Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Animal Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav

Special Issue

An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural


plasticity
Emilie C. Snell-Rood*
Department of Ecology, Evolution and Behaviour, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, MN, U.S.A.

a r t i c l e i n f o
I outline how understanding the mechanism of behavioural plasticity is important for predicting how
Article history: organisms will respond to rapidly changing and novel environments. I define two major forms of
Received 8 August 2012 behavioural plasticity: developmental and activational. Developmental plasticity refers to the capacity of
Initial acceptance 8 October 2012 a genotype to adopt different developmental trajectories in different environments. Activational plasti-
Final acceptance 19 December 2012 city refers to differential activation of an underlying network in different environments such that an
Available online 5 March 2013 individual expresses various phenotypes throughout their lifetime. I suggest that the costs and benefits
MS. number: ASI-12-00615 of these two forms of behavioural plasticity may differ: developmental plasticity is slow, but results in
a wider range of more integrated responses. Furthermore, the neural costs associated with activational
Keywords: plasticity may be greater because large neural networks must be maintained past an initial sampling and
cognition
learning phase. While the benefits of plasticity are realized in variable environments, I argue that fine-
human-induced rapid environmental
grained and coarse-grained variation may differentially select for activational and developmental plas-
change (HIREC)
life history
ticity, respectively. Because environmental variation experienced by an organism is largely determined
niche construction by behaviour, developmental plasticity may still evolve in fine-grained environments if niche choice
phenotypic plasticity results in coarse-grained ‘realized’ variation. Behavioural plasticity should impact evolution in novel
variable environment environments because it increases the chances of survival in these environments. Developmental
behavioural plasticity may be particularly important for diversification in novel environments because it
can impact not only survival, but also the development of signals and preferences important in mate
choice. Future areas of research on behavioural plasticity and rapid environmental change include stress
as a mechanism underlying rapid integrated responses and life history perspectives on predicting
developmental versus evolutionary responses.
Ó 2013 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Biologists have long been fascinated with understanding antibiotic resistance in bacteria and pesticide resistance in insects
organismal responses to novel and variable environments. Differ- (Palumbi 2001). However, scientists have surmised that the rate of
ences in how species survive in the face of glacial advances, current environmental change exceeds the evolutionary response
changing atmospheric composition, faunal interchanges and cata- rate of many populations (Bell & Collins 2008; Chevin et al. 2010;
strophic events such as meteor impacts have shaped the compo- Hoffmann & Sgro 2011). In particular, for many species, the repro-
sition of modern ecosystems. Today, organisms are presented with ductive rates of species are too slow, the generation time too long,
novel and rapidly changing environments with the spread of or the population size is too small to result in rapid evolutionary
invasive species, climate change, pollutants, habitat destruction, responses (Lande 1998; Reznick & Ghalambor 2001; Bell &
and conversion to agriculture and urban areas. A major topical Gonzalez 2009). Thus, more and more biologists have turned to
question is understanding how species respond to such rapid studying developmental responses, in particular, adaptive pheno-
human-induced environmental change (Sih et al. 2011; Tuomainen typic plasticity, to understand how organisms will respond to
& Candolin 2011). rapidly changing environments (Price et al. 2003; Ghalambor et al.
Many populations show evolutionary responses that track such 2007; Lande 2009).
novel and changing environments, such as the evolution of Adaptive phenotypic plasticity, the ability of a genotype to vary
its phenotype across environments, and thus maintain high per-
formance across that environmental gradient, is important for
survival in variable environments (Schlichting & Pigliucci 1998;
* Correspondence: E. C. Snell-Rood, Department of Ecology, Evolution and
Behaviour, University of Minnesota, 1987 Upper Buford Circle, Ecology 100, St Paul,
West-Eberhard 2003; Bateson & Gluckman 2011). In particular,
MN 55108, U.S.A. behavioural plasticity should be important in novel and variable
E-mail address: emilies@umn.edu. environments given that the development and expression of

0003-3472/$38.00 Ó 2013 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.031
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011 1005

