pattern together, partially confirming Hypothesis 1 . However,
And Hard Rules Under Each Condition when the factor Rule is considered differences between the
conditions emerge. For the incidental condition there is a significant
difference in accuracy for performance on easy and hard rules,
132 Analyses and results Analyses and results 133

whereas there is no significant difference in accuracy of These results, therefore, lend only partial support to Hypothesis
performance on easy and hard rules for the implicit condition. This 1 . Whereas implicit and incidental conditions produce similar levels
result is addressed in more detail in section 6. 1 .3 . of accuracy and reaction time across all sentences in the transfer
While the implicit and incidental conditions show no significant session, there is a significant difference in accuracy of performance
differences in accuracy or reaction time, there are significant between those trained under rule-search and instructed conditions.
differences between the rule-search and instructed conditions with
respect to accuracy. They cannot, therefore, be said to pattern 6.1.2 Analyses for Hypothesis 2
together. The instructed condition is significantly more accurate Whereas the analyses for Hypothesis 1 were based on accuracy and
than the rule-search group. There is also a trend supporting the reaction time to all sentences in the training set, the analyses
superior speed of the rule-search condition relative to other performed to address Hypothesis 2 specifically distinguish
conditions. The planned comparisons show an almost significant performance on easy and hard rules. The second hypothesis states
effect for a difference between the rule-search and instructed that the implicit and incidental will be clearly superior to the rule­
conditions (p = .0 1 75). The fast performance of those in the rule­ search and instructed conditions in learning complex rules. T o
search condition may reflect the fact that they had learned little address this hypothesis the factor Rule was removed from the
during training and consequently that the guesses made during repeated measure ANOVA for accuracy and reaction time and
transfer are faster than the more calculated decisions of the separate repeated measure ANOVA were performed for accuracy
instructed condition. Both the greater accuracy of the instructed and reaction time for hard rule sentences, and for easy rule
condition and the greater speed of the rule-search condition may sentences.
also reflect transfer of specific aspects of task demands made during
training. Task demands in the rule-search condition are more similar 6.1.2.1 Complex rule accuracy
to the demands of the transfer task than are the demands of the Results of the repeated measure ANOVA for accuracy on hard rule
other conditions, since in the rule-search condition subjects are sentences shows no significant main effect for Condition, but a
required to search sentence strings simply to identify grammatical significant main effect for Grammaticality (F ( 1 / 1 00) = 1 1 .3 08 ,
rules, without attending to meaning, or memorizing the strings, or p<.0 1 ) (see Table 9 ) . The relationship o f hard rule accuracy t o
remembering previously taught rules. Both attending to meaning Grammaticality across conditions is illustrated in Figure 1 6. The
and memorizing, however, would seem likely to slow reaction time interaction between Grammaticality and Condition is almost
if these processing modes are carried over into the transfer task. significance (F (3/1 00) = 3 .722, p = . 0 1 39). All subjects are more
Similarly, transfer of the processing mode adopted during the accurate in judging grammatical sentences than they are in judging
application of rules in the instructed condition would seem likely t o ungrammatical sentences, with this difference being most
lead to slower processing o f the transfer strings, relative to a rule­ pronounced in the case of instructed subjects.
search condition, since this would involve recall of the full details of The planned comparisons show no significant difference between
the pedagogic rule presented during training. It appears to be true the implicit and incidental conditions in accuracy of performance
that the rules taught in the instructed condition are successfully on the hard rule. However, there is a significant difference between
applied to the task of discriminating grammatical from the rule-search and instructed conditions (F ( 1 /50) = 8.5 1 7, p<.O 1 ).
ungrammatical strings. However, there is a significant interaction o f There is no significant difference between the combined implicit
Grammaticality, Rule and accuracy fo r the instructed condition. and incidental versus the combined rule-search and instructed
Subjects in the instructed condition are significantly more accurate conditions. Results of Scheffe post-hoe comparisons of accuracy o n
in judging grammatical strings based on the easy rule than are the hard rule across all conditions show significant differences at the
subjects trained under other conditions, but significantly worse at p<. 0 1 level only between the instructed and rule-search conditions.
judging ungrammatical strings based on the hard rule. This result 1s
further examined in section 6 . 1 .2 and Chapter 7.
1 34 Analyses and results Analyses and results 135

Table 9. ANOVA Table For Accuracy T o Hard Rule S e ntences


0 Grammatical
In The Transfer Set
• Ungrammatical
.85 Source df Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value
Squares Square
.8 c 3 2.98 1 .994 3 .255 .0248
G'
C<! Subject(Group) 1 00 3 0 .527 .305
8
(,)
.75 G 1 8.633 8.633 1 1 .308 .OO l l
<!'. G * C 3 8.525 2 . 842 3 .722 .0 1 3 9
.7 G * Subject(Group) 100 76.342 .763

'""O .65
s 9 22 . 1 42 2.460 1 3 .02 1 .000 1
!il S * C 27 5.308 . 1 97 1 .040 .4087
::c S * Subject(Group) 900 1 70.050 . 1 89
<+-< .6
0 G * S 9 8.7 1 3 .968 5 .729 .000 1
"'
ta .55 G * S * C 27 9.706 .359 2. 1 27 .0008
tl)
G * S * Subject�GrouE} 900 1 52.0 8 1 . 1 69

.5 Dependent: Hard Rule Accuracy

u
.45 Key: C = Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed)
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical)
S = Sentence token
condition

Table 10. ANOVA Table For Accuracy To Easy Rule


Figure 16. Mean Accuracy For Performance On
Sentences In The Transfer Set
Grammatical And Ungrammatical Hard Rule Sentences
Source df Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value
Squares Square
6.1.2.2 Simple rule accuracy
c 3 9 .928 3 .309 7.494 .000 1
Results of the repeated measure ANOVA for easy rule accuracy are Subject(Group) 100 44. 1 63 .442
given in Table 1 0 . There is a significant main effect for Condition G 1 .024 .024 .048 .8276
(F (3/ 1 00) = 7.494, p<. 0 1 ) . Mean accuracy on grammatical and G * C 3 2.209 .736 1 .490 .22 1 8
ungrammatical sentences conforming to the easy rule is highest for G * Subject(Group) 100 49.4 1 7 .494
those in the instructed condition (see Figure 1 7). There is no s 9 1 2.366 1 .3 74 9. 1 72 .000 1
significant main effect for Grammaticality, and no significant S * C 27 5 .067 . 1 88 1 .253 . 1 76 1
interaction of Grammaticality and Condition. Planned comparisons S * Subject(Group) 900 1 34. 8 1 7 . 1 50
show no significant difference between implicit and incidental G * S 9 14. 193 1 .577 1 0.980 .000 1
conditions, but a significant difference between the rule-search and G * S * C 27 6.40 1 .237 1 .65 1 .0202
instructed conditions (F ( 1/50) = 1 3 .246, p<. 0 1 ). There are also G * S * Subject�GrouE2 900 1 29.256 . 1 44
significant differences between the combined implicit and incidental Dependent: Easy Rule Accuracy
conditions versus the combined rule-search and instructed conditions
(F ( 1 / 1 00) = 7 . 1 42, p<.0 1 ), though this is attributable to the
Key: C = Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed)
G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical)
contribution made by instructed subjects to the total score for the S = Sentence token
136 Analyses and results Analyses and results 137

Table 1 1 . ANOVA Table For Reaction Time To Hard Rule 0 Grammatical


Sentences In The Transfer Set
• Ungrammatical
Source df Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value
Squares Square

f
c 3 1 . 1 56E9 3 .854E8 2.727 .04 8 1 .9
Subject(Group) 100 l .4 1 3E 1 0 l .4 1 3E8
G 1 1240067 1 5.569 l .24E8 5 .373 .0225 � .85
G * C 3 145853 1 78.527 4. 862E7 2. 106 . 1 042 <l)

G * Subject(Group) 100 2.308E9 2.308E7 2 .8


s 9 2.685E9 2.983E8 1 5 . 6 1 7 .000 1 i;;>
S * C 27 720750958.33 1 2.669E7 1 .398 .0865 � .75
......
S * Subject(Group) 900 l .7 1 9E 1 0 l .9 1E7 0

G * S 9 729766843.796 8. 1 09E7 3 .983 .000 1 fj) .7


G * S * C 27 403005996.03 1 l .493E7 .733 . 8369
§
<l)

G * S * Subject�Group) 900 l .832E 1 0 2.03 6E7 :E .65


Dependent: Hard Rule Reaction time

Key: C = Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed) Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical)
S Sentence token
condition
=

Table 12. ANOVA Table For Reaction Time To Easy Rule Figure 17. Mean Accuracy For Performance On
Sentences In The Transfer Set Grammatical And Ungrammatical Easy Rule Sentences

Source df
Sum of Mean F-Value P-Value
Squares Square combined explicit conditions. The Scheffe post-hoe analyses show
c 3 l .804E9 6.01 3E8 3 .508 .01 8 1 significantly greater accuracy on easy rules in the instructed
Subject(Group) 1 00 l .7 1 4E l 0 l .7 14E8 condition compared with the implicit and rule-search conditions and
G 1 1 1 53 149.33 1 1 53 1 49.33 .044 .8337 an almost significant difference between instructed and incidental
G * C 3 65 1 25745.06 2 . 1 7 1 E7 .834 .478 1
conditions (p = .02)
G * Subject(Group) 1 00 2.602E9 2.602E7
s 9 4.46E9 4.956E8 1 8 .52 1 .000 1
S * C 27 920876267.86 3 .4 1 1 E7 1 .275 . 1 592 6.1.2.3 Complex rule reaction time
S * Subject(Group) 900 2.408E l 0 2.676E7 The results of the repeated measure ANOVA for hard rule reaction
G * S 9 4.8 1 5E9 5 .35E8 2 1 . 1 2 1 .000 1 time are given in Table 1 1 . The main effect for Condition
G * S * C 27 6245 1 6836.44 2.3 1 3E7 .9 1 3 .5936 approaches significance (F (3/1 00) = 3 . 508, p = .0 1 8 1 ). There is n o
G * S * Subject�GrouE2 900 2.28E10 2.533E7 significant main effect for Grammaticality and no significant
Dependent: Easy Rule Reaction time interaction of Grammaticality and Condition (see Figure 1 8). The
planned comparisons reveal no significant difference in reaction
Key: C = Condition (Implicit, Incidental, Rule-search, Instructed) time to hard rule sentences between implicit and incidental
G = Grammaticality (Grammatical, Ungrammatical) conditions but an almost significant difference between the rule­
S = Sentence token search and instructed conditions (F ( 1 /50) = 5 .0 1 9, p = . 0273 ).
138 Analyses and results
Analyses and results 139

There is no significant difference between the combined implicit


and incidental versus the combined rule-search and instructed 0 Grammatical
conditions. Scheffe post-hoe comparisons also show no significant
• Ungrammatical
differences between the conditions.

6.1.2.4 Simple rule reaction time


The results of the repeated measure ANOVA for reaction time to § 9000
easy rule sentences are given in Table 1 2 . There is no significant
main effect for Condition, or for Grammaticality and no significant -�
interaction of Grammaticality and Condition. As illustrated in � 8500
0
Figure 1 9 the relative speed of conditions is similar to that for e 8000
performance on the complex rule, with faster resonses by rule­ �

search learners. As with the results for the complex rule, planned µ.)
comparisons and Scheffe post-hoe tests reveal no significant 'O 7500
differences in reaction time between the conditions. §
� 7000
0 Grammatical
• Ungrammatical
.§ 1 1 000 Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed

condition

.....
1 0500
.....
()
o:s
0 1 0000 Figure 19. Mean Reaction Time For Grammatical And
i:c: Ungrammatical Easy Rule Sentences
0
;:;
.....
9500
--0
.....
o:s
::r: 9000 6.1.2.5 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 2
<.;..,
0
Hypothesis 2 states that the implicit and incidental conditions will
{/) 8500 be clearly superior to the explicit conditions in performance on
§
0 hard rules. The results of Hypothesis 1 support the claim for

-
8000 similarity between these conditions across all sentences, since there
Q) is no significant difference between them in accuracy and reaction
u 7500 time to transfer set sentences. The analyses presented above also
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed support the hypothesis that the two conditions perform similarly
on easy and hard rules since there is no significant difference
condition between them in accuracy or reaction time on easy and hard rule
sentences. However, these conditions do not perform significantly
Figure 18. Mean Reaction Time For Grammatical And better than the explicit conditions on the hard rule, either in
Ungrammatical Hard Rule Sentences accuracy or reaction time. Hypothesis 2 is therefore not supported.
The analyses reported above also extend the finding that rule­
search and instructed conditions do not perform similarly, contrary
to Hypothesis 1 . The instructed condition is significantly more
1 40 Analyses and results Analyses and results 141

accurate than the rule-search condition in performance on the hard on easy rule grammatical sentences compared with ungrammatical
rule and on the easy rule. In addition, the post-hoe comparisons sentences (F ( 1 /25) = 7.7 1 4, p = . 0 1 02).
reported above also show a significant difference between the more
accurate instructed condition and the implicit condition, and an 6.1.3.3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 3
almost significant difference between the instructed and incidental The analyses reported above support Hypothesis 3 . There is a
conditions (p = .02). significant difference in accuracy of responses to sentences
conforming to easy and hard rules for subjects trained in the
6.1.3 Analyses for Hypothesis 3 incidental, rule-search and instructed conditions. In all cases
Hypothesis 3 states that performance on easy rule sentences will be accuracy on the sentences conforming to the easy rule is higher
faster and more accurate than performance on hard rule sentences. than accuracy on sentences conforming to the hard rule (see Figure
To address this hypothesis the factor Condition was removed from 1 4). There is no significant difference, however, between accuracy
the repeated measure ANOVA for accuracy and reaction time and of judgment on the easy and hard rule for subjects trained under the
separate ANOVA were performed for each condition. Significant implicit condition, though, again, mean accuracy on the easy rule is
main effects for the factor Rule, when supported by evidence o f higher than mean accuracy on the hard rule and the difference
higher mean accuracy and lower mean reaction time for the easy approaches significance (p = .06). Similarly the reaction time data
rule as opposed to the hard rule, were interpreted as evidence supports the hypothesized order of difficulty of sentences
supporting the hypothesis for each particular condition. conforming to these two rules. In all conditions reaction time t o
easy rule sentences is significantly faster than reaction time to hard
6.1.3.1 Simple and complex rule accuracy rule sentences (see Figure 1 5).
The results of the ANOVA for accuracy on easy and hard rules show
no significant main effect for the factor Rule at the p<.0 1 level fo r 6.2 Rule awareness data
subjects trained in the implicit condition (F ( 1/25) = 3 . 779, p = The awareness data is based on responses to a questionnaire given
.0632). For subjects trained in the incidental condition (F ( 1 /25) = after the training and transfer sessions had been completed. Subjects
1 4 . 8 1 8, p<.0 1 ), the rule-search condition (F ( 1 /25) =23 . 73 6 , were asked to write responses in their L l . These were translated
p<.O l ) and the instructed condition, however, the factor Rule is into English by the researcher and native-speaker informants.
significant. There is a significant main effect for Grammaticality (F Answers to three of the questions; 'Did you notice any rules o f
( 1 /25) = 32.529, p<.0 1 ), and a significant interaction between Rule grammar when you did the first two exercises o n the computer?',
and Grammaticality (F ( 1 /25) = 1 0 .740, p<. 0 1 ) only for instructed ' Were you looking for grammar rules when you did the first two
subjects, which, as Figure 1 7 shows is attibutable to their poor exercises on the computer?' , and 'Can you say what the rules are?' ,
performance on ungrammatical hard rule sentences, relative to were used to code subjects as aware at the levels of noticing, looking
grammatical hard rule sentences. for rules, and able to verbalize rules. Only answers to the third
question were used to address Hypothesis 4 (see Chapter 7 for
6.1.3.2 Simple and complex rule reaction time further analyses). Complete explanations of the rules were n o t
There is a significant difference between reaction times to sentences required. If subjects attempted to describe any elements o f the easy
conforming to easy and hard rules for subjects trained under the and hard rules presented during training, either by a structural
implicit (F ( 1 /25) 3 0.254, p<. 0 1 ), incidental (F ( 1 /25) = 2 8 .696,
=
description, or by presenting and commenting upon an example o f
p<.0 1 ), rule-search (F ( 1/25) = 1 8 . 8 1 3 , p<.0 1 ) and instructed an easy o r hard rule sentence, they were coded as able to verbalize.
conditions (F ( 1 /25) = 1 2 .489, p<. 0 1 ). There is no significant main The coding also included information about which rule was
effect for Grammaticality for any condition, but there is a verbalized. There are thus three categories of subjects who
significant interaction of Rule and Grammaticality for instructed verbalized the rules, those who verbalized only the easy rule, those
subjects, which as Figure 1 9 shows is the result of faster perfomance who verbalized only the hard rule, and those who verbalized both
142 Analyses and results Analyses and results 1 43

easy and hard rules. Frequencies for subjects who were coded as able 6.2.1 Analyses for Hypothesis 4
to verbalize, divided by condition, are given in Table 1 3 . Tables 1 4 The verbalizability data displayed above was used to address
and 1 5 give frequencies of subjects able to verbalize only one rule, Hypothesis 4, that subjects in all conditions would attest to being
and both rules . Table 1 6 gives frequencies for the total more aware of simple rules than of complex rules. Only subjects
verbalizations of easy and hard rules across conditions. who verbalized one rule were included in the analysis. Condition was
not entered as a factor since it is not integral to the hypothesis. A
one-way Chi-square was performed on the total frequencies
Table 13. Frequencies Of Subjects Able To Verbalize The displayed in Table 14. There is only a small difference between the
Rules groups verbalizing one rule, 9 verbalizing the easy rule and 1 1
verbalizing the hard rule. The results pf the Chi-square with Yates'
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
correction are not significant (X2 =.200, p>.0 1 ). Subjects in the
instructed condition who verbalized one rule, all reported the easy
didn't verbalize 20 19 19 12 70
did verbalize 6 7 7 14 34
rule. There is a trend in the reverse direction, towards reporting the
hard rule, for subjects in the other conditions. A Chi-square of the
26 26 26 26 Totals
frequency values for easy and hard rule verbalizability by those in
the combined implicit and incidental group, versus the combined
Table 14. Frequencies Of Subjects Verbalizing One Rule
rule-search and instructed group, with Yates' correction, was also
not significant (X2= 3 .3 6, p>.0 1 ). However, pairwise comparisons
of easy and hard rule verbalization for the instructed and implicit
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
learners (X2 = 1 1 .66), incidental learners (X2 = 8.67) and rule­
easy rule 1 1 1 6 9
search learners (X2 = 8.53) are all significant at the p<.0 1 level.
hard rule 5 4 2 0 11
6 5 3 6 Totals
6.2.1.1 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 4
There is no evidence to support Hypothesis 4. The number o f
subjects attempting to verbalize any o f the rules i s low, as Table 1 3
Table 1 5 . Freq uencies Of Subjects Verbalizing Both Rules shows. There is no significant difference between the numbers o f
subjects verbalizing the easy and the hard rules. There i s a trend,
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed apparent from the raw frequencies displayed in Table 1 6, for
both rules 0 2 4 8 14 subjects in the instructed condition to report the easy rule, and for
Total subjects in the implicit and incidental conditions to report the hard
rule. The relationship of the verbalizability data to the other
questionnaire evidence of rule awareness and conscious rule-search is
Table 16. Frequency Of Verbalization Of Easy And Hard discussed in more detail in the following chapter.
Rules
6.3 Variance data
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed Two types of variance data were used to address Hypothesis 5 ;
easy rule 1 3 5 14 23 variance in accuracy of performance across conditions in the
hard rule 5 6 6 8 25 transfer grammaticality judgment test, and variance across
6 9 11 22 Totals conditions in the time taken to make grammaticality judgments.
Descriptive statistics for this data are summarised in Table 1 7.
144 Analyses and results Analyses and results 145

Table 17. Descriptive Statistics For The Variance Data 6.3.J.2 Reaction time
The results of the paired F-tests for reaction time given in Table 1 9
Condition Accuracy Reaction Time also show no significant difference between the implicit and
Mean Variance SD Mean Variance SD incidental condition, but a significant difference between the rule­
Implicit .638 .23 1 .48 1 9052.633 3 6 8 78737.992 6072.787 search and instructed condition (F ( 1 039/1 039) = . 846, p<.0 1 ). I n
Incidental .655 .226 .476 9648 .442 3 4 1 30566.3 1 4 5842. 1 3 7 addition the variance o f performance i n the rule-search condition is
Rule-search .633 .233 .482 743 9 . 1 8 8 28055756.049 5296.768 significantly lower than the variance of performance in the implicit
Instructed .758 . 1 84 .429 9 1 99.482 3 3 1 8 1 1 42 1 .657 5760.332
condition (F ( 1 039/ 1 03 9) = 1 .3 1 4, p<.0 1 ), and the incidental
condition (F ( 1 039/ 1 03 9 = 1 .2 1 7, p<.0 1 ).
6.3.1 Analyses for Hypothesis 5
Hypothesis 5 states that variance in performance under implicit and Table 19. Results Of Paired F-Tests For Reaction Time
incidental conditions will be equivalent, and less than the variance in
performance under rule-search and explicit conditions, which wiH Comparison Variance ratio Num.df Den.df F-Value P-Value
also be equivalent. To address this hypothesis F-tests comparing the
variance of each experimental condition, were performed using Implicit v. Incidental 1 .0 8 1 1 03 9 1 03 9 1 .08 1 .2 122
measures of accuracy and reaction time to sentences presented in Implicit v. Rule-search 1 .3 1 4 1039 1 03 9 1 .3 1 4 .000 1
the transfer task. Implicit v. Instructed 1.1 1 1 1039 1 03 9 1.1 1 1 .0888
Incidental v. Rule-search 1 .2 1 7 1 03 9 1 03 9 1 .2 1 7 . 00 1 6
6.3.1.1 Accuracy Incidental v . Instructed 1 .209 1039 1 03 9 1 .029 .6496
The results of the paired F-tests for accuracy are given in Table 1 8 . Rule-search v. Instructed 0.846 1039 1 03 9 0 . 846 .0069
There is no significant difference between variance of performance
under the implicit and incidental conditions, though there is a
significant difference between the rule-search and instructed 6.3. 1.3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 5
condition (F ( 1 039/ 1 039) = 1 .266, p<.O 1 ). In addition, variance in The results reported above do not support Hypothesis 5 . There is
accuracy in the instructed condition is significantly less than no significant difference between the variance of performance of
variance in accuracy in the implicit (F ( 1 039/ 1 039) = 1 .257, p<.0 1 ) those in the implicit and incidental conditions, there is a significant
and incidental conditions (F ( 1 039/1 039) = 1 .23 1 , p<. 0 1 ). difference between the rule-search and the instructed condition.
There is significantly less variance in reaction time under the rule­
search condition than under the instructed condition, and
Table 18. Results Of Paired F-Tests For Accuracy significantly less variance in accuracy under the instructed condition
than under the rule-search condition. The rule-search and instructed
Comparison Variance ratio Num.df Den.df F-Value P-Value conditions are therefore not equivalent. Further, it is the instructed
condition that has significantly lower variance in accuracy than all
Implicit v. Incidental 1 .021 1 03 9 1 03 9 1 .02 1 .7352 other conditions, and the rule-search condition that has
Implicit v. Rule-search 0 .993 1 03 9 1039 0 .993 .9 1 3 3 significantly lower variance in reaction time than all other
Implicit v. Instructed 1 .2 5 7 1 03 9 1039 1 .257 .0002
conditions. The implicit and incidental conditions do not exhibit
Incidental v. Rule-search 0.973 1 03 9 1 03 9 0.973 .6549
less variance in performance than the explicit conditions.
Incidental v. Instructed 1 .23 1 1 03 9 1 03 9 1 .2 3 1 .0008
Rule-search v. Instructed 1 . 266 1 03 9 1 03 9 1 .266 .000 1
6.4 Aptitude data
All subjects completed two subtests of the Modern Language
Aptitude Test (Carroll & Sapon 1 959), the words-in-sentences test
1 46 Analyses and results Analyses and results 1 47

of grammatical sensitivity and the paired-associates test o f administration time (as is the case with DLAB, see Parry & Child
memory. The two subtest scores were combined to give a composite 1 990:53). Due to the heavy demands on their time made by the
measure of aptitude. Descriptive statistics for the aptitude data show pretesting and experimental treatment, no proficiency measures
scores on the memory test are normally distributed. There is n o were obtained from subjects in the present study which could have
excessive kurtosis (-.258) o r skewness (-.252). The total score been used to directly address the issue of the correlation of aptitude
available on this test is twenty four. Scores are distributed from 2 t o scores and proficiency. However, three pieces of evidence support
2 4 with a mean score o f 1 4 . Reliability o f the subtest is .7728, the claim for the independence of aptitude and proficiency.
(KR20). Scores on the grammatical sensitivity test are also
normally distributed, with no significant kurtosis (-. 762) or (i) Evidence from a comparable group of subjects. The
skewness (. 1 49). The total score available on this test is forty five. correlation of proficiency and scores on the aptitude tests was
Scores are distributed from 9 to 39 with a mean score of 2 2 . examined for a group of subjects comparable in age, L l background,
Reliability o f the subtest i s .8 1 3 1 , (KR20). Finally, scores o n the and type and quality of previous formal instruction in English to the
composite aptitude test are normally distributed (kurtosis = - .63 8, subjects who took part in the experimental training sessions. This
skewness = .006). The total score available on this test is ninety was a group of 45 native speakers of Japanese, aged between 1 9 and
three, based on a score out of forty five for grammatical sensitivity 20 years, who were currently enrolled in English programs in two
and a score out of forty eight for the memory test. The memory Japanese universities, Tokai University in Tokyo and Tokai
test contribution was obtained by doubling the scores obtained on University in Kyushu. This group were in Hawai' i attending a short­
the subtest. Scores on the composite aptitude tests are distributed term English language program at Tokai University of Honolulu.
from 20 to 80 with a mean of 5 1 . Reliability is .94 1 6, (KR20). Like the majority of young adult Japanese subjects who took part in
Three one-way ANOVA showed no significant main effect for the present study they had all received six years of formal
condition on memory scores, grammatical sensitivity scores, and instruction in English in the Japanese school system, and had
the combined aptitude scores, providing support for the assumption limited exposure to English in a naturalistic setting. These subjects
of homogeneity of aptitude across conditions which it was hoped completed the two subtests of the MLAT, and a proficiency test,
would follow from the procedure of randomly assigning subjects. the Michigan English Placement Test (MEPT). The MEPT
It is also necessary to show that the aptitude measures obtained consists of four subtests of listening, reading, vocabulary and
in the present study capture more than just differences in grammatical knowledge. The correlations of these subtests and the
proficiency since these tests were administered in English, which is total MEPT scores, with the MLAT subtests and composite scores
not the native language of the subjects for the study. No Japanese are reported in Table 20.
versions of MLAT subtests used, or of any other recognized
aptitude test, are currently publicly available (though see Murakami
1 974 who reports the results of an early attempt to develop a Table 20. Correlations Of Aptitude And MEPT Proficiency
Japanese aptitude test with similar subtests to the MLAT) and Subtest Scores For An Equivalent Group Of Subjects
MLAT has been shown to discriminate equally well within both
native-speaking English, and native-speaking Japanese populations MEPT/total Listening Grammar Vocabulary Reading
(Sawyer 1 993) . There are alternative aptitude tests to the MLAT,
but either they are less suitable for the subject population in the Aptitude .617* .443* .507* .526* .534*
Memory .382* .3 7 1 .256 .313 .365
present study (e.g., Pimsleur's Language Aptitude Battery which was
Grammatical .652* .330 .617* .573 * .5 14*
developed for use with school age adolescents, see Pimsleur 1 966)
Sensitivity
or they have been privately developed for small scale studies (as in
Sasaki 1 993) and are less well developed than the MLAT (as is the
* p< . 0 1
case with VORD, an aptitude test developed for use with military =

personnel, see Parry & Child 1 990), or require excessively long


1 48 Analyses and results Analyses and results 1 49

As can be seen from Table 20 there is no significant correlation conditions. If scores on the tests merely reflected proficiency
of the memory subtest with any of the MEPT subtests, at the p<.O l differences, then, following the procedure of random assignment t o
level, but significant correlations of the grammatical sensitivity conditions, which ensures that subjects representing a similar range
subtest with the grammar, vocabulary and reading subtests of the of proficiencies are assigned to each condition, there should have
MEPT. The combined aptitude score correlates significantly with been similar correlations across conditions.
the total MEPT score, and all the subtests. These correlations,
although significant, are not high enough, however, to support the 6.4.J Analyses for Hypothesis 6
claim that the aptitude test and the MEPT measure the same Hypothesis 6 states that measures of individual differences in
abilities. There are no correlations in the high .80s or .90s which language learning aptitude will be related to performance under the
would be necessary to support this claim (Hatch & Lazaraton rule-search and instructed conditions, but unrelated to performance
1 99 1 :44 1 ). The aptitude tests measure other abilities, apart from under the implicit and incidental conditions. To address this
those tapped by estimates of proficiency. hypothesis scores on the three aptitude measures were correlated
with accuracy and reaction time for easy and hard rules under all
(ii) No significant correlation of aptitude subtest scores. As can conditions. Results of these correlations, corrected for attenuation,
be seen from Table 20 the memory subtest clearly measures abilities are reported in Tables 22 and 23 .
not captured in estimates of proficiency. Further evidence of the
independence of the aptitude measures from measures of 6.4.1.1 Accuracy and aptitude
proficiency lies in the lack of significant correlation between the For the implicit condition there is a significant correlation of
subtest scores. Table 21 shows the correlation of memory and grammatical sensitivity and accuracy on the easy (r .693, p<.0 1 )
=

grammatical sensitivity subtest scores for those subjects in the and on the hard rule (r .746, p<.0 1 ). There is also a significant
=

present study, divided by condition. There are no significant correlation with the composite aptitude score for easy rule accuracy
correlations at the p<.O l level for the twenty six subjects in each (r .52 1 , p<.0 1 ) and hard rule accuracy (r .520, p<.0 1 ). Scores
= =

condition, providing evidence that the subtest scores measure on the memory subtest do not correlate significantly with easy o r
different abilities, regardless of current level of proficiency. hard rule accuracy. In contrast, fo r the incidental condition there is
no significant correlation of easy and hard rule accuracy with
performance on any of the aptitude measures at the p<.O 1 level.
Table 2 1 . Correlations Of Memory And Grammatical For the rule-search condition there is a significant correlation of
Sensitivity S ubtest Scores For Subjects In Each Condition aptitude and accuracy on the easy rule (r = .5 56, p<.0 1 ). The
correlation of memory and aptitude with hard rule accuracy is not
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed significant at the p<.0 1 level, but is significant at the p<.05 level.
There is no significant correlation of memory and performance on
.457 . 1 59 .335 .101 the easy rule. Grammatical sensitivity correlates significantly with
performance on the easy rule (r .604, p<.0 1 ), but not the hard
=

No correlations are significant at the p<.01 level. rule. For the instructed condition, as predicted, grammatical
sensitivity correlates significantly with hard rule accuracy (r . 5 6 3 ,
=

(iii) No even distribution of correlations across conditions. p<.0 1 ), and easy rule accuracy (r .540, p<. 0 1 ) . The correlation o f
=

There is no pattern of correlations of aptitude scores with memory with performance o n the easy rule (r .489), and the hard
=

performance on easy and hard rules across learning conditions as rule (r .46 1 ) is not significant at the p<.O l level, but is significant
=

shown in Table 22, and discussed in the following section. It appears at the p<.05 level. There is a significant correlation of easy (r =

from this Table that the aptitude tests measure abilities that are . 626, p<.0 1 ) and hard rule performance (r = .620, p<.0 1 ) with the
differentially important to learning under the different task combined score for aptitude.
150 Analyses and results Analyses and results 151

Table 22. Correlations Of Aptitude Subtests And Accuracy 6.4.1.2 Reaction time and aptitude
On Easy And Hard Rules Under Each Condition There are no significant correlations of reaction time and
performance under any condition, apart from rule-search
Condition/Rule Grammatical Sensitivity Memory Aptitude performance on the easy rule which correlates significantly with
grammatical sensitivity (r = -.502, p<.0 1 ). For the rule-search
Implicit condition the aptitude score correlates with easy rule reaction time
Easy rule .693 * * .299 . 52 1 * * at the p<.05 level of significance (r = - .4 78) .
Hard rule .746 * * .250 . 520* *
Incidental
6.4.1.3 Summary of the evidence for Hypothesis 6
Easy rule .353 .309 .389
Hard rule .285 . 1 36 .234
There is partial support for Hypothesis 6 from the results of
Rule-search correlations for those trained under the incidental and instructed
Easy rule .604 * * .4 1 9 .556** conditions. As Hypothesis 6 predicts, performance in the transfer
Hard rule .374 .5 1 1 * . 504* task is unrelated to performance on the language learning aptitude
Instructed tests for those subjects trained under the incidental condition. Also
Easy rule . 540 * * .489* .626 * * in line with this hypothesis is the finding that performance by those
Hard rule .563 * * .46 1 * .620 * * trained in the instructed condition correlates significantly with
aptitude and grammatical sensitivity. Correlations for memory and
** = p<.0 1 * = p<. 0 5 performance on both rules also approaches significance for the
instructed condition. However, performance on both easy and hard
rules by those trained under the implicit condition correlates with
scores on the grammatical sensitivity test. This is contrary to
Table 23. Correlations Of Aptitude Subtests And Reaction Hypothesis 6. Performance on the easy rule by those trained under
Time On Easy And Hard Rules Under Each Condition the rule-search condition correlates significantly with grammatical
sensitivity, but there is no significant correlation with performance
Condition/Rule Grammatical Sensitivity Memory Aptitude on the hard rule.

