Sunteți pe pagina 1din 5

Jlemor.

\' & Cognition


1974.1"01. :;..Vo. 1A. 189·193

The influence of meaningfulness upon intentional


and incidentallearning of verbal material
STEPHEN WOLK
University 0/ Mary lall d, College Park. Maryland 20742

The prcsent investigation concerned the relationship between intentional and incidental leaming of verbal material.
Both task meaningfulness (sequential dependency of the material) and shifts in performance over time were exarnined
for differential patterns under both types of learning conditions. Cover task performance consisted of the search for
typographical errors across four paragraphs of a narrative. learning the recall and recognition of exarnples of categories
of objects in the text. There was lack of support for the argument that incidental and intentionallearning represent two
distinct types; rather Ss responded similarly to increased task meaningfulness under both learning conditions and
evidenced identical performance patterns over time. Results were interpreted within an intentional-attentional
hypo thesis of incidentallearning.

The area of incidental learning remains a most differences the learner makes under varying levels of task
differentiated collection of research. Both McLaughlin meaningfulness. Suffice it to say, the literature has not
(I 965) and Ryan (1970) present adequate overviews of afforded a clear answer to this issue. Kaswan (1957)
the area and address themselves to general major issues used pairs of nonsense figures presented to incidental
that are of concern to the present study: (a) whether a ("experiment concerned with ESP") and intentional
continuity exists between intentional and incidental ("memorize the figuresjn preparation for a recall test")
learning, implying nonqualitative differences between groups. These pairs of flgures were either categorized as
the two. 01' whether there are factors that are fitting 01' nonfitting. primarily as a function of good
d ifferentially involved in these phenomena and continuation. Kaswan, while he does not report overall
(b) identification of subject and task variables that best group means, does indicate that the mean difference
predict the level of incidental learning. The research between fitting and nonfitting pairs correctly recalled
su rrounding incidental learning manifests certain was 1.04 for the ESP group and .93 for the intentional
weaknesses. several of which Kessler and Loyd (1970) group. This report suggests a lack of a differential effect
have noted. Related to restricted tasks and tests of of task meaningfulness upon intentional and incidental
incidental learning, these reduce the generalizability of learning and. indeed. the beneficial effect of high task
the findings. These include specifically: (a) reliance upon meaning upon incidental learning. Postman and Adams
extrernely controlled. discrete tasks of little relevance to (1960). using verbal material and the manipulation of
more continuous. ongoing learning tasks: (b) use of contextual relationships as an index of meaningfulness,
narrowly defined measures of incidental learning (most argued that instructions to learn increase sensitivity to
often an immediate or short-term test of retention): and serial order; hence. sequential dependencies would be
(c) employment of tasks that often artificially "isolate" more beneficial. i.e., increase learning. under intentional
the incidental material. relative to the intentional task, as opposed to incidental conditions. Their data support
making it extremely extrinsic to the goals of the learner. this to some degree, contradicting the results of Kaswan.
(For exarnple. learning of geometric forms. with the who used nonverbal material. as weil as an earlier study
incidental material being the colors associated with these by Postman. Adams. and Phillips (1955) in which the
fonns.) main difference between intentional and incidental
While the Kessler and Loyd article can be faulted for conditions was for low-meaningful (nonsense syllabies)
some lack of control over stimulus presentation. material. Mechanic (1962) has essentially supported this
reducing the validity of the results. it is clear that such finding.
methodological controls must be integrated within tasks It should be pointed out that. in regard to the task
that afford more generalizability. This was the general parameter of meaningfulness. only the Postman and
interest with which the specific questions of the present Adams study (1960) used sequences of material as
study were pursued. opposed to arbitrary syllable lists. The findings of this
Both the theory and research associated with study of a differential effect of meaningfulness upon
incidental learning h ave considered level of intentional and incidcntal learning are somewhat
meaningfulness as an important task parameter for such discrepant from the bulk of the research. The evaluation
learning. Evidence of a differential effect upon of this apparent discrepancy must await further
intentional and incidental learning of meaningfulness experimentation in which sequences of material are
would suggest qualitative differences between these used. Kessler and Loyd (I970) also used connected
types of learning, as weil as the nature of any response material (Li-page article ) and found no differences