behaviour is remarkably sensitive to environmental conditions. To emphasize the distinction further, imagine a neural network
However, a comprehensive framework for understanding the ori- underlying behaviour, with a sensory (input) layer, a processing
gins and consequences of behavioural plasticity remains elusive, layer and a motor (output) layer (Fig. 1). Activational behavioural
largely because what we mean by ‘plasticity’ encompasses a broad plasticity represents the differential activation of this underlying
range of mechanisms. In this review, I argue that distinguishing network in different environments (Fig. 1a). In this way, this form of
between types of behavioural plasticity, and outlining the under- behavioural plasticity is analogous to physiology, where underlying
lying mechanism, is important for understanding how behavioural metabolic networks result in different enzymes being expressed
plasticity will impact survival in novel and variable environments. under different nutritional conditions. In contrast, developmental
behavioural plasticity is the development of different neural net-
BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY: AN OVERVIEW OF TYPES works in different environments (Fig. 1b). Such plasticity thus in-
volves learning and neural plasticity, but may also encompass
Distinguishing between Developmental and Activational developmental changes in associated sensory systems and mor-
Behavioural Plasticity phologies. As an example, activational plasticity could refer to
hiding or foraging behaviour of an individual in the presence or
Behavioural plasticity can be broadly classified into two absence of predators, whereas developmental plasticity could refer
types: developmental and activational. Developmental behav- to individuals developing into relatively more shy or bold behav-
ioural plasticity corresponds to the traditional definition of ioural types in the presence or absence of predators. Some behav-
phenotypic plasticity, where a genotype expresses different ioural phenomena include aspects of both activational and
behavioural phenotypes in different environments as a result of developmental behavioural plasticity. For instance, listening to
different developmental trajectories triggered by those envi- conspecific song may result in activational plasticity in the short
ronments. Developmental behavioural plasticity encompasses all term (such as territory defence) and developmental plasticity in the
of what is generally defined as ‘learning’, or any change in the long term (such as learning neighbour songs or changing testos-
nervous system as a result of experience. However, devel- terone expression). This review argues that distinguishing between
opmental behavioural plasticity also includes developmental activational and developmental behavioural plasticity (or under-
changes in morphology and physiology relevant to a particular standing the relative contribution of either type of plasticity) is
behaviour, such as changes in muscles, limbs or bones that in- critical for understanding the costs and consequences of plasticity.
fluence foraging or locomotion (e.g. Wainwright et al. 1991;
Losos et al. 2000; Young & Badyaev 2010). Trade-off between Response Time and Phenotypic Integration
Developmental behavioural plasticity is different from ‘activa-
tional’ behavioural plasticity, referred to elsewhere as ‘behaviour as One of the major differences between activational and devel-
plasticity’ (Dukas 1998) or ‘innate’ behavioural plasticity (Mery & opmental behavioural plasticity comes as a trade-off between
Burns 2010). Here, the external context results in the expression response time and phenotypic integration. Activational behavioural
of a particular behaviour (Stamps & Groothuis 2010) such that an plasticity is an immediate response to the environment. The un-
individual expresses different behaviours as it encounters different derlying neural networks are present, and it is only a matter of
environments or conditions. Activational plasticity refers to the activating different neurons and muscles. Developmental behav-
differential activation of an underlying network. ioural plasticity, by definition, requires developmental changes

(a)

Environment 1 Motor output 1

Environment 2 Motor output 2

(b)
Development in environment 1 Development in environment 2

Figure 1. A neural network illustration of the two forms of behavioural plasticity. (a) Activational behavioural plasticity refers to differential activation of an underlying network by
different environments. Thus, different motor outputs result in different environments. (b) Developmental behavioural plasticity refers to the differential development of neural
networks in different environments such as a change in synaptic weights as a result of experience.
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