Implicit 6.5 Summary of research hypotheses and results


Easy rule -.385 -. 1 2 5 .016
Hard rule -. 1 1 9 . 1 24 -.282 • Hypothesis 1 is partially supported. There is no significant
Incidental difference between the implicit and incidental conditions either in
Easy rule -. 1 95 . 1 36 .006
accuracy or reaction time of responses to all sentences presented in
Hard rule -.278 .067 -.094
the transfer set, but there is a significant difference between the
Rule-search
Easy rule - . 502* * -.3 1 3 -.478*
rule-search and instructed conditions in accuracy of response. The
Hard rule -.327 -.269 -.358 instructed condition is more accurate than the rule-search condition,
Instructed and they do not therefore pattern together.
Easy rule -.200 - . 1 36 - .2 1 8
Hard rule -.289 -.048 -.203 • Hypothesis 2 is not supported. The implicit and incidental
conditions are not superior to the rule-search and instructed
** = p<.O l * = p<. 0 5 conditions in accuracy or reaction time to sentences conforming t o
the hard rule in the transfer set.
1 52 Analyses and results

idental, rule­
for those trained in the inc
Hypothesis 3 is supported ts accuracy is
ditions. For these subjec

search and instructed con ten ces than for Chapter 7


ponses to easy rule sen
significantly higher for res is no sig nifican t
ten ces . However, there
responses to hard rule sen d rul e sen tences
response to easy and har
difference in accuracy of tim e to easy Further Analyses
plicit condition. Reaction
for those trained in the im e to ha rd rule
tly faster than reaction tim
rule sentences is significan
ined in all conditions.
sentences for subjects tra
is no significan t difference
4 is not supported. There
7. 0 Introduction
.
• Hy po the sis

in the verbalizability of eas


y and hard rules. ; �
This chapter addresses in more0det 1· 1 th ee Issues a�ising from the
previously presented analyses I o ': �t extent Is learning as a
.
5 is not supported . Variance
of performance by tho se fi
result of exposure under i� lic1t, 1�c1dental, rule-search and
• Hypothesis

trained in the im plicit and


incidental conditions is no
t significantly �.
instructed conditions different"a y sens 1ve to the grammatical and
lower than for those tra
ined in the rule-search
and instructed �;
ungrammatical status of sent� c s, an to t�e different easy and
_ .
CE�r 7;�
res of reaction
performance using measu hard rule sentence types rese du trammg? A pr�vious study
condition s. Variance in up tha n for all fu
for the rule-search gro of implicit L2 leam g is 3 ) , and studies of L 2
time is significantly lower for ma nce is
iance in accuracy of per
other conditions, and var tructed condition than for all oth er
ins
grammaticality judgment tests
as well as studies of implicit lea ��� �
hacht�r, Ty�on & Diffley 1 976),
_
smg art1fic1al grammars (e.g.,
significantly lower for the McAndrews & Moscovitch 1 985·; o ey & Brooks 1 992), have all
condition s. produced .evidence that sub�ects tend to wrong1y accept
Hypothesis 6 is partially
supported. There are sig
nificant e
ungrammatical stimuli more oft n t an th�y wrongly reject
and accuracy of perfor
ma nce in the gram�atical stimuli. Informatio : � out differences between
correlations of aptitude

instructed conditions, bu
t no t in the �:
cond1t10ns in responses to g mm�t1cal versus ungrammatical
implicit , rule-search and acc ura cy and
the implicit condition sentences, and in responses to fi e different types motivating the
incidental condition. In d. Ac cur acy of
significantly correlate design of the training and tr�ns �r . sets, thus �x�ends the analysis o f
grammatical sensitivity are nd itio n sho ws the l 1 1t nd exphc1t . con�itions beyond
ined in the instructed co what can be learned under
u::J:d � ; �
performance by those tra under the rule­
aptitude. For those trained the distinction between easy a rul s exammed m the previous
strongest correlation with ate s significantly ��
atical sensitivity correl chapter. (ii) How does transfer t s per ormance on sentences o f
search condition gramm �� 1- ied as ungrai:iimatical during the
time on the easy rule. the type subjects wron l 1
with accuracy and reaction pretest relate to perfor �:U �
c n s�ntences I� the transfer task?
This question is important to addr : _
given the h�1ted pretesting of
::
grammatical knowledge in the pr ent study. (111) Is awareness of
r !
rules, and the ability to verba 1ze hem " related to the measures o f
aptitude obtained from subJ1iects m this study? Th"is question · ·
1s
��::
• • • . . ·

important smce it may be th t ness is tr1g?ered either by an


ability to detect similaritie : . . en memonzed examples of
: �:�� �
sentences experienced durin t a n or by an ability to detect
he nd rlym .
grammatical relationships e u_ _ � ? struc�res o f
exa�p_les, or by a combination of the se ab1ht1es. Smee the implicit
cond1t1on requires memorizafton, one would expect to see a stronger
1 54 Further analyses Further analyses 155

relationship between awareness and the memory component o f p�rf?�ance o n each sentence type. However, the Fundamental
aptitude for subjects reporting awareness in this condition. Since the S1m1�a�1ty Hy�othesis described in 4.3 .4, which holds learning in all
rule-search condition draws attention to the grammatical status of �on�1�10n,s . will be simi�ai: since it results from the variety of
examples, one would expect to see a stronger relationship between not1cmg mduced by trammg task demands, suggests differences in
grammatical sensitivity and awareness in this condition. Of course perfo �mance are likely since the different conditions direct
.
this may not be so, and an examination of the relationship o f a�tentlon. at different aspects of the stimulus display, and this should
aptitude to awareness may suggest that the processing strategies differ�ntial �y . affect the learning of types across conditions. T 0
leading to awareness are unrelated to those encouraged by the examme this issue Type was entered as a factor in repeated measure
different task demands. ANOVA for accuracy and reaction time to grammatical and
These issues are addressed in the following order. Firstly, in ungrammatical easy and hard rule sentences.
section 7 . 1 , the relationship of the factor of Grammaticality to the
different types of easy and hard rule sentences is assessed. Following
this, in section 7 .2, performance across conditions is compared with Table 24. Summary Of The Transfer Set Sentence Types
respect to accuracy on sentences conforming to the specific types
used in the pretest of easy and hard rule awareness. Finally, in Easy Rule Grammatical Types
section 7.3, the data on rule verbalizability, described in addressing Type 1 (S V Location) e.g., Lisa fell onto the grass.
Hypothesis 4, are examined in relation to other measures of rule Type 2 (Location V S) e.g., Across the street raced Tom.
awareness obtained from the debriefing questionnaire, and m Type 3 (Location S V) e.g., Through the window Maria stared
Type 4 (S V Time) e.g., Peter woke at eight o 'clock.
relation to the measures of aptitude used to address Hypothesis 6.
Type 5 (Time S V) e.g. , On her birthday Gabi sang.
Easy Rule Ungrammatical Types
7.1 The effect of grammaticality and sentence type
Significant interactions of Rule and Grammaticality, and o f Type l(Time V Sino inversion without Location first) e.g., On Saturday
night danced Charlie.
Grammaticality and Condition, were revealed i n the analysis o f
Type 2 (V S Adv/no inversion without Location first) e.g., Golfed Debbie
accuracy o f response to all sentences i n section 6 . 1 . 1 (see Table 7 ) . in the morning.
The analyses fo r Hypothesis 2 described i n 6. 1 .2 showed the
Hard Rule Grammatical Types
interaction to be attributable to performance on the hard rule,
Type l(S V Location and S V Location) e.g., The pen is in the box and
(Table 9, Figure 1 6). There was no significant main effect for the
pencil is in the drawer.
Grammaticality in accuracy of performance on the easy rule (Table Type 2 (S singular + be) e.g., Where the car is is in the driveway.
1 0, Figure 1 7). The analyses for Hypothesis 3 showed the Type 3 (S plural + be) e.g., Where the apples are is in the bowl
interaction of Grammaticality and Condition resulted form the Type 4 (S singular + lexical verb) e.g., Where Judy teaches is a; the
performance of instructed learners who are significantly less University.
accurate at judging ungrammatical hard rule sentences than they are Type 5 (S plural + lexical verb) e.g., Where Mary and Joe relax is by the
at judging any other sentences. ool.
Although there was no significant main effect for Hard Rule Ungrammatical Types
Grammaticality on accuracy for implicit, incidental and rule-search Type 1 (�o in� ersi�n with 'Where' ) e.g., Where is Bill is in the country.
conditions there may, however, be significant effects across Type 2 (mvariant smgular 'be') e.g., Where Carl writes are at a desk.
conditions for responses to sentences grouped according to the type Type 3 (requ�red tense agreement) e.g., Where the bird is was in the sky.
categorization used to formulate the easy and hard rules (see 5 .3 .4 Type 4 (reqmred ' Where' ) e.g., What the King lives is in a palace.
and 5.3.5 and Table 24), and significant interactions between Typ e Type 5 (required 'not' for contrasting Locations) e.g., Where Mark works is
and Condition. I f the implicit and incidental conditions are in America in Japan.
equivalent, as suggested by the results of analyses performed t o
address Hypothesis 1 , then they should be equivalent m
1 56 Further analyses Further analyses 157

7.1.1 Simple rule grammatical sentence types Using the Chi-square statistic to calculate the probability o f the
There are five easy rule grammatical sentence types. Two sentences distribution of accurate and inaccurate responses to the ten easy rule
of each type were contained in the transfer set. These types are grammatical sentences for the 26 subjects in each condition gives a
summarized in Table 24 and the transfer set sentences are illustrated ratio of 1 5 1 to 1 09 as significantly above chance at the p<. 0 1 level
in Appendix J. Means for accuracy and reaction time to easy rule of significance, (JO ( 1 5 1 ) -/E ( 1 30) = 2 1 *2 1 / 1 3 0 = 3 .3 9 *2 6 . 7).
=

grammatical sentence types across conditions are illustrated in This means those scoring over 58% accurate or below 42% accurate
Figures 20 and 21 and results of the ANOVA are reported below. are performing above chance. Responses to type 2 sentences are
!he�efore low enough to indicate that those in the implicit,
7. 1. 1. 1 Accuracy
mc1dental and rule-search conditions are consistently applying
wrongly held beliefs about the ungrammatical status of the
There are significant main effects for Condition (F (3/1 00) =

sentences, rather than randomly responding to them. In contrast,


1 0.650, p<.O l ), and for Type (F (4/400) = 27.208, p<.O l ). There is
performance is significantly above chance for responses to all other
a significant interaction between Type and Condition (F ( 1 2/400) =

easy grammatical sentences for subjects in all conditions. Type 2


2.3 7 1 , p<.0 1 ), which is illustrated in Figure 20. This appears to be
attributable to the fact that subjects trained in the instructed sentences, it should be noted, were used in the pretest, together with
condition perform much more accurately on type 2 sentences than type 2 hard sentences, as a basis for selecting subjects for the
subjects trained in the other conditions. Mean accuracy for these present study. Implications of the failure of subjects in the implicit,
incidental and rule-search conditions to accept them as grammatical
sentences is 70% for those in the instructed condition, 3 6% for
are considered in 7.2.
those in the implicit and incidental conditions, and 28% for those in
the rule-search condition.
Q)

1
..§ 10500
;;.... i:::
g :§ 1 0000
3
0
.9 0
cd
0 Q)
.....
9500
cd
.8 0 type 1 Oil
Oil 0 9000 0 type 1
0
-� • type 2 -�
.7 a 8500 • type 2

.....
Oil
.6
A type 3 §
.....
Oil
8000 6 type 3
e type 4 Gi' 7500
Gi' .5 cd • type 4
cd Q)
Q) <> type 5 """' 7000
"""' 0 <> type 5
0 .4 "'
"'
§ 6500
§ Q)
Q) .3 ::;E 6000
::;E
-
.2 v 5500
v u
u Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed

condition condition

Figure 20. Mean Accuracy For Easy Rule Grammatical Figure 2 1 . Mean Reaction Time For Easy Rule Grammatical
Sentence Types Across Conditions Sentence Types Across Conditions
158 Further analyses Further analyses 1 59

7. 1. 1.2 Reaction time 7.1.2.2 Reaction time


There is a significant main effect for Type (F (4/400) 6.048,
= There is no significant main effect for Condition, and no significant
p<.0 1 ), but no significant main effec� for Con?ition, and no interaction of Type and Condition. There is a significant main
.
significant interaction of Type and Condition (see Figure 2 1 ). effect for Type (F ( 111 00) 27.206, p<.0 1 ). As Figure 23 shows,
=

responses to easy ungrammatical type 2 sentences are slower across


7. 1.2 Simple rule ungrammatical sentence types
. all conditions than responses to easy ungrammatical type 1
There are two easy rule ungrammatical sentence types. Five sentences.
sentences of each type were contained in the transfer set. These
types are described in 5 .6.5 and summarized in Ta�le 24. T � e
(])

transfer set sentences exemplifying these types are illustrated m ·� 1 0500


i::

Appendix J. Results of the repeated measure A�OVA for accuracy .g 1 0000


and reaction time to easy rule grammatical sentences are g
(])
.... 9500
summarized below. �
u
9000
·�
7. 1.2.1 Accuracy a 8500
0
There are no significant main effects for Condition, or Type, and §
.... 8000
type 1

no significant interaction of Type and Conditi?n . . Respons(ls t o •


Oil
c type 2

both easy rule ungrammatical sentence -�pes are si�mficantly a�ove


::l 7500

chance for subjects trained in all conditions (see Figure 22), with a <>I
(]) 7000
higher mean accuracy on type 2 for implicit, incidental and rule­
<.+...
0
"' 6500
search learners, and on type 1 for instructed learners. fa
(]) 6000

G' 5500
Q)
� . 875 ....-�-1..����l--��'--�1 u
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
g .85
ta condition
.8 .825
ti!
s .8

� .775
Oil
0 type 1 Figure 23. Mean Reaction Time For Easy Rule
§ .75 • type 2 Ungrammatical Sentence Types Across Conditions
_..,
� .725
(])
'O .7
Type 2 sentences (see Table 24), are those in which subject-verb
§
(])
.675
inversion occurs preceding adverbials of time and location (Crawled
� .65 Mick through the tunnel). It appears that these sentences pose
� .625 ..L�-.-����.-���.-����r-�..... more of a problem, as indexed by the lengthy reaction times they
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed require, than type 1 ungrammatical easy rule sentences. Type 1
condition
sentences are those in which subject-verb inversion occurs following
a time phrase (On Friday night danced Charlie). Subjects correctly
Figure 22. Mean Accuracy For Easy Rule Ungrammatical classify these as ungrammatical, but have not learned the fact that
Sentence Types Across Conditions subject-verb inversion is allowed following adverbials of location, as
in type 2 grammatical easy rule sentences. As Figure 20 shows, type
1 60 Further analyses Further analyses 161

2 grammatical easy rule sentences (Into the house ran John) are �ule-search condition; indicating chance performance. Performance
consistently wrongly rejected as ungrammatical by subjects in the IS at chance on type 3 sentences for the incidental condition, and
implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions where a rule of 'reject on ty�e 4 s�ntences for the implicit condition. Performance by
subject-verb inversion' seems to be being applied. Subjects in the those m the mstructed condition is significantly above chance for
instructed condition, however, have learned the exception to this all sentenc� types. Performance on type, 2 grammatical hard rule
rule and correctly accept type 2 easy rule grammatical sentences �en�en�es IS below 42% for those in the rule-search condition,
over 70% of the time. mdicatmg wrongly held beliefs about the grammatical status of these
sentences. These sei:itences are the same type as the exponents o f
7.1.3 Complex rule grammatical sentence types the . hard rule used m the pretest o f grammatical knowledge for
_
There are five hard rule grammatical sentence types, as summarized subjects m the present study (Where the book is is on the desk not
in Table 24 and illustrated in Appendix J. Mean accuracy and on the �oor). Rule-search subjects have maintained their wrongly
reaction time to each type are illustrated in Figures 24 and 25 . held . beliefs about the ungrammatical status of these sentences
?esp �t� e�po_sure to them during training. In contrast, subjects in the
7.1.3.1 Accuracy Imphcit, mcidental and instructed conditions correctly classify them
There is a significant main effect for Condition (F (3/1 00) = 6 . 667, as grammatical at above chance levels.
p<. 0 1 ), and a significant interaction of Type and Condition (F
( 1 2/400) = 2 .3 8 1 , p<.0 1), but no significant main effect for Type.

;>-.
g
..... .9
g .85
g
.8 0 type 1
«!
u 0 type 1
•t; .75 • type 2
a .7 • type 2
ll. type 3.

..... .65 A type 3
Oil • type 4
"O .6
a e type 4 <> type 5
..<::: .55
<.+...;
0 <> type 5
"'
.5
a
(!)
.45
� .4
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
Q) . 3 5
u condition
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed

Figure 25. Mean Reaction Time For Hard Rule Grammatical


condition
Sentence Types Across Conditions
Figure 24. Mean Accuracy For Hard Rule Grammatical
Sentence Types Across Conditions
7. 1.3.2 Reaction time
There is a significant main effect for Type (F (4/400) 2 2 . 3 67,
As can be seen from Figure 24, performance on two of the sentence
=

p<.0 1 ), and an almosfs ignificant effect for Condition (F (3/1 00) =


types (types 3 and 5) is between 5 8% and 42% for those in the
1 62 Further analyses
Further analyses 1 63

3 . 675, p = . 0 1 47). There is no significant interaction of Type and


trained in the incidental condition perform at chance on types 1 , 4 ,
Condition. Figure 25 illustrates the mean reaction time to hard rule
and 2 , and at a level of 3 8% accuracy on type 3 sentences, which
arammatical sentence types across conditions. Mean reaction time
violate the tense agreement condition. Subjects in the implicit
fs faster for type 4 grammatical hard rule sentences for those in the
condition, in contrast, perform at above chance levels on four of
incidental, rule-search and instructed conditions (e.g., Where the
the hard ungrammatical rule sentence types, and at a chance level
President lives is in Washington not in Chicago), and longer for the
only on type 4 sentences. Subjects in all conditions correctly
type 2 sentences discussed above, for subjects trained in these three
classify type 5 sentences (Where Mark works is in America in
conditions.
Japan), which violates the condition that adjoined location phrases
must be contrasted by 'not' , as ungrammatical at significantly above
7.1.4 Complex rule ungrammatical sentence types
. . chance levels.
There are five hard rule ungrammatical sentence types, descnbed m
5 .3 .5, summarized in Table 24, and illustrated in Appendix J. The
results of the repeated measure ANOVA for accuracy and reaction .8
G'
time are described below. c<l
3
{) .7

7.1.4.1 Accuracy (;!
There is no significant main effect for Condition, but a s� gn�ficant �
{)
.6 O type 1
main effect for Type (F (4/400) = 1 0. 1 0 1 , p<. 0 1 ), and a s1gmficant 8
• type 2
interaction of Type and Condition (F ( 12/400) = 2 .489, p<.0 1 ). As
discussed in the analyses performed to address Hypotheses 1 , 2 . and '
;:s
.5
b. type 3
3 there is an interaction of Grammaticality and Rule for subjects -0
• type 4
a
t;ained in the instructed condition. Performance on ungrammatical ..i::
.4
4-< 0 type 5
hard rule sentences is significantly worse for instructed subjects than "'
0

performance on ungrammatical easy rule sentences. Figure 26 shows §


Q)
.3

this to be largely due to performance on two types of �


ungrammatical hard rule sentences. Inaccurate responses to . type 2 Q) .2
. u
and type 4 ungrammatical hard rule sentences are s1gmficantly
_
above the level attributable to chance. Instructed subjects correctly Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed

classify only 23% of type 4 sentences as ungrammatical (What t� e condition


King lives is in a palace not in an apartment), and correctly classify
only 34% of type 2 sentences as ungrammatical (Wher� the soldiers Figure 26. Mean Accuracy For Hard Rule Ungrammatical
fight are in Europe not in Hawai' i). These s�ntences v1ola�e two of Sentence Types Across Conditions
the conditions on the hard rule taught to mstructed subjects, the
condition that the form of the verb 'be' following the main verb is
invariantly singular (type 2) and the condition that pseudo-clefts of 7.1.4.2 Reaction time
location begin with 'where' , and not any other wh-word (type 4). In There is no significant main effect for Condition, and no significant
addition, performance on type 3 ungrammatical hard ��ntences interaction of Type and Condition but a significant main effect for
(Where the bird is was in the sky), which vi� late the cond1t1on that Type (F (4/400) = 1 8.026, p<.0 1 ). Figure 27 shows that mean
verbs must agree in tense, is at chance for mstructed subjec _ !s, who
responses to type 2 sentences are longer for all conditions, and
correctly classify only 55% of these sentences as ungrammatical. responses to type 1 sentences are shorter, with a mean difference of
Subjects trained in the rule-search condition perform at ch �nce over three seconds in response time across conditions between the
on ungrammatical hard rule sentence types 1, 2, 3 and 4. Subjects types. As with responses to easy rule ungrammatical sentences
1 64 Further analyses
Further analyses 1 65

(Fi�ure 23) and to hard rule grammatical sentences (Figure 2 5 ) The effects of these different types on accuracy of performance in
subjects i n all conditions maintain similar rank orders o f reaction each condition, illustrated in Figures 2 1 , 25 and 27, is summarized in
time to sentence types. Table 25, which shows differences with respect to chance
performance on types across conditions. To examine whether there
v
s
was a significant difference in the frequency of accurat� and
·;:;
� 1 2000 inaccurate responses to each of the two sentences conformmg to
.9 easy grammatical types and hard grammatical and hard
t) .
ro
....
v 1 1 000 ungrammatical types by subjects in each condition, two way C� i­

(,)
squares were performed for accurate and inaccurate responses usmg
·:g 1 0000
0 type 1
McNemar's test for correlated frequencies. These show no
s
significant difference at the p<.0 1 level for fr�quencies of ac�urate
§.... 9000
• type 2
and inaccurate responses to sentences conformmg to all types m . the
!>I)
lJ,.
§ type 3 implicit, rule-search and instructed conditions, th�ugh there is a
"O
a 8000 • type 4 significant difference for responses to easy gr�mmatical type 1 , and
hard grammatical type 1 , and hard ungrammatica! types 2 and 5 for
..c
..... () type S -
"'
0 .
7000
those subjects trained in the incidental condit10n. Responses to
§
v these types have therefore been excluded from the summary of � he
::E
OJ
6000
relationship between type and chance perform��ce. An e'.'-planah? n
u of this finding for those in the incidental condition, consistent with
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed . .
the Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis, is offered m section 8 . 3 .3
condition
of the following chapter. .
As Table 25 shows performance on type 2 easy grammatical
Figure 27. Mean Reaction Time For Hard Rule
sentences is significantly above chance for those in the instructed
Ungrammatical Sentence Types Across Conditions
condition who correctly accept them as grammatical, and for those
in the implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions who .wrongly
reject them as ungrammatical. This indicates s�bjects m these
7. 1.5 Summary of the effects ofgrammaticality and sentence type
conditions have maintained wrongly held beliefs about t? e
There is a consistently significant main effect for Type in measures
ungrammaticality of type 2 grammatical easy rule sentences despite
of reaction time to grammatical and ungrammatical sentences.
exposure to them during training. Table 25 also shows that
There is no significant interaction of Type and Condition for .
performance on hard gram.mati��l rule �e.ntence types differs across
reaction time, each condition tending to preserve the same rank
conditions. Those in the implicit condition perform at chance o n
order of response time to sentence types. There is no main effect
type 4 sentences. Subjects trained i n the rule-search condition are at
for Condition, or for Type, and no significant interaction between
chance on type 3 and 5 and wrongly accept type 2 sentences at
Type and . Condition for accuracy of response to easy rule .
significantly above chance levels, indicating wrongly held behefs
ungrammatical sentence types. Accuracy of response to each type is _
about their grammatical status. Accuracy is above chance . on these
above 5 8% in all conditions and therefore significantly above
types for those in the instructed condition. However, mstructed
chance. Differences between the conditions do emerge, though,
subjects are inaccurate, at above chance levels, on type 2 and type 4
from the accuracy data for responses to easy grammatical types,
hard ungrammatical sentences and perform at chance on type 3
and hard grammatical and ungrammatical types.
Ther� is a consistent main effect for Type, and a significant sentences. The effects of instruction on the hard rule do not appear
. to have contributed to an ability to correctly judge these three types
mteract10n of Type and Condition for easy rule grammatical
sentences, and hard rule grammatical and ungrammatical sentences. of sentence to be ungrammatical. Explanations of this finding are
offered in the following chapter.
Further analyses 1 67
1 66 Further analyses

Table 25. (Continued) Percentage Correct Responses To Easy


Table 25. The Relationship Between Chance/Non-chance And Hard Rule Sentence Types Presented In The Transfer
Performance, Transfer Set Sentence Type And Condition Task

Sentence Type Chance/Non-chance Performance Sentence Type Chance/Non-chance performance

Easy Rule Grammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst Hard Rule Ungrammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst

Type 1 (S V Location) A x A A Type l(no inversion with Where) A c c A


e.g., Lisa/ell onto the grass. e.g., Where is Bill is in the country.
Type 2 (Location V S) I I A Type 2 (invariant singular 'be') A x c I
e.g., Across the street raced Tom. e.g., Where Carl writes are at a desk.
Type 3 (Location S V) A A A A Type 3 (required tense agreement) A c c
e.g., Through the window Maria stared e.g., Where the bird is was in the sky.
Type 4 (S V Time) A A A A Type 4 (required 'Where') c c c I
e.g., Peter woke at eight o 'clock e.g., What the King lives is in a palace.
Type 5 (Time S V) A A A A Type 5 (required 'not' for contrasting
e.g., On her birthday Gabi sang. locations) A x A A
e.g., Where Mark works is in America
Easy Rule Ungrammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst in Japan.