I xe)
190 WOLK

between an intentional and incidental group in regard to settlement of a nonexistent western town (both written bv the
incidental material. However. the operations used to E). Each story was cornposed of four paragraphs of seven
sentences each. with approxirnately 200 words per paragraph.
define the groups,as weIl as the controls. over the
experimental procedure reduce the significance of these
findings. For example, the authors simply instructed Ss Procedure and Design
to read the article at their convenience and placed no The task was administered individually to insure experimental
time restrictions on this reading. Additionally, the use of control over Ss' responses. As a cover task for both the
intact ongoing classes did not preclude a high degree of intentional and ineidental groups. Ss were instructed to read and
communication between the "intentional" and search the stories for errors "as if they were proofreaders."
"incidental " Ss. Thus. the absence of differences These errors were of the general typographical variety: obvious
misspellings, run-on words, duplicated letters, letter reversals.
between groups could be attributed to the failure of the and inappropriate symbols. There were 20 errors in each
author to exert more controlover stimulus presentations paragraph. and Ss were to read and circle as many of these errors
and administration of experimental conditions. as possible, Ss were given 2 min to read each paragraph and circle
The possibility of a differential effect of errors.
The material that was tested for retention under both
meaningfulness upon intentional-incidental learning
intentional and incidental learning conditions consisted of
relates most directly to the theoretical concern of the specific examples of various categories of items presented in the
possible discontinuity between intentional and stories, A total of 36 exarnpies of these categories (seven
incidental learning. One dominant point of view is that categories in each story) were dispersed by having nine present in
intention is the manipulated variable in an eaeh paragraph. Several categories of the "drug" story were:
numbers, colors. animals, and parts of the body. F or the
intentional-incidental paradigm. and. thus, intention aets "western towri" story these included: tools and rnaterials,
indireetly through its effeets upon the attentional geographical locations. foods, and businesses.
processes of the subject. While the nature of these Related to one of the experimental manipulations, half of the
attentional processes has been suggested (for example, Ss were tested und er an incidental learning condition.
Specifically. no rnention was made of any test of specific
cognitive prominence, organization. and arousal) but not
eontent of the stories. The instruetions given to these Ss were as
fully delineated, this Interpretation of incidentallearning follows: "In the following task your job will be to find as many
offers both the most parsimonious and powerful errors as possible in the paragraphs of a story which you will
approach, as compared to rehearsal or differential read. Since you will be given only 2 minu tes per paragraph. it is
responding. Those pursuing this attentional hypothesis important that you work quickly but accurately. When you find
an error in the paragraph. eircle it with your pencil. When you
have included Gleitman and Gillett (1957), Eagle and finish the paragraph. put your peneil down and wait. Do not 1'0
Leiter (1964), and Schneider and Kintz (1967). This on to the next page until you are told to do so. Those errors are
hypothesis remains to be extended for data collected of a typographieal nature: misspellings. run-on words, duplicated
from a more continuous type of task. letters, ete. It is irnportant that you werk as accurately as
possible. "
Finally, data related to possible differential changes in Thus. f'ollowing the reading of both stories. Ss were given tests
intentional and incidental learning over time remain to of learning. Initially presented was a grid having the seven
be systernatically colleeted. Given the discrete nature of eategories of elements listed. Instructions indicated to list as
rnost incidental tasks, such a question has been difficult rnany of the exarnples of these categ ories as appeared in the
to consider: however. within the eontext of a continuous story. 55 were given a maximurn of 6 min for this (recall measure
of incidental learning). F ollowing this. Ss were presented with a
interrelated task, the possibility of ehanging eognitive listing of the seven eategories of elements. under each of which
strategies for these types of learning, with inereased were 15 exarnples. Among these were the actual ones that had
familiarity or exposure to material, presents itself as a appeared in the story. r or eaeh category. the 5 was instructed
most compelling issue. It is in regard to the issues of task how many had actually appeared (either five or six) and was told
to circle only that number for his choices, but no rnore. Six
meaningfulness and related attentional shifts that the
minutes rnaxirnum were also allotted for the test (recoznition
present paper eonsiders intentional and incidental measure of ineidental learning). It should be noted that these
learning. tests were subsequent to the reading of both stories (eight
paragraphs). Lach story was presented individually with a
description of the cover task.
METHOD It should be noted also that none of the ineidental stimuli
were involved with any type of error. This insured that the
Subjects ineidental nature of the examples would not be contaminated by
assoeiation with the presence of errors. Also, the last sentence of
One hundred and thirty-six students from lower level graduate eaeh paragraph was free of exarnplcs to insure that differential
courses in educational psychology comprised the sampie (58 reading speeds would not determine the number of examples
males and 78 females). encountered.
The rernaininz half of the Ss were run under an intentional
Materials learning conditi~n. The instructions to these Ss differed from
those to the ineidental group with the addition of the f'ollowing
An intentional-ineidentallearning task consisting of connected statement: "In addition to how weil you can find errors in the
stimuli (textual material). the general format for several previous story. you will also be tested for how weil vou can remember
investigations fDuCette & Wolk. 1973; \Volk & DuCette, 1972). particular facts about the story. Spccifically. you will be asked
was employed. The material consisted of two separate stories: to supply examples of the following catcgories of things which
one concerning the discovery of a fictitious drug. the other the you encounter in yOUT reading: (categorie'\ appropriate to each
Ir\FLCE:\CE OF MEAl'\INGFULNESS UPOr\ LEARc'iING 191

