Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/325808795

Perceived inequity predicts prejudice towards age-gap relationships

Article  in  Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.) · June 2018


DOI: 10.1007/s12144-018-9895-6

CITATIONS READS

0 250

2 authors, including:

Brian Collisson
Azusa Pacific University
31 PUBLICATIONS   187 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Brian Collisson on 12 September 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Current Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9895-6

Perceived inequity predicts prejudice towards age-gap relationships


Brian Collisson 1 & Luciana Ponce De Leon 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Age-gap couples often elicit negative stereotypes and prejudice. According to social exchange and equity theories, we predicted
that prejudice towards age-gap couples may stem from perceived relational inequity. We hypothesized that age-gap, as compared
to age-matched, couples were perceived as less equitable and, as a result, less liked. To test these hypotheses, people evaluated,
and inferred the equity of, age-gap and age-matched relationships. We found that age-gap, as compared to age-matched, couples
were more disliked and perceived as less equitable. Within age-gap relationships, older men and women were perceived as
reaping greater rewards than their younger partners. Importantly, perceived inequity predicted prejudice towards age-gap, but not
age-matched, couples. In exploratory analyses, age-gap couples consistently elicited significantly more prejudice than other types
of couples. Implications for age-gap relationships and future research are discussed.

Keywords Romantic relationship . Age . Prejudice . Age-gap . Cougar . Cradle robber

Within romantic relationships, age is more than a number. similar partners are more likely to form and maintain relation-
Couples who differ in age - commonly referred to as age-gap ships than dissimilar partners (Berscheid et al. 1971; Bryne
or May–December relationships - often elicit negative stereo- and Nelson 1964; Crow and Felsenstein 1968). Certainly, cul-
types and prejudice (Banks and Arnold 2001; Lehmiller and tural differences exist regarding acceptable age gaps within
Agnew 2011). For example, people often denigrate those who relationships (United Nations 2000). Nonetheless, within
date someone much younger or older than themselves by call- North America, the average age difference among married
ing them Bcougars^ and Bcradle robbers,^ or Bgold-diggers^ couples is small (2.30 years; United Nations 2000). Within
and Bsugar daddies/mommas.^ Such negative stereotypes sug- the United States, 60% of married couples have age differ-
gest that age-gap relationships are inequitable, with the one ences less than three years; 92% less than 10 years (U.S.
partner benefitting more than the younger partner. Indeed, Census Bureau 1999). Thus, it appears large age-gaps within
Bcougar^ implies that older women may be reaping greater romantic relationships are atypical.
rewards by predatorily preying upon younger men. Likewise, Evolutionary theory argues that heterosexual men should
Bcradle robber^ implies that older men are thieves, benefiting prefer partners slightly younger than themselves and women
by taking younger, more valuable women to whom they may should prefer partners slightly older than themselves (Buss
not have access. Terms, such as Bgold-digger^ and Bsugar dad- 1989; Kenrick et al. 1996; Kenrick and Keefe 1992). Such
dy/momma^ imply that the younger partner is taking advantage small age differences, favoring slightly older males, maximize
of the older partner’s financial resources. reproductive fitness and long-term resource attainment for men
In these instances, the younger or older partner within an and women (see Kenrick et al. 1996). Larger age-gaps, espe-
age-gap relationship may aggressively pursue an age- cially those containing older women, violate evolved mate pref-
discrepant partner because such relationships are unlikely to erences. To the extent to which people dislike evolutionarily
form otherwise. Research on assortative mating suggests disadvantageous relationships, people should express prejudice
towards age-gap couples (Banks and Arnold 2001).
Surprisingly, and unfortunately, few studies have assessed
* Brian Collisson age-gap relationships (e.g., Banks and Arnold 2001; Kenrick
bcollisson@apu.edu and Keefe 1992; Lehmiller and Agnew 2006, 2008) and even
fewer have asked why people dislike age-gap couples. Indeed,
1
Department of Psychology, Azusa Pacific University, 901 E. Alosta a chapter reviewing the potential reasons why people would
Ave, Azusa, CA 91702, USA form age-gap relationships relied mostly on theoretical
Curr Psychol