Type 1 (Time V Sino inversion Key: C = performance at chance


without location first) A A A A A = non-chance performance and accurate
e.g., On Saturday night danced Charlie. I = non-chance performance, but inaccurate
Type 2 (V S Adv/no inversion X = excluded from this analysis
without location first) A A A A
e.g., Golfed Debbie in the morning.
Performance on hard ungrammatical sentence types is at chance
Hard Rule Grammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst
for types 1 , 2, 3 and 4 in the rule-search condition, and at chance
for types 1 , and 4 in the incidental condition, and inaccurate at
Type 1( S V Location/S V Location) A x A A
above chance levels for performance on type 3 sentences, again
e.g., The pen is in the box and the
indicating wrongly held beliefs about the grammatical status of these
pencil is in the drawer.
A A I A sentences. Subjects trained in the implicit condition are above
Type 2 (S singular + be)
e.g., Where the car is is in the driveway.
chance on all hard ungrammatical sentence types, with the
Type 3 (S plural + be) A A c A exception of performance on type 4 sentences, which are
e.g., Where the apples are is in the bowl. problematic for subjects in all conditions. Interestingly, the
Type 4 (S singular + lexical verb) C A A A performance of those in the implicit condition, using the measures
e.g., Where Judy teaches is at the of chance and non-chance performance to sentence types described
University. here, appears to support Reber's claims about the relationship
Type A c c A
5 ( S plural + lexical verb) between learning condition and rule complexity. Performance on
e.g., Where Mary and Joe relax is by more hard rule sentence types is at above chance levels for those in
the pool. the implicit condition than for those in any other condition.
Performance by those in the instructed condition is at above chance
168 Further analyses Further analyses 1 69

levels for all easy rule sentence types, and is therefore better than
0 hard type
performance by those in the other conditions. Of course, these
patterns of results must be interpreted in the light of the c • easy type
significantly higher overall accuracy of performance of the �(.) .9
(.)
instructed condition on the easy rule, and their superior overall o:!
.8
<l)
mean accuracy on the hard rule, as reported earlier in the analyses
2(.)
for Hypotheses 1 , 2 and 3 . These patterns of results are discussed .7

further in Chapter 8. gCJ) .6


....
CJ)
.
<l)
.... . .5
7.2 Simple and complex rule pretest structures <l)
....
The analyses in section 7 . 1 revealed that subjects trained in the
0..
'-+-< .4
0
implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions performed CJ)
.3
inaccurately, at above chance levels, on type 2 grammatical easy §
<l)
.2
sentences. A judgment of ungrammatical to a sentence of this type, :;E
together with a judgment of ungrammatical to a sentence � Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
u
conforming to the type 2 hard grammatical type, was used to select condition
subjects for the present study following the procedure described in
section 5 .3 .2. More extensive pretesting of subjects' prior Figure 28. Mean Accuracy On Transfer Set Sentences
knowledge of the structures conforming to the easy and hard rules Conforming To The Types Used In The Pretest For Subjects
was avoided for reasons given in sections 1 .6 and 5 .3 .2 . In All Conditions
The relationship between performance o n these structures
across conditions is illustrated in Figure 28. In the case of both easy Like the findings reported in section 7 . 1 above, this result must
and hard pretest structures performance by the instructed condition be interpreted in the context of the analyses performed to address
shows the highest accuracy and performance by the rule-search the main hypotheses in Chapter 6, particularly the finding that
condition shows the lowest accuracy. For all conditions mean performance on sentences conforming to the the easy rule is
accuracy to the easy pretest structure is lower than mean accuracy significantly more accurate than performance on sentences
for the hard pretest structure. It thus appears that the easy pretest conforming to the the hard rule for the incidental, rule-search and
structure is harder than the hard pretest structure. instructed conditions, and significantly faster for subjects in all
To examine the relationship between performance on easy and conditions. This result does demonstrate, though, that the maj ority
hard pretest structures for subjects in each condition paired one-way of subjects in the implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions
t-tests were performed. These show a significant difference between have not modified their beliefs about the ungrammatical status o f
performance on the easy and hard structures for subjects trained easy grammatical type 2 sentences, and so, to this extent, have no t
under the implicit condition (df 5 1 , t == 3 .438, p<.0 1 ), and for learned an important condition on the easy rule. The accurate
subjects trained under the incidental condition (df 5 1 , t = 4.009, performance of those in the implicit and incidental conditions o n
p<. 0 1 ) . There is no significant difference between performance on hard grammatical type 2 sentences does not imply that they have
the two types of sentence for subjects trained under the rule-search learned the rules regulating these structures, as the overall results for
condition (df 5 1 , t = 1 .287, p>. 0 1 ), or for subjects trained under the accuracy on the hard rule reported in Chapter 6 show. They do
instructed condition (d/ 5 1 , t = 1 .399, p>.O l ). suggest that the particular configuration of two forms of the verb
'be' in sentences of this type are noticeable, and therefore
memorable. It is probably this knowledge of allowable bigram
pamngs, and memory for previous examples that guides the
decisions about grammaticality of those in the implicit and
1 70 Further analyses Further analyses 171

incidental condition. It is not clear why this should not also be so conditions (X2 = 5 . 597, p = . 1 3 3 0). Frequencies of subjects
for those in the rule-search condition, but it appears to be the case reporting they noticed grammatical rules is lowest for those in the
that rule-search learners are relying on more than memory for implicit condition, and highest for those in the instructed condition
bigrams and previously encountered instances in making their (see Table 26).
responses, and that the instruction to search for rules promotes a
processing strategy in which accumulated memories for co-occurring
words plays little part. This issue is further discussed in Chapter 8. Table 26. Observed Frequencies Of Subjects Reporting They
Noticed Rules Across Conditions
7. 3 Awareness and aptitude
Three of the questions in the debriefing questionnaire were used t o Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed
code subjects as noticing rules, having looked for rules and as able t o Noticed rules 11 17 16 19 63
verbalize the rules. These questions were 'Did you notice any rules Did not notice rules 15 9 10 7 41
of grammar when you did the first two exercises on the computer?' ; 26 26 26 26 Totals
'Were you looking for grammar rules when you did the first two
exercises on the computer?' and 'Can you say what the rules are?'
The data presented to address Hypothesis 4 in Chapter 6 consisted Nonetheless, the fact that seven subjects in the instructed
only of responses to the question 'Can you say what the rules are?' condition report that they did not notice rules is unusual considering
which was used to code subjects as able to verbalize the easy rule, the they had been taught them. Of course, subjects must interpret the
hard rule, both rules, or not able to verbalize rules. In this section I question about noticing rules in the light of the particular training
examine responses to the three questions in relation to differences condition they were assigned to, and it may be that these seven
induced by performance under the different training conditions. subjects are reporting the fact that they experienced problems in
This is important to address since it provides information about applying the rules they had been taught to examples during training,
whether the treatments were actually delivered. Large numbers o f rather than lack of awareness of rules.
subjects in the implicit and incidental conditions claiming to have
noticed and to have been looking for rules, for example, would 7.3.1.2 Looking for rules across conditions
imply that these conditions induced a mode of processing similar t o The results of the two-way Chi-square for rule-search across
that encouraged by the task demands of the rule-search condition. I conditions show a significant difference between the frequencies of
also examine the relationship between responses to these questions subjects reporting that they were looking fo r rules in the different
and measures of aptitude used to address Hypothesis 6 in an attempt training conditions (X2 = 1 3 .325, p<.0 1 ). The observed frequencies
to clarify what abilities contribute to awareness at the levels of for the Chi-square analysis are reported in Table 27. The numbers
noticing, rule-search and rule verbalizability under each condition. of subjects reporting that they were looking for rules is highest in
the rule-search and instructed conditions, and lowest in the implicit
7.3.l Measures of awareness and the effects of learning condition condition. This is to be expected. However, as with the result
To examine the relationship between the measures of awareness and reported above it is unusual that six subjects in the rule-search
learning condition three two-way Chi-square analyses of frequencies condition report themselves not to have been looking for rules,
of responses to each of three questions described above were since this was the requirement of their training condition. Again it is
performed, using Condition as a grouping factor. possible that these subjects could have been reporting themselves t o
have been unsuccessful in finding the rules, rather than not looking
7.3.1. 1 Noticing across conditions for them. It is also possible, of course, that these subjects were not
The results of the two-way Chi-square for awareness across learning following training instructions, due to fatigue or boredom.
conditions shows no significant difference between frequencies of
subjects reporting they had noticed rules in the four learning
1 72 Further analyses Further analyses 1 73

Significant numbers of subjects in the incidental condition also While the ratio of subjects reporting noticing and reporting that
report themselves to have been looking for rules. The total o f they were looking for rules is almost exactly the same, (4 1 /63 and
fifteen i s slightly less than the total o f seventeen who reported 40/64 favoring the 'noticed rules' and ' looking for rules' groups),
themselves as having noticed rules. The significant effect for the the ratio of subjects able to verbalize rules to those unable to
Chi-square is largely due to the smaller numbers of subjects in the verbalize is much lower, a ratio of 3 4 to 70. As discussed in the
implicit condition who report themselves looking for rules (post­ analyses for Hypothesis 4, the number of subjects able to verbalize
hoc cell contribution 3 .26 and -3 .26), compared to the larger is largest in the instructed condition. Although there is no necessary
numbers in the explicit rule-search and instructed conditions (post­ equation between being taught rules, using them, and being able t o
hoc cell contributions 1 .86 and - 1 .86). Even so, the fact that nine verbalize them or recall them, it i s unusual that larger numbers o f
subjects in the implicit condition reported that they were looking subjects in the instructed condition did not attempt to verbalize
for rules raises questions about the validity of the claim that rules. The procedure for coding subjects as able to verbalize that was
learning under implicit conditions, at least as operationalized in this used in the present study did not require that complete summaries of
study, is an 'unconscious' process of induction that takes place rules be given, only that subjects were able to report some aspect o f
largely outside of awareness of the structure of the stimulus domain. the stimulus domain relevant to each o f the rules and make some
comment on it. Perhaps the low numbers of instructed subjects
attempting to verbalize is again attributable to fatigue, since the
Table 27. Observed Frequencies Of Subjects Reporting They debriefing questionnaire was administered at the end of a lengthy
Were Looking For Rules Across Conditions experimental session. The methodology of using written reports as
measures of awareness is also likely to be less sensitive than other
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed procedures, like oral interview, or the requirement to give
Looked for rules 9 15 20 20 64 instructions to a yoked group of subjects, as used in Matthews et al.
Did not look for rules 17 11 6 6 40 ( 1 989).
26 26 26 26 Totals
7.3.2 Measures of awareness and aptitude
To assess the relationship between the awareness data reported
7.3.1.3 Verbalizability across conditions above and measures of language learning aptitude, unpaired t-tests of
The results of the two-way Chi-square for verbalizability across scores on the memory and grammatical sensitivity subtests by
learning conditions shows no significant difference between subjects in noticing/not noticing, looking for rules/not looking for
frequencies of subjects able to verbalize rules in the four learning rules, and able to verbalize/unable to verbalize groups, divided by
conditions (X2 = 7 . 1 66, p = .0668). The observed frequencies are condition, were performed. Results of the t-tests are reported below.
reported in Table 28.
7.3.2.1 Awareness and memory
There is no overall significant effect for differences in memory as
Table 2 8 . Observed Frequencies Of Subjects Who Were Able measured by the paired-associates aptitude task between those who
To Verbalize Rules Across Conditions noticed and did not notice rules. There is an effect approaching
significance for a relationship between memory and rule noticing
Implicit Incidental Rule-search Instructed for those trained in the instructed condition (df 24, t = -2.65 8, p =
Verbalized rules 6 7 7 14 34
.0 1 3 8). There is a mean memory score of 1 0 for those who noticed
Did not verbalize rules 20 19 19 12 70 rules and a mean score of 1 5 for those who did not notice.
26 26 26 26 Totals There is no overall significant effect for differences between
looking for rules/not looking for rules groups on this measure and
no significant effect for groups trained under any condition.There is
1 74 Further analyses Further analyses 1 75

no overall significant effect for differences between groups who can The large numbers of subjects in the incidental condition
and cannot verbalize the rules on this measure and no significant reporting they had noticed rules leads to the conclusion that this is a
effect for groups trained under any condition. variety of conscious learning, comparable to that in the rule-search
and instructed conditions, and differs from them only in terms of
7.3.2.2 Awareness and grammatical sensitivity the nature of the interaction of awareness and noticing with the
There is no significant overall difference in the grammatical specific characteristics of the different task demands. Similarly, the
sensitivity of groups at the level of noticing but a significant effect 1 1 subjects reporting they had noticed rules in the implicit
for differences between those who noticed rules and those who did condition raises questions about the claim that the instruction to
not in the rule-search condition (dj 24, t = -3 .453, p<.O 1 ) . Those attend and memorize induces a variety of learning different from
who noticed rules in the rule-search condition have a mean that occurring in the explicit conditions. This finding for the
grammatical sensitivity score of 25 compared to a mean of 1 6 for implicit and incidental conditions probably reflects a similar
those who did not notice rules. propensity for second language learners to consciously notice rules
There is no significant overall difference in the grammatical of the second language in classrooms during supposedly meaning­
sensitivity of those who looked for rules and those who did not, but focused activities, and raises doubts over claims that meaning­
there is an an effect approaching significance for those in the focused treatments of themselves necessarily preclude learners from
implicit condition (dj 24, t = -2.683 , p = .0 1 3 0). Subjects who consciously attending to and noticing the form of input (see Beretta
looked for rules in this condition have a mean grammatical 1 989; Schmidt & Frota 1 986; Slimani 1 992 for discussion).
sensitivity score of 26 compared to a mean of 1 8 for subjects who Nonetheless, a larger number of implicit learners claimed not to
report themselves not to have looked for rules. have noticed rules, compared to learners in other conditions. The
Finally, there is no significant overall difference in the poor performance of the implicit condition on overall measures of
grammatical sensitivity scores of those who were able to verbalize accuracy reported in the analyses for Hypotheses 1 , 2 and 3 is likely
rules versus those who were not, but a significant effect for a to be attributable to just this differential in awareness. Less learning
difference in grammatical sensitivity scores of those able to occurred because fewer numbers of subjects were attending to the
verbalize and unable to verbalize who were trained in the implicit rule-governed nature of the stimulus domain.
condition (df 24, t = -3 . 1 73 , p<. 0 1 ). There is a mean grammatical There was a significant difference between the numbers of
sensitivity score of 29 for the able to verbalize rules group versus 1 8 subjects reporting they were looking for rules in each condition (see
for the unable to verbalize rules group. Table 27). This is attributable to the contrast between the large
numbers looking for rules in the rule-search and instructed
7.3.3 Summary ofthefindings for awareness conditions, compared to the smaller numbers in the implicit
No significant difference was found between the frequencies of condition, as the previously mentioned post-hoe cell contributions
subjects reporting themselves to have noticed rules in each for these cells illustrate. This result is to be expected, given the
condition (see Table 26). To this extent, claims for a causal nature of the differences in task instructions. Nonetheless, large
difference between the learning conditions and the learning numbers of incidental and implicit subjects report themselves to be
processes facilitated in this study attributable to the role of engaged in active rule-search, again casting doubt on the assertion
conscious awareness are questionable. This measure of awareness that the different conditions induce different varieties of learning,
-the self-reported act of noticing-does not neatly distinguish attributable to differences along the dimension of conscious
those in the rule-search and instructed conditions who are awareness of structure. There was no significant difference between
consciously aware of the rule-governed nature of the stimulus the conditions in terms of the ability to verbalize rules, though
domain to be learned, and who thus learn 'explicitly,' from those in twice as many subjects were able to verbalize rules in the instructed
the implicit and incidental conditions who are not conscious of the condition compared to other conditions (see Table 28).
What cognitive abilities contribute to awareness, given the
demands of particular task conditions? There were differences
1 76 Further analyses Further analyses 1 77

between learners who noticed and did not notice rules in the aptitude test reported in the analyses for Hypothesis 6 in Chapter 6.
instructed condition that approach significance for memory (p = It should also be noted that there are no correlations of measures
. O 1 3 ), but no significant effect for differences in grammatical of aptitude with the reported awareness of subjects in the incidental
sensitivity on awareness for these subjects. It would have been condition, a result which again supports the earlier reported findings
unusual if strong effects for a relationship between aptitude and for no significant correlation between aptitude and successful
noticing rules had been found for subjects in the instructed performance in the transfer task for this group. In contrast, for
condition, given that the task demands explicitly made them aware subjects trained under the implicit condition, differences in
of the structure of the stimulus display. However, as discussed above, grammatical sensitivity are linked to degree of awareness along the
the 7 subjects reporting themselves not to have noticed rules in this dimension of looking for rules and verbalizing rules, and also fo
condition may have been interpreting the question to mean, 'Could successful performance on easy and hard rules. For both implicit and
you apply the rule to examples successfully?' or, 'Do you incidental learners, then, the variety of learning induced by the
understand the rules you were taught?' or simply, ' Do you memorization or meaning-focused tasks demands does not appear to
remember the rules?' In this light the role of memory for those in affect their propensity to notice or consciously search for rules.
the instructed condition could be explained, since subjects claiming However, only for implicit learners is awareness related to aptitude,
not to have noticed rules are those with poorer memory scores, and as measured by the grammatical sensitivity subtest. Coupled with the
consequently they may have been unable to remember and so apply results of the earlier reported correlations of accuracy and aptitude
the rules. No effects for differences in memory ability contributing for implicit learners, this suggests that the high aptitude of a small
to the ability to notice rules was found for subjects in the implicit number of learners in this condition contributed to their ability t o
condition, despite the fact that this condition required successfully notice and learn the easy and hard rules. I n contrast, the
memorization. There was a significant effect for differences processing demands of the incidental learning task disengaged
between those who noticed and did not notice rules in the rule­ aptitude and awareness.
search condition with regard to differences in measures of
grammatical sensitivity, and this is to be expected given the nature 7.4 Summary offurther analyses and results
of the task demands which require rule-search subjects to find rules, The conclusions to be drawn from the further analyses are as
since subjects in this condition probably rely heavily on prior follows:
training in making metalinguistic judgments, and the procedures o f
formal grammar learning which are likely to b e linked to the degree • The grammaticality of sentence stimuli affects accuracy but not
of grammatical sensitivity to the structural characteristics of reaction time. There are significant differences in accuracy of
language. response to sentence types, with all subjects performing more
There were no significant differences in measures of aptitude in poorly on hard rule sentence types than on easy rule sentence
groups that were divided by the factor of looking for rules. types. Subjects in the implicit condition perform at above chance
However, there was an effect approaching significance for levels of accuracy on more hard rule ungrammatical sentence types
differences in grammatical sensitivity for subjects in the implicit than subjects in any other condition. Grammaticality affects
condition (p = .0 1 3). Those subjects with higher grammatical accuracy of response to the extent that subjects in the instructed
sensitivity were more likely to engage in active rule-search. Finally, condition perform more poorly on hard rule ungrammatical
the implicit condition again shows the only significant effect for sentences than on any others.
grammatical sensitivity on the ability to verbalize rules (p<.01). I t
should be noted that these results for the importance o f • There is a significant difference in accuracy of response to those
grammatical sensitivity to measures o f awareness for those in the sentence types conforming to the structures used in the pretest for
implicit condition support the previous results reported for subjects in the implicit and incidental conditions, but not for
correlations between success in performance on the transfer tas.k subjects in the rule-search or instructed conditions.
and scores on the grammatical sensitivity component of the
1 78 Further analyses

• There is no significant difference across conditions in measures


of noticing rules, or measures of the ability to verbalize rules but a
significant difference between groups in the extent to which they Chapter 8
were looking for rules, with more subjects in the rule-search and
instructed conditions looking for rules than subjects in the incidental
or implicit condition. The Fundamental Similarity Of
• Measures of aptitude do not differ significantly for those in the Implicit And Explicit L2 Learning
incidental condition or in the instructed condition with regard to
groupings based on the awareness data. There is a significant
difference for those in the rule-search condition where subjects who
attest to having looked for rules have significantly higher 8. 0 Introduction
grammatical sensitivity scores than subjects who did not claim to be In this chapter conclusions are drawn concerning the role o f
looking for rules. There is a significant difference for those in the awareness and attention to form in the different experimental
implicit condition where subjects who are able to verbalize the rules conditions (section 8 . 1 ), and concerning the effects of learning
have significantly higher grammatical sensitivity scores than under these conditions at the levels of rule, grammaticality and
subjects who cannot verbalize the rules. sentence type (section 8 .2). It is suggested that the learning effects
observed at these levels are a consequence of the conscious
processing strategies encouraged by the task demands made during
training in each condition, as predicted by the Fundamental
Similarity Hypothesis described in section 4.3 .4, and that such
effects do not constitute evidence for separate conscious and
nonconscious learning processes. In section 8.3, explanations of the
performance of subjects in each condition are proposed which relate
the processing demands of the different training conditions t o
differential performance on sentence types i n the transfer task.

8. 1 Awareness and task demands


The claim that implicit and incidental learning differ from learning
following the instruction to search for rules, or following explicit
instruction in the rules, along a dimension of awareness of the
structure of the stimulus domain, has not been shown to be true in
the present study. As the further analyses of responses to the
debriefing questionnaire reported in section 7 .3 showed, there was
no significant difference between the conditions in measures of
awareness of structure. In response to the question 'Did you notice
any rules of English grammar in the first two exercises you did o n
the computer?' large numbers o f subjects in all conditions replied
that they had. To this extent, none of the effects for learning in
any of the conditions can be attributed to a nonconscious process o f
abstraction. Neither was there a significant difference between the
groups in terms of their ability to verbalize rules, though
1 80 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 181

significantly greater numbers of subjects in the rule-search and search may in some cases play a part. The evidence from the
instructed conditions claimed to have been actively looking for rules awareness data, for example, shows that some subjects in all
when compared to subjects in the implicit and incidental conditions, conditions attested to looking for rules. Similarly, although the rule­
a fact which is to be expected considering the task instructions in search and instructed conditions predispose learners to search for, or
the explicit conditions. These results, then, demonstrating apply rules, they do not preclude the possibility that learners will
widespread awareness of structure across conditions, are support for also gather information by memorizing examples. As the awareness
the Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis proposed in Chapter 4. This data showed, despite the fact that significantly greater numbers o f
holds that all L2 learning is the result of conscious awareness of subjects in the explicit conditions claimed to have been looking for
form at the point of input, which is explicitly attended following rules, there were a number of subjects in each of these conditions
the information processing operations described in Chapter 3 , and who did not report that this is what they were doing.
that the effects of learning under implicit and explicit conditions The varieties of learning and awareness that take place under
are artifacts of the variety of conscious processing encouraged by each learning condition are also likely to be affected by individual
different task demands. differences and propensities for processing language data, in addition
to particular task demands. The data examining the relationship o f
8.1.1 Varieties of awareness under different learning conditions grammatical sensitivity and memory to awareness showed a
To the extent that the task demands in the four experimental significant difference between the grammatical sensitivity scores o f
conditions differ, so too does the variety of awareness of structure those implicit subjects who were able to verbalize rules and those
that the task demands facilitate. Schmidt ( 1 990), Whittlesea and who could not, higher grammatical sensitivity scores being
Dorken ( 1 993), and Shanks and St. John ( 1994) have all argued that associated with greater ability to verbalize the rules. The sanie
task demands are powerful determinants of what gets noticed. The difference in grammatical sensitivity scores for the implicit group
Fundamental Similarity Hypothesis described in section 4.3 .4 approached significance for the distinction between those who were
claimed that different learning conditions are likely to lead to a looking for rules, and those who were not. Grammatical sensitivity
differential reliance on strategies for accumulating knowledge of the scores were also significantly different for those subjects in the rule­
stimulus domain, but do not lead to differential reliance o n search condition who claimed to have noticed rules, and those who
conscious and nonconscious information processing. Subjects in all did not. No significant differences in aptitude scores were found for
conditions are similar in this respect, since they adopt conscious any groupings by measures of awareness for those in the incidental
strategies for meeting the demands of the particular task conditions. or instructed conditions, however. This is to be expected for the
This often led, across conditions in the present study, to noticing of instructed condition, since they had been explicitly made aware of,
rules of grammar underlying the stimulus domain. However, the and taught the rules in the training sessions. The results for the
different task conditions do lead to different varieties of awareness incidental condition parallel the findings of no significant
of structure. These varieties of awareness are largely a consequence correlation with aptitude on easy or hard rule accuracy in the
of the processing demands of particular task conditions. The transfer set.
implicit condition, with its instruction to memorize, is likely to In the incidental condition, successful learning, and the abilities
facilitate the development of a knowledge base in which memory leading to awareness or noticing of rules, are unrelated to those
for co-occurring words plays a large part, whereas the rule-search abilities measured by aptitude tests. This is an interesting finding,
and instructed conditions are likely to facilitate the development o f since it suggests that processing in the incidental condition draws on
a knowledge base in which the inductive search for, or deductive different abilities than processing in the other conditions and
application of abstract configurational relations between categories partially confirms Hypothesis 6, based on Reber's and Krashen's
plays a large part. However, each condition will encourage a blend claims, that learning under the implicit conditions will be insensitive
of these types of processing. The effects for learning under the to individual differences in measures of relevant cognitive abilities
implicit condition, for example, will be largely attributable to the like aptitude. However, the finding that aptitude correlates with
process of noticing co-occurring words, though conscious rule- successful learning of both rules in the implicit condition contradicts
1 82 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 83

this hypothesis (for further analyses of the awareness, aptitude and order to understand meaning, as well as following instructions to
learning interactions in the present study see Robinson 1 995b). apply rules or instructions to rule-search. Just as in the implicit
condition, so in the incidental condition, which Krashen ( 1 982,
8.1.2 The sensitivity of measures of awareness 1 985, 1 994) has speculated creates the conditions for acquisition
Reber's claim that implicit learning is a process of rule abstraction processes to occur, there is no evidence for a neat dissociation of
that takes place outside of conscious awareness of the structure of unconscious learning and articulable knowledge. Rather, the process
the stimulus domain is based on the evidence of failure to articulate of attending to meaning facilitated by the incidental condition was
the rules following transfer test performance that shows sensitivity often accompanied by an awareness of the grammatical structure of
to the structure of letter strings generated by artificial grammars. what was read, and in a number of cases by the attempt to find
Reber has been criticized on the grounds that the typical measures grammatical rules, which were sometimes articulable.
of awareness used, oral reports, are not sensitive enough to the The readiness of large numbers of subjects in the implicit and
contents of subjects' awareness (see Brody 1 989; Dulany, Carlson & incidental conditions to attest to awareness of the rule-governed
Dewey 1 984, 1 985; Perruchet & Gallego 1 993; Shanks & St. John nature of the stimuli in present study parallels, to an extent, the
1 994). Shanks and St. John have also claimed that these measures findings from the experimental studies of artificial grammar
do not meet the 'information' criterion of asking subjects exactly learning. In those studies subjects in implicit conditions do often
what they were aware of and had learned. Since it may not have claim to be aware of having learned something, but they are
been rules, and may instead have been knowledge of bigrams or predominantly unable to articulate the rules themselves. Likewise,
chunks that they were aware of, such measures create the very few subjects in the implicit or incidental conditions of the present
effect that they claim is causal-an inability to articulate the study were able to verbalize any aspect of the rules regulating the
knowledge base that guides decision making about grammaticality. sentences viewed during training. There are, however, some rather
Similar criticisms could be made of the measures of awareness used obvious differences between the subjects for the present study and
in the present study. Although many subjects claimed to have the subjects for experimental studies like those of Reber. The L2
noticed rules, few were able to articulate them, and consequently the learners in the present study already have a knowledge base to draw
small numbers of subjects able to verbalize rules revealed n o on in performing the tasks presented during training and in transfer.
significant difference between conditions i n this respect. If, They are also experienced and motivated learners of the language
however, subjects had been additionally asked which sentences, or presented during training. Given this experience, motivation, and
combinations of words they remembered, as well as any rules they background knowledge it seems likely to be the case that presenting
identified, larger numbers of subjects may have responded. The use unfamiliar sentences and structures, in any of the conditions, will
of debriefing recognition tasks in addition to recall tasks might have trigger access to, and conscious reflection on, some previous
produced more evidence of the precise components of rules and experience of language learning. Subjects for Reber's implicit
structures that subjects were aware of in all conditions, since direct learning studies, on the other hand, have no previous experience of
recall of rules is a measure of awareness biased towards transfer of artificial grammar learning, no motivation to do it, and no
the processing strategies encouraged at task in the rule-search and background knowledge to invoke. With this in mind it is surprising
instructed conditions. Additional use of recognition tasks t o that larger numbers of subjects in the present study did not attempt
determine what subjects noticed would facilitate a similar transfer o f to verbalize the rules they claimed to have been aware of.
the processing strategies encouraged at task in the implicit and As with experimental research in artificial grammar learning, so
incidental conditions. L2 research into implicit learning also needs to develop sensitive
Nonetheless, despite these shortcomings, the measure of measures of awareness that properly meet the information criterion
awareness used for the present study suggests that large numbers of by not unnecessarily restricting the scope of possible answers t o
subjects in all conditions were aware of rules in the sentences they questions about the objects o f awareness during learning. The
processed, whether they were doing this in order to memorize or in information criterion will be important to meet in cases like the
present study where large numbers of subjects attest to awareness,
1 84 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 85

since it is important to be able to distinguish the objects of clear advantages for instruction on easy rules relative to learning
attention and hypothesis testing during training from the other under the other conditions operationalized in the present study, and
contents of background knowledge and previous learning experience clear disadvantages for those attempting to consciously search for
that are invoked by association with the present task, but are not hard rules, relative to those who have been instructed in them .
intrinsic or relevant to it. This may have been the case in the These findings contradict the hypotheses based on Reber and
present study if subjects reported awareness on the basis of noticing Krashen, and also the more recent hypothesis of Hulstijn and
examples of third person singular morphemes or the occurrence o f DeGraaff ( 1 994: 1 03 ) that "explicit instruction has more effect in
regular and irregular past tense markers-things which were n ot the case of complex rules than in the case of simple rules."
critical to learning the easy and hard rules underlying the sentences Easy rule sentences were clearly easier to learn than hard rule
presented during training. sentences for all subjects using measures of reaction time, since they
elicited significantly faster responses than hard rule sentences in all
8.2 Learning at different levels of the dependent variable conditions. They were also easier for the incidental, rule-search and
The first two main hypotheses for the present study concerned the instructed conditions using measures of accuracy, since these
differential effectiveness of the implicit and explicit learning conditions were significantly more accurate on easy rule sentences.
conditions in learning pedagogic rules that were judged to differ in Therefore Hypothesis 3, that the distinction between easy and hard
complexity, following the procedure described in section 2 .4. These rules would be reflected in more accurate, and faster responses t o
hypotheses were motivated by the claims of Reber ( 1 989, 1 99 3 ) easy rule sentences, was largely supported. However, there was n o
and Krashen ( 1 982, 1 985) that learning as a consequence o f significant difference i n accuracy of response to easy and hard
exposure under implicit and incidental conditions would be superior sentences for the implicit condition. Subjects in this condition,
to learning under rule-search and instructed conditions where the despite having the lowest mean accuracy of response to the easy
stimulus domain was complex. By replicating Reber's implicit and rule, did perform above chance on both rules, and therefore showed
explicit rule-search conditions and matching them with incidental evidence of knowing something about the easy and hard rules.
and instructed conditions, the study also examined the extent of the However, they did not display the sensitivity to the complexity o f
generalizability of claims made by Reber, and results obtained using the stimulus domain that learners · in the other conditions did.
his methodology, to conditions invoked by Krashen in discussion The effects just described were the result of comparisons of
about the role of consciousness in SLA. performance on all easy and hard rule sentences across all
There was no support for Hypothesis 2, which claimed that the conditions. In order to examine in more detail the differential
implicit conditions would be superior in measures of speed and effects of learning under the four conditions, the accuracy and
accuracy of response to structures conforming to the hard rule, reaction time data were further examined in Chapter 7. There the
when compared to performance by subjects in the rule-search and effects of the factors of grammaticality and sentence type were
instructed conditions. There was only partial support for considered across conditions. The distinction, then, between the
Hypothesis 1 , which claimed implicit and incidental conditions analyses for Chapter 6, and the main hypotheses addressed there,
would pattern together in performance on all sentences, as would and the further analyses in Chapter 7, lies in the level of the
rule-search and instructed subjects. Although the implicit and dependent variable at which the differential effects of learning under
incidental conditions performed similarly on both rules, there was a implicit, incidental, rule-search and instructed conditions were
significant difference in accuracy of performance on the hard rule observed. These levels are distinguished below before the results of
between instructed and rule-search subjects. Instructed subjects were the further analyses performed in Chapter 7 are summarized.
significantly more accurate than rule-search subjects. There was also
a significant difference in accuracy of response to easy rule 8.2. 1 The level of rules
sentences for instructed subjects versus subjects trained in all other The notion of rule operationalized in the present study was one
conditions. Again, instructed subjects were significantly more which grouped together sets of sentences with the intention, in the
accurate, with a mean accuracy rate of over 84%. There are thus instructed condition, of drawing attention to allowable and non-
1 86 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 87