story). The exarnples of thcse catecorie s are dispersed Table 2


throughout the story." ~ Mean Performance Levels for Incidental and Intentional
Inorder to facilitate this intentional learning, a listins 01' the Learning (Recall and Recognition) by Paragraphs
seven categories was given at the top of each 01' the four
paragraphs for each story. Identical testing and time parameters Paragraph
used for the incidental group were employed. 2 4
3
In regard to the other experimental manipulation. Ss in both
the incidental and intentional groups received both a high- and Incidental Lcarning
low-meaningful story. The low-meaningful version of each storv Recall 1.41 1.11 1.21 1.75
was achieved through randomization of the seven sentences in Recognition 5.53 5.00 4.75 5.92
each paragraph (with the exception of Sentence 7) and the
In tentional Learning
randomization of words wirhin each sentence. The
Recall 3.42 2.80 3.02 3.60
high-rneaningful story was a normal narrative passage. Because 01'
Recognition 7.51 6.81 . 7.23 7.89
t n e nature of the low-rneaningful material and its
inappropriateness as the first story. each S always received the
low-mea~i~gful story second. although this "nested" a practice
(F=42.64, df=3/402, p<.001). Again no significant
effect wirhin one level of meaningfulness. Both stories occurred
in tel' actions were present. Comparisons between
equally often as the first (high meaningful) to be presented.
paragraphs indicated that there were no significant
F or each S three scores were derived: the number of errors
correctly found in each paragraph (cover task performance. total
differences between Paragraphs 1 and 4 01' 2 and 3.
possible score of 20) and both a recall and rccoznition score of
correctly remembered cxarnples of the cat;gories in each A similar statistical design was applied to cover task
b\
paragraph (total possible score of 9). A 2 by 2 perforrnance,
4 analvsis of i.e.. the number of errors correctlv found
in each paragraph. The F tests obtained were: leaming
variance (Iearning condition: incidental vs intentional bv task
meaningfulness by paragraphs) with repeated measures on the condition (F = 5.25, df = 1/134, p< .05), paragraphs
last two factors was performed on each 01' these three scores,
(F = 48.42, df = 3/402, p< .001), and meaningfulness
(F = 54.23. df= 1/134, p< .001). The means associated
RESULTS with levels of meaningfulness were: low meaningfulness
= 12.56, high meaningfulness = 15.18. Table 3 presents
Presented in Table 1 are the mean performance levels
cover task performance level by learning condition and
of the two measures of learning (recall and recognition)
paragraph of the story to fully explicate the nature of
by type of learning condition (incidental vs intentional)
the phenomena under investigation, For both learning
and level of task meaningfulness averaged across
conditions. only Paragraphs 2 and 3 were not
paragraphs. Table 2 presents the performance levels of
significantly different from each other in level of cover
the sarne variables by learning condition and paragraph
task performance. It should also be noted that no
of the story. The F tests associated with these means
significant interactions between factors were obtained.
were as follows: for recall. rnain effects for learning
condition (F=21.65. df=I/l34. p<.OOI). task
DISCUSSION
meaningfulness (F=IO.Ol. df=I/134. p<.Ol). and
paragraphs (F=12.18. df=3/402. p<.OOl). Thm
The results allow several points of some consequence
were no significant interactions present between any of
to be made concerning incidental learning. Wirhin the
the factors. For the effect of paragraphs. comparisons
between means (Tukey procedure) indicated the lack of context of a continuous verbal task. in which the
incidental material is fairly intrinsic to the intentional
significant differences (p< .05) in recall scores between
Paragraphs 1 and 3. land 4. and :2 and 3. task. task rneaningfulness does not differentially affect
intentional and incidental learning. The absence 01' a
A similar pattern of statistical findings was obtained
significant interaction of Learning Condition by Task
for recognition: main effects for learning condition
Meaningfulness as weil as any trend in the means
(F = 48.13, df = 1/134. p< .001). task meaningfulness
supports this conclusion. It should be noted that. while
(F = 15.21. df = 1/134. p< .001). and paragraphs
the material was presented in connected form. onlv
Table 1
recall and recognition of disconnected items we;e
Mean Performance Levels for Incidental and Intentional assessed. However. the c o nsistent effect of
Learning (Recall and Recognition) by Levels mearungfulness across learning conditions does indicate
of Task Meaningfulness that both intentional and incidentallearning of such iterns
Iask Meaningfulness are influenced bv the context in which these stirnuli
are presented. Given the qualification, it is nonetheless
Low High Mean
noteworthy that this finding contrasts with that 01'
Incidental Leaming Postman and Adams (1960). who found a greater effect
Recall 1.26 1.48 1.37 01' rneaningful sequence for intentional as opposed to
Rccognition 5.15 5.45 5.30 incidental learning. although that study considered
Intentional Learning several levels 01' meaningfulness, whereas the present
Recall 3.01 3.41 J.~ 1 investigation considered two somewhat extreme olles. Ir'
Recognition 7.14 7.59 7.~6
this. tht?ll. suggests same possible cur\'ilinear relatil11lship
- - - - - - - - -- - - - - - - - - - - ---_ _---- ..
192 WOLK