speculation because there are so few studies on the topic investment, and vitality to a relationship (Buss 1989; Kenrick
(Lehmiller and Agnew 2011). Only one qualitative study ma- et al. 1996; Kenrick and Keefe 1992). Indeed, age-gap relation-
nipulated the age-gap within couples (Banks and Arnold ships may only form when older partners provide similarly
2001). Its findings suggest that people think age-gap couples positive and equitable contributions, such as wisdom, maturity,
are socially unacceptable, less satisfying, short-lived, and or financial resources (Lehmiller and Agnew 2011). If people
overly maternal or paternal (Banks and Arnold 2001). This stereotypically infer that older partners are contributing less to
single study’s findings also revealed that people believe larger age-gap relationships than their younger counterparts, then peo-
age-gaps are related to a greater emphasis on an older partner’s ple may dislike age-gap couples.
resources. That is, people infer younger men and women date
older partners for financial benefit (i.e., in search of a sugar
mama or sugar daddy; Banks and Arnold 2001). These find- Current Research and Hypotheses
ings fit evolutionary theory and imply that people do not typ-
ically form age-gap relationships because of a perceived im- To test whether perceived inequity predicts prejudice towards
balance or inequity within the relationship. age-gap relationships, we asked people to evaluate the rela-
Given the lack of research on people’s prejudicial attitudes tionships of romantic couples with similar (age-matched) and
towards age-gap relationships, the current study seeks to dissimilar (age-gap) ages. Specifically, we asked people to
quantitatively measure people’s prejudice and determine express the degree to which they liked age-gap and age-
whether such prejudice is related to perceived inequity within matched couples and the degree to which one partner was
the relationship. In an exploratory fashion, we also compare benefitting more than the other. We first predicted that age-
whether people’s own age or gender may moderate their prej- gap couples would elicit greater prejudice than age-matched
udicial attitudes towards age-gap couples. To better under- couples. Second, we predicted that age-gap couples would be
stand the extent of people’s prejudice towards relationships, perceived as less equitable than age-matched couples, such
in general, we explore how the prejudice elicited by age-gap that older partners reaped greater rewards within age-gap re-
couples compares to that of other marginalized couples, such lationships. Third, and most importantly, we predicted that
as those who differ in race, weight, and socioeconomic status. perceived inequity would predict prejudice towards age-gap,
but not age-matched, couples. Age-matched couples offer
similar contributions to their relationships. Therefore, any
Theoretical Framework prejudicial attitudes elicited by age-matched couples should
be unrelated to perceived inequity.
Drawing upon social exchange and equity theories, we pro- In an exploratory fashion, we also explored the degree to
pose that people may express prejudice towards age-gap cou- which age and gender may moderate people’s prejudicial atti-
ples because they are perceived as inequitable. According to tudes towards age-gap couples. Furthermore, we compared the
social exchange theory (Thibaut and Kelley 1959), people level of prejudice elicited by age-gap couples to other margin-
view romantic relationships much like behavioral economists alized couples, specifically those who differ in regard to race,
view business exchanges - as cost/benefit analyses. A roman- weight, and socioeconomic status. Although we do not make
tic relationship is a series of transactions of valued resources. explicit predictions about gender or age differences, nor about
Relationships which accrue greater rewards (e.g., feelings of the comparative prejudice levels among couples, such explor-
satisfaction, meaningfulness) than costs (e.g., conflict, emo- atory analyses may reveal meaningful avenues for future re-
tional baggage) are evaluated positively and are likely to per- search and, potentially, lead to future theoretical developments.
sist (Nakonezny and Denton 2008). Furthermore, people are
likely to compare their contributions to a relationship with
their partner’s contributions (Festinger 1954; Markey and Method
Markey 2011). According to equity theory, relationships are
less positive if one partner believes he or she is contributing Participants
much more, or less, than the other (Adams 1963). Therefore,
people should evaluate equitable relationships, in which both Ninety-nine participants (Mage = 29.49; SD = 7.23) were re-
partners contribute and reap equally positive rewards, more cruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) and paid
favorably than inequitable relationships. US$0.26 as remuneration. MTurk is a public website where
Age-gap relationships, in particular, may stereotypically be participants can voluntarily agree to participate in psychology
viewed as one-sided. The relational attractiveness and per- studies for a small payment credited to their Amazon.com
ceived mate-value of younger partners is higher than that of account. Researchers in the social sciences often choose to
older partners (Edlund and Sagarin 2010). Younger partners, collect data from MTurk participants, rather than traditional
presumably, provide greater reproductive fitness, long-term undergraduate students, because MTurk participants tend to
Curr Psychol