allowable patterns of covariance in sentence structure. So, with the evidence of what is possible in the language, without the benefit of
easy rule, sentences that illustrated the possibilities of subject-verb negative evidence of what is not possible. As in the artificial
inversion following the fronting of adverbials of two different types grammar learning experiments by Reber ( 1 989) and others,
were grouped together. For the hard rule sentences were grouped described in section 4 . 1 , learning was assessed in a transfer task
together to illustrate the different forms that pseudo-clefts of during which subjects had to classify equal numbers of grammatical
location could take. At a level of abstraction from the facts of and ungrammatical sentences. There was a significant effect for the
individual sentences, then, knowing the rule requires access t o interaction of Grammaticality and Rule for the instructed condition.
general principles of configurationality, like word order, and notions Instructed subjects performed much less accurately on hard rule
of agreement, tense and number, and an ability to identify parts of ungrammatical sentences than on other sentences. There was n o
structures that these principles relate to, and to make decisions such effect fo r Grammaticality for the instructed condition o n the
about whether they conform to the relations between the general easy rule, or for any other condition on either rule.
principles. Knowing the rule implies an ability to make judgments
based on these principles across the entire set of sentences 8.2.3 The level of type
exemplifying the rule, regardless of whether they are novel or have As the further analyses for Chapter 7 showed, there were differences
been previously experienced. The analyses for the main hypothesis across all conditions with respect to performance on individual
compared performance on all easy and hard rule sentences across types of sentences, using measures of chance and non-chance
learning conditions, using measures of accuracy and reaction time, performance. The level of sentence type was manipulated t o
and identified an advantage for the instructed condition over the conform to the particular conditions on rules that were explained in
others in accuracy of performance on the easy rule, but no clear the instructed condition. The significant interaction of Rule and
advantage for any single condition in accuracy of performance o n Gammaticality in measures of accuracy for the instructed condition
the hard rule, and n o advantage fo r any single condition o n either of was clearly shown to relate to above chance inaccurate performance
the rules with respect to reaction time. As stated above, these on two ungrammatical hard rule sentence types, as illustrated in
findings clearly refute the hypothesized advantage for the implicit Table 24 (section 7. 1 ). The instructed condition performed at
and incidental conditions on hard rules, and demonstrate the chance on a third ungrammatical hard rule sentence type. Rule­
positive effects of instruction on learning easy rules. search and incidental subjects performed at chance on four of the
The further analyses in Chapter 7, however, showed effects for hard rule ungrammatical sentence types. However, subjects in the
differences in the status of grammatical and ungrammatical implicit condition performed above chance on four of the hard rule
sentences, and for individual sentence types conforming to easy and ungrammaticartypes, and to this extent performed better than the
hard rule sentences. No group performed at above chance on all instructed, or any other condition on the hard rule. The analyses of
sentence types, and to this extent no group could be said to have mean accuracy and reaction time across all easy and hard rule
learned both rules successfully. In fact, using the criterion of above sentence types did not reveal this effect, since there were n o
chance performance on all sentence types illustrating a rule, only significant differences between the implicit, incidental or instructed
the instructed group learned a rule successfully, that rule being the conditions in performance on hard rule sentence types, though
easy rule. Greater length of exposure may have increased the there was a significant difference between the superior instructed
amount of learning of the hard rule, but the amount of exposure in condition and the rule-search condition.
the present study, two trials, or runs, of 40 sentences, was In the sections below, the issue of performance on hard rule
comparable to that in the implicit learning studies summarized in ungrammatical sentence types is examined in relation to the
Table 3, section 4. 1 . 1 . specific processing demands of the different learning conditions, and
the nature of the grammaticality violations contained in each type.
8.2.2 The level of grammaticality This is an important issue to address, since performance on hard
All sentences presented during training were grammatical. The ungrammatical sentence types contradicts the effects shown for
training sessions were thus examples of learning from positive instruction in performance on the easy rule sentence types, and o n
1 88 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 89

hard grammatical sentence types. Explanations are also considered Table 29. Percentage Correct Responses To Easy And Hard
for why the instructed condition differed from the other conditions Rule Sentence Types Presented In The Transfer Task
in performance on type 2 easy grammatical types, since this effect
contributed in large part to the resulting significant advantage of the Sentence Type % Correct
instructed group in performance on the easy rule. Performance in
the implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions was above chance, Easy Rule Grammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst
and inaccurate in performance on this type, while performance m
the instructed condition was above chance and accurate. Type 1 (S V Location) 86.5 82.7 84.6 92.3
e.g., Lisa fell onto the grass.
8.3 Explanations of the effects of grammaticality and type on Type 2 (Location V S) 3 6 . 5 ? 36.5? 28.8? 78.8
learning e.g., Across the street raced Tom.
Explanations of results of implicit learning studies have either Type 3 (Location S V) 6 5 .4 73 . l 6 1 .5 96.2
e.g., Through the window Maria stared
argued that the results of such learning are evidence for abstraction
Type 4 (S V Time) 67.3 73 . l 90.4 88.5
of rule-based knowledge (Reber 1 989), or knowledge based o n
e.g., Peter woke at eight o 'clock.
memory fo r fragmentary instances encountered during training Type 5 (Time S V) 69.2 76.9 76.9 92.3
(Dulany et al. 1 984; Perruchet & Pacteau 1 990; Rothkopf & e.g., On her birthday Gabi sang.
Dashen 1 995; Vokey & Brooks 1 992). Some have argued that the
effects of implicit learning are attributable to a synergy of both Easy Rule Ungrammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst
forms of knowledge (Matthews et al. 1 989). This debate was
reviewed in Chapter 4. Like Shanks and St. John ( 1 994), Whittlesea Type 1 (Time V Sino inversion
and Dorken ( 1 993) and McAndrews and Moscovitch ( 1 985) I have without location first) 64 .6 73.8 67.7 84.6
argued there is no reason, a priori, to associate the products o f e.g., On Saturday night danced Charlie.
learning under any condition with a knowledge base composed o f Type 2 (V S Adv/no inversion
either entirely rule-based o r entirely memory-based knowledge and without location first) 70.8 78.5 73.8 74.6
that the processing demands of tasks are likely to encourage the e.g., Golfed Debbie in the morning.
development of a knowledge base composed of a blend of these two
sources of knowledge. However, task conditions will predispose Hard Rule Grammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst
learners to rely more heavily on one than on the other.
Type 1( S V Location/S V Location) 7 1 .2 7 1 .2 80.8 80.8
e.g., The pen is i n the box and the
8.3.1 Learning in the instructed condition
pencil is in the drawer.
Training in the instructed condition requires subjects to apply rules Type 2 (S singular + be) 6 5 .4 75 40.4? 88.5
consciously to a display of positive examples. In the transfer phase e.g., Where the car is is in the driveway.
subjects are required to use this knowledge to discriminate Type 3 (S plural + be) 63.5 6 1 .5 5 1 . 9* 88.5
grammatical from ungrammatical strings. The training phase thus e.g., Where the apples are is in the bowl.
encourages them to look for positive evidence of structural Type 4 (S singular + lexical verb) 50* 6 1 .5 6 1 .5 84.6
configurations that conform to the rules they have been taught. e.g., Where Judy teaches is at the
Sentences presented during transfer in all conditions contained equal University.
numbers of grammatical and ungrammatical examples corresponding Type 5 ( S plural + lexical verb) 6 1 .5 55.8* 57.7* 76.9
to the easy and hard rule sentence types presented during training. e.g., Where Mary and Joe relax is by
the pool.
These types are summarized in Table 29, and Appendices G and J
contain the full set of training and transfer task sentences.
1 90 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 191

Table 29. (Continued) Percentage Correct Responses To Easy singular (e.g., Where Carl writes are at a desk not o n the table).
And Hard Rule Sentence Types Presented In The Transfer Thirdly there were sentences violating the constraint that the verbs
Task used must agree in tense (e.g., Where the bird is was in the sky).
Fourthly there were sentences violating the constraint that
Sentence Type % Correct sentences contrasting locations using this construction should begin
with 'where'(e.g., What the King lives is in an apartment not in a
Hard Rule Ungrammatical Types Imp Inc Rule-s Inst palace). Finally there were sentences of the type (Where Mark
works is in America in Japan), which violates the constraint that
Type l(no inversion with Where) 6 1 .5 5 3 .8* 53 .8* 7 1 .2 the contrasting location phrase must be joined by 'not.'
e.g., Where is Bill is in the country.
Type 2 (invariant singular 'be') 5 9 .6 44 .2* 5 3 .8* 3 4 .6? 8.3.1.1 Confirmation bias and grammaticality
e.g., Where Carl writes are at a desk. Subjects in the instructed condition performed accurately at above
Type 3 (required tense agreement) 63.5 38.5? 48. 1 * 53.8*
chance levels on all types apart from three hard ungrammatical
e.g., Where the bird is was in the sky.
types. Performance on type 3 sentences was at chance, and
Type 4 (required 'Where') 48 . 1 * 4 8 . 1 * 5 5 . 8 * 23 . 1 ?
e.g., What the King lives is in a palace.
performance on types 2 and 4 was above chance but inaccurate,
Type 5 (required 'not' for contrasting indicating wrongly held beliefs about these sentences (see Table 29).
locations) 69.2 76.9 65.4 67.3 Why should performance on these ungrammatical types be poorer
e.g., Where Mark works is in America in the instructed condition than performance on ungrammatical
in Japan. easy rule sentence types, which was significantly above chance and
accurate? One explanation lies in the additional processing and
Key: Imp Implicit condition *
= performance at chance
= reasoning demands imposed by making judgments using knowledge
.
Inc Incidental condition ? performance above chance but maccurate
= = of the hard rule relative to making judgments based on knowledge of
Rule-s Rule-search condition
=
the easy rule.
Inst Instructed condition
=
To make a judgment of grammaticality one can either scan the
sentences to find confirming evidence that it is grammatical, or
scan the sentence to find disconfirming evidence o f
In the transfer phase, ten grammatical and ten ungrammatical ungrammaticality. The latter i s the proper procedure, since transfer
sentences easy rule sentences were presented. The ten set sentences in the present study always contained a number of
ungrammatical examples were five tokens of two types of senten� e correctly applied grammatical subrules which could have been
that violated the rule presented during training (see Figure 5 m mistakenly taken as confirming evidence of grammaticality in cases
section 2.4.3): easy rule ungrammatical type 1 sentences in w?i�h where the sentence also contains a grammaticality violation. In the
subject-verb inversion occurred sentence initially (Golfed Debbie � n case of the easy rule it is relatively easy to check disconfirming
the morning), and easy rule ungrammatical type 2 sentences m evidence. If there is subject-verb inversion, check to see if
.
which subject-verb inversion occurred following fronted adverbmls adverbials of location are at the front. If they aren't, only then is it
of time (On Saturday night danced Charlie). ungrammatical. Of course, instructed subjects were made aware o f
For the hard rule (see Figure 6 in section 2.4.3) there were five this rule during training. It i s difficult to discover it through rule­
types of ungrammatical sentences, eaeh vi� l�ting �ne of the search, or to otherwise induce it, as the poor accuracy of
.
conditions on the hard rule taught durmg trammg. Firstly, there performance of subjects in the other conditions in the present study
were sentences violating the constraint on subject-verb inversion demonstrates. To correctly judge an ungrammatical hard rule
following ' Where' (e.g., Where is Bill is in the house not in the sentence, however, subjects must scan the sentence in order to
garden). Secondly there were sentences violating the c�ns.traint that identify whether any of the five possible violations occur. This may
.
the form of the verb ' be' following the first verb 1s mvanantly be done serially, by checking to see if each of the possible violations
1 92 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 93

has occurred, or in parallel. There is no simple rule of thumb t o hard ungrammatical type 2 and 3 sentences, which are
guide this search (see Faerch 1 986). Here the search fo r ungrammatical by virtue of the constraints on the allowable co­
disconfirming evidence is potentially more effortful than i n the occurrence of verbs which differ in tense, and of verbs which differ
ungrammatical easy rule sentences, since on occasions it may be the in number marking (see Table 29). A similar explanation could be
last of the five violations that is checked which leads t o given for above chance performance on hard ungrammatical type I
disconfirmation of the hypothesis o f grammaticality. There could sentences, since in these sentences the two verbs are separated by a
also be parallel search with the same result if some violations are subject nominal. The co-occurrences of verb forms in hard
more salient than others. ungrammatical sentence types I , 2 and 3 therefore contradicts the
Given the effortfulness of this task, then, the results for poor bigram and trigram knowledge picked up during the memorization
performance on hard rule ungrammatical sentences may reflect task. Like all other subjects, those in the implicit condition
'confirmation bias' in the instructed group, i.e., the tendency t o performed at significantly above chance accuracy on type 5
seek confirmation o f a hypothesis by looking fo r confirming sentences . Type 5 sentences are noticeably semantically ill-formed,
_
examples rather than by looking for disconfirming examples (see and possibly they are rejected on this basis by subjects in the
Evans et al. 1 993 ; Klayman & Ha 1 987; Wason 1 960). In this case implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions. They were only at
noticing that the first or second, or the most salient, of the possible chance on type 4 sentences. Type 4 sentences violate no bigram
hard rule grammaticality violations has not been made could lead knowledge that could have been picked up during training, and could
subjects to wrongly assume that none of the others had been made. have been wrongly accepted for the same reason given above for
This difference in confirmation, disconfirmation bias could explain those in the instructed condition. Alternatively, and in line with the
the difference in accuracy to ungrammatical easy rule and explanation given for above chance performance on types 1, 2 and
ungrammatical hard rule sentences for the instructed condition. The 3, these sentences could have been wrongly accepted by those in the
above chance performance on type I and type 5 hard implicit condition because the bigram 'is in,' which these sentences
ungrammatical violations suggests that these violations were contain, does occur in the grammatical sentences, and subjects could
checked first. If they were not violated then the sentence was have wrongly based their judgement of grammaticality on this
wrongly judged grammatical. This suggests a depth of search of two knowledge.
grammaticality violations for the subjects in the instructed Although the explanation of performance in the implicit
condition. Violations in type 2 and type 3 hard ungrammatical condition offered above is similar to that of Hayes and Broadbent
sentences, which contain a potentially confusing combination o f ( 1 988:25 1 ), who describe implicit learning as the "passive
verb tense agreement and number agreement violations, do n o t aggregation of information about the co-occurrence o f
appear to have been consistently checked by those i n the instructed environmental events and feature£," and similar also to that offered
condition. Neither were type 4 hard ungrammatical violations, by Perruchet and Pacteau ( 1 990; cf. Perruchet, Gallego & Savy
which posed problems across all conditions, consistently checked by 1 990; Vokey & Brooks 1 992) for performance in artificial
instructed learners, due possibly to the fact that background grammar learning experiments, there is an important modification
knowledge of the acceptability of sentence initial wh-words like necessary for the purposes of explaining implicit learning of the
'what' interfered with the application of the rule regarding this data presented in this study. Unlike the artificial grammar
violation and led learners to overgeneralize the rule. experiments, where the same letters occurred in the same bigram
pairings in the training and transfer phase, the same words did not
8.3.2 Learning in the implicit condition co-occur during training and transfer in this experiment.
· Transfer
In contrast to the instructed condition, processing in the implicit sentences were novel examples of the same type presented during
condition encourages subjects to search for allowable co-occurrences training. While the verb ' is' and its forms, which are critical t o
of words. Noticing allowable co-occurrences of words that relate to identifying the violations in type 1 , 2 and 3 hard rule
specific hard rule violations may thus have facilitated performance ungrammatical sentences, were necessarily rep.eated, the other
by implicit subjects. This could account for superior performance on lexical verbs were not repeated, nor were the subjects of sentences
1 94 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 195

which separate the verbs in type 1 violations. Thus, memory for 8.3.3 Learning in the incidental condition
the pairs of co-occurring verbs presented in training would need to While any learning that occurred in the implicit condition may
be accessed together with knowledge of differences in plural and have resulted largely from the memorization strategy encouraged
singular forms and past and present tense forms which moderated during training, learning in the incidental condition is likely to have
their relationship in the training set sentences in order to make the been constrained by the necessity to read for meaning and impose
judgment of ungrammaticality on transfer set sentences of types 1 , sets of propositional relations on sentences. To this extent the
2 and 3 . Activation of prior knowledge of grammatical subrules focus of attention would have been on identifying the semantic
therefore likely played a part in the decisions about grammaticality roles of subjects and objects in the sentences in order to establish
made in the implicit condition, which were, however, triggered by propositional meaning, and not on the features of tense and number
noticing restrictions on co-occurring words, rather than by a search agreement discussed above. Performance for the incidental group is
for more global rules. It could have been this grammatical above chance, as for subjects in all other conditions, on type 5 hard
knowledge that those claiming awareness in the implicit condition rule ungrammatical sentences which are semantically ill-formed by
had in mind. In order to confirm this, of course, more sensitive virtue of missing the negative conjunctive coordinator 'not' which
measures of the information the subjects were aware of would have is important to understanding the propositional value of these
to have been made, as acknowledged in section 8. 1 .2 sentences. However, there is a significant difference in accuracy of
The explanation offered above for differences in performance response to the two sentences illustrating this type for subjects in
by implicit and instructed subjects on hard ungrammatical sentence the incidental condition, and this is discussed below. Performance is
types suggests a distinction between the learning resulting from above chance but inaccurate on hard rule ungrammatical type 2 and
exposure in each condition which is attributable to differences in the 3 sentences, suggesting that the tense and number agreement
variety of conscious processing encouraged by the specific training violations in these sentences are overlooked, and are possibly
task demands. Although subjects in the implicit condition did redundant to the attribution of propositional sense, which is the
perform at above chance levels on more ungrammatical hard rule basis of decisions of well-formedness encouraged by transfer of the
sentence types than the instructed subjects, the mean performance processing requirements encouraged during training. Performance is
for accuracy on all hard ungrammatical sentences for this group is at chance on hard ungrammatical rule types 1 and 4.
still low, about 60%. While implicit learning due to memorization Subjects in the incidental group perform differently from subjects
could have occurred following the explanation given above, it is in other conditions in two respects, both of which suggest that they
thus likely to have been piecemeal associative knowledge of base their decisions about grammaticality on more than grammatical
allowable co-occurrences of words, supported by access to knowledge, and that they are influenced, to a greater extent than
knowledge of plurality and tense. To this extent the memorization subjects in the other conditions, by semantic acceptability and
strategies resulting in this knowledge base are likely to have been lexical aspects of transfer sentences. Firstly, there is the finding,
insufficient for the acquisition of knowledge of the allowable reported as evidence supporting Hypothesis 6, that aptitude scores
discontinuities in word order which characterize the easy rule for subjects in this condition, unlike those for all other conditions,
grammatical type 2 structures, since these take place over a wider show no significant correlation with success on easy or hard rule
window of associative learning and attention than two or three sentences. The abilities that contribute to successful learning in this
words. This rule also requires that subjects making correct judgments condition are not those captured in estimates of grammatical
of acceptability of easy rule grammatical type 2 sentences learn to sensitivity, or memory based on paired-associate learning. The
distinguish adverbials of location, which allow subject-verb inversion learning that results from performance in this condition thus
when fronted, from adverbials of time, which do not. Noticing this appears to be independent of traditional estimates of the ability t o
type of information is unlikely to result from the memorization focus on form, or remember examples. This is i n contrast t o
strategy, and so learning of this constraint on the easy rule did not learning in the rule-search and instructed conditions, which is
take place in the implicit condition. significantly correlated with measures of both of these abilities, and
1 96 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA 1 97

to learning in the implicit condition which is significantly correlated


_ be evidence of transfer of processing strategies encouraged by the
with measures of grammatical sensitivity. specific demands of their training condition task.
S�condly, subj_ects in this condition, unlike subjects in all other
.
cond�t10n�, a�e, m a number of cases, not sensitive to the type 8.3.4 Learning in the rule-search condition
class1ficatlon imposed on sentences in the transfer set. As reported As in Reber's studies, those in the rule-search condition performed
in Chapter 7, section 7. 1 .5 , McNemar tests were performed t o poorly on hard rule sentence types, being above chance and accurate
calculate the frequency o f accurate responses to the two sentence only in responses to type 1 and type 4 grammatical hard rule
tokens exemplifying each of the five easy grammatical types, and sentences, and type 5 ungrammatical hard rule sentences (see Table
each of the five hard grammatical and five hard ungrammatical 29). To this extent they performed more poorly than those in the
types for subjects in each condition. The results of these implicit condition, who were below chance on only two hard rule
calcu�ations show that for the_ incidental condition alone responses sentence types, though the results of overall hard rule performance,
to pairs of sentences representmg types were significantly different reported in Chapter 6, show no significant difference between the
In particular, the frequencies of accurate responses to sentence� groups. Unlike the findings from Reber's studies, those in the rul�­
exemplifying four of the transfer set sentence types were search condition do not perform significantly better than those m
significar_itly different (p<. 0 1 ). These four types were easy the implicit condition on easy rule sentences. The explanation
grammatical type l (sentence 1 a mean accuracy of .962, versus given for poor performance by rule-search subjects on complex
sentence 2, a mean accuracy of .692); hard grammatical type 1 stimuli given by Reber is that subjects in this condition search for
(sentence 1 a mean accuracy of .462, versus sentence 2, a mean and find rules, but these are based on false hypotheses which are n o t
accuracy of .962); hard ungrammatical type 2 (sentence 1 a mean commensurate with the structure o f the complex stimulus domain.
accuracy of . 6 1 3, versus sentence 2, a mean accuracy of .259); and The findings from the present study, however, do not suggest the
hard ungrammatical type 5 (sentence 1 a mean accuracy of . 6 1 5, rule-search subjects had found and consistently misapplied any rules,
versus sentence 2, a mean accuracy of .923). The two sentences either easy or hard. With the exception of performance on hard
exemplifyi�g each of these ty�es are illustrated in Appendix J,
. rule grammatical type 2 sentences, there are no responses to hard
together with sentences exemphfymg all other types used in the rule sentence types that are both above chance and inaccurate which
transfer set. For example, as Appendix J shows the first of the hard
. would support this conclusion. Subjects trained in the rule-searc.h
'
ungrammatical type 5 sentences is less noticeably semantically ill- condition perform worse on hard rule sentences than subjects in any
formed (Where the horse stands is in the field in the barn) than the other condition, being above chance and accurate only in response
�econd (Wh�:e Mark work� �s in America in Japan). While subjects to hard rule grammatical types 1 and 4, and hard rule ungrammatical
m all cond1t1ons are sensitive to the ungrammaticality of these type 5 sentences . It must be noted, however, that this training
sentences, and are above chance and accurate in responding t o condition differed from the othe:·s in the present study in not being
t�em, onl� i n the incidental condition is there a significant given feedback about the accuracy of their responses during training.
difference m acc�racy of response to the two sentences. It is clearly Such feedback was, instead, limited to questions asking if they were
not the gram�at1cal status of the sentences that distinguishes them, maintaining rule-search. This option was chosen due to the fact that
but !he�_r relative de�:ee of semantic acceptability, and it is this that asking specific questions about the structure of training examples ,
_
the mc1dental cond1tion-m contrast to the implicit memorization and giving feedback on the answers, would have preempted the
condition and the rule-focused explicit conditions-a- re most subjects' own rule-search decision-making processes, and guided
sensitive to, since this was the aspect of the stimulus sentences that their rule-search. In order to ensure comparab ility with Reber' s rule­
incidental subj ects w_ere instructed to attend to during training. Just search condition this alternative was avoided. However, this m ay
as for subjects m the other conditions, therefore the have negatively affected their performance relative to the other

grammaticality judgments of incidentally trained subjects ap ear t o conditions, who all received some form of instructional feedback
appropriate to their training conditions, albeit feedback m the
implicit and incidental conditions that was not targeted at the
1 98 Fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit SLA

formal characteristics of the sentences regulated by the easy and


hard rules.

8.4 Summary
Chapter 9
The explanation of behavioural differences in performance o n
transfer set sentence types offered above i s compatible "'.ith Summary And Conclusions
.
Schmidt's 'noticing' hypothesis, and the model of noticmg
proposed in 3 .2.6. Differential perfor� ance on tra?sfer set
sentence types is consistent with the claim that all subjects were
transferring the consciously mediated processing demands of the 9. 0 Introduction
different training tasks to the transfer task (cf. B laxton 1 989; Graf In this chapter the results of the present study are related to other
& Ryan 1 990; Hamann 1 990; Roediger et al. 1 989; s�adler 1 99 5 ),
. research into the differences between implicit and explicit L2
and that variations in the attentional demands of trammg tasks led learning and conclusions are drawn regarding the comparability o f
to variation in the extent of learning across conditions-not access Reber's claims about implicit learning to Krashen 's claims about
to separate conscious and unconscious learning systems (cf. K�ashen acquisition. Finally, after describing the implications and limitatio ns
1 982, 1 985, 1 994; Paradis 1 994; Zobl 1 992, 1 995). The evidence of the present study, the main findings are summarized.
of performance on hard rule sentences does not supp�rt Kr�shen
and Reber's claims that implicit and incidental learnmg will be 9.1 Implicit and explicit L2 learning
superior to explicit rule-search or instructed learni�g where . the The research questions addressed in this study are of basic
stimulus domain is complex. Although clearly supenor to subj�cts importance to SLA theory, as well as to prescriptions for L2
trained in other conditions on the easy rule and on grammatical pedagogy based on its findings. As stated in Chapter 1 there is
examples of hard rule sentences, the poor performa!lce of instructed considerable debate in L2 pedagogy about the relative merits of a
learners on ungrammatical hard rule sentences pomts to the need focus on form, a focus on meaning, or a combination of both (see
for negative evidence of ungrammaticality in ?�der t� prevent e.g., Hulstijn 1 990a; Krashen 1 982, 1 985, 1 993 , 1 994; VanPatten
overgeneralization of instruction based on positi�e eviden.ce o f 1 990; VanPatten & Cadierno 1 993). Arguments have been put
grammaticality alone (cf. White 1 99 1 ). Further, differences i� the forward that instruction is beneficial to subsequent L2 develop ment,
extent of awareness across conditions also reflect the attent10nal and that its role in facilitating attention to form is causal (Ellis
demands of the training tasks. However, even in the implicit and 1 990, 1 993a, 1 994b; Long 1 988, 1 99 1 , 1 994; Schmidt 1 990,
incidental conditions-which do not explicitly focus subjects' 1 995). Effect of instruction studies examining the relationship
attention on form-large numbers of subjects claim to have noticed between instruction, attention to form, and learning have typically
rules. In the case of implicit learners, those who noticed and were been conducted under classroom conditions (e.g., Gass 1 982; Harley
able to verbalize rules were those who had significantly higher 1 989, 1 993 ; Leeman et al. 1 995; Lyster 1 994; Spada & Lightbown
aptitude as measured by the grammatical sens �tivity subtest of the 1 993 ; Yip 1 994). Given the nature of such research it is unclear
.
MLAT. Coupled with the finding of high correlations o f what learning processes underlie the aggregate advantage of
grammatical sensitivity and accuracy o n easy and hard �les for instructed groups, and what the significant interactions of learning
implicit learners, these results suggest that those wh? did �earn conditions, as distinct from instructional treatment, and the formal
.
effectively in the implicit condition, were those with high aptitude characteristics of language are. Experimental studies of the effect of
and who therefore were more likely to · notice, and so learn, the instruction are necessary to identify such processes, and to thereby
patterns of structural covariation described by the pedagogic rules. address Krashen 's claim ( 1 985) that the aggregate advantages of
instruction summarized by Long ( 1 983 , 1 988) are due not t o
conscious selective attention to form facilitated by instructional
200 Summary and conclusions Summary and conclusions 20 1

treatments, but to the fact they represent acquisition-rich learners, then it will be important to provide a parallel estimate o f
environments in which comprehensible input is available in the difficulty o f the pedagogic explanation o f these structures, as in
sufficient quantities to promote incidental learning or acquisition. the present study, since the pedagogic explanation itself must be
Given, also, that a large part of the debate about the effectiveness appropriate to the level of structural detail it describes (see Figure 1 ,
of formal instruction has revolved around the question of whether section 2 . 1 .3).
to teach grammar rules (Krashen 1 992; Lightbown & Pienemann The issue of rule complexity and learning under implicit and
1993; Odlin 1 994; Rutherford & Sharwood Smith 1 988), it is explicit conditions has recently been addressed by other L2
particularly necessary to supplement quasi-experimental researchers. Hulstijn and DeGraaff ( 1 994) have motivated a series
demonstrations of the effectiveness of instruction in pedagogic rules of hypotheses about the relationship between implicit and explicit
(e.g., Alanen 1 995; Doughty 1 99 1 ; Eckman et al. 1 988; Gass 1 98:2; learning and structural complexity. They claim explicit learning will
Lightbown & Spada 1 990; Lightbown, Spada & Ranta 1 99 1 ; Lyster be more effective than implicit learning in the case of properties o f
1 994; Master 1 994) with evidence from studies using pure language falling outside the scope o f Universal Grammar. Explicit
experimental designs, as in N.Ellis (1993), and the present study. instruction, they claim, will be more effective than implicit learning
Second language acquisition theory has often invoked implicit where the targets of instruction are structures conforming to a
knowledge as one of a number of explanatory constructs in models parameter which is a subset of the setting for the L l . For the
of instructed L2 development (see Bialystok 1 98 1 ; Ellis 1 990, reasons given in Chapter 2, the present study avoided motivating
1 993a, 1 994a, 1 994b, 1 994c, 1 995; Krashen 1985, 1 994; Towell & the distinction between easy and hard rules using Universal
Hawkins 1 994) without addressing in empirical detail how such Grammar. However, Hulstijn and DeGraaff clearly see the influence
knowledge results from conditions of learning like those examined of Universal Grammar as separable from the issue of rule
in the present study. The debate about the representation o f complexity, since this is addressed in a further hypothesis, th at
implicit knowledge, and the extent o f its activation and explicit instruction will have more effect in the case of complex
development during implicit learning, reviewed in Chapter 4, is pedagogic rules than in the case of simple rules. Complexity is
important to address in the context of SLA if clear prescriptions described by Hulstijn and DeGraaff in 'cognitive' terms, and is
about the role of implicit knowledge in syllabus design and L2 illustrated by two hypothetical examples of rules of plural
methodology are to be given empirical substance. Recent L2 suffixation in two languages which differ in the number o f
research has attempted to address these issues using experimental derivational steps they require. Their notion o f complexity, then, is
methodologies, and some have addressed the question of the related to the number of steps needed to decide on whether a form
relationship between implicit learning and rule complexity applies in a structural context. They distinguish this from
examined in the present study. ' linguistic' descriptions of complexity (presumably those
motivating the first two Universal Grammar-based hypotheses), and
9.1.1 Second language learning and rule complexity evidence of complexity inferred from studies of development
The easy and hard pedagogic rules presented in the present study showing some structures to be early, and some to be late acquired.
were identified by the process of empirically validating the Their hypothesis, of course, is the opposite one to the hypothesis
distinction using ratings of the rules, and the structures they about the relationship of complexity to learning condition in the
contained, by experienced L2 teachers. As discussed in Chapter 1 , present study, motivated by the claims of Krashen and Reber, and it
other approaches to distinguishing the complexity of structures t o is not supported. The evidence from this study does not suggest th at
be learned by L2 learners have been motivated by linguistic notions explicit instruction is more effective on the hard rule than learning
of accessibility and entailability, within the framework of Universal under implicit or incidental conditions. It also shows clear
Grammar, and by the notion of generalizability based on research advantages for the instructed condition in learning the easy rule
into typological markedness (see sections 1 . 1 .2-4 ). If these criteria relative to the other conditions. The fact that the simple rule used
are adopted for distinguishing structures of different orders o f in the present study is reducible to a two-step rule of thumb, t o
complexity in studies o f implicit and explicit learning by L 2 illustrate the two conditions which would violate it were they not
202 Summary and conclusions Summary and conclusions 203