1006 E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011

such as neuron or muscle growth. These processes take time. For which often comes down to understanding costs and benefits of
example, even long-term memory formation in Drosophila takes at plasticity. The costs of developmental behavioural plasticity stem
least 5 h (Margulies et al. 2005). from the fact that much of this form of plasticity is underlain by
Developmental behavioural plasticity, while a slower process, ‘trial-and-error’ or developmental selection processes. Devel-
has the potential to result in a much wider range of highly inte- opmental or somatic selection involves both sampling of a range of
grated behavioural phenotypes. Cues on the environmental state phenotypes and environmental feedback on which phenotypes
received early in development can influence the subsequent perform well in current conditions (West-Eberhard 2003; Snell-
development of many traits (Fig. 2). There are constraints on Rood 2012). Such selective processes in development are costly in
altering phenotypes once developed, an idea broadly referred to as time, energy and exposure.
the epiphenotype problem (DeWitt et al. 1998). This idea has been While costs associated with all forms of phenotypic plasticity
put forward to explain why cues received earlier in development remain elusive (Van Buskirk & Steiner 2009), costs associated with
(relative to those received later) can result in a greater range and developmental behavioural plasticity, in particular learning, are
degree of integration of adult phenotypes (Wheeler 1986; Bruni clearer (e.g. Mery & Kawecki 2003, 2004, 2005). Sampling, whether
et al. 1996; Zera & Denno 1997; Hoverman & Relyea 2007). Thus, exploring resources, the environment or a range of phenotypes,
developmental behavioural plasticity has a much greater chance of requires time, energy and necessitates making mistakes (Laverty &
resulting in differences in the neural pathways, morphology and Plowright 1988; Ericsson et al. 1993; Janz & Nylin 1997; Byers et al.
physiology underlying a behaviour. For example, developing in 2005), referred to as the cost of naiveté (Dukas 1998), or the
different resource environments may result in the development of exploration-exploitation trade-off (Kaelbling et al. 1996). Increasing
not only different foraging and food handling behaviour (Burghardt the breadth of possible learned phenotypes necessitates an
& Krause 1999; Slagsvold & Wiebe 2007; Woo et al. 2008), but also increase in neural network sensory input (Hampson 1991; Zohary
different food handling morphology, such as mouthpart shape 1992; Eurich et al. 1997; Huerta et al. 2004) and motor output
(Wainwright et al. 1991; Wimberger 1991; Adams et al. 2003) and (Hopfield 1982; Aboitiz 1996; Sporns et al. 2000), which is signif-
digestive enzyme activity (Cahu et al. 1998; Sabat et al. 2005; Brzek icant because neural tissue is one of the most metabolically
et al. 2009). expensive tissues (Laughlin et al. 1998; Attwell & Laughlin 2001;
Distinguishing between activational and developmental Niven et al. 2007).
behavioural plasticity in this way can give important insights into Studies show that variation in learning and cognition is often
common questions that arise in the animal personality and correlated with variation in neural investment, both within and
behavioural syndrome literature (Sih et al. 2004; Bell 2007). Why across species (Sherry et al. 1992; Lefebvre & Sol 2008; Roth &
do we often observe inflexibility in individual behaviour given that Pravosudov 2009; Snell-Rood et al. 2009), and as a product of
environment-specific expression of behaviour is often advanta- artificial selection (Herman & Nagy 1977; Jensen & Fuller 1978). The
geous? In many cases, the physiology, morphology and behaviour costs associated with exploration and neural tissue can explain why
of an individual may be an integrated suite of traits (Duckworth species and genotypes with increased investment in learning and
2010) that developed to ‘match’ to a particular environment, neural tissue tend to have delays in reproduction and reduced
resource or starting phenotypic environment such as body size fecundity (Mace & Eisenberg 1982; Iwaniuk & Nelson 2003;
or social environment. This is similar to the idea that initial dif- Lefebvre et al. 2006; Barrickman et al. 2008; Snell-Rood et al. 2011).
ferences in individual state (such as body size) may be amplified Thus, the costs of learning, and any developmental selection pro-
over development as individuals adopt different strategies best cess more generally, should result in a shift, along the continuum of
suited to their differences in state (Dingemanse & Wolf 2010). life history strategies, towards more K-selected or ‘slow’ life history
While it is possible that differences in the environment may drive traits (Snell-Rood 2012). This could occur because the costs of
the development of different behavioural types or personalities, learning select against ‘fast’ life history traits or because species
studying the development of behavioural syndromes is still an with ‘slower’ life history traits can afford the costs of learning.
open area of research (Stamps & Groothuis 2010).
Costs of Activational versus Developmental Behavioural Plasticity
WHY DOES BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY VARY AMONG
INDIVIDUALS? Different mechanisms underlying plastic responses generally
come with different costs and constraints (Snell-Rood et al. 2010;
Costs of Developmental Behavioural Plasticity Snell-Rood 2012). It is likely that the two major categories of
behavioural plasticity also come with different costs. Increasing
Biologists have long been interested in understanding why activational behavioural plasticity would correspond to an increase
phenotypic plasticity varies within and between species, a question in the possible motor responses to a large range of different envi-
ronmental states. To respond to finer variation in environmental
state, a nervous system must have an increase in sensory input
Cells making up different traits
(Fig. 3). For example, increased sensory perception often corre-
Developmental time

Cue 2 sponds to larger regions of dedicated neural space (Catania & Kaas
1997; Glendenning & Masterton 1998; Hutcheon et al. 2002).
Likewise, a greater or finer range of motor responses, such as
manual dexterity or behavioural repertoire size, also corresponds to
Cue 1
an increase in neural investment (Iwaniuk et al. 1999; Changizi
2003). Thus, an increase in activational behavioural plasticity
Zygote should correspond to an increase in neuron number and overall
brain size. Given the costs associated with neural tissue (Laughlin
Figure 2. Developmental plasticity can result in a wide range of integrated adult et al. 1998; Attwell & Laughlin 2001; Niven et al. 2007), this is
phenotypes. A cue received early in development (cue 1) can more easily affect the
development of all subsequent traits than a cue received later in development (cue 2),
likely a substantial cost.
which must be independently communicated to traits that have already differentiated. Increasing developmental behavioural plasticity should also
This is referred to as the epiphenotype problem (DeWitt et al. 1998). correspond to increased neural investment, but only early in
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011 1007