be a more diverse and representative population (Buhrmester, couples. Participants indicated the degree to which they felt
Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). favorable towards each relationship using a feeling thermom-
The online sample consisted of 62 males, 35 females, and 2 eter bar, which slid from 0 (cold, unfavorable) to 99 (warm/
participants choosing not to report a gender. Participants iden- favorable).
tified as Caucasian (50%), Asian (37%), African American
(7%), and Hispanic (4%). Participants ages ranged from 19 Perceived Inequity Participants inferred the extent to
to 63 years. which each relationship was inequitable using a 7-
point scale, ranging from 1 (He is getting a much better
Materials and Procedure deal than she is) to 7 (She is getting a much better deal
than he is). The midpoint of the scale was labeled 4
Participants were presented with a BSocial Attitudes Survey,^ (They are both getting the same, equal deal).
which they were told would assess their feelings towards a
variety of different social topics and groups. At the beginning
of the survey, participants were presented with an Informed Results
Consent Form. After consenting, participants were presented
with a total of 16 relationships which varied in regard to race Do People Express Prejudice towards Age-Gap
(i.e., White/Black man and a White/Black/woman), weight Couples?
(i.e., skinny/fat man and a skinny/fat woman) socioeconomic
status (i.e., a rich/poor man and a rich/poor woman), and most To assess whether prejudice varied as a function of each part-
importantly, age (i.e., a young/old man and a young/old wom- ner’s age, we conducted a 2 (Men’s age: young, old) X 2
an). Instead of specifying a numerical age-gap (see Banks and (Women’s age: young, old) repeated measures ANOVA.
Arnold 2001), we chose to use the colloquial wording of There was a significant main effect of men’s age, F(1,96) =
Byoung^ and Bold^ to mirror colloquial wording of other cou- 6.03, p < .05, partial η2 = .06, such that relationships of older
ples (e.g., White/Black, skinny/fat, rich/poor). For each couple, men were liked less than relationships of younger men. There
participants expressed their prejudicial attitudes and perceived was no main effect of women’s age, F(1,96) = 1.07, p = .30,
inequity (see below). They then completed basic demographic partial η2 = .01. As predicted, prejudice significantly varied as
items, were thoroughly debriefed, and remunerated. a function of men and women’s age, F(1,96) = 73.11, p < .001,
partial η2 = .43. As seen in Fig. 1, people expressed greater
Prejudicial Attitudes The primary dependent variable was par- prejudice towards young men’s relationships when they
ticipants’ evaluations of randomly presented romantic partnered with older (M = 55.45, SD = 32.60), rather than

Fig. 1 Prejudice as a function of Prejudice as a function of men’s and women’s ages.


men’s and women’s ages. Note.
Higher scores indicate more 100
Young man
favorable attitudes. Error bars
90 Old man
represent 95% confidence
intervals 80