met (see section 8 .3 . 1 ), in contrast to the hard rule which has a contrast, DeKeyser claimed implicit learning would be equal to or
larger number of potential violations, suggests that Hulstijn and better than explicit learning in the case of prototypical rules, which
DeGraaff would also be satisfied with the characterization of the are probabilistic but not categorical. Like the present study
complexity differential for the two rules used in the present study. DeKeyser also hypothesized that the key dependent variables,
Hulstijn and DeGraaff have not reported any findings based on their categorical and prototypical rules, would differ in terms of their
hypotheses to date. learnability. Prototypical rules, he claimed, would be harder to learn.
A small-scale pilot study, using only six subjects (DeKeyser Assessment of the learning that took place in 25 twenty-minute
1 994) has also addressed the issue of the relationship between rule learning trials for 3 implicit and 3 explicit learners was done by
complexity and implicit learning. The first hypothesis for means of a grammatical classification task on counterbalanced
DeKeyser's study was identical to the second hypothesis for the grammatical and ungrammatical sentences produced by the rules.
present study, and the opposite of the prediction made by Hulstijn The results of the pilot study are similar to the results of the
and DeGraaff: implicit learning is better than explicit learning for present study. DeKeyser found that explicit learners performed
complex rules. The attribution of DeKeyser's second hypothesis, better on simple rules, and that implicit and explicit learners
that explicit learning is better than implicit learning for simple performed equivalently, but poorly, on hard prototypical rules.
rules, to Krashen and Reber is not quite accurate. In the case o f These findings were confirmed by the results of a larger scale study
Krashen ( 1 982, 1 985) the qualification that the advantages for using the same design (DeKeyser 1 995).
explicit learning will only be short-lived, must be added, since
Krashen has argued that despite short-term rate advantages for 9.1.2 Generalizing from Reber's claims about implicit learning to
learning on easy rules, implicit learning and the acquired system Krashen 's claims about acquisition
eventually catches up with the development of the learned system .
Similarly, it appears to follow from Reber's claim that implicit (i) The unconscious and conscious conditions. The present study
learning is always more robust than explicit learning, and develops matched learning conditions motivated by Reber's experimental
in advance of it (see Reber 1 993 and section 4. 1 ), that there could methodology with conditions implied by Krashen's distinction
be no more than equivalency in performance on simple rules under between conscious learning and unconscious acquisition. The
implicit and explicit conditions. It is true, though, that Reber et al. analyses for Hypotheses 1 and 2 support the claim for
( 1 980) did demonstrate the superiority of explicit instruction, comparability of performance by those in the implicit and
followed by structured exposure to relevant examples, over incidental conditions, but not the claim for comparability o f
instruction following exposure to examples, and over implicit performance by those in the rule-search and instructed conditions.
learning following instructions to memorize. Given the nature o f There are no significant differences between implicit and incidental
Reber's experimental treatments it was not possible to assess the learners in speed and accuracy of performance on transfer set
duration of this learning effect. However, Reber et al. add the exemplars of the easy and hard rules. However, instructed learners
further caveat that even for instruction on simple rules to be are significantly more accurate than rule-search learners in
effective when followed by structured exposure, attention must be performance on easy and hard rule structures, and there is an effect
consistently directed at those aspects of the stimulus display which approaching significance for the greater speed of the rule-search
are maximally coordinate with underlying rule. Explicit instruction learners relative to the instructed learners on both rules.
on simple rules, that is, can be less effective than implicit learning if The results for the instructed group parallel the findings of Ellis's
the rule itself is inefficient in directing learners' attention to the ( 1 993) finding for superior performance by a structured group, who
critical aspects of the structure to be learned (see Figure 1 , section were given rule instruction and exposure to relevant examples,
2 . 1 . 3 ). relative to performance by subjects instructed in rules with minimal
DeKeyser's second hypothesis was also modified by the exposure to examples, or to subjects receiving exposure alone. T o
additional claim that the simple rules must be categorical in nature the extent that superior learning by the instructed group in the
(rules that always apply when their structural description is met). In present study, and by Ellis' structured group, can be attributed t o
204 Summary and conclusions Summary and conclusions 205

task conditions which focus attention on relevant aspects of the Krashen' s (198 1 , 1 982, 1 985) claims. Instructed learners are
stimulus domain, and so facilitate noticing, they also support the significantly more accurate than incidental learners in performance
findings of other studies that have shown superior effects for on easy rules, and the analyses of chance and above chance
learning as a result of structured exposure via ' input-enhancement' performance on hard rule sentence types also reveals superior
(Sharwood Smith 1 99 1 ), e.g., the use of multiple-choice margin learning of the hard rule by the instructed condition, since they are
glosses for underlined words in Watanabe ( 1 992); the use o f above chance and accurate in response to seven of the ten types,
italicization i n Alanen ( 1 995); the use o f colour-coded highlighting compared to above chance and accurate performance on only five
in Doughty ( 1 99 1 ); the use of balded upper-case lettering ' in Shook hard rule sentence types by those in the incidental conditioIJ.
( 1 994); and the use of instructions to attend to form and meaning Finally, although the main analyses for the study reveal implicit and
in Hulstijn ( 1 990a). Likewise, Ellis found evidence for implicit incidental learners to be performing similarly, there is some
learning by the random exposure group in terms of above chance behavioural evidence of differences between the groups. Implicit
performance on a transfer task requiring grammaticality judgements learners, as noted above, perform accurately and above chance on
to novel examples of structures presented during training, but more hard rule sentence types than any other condition. The
concludes, "it is certainly laboriously slow and it cannot compare performance of implicit learners on easy and hard rules also
with the superior levels that can be encouraged by structured correlates significantly with measures of aptitude, as reported in
exposure to relevant exemplars" ( 1 993 :3 09). This also appears t o Chapter 6. In contrast, there is no significant correlation o f
b e true o f the evidence for implicit learning in the present study. performance by the incidental condition with aptitude o n either rule
(see Robinson l 995b ). Responses in the incidental condition also
(ii) Reber 's implicit and rule-search conditions. A comparison of appear sensitive to lexical and propositional information which
the findings for Reber's implicit and rule-search conditions also leads to significantly different responses to tokens of four of the
reveal similar findings to those reported by Reber ( 1 989, 1 993) in sentence types, whereas there are no significant differences in
studies of artificial grammar learning. Although the analyses for the response to tokens of the same type by subjects in other conditions.
main hypotheses revealed no significant differences between these
conditions in speed and accuracy of response on easy and hard rules, 9.2 Limitations of the study and directions for future research
the further analyses of chance and above chance performance on The findings of the present study must be interpreted in the light o f
sentence types reported in Chapter 7 and above (see Table 29 in its limitations and these are listed below.
section 8 .3 . 1 ) do suggest differences in line with Reber's claims. As
Figure 14 shows (see section 6. 1 . 1 ) implicit learners have a higher (i) The generalizability of the experimental learning conditions.
mean accuracy of response to hard rule sentences than rule-search Minor adjustments to instructional variables can cause potentially
learners, while rule-search learners have a higher mean accuracy of major changes in learning effects. As the review of implicit learning
response to easy rule sentences. As Table 29 shows, implicit studies in Chapter 4 showed (see Table 3 , section 4. 1 . 1 ), there is
learners perform better than any other condition on hard rule some inconsistency across the studies of artificial grammar learning
sentences, using measures of above chance performance to sentence in cognitive psychology, unrelated to manipulations of variables o f
types. The rule-search learners are worst, being accurate and above interest, which, given the small effect sizes typical in such studies
chance only on responses to three of the hard rule sentence types, (see Table 4, section 4. 1 . 1 ), can obscure the interpretation of
confirming Reber' s claim that conscious rule-search can be effective individual results and limit claims for generalizability made on the
where the stimulus domain is simple, but will be less effective than basis of groups of results from supposedly similar studies. The
implicit learning where it is complex. present study has motivated the research hypotheses with specific
reference to current work in cognitive psychology and examined
(iii) Krashen 's incidental and instructed conditions. A the generalizability of these claims to the field of SLA research.
comparison of the relative effectiveness of incidental and instructed This has the advantage of locating current SLA research in relation
conditions on easy and hard rules, however, does not support to issues explored in detail in another field, whose findings can
206 Summary and conclusions Summary and conclusions 207

thereby be more clearly related to SLA data and deepen its number of examples presented during trammg might also have
interpretation. Nonetheless, it could be argued that the implicit, affected performance under the explicit rule�based conditions. It is
incidental and rule-search conditions in the present study are less possible that with greater exposure, a transition in strategies for
ecologically valid, and so produce results which are less generalizable making grammaticality judgments, from rule-based to instance-based
to L2 pedagogy, than conditions operationalized in Alanen ( 1 995), memory retrieval, might occur in the explicit conditions. This has
Doughty ( 1 99 1 ) and Watanabe ( 1 992) which simulate learning been proposed by Logan ( 1 988a; Logan & Klapp 1 99 1 ; Logan &
conditions more familiar from contemporary L2 pedagogy. Given, Stadler 1 99 1 ) in relation to rule-based learning of alphabet
also, the differences in operationalizing learning conditions in those arithmetic problems, and has been examined in relation to L2 rule
studies, and the present study, further research is needed to confirm learning under explicit conditions by Robinson and Ha ( 1 993), who
the generalizability of the apparently similar findings regarding the found some evidence for the hypothesized strategy switch. This
relative effectiveness of implicit and explicit learning. transition may also be affected by the complexity of the stimulus
domain, with instruction on complex stimuli being relatively
(ii) The generalizability of the easy/hard rule distinction. The impervious to memory-based processing, in contrast to instruction
motivation for distinguishing rules and structures of different orders on simple stimuli, or vice versa.
of complexity in the studies of Hulstijn and DeGraaff ( 1 994), and
DeKeyser ( 1 994), reviewed above, also differs from the procedure (iv) The effects of positive and negative evidence. The training
adopted in the present study. To this extent the generalizability o f conditions for the present study involved the presentation o f
claims based o n the complexity differential are limited to positive examples o f grammatical sentences. Negative evidence o f
distinctions between pedagogic rules of different orders o f what i s not possible was not presented during training. I t i s possible,
complexity based on a consensus agreement among experienced however, that the significantly poorer performance of the
teachers about the relative difficulty of the structures and the rule instructed group on hard rule ungrammatical sentences, relative t o
explanations following the procedure described in section 2 .4 . performance o n hard rule grammatical sentences, would have been
Further confirmatory research regarding the differential improved if ungrammatical examples had been presented and judged
effectiveness of learning conditions is needed which adopts other during training. There was no significant interaction o f
motivations for distinguishing rule complexity, described in sections grammaticality and accuracy or speed o f performance o n either rule
1 . 1 . 1-4. Despite the problems of control, though, there are for subjects trained in other conditions, or on easy rules for those in
considerable advantages in terms of external validity if actual the instructed condition. The present study thus provides evidence
language samples are used as the dependent variables, as in the of the limitations of instruction in hard rules when this is
present study, and in Doughty ( 1 988, 1 99 1 ), Alanen ( 1 995), accompanied by positive evidence of what is possible alone. Further
Watanabe ( 1 992) and Ellis ( 1 993 ), rather than artificial or semi­ research is needed into the reasoning processes of subjects in
artificial examples as in DeGraaff ( 1 995), Hulstijn ( 1 990a) and instructed conditions. Little evidence for implicit learning was
DeKeyser ( 1 994, 1 995). shown in the present study, though the explanation given in section
8.3.2 suggests that such learning is possible from positive evidence,
(iii) Amount of exposure. The findings of the present study are but is likely to be most effective in the case of patterns of co­
based on limited exposure to examples during training. It is possible occurrence occurring within a small window of attention, and may
that clearer effects for memory-based learning in the implicit be limited to two or three features. Rules regulating structures of this
condition, and learning in the incidental condition, would be level of complexity and perceptual detail may be the most
obtained if more examples had been presented, particularly given susceptible to implicit learning.
Ellis' ( 1 993) characterization of implicit learning as 'slow' and
Krashen's ( 1 985, 1 994) characterization of acquisition as a process (v) Duration of learning effects. The duration of learning effects
taking place over longer periods of time than instructed learning, is also a question for further research. The present study measured
and requiring large amounts of comprehensible input. Increasing the only short-term learning apparent in a transfer task immediatly
Summary and conclusions 209
208 Summary and conclusions

following training. Consequently it is unclear whether the short­ implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions. Only subjects trained
term gains attributable to instruction on easy rules are also evident in the instructed condition are above chance and accurate in
over longer periods of time, or whether they are short-lived relative response to all easy rule sentence types.
!o !he smaller i!1�remental gains from learning under implicit and
mc1den�al conditions. Reber has claimed that implicit learning is 2. The Hard Rule. Instructed subjects are significantly m ore
robust m the face of both clinical disorders and aging processes (see accurate in their response to hard rule sentences than those trained
Abrams & Reber 1 98 8). Consequently its effects may be retained in the rule-search condition. There is no significant difference in
over longer intervals than is the case with explicit learning. accuracy of response to hard rule sentences for those trained in
instructed, implicit and incidental conditions.
(vi) Limitations of the transfer test of learning. As in Reber s
;
3. Gram maticality. The positive effect of instruction, apparent
�tudies, the presen� study used responses to a grammaticality from the superior accuracy of instructed subjects on easy rule
Judgment test as an mdex of learning. However, additional evidence
sentences and hard rule grammatical sentences relative to subjects
?f avail�bility . for productive use is necessary to supplement trained in other conditions, is not reflected in accuracy o f
mfor� ation gamed by such tests. The transfer test also required
consc10us attempts to make grammaticality judgements, and to this performance on hard rule ungrammatical sentences. Instructed
extent favours transfer of the processing strategies encouraged by subjects perform poorly on these types. Those trained in the
the rul.e� search and instructed task conditions. It is possible that implicit condition are above chance in accuracy of response t o
recogmt10n tests, such as those used in the implicit memory tests more hard rule ungrammatical sentence types than those trained in
described in section 4.2.3, would be more sensitive to the incidental, rule-search or instructed conditions.
information obtained during training under implicit and incidental
4. Awareness. There are no significant differences in measures of
conditions.
having noticed rules, or ability to verbalize rules for those trained in
(vii) The scope of the knowledge base involved in implicit and instructed, implicit, incidental and rule-search conditions.
explicit learning. The present study has dealt only with grammatical
Significantly greater numbers of instructed and rule-search subjects
knowledge presented and tested in the visual modality. Given the than implicit and incidental subjects report that they were looking
for rules during training.
�cope of.the knowledge and skill base involved in L2 development,
mformat10n about the relative effectiveness of implicit and explicit
5. Aptitude. Aptitude correlates significantly with accuracy of
learning of pragmatic, phonological and lexical aspects of L2
knowledge is needed, as is information about cross-modal transfer o f response for subjects trained in instructed, implicit and rule-search
information learned under different conditions. There has been little conditions. There are no significant correlations of aptitude with
research i ?to modality effects in artificial grammar learning, but accuracy of response to easy or hard rules for those trained in the
what studies there are (e.g., Whittlesea & Dorken 1 993) have incidental condition.
shown that where implicit learning occurs it is largely modality
specific. This is a particularly important issue for SLA research t o 6. Variance. There is significantly less variance in accuracy for
address. instructed subjects versus all other subjects, and significantly less
variance in reaction time for rule-search subjects versus all others.
9.3 Summary offindings
The main findings of the present study can be summarized as
follows:

1. The � asy Rule. Instructed subjects are significantly more


accurate m response to easy rule sentences than subjects trained in
Appe n dix A

GRAMMATICALITY JUDGMENT TEST


USED TO IDENTIFY UNFAMILIAR
STRUCTURES AS INPUT TO THE RATING
SCALE SURVEY OF RULE COMPLEXITY

Choose one answer for each sentence:


e.g., Mary is a teacher.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure

1 . It's Mary who eats cake.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

2 . John eats bread and Mary eats cake.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

3 . I placed book that on the table.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

4 . Who did you suggest I apply to?


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

5 . That there cow gives a lot of milk.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

6 . Who Mary is happy to see?


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

7 . What Mary does is eat cake.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure

8 . The cheese is in the bag and the bread is in the basket.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure
212 Appendix A Appendix A 213

9 . Who did John give the book to? 2 3 . Who did you suggest that should apply?
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 0 . To whom did John give the book? 24. I saw the dog barked.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 1 . Where the cheese is is in the bag not in the basket. 2 5 . Is what Mary does good?
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
12. What Mary does is good. 26. He jogs in the morning.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
13. Mary John likes very much. 27. I saw the dog that you fed.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
14. A copy of that book exists. 2 8 . Who did you suggest should apply?
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 5 . To John yesterday I gave the book that I read. 2 9 . This cow here gives a lot of milk.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 6 . In the morning he jogs. 3 0 . John hit him so I was angry.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 7. On Tuesday he jogs always. 3 1 . Who did you suggest that I apply to ?
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 8. I have never seen him. 3 2 . It' s cake Mary that eats.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
1 9 . Into the house ran John. 33. Never have I seen him.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
2 0 . I saw the dog that barked. 34. John ran into the house.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
21. From school Mary John took. 3 5 . That cow there gives a lot of milk.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
22. I saw the dog you fed. 3 6 . There exists a copy of that book.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure grammatical ungrammatical not sure
214 Appendix A

3 7 . I sent the book that I read yesterday to John.


grammatical ungrammatical not sure
Appendix B
3 8 . This here cow gives a lot of milk.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure
RATING SCALE SURVEY OF RULE
3 9 . John' s hitting him angered me. COMPLEXITY
grammatical ungrammatical not sure

4 0 . Into the house John ran. Could you please give the following information?
grammatical ungrammatical not sure Name. _________

Native language·------=-=------;----------
4 1 . John gave the book to Mary. No. of months/years ESL teaching experience. ______
_

grammatical ungrammatical not sure No of months/years experience teaching other languages. ___

No. of languages learned, and degree of proficiency in each.


42. John eats bread and Mary eats cake.
grammatical ungrammatical not sure -
- - -
Degreesand/or professional qualifications earned orcurrently
43 . I sent to John the book that I read yesterday. studying toward s.__________ __ __

grammatical ungrammatical not sure


_ __ _

Please look through the following eight rules of English. Rate t�em
in terms of their difficulty for a second language learner of Enghsh
using the following rating scales. A rating of -3 means very easy. A
rating of + 3 means very difficult.

Rule 1 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 2 -3 -2 -l 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 3 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 4 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 5 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 6 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 7 -3 -2 -1 0 +l +2 +3

Rule 8 -3 -2 -1 0 +1 +2 +3
216 Appendix B Appendix B 217

RULE 3
RULE 1
Some sentences describe an action and a response to that action.
Some sentences contain a subject a verb and a location phrase or a Here are some examples.
time phrase. Here are some examples:
John hit Eric. (action) Bill was angry. (response)
Joan (subject) crashed (verb) into the wall. (location phrase) Mary told a joke. (action) I was amused. (response)
John (subject) slept (verb) in the morning. (time phrase)
These sentences can be combined, as in the following examples:
Both location and time phrases can be moved to the front of the
sentence: John 's hitting Eric angered Bill.
Mary 's telling a joke amused me.
Into the wall Joan crashed.
On Tuesday morning John slept. To make sentences like these make the subject of the first (action)
sentence possessive and change the verb to the 'ing' form. The
The verb can also come before the subject, but only when location whole of the first (action) sentence then becomes the subject. For
phrases are at the front of the sentence: example:

Into the wall (location phrase) crashed (verb) Joan. (subject) John 's hitting Eric (subject) . . .
* On Tuesday morning (time phrase) slept (verb) John. (subject) Mary 's telling a joke. . .

The * means this sentence is ungrammatical. Delete the subject and verb of the second (response) sentence and
change the adjective into a verb (angry > angered) that carries
tense. The deleted subject of the second sentence is then changed to
the object.
RULE 2
. . . angered (verb) Bill. (object)
. . . . amused (verb) me. (object)
Some sentences consist of a subject, a verb and a time phrase. Here
are some examples.

He (subject) jogs (verb) in the morning. (time phrase)


John studies in the evening.

Time phrases can be moved to the front of the sentence:

In the morning (time phrase) he (subject) jogs. (verb)


In the evening John studies.
218 Appendix B Appendix B 219

RULE 4 RULE S

Some sentences tell about the locations of two things e.g., Some sentences describe transfer of possession from one person to
another. For example:
Alice stands on the right and Judy stands on the left.
John gave the book to Mary.
We can change this sentence to focus on one of the locations, e.g.,
It is possible to turn this sentence into a question if the person
Where Alice stands is on the right. receiving the object is unknown.

To make sentences like these, first, choose the subject you want to Who did John give the book to?
emphasize, 'Alice', then place 'where' in front of it:
To make sentences like these first choose a wh-word that agrees
Where Alice . . . with the unknown indirect object you are questioning. This goes at
the front of the sentence. For people the wh-word is always'who' :
Next, follow the subject with the verb, 'stands' :
John gave the book to Mary.
Where Alice (subject) stands (verb) . . . * What did John give the book to?

Note that the verb cannot come before the subject: The * means this sentence is ungrammatical.

* Where stands (verb) Alice (subject) ... Next add a form of the verb 'do' that takes the tense of the
sentence, and follow this with the subject and the main verb. Note
The * means this sentence is ungrammatical. that the main verb is in the infinitive form, it does not carry tense:

Next add a singular form of the verb 'be' which agrees in tense Who did John (subject) give (verb) . . .
followed by the phrase describing the location of the subject: * Who did John (subject) gave (verb) . . .

Where Alice stands is on the right. Place the object after the verb:
Where Alice stood was on the right.
Who did John (subject) give (verb) the book. (object)
If the verb does not agree in tense, the sentence is ungrammatical:
Finally add the preposition from the first sentence:
* Where Alice stands was on the right.
Who did John give the book to?
For special emphasis, the second location can be contrasted with the
focus location by joining the locations using 'not' . This 'not' is
required. Without it the sentence is ungrammatical.

Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.


* Where Alice stands is on the right the left.
220 Appendix B Appendix B 22 1

RULE 6 RULE 7

Some sentences contrast two activities. For example: Some sentences are used to indicate a specific subject.

John eats bread and Mary eats cake. That cow gives a lot of milk.

It is possible to focus on one of these by making sentences like this: We can use an adverb of location (there) to emphasize where the
subject is. This can be placed after the subject:
What Mary does is eat cake.
That cow (subject) there (adverb) gives a lot of milk.
To make sentences like these, first, choose the subject whose
activity you want to emphasize, 'Mary', then place 'what' in front It is possible to put the adverb of location before the subject as well,
of it. e.g.,

What Mary. . . That there (adverb) cow (subject) gives a lot of milk.

Next, follow the subject with a form o f the 'do' verb: To make sentences like these first identify the subject, then either
place the adverb between the noun and the verb,
What Alice (subject) does ('do' verb) . . .
That cow there gives a lot of milk.
Note that the verb cannot come before the subject:
or place it between the determiner 'that' and the noun.
* What does (verb) Mary (subject) . . .
That there cow gives a lot of milk.
The * means this sentence i s ungrammatical.

Next add a singular form of the verb 'be' which agrees in tense
followed by the phrase describing the activity:

What Mary does is ('be') eat cake.

If the verb 'be' does not agree in tense with the following verb the
sentence is ungrammatical

* What Mary does is ate cake.


222 Appendix B

RULE 8

Some sentences describe transfer of a thing from one person to Appendix C


another. For example:
COMPUTERIZED INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE
John gave the book to Mary.
IMPLICIT CONDITION FAMILIARIZATION
We can tum this sentence into a question if the person receiving PHASE OF TRAINING
the object is unknown.
Screen 1
To whom did John give the book?
You will now see some sentences in English. Try to
To make sentences like these first choose a wh-word that agrees remember them. At the end of this experiment you
with the unknown indirect object you are questioning. This goes at will be given a memory test to see if you can
the front of the sentence, after the preposition. For people the wh­ remember the sentences you saw. After you see each
word is always'whom' : sentence you will be asked a question. For example,
try to remember this sentence:
John gave the book to Mary.
* To what did John give the book? Joan crashed into the wall.

The * means this sentence is ungrammatical. Press any key to continue.

Next add a form of the verb 'do' that takes the tense of the
sentence, and follow this with the subject and the main verb. Note Screen 2
that the main verb is in the infinitive form (i.e., to give), it does
not carry tense: Were the words
To whom did John (subject) give (verb) . . . crashed wall
* To whom did John (subject) gave (verb) . . .
next to each other in the sentence?
Finally, place the object after the verb: If you think they were press 'c' for correct.
If you think they weren't press 'm' for mistake.
To whom did John (subject) give (verb) the book ? (object)

Screen 3

Correct/ Incorrect

Press any key to continue.


224 Appendix C Appendix C 225

Screen 4 Screen 7

The answer was 'm' for mistake. Correct/ Incorrect


The words

crashed wall Press any key to continue.

were not next to each other in the sentence;


Screen 8
Joan crashed into the wall.
The answer was 'c' for correct.
The words
Press any key to continue.
Alice stands

Screen 5 are next to each other in the sentence;

Now practice again. Remember this sentence: Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.

Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.


Press any key to continue.

Screen 6 Screen 9

Were the words You will now see some sentences. You must try t o
remember them. After each sentence you must answer
Alice stands a question about the words in the sentence. You press
'c' for correct if the answer is yes, and 'm' for
next to each other in the sentence? mistake if the answer is no.

Remember, press 'c' for correct if they were. Are you ready?
Press 'm' for mistake if they weren't.

Press any key to continue.


Appendix D

COMPUTERIZED INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE


INCIDENTAL CONDITION FAMILIARIZATION
PHASE OF TRAINING

Screen 1

You will now see some sentences in English. Try t o


understand them. At the end o f this experiment you
will be asked to answer comprehension questions o n
some of the sentences you saw. After you see each
sentence you will be asked a question. For example,
read this sentence:

Joan crashed into the wall.

Press any key to continue.

Screen 2

Did Joan crash into a car?

If the answer is 'yes', press 'c' for correct.


If the answer is 'no', press 'm' for mistake.

Screen 3

Correct/ Incorrect

Press any key to continue.


228 Appendix D Appendix D 229

Screen 4 Screen 8

The answer was 'm' for mistake. The answer was 'c' for correct.
Joan crashed into the wall. Where Alice stands is on the right.
She did not crash into a car. She does not stand on the left.

Press any key to continue. Press any key to continue.

Screen 5 Screen 9

Now practice again. Read this sentence: You will now see some more questions. Read them and
try to understand them. After each sentence you will
Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left. be asked a question to test your understanding.You
press 'c' for correct if the answer is yes, and 'm' for
mistake if the answer is no.
Screen 6 Are you ready?
Does Alice stand on the right?
Press any key to continue.
Remember, press 'c' for correct if answer is yes.
Press 'm' for mistake if the answer is no.

Screen 7

Correct/ Incorrect

Press any key to continue.


Appendix E

COMPUTERIZED INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE


RULE-SEARCH CONDITION FAMILIARIZATION
PHASE OF TRAINING

Screen 1

You will now see some sentences in English. They are


examples that illustrate two rules of English grammar.
As you see the sentences try to find the rules. At the
end of this experiment you will be asked to say what
the rules are in your own words.

Press any key to continue.

Screen 2

After you see each sentence you will


be asked a question. For example:

Have you found the rules yet?

If the answer is yes, you must press 'c' for


correct. If the answer is no, you must press 'm'
for mistake

Press any key to continue.

Screen 3

For example, study this sentence:

Joan crashed into the wall.


232 Appendix E

Screen 4 Appendix F
Does this sentence illustrate a rule that you
know? COMPUTERIZED INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE
INSTRUCTED CONDITION FAMILIARIZATION
Pres � '�' for c?rrect, or if the answer is yes, PHASE OF TRAINING
and m for mistake, if the answer is no.
L I
Screen 1
Screen 5
Here are two sentences based on the rules you studied.
Now study this sentence:
Into the wall crashed Joan.
Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left. RULE 1

Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.


Screen 6 RULE 2

Did this sentence contain a rule that you know? You will now see some questions about these
sentences. Study the questions and make sure you
�e!11.ember, answer 'c' if the answer is yes, and understand the answers. You may look at the rules
if
m if the answer is no. you want to.

If the answer to the question is yes, press 'c' for


correct . If the answer is no, press 'm' for mistake.
Screen 7

You will now see some more sentences. You must try
Press any key to continue.
to find the rules that these sentences illustrate. After
;a�h sentence yo_u must answer a question. You press
c for �orrect if the answer is yes, and 'm' for
.
mIStake if the answer is no.

Are you ready?

Press any key to continue.


234 Appendix F
Appendix F 235

Screen 2
Screen 5
RULE 1
Correct/ Incorrect
Into the wall crashed Joan.
Press any key to continue.
Study these questions and understand the answers.

1 . Was a location phrase used in the sentence?


(answer 'c', yes) Screen 6
2 . Did the subject of the sentence come after the
verb? (answer 'c', yes) The answer was 'c' for correct.
3 . Was the phrase of time or location at the front of In the sentence:
the sentence? (answer 'c', yes)
4. Was a time phrase used in the sentence? Joan crashed into the wall.
(answer 'm', no)
'into the wall' is a location phrase.
Press any key to continue.

Press any key to continue.


Screen 3

Now study this sentence: Screen 7

RULE 2 .
Joan crashed into the wall.
Where Alice stands is on the right not on the left.

Study these questions and understand the answers.


Screen 4
I. Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not'
used? (answer 'c', yes)
Was a location phrase used in the sentence?
2. Did the subject of the sentence come after the
verb? (answer 'm', no)
3 . Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form?
Answer 'c' for correct or yes.
(answer 'c', yes)
Answer 'm' for mistake or no.
4. Did the verbs used agree in tense? (answer 'c', yes)

Press any key to continue.


236 Appendix F Appendix F 23 7

Screen 8 Screen 12

Study this sentence: You will now see some more sentences that are
formed by the rules you have studied. Try t o
remember the rules when you see the sentences.
Where Alice and Judy stand is on the left. Remember, when you answer the questions you must
press 'c' for correct if the answer is yes, and 'm' for
mistake, if the answer is no.

Are you ready?


Screen 9

Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form? Press any key to continue.

Press 'c' for correct if the answer is yes.


Press 'm' for mistake if the answer is no.

Screen 1 0

Correct/ Incorrect

Press any key to continue.

Screen 1 1

The answer was 'c' for correct.


In the sentence:

Where Alice and Judy stand is on the left.

the verb 'to be' is in the singular form 'is'.

Press any key to continue.