Table 3 incidental learning may not indeed be so "incidental."


Mean Cover Task Performance Level by Learning Condition This type of learning may be only less intentional, as
and Paragraph of the Story defined by task instructions (presence or absence of a
2 3 4 Mean communicated intent to learn). Ryan's (1970) comment
Incidental
that incidental learning should be more accurately
Learning 13.78 15.36 15.37 14.39 14.72 defined as "less intentional" learning may indeed be
Inten tional
appropriate. Thus, the apparent reciprocal relationship
Learning 12.12 14.35 14.10 11.51 13.02 between cover task perfonnance and learning, under
both the intentional and incidental conditions (a
relationship noted in previous research by Tresselt &
between meaningfulness and incidental learning, then Mayzner, 1960 and Schneider & Kintz, 1967), may be
further research is warranted. However, the employment indicative that the degree of incidental learning is a
of a more integrated task (i.e., the intrinsic relationship
function of the intensity of attentional processing.
between cover task requirements and material tested for Perhaps as a task progresses, the primary or cover task
learning) may indicate that the absence of explicit requirements become progressively less demanding,
intention to learn does not necessarily preclude some necessitating less intense attention. Indeed, even under
response by the learner to increased task the intentional learning condition, the Ss primary
meaningfulness. This response may indeed be relatively consideration was directed toward the search for
equal in magnitude to that of the intentionallearner, an typographical errors. Thus, with repeated exposure to
issue still of concern to researchers in this area. the stimuli of the task, the Ss' attention is less restricted
A further implication of this finding concerns the to just those elements necessary for adequate cover task
relationship between intentional and incidentallearning. perfonnance and comes to be directed toward the more
The presence of an effect of stimulus meaningfulness incidental characteristics of the material. Thus, in an
upon both measures of incidental and intentional ongoing task, there are apparent shifts in attention, and
learning lends support to the generalization that both these shifts may be related to the degree of incidental
types of performance may be affected similarly by learning experienced by an S. It is also suggested that
important task characteristics. Any observed discrepancy these shifts may result as a function of a reduced
between levels of i ncidental and intentional demand by the orienting or cover task with repeated
perfonnance, therefore, may be attributable to the exposure. With astated intent to learn, the attention is
presence or absence of some factor that determines the more intensely directed toward the material. Under
degree of learning. For example, the argument that incidental Iearning conditions, less learning is
incidental learning is less intentional learning suggests experienced as a function of the intensity with which
that the intent of the learner defines, possibly, the the material is attended. Under either condition,
degree of attention given to material to be learned. however, degree of attentional processing, as evidenced
Within the present set of data, for example, there by the similar pattern in performance with repeated
appears to be some support for the thesis that incidental exposure to each story, may be a most important
learning is influenced by the degree of attentional determinant of learning.
processing given to the more "irrelevant" (as defined by Coupled with the finding of similar systematic
the task) stimuli. This is found in an inspection of the changes in performance over time in both intentional
shifts in perfonnance of both the cover task and and incidental learning, as well as the absence of an
incidental learning measures under both learning interactive effect between intent and meaningfulness,
conditions (Tables 2 and 3). There appears to be the present study may lend support to the argument that
essentially the same pattern in shifts in performance incidental learning rnay not be qualitatively different
under both learning conditions: an initial emphasis upon from intentional learning. Perhaps the present study
both types of performance (paragraph 1), more contributes somewhat to a firmer theoretical position.
attention given to cover task performance intermediate Clearly, more research is needed in which important task
in the task as the perfonnance approaches asymptotic variables, such as meaningfulness, are explored for
level (paragraphs 2 and 3), and a final emphasis upon possible differential effects upon learning under
leaming at the expense of cover task performance intentional and incidental conditions.
(paragraph 4). It is believed that this "emphasis" is More studies are also necessary in which examination
primarily translated through the intent of the learner. of shifts in performance over time (reflecting similarities
While this is somewhat of an indirect inference, the or differences in cognitive strategies associated with
presence of an intent to learn appeared to have had two intentional and incidental learning) is conducted. The
basic effects: an obvious heightening of learning the present study offers one contribution to this continuing
examples of the categories but a depression in cover task theoretical delineation of incidentallearning.
perfonnance (locating errors). However, the pattern of
perfonnance across paragraphs is very sirnilar to the REFERENCES
condition in which an intent to learn is not present. This
may argue for the fact that, in a well-integrated task, DuCette, 1. P.. & Welk, S. Attentional processes in incidental
It\FLUE(\;CE OF MEANINGFUL(\;ESS UPON LEARNIl"G 193