70
Rating of Couple

60

50

40

30

20

10

0
Young woman O ld w oman
Woman’s Age
Note. Higher scores indicate more favorable attitudes. Error bars represent 95% confidence
intervals.
Curr Psychol

younger women (M = 81.95, SD = 20.17), t(97) = −7.45, p the zero-order correlations between inequity and prejudice for
< .001, d = .97.1 Similarly, people expressed greater prejudice the two, specific age-gap couples (i.e., young man/older wom-
towards older men’s relationships when they partnered with an, old man/young woman) and the two, specific age-matched
younger (M = 50.70, SD = 33.04), rather than older women couples (i.e., young man/young woman, old man/old woman).
(M = 80.08, SD = 22.45), t(97) = −8.34, p < .001, d = 1.04. For age-gap couples, the relation between inequity and preju-
Planned contrasts revealed that, in general, age-gap couples dice was surprisingly not significant for young man/older
(M = 52.84, SD = 31.36) were liked significantly less than woman relationships, r(97) = .11, p = .28; as expected, the re-
age-matched couples (M = 80.70, SD = 19.19), t(97) = lation between inequity and prejudice was significant for older
−8.61, p < .001, d = 1.07. man/young woman relationships, r(97) = .33, p < .001. Such
that, the more people perceived the old man was getting a
Are Age-Gap Relationships Inequitable? better deal than the young woman, the more prejudice they
expressed towards the relationship. For age-matched couples,
To assess whether perceived inequity varied as a function of the relation between inequity and prejudice was not significant
each partner’s age, we again conducted a 2 (Men’s age: young, for young man/young woman relationships, r(97) = .16,
old) X 2 (Women’s age: young, old) repeated measures p = .11 and for old man/old woman relationships,
ANOVA. There was a significant main effect of men’s age, r(97) = .08, p = .45 (Table 1).
F(1,96) = 33.70, p < .001, partial η2 = .26, such that older men To assess whether perceived inequity predicted prej-
were perceived to reap greater rewards within relationships udice towards age-gap couples, we first created a single
than younger men. Similarly, there was a significant main measure of perceived inequity in age-gap versus age-
effect of women’s age, F(1,96) = 21.87, p < .001, partial matched relationships. To do this, we subtracted each
η2 = .19, such that older women were perceived to reap greater inequity item from the midpoint of the scale (i.e.,
rewards within relationship than younger women. There was They’re each getting the same, equal deal), saving its
also a significant interaction between men and women’s age, absolute value. Thus, larger scores indicated greater in-
F(1,96) = 8.67, p < .01, partial η2 = .08. equity. We then averaged the inequity scores for the two
As seen in Fig. 2, people perceived greater inequity within age-gap items (i.e., younger man/older woman, older
older men’s relationships when they partnered with younger man/younger woman) together, creating a single inequi-
(M = 3.44, SD = 1.91), rather than older women (M = 4.44, ty score for age-gap relationships. We did the same for
SD = 1.08), t(97) = 5.70, p < .001, d = .65. Such that, older the two age-matched items.3 Finally, we regressed the
men were perceived to benefit more when they partnered with inequity score for age-matched relationships onto the
younger women than older women.2 Similarly, people per- inequity score for age-gap relationships, saving the un-
ceived significantly greater inequity within younger men’s standardized residuals. Thus, we created a single item
relationships when they partnered with older (M = 4.95, which captured the unique variance of the extent to
SD = 1.60), rather than younger women (M = 4.58, which people perceive age-gap relationships as less eq-
SD = 1.06), t(96) = −2.02, p < .05, d = 0.27. Planned contrasts uitable than age-matched relationships. Afterward, we
revealed that, in general, age-gap couples (M = 52.84, created a measure of age-gap prejudice by averaging
SD = 31.36) were liked significantly less than age-matched the two prejudice ratings for age-gap couples (younger
couples (M = 80.70, SD = 19.19), t(97) = −8.61, p < .001, man/older woman, older man/younger woman). We did
d = 1.07. the same to create a measure of age-matched prejudice
as well.
Does Perceived Inequity Predict Prejudice towards Two linear regression analyses were then used to test wheth-
Age-Gap Couples? er perceived inequity predicted age-gap and age-matched