Appendix G
THE TRAINING SET SENTENCES

Sentences presented during the training sessions


Five easy rule and five hard rule grammatical sentence types were
presented, and exemplified by four tokens each during training,
making a total of forty tokens presented during each trial of
training. These correspond to the sentence types used to illustrate
the easy and hard rules in the rating scale survey, and in the rule
formats presented to subjects in the instructed condition. For hard
sentence types the optional contrasting location phrase introduced
by 'not' was present in two tokens of each of the four types t o
which it is possible to attach it, and absent from the other two
tokens.

Easy rule sentences

easy grammatical type 1


i) Paul drove across the city.
ii) Mary sank under the sea.
iii) Jill walked over the bridge.
iv)John ran into the house.

easy grammatical type 2


i) Up the road came Eric.
ii) Off the horse fell Amy.
iii)Down the hill slid Jack.
iv) Onto the boat jumped Sue.

easy grammatical type 3


i) Round the track Bill raced.
ii) Through the forest Jane went.
iii)Past the river Helen rode.
iv )Out of the shop Jim rushed.

easy grammatical type 4


i) Lori ate at five'o clock.
ii) Ron left on Thursday night.
240 Appendix G

! ii)Roland returned at midnight.


1v)Peggy slept on Tuesday morning.
Appendix H
�asy grammatical type 5
1) In the afternoon Elaine arrived.
ii) At the weekend Randy exercised. TRAINING SET SENTENCES AND FOLLOW-UP
!ii) On Christmas day Lily phoned. QUESTIONS VIEWED IN EACH CONDITION
1v)In the morning Claire jogged.
Tokens of each easy and hard rule sentence type are followed by the
Hard rule sentences questions which followed them in each condition. (Imp = implicit;
Inst = instructed; Rule-s = rule-search; Inc = incidental condition).
hard grammatical type 1
!! The girl pl_ay� in the park and the boy plays in the yard. easy grammatical · type 1
��! The letter 1s m the bag and the book is on the chair.
� n)John works in the shop and Mary works in the hospital . i) Paul drove across the city.
1v) The milk is in the glass and the coffee is in the jar. Imp Were the words Paul across next to each other in the
sentence?
hard grammatical type 2 Inst Was a time phrase used in the sentence?
i) Where Helen is is in New York. Rule-s Did you notice any rules?
!!! Where the phone is is in the bedroom. Inc Was Paul travelling by sea?
� n) Where Sue is is in the car not in the boat.
iv) Where the cheese is is on the plate not in the basket. ii) Mary sank under the sea.
Imp Were the words sank sea next to each other in the
hard grammatical type 3 sentence?
i) Where the children are is on the beach . Inst Was a location phrase used in the sentence?
ii) Where Eric and Jill are is at home. Rule-s Does this sentence contain a rule that you know?
!ii) Where Jim and Amy a:e is in the kitchen not in the garden. Inc Was Mary at home?
1v) Where the people are is in the park not in the church.
iii) Jill walked over the bridge.
hard grammatical type 4 Imp Were the words walked over next to each other in the
i) Where the cat sleeps is on the floor. sentence?
ii) Where Bill eats is at the table. Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb?
!ii) Where the President lives is in Washington not in Boston. Rule-s Did you notice the rule?
iv) Where Jane sits is on the sofa not on the stool. Inc Was Jill taking a walk?
hard grammatical type 5 iv) John ran into the house .
i) Where the birds nest is in the tree. Imp Were the words the mouse next to each other in the
!!! Where Paul and Jack surf is at Waikiki. sentence?
� n) Where Steve and Mark swim is in the pool not in the ocean. Inst Was the phrase of time or location at the front of the
Iv) Where the students meet is in the library not in the sentence?
restaurant. Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know?
Inc Was John driving a car?
Appendix H 243
242 Appendix H

easy grammatical type 2 Imp Were the words through Jane next to each other in the
sentence?
i) Up the road came Eric. Inst Was a location phrase used in the sentence?
Imp Were the words road came next to each other in the Rule-s Have you found the rules yet?
sentence? Inc Was Jane watching a movie?
Inst Was a time phrase used in the sentence?
Rule-s Have you found any rules yet? iii) Past the river Helen rode.
Inc Was Eric travelling by plane? Imp Were the words Helen rode next to each other in the
sentence?
ii) Off the horse fell Amy. Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb?
Imp Were the words horse Amy next to each other in the Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know?
sentence? Inc Did Helen fall into the river?
Inst Was a location phrase used in the sentence?
Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know? iv) Out of the shop Jim rushed.
Inc Was Amy riding a camel? Imp Were the words shop rushed next to each other in the
sentence?
iii) Down the hill slid Jack. Inst Was the phrase of time or location at the front of the
Imp Were the words Down the next to each other in the sentence?
sentence? Rule-s Are you still looking for the rules?
Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb? Inc Was Jim in a hurry?
Rule-s Are you still looking for the rules?
easy grammatical type 4
Inc Was Jack in the classroom?

iv) Onto the boat jumped Sue. i) Lori ate at five' o clock.
Imp Were the words onto boat next to each other in the Imp Were the words Lori at next to each other in the
sentence? sentence ?
Inst Was the phrase of time or location at the front of the Inst Was a time phrase used in the sentence?
sentence? Rule-s Did you notice any rules?
Rule-s Does this sentence contain a rule that you know? Inc Was Lori eating her lunch?
Inc Did Sue get onto the boat?
ii) Ron left on Thursday night. .
easy grammatical type 3 Imp Were the words left night next to each other m the
sentence?
i) Round the track Bill raced. Inst Ws a location phrase used in the sentence?
Imp Were the words track raced next to each other in the Rule-s Have you found the rules yet?
sentence? Inc Did Ron stay until Friday?
Inst Was a time phrase used in the sentence?
Rule-s Did you notice any rules? iii) Roland returned at midnight.
Inc Was Bill studying at home? Imp Were the words returned at next to each other in the
sentence?
ii) Through the forest Jane went. Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb?
244 Appendix H Appendix H 245

Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know? hard grammatical type 1
Inc Was Roland asleep by midnight?
i) The girl plays in the park and the boy plays in the yard.
iv) Peggy slept on Tuesday morning. Imp Were the words plays the next to each other in the
Imp Were the words Peggy morn ing next to each other in sentence?
the sentence? Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb?
Inst Was the phrase of time or location at the front of the Rule-s Did you notice any rules?
sentence? Inc Were the boy and girl playing together?
Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this?
Inc Was Peggy tired? ii) The letter is in the bag and the book is on the chair.
Imp Were the words book on next to each other in the
easy grammatical type 5
sentence?
Inst Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form?
i) In the afternoon Elaine arrived. Does this sentence contain a rule that you know?
Rule-s
Imp Were the words in afternoon next to each other in the Is the letter on the chair?
Inc
sentence?
Inst Was a time phrase used in the sentence? iii) John works in the shop and Mary works in the hospital.
Rule-s Have you found the rules yet? Imp Were the words Mary works next to each other in the
Inc Did Elaine arrive at night? sentence?
Inst Did the verbs used agree in tense?
ii) At the weekend Randy exercised. Rule-s Did you notice the rule?
Imp Were the words the Randy next to each other in the Inc Is John a doctor?
sentence?
Inst Was a location phrase used in the sentence? iv) The milk is in the glass and the coffee is in the jar.
Rule-s Does this sentence contain a rule that you know? Imp Were the words coffee cup next to each other in the
Inc Did Randy exercise every day? sentence?
Inst Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not' used ?
iii) On Christmas day Lily phoned. Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know?
Imp Were the words Lily phoned next to each other in the Is there any coffee left?
Inc
sentence?
Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb? hard grammatical type 2
Rule-s Have you found the rules yet?
Inc Did Lily forget to call on Christmas day? i) Where Helen is is in New York.
Imp Were the words Helen is next to each other in the
iv) In the morning Claire jogged. sentence?
Imp Were the wordson the next to each other in the sentence? Inst Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not' used?
Inst Was the phrase of time or location at the front of the Rule-s Have you found any rules yet?
sentence? Inc Is Helen in Honolulu?
Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this?
Inc Did Claire exercise in the morning? ii) Where the phone is is in the bedroom.
Imp Were the words where phone next to each other in the
246 Appendix H Appendix H 24 7

sentence? iv) Where the people are is in the park not in the c.hurch.
Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb? Imp Were the words are in next to each other m the sentence?
Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know? Inst Did the verbs used agree in tense?
Inc Is there a phone in the bedroom? Rule..:s Are you still looking for the rules?
Inc Are there any people in the park?
iii) Where Sue is is in the car not in the boat.
Imp Were the words is the next to each other in the sentence? hard grammatical type 4
Inst Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form?
Rule-s Are you still looking for the rules? i) Where the cat sleeps is on the floor.
Inc Is Sue in the boat? Imp Were the words sleeps on next to each other in the
sentence?
iv) Where the cheese is is on the plate not in the basket. Inst Did the verbs used agree in tense?
Imp Were the words plate basket next to each other in the Rule-s Did you notice any rules?
sentence? Inc Does the cat sleep on the floor?
Inst Did the verbs used agree in tense?
Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this? ii) Where Bill eats is at the table.
Inc Is the cheese in the basket? Imp Were the words Bill is next to each other in the sentence?
Inst Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form?
hard grammatical type 3 Rule-s Have you found the rules yet?
Inc Does Bill eat off the floor?
i) Where the children are is on the beach.
Imp Were the words are on next to each other in the iii) Where the President lives is in Washington not in Boston.
sentence? Imp Were the words not Boston next to each other in the
Inst Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form ? sentence?
Rule-s Did you notice any rules? Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb?
Inc Are the children in bed? Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know?
Inc Does the President live in New York?
ii) Where Eric and Jill are is at home.
Imp Were the words at home next to each other in the iv) Where Jane sits is on the sofa not on the stool. .
sentence? Imp Were the words Jane sits next to each other m the
Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb? sentence?
Rule-s Have you found the rules yet? Inst Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not' used?
Inc Is Eric alone in the house? Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this?
Inc Does Jane sit on the sofa?
iii) Where Jim and Amy are is in the kitchen not in the garden.
Imp Were the words garden not next to each other in the hard grammatical type 5
sentence?
Inst Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not' used? i) Where the birds nest is in the tree. .
Rule-s Did this sentence contain a rule that you know? Imp Were the words birds nest next to each other m the
Inc Are Jim and Amy swimming? sentence?
Inst Was a contrasting location introduced by 'not' used?
248 Appendix H

Rule-s Have you found the rules yet?


Inc Do the birds nest in the garage?
Appendix I
ii) Where Paul and Jack surf is at Waikiki.
Imp Were the words Paul surf next to each other in the
COMPUTERIZED INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE
sentence?
Inst Did the subject of the sentence come after the verb? FAMILIARIZATION PHASE OF THE TRANSFER
Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this? TASK
Inc Do Paul and Jack surf at Ala Moana?
Screen 1
iii) Where Steve and Mark swim is in the pool not in the ocean.
Imp Were the words where Mark next to each other in the You will now see some more sentences. You must
sentence? decide if they are grammatical or ungrammatical. I f
Inst Was the verb 'to be' in the singular form? you think the sentence is grammati�al press 'c' for
Rule-s Have you found the rules yet? _
correct. If you think the sentence is ungrammatical
Inc Does Steve swim in the pool? press 'm' for mistake.

iv) Where the students meet is in the library not in the restaurant. Press any key to continue.
Imp Were the words meet library next to each other in the
sentence?
Inst Did the verbs used agree in tense? Screen 2
Rule-s Do you know the rules for sentences like this?
Inc Do the students meet in the restaurant?
For example, read the sentence bel�w.
Press 'c' if you think it is grammatical:
Press 'm' if you think it is ungrammatical.
Do this as quickly as you can.

The tall woman ate the pizza.

Screen 3

The answer was 'c' for correct.

Now try to work as quickly as you can.


When you see the sentences press 'c' if theJ'. are
grammatical and 'm' if they are ungrammatical.

Are you ready?

Press any key to continue.


Appendix J

TRANSFER SET SENTENCES

Sentences presented during the transfer sessions


The forty transfer set sentences included two new tokens of the
easy and hard grammatical types which were presented during the
training session, plus five tokens of easy ungrammatical types 1 and
2 and two tokens of hard ungrammatical types 1-5 . The
ungrammatical sentence types were violations of the conditions on
the rules presented during the rating scale survey, and presented to
learners during the instructed condition.

easy grammatical type 1


i) Craig walked into the room.
ii) Lisa fell onto the grass.
easy grammatical type 2
i) Across the street raced Tom.
ii) Out of the door ran Kate.
easy grammatical type 3
i) Through the window Maria stared.
ii) Over the town Lucy flew.
easy grammatical type 4
i) Peter woke at eight' o clock.
ii) Wendy started on Monday morning.
easy grammatical type 5
i) On her birthday Gabi sang.
ii) In the evening Dick studied.
easy ungrammatical type 1
i) On Saturday night danced Charley.
ii) At nine thirty left Robert.
iii) In the afternoon spoke Ian.
iv) On Sunday painted Graham.
v) At night ate Judy.
easy ungrammatical type 2
i) Golfed Debbie in the morning.
ii) Died Stan on Friday night.
iii) Departed Fred at nine thirty.
iv) Crawled Mick through the tunnel.
252 Appendix J

v) Slipped Maggy off the roof.


hard grammatical type 1
i) The pen is in the box and the pencil is in the drawer.
ii) Lisa reads in the bedroom and Mick reads in the study. Appendix K
hard grammatical type 2
i) Where L.A is is in California. SUBJECT INFORMATION
ii) Where the car is is in the driveway not in the road.
hard grammatical type 3
i) Where the apples are is in the bowl. Table 95. Details of Subj ects For The Present Study
ii) Where Ann and Ian are is at the movies not at the zoo.
hard gramm atical type 4
Subject Native Native Gender Age Condition
i) Where Judy teaches is at the univers ity.
No. Country Language
ii) Where Peter waits is by the stairs not in the cafe.
hard grammatical type 5
i) Where my parents vacation is in Europe. 1 Japan Japanese Female 23 Implicit
ii) Where Maria and Joe relax is by the pool not on the beach. 2 Japan Japanese Female 25 Incidental
hard ungram matical type 1 3 Japan Japanese Female 22 Implicit
i) Where is Bill is in the country not in the city. 4 Japan Japanese Female 34 Incidental
ii) Where cooks Jim is in the kitchen not in the bathroom. 5 Japan Japanese Female 20 Implicit
hard ungram matical type 2 6 Japan Japanese Female 26 Instructed
i) Where Carl writes are at a desk not on the floor. 7 Japan Japanese Female 31 Rule-search
ii) Where the soldiers fight are in Europe not in Hawai'i . 8 Japan Japanese Female 26 Rule-search
hard ungram matical type 3 9 China Mandarin Female 29 Incidental
i) Where the bird is was in the sky not in the sea. 10 Japan Japanese Female 21 Implicit
ii) Where the plane lands was in the airport not at the dock. 11 Japan Japanese Female 28 Instructed
hard ungram matical type 4 12 Japan Japanese Female 23 Instructed
i) What the King lives is in a palace not in an apartment. 13 Japan Japanese Female 31 Instructed
ii) What the dog is is in the yard not in the house. 14 Korea Korean Male 23 Instructed
hard ungram matical type 5 15 China Mandarin Male 22 Incidental
i) Where the horse stands is in the field in the barn. 16 Japan Japanese Female 19 Implicit
ii) Where Mark works is in America in Japan. 17 Korea Korean Female 21 Incidental
18 India Urdu Female 24 Rule-search
19 Japan Japanese Female 21 Implicit
20 Korea Korean Female 22 Instructed
21 Japan Japanese Female 21 Instructed
22 Japan Japanese Male 23 Implicit
23 Japan Japanese Female 22 Rule-search
24 Japan Japanese Female 22 Incidental
25 Japan Japanese Female 23 Instructed
26 Japan Japanese Female 22 Rule-search
27 Japan Japanese Female 23 Rule-search
28 Japan Japanese Female 24 Incidental
29 Japan Japanese Female 23 Rule-search
254 Appendix K Appendix K 255

Table 95. (Continued) Details of Subj ects For The Present Table 95. (Continued) D etails of Subjects For The Present
Study Study

Subject Native Native Gender Age Condition Subject Native Native Gender Age Condition
No. Country Language No. Country Language
30 Japan Japanese Male 23 Implicit 64 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
31 Japan Japanese Female 25 Rule-search 65 Japan Japanese Female 19 Implicit
32 Japan Japanese Female 24 Incidental 66 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
33 China Mandarin Female 29 Instructed 67 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
34 China Mandarin Female 22 Implicit 68 Japan Japanese Male 19 Instructed
35 Japan Japanese Female 23 Incidental 69 Japan Japanese Male 19 Incidental
36 Japan Japanese Female 24 Instructed 70 Japan Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
37 Japan Japanese Male 24 Instructed 71 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
38 Japan Japanese Male 24 Incidental 72 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
39 Japan Japanese Female 25 Implicit 73 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
40 Japan Japanese Male 26 Rule-search 74 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
41 Japan Japanese Male 26 Rule-search 75 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
42 Japan Japanese Male 25 Implicit 76 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
43 Japan Japanese Female 25 Incidental 77 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
44 Japan Japanese Female 24 Incidental 78 Japan Japanese Female 20 Implicit
45 Japan Japanese Female 24 Incidental 79 Japan Japanese Female 19 Implicit
46 Japan Japanese Female 23 Instructed 80 Japan Japanese Female 19 Instructed
47 Japan Japanese Female 25 Instructed 81 Japan Japanese Male 19 Instructed
48 Korea Korean Male 26 Implicit 82 Japan Japanese Male 19 Instructed
49 China Mandarin Female 27 Implicit 83 Japan Japanese Male 19 Instructed
50 Japan Japanese Male 25 Implicit 84 Japan Japanese Male 19 Instructed
51 Japan Japanese Male 24 Incidental 85 Japan Japanese Female 20 Implicit
52 Japan Japanese Female 22 Incidental 86 Japan Japanese Male 19 Implicit
53 Japan Japanese Female 25 Instructed 87 Japan Japanese Male 19 Implicit
54 Japan Japanese Female 30 Implicit 88 Japan Japanese Male 19 Implicit
55 Japan Japanese Female 27 Implicit 89 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
56 Korea Korean Female 24 Incidental 90 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
57 Japan Japanese Male 29 Incidental 91 Japan Japanese Female 19 Incidental
58 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search 92 Japan Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
59 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search 93 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search
60 Japan Japanese Female 19 Rule-search 94 Japan Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
61 Japan Japanese Female 19 Instructed 95 Japan Japanese Male 20 Rule-search
62 Japan Japanese Female 19 Implicit 96 Japan Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
63 Japan Japanese Female 19 Implicit 97 Japan Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
256 Appendix K

Table 95. (Continued) Details of Subj ects For The Present


Study Bibliography

Subject Native Native Gender Age Abrams, M. ( 1 987). Implicit learning in the psychiatrically
Condition
No. Country Language impaired. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University
of New York.
98 Japan Abrams, M., & Reber, A. ( 1 98 8). Implicit learning: Robustness in
Japanese Male 19 Rule-search
99 Japan the face of psychiatric disorders. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Japanese Female 19 Instructed
1 00 Japan Research, 1 7, 425-43 9.
Japanese Female 19 Instructed
101 Japan Ackerman, P.L. ( 1 988). Determinants of individual differences
Japanese Female 19 Instructed
1 02 Japan during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information
Japanese Male 19 Instructed
1 03 China processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
Mandarin Female 36 Implicit 11 7, 2 8 8-3 1 8 .
1 04 Korea Korean Female 26 Instructed Adams, F., Aizawa, K., & Fuller, G. ( 1 992). Rules in programming
languages and networks. In J. Dinsmore (Ed.), The symbolic
and connectionist paradigms: Closing the gap, pp.49-6 7 .
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Alanen, R. ( 1 995). Input enhancement and rule presentation in
second language acquisition. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), A ttention
and awareness in foreign language teaching and learning,
pp. 5 3 - 8 1 . University of Hawai' i at Manoa: Second Language
Teaching and Curriculum Center Technical Report # 9.
Alkon, D. ( 1 988). Memory traces in the brain. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Allwright, R.L. ( 1 984). Why don't learners learn what teachers
teach? In D. Singleton & D. Little (Eds.), Language learning
in formal and informal contexts, pp. 1-1 8. Dublin: IRAAL.
Anderson, J.R. ( 1 983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Ashby, F.G. ( 1 992). Multidimensional models of categorization. In
F.G. Ashby (Ed.), Multidimensional models of perception and
categorization, pp.449-485. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbauni.
Asher, J.J. ( 1 979). Motivating children and adults to acquire a
second language. SPEAQ Journal, 3, 87-99.
Atkinson, M. ( 1 992). Children 's syntax: An introduction to
principles and parameters theory. Cambridge, MA: Basil
Blackwell.
Atkinson, R., & Shiffrin, R. ( 1 968). Human memory: A proposed
system and its control processes. In K. Spence (Ed.), The
psychology of learning and motivation, Volume. 2, pp . 8 9-
1 95. New York: Academic Press.
258 Bibliography Bibliography 259

Baars, B.J. ( 1 988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge, Blaxton, T.A. ( 1 989). Investigating dissociations among memory
MA: Cambridge University Press. measures: Support for a transfer appropriate processing
Baddeley, A. ( 1 986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Press. Memory and Cognition, 15, 6 5 7-68 8 .
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. ( 1 974). Working memory. In G. Bower Bley-Vroman, R . ( 1 986). Hypothesis testing in second language
(Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation, pp.47-90. acquisition theory. Language Learning, 36, 3 5 3-3 76 .
New York: Academic Press. . ( 1 989). What is the logical problem of foreign language
__ . ( 1 993 ). The recency effect: Implicit learning with explicit -- learning? In S. Gass & J. Schacter (Eds.), Linguistic
retrieval? Memory & Cognition, 21, 1 46- 1 5 5 . perspectives on second language acquisition, pp.4 1 -6 8 . New
Baker, C.L. ( 1 989). English syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. York: Cambridge University Press.
Balota, D.A. ( 1 983). Automatic semantic activation and episodic Bley-Vroman, R., Felix, S., & Ioup, G. ( 1 988). The accessibility of
memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Universal Grammar in adult language learning. Second
Behavior, 22, 88- 1 04. Language Research, 4, 1-3 2 .
Bardovi-Harlig, K. ( 1 987). Markedness and salience in second Bowers, K.S., & Meichenbaum, D . (Eds.) ( 1 984). The unconscious
language acquisition. Language Learning, 3 7, 3 85-407. reconsidered. New York: John Wiley.
Beretta, A. ( 1 989). Attention to form or meaning: Error treatment Braine, M.D.S. ( 1 965). The insufficiency of a finite-state model for
in the Bangalore Project. TESOL Quarterly, 23, 2 83-3 03 . verbal reconstructive memory. Psychonomic Science, 2, 2 9 1 -
Besson, M., Boaz, T., Fischler, I. & Raney, G. ( 1 992). Effects o f 292 .
automatic associative activation on explicit and implicit Braine, M.D.S., Brody, R.E., Brooks, P., Sudhalter, V., Ross, J.A.,
memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Catalano, L., & Fisch, S .M. ( 1 990). Exploring language
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 89-1 0 5 . acquisition in children with a miniature artificial language:
Best, J.B. ( 1 992). Cognitive psychology. (3rd edition) New York: Effects of item and pattern frequency, arbitrary subclasses and
West Publishing. correction. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 5 9 1 -6 1 0.
Bialystok, E. ( 1 978). A theoretical model of second language Bresnan, J., & Kanerva, J.M. ( 1 989). Locative inversion in
learning. Language Learning, 28, 69-84. Chichewa: A comparative study of factorization in grammar.
. ( 1 979). Explicit and implicit judgments of L2 Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 1 -5 0 .
-- grammaticality. Language Learning, 29, 8 1 - 1 03 . Brewer, W.F. ( 1 974). There i s no convincing evidence for operant
__ . ( 1 982). On the relationship between knowing and using or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W.B. Weimer &
linguistic forms. Applied Linguistics, 3, 25 5-262 . D.S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes,
-- . ( 1 988). Psycholinguistic dimensions of second language pp. 1-42. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
proficiency. In W. Rutherford & W. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Broadbent, D. ( 1 954). A mathematical model for human attention
Grammar and second language teaching, pp. 1 9-4 1 . Rowley, and immediate memory. Psychological Review, 64, 205 .
MA: Newbury House. . ( 1 95 8). Perception and communication New York:
. ( 1 990). The dangers of dichotomy: A reply to Hulstijn. Pergamon.
-- Applied Linguistics, 1 1, 46-5 1 . Broadbent, D., Fitzgerald, P., & Broadbent, M.H.P. ( 1 986). Implicit
Bialystok, E., & Sharwood Smith, M. ( 1 985). Interlanguage is not a and explicit knowledge in the control of complex systems.
state of mind. Applied Linguistics, 6, 1 0 1 -1 1 7 . British Journal of Psychology, 77, 33-5 0 .
Bickerton, D. ( 1 99 1 ). Consciousness I and consciousness II. Brody, N . ( 1 989). Unconscious learning o f rules: comment o n
University of Oregon Lectures on the Philosophy of Mind. Reber' s analysis of implicit learning. Journal of Experimental
Unpublished manuscript, Department of Linguistics, Psychology: General, 118, 236-23 8 .
University of Hawai'i at Manoa. ·Bunge, M . ( 1 973). Model, method, matter. Dordrecht: Reidel.
260 Bibliography Bibliography 261

Carlson, R.A., Khoo, B.H., Yaure, R.G., & Schneider, W. ( 1 990). of human intelligence, Volume 1, pp.7-76 . Hillsdale, NJ:
Acquisition of a problem solving skill: Levels of organization Lawrence Erlbaum.
and use of working memory. Journal of Experimental Chomsky, N. ( 1 959). Review of Skinner's 'Verbal Behavior. '
Psychology: General, 1 1 9, 1 93-2 1 4. Language, 35, 26-5 8 .
Carr, T.H., & Curran, T. ( 1 994). Cognitive factors in learning about __ . ( 1 965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA:
structured sequences: Applications to syntax. Studies in MIT Press.
Second Language Acquisition, 1 6, 205-22 5 . __ . ( 1 9 8 1 ). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht:
Carrell, P. ( 1 984). Schema theory and ESL reading pedagogy. Foris.
TESOL Quarterly, 18, 1 52- 1 69 . __ . ( 1 986). Knowledge of language: Its nature origin and use.
Carroll, J.B . ( 1 973). Implications of aptitude test research and New York: Praeger.
psycholinguistic theory for foreign language teaching. __ . ( 1 990). Accessibility "in principle." Behavioral and Brain
International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 2, 5- 1 4 . Sciences, 13, 600-60 1 .
__ . ( 1 98 1 ). Twenty five years o f research o n foreign language Cohen, A., Larsen-Freeman, D., & Tarone, E. ( 1 9 9 1 ). The
aptitude. In K. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and contribution of SLA theories and research to teaching
universals in language learning aptitude, pp.83-1 1 8 . Rowley, language. In E. Sadtono (Ed.), Language acquisition and the
MA: Newbury House. second/foreign language classroom. pp. 1 3 6-1 56 . Singapore:
__ . ( 1 990). Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: RELC.
Then and now. In C. Stansfield (Eds.), Language aptitude Cohen, M.M., & Massaro, D. ( 1 992). On the similarity of
reconsidered, pp. 1 1-30 . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. categorization models. In F.G. Ashby (Ed.), Multidimensional
__ . ( 1 993). Cognitive abilities: A factor analytic approach to models of perception and categorization, pp.3 95-449.
research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Carroll, J.B., & Sapon, S.M. ( 1 959). The modern language aptitude Cohen, N.J. ( 1 984). Preserved learning capacity in amnesia:
test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Evidence for multiple systems. In L. Squire & N. Butters
Ceci, S.J., & Liker, J.K. ( 1 986). A day at the races: A study of IQ, (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory, pp. 83- 1 03 . New York :
expertise and cognitive complexity. Journal of Experimental Guildford Press.
Psychology: General, 1 15, 25 5-266. Cook, V. ( 1 977). Cognitive processes in second language learning.
Celce-Murcia, M., & Larsen-Freeman, D. ( 1 983). The grammar International Review of Applied Linguistics, 15, 1 -20.
book. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. __ . ( 1 979). Aspects of memory in secondary school language
Chandler, S. ( 1 993). Are rules and modules really necessary for learners. Interlanguage Studies Bulletin, 4, 1 6 1- 1 72.
explaining language? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, __ . ( 1 986). Do second language learners have a cognitive
22, 5 93-606. deficit?. In V. Cook (Ed.), Experimental approaches to
Chapelle, C., & Green, P. ( 1 992). Field dependence/independence in second language acquisition, pp. 73-79. Oxford: Pergamon
second language acquisition research. Language Learning, 42, Press.
47-8 3 . __ . ( 1 993). Linguistics and second language acquisition.
Chapelle, C., & Roberts, C . ( 1 986). Ambiguity tolerance and field London: Longmans.
independence as predictors of proficiency in English as a Corder, S.P. ( 1 988). Pedagogic grammars. In W. Rutherford & M.
second language. Language Learning, 36, 27-45. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and second language
Cherry, E.C. ( 1 953). Some experiments on the recognition of teaching, pp. 1 23-145. New York: Newbury House.
speech with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Cotman, C.W., & Lynch, G.S. ( 1 989). The neurobiology of learning
Society of America, 25, 975-979. and memory. Cognition, 33, 2 0 1 -24 1 .
Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. ( 1 982). Expertise in problem Cowan, N. ( 1 988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage,
solving. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology selective attention and their mtual constraints within the
Bibliography 263
262 Bibliography

doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Ann Arbor:


human information processing system. Psychological
University Microfilms International no. 8 8 1 6 1 2 1 .
Bulletin, 1 04, 1 63-1 9 1 .
. ( 1 99 1 ). Second language instruction does make a difference:
. ( 1 993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory.
__

Evidence from an empirical study of relativization. Studies in


__

Memory and Cognition, 2 1, 1 62-1 67.