learning 01' cormected material. Paper pre sented at the Lastern The effects 01' contextual determination. Journa! 01'
Psychological Association meeting. Washington. D.C.. \lay Experimental Psychology, 1960.59.153-164.
1973. Postman. L., Adams, P. A., & Phillips, L. W. Studies in incide ntal
Eagle. \1.. & Leiter. E. Recall and recosnition in intentional and learning: 11. The effects 01' association value and 01' method 01'
incidental learning Journal 01' E~perimental Psychology, testing. Journal 01' Experimental Psvchology, 1955.49. 1-10.
1964.68.58-63 Ryan, T. A. Intentional behavior: An approach to human
Gleitman. H., & Gillett. E. The effect 01' intention upon learning. ·motivation. New York: Ronald, 1970.
Journal 01' General Psvcholozv. 1957.57.137-149. Schneider. F. W.. & Kintz, B. L. An analysis 01' the
Kaswan, J. Association 01' nonsense-figures as a function 01' in c id e n tal-intentional learning dichotorny. Journal 01'
fittingnass and intention to learn. American Journal 01' Experimental Psychologv, 1967.73.85-90.
Psvcholozv, 1957. 70. 447-450. Tresselt, "I. E .. & Mayzner, \1. S. A study 01' incidentallearning.
Kessier. C.-C.. & Loyd. G. O. Comparison 01' incidental and Journal of Psychology, 1960.50,339-347.
intentional learning 01' meaningful material in an educational Welk, S.. & DuCette. 1. P. Intentional performance and
setting. Proceedings 01' the 78th Annual Convention 01' the incidental learning as a function 01' personality and task
American Psychological Association. 1970. dimensions. Journal 01' PersonaJity & SociaJ Psychology, 1973.
Mcl.aughlin. B. "Intentional" and "incidental" learning in in press.
human subjects: The role 01' instructions to learn and
motivation. Psvcholozical Bulletin. 1965.63.359-376.
Mechanic. A. Th~ distribution 01' recalled items in simultaneous
intentional and incidental learning. Journal 01' Experimental (Received for pubIication Januar)' 31. 1973'
Psvcholosv. 1962.64.593-600. revision received April 1, 1973:
Post~an. L~: & Adams. P. A. Studies in incidentallearning: VIII. secend revision accepted "Iay 31. 1973.)

S-ar putea să vă placă și