3
Before testing whether perceived inequity predicts prejudice For this direct test of our hypothesis, we needed to create a combined per-
ceived inequity for age-gap couples (i.e., older man/younger woman, younger
towards age-gap and age-matched couples, we first explored man/older woman) and age-matched couples (i.e., young man/young woman,
old man/old woman), respectively. Because the inequity variable was scored
1
It is important to note that participants’ prejudice was directed towards the from 1 (He’s getting a better deal) to 7 (She’s getting a better deal), we could
couple, rather than the man or woman specifically. Therefore, the interaction not simply average these variables together because any perceived inequity
results listed previously could also be written with a focus on women (e.g., among age-gap couples (older man/younger woman, younger man/older
Bpeople expressed greater prejudice towards older women’s relationships woman) would not be captured by an average score. That is, if someone
when they partner with younger, rather than older, men.^) perceived a great deal of inequity among the age-gap older man relationship
2
This interaction could also be written with a focus on women (e.g., Bpeople (score of 1 - He’s getting a much better deal) and age-gap younger man
perceived greater inequity within younger, rather than older, women’s relation- relationship (score of 7 – She’s getting a much better deal), the average (7 +
ships when they partnered with an older man. Such that younger women were 1)/2 would incorrectly reveal 4 (They’re each getting the same equal deal).
perceived to benefit less than older women when they partnered with an older Therefore, we needed to take the absolute value of each score from the mid-
man.^) point of 4 before averaging the scores.
Curr Psychol

Fig. 2 Perceived inequity as a 7


function of men’s and women’s
ages. Note. Error bars represent Young man Old man
95% confidence intervals 6

1
Young woman Old woman

prejudice, respectively. As predicted, perceived inequity pre- nor a young man/old woman couple, t(95) = 0.10, p = .92,
dicted age-gap prejudice, F(1,96) = 7.28, p < .01, r2 = .07, such d = 0.02.
that greater perceived inequity predicted more negative atti-
tudes towards age-gap couples (β = −.27, p < .01). However, Comparisons among Couple Types To explore how the level
and as predicted, perceived inequity did not predict age- of prejudice elicited by age-gap couples compared to other
matched prejudice, F(1,96) = 0.03, p = .87, r2 < .01. dissimilar couples (i.e., couples who differed in race, weight,
and socioeconomic status), we first created a total prejudice
score for each couple by averaging participants’ ratings of
Exploratory Analyses each dissimilar couple (e.g., old man/young woman, young
man/old woman; White man/Black woman, Black man/White
Age To explore whether participants’ own age was related to woman). The newly created prejudice scores for age-gap
their prejudicial attitudes towards age-gap couples, we corre- (α=.91), mixed-weight (α=.88), interracial (α=.76), and
lated participants’ age with their rating of the two age-gap mixed-socioeconomic status (α=.82) couples each demon-
couples (i.e., old man/young woman couple, young man/old strated acceptable reliability.
woman couple). Age was not significantly related to the prej- We then used a repeated-measures, one-way ANOVA to
udice elicited by an old man/young woman couple, r (97) = test whether people’s prejudice scores varied by couple type.
−.06, p = .59, nor by a young man/old woman couple, r (97) = Interestingly, prejudice significantly depended on the type of
−.08, p = .41. Therefore, we did not continue further, explor- couple, F(3, 94) = 19.85, p < .001, ω2 = .33. As seen in Fig. 3
atory regression analyses because age was uncorrelated with and follow-up Helmert contrasts, people expressed signifi-
either variable. cantly more negative attitudes towards age-gap couples than
all other couples, F(1,96) = 51.15, p < .001, ω2 = .50.
Gender To explore whether participants’ own gender related
to their prejudicial attitudes towards age-gap couples, we con-
ducted independent samples t-tests, with gender as the quasi- Discussion
independent variable. Men and women did not significantly
differ in regard to their prejudicial attitudes towards an old In sum, our hypotheses were largely confirmed. People
man/young woman couple, t(94) = −0.14, p = .89, d = 0.03, expressed greater prejudice towards age-gap couples than

Table 1 Summary of regression


results testing whether perceived Criterion variable B SE (B) β t p r2
inequity predicts prejudice
Prejudice towards age-gap relationships −8.71 3.23 −.27 17.21 <.01* .07
Prejudice towards age-matched relationships 0.34 2.05 .02 0.16 .87 <.01
Curr Psychol