Second Language Acquisition, 13, 43 1-469.
Craik, F.K., & Lockhart, R.S. ( 1 972). Levels of processing: A
Dulany, D., Carlson, R., & Dewey, G. ( 1 9 84). A case of syntactical
framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning
learning and judgment: how conscious and how abstract?
and Verbal Behavior, 1 1, 671-684.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1 13, 541-5 5 5 .
Crick, F. ( 1 995). The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search
. ( 1 985). On consciousness in syntactic learning and
for the soul. New York: Simon & Schuster.
__

judgment: A reply to Reber, Allen and Regan. Journal of


Crowder, R.G. ( 1 993). Short-term memory: Where do we stand?
Experimental Psychology: General, 1 1 4, 25-3 2 .
Memory and Cognition, 2 1, 1 42- 1 4 5 .
Dupuy, B . , & Krashen, S . ( 1 993). Incidental vocabulary acquisition
Curran, T., & Keele, S.W. ( 1 993). Attentional and nonattentional
in French as a foreign language. Applied Language Learning,
forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental
4, 5 5-63 .
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 1 89-202.
Eckman, F. ( 1 977). Markedness and the contrastive analysis
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A. ( 1 983). Individual differences in
hypothesis. Language Learning, 2 7, 3 1 5-330.
integrating information between and within sentences.
. ( 1 985). Some theoretical and pedagogical implications o f
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
__

the markedness differential hypothesis. Studies in Second


Cognition, 9, 5 6 1-5 83 .
Language Acquisition, 7, 289-3 07.
Dawson, M.E., & Schell, A.M. ( 1 982). Electrodermal reponses to
( 1 99 1 ). The structural conformity hypothesis and the
a.tten�ed and unattended significant stimuli during dichotic
__ .

acquisition of consonant clusters in the interlanguage of ESL


hstenmg. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 23-4 1 .
Perception & Performance, 8, 3 1 5-324.
Eckman, F., Bell, L. & Nelson, D . ( 1 988). On the generalization o f
Day, E., & Shapson, S. ( 1 99 1 ). Integrating formal and functional
relative clause instruction in the acquisition of English as a
approaches in language teaching in French immersion: An
second language. Applied Linguistics, 9, l -20.
experimental study. Language Learning, 41, 25-58.
Elley, W.B. ( 1 99 1 ). Acquiring literacy in a second language: The
Declerck, R. ( 1 994). The taxonomy and interpretation of clefts and
effect of book-based programs. Language Learning, 41, 3 7 5-
pseudo-clefts. Lingua, 93, 1 83-220.
411.
DeGraaff, R. ( 1 995). The experanto experiment; Acquiring the rules
of an artificial language. Unpublished manuscript' Free
Ellis, N. ( 1 993 ). Rules and instances in foreign language learning:
interactions of explicit and implicit knowledge. European
University of Amsterdam.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 289-3 1 8.
DeKeyser, R. ( 1 994). Implicit and explicit learning of L2 grammar:
A pilot study. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 1 88-1 94. __ . ( 1 994). Implicit and explicit learning: An overview. In N .
. ( 1 995). Learning second language grammar rules: An Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of language, pp. 1 -
1 6. London: Academic Press.
__

experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in


Second Language Acquisition, 1 7, 379-4 1 0.
Ellis, R. ( 1 9 89). Are classroom and naturalistic acquisition the
Dennett, D. ( 1 99 1 ). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA : Little same? A study of the acquisition of German word order rules.
Brown. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1 1, 305-328.
Dienes, Z. ( 1 992). Connectionist amd memory-array models of __ . ( 1 990). Instructed second language acquisition. Oxford:
artificial grammar learning. Cognitive Science, 1 6, 4 1-79. Basil Blackwell.
Doughty, C. ( 1 988). The effect of instruction on the acquisition of __ . ( 1 992). Second language acquisition and language
pedagogy. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
relativization in English as a second language. Unpublished
264 Bibliography Bibliography 265

__ . (l 993 a). The structural syllabus and second language Felix, S., & Weigl, W. ( 1 99 1 ). Universal Grammar in the classroom:
acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 2 7, 9 1-1 1 3 . the effects of positive and negative evidence in the classroom.
__ . ( 1 993b). Second language acquisition research: How does it Second Language Research, 7, 1 62-1 80.
help teachers? ELT Journal, 4 7, 3-1 1 . Fletcher, P. ( 1 980). Paraphrase relationships among clefted
__ . ( l 994a). Implicit knowledge and L2 pedagogy. TESOL sentences. In G.D. Prideaux, B.L. Derwing & W.J. Baker
Quarterly, 28, 1 67-1 73 . (Eds.), Experimental psycholinguistics, pp. 1 86-20 1 . Alberta:
__ . ( 1 994b ). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford: University of Alberta Press.
Oxford University Press. Flynn, S. ( 1 987a). Contrast and construction in a parameter-setting
__ . ( l 994c). A theory of instructed second language acquisition. model of L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 3 7, 1 9-62 .
In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of language, . ( 1 9 87b). A parameter-setting model of L2 acquisition.
pp.79-1 1 4 . London: Academic Press. -- Dordrecht: Reidel.
__ . ( 1 995). Interpretation tasks for grammar teaching. TESOL Fodor, J., & Crain, S. ( 1 987). Simplicity and generality of rules in
Quarterly, 29, 87- 1 0 5 . language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of
Elman, J . ( 1 988). Finding structure in time. CRL Technical Report language acquisition, pp.35-63. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
no. 880 1 . Center for Research on Language, University of Erlbaum.
California, San Diego. Fotos, S. ( 1 993). Consciousness-raising and noticing through focus
Ericsson, K.A., & Simon, H. ( 1 980). Verbal reports as data. on form: Grammar task performance versus formal
Psychological Review, 8 7, 2 1 5-25 1 . instruction. Applied Linguistics, 14, 3 8 5-407.
Estes, W.K. ( 1 986). Array models for category learning. Cognitive Fotos, S., & Ellis, R. ( 1 99 1 ). Communicating about grammar: A
Psychology, 18, 500-549. task-based approach. TESOL Quarterly, 25, 87-1 1 2 .
__ . ( 1 992). Mental psychophysics of categorization and Gass, S . ( 1 979). Language transfer and universal grammatical
decision making. In H. Geissler, S. Link & J.T. Townsend relations. Language Learning, 29, 327-344 .
(Eds.), Cognition, information processing and psychophysics: --. ( 1 982). From theory to practice. In W. Rutherford, & M.
Basic issues, pp. 1 23-14 1 . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hines (Eds.), On TESOL '81, pp . 1 29- 1 3 9. Washington:
Eubank, L. (Ed.) ( 1 99 1 ). Point counterpoint: Universal Grammar TESOL.
in the second language: Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Gasser, M. ( 1 990). Connectionism and universals of second language
Evans, J. St.B.T., Newstead, S., & Byrne, R.M. ( 1 993). Human acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1 2,
reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: 1 79- 1 99 .
Lawrence Erlbaum. Givon, T . ( 1 985). Function, structure and language acquisition. I n
Faerch, C. ( 1 986). Rules of thumb and other teacher formulated D. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language
rules in the foreign language classroom. In G. Kasper (Ed.), acquisition, Volume 1, pp. I 005- 1 028. Hillsdale, NJ:
Learning, teaching and communication in the foreign Lawrence Erlbaum.
language classroom, pp.65-79. Aarhus: Aarhus University Glass, A., & Holyoak, K. ( 1 986). Cognition. New York: Prentice
Press. Hall.
Faerch, C., & Kasper, G. ( 1 984). Two ways of defining Gluck, M., & Granger, R. ( 1 993). Computational models of the
communication strategies. Language Learning, 34, 45-63 . neural bases of learning and memory. Annual Review of
Felix, S. ( 1 99 1 ). The accessibility of Universal Grammar in second Neuroscience, 1 6, 667-706.
language acquisition. In L.Eubank (Ed.), Point counterpoint: Goodluck, H. ( 1 99 1 ). Language acquisition: A linguistic
Universal Grammar in the second language, p p . 89- 1 04 . introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Amsterdam:John Benjamins. Gordon, P. ( 1 990). Learnability and feedback. Developmental
Psychology, 26, 2 1 7-220.
266 Bibliography Bibliography 267

Graf, P. ( 1 990). Life span changes in implicit and explicit memory. Hatch, E., & Lazaraton, A. ( 1 99 1 ). The research manual: Design
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 3 53-3 5 8 . and statistics for applied linguistics. New York: Newbury
Graf, P . , & Mandler, G. ( 1 984). Activation makes words more House.
accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Hawkins, J. ( 1 988). On generative and typological approaches to
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 5 5 3-568. universal grammar. Lingua, 74, 85-1 00.
Graf, P., & Ryan, L. ( 1 990). Transfer-appropriate processing for Hayek, F.A. von. ( 1 962). Rules, perception and intelligibility.
implicit and explicit memory. Journal of Experimental Proceedings of the British Academy, 48, 3 2 1 -344.
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 6, 978-992. Hayes, N., & Broadbent, D. ( 1 988). Two modes of learning for
Green, P .S., & Hecht, K. ( 1 992). Implicit and explicit grammar: an interactive tasks. Cognition, 28, 249-276.
empirical study. Applied Linguistics, 13, 1 60-1 84 . Hebb, D.O. ( 1 949). The organization of behavior. New York:
Green, R.E.A., & Shanks, D.R. ( 1 993). On the existence o f Wiley.
independent explicit and implicit systems: A n examination o f Henning, M.J. ( 1 993). A connectionist model of the second
some evidence. Memory and Cognition, 21, 3 04-3 1 7. language acquisition of negation. Unpublished M.A. thesis,
Gregg, K. ( 1 984). Krashen's Monitor and Occam 's razor. Applied University of Hawai'i at Manoa, Department of English as a
Linguistics, 5, 79-1 00. Second Language.
__ . ( 1 988). Epistemology without knowledge: Schwartz on Hilles, S. ( 1 986). Interlanguage and the pro-drop parameter. Second
Chomsky, Fodor and Krashen. Second Language Research, 4, Language Research, 2, 33-52.
66-8 0 . Hintzman, D.L. ( 1 990). Human learning and memory: connections
Greenberg, S . ( 1 966). Universals of language typology. Stanford: and dissociations. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1 09-
Stanford University Press. 1 39.
Grossberg, S. ( 1 980). How does a brain build a cognitive code? Holding, D.H. (Ed.)( 1 989). Human skills: Studies in human
Psychological Review, 8 7, 1-5 1 . performance. New York: John Wiley.
__ ( 1 988). Neural networks and natural intelligence. Holland, J.H., Holyoak, K.J., Nisbett, R.E., & Thagard, P.R. ( 1 986).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Induction: Processes of iriference, learning, and discovery.
Hamann, S.B. ( 1 990). Level-of-processing effects in conceptually Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
driven implicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Howard, J.H., & Ballas, J.A. ( 1 980). Syntactic and semantic factors
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 6, 970-977. in the classification of non speech transient p atterns.
Hanson, S.J., & Burr, D.J. ( 1 990). What connectionist models learn: Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 43 1 -43 9.
Learning and representation in connectionist networks. Howard, J.H., Mutter, S.A., & Howard, D.V. ( 1 992). Serial pattern
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 471-5 1 8. learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental
Harley, B. ( 1 989). Functional grammar in French immersion: A Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 8, 1 029-
classroom experiment. Applied Linguistics, 1 0, 3 3 1-359. 1 040.
__ . ( 1 993). Instructional strategies and SLA in early French Hulstijn, J. ( 1 989). Experiments with semi artificial input in second
immersion. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1 5, language acquisition research. Scandinavian Working Papers
245-2 5 9 . on Bilingualism, 8, 28-40.
Harley, B . , & Swain, M. ( 1 984). The interlanguage of immersion __ . ( 1 990a). Implicit and incidental second language learning:
students and its implications for second language teaching. In Experiments in the processing of natural and partly artificial
A. Davies, C. Criper & A. Howatt (Eds.), Interlanguage, input. In H.W. Dechert (Ed.), Interlingual processing, pp . 5 0-
pp.29 1 -3 1 1 . Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 73 . Tubingen: Gunter Narr.
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M. ( 1 992). L2 working memory __ . ( 1 990b). A comparison between the information processing
capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language and analysis/control approaches to language learning. Applied
Acquisition, 1 4, 25-3 8 . Linguistics, 1 1, 3 0-45 .
268 Bibliography Bibliography 269

Hulstijn, J., & Hulstijn, W. ( 1 984). Grammatical errors as a function Keil, F .C. ( 1 989). Concepts, word meaning and cognitive
of processing constraints and explicit knowledge. Language development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Learning, 34, 23-43 . Kihlstrom, J.F. ( 1 984). Conscious, subconscious, unconscious: A
Hulstijn, J., & DeGraaff, R. ( 1 994). Under what conditions does cognitive perspective. In K. Bowers & D. Meichenbaum
explicit knowledge facilitate the acquisition of implicit (Eds.), The unconscious reconsidered, pp . 1 49-2 1 1 . New
knowledge? A research proposal. In J. Hulstijn & R. Schmidt York: Wiley.
(Eds.), Consciousness and second language learning: Kirsch, D. ( 1 99 1 ). PDP, learnability and innate knowledge of
Conceptual, methodological and practical issues in language language. UCSD Center for Research in Language Newsletter,
learning and teaching. A/LA Review, 1 1, 97-1 1 2 . 6, 3-1 7.
Hyams, N. ( 19 87). The theory o f parameters and syntactic Klayman, J., & Ha, Y. ( 1 989). Hypothesis testing in rule discovery:
development. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter Strategy, structure and content. Journal of Experimental
setting, pp.3 1-49. Dordrecht: Reidel. Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 5 9 6-604.
Jackendoff, R. ( 1 987). Consciousness and the computational mind. Knowlton, B ., & Squire, L. ( 1 994). The information acquired during
New York: Academic Press. artificial grammar learning.Journal of Experimental
Jacobs, B . ( 1 988). Neurobiological differentiation of primary and Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 79-9 1 .
secondary language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Kohonen, V. ( 1 992). Experiential language learning: Second
Acquisition, 1 0, 3 03-3 3 7 . language learning as cooperative learner education. In D.
Jacoby, L.L. ( 1 983). Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive Nunan (Ed.), Collaborative language learning and teaching,
processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal pp. 1 4-39. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Behavior, 22, 485-508. Krashen, S. ( 1 976). Formal and informal linguistic environments in
Jacoby, L.L., & Dallas, M. ( 1 9 8 1 ). On the relationship between language acquisition and language learning. TESOL Quarterly,
autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of 1 0, 1 5 7-1 68 .
Experimental Psychology: General, 1 1 0, 3 06-340. __ . ( 1 979). A response to McLaughlin, 'The monitor model:
Johnson, M.K., & Hasher, L. ( 1 987). Human learning and memory. ·
some methodological considerations'. Language Learning,
Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 63 1-668. 29, 1 5 1-1 67.
Jourdenais, 0., Ota, M., Stauffer, S., Boyson, B., & Doughty, C. __ . ( 1 98 1 a). Second language acquisition and second
( 1 995). Does textual enhancement promote noticing? A language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
think-aloud protocol analysis. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention __ . ( 1 9 8 1 b ). Aptitude and attitude in relation to second
and awareness in foreign language teaching and learning, language acquisition and learning. In K. Diller (Ed.),
pp. 1 32- 1 69. University of Hawai'i at Manoa: Second Individual differences and universals in language learning
Language Teaching and Curriculum Center Technical Report # aptitude, pp. 1 55-1 75 . Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
9. __ . ( 1 982). Principles and practice in second language
Kadlec, H., & Townsend, J.T. ( 1 992). Signal detection analyses of acquisition. Oxford, New York: Pergamon.
dimensional interactions. In F.G. Ashby (Ed.), __ . ( 1 985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications.
Multidimensional models of perception and categorization, Oxford, New York: Pergamon.
pp. 1 8 1-229. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. __ . ( 1 989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading:
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. ( 1 982). Judgments under More evidence for the input hypothesis. Modern Language
uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge Journal, 73, 440-464.
University Press. __ . ( 1 992). Under what circumstances, if any, should formal
Keenan, E., & Comrie, B . ( 1 977). Noun phrase accessibility and grammar instruction take place? TESOL Quarterly, 26, 409-
universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 63-99. 41 1 .
270 Bibliography Bibliography 271

__ . ( 1 993). The power of reading. Englewood, CO: Libraries learning, pp. 1 03 - 1 3 1 . University o f Hawai'i at Manoa:
Unlimited. Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center Technical
__ . ( 1 994). The input hypothesis and its rivals. In N. Ellis Report # 9.
(Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of language, pp.45-79. Lewicki, P. ( 1 985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances
London: Academic Press. on subsequent judgments. Journal of Personality and Social
Krashen, S., & Seliger, H. ( 1 975). The essential contributions o f Psychology, 48, 5 63-574.
formal and instruction in adult second language acquisition. __ . ( 1 986). Nonconscious social information processing. New
TESOL Quarterly, 9, 1 73-1 83 . York: Academic Press.
Krashen, S., & Terrell, T. ( 1 983). The natural approach: Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. ( 1 988). Acquisition of procedural
Language acquisition in the classroom. California: Alemany skill about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated.
Press. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24-37.
Krashen, S., Terrell, T., Ehrman, M., & Herzog, M. ( 1 984). A Light, L.L., & Singh, A. ( 1 987). Implicit and explicit memory in
theoretical basis for teaching the receptive skills. Foreign young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Language Annals, 1 7, 26 1-275. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 5 3 1 -54 1 .
Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R.B. ( 1 980). Affective discrimination Lightbown, P., & Pienemann, M . ( 1 993). Comments on Stephen D .
of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 5 5 7-55 8 . Krashen's "Teaching Issues: Formal Grammar Instruction. "
Kyllonen, P.C., & Christal, R . ( 1 989). Cognitive modelling o f TESOL Quarterly, 2 7, 7 1 7-722.
learning abilities: A report o n LAMP. In R.F. Dillon & J. Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. ( 1 990). Focus-on-form and corrective
Pellegrino (Eds.), Testing: Theoretical and applied feedback in communicative language teaching: Effects o n
perspectives, pp. 1 46- 1 7 3 . New York: Praeger. second language learning. Studies In Second Language
Lachter, J., & Bever, T.G. ( 1 988). The relation between linguistic Acquisition, 12, 429-448.
structure and associative theories of language learning-A Lightbown, P., Spada, N., & Ranta, L. ( 1 99 1 ). The effect o f
constructive critique of some connectionist learning models. instruction on IL-formed questions. Paper presented at the
Cognition, 28, 1 95-247. Second Language Research Forum, UCLA, Los Angeles, USA.
Lado, R. ( 1 965). Memory span as a factor in second language Lightfoot, D. ( 1 989). Degree-0 learnability and the child's trigger
learning. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 1, 1 23 - experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 3 2 1 -334.
1 30. . ( 1 99 1 ) . How to set parameters: Arguments from language
Langer, E.J. ( 1 978). Rethinking the role of thought in social -- change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
interaction. In J.H. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R.F. Kidd (Eds.), New Logan, G.D. ( 1 988a). Toward an instance theory of automatization.
directions in attribution research, Volume 2, pp .3 5-5 5 . Psychological Review, 95, 492-527 .
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. . ( 1 988b). Automaticity, resources and memory: Theoretical
Larson-Freeman, D., & Long, M.H. ( 1 99 1 ). An introduction to -- controversies and practical implications. Human Factors, 3 0,
second language acquisition research. New York: Longman. 5 83-5 9 8 .
Lee, C., & Gasser, M. ( 1 992). Where do underlying representations --. ( 1 990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common
come from?: A connectionist approach to the acquisition o f underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1-35.
phonological rules. In J . Dinsmore (Ed.), The symbolic and Logan, G.D., & Etherton, J.L. ( 1 994). What is learned in
connectionist paradigms: Closing the gap, pp. 1 79-2 0 8 . automatization? The role of attention in constructing an
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. instance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Leeman, J., Arteagoitia, Fridman, B., & Doughty, C. ( 1 99 5 ) . Memory and Cognition, 20, 1 022- 1 050.
Integrating attention to form with meaning: Focus o n form in Logan, G.D., & Klapp, S.T. ( 1 99 1 ). Automatizing alphabet
content-based Spanish instruction. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), arithmetic problems 1 : Is extended practice necessary t o
Attention and awareness in foreign language teaching and
272 Bibliography
Bibliography 273

produce automaticity? Journal of Experimental Psychology


: MacWhinney, B., Leinbach, J., Taraban, R., & McDonald, J.
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 7, 1 79-1 95.
Logan, G.D., & Stadler, M. ( 1 99 1 ). Mechanisms of perfo ( 1 989). Language learning: Cues or rules? Journal of Memory
rmance and Language 28, 2 5 5 -2 7 7:
improvement in consistent mapping memory search . .
: Malt, B. ( 1 989). An on-line mvestlgat10n of prototyp� and
Automaticity or strategy shift? Journal of Experiment
al exemplar strategies in classification. Jour��l of Experimental
Psychology: Learn ing, Memory and Cognition, 1 7, 478-4
96. Psychology, Learning, Memory and Cognztwn, 15, 539-5 5 5 .
Long, M.H. ( 1 983). Does second language instruction
make a Mandler, G . ( 1 992). Toward a theory o f consciousness. I� H .
difference? A review of research. TESOL Quarterly, 1 7, 3 5 .
9- Geissler, S . Link, & T . Townsend (Eds.), Cognztwn,
3 82.
. ( 1 985). A role for instruction in second langua information processing and psychophysics, pp.43-67 ·

ge
__

development: Task-based language learning. In K. Hyltenstam Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.


& M. Pienemann (Eds.), Modelling and assess Marcus, G., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T., &_ ?Cu,
.

ing second F. ( 1 992). Overregularization in language acqulSlt1�n.


language acquisition, pp.77-99. Clevedon, .
Avon : Monographs of the Society for Research m Child
Multilingual Matters.
__ . ( 1 988). Instructed interlanguage development. In L. Beebe Development, 57, 4 (Serial no. 228). .
(Ed.), Issues in second language acquisition: Multip Master, P. ( 1 994). The effect of systemati� instruction on l� arnmg
le the English article system. In T. Odlm (Ed.), Perspective� on
perspectives, pp. 1 1 5-14 1 . New York: Newbury House.
__ . ( 1 989). Task, group and task-group interactions. University pedagogical grammar, pp.229-252. New York: Cambndge
of Hawaii Working Papers in ESL, 8, 1-26. University Press. .
. ( 1 990). Maturational constraints on language devel opme Matthews, R.C., Buss, R.R., Stanley, W.B., Blanchar�-F1�l�s, F.,
nt.
__

Ryeul Cho, J., & Druhan, B . ( 1 9 89). Role of imphc1t �d


Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 2 5 1 -286. .
__ . ( 1 99 1 ). Focus on form: A design feature in language explicit processes in learning from examples: A synerg1�tlc
teaching. In K. de Bot., R. Ginsberg & C. Kram sch (Eds.) effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning,
, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1 083-1 100.
Foriegn language research in cross cultural persp
ective,
.
pp.39-52. Amsterdam: John Benja mins. McAndrews, M.P., & Moscovitch, M. ( 1 985). Rule-based and
.
__ . ( 1 994). The role of the linguistic environment in second exemplar-based classification in artificial grammar learnmg.
language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie (Ed.), Handbook Memory & Cognition, 13, 469-475.
of Mccawley, J. (1 988). The Syntactic phenomena of Englzsh: . Volume
language acquisition, Volume 2: Second language
acquisition. New York: Academic Press. 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Long, M.H., & Crookes, G. ( 1 992). Three approaches to task-b McClelland, J., & Elman, J. ( 1 986). The TRACE model of speech
ased perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86.
syllabus design. TESOL Quarterly, 26, 1 34-1 56. .
Loschky, L., & Bley-Vroman, R. ( 1 993). Grammar and task-b McKoon, G., Ratcliff, R., & Dell, G. ( 1 986). A critical evalu�t10n of
ased the semantic episodic distinction. Journal of Experimental
learning. In S. Gass & G. Crookes (Eds.), Task-based language
teaching: Pedagogical perspectives. Clevedon: Multilingual Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 295-3 ? 6.
Matters. McLaughlin, B. ( 1 978). The monitor model: Some methodological
.
Lyster, R. ( 1 994). The effect of functional-analytic teaching considerations. Language Learning, 28, 3 09�332. .
aspects of French immersion students' sociolinguistic
on . ( 1 9 80). On the use of miniature arti�cial languages m
_ .
competence. Applied Linguistics, 15, 2 62-28 7. --second language research. Applied Psycholmguzstzcs, 1, 3 5 7-
Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. ( 1 995). Getting learners to notice 3 69 .
: of second language learning. L on don:
Negotiation ofform as negative evidence. Paper presented at --. ( 1 987). Theories
the Second Language Research Forum, Cornell University, Edward Arnold.
Ithaca, New York, USA. __. ( 1 990a). Restructuring. Applied Linguistics, 1 1, 1 1 3-1 28.
274 Bibliography
Bibliography 275
__. ( 1 990b). "Conscious" versus "unconscio
us" learning. TESOL
Quarterly, 24, 6 1 7-6 3 4 . Murphy, G.L., & Medin, D.L. ( 1 985 } . The role of theories in
McLaughlin, B . , Rossman, T .
. , & McLeod, B . ( 1 983 ). conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 2 89-3 1 6 .
language learning: An informa Second Naiman, N., Frolich, M., Stern, H.H., & Todesco? A . 0 978). The
tion processing perspectiv e. .
Language Learning, 33, 1 3 5-1
57. good language learner. Research in �ducation Senes 7 : The
Medin, D. � ., & Smith, E.E . Ontario Institute for Studies in Educat10n.
( 1 98 1 ). Strategies and classifi .
learnmg. Journal of Experim cation Nation, R., & McLaughlin, B. ( 1 986). Experts and novices: an
ental Psychology: Huma n
Learning and Memory, 7, 24 1-2
53 . information processing approach to the good language learner
Meike, B . ( 1 988). Mind/ah 2.
1 . Dartmouth College: Coursew problem. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 4 1-5 � .
De vel opm ent De partment. are Navon, D. ( 1 984). Resources: A theoretical soupstone.
Meisel, J., Clahsen, H., & Psychological Review, 91, 2 1 6-234.
Pienemann, M. ( 1 98 1 ). On
deve!op�ental stages in natura Navon, D., & Gopher, D. ( 1 980). On the economy of the human
l second language acquisition.
Studies zn Second Language Acq
uisition, 3, 1 09- 1 3 5 . processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 254-2 5 5 .
Mellow, D , & Cum i g, A .
: i_n � ( 1 994 ). Concord in interlangua Nelson, D.L. ( 1 989). Implicitly activated �nowledg� and memory.
ge: .
. Efficiency or pnmmg? Applied Linguistics, 1 5, 442-47 1 . In C. Izawa (Ed.), Current issues zn cognitive processes,
Menckle, �·!"f ·, & R in old, pp.3 69-3 87. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
� � E.M . ( 1 99 1 ). Comparing dire .
(Exphc19 and mdirect (Im
plicit) measures to study
ct Nelson, D.L., Canas, J., Bajo, M., & Keelean, P. ( 1 9 � 7). Comparmg
uncon�cious memory. Journal word fragment completion and cued recall with letter cues.
of Experimental Psychology, .
Learning, Memory and Cogniti Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory and
on, 1 7, 224-23 3 .
Mohle, D., & Raupach, M . Cognition, 13, 542-5 52.
( 1 989 ). Language transfer and
procedur�l knowledge. In H. Dec Nelson, D.L., & McEvoy, C.L. ( 1 979). Encoding context and �et
hert & M. Raupach (Eds.),
Transfer zn language production
, pp. 8 1 - 1 03 . Norwood, NJ: size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning
Ablex. and Memory, 5, 292-3 14.
Moray, N. ( 1 959 ). Attention in Nisbett, R.E., & Wilson, T.D. ( 1 977). Telling more than we � an
dichotic listening task s:Affective
cues and the influence of instruc know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological
tion s. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1 1, 5
6-60. Review, 84, 23 1 -2 5 9 .
Morgan, J., & Newport, E. ( 1 98 Nissen, M.J., & Bullemer, P. ( 1 987). Attentional requirement� of
1 ). The role of constituent structur .
in the induction of an artificial e learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive
. language. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20,
67- 8 5 . Psychology, 1 9, 1-32.
Morg�n, J., �eier, R., & Newpor .
t, E . ( 1 9 87) . Structural packaging Norman, D.A. ( 1 968). Toward a theory of memory and attent10n.
m the mput to language learning Psychological Review, 84, 23 1-259.
. . : Contributions of prosodic .
and morphologica l markmg of phrases in the acq Nosofsky, R.M. ( 1 992). Exemplar-based a��roach to relatmg
language. Cognitive Psychology, uisition o f categorization, identification and recogmtion. In F :G. Ashby
1 9, 498-55 0.
( 1 989 ). Facilitating the acquisition (Ed.), Multidimensional �odels of perception and
of syntax with cross­
.
_

sentential cues to phrase structur categorization, pp.363-395 . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
e. Journal of Memory and
Language, 28, 3 60-3 74. Nosofsky, R.M., Clark, S .E., & �hin, H.J. ( 1989). Rules .and
Munsell, P ., & Carr, T.H . ( 1 98 . . . _
exemplars in categorization, identification and recogmt10n.
.

revie': o ' Second Language Acquisi
Lea�nm g by S.D .Krashen. Langua
1 ). Monitoring the monitor: A
tion and Second Language Journal of Experimental Psychology: � .
rning, Memory and
Cognition, 15, 282-3 04.
ge Learning, 31, 493 -50 2. . .
Murakami, K. ( 1 974 ). A languag
e aptitude test for the Japanese. Nosofsky, R.M., Kruschke, J., & McKinley, S. ( 1 992). Com�mi?g
System, 2, 3 1-47. exemplar-based category representations and connect10� ist
learning rules. Journal of Experimental psychology: Learning,
Memory and Cognition, 18, 2 1 1 -23 3 .
276 Bibliography Bibliography 277

Nunan, D. ( 1 988). Syllabus design. Oxford: Oxford Unive rules in complex experimental situations. Cognitive
rsity
Press. Psychology, 22, 493-5 16.
. ( 1 989). Designing tasks for the communicative classroom. . .
__
Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. ( 1 990). Synthetic grammar learnmg:
. Camb ridge: Cambridge University Press. Implicit rule abstraction or fragmentary knowledge? Journal
Odlm, T. ( 1 9 � 6). On �he nature and use of explicit knowl of Experimental Psychology: General, 1 19, 264-275.
edge.
International Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, 1 23-14 4. Pica, T. ( 1 983). Adult acquisition of English as a second language
__ ( 1 994). (Ed.) 1:ersP_ectives on pedagogical grammar. under different conditions of exposure. Language Learnmg, _
Oxford: Oxford Umversity Press. 33, 465-497.
O' Grady, w. 0 ? 93). Syntactic development: The acquisition . ( 1 985). The selective impact of classroom instruction o n
_ h. Chica _ rsity of of
Englts go: Umve Chicago Press. -- second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 6, 2 1 4-222.
O' Malley, M., & Chamot, A. ( 1 990). Learn ing strategies in second
language acquis ition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. --. ( 1 994). Questions from the language classroom: A research
perspective. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 49-80.
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. ( 1 99 1 ). Phono logica Pimsleur, P. ( 1 966). The Pimsleur language aptitude battery. New
l
short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich .
._Jo urnal of Memory and Language, 30, 3 3 1-347 . Pienemann, M. ( 1 992). Teachability theory. University of Sydney:
Paradi s, M. ( 1 994 ). �eurolinguistic aspects of implic it and explic Language Acquisition Research Centre.
_ _ it .
mem?�y: implic ations for bilingualism. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Pienemann, M., Johnston, M., & Brindley, G. ( 1 988). Developmg
Impltc1t and explicit learning of language, pp.39 3-4 an acquisition-based procedure for second language assessment.
1 9.
London: Academic Press. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1 0, 2 1 7-243 .
Parry, T., & Child, R. ( 1 990). Field testing a language aptitud Pinker, S. ( 1 989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of
e
me�sure: the cas� of VORD. In C. Stansfield (Ed.), Language argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
aptitude reconsidered, pp.3 1-6 1 . Washington: Center
for __ . ( 1 99 1 ). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530-? 35.
Applied Linguistics. . ( 1 994). The language instinct. London: Pengum Boo�s .
.
__

Patkowski, M.S. ( 1 980). The sensitive period for the acquisition Pinker, S., & Prince, A. ( 1 988). On language and connectiomsm:
of
syntax . Language Learning, 30, 449-4 72. analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language
Pavesi , M. ( 1 986). Markedness, discoursal modes, and relativ acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73- 1 93.
e . .
clause formation in a formal and an informal context. Studies Pitts, M., White, H., & Krashen, S. ( 1 989). Acqmnng second
In Second Language Acquisition, 8, 3 8-55 . languagevocabulary through reading: A replication of the
Pekala, R.J. ( 1 99 1 ). Quantifying consciousness: A n empir .
ical Clockwork Orange study using second language acqmrers.
approach. New York: Plenum Press. ·
Reading in a Foreign Language, 5, 27 1-275.
Perruchet, P., & Amorim, M.A. ( 1 992). Conscious knowledge and Polanyi, M. ( 1 95 8). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical
c� ang: s �_ n performance in sequence learning: Evidence against philosophy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
dissoc iation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn ing, Prabhu, N.S. ( 1 987). Second language pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford
Memory and Cognition, 18, 78 5-800 . University Press.
Perruchet, P., & Gallego, G. ( 1 993). Association between conscious Preston, D. ( 1 989). Sociolinguistics and second language
knowledge and performance in normal subjects: A reply t o acquisition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Cohen and Curran ( 1 993), and Willingham, Greeley and . .
Prince, E. ( 1 978). A comparison of wh-clefts and it-clefts m
Bardo�e ( 1 993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: discourse. Language, 54, 8 83-907.
Learnmg, Memory and Cognition, 1 9, 1 43 8-1 444. ·
Raaijmakers, J.G.W., & Shiffrin, R.M. ( 1 992). Models of memory.
Perruchet, P., ?allego, J., & Savy, I. ( 1 990). A critical reappraisal Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 205-234.
of the evidence for unconscious abstraction of determ inistic .
Radford, A. ( 1 990). Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English
syntax. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Bibliography 279
278 Bibliography

contingencies and instructional control, pp.36-5 1 .


New York:
Rankin, J. (1 990). A case for closedmindedness: Accuracy and
complexity in performance on open and closed second Plenum Press.
of mem ory .
langu�[fe tasks. Unpublished manuscript, University o f Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. (1 988). Measures
Hawm 1 at Manoa, Department o f English as a Second Annual Review of Psychology 39, 475- 543 .
dural skill
Language. Robbins, S. ( 1 992). A neurobiological model of proce
5.
Ramat, A.G. ( 1 992). Grammaticalization processes in the area o f acquisition. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 3, 23 5-26
in the units of
temporal and modal relations. Studies in Second Language Robinson, P. ( 1 986). Constituency or dependency er' s
the learn
Acquisition, 1 4, 297-3 22. language acquisition? An approach to describing
1 0, 4 1 7-
Reber, A.S. (1 967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal analysis of formulae. Lingvisticae Investigationes,
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 8 5 5-863 . 437.
learning.
. (1 969). Transfer of syntactic structure in synthetic . ( 1 9 89). Procedural vocabulary and language
--
Journal of Pragmatics, 13, 523-546.
__

languages. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 1 1 5-1 1 9. data:


. ( 1 990). Metaphors for the description of acquisition
--: (1 976 !. Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of --
From constituency 'trees ' to dependency
' frame s' .
mstructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 292 .
Human Learning and Memory, 2, 88-94. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 28, 273-
tiation and
. ( 1 989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of . ( 1 992) . Discourse semantics, interlanguage nego
1,
--the lexicon. University of Hawai 'i Working Papers ESL, 1 in
__

Experimental Psychology: General, 1 1 8, 2 1 9-23 5 .


. (1 990). On the primacy of the implicit: Comment on 5 1 -90 .
cit/explicit
. ( 1 993a). Problems of knowledge and the impli
__

Perruchet and Pacteau. Journal of Experimental Psychology: -- i 'i Working


General, 1 1 9, 3 40-342. distinction in SLA theory. University of Hawa
. ( 1 992). The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary Papers in ESL, 12, 90-1 3 8 .
for the
. ( 1 993b ). Review of David Nunan's 'Designing tasks
__

perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, J , 93- 1 3 3 . , 14, 442- 446 .


. ( 1 993 ). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Oxford: --communicative classroom ' . Applied Lingulangu istics
age acqu isitio n
. ( 1 994a ). Implicit knowledge, second
__

Clarendon Press. -- 1 60-1 66.


Reber� A.S., & �llen, R. ( 1 978) . . Analogy and abstraction strategies and syllabus construction. TESOL Quarterly, 28,
sses: Noun
m sy�t�etic grammar learnmg: A functionalist interpretation. . ( 1994 b). Universals of word formation proce
an as a second
Cognztzon, 6, 1 89-22 1 .
·

--incorporation in the acquisition of Samo


Reber, A., Kas� im, S., Lewis, s ., & Cantor, G. ( 1 980). On the language. Language Learning, 44, 569-6 1 5 .
locative
. .
relati�msh1p between 1mphcit , and explicit modes in the . ( 1 994c ). Modeling the acquisition of the
isitio of verb
n
learnmg of a complex rule structure. Journal of Experimental --alternation: Rules and associations in the acqu University o f
Psychology, 6, 492-502. argument structure. Unpublished manuscript,
a Second
Reber, A.s .,, & Millw�rd, R.B. ( 1 97 1 ) . Event tracking in probability Hawai' i at Manoa, Department of English as
learnmg. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 85-99. Language.
narrative
Reber, A.�., Walke�field, F .,, � Hernstadt, F. ( 1 99 1 ). Implicit and . ( 1 995a ). Task comp lexity and second language
--
.
exphc�t learnmg: Ind1v1dual differences and IQ. Journal of discourse. Language Learning, 45, 99-1 40.
similarity
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition' . ( 1 995b ). Aptitude, awareness and the fundamental
learn ing. In R.
1 � 8 8 8-89 6 . --of implicit and explicit second language n lang uage
Reed, S., Ackinclos�, �.�., & Voss, A. ( 1 990). Selecting analogous Schmidt (Ed.) , Attention and awareness in foreig at
of Haw ai'i
problems: S1m1lanty versus inclusiveness. Memory & teaching and learn ing, pp.3 1 1-36 2. University
culum Cent er
Cognition, 18, 83- 98. Manoa: Second Language Teaching and Curri
Reese, H. ( 1 989). Cognitive and behavioristic views of rule use. I n Technical Report # 9 .
S.C. Hays (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition,
280 Bibliography
Bibliography 281

. (1 995 c). Attention, memory and


the 'noticing' hypoth esis .
__

Language Learning, 45, 283 -3 3 1 application o f experience. Journal ?f Experimental


. .
__ . ( 1 995 d). Implicit learning, Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognztwn, 21, 469-4 8 2 .
memory and knowledge:
Conceptual and methodological Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J. ( 1 986). O n learning the past
issues for experimental
second language research.
Manuscript submitted for tenses of English verbs. In J. Mcclelland, D. Rumelhart, & the
PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: _
publication.
. ( 1 996 ) Le�n_iing i ple and com Explorations in the microstructures of cognition, Volume 2:
: �� plex second language rules
__

under 1mphc1t, mc1dental, rule Psychological and biological models, pp.2 1 6-2 7 1 .
-search and instructed
. conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, ( 1 ) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
. . . .
Robmson, P., & Ha, M . ( 1 993 ). Russo, R., & Parkin, A. ( 1 993). Age differences m 1mphc1t
Instance theory and second
language rule learning under explicit memory: More apparent than real. Memory & Cognition, 2 1 ,
conditions. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 1 5, 73-8 0 . . . .
4 1 3-43 8.
Robinson, P., & Ross, S. ( 1 996 ). Rutherford, W. ( 1986). Grammatical theory and L2 acqms1t1on: A
The development of task-based
testing in English for Academic Pur brief overview. Second Language Research, 2, 1-1 5 .
poses programs. Applied
Linguistics, 1 7 (2). . ( 1 987). Second language grammar: Learning and
Robinson, P., Ting, S., & Urwin, teaching. London, New York: Longman.
J. ( 1 995 ). Investigating second --

language task complexity. RELC Jou Rutherford, W., & Sharwood Smith, W. (Eds.) ( 1 988). Grammar
. rnal 26 (2).
Roediger, H.L ., & Blaxton, T. ( 1 987 and second language teaching. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
). Effects of varying mod ality,
surface features and retention inte Sasaki, M. ( 1 993). Relationships among se�ond . language
rval on priming in word­
frag ment completion. Memory & Cogniti proficiency, foreign language aptitude and mtelhgence: A
. on, 15, 3 79-3 88 . .
Roed1g�r, H L Weldon, M.S . & Cha structural equation modeling approach. Language Learning,
: ·; llis, B .H. ( 1 989 ). Explaining
d1ssoc1at10ns between implicit and 43, 3 1 3-344.
explicit measures of
rete!1tion: A processing account. Sato, E., & Jacobs, B. ( 1 992). From input to intake: towards a
In H.L . Roediger, & F .I.
Cra1k (Eds.), Varieties of memory brain-based perspective on selective attention. Issues In
and consciousness, pp. 3-
4 1 . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Applied Linguistics, 3, 267-292.
. .
Rosansky, E.J. ( 1 975 ). The critical Sawyer, M. ( 1 993). The modern language aptitude test: A Ras eh
period for the acquisition o f .
language: Som e cognitive develop analysis of its behavior with native an� non-natz��
mental con siderati ons . . . ,
Working Papers on Bilingualism, 6, populations. Unpublished manuscript, Umvers1ty of Hawm I
1 1 5-1 2 1 .
Rosch, E . ( 1 973 ). Natural categor at Manoa, Department of English as a Second Language.
ies. Cognitive Psychology 4
3 2 8-3 5 0 . ' ' Schacter, D.L. ( 1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
Rosenb latt, F. ( 1 958 ). The Percep Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
tron : A probabilistic mod el for
information storage and organiza Cognition, 13, 501-5 1 8 . .
tion m the brain.
Psychological Review, 65, 386-40
8. __ . ( 1 989). On the relation between �emory an� consc10usness:
Ross, B .H. ( 1 989 ). Remindings in dissociable interactions and consc10us expenence. In H.L.
learning and instruct ion. In S.
Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Sim Roediger, & F.I. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and
ilarity and analogical
reason ing, pp.43 8-4 69. Cambrid consciousness, pp.35 5-3 89. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawre�c� Erlbaum.
ge: Cambridge Univers ity
Pre ss. Schacter, D.L., Chiu, P., & Ochsner, K. ( 1 993). Imp licit memory :
Ros s, s..a: ., & Kenned , P.T . ( 1 990 A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 1 6, 1 5 9-
� ). Generalizing from the use o f
earher exam ples m problem solving. 1 82 .
Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Schacter, J . ( 1 990). The issue o f completeness in second language
Cognition, 1 6, 42- 5 5 .
Rothkopf, E.Z ., & Dashen, M.L acquisition. Second Language Research, 6, 93-124 . . .
. ( 1 995 ). Particularization: _
Inductive speeding of rule-governe Schachter, J., Tyson, A., & Diffley, F. ( 1 976). Learner mtmt10ns of
d decisions by narrow
grammaticality. Language Learning, 26, 67-76.
282 Bibliography Bibliography 283

Schmidt, R. ( 1 988). The potential of parallel distributed processing Schwartz, B.D. ( 1 986). The epistomological status of second
for SLA theory and research. University of Hawai 'i Working language . acquisition. Second Language Research, 2, 1 2 0-
Papers in ESL, 7, 5 5-66. 1 59 .
__ . ( 1 990). The role of consciousness in second language . ( 1 988). A reply to Gregg: In defence of theory building.
learning. Applied Linguistics, 1 1, 129- 1 5 8 . -- Second Language Research, 4, 1 5 7- 1 73 .
__ . ( 1 992). Psychological mechanisms underlying second --. ( 1 992). Testing between UG-based and problem-solving
language fluency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, models of L2 Acquisition: Developmental sequence data.
1 4, 3 5 7-3 87. Language Acquisition, 2, 1-19.
__ . ( 1 993a). Awareness and second language acquisition. Annual . (1 993). On explicit and negative data effecting and
Review of Applied Linguistics (1993), 1 3, 206-226 . -- affecting competence and linguistic behavior. Studies in
( 1 993b). Consciousness, learning and interlanguage Second Language Acquisition, 1 5, 1 47-1 63 .
pragmatics. In G. Kasper & S. Blum-Kulka (Eds.), Schwartz, B.D., & Gubala-Ryzak, M. ( 1 992). Learnability and
Interlanguage pragmatics, pp.2 1-42. New York: Oxford grammar reorganization in L2A: Against negative evidence
University Press. causing the unlearning of verb movement. Second Language
__ . ( 1 994a). Deconstructing consciousness in search of useful Research, 8, 1-38.
definitions for applied linguistics. In J. Hulstijn & R. Schmidt Schwartz, B., Rosse, R., & Deutsch, S. ( 1 993). Limits of the
(Eds.), Consciousness and second language learning: processing view in accounting for dissociations among
Conceptual, methodological and practical issues in language memory measures in a clinical population. Memory &
teaching and learning. A/LA Review, 1 1, 1 1-26. Cognition, 21, 63-72.
__ . ( 1 994b). Implicit learning and the cognitive unconscious: Of Scovel, T. ( 1 988). A time to speak. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
artificial grammars and SLA. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and Searle, J. ( 1 990). Consciousness, explanatory inversion and
explicit learning of language, pp . 1 65-209. London: cognitive science. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 13, 5 8 5-
Academic Press. 596.
__ . ( 1 995). Consciousness and foreign language: A tutorial o n __ . ( 1 992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: M I T
the role o f attention and awareness in learning.. In R. Schmidt Press.
(Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language teaching Seliger, H. ( 1 975). Inductive method and deductive method in
and learning, pp. 1 2-52. University of Hawai'i at Manoa: language teaching: A re-examination. International Review of
Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center Technical Applied Linguistics, 13, 1-1 8 .
Report # 9. . ( 1 979). On the nature and function of language rules in
Schmidt, R., & Frota, S. ( 1 986). Developing basic conversational -- language teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 13, 359-396.
ability in a second language: A case study of an adult learner of Servan-Schreiber, E., & Anderson, J.R. ( 1 990). Learning artificial
Portuguese. In R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in grammars with competitive chunking. Journal of
second language learning, pp.23 7-326. Rowley, MA: Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,
Newbury House. 1 6, 5 92-60 8 .
Schneider, W. ( 1 993). Varieties of working memory as seen in Shanks, D.R., & St. John, M.F. ( 1994). Characteristics o f dissociable
biology and in connectionist/control architectures. Memory human systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1 7, 3 6 7-44 7.
and Cognition, 21, 1 84- 1 92 . Shannon, C.E. ( 1 948). A mathematical theory of communication.
Schneider, W., & Detweiler, M . ( 1 988). The role o f practice in Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 3 79-243 .
dual-task performance: Toward workload modeling in a Sharwood Smith, M. ( 1 980). Consciousness-raising and the second
connectionist-control architecture. Human Factors, 30, 5 3 9- language learner. Applied Linguistics, 2, 1 59-1 68.
566.
284 Bibliography Bibliography 285

__ . ( 1 99 1 ). Speaking to many minds: On the relevance of task. In C. Izawa (Ed.), Current issues in cognitive processes,
different types of language information for the L2 learner. pp. 1 3 5- 1 74. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Second Language Research, 7, 1 1 8- 1 3 2 . Spada, N., & Lightbown, P. ( 1 993). Instruction and the
__ . ( 1 993 ) . Input enhancement in instructed SLA. Studies in development of questions in L2 classrooms. Studies in Second
Second Language Acquisition, 1 5, 1 65-1 79. Language Acquisition, 15, 205-224.
__ . ( 1 994). The unruly world of language. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Squire, L. ( 1 987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford
implicit and explicit learning of language, pp. 3 4-43 . University Press.
London: Academic Press. . ( 1 992). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple
Shiffrin, R.M. ( 1 993). Short-term memory: A brief commentary. -- brain systems supporting learning and memory. Journal of
Memory & Cognition, 21, 1 93-197. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 232-243 .
Shiffrin, R.M., & Schneider, W. ( 1 977). Controlled and automatic Squire, L., Amaral, D., & Press, G. ( 1 990). Magnetic resonance
_ .
human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, imaging measurements of htppocampal format10n and
automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological mammillary nuclei distinguish medial temporal lobe and
Review, 84, 1 27-1 90. diencephalic amnesia. Journal od Neuroscience, 1 0, 3 1 0 6-
Shirai, Y. ( 1 992). Conditions on transfer: A connectionist 3 1 l 7.
approach. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 3, 9 1- 1 20. Squire, L., & Cohen, N. ( 1 9 84). Human memory and amnesia. In
Shook, D.J. ( 1 994). FL/L2 reading, grammatical information and J.McGaugh, G. Lynch, & N. Weinberger (Eds.), Proceedings
the input-to-intake phenomenon. Applied Language of the conference on the neurobiology of learning and
Learning, 5, (2), 5 7-93 . memory, pp.3-64. New York: Guildford Press.
Skehan, P. ( 1 986). The role of foreign language aptitude in a model Squire, L., & McKee, R. ( 1 993). Declarative a�d nondeclarati":e
of school learning. Language Testing, 3, 1 88-22 1 . memory in opposition: When prior events mfluence amnesic
__ . ( 1 987). Cluster analysis and the identification of learner patients more than normal subjects. Memory & Cognition, 21,
types. In V. Cook (Ed.), Experimental approaches to second 424-43 0.
language acquisition, pp. 8 1-94. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Squire, L., & Zola-Morgan, S. ( 1 99 1 ). The medial temporal lobe
__ . ( 1 989). Individual differences in second language learning. memory system. Science, 253, 1 3 80-1 3 86.
London: Edward Arnold. Stadler, M. ( 1 995). Role of attention in implicit learning. Journal
Skousan, R. ( 1 990). Analogic modeling of language structure. of Experimental Psychology; Learning, Memory and
Dordrecht: Reidel. Cognition, 21 , 674-685.
Slimani, E. ( 1 992). Evaluation of classroom interaction. In J.C. Stansfield, C. (Ed.) ( 1 990). Language aptitude reconsidered.
Alderson & A. Beretta (Eds.), Evaluating second language Washington: Center for Applied Linguistics.
education, pp. 1 97-220. Cambridge: Cambridge University Stem, L. ( 1 985). The structures and strategies of human memory.
Press. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.
Slobin, D . ( 1 979). Psycholinguistics. (2nd edition) Glenview, IL: Stevick, E. ( 1 976). Memory, meaning and method. Rowley, MA :
Scott Foresman . Newbury House.
__ . ( 1 985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language making Strayer, D.L., & Kramer, A. ( 1 990). Attentional requirements o f
capacity. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of controlled and automatic processing. Journal of Experimental
language acquisition, Volume 2, pp. 1 1 5 7- 1 249. Hillsdale, Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 6, 67-82.
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sweller, J. ( 1 988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on
Smolensky, P. ( 1 988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257-2 8 5 .
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1 1, 1-74. Sweller, J., Chandler, P., Tierney, P . , & Cooper, M . ( 1 990).
Snodgrass, J.G. ( 1 989). How many memory systems are there Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical
really?: Some evidence from the picture fragment completion
286 Bibliography Bibliography 287

material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1 1 9, VanPatten, B., & Cadierno, T. ( 1 993). Explicit instruction and
1 76- 1 92 . input processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Tarone, E . ( 1 985). Variability in interlanguage use: A study o f style 1 5, 225-243 .
shifting in morphology and syntax. Language Learning, 3 5, Velmans, M. ( 1 99 1 ). Is human information processing conscious?
3 73-403 . Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 14, 65 1-726.
__ . ( 1 988). Variation in interlanguage. London: Edward Vokey, J., & Brooks, L. ( 1 992). Salience of item knowledge in
Arnold. learning artificial grammars. Journal of Experimental
Taylor, M.M., Lindsay, P.H., & Forbes, S.M. ( 1 967). Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1 8, 3 2 8-344.
Quantification of shared capacity processing in auditory and Voss, J.F. ( 1 986). Informal reasoning and subject matter knowledge
visual discrimination. Acta Psychologica, 2 7, 223-229. in the solving of economics problems by naive and novice
Terrell, T. ( 1 9 82). The natural approach to language teaching: an individuals. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 269-302.
update. Modern Language Journal, 65, 24 1-247. Voss, J.F., Perkins, D., & Segal, J. ( 1 99 1 ). Informal reasoning and
Thompson, R.F. ( 1 986). The neurobiology of learning and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
memory. Science, 233, 94 1-947. Wason, P.C. ( 1 960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a
Tomlin, R., & Villa, V. ( 1 994). Attention in cognitive science and conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1 6, 1 85-204. Pschology, 12, 1 29- 1 40.
Towell, R. & Hawkins, R. ( 1 994 ). Approaches to second language Watanabe, L. ( 1 980). Selective listening and attention. Japanese
acquisition. Clevedon, Avon: Multilingual Matters. Psychological Review, 23, 3 3 5-3 54.
Trahey, M., & White. L. ( 1 993). Positive evidence and preemption Watanabe, Y. ( 1 992). Effects of increased processing on incidental
in the second language classroom. Studies in Second learning offoriegn language vocabulary. Unpublished M.A.
Language Acquisition, 15, 1 8 1 -204. thesis, University of Hawai'i at Manoa, Department o f
Tulving, E. ( 1 984). Precis of 'Elements of episodic memory. ' English as a Second Language.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 223-238. Wellman, H.M. ( 1 990). The child 's theory of mind. Cambridge,
__ . ( 1 985). How many memory systems are there? A merican MA: MIT Press.
Psychologist, 4, 3 8 5-3 98. Wesche, M. ( 1 9 8 1 ). Language aptitude measures in streaming:
__ . (1 986). What kind of hypothesis is the distinction between matching students with measures and diagnosis of learning
episodic and semantic memory? Journal of Experimental problems. In K. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and
. Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 307-3 1 1 . universals in language learning aptitude, pp . 1 1 9- 1 5 4.
Tulving, E., Schacter, P., & Stark, H. ( 1 982). Priming effects in Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
word fragment completion are independent of recognition Westney, P. ( 1 994). Rules and pedagogic grammar. In T. Odlin
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, (Ed.), Perspectives on pedagogical grammar, pp. 72-96. New
Memory and Cognition, 8, 336-342. York: Cambridge University Press.
Turner, C.W., & Fischler, LS. ( 1 993 ). Speeded tests of implicit White, L. ( 1 987). Markedness and second language acquisition. The
knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, question of transfer. Studies In Second Language Acquisition,
Memory and Cognition, 19, 1 1 65- 1 1 77. 9, 26 1-285.
Vallar, G., & Shallice, T. ( 1 990). Cognitive impairments of short­ . (l 989a). Universal grammar and second language
term memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. -- acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
VanPatten, B. ( 1 988). How juries get hung: Problems with the . ( 1 989b). Linguistic universals, markedness and learnability:
evidence for a focus on form in teaching. Language Learning, -- Reconciling different approaches. Second Language
38, 243-260. Research, 5, 1 27-1 40.
__ . ( 1 990). Attending to form or meaning in the input. Studies __ . ( 1 990). Second language acquisition and universal grammar.
in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 287-3 0 1 . Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 1 2 1 - 1 3 3 .
288 Bibliography

__ . ( 1 9 9 1 ). Adverb placement in second language acquisition:


Some effects of positive and negative evidence in the
classro om. Second Language Research, 7, 1 22-1 6 1 . S ubject Index
__ . ( 1 992). On triggering data in L2 acquisition: A reply t o
Schwartz and Gubala-Ryzak . Second Language Research, 8,
1 2 0- 1 3 7 .
White, L., Spada, N., Lightbown, P.M., & Ranta, L. ( 1 99 1 ). Input
enhancement and L2 question format ion. Applied Linguistics,
12, 4 1 6-432. Anderson's ACT* Theory of rule-based models, 99-
White, R. ( 1 988). The ELT curriculum: Design, management and skill acquisition, 52, 66, 1 0 1 , 207
innovation. Oxford : Basil Blackwell. 72-73 prototypes and, 99-1 00,
Whittlesea, B.W.A ., & Dorken, M.D. ( 1 993). Incidentally, things aptitude, 66, 77-79, 1 1 1 , 1 1 5- 202-203
in general are particul arly determined: An episodic-processing 1 1 6, 1 45-1 5 1 , 1 73-1 77 connectionism, 24, 5 3 , 5 8, 1 00-
account of implicit learning. Journal of Experimental and learning under 101
Psychology: General, 122, 227-24 8. different conditions, 1 70- consciousness, 1 , 9-1 4, 5 1-79
Wickens, C.D. ( 1 980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. 173, 1 77 as awareness, 9-1 0, 2 1 ,
Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII, pp .23 9- and awareness under 5 1 -75, 1 70-1 77, 1 79-1 84
257. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. different conditions, 1 73- as explicit knowledge, 1 0-
( 1 984). Processing resources in attentio n. In R. 1 77 1 1 , 26-27, 1 02-1 04
Parasuraman & D. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention, articulable knowledge, 1 0, 34- as intention, 9-1 0, 2 1
pp.63-1 02. New York: Academic Press. 3 5 , 9 1 , 1 4 1-143, 1 73-174, cross-modal transfer and
__ . ( 1 989). Attention and skilled performance. In D. Holding 1 80-1 82 learning, 208
(Ed.), Human skills, pp. 7 1 - 1 0 5 . New York, London: John artificial grammars, 6, 14, 1 5 ,
Wiley. 30, 82-87, 1 53 , 1 83-1 84, detection, 9, 53-54, 5 8-75
Yip, V. (1 994). Grammatical consciousness-raising and learnabi lity. 1 93 developmental stage, 3, 3 4, 49,
In T. Odlin (Ed.), Perspectives on pedagogical gramm ar, attention, 9-1 2 , 26, 5 1-75 , 78- 200
pp. 1 23-1 78. New York: Cambridge University Press. 79, 1 98, 202
Zalewski, J.P. ( 1 993). Number/person errors in an information as a filter, 53-54 effect of instruction studies, 5-
processing perspective: Implications for form-focused as capacity, 54-57 6, 1 99-200
instruction. TESOL Quarterly, 2 7, 69 1-704. short-term memory and,
Zobl, H. (1 980). The formal and developmental selectivity of L 1 57-67, 74-75 focus on form, 1, 2-5, 3 1, 1 99-
influence on L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 3 0, 43-57. automaticity, 1 0-12, 66--68, 69- 200
. ( 1 983). Markedness and the projection problem. Language
·
70, 74, 207 Fundamental Difference
Learning, 33, 292-3 1 3 . Hypothesis of adult second
. ( 1 985). Grammars in search o f input and intake. In S . Gass Bangalore Procedural Syllabus, language learning, 1 04-
-- & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition, 9, 19, 25-26 1 05
pp.329-344. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Bialystok's Analysis/Control Fundamental Similarity
. ( 1 992). Sources of linguistic knowledge and uniformity o f Model of SLA, 1 1-12, 25 Hypothesis of
-- non-native performance. Studies in Second Language implicit/explicit second
A cquisition, 1 4, 3 8 7-403. categorization, 99-102 language learning,
__. ( 1 995). Converging evidence for the acquisition/learning exemplar-based models, 1 03-1 05, 1 55, 1 75-1 98
distinction. Applied Linguistics, 1 6, 3 5-5 7. 1 0 1-1 02, 207
Subject index 29 1
290 Subject index

of gerundivization, 3 9-41
hypothesis-testing, markedness, 3, 5, 33-34, 35, 5 5, of subjacency, 40-41
and confirmation bias, I 03, 200 of third-person agreement,
1 9 1- 1 92 maturational constraints, 1 3 , 3 1, 3 8
and overgeneralization, 1 04-1 05 o f wh-question formation,
1 92, 1 98 McLaughlin's Information
32, 33-34, 3 9-4 1
Processing Model of SLA,
implicit knowledge, 1 0-12, 24- 1 0, 1 1-12, 52, 66
26, 32, 33, 3 5 , 98- 1 03, memory systems, 52-53, 64-78 task-based language teaching,
1 92- 197, 200 20, 3 1 , 53, 5 7
implicit learning, 1 0-12, 1 3-14, Natural Approach, 8-9, 1 9, 78 teacher knowledge, 36-38, 42-
29, 3 0, 8 1-92, 125-1 40, negative evidence, 3, 4, 6, 207 44
1 53-1 79, 1 92-1 97, 1 99- noticing, 1 -2, 9-1 0, 27, 5 1 , 5 7-
20 1 , 208 Universal Grammar,
64, 77-79, 1 05, 1 4 1 , 1 55 ,
implicit memory, 65, 68-75, 1 70-1 77, 1 82-1 83, 1 98- accessibility of, 4, 29, 33,
92-98, 208 1 99 200-20 1
information processing, Noun Phrase Accessibility and explicit learning, 20 1
and the implicit/explicit Hierarchy, 6, 1 7 and implicit knowledge,
distinction, 5 1-75, 79 1 0, 1 00-1 02
individual differences in perceptual salience, 33-34, 3 5 and parameter-setting, 3,
resources and, 75-79 physicalism, 24 24, 1 0 1 , 200
input-enhancement, I , 3 1 , 207 positive evidence, 4, 6, 207 and triggering, 3, 24, 1 0 1 ,
200
priming, 1 , 68-69, 78, 95, 96
Kahneman's Capacity Model of
attention, 54-56 rules, Wickens' Multiple Resource
Krashen's Monitor Model of and connectionism, 24, 32, Model of attention, 52-59,
SLA, I , 7-1 0, 26, 3 1-33, 1 00-1 02 76
64, 1 08-1 09, 1 84-1 85, and rule understanding, 9,
204-205 1 0, 25, 26-27
and rules of thumb, 27, 49,
learning conditions, 20 1
implicit, 6, 1 7- 1 9, 88, 1 1 7, complexity of second
1 92-1 94, 204 language, 1 , 20, 28-49,
incidental, 6, 9, 1 6-19, 108- 1 1 0, 1 2 1-123, 200-
1 1 7- 1 1 8, 1 82-1 83, 195- 202
1 96, 204 familiarity of pedagogic,
instructed, 6, 1 6- 1 9, 2 1 , 3 8-42
89, 1 1 8-1 1 9, 1 90-1 92, 204 in linguistic theory, 23-24,
rule-search, 2 1 , 88, 1 1 8, 32-33, 36, 1 00-1 02
1 97- 1 98, 204 in second language
Logan's Instance Theory of pedagogy, 23-49, 200
automaticity, 1 0-1 1 , 66- of article use, 3 1 , 32, 3 5
68, 69-70, 74, 207 of focus, 39-4 1 , 44-49

S-ar putea să vă placă și