Fig. 3 Prejudice towards Prejudice towards dissimilar couples as a function of couple type.
dissimilar couples as a function of
100
couple type. Note. Higher scores
indicate more favorable attitudes. 90
Error bars represent 95%
confidence intervals 80

70

Rating of Couple
60

50

40

30

20

10

0
Age-gap Mixed-Weight Interracial Mixed-SES

Couple Type
Note. Higher scores indicate more favorable attitudes. Error bars represent 95% confidence
intervals.

age-matched couples. People also perceived greater inequity Although exploratory analyses are interesting and poten-
among age-gap couples, such that they perceived that older tially meaningful for future theory development, exploratory
partners reap greater rewards than their younger partners. findings should be interpreted cautiously. They were not pre-
Furthermore, perceived inequity predicted prejudice towards dicted and are currently not supported by theory. For instance,
age-gap, but not age-matched, couples. That is, the more one- it is possible that people may perceive that age-gap couples are
sided people perceived age-gap relationships to be, the more more inequitable than other types of couples and therefore,
prejudice they expressed. elicit greater prejudice. It is also possible that people perceive
However, it is also important to note that for both types of age-related prejudice as more socially acceptable than race or
age-gap couples (i.e., old man/young woman, young man/old weight-related prejudice and therefore, feel more comfortable
woman), people expressed greater prejudice and perceived rating age-gap couples negatively. Replicating and explaining
greater inequity than they did for age-matched couples (i.e., why age-gap couples elicit greater prejudice than other cou-
young man, young woman, old man/old woman). However, ples may be a fruitful avenue for future research.
the relation between perceived inequity and prejudice was
greater for old man/young woman couples than young man/ Implications
old woman. Although this null effect should be interpreted
cautiously, it does suggest that the degree to which people These findings may have implications for people currently
perceive certain relationships as inequitable may better predict within, or who may later form, age-gap relationships. For those
prejudice towards some couples more than others. currently in age-gap relationships, people’s perceptions of in-
Furthermore, our exploratory analyses found that partici- equity and corresponding prejudice may stigmatize the couple
pants own age and gender did not moderate their prejudice and possibly lead to relationship dissolution. Indeed, age-gap
towards age-gap couples. However, exploratory analyses did couples tend to be less committed to their relationships than
reveal differences in the amount of prejudice elicited by dif- non-stigmatized couples (Lehmiller and Agnew 2006, 2008).
ferent types of couples (e.g., those who differ in race, weight, Future research is needed to more clearly identify how preju-
or finances). Interestingly, people evaluated age-gap couples dice towards age-gap relationships may lead to increased con-
less favorably than interracial, mixed-weight, or mixed- flict, dissatisfaction, and possibly relationship dissolution.
socioeconomic status couples. Because this finding was not Future research studies may also explore whether age-gap,
predicted, it should be interpreted cautiously. Nonetheless, it and other marginalized, couples’ lower levels of commitment
suggests the need for further research on age-gap couples, in to their relationships reflect societal disapproval and stigma or
general, and the potential reasons why age-gap couples might true differences in inequity. More research is needed to survey
elicit greater prejudice than other types of couples, actual age-gap couples and determine the extent to which one
specifically. partner contributes more, or less, than the other. If people’s
Curr Psychol

stereotypic perceptions of inequity are accurate, then actual using other, more naturalistic paradigms. Indeed, viewing a
age-gap couples may be less committed than other couples picture or video of an age-gap couple may be another way to
because of their inequitable investment. Certainly, more re- assess people’s perceived inequity and prejudicial attitudes. In
search is needed to explore the commitment experienced with- the current study, it may have been difficult for participants to
in age-gap relationships. imagine age-gap couples given its hypothetical and generic
For those who may later form age-gap relationships, a wording. In more realistic scenarios, people may have more
younger partner may stereotypically infer that the older part- information of the couple to base their level of prejudice and
ner contributes more to the relationship as an effort to equalize perceptions of equity. When this information is lacking, it
the partnership. Similarly, it is possibly that the partner per- appears that people generally express prejudice towards age-
ceived to benefit more may feel pressure to highlight the re- gap couples and perceive them as inequitable.
wards he or she may bring to a relationship to provide equity. Furthermore, people’s prejudicial attitudes towards age-
For instance, older partners may highlight their wealth or gap couples were limited to response scales. Future research
wisdom to attract younger partners and combat perceived in- may choose to assess prejudice and potential discrimination
equity inferences from others (e.g., friends and family). towards age-gap couples in more ecologically valid, real
Conversely, people may stereotypically infer that the youn- world situations, such as dating scenes or marriage venues.
ger partner in an age-gap relationship contributes more than Additionally, the findings were limited in their experimental
his or her partner. It is possible that this inference may pressure realism. For instance, asking participants to rate hypothetical
younger partners to downplay his or her contributions to the couples not reflect the true feelings people may experience
relationship and thus avoid perceived inequity from others. when witnessing such couples. Future studies may ethically
Future studies which survey the experiences of actual couples replicate and extend the current research in more natural
within age-gap relationships seems like a logical extension of settings.
the current research. In addition, the current research offered a more general
Furthermore, the current research may also have implica- description of age-gaps (e.g., a young person dating an old
tions for later stigma-reduction interventions. If perceived in- person). Future studies may choose to use more specific inter-
equity underlies people’s prejudice towards age-gap couples, val ranges (e.g., dating someone 5, 10, 15 years younger/
then future studies which manipulate perceived equity may older). More specific age ranges would allow researchers to
find decreases in prejudice. Indeed, other studies regarding test whether the specific age range affects people’s prejudice
Bmismatched^ couples, such as interracial relationships and perceptions of equity.
(Miller et al. 2004a, b) and mixed-weight relationships Another limitation of the current research is in regard to its
(Collisson et al. 2016) show that perceived inequity is indeed online sample of participants. Although Amazon’s
related to people’s attitudes towards couples. It is possible that Mechanical Turk allows researchers to recruit a significantly
highlighting equity among age-gap couples, and other dissim- more diverse and representative sample than traditional col-
ilar couples, may reduce prejudice. lege students (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011), it still is
not a fully representative sample. Online samples tend to be
Strengths & Limitations more educated and participate for intrinsically motivating rea-
sons, such as enjoyment of research. Future studies may
The current research has many strengths, as well as limita- choose to selectively recruit a representative sample which
tions. In regard to strengths, first, the current research repli- varies more widely in age, education, and ethnicity to more
cates and extends the only study which qualitatively assessed aptly test whether participants’ own demographic variables
age-gap prejudice (Banks and Arnold 2001) and provides the- relate to their perceptions of age-gap couples. It may be pos-
oretical support for the role of equity within age-gap relation- sible, for instance, that younger people may give greater
ships (Lehmiller and Agnew 2011). Second, the current re- weight to appearance and vitality; whereas, older people
search draws upon social exchange and relationship theories may give greater weight to financial stability and life manage-
to offer an empirically supported explanation regarding why ment skills.
people may dislike age-gap relationships. Indeed, it bridges
romantic relationship and prejudice literatures in a novel and Conclusion
theoretically meaningful way. Third, the current research
shows the relationship between perceived inequity and preju- Within age-gap relationships, partners are often derogatorily
dice towards age-gap couples in a hypothetical context. referred to Bcougars,^ Bcradle robbers,^ Bgold diggers,^ and
Indeed, people may describe unknown couples in such generic Bsugar daddies/mommas.^ Such negative stereotypes pre-
terms, such as the Byoung man^ dating the Bolder woman.^ sume that the relationship is inequitable, such that one partner
Regardless, given psychology’s concern about replicability is predatorily preying upon, stealing away, or financially
(see Klein et al., 2014), these findings would be replicated benefiting from the other. In the current study, we provided
Curr Psychol

evidence that people perceive greater inequity, and express Relationships. First published online April, 27, 2016–2540. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0265407516644067.
greater prejudice towards, age-gap couples than age-matched
Crow, J. F., & Felsenstein, J. (1968). The effect of assortative mating on
couples. Furthermore, perceived inequity positively predicted the genetic composition of a population. Eugenics Quarterly, 15,
prejudice towards age-gap couples. In sum, the more people 85–97.
believe older partners reap greater rewards than younger part- Edlund, J. E., & Sagarin, B. J. (2010). Mate value and mate preferences:
An investigation into decisions made with and without constraints.
ners within age-gap relationships, the more prejudice they
Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 835–839. https://doi.
express towards age-gap couples. org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.004.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Acknowledgements We acknowledge and greatly appreciate the contri- Relations, 7, 117–140.
butions of my research team: Juliana D’Aoust, Camille Brandon, Angele Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect
Doakes, Gabriel Lee, Marlene Martinez, and Crystal Saludes. sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 15, 75–133.
Kenrick, D. T., Gabrielidis, C., Keefe, R. C., & Cornelius, J. S. (1996).
Compliance with Ethical Standards Adolescents’ age preferences for dating partners: Support for an
evolutionary model of life-history strategies. Child Development,
Ethical Approval All applicable international, national, and/or institu- 67, 1499–1511.
tional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. All Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Jr., Bahník, Š.,
procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in Bernstein, M. J., et al. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability:
accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national A Bmany labs^ replication project. Social Psychology, 45(3), 142–
research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later 152. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000178.
amendments or comparable ethical standards. Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2006). Marginalized relationships: The
impact of social disapproval on romantic relationship commitment.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 40–51. https://doi.
participants included in the study. org/10.1177/0146167205278710.
Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2008). Commitment in age-gap hetero-
Conflict of Interest The author has not received research grants to sup- sexual romantic relationships: A test of evolutionary and socio-
port this research nor has any conflict of interests. cultural predictions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 74–82.
Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2011). May-December paradoxes: An
exploration of age-gap relationships in western society. In W. R.
Cupach, B. H. Spitzberg, W. R. Cupach, B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.),
References The dark side of close relationships II (pp. 39–61). New York:
Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Markey, C., & Markey, P. (2011). Romantic partners, weight status, and
Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity. Journal of
weight concerns an examination using the actor-partner interdepen-
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 422–436.
dence model. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 217–225. https://
Banks, C. A., & Arnold, P. (2001). Opinions towards sexual partners with
doi.org/10.1177/1359105310375636.
a large age difference. Marriage and Family Review, 33, 5–18.
Miller, S. C., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004a). Perceived reactions to
Berscheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W. (1971). Physical interracial romantic.
attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the matching hypothesis. Miller, S. C., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004b). Perceived reactions to
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7(2), 173–189. https:// interracial romantic relationships: When race is used as a cue to
doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(71)90065-5. status. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 354–369.
Bryne, D., & Nelson, D. (1964). Attraction as a function of attitude https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430204046143.
similarity-dissimilarity: The effect of topic importance. Nakonezny, P. A., & Denton, W. H. (2008). Marital relationships: A
Psychonomic Science, 1(5), 93–94. https://doi.org/10.3758/ social exchange theory perspective. American Journal of Family
BF03342806. T h e r a p y, 3 6 ( 5 ) , 4 0 2 – 4 1 2 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 /
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: 01926180701647264.
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups.
Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49. New York: Wiley.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's U.S. Census Bureau. (1999). America’s families and living arrangements.
Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, Retrieved April 8, 2009 from: http://www.census.gov/population/
data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. www/socdemo/hh-fam/p20-537_99.html
Collisson, B., Howell, J. L., Rusbasan, D., & Rosenfeld, E. (2016). Date United Nations. (2000). World Marriage Patterns 2000. Retrieved April
someone your own size. Prejudice and discrimination towards 17, 2009 from: http://www.un.org/esa/population/publications/
mixed-weight relationships. Journal of Social and Personal worldmarriage/worldmarriage.htm

View publication stats

S-ar putea să vă placă și