Lower activational behavioural plasticity

Phenotype
No variation: A A
Genotype
No plasticity
or individual

Enviro 1 Output 1 A B

Phenotype
Coarse-grained
Enviro 2 Output 2 A B
variation:
Developmental Genotype
plasticity

A B

Higher activational behavioural plasticity

Phenotype
Fine-grained
variation: A B
Enviro 1 Output 1
Activational plasticity Individual

Lifetime of individual
A B
Enviro 2 Output 2
Environment

Figure 4. Environmental variation and behavioural plasticity. No environmental var-


Output 3 iation selects for fixed behaviour, both for individual behaviour over their lifetime and
Enviro 3 for the capacity of a genotype to develop differently in different environments. Coarse-
grained variation, where the environment varies more between than within genera-
tions, should select for developmental plasticity, where a genotype develops differ-
ently based on the environment. Fine-grained variation, where the environment varies
Enviro 4 Output 4 within an individual lifetime, should select for activational plasticity, where an un-
derlying network (e.g. neural network or metabolic network) is differentially activated
Figure 3. Neural costs of increasing activational plasticity. Higher activational plasti- in different environments, resulting in an individual varying its behaviour across
city corresponds to an increase in the range of environments that an individual can environments.
detect and in the range of motor responses that the individual can match to these
different environments. Theoretically, this should correspond to an increase in the size
of the neural network underlying the response, although there may be network de-
signs (e.g. parallel structure) that minimize increasing costs.
benefits of plasticity are maximized, while the costs of plasticity are
minimized. In particular, if the environment remains constant
development. To develop appropriate motor responses to a range of within a generation, the costs of phenotypeeenvironment mis-
possible environments or resources, an individual must initially match are minimized (Padilla & Adolph 1996; DeWitt et al. 1998),
sense a broad range of inputs and express a broad range of outputs. along with any costs of relearning a behavioural phenotype or
Such trial-and-error learning includes a broad initial sampling altering a morphological phenotype.
period, followed by reinforcement and consolidation of high- Fine-grained variation, where the environment varies within
performing phenotypes. At the neural level, this developmental the lifetime of an individual (Fig. 4), should select for reversible
process corresponds to very high numbers of neurons early in phenotypic plasticity to avoid costly mismatches. In the case of
development, which are reduced and refined with experience behaviour, activational behavioural plasticity should be favoured in
(Huttenlocher 1979; Oppenheim 1991; Purves et al. 1996; Luo & the presence of fine-grained environmental variation such that
O’Leary 2005). Because larger neural networks are necessary to different behaviours are expressed in the different environments
produce more rearrangements and ultimately a better match to encountered by an individual. Fine-grained environmental varia-
local conditions (Aboitiz 1996), the initial neural costs associated tion should be particularly high for very long-lived species, which
with activational behavioural plasticity and developmental encounter multiple environments over their lifetime. Such varia-
behavioural plasticity should be comparable. However, the overall tion will also be pronounced for nonmigratory, temperate species
costs are likely lower for developmental behavioural plasticity due that do not diapause or hibernate. The ‘cognitive buffer hypothesis’,
to pruning and refinement of neural networks over developmental which states that increased brain size offers benefits for environ-
time. mental change within the lifetime of an individual (Sol 2009),
formalizes the link between activational behavioural plasticity and
Environmental Variation Selects for Plasticity fine-grained environmental variation. This can explain why longer-
lived species and those that experience more seasonality tend to
have larger neural investment (Sol et al. 2007; Roth & Pravosudov
The benefits of phenotypic plasticity are realized in variable and
2009; Gonzalez-Lagos et al. 2010; Roth et al. 2011).
novel environments. The plasticity literature has shown that one of
the most important selective factors for phenotypic plasticity is
environmental variation, in particular coarse-grained variation The Importance of Niche Choice in Determining Realized
(Levins 1968; Moran 1992; Stephens 1992). Coarse-grained envi- Environmental Variation
ronmental variation refers to situations where the environment
varies between generations, but remains relatively constant within Environmental variation is often considered as external to an
generations (Fig. 4). Models of developmental plasticity, where organism. However, all students of animal behaviour know the
reversibility of phenotype is difficult or absent, generally suggest importance of behaviour in structuring environmental variation.
that coarse-grained environmental variation is most favourable for For example, individuals may experience more environmental
the evolution of plasticity. In coarse-grained environments, the variation through dispersal between environments or through
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

1008 E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011

neophilia, exploring a wider range of objects or resources within development of alternate phenotypes that differ in morphology,
their environment (Greenberg 1983, 1989; Mettke-Hofmann et al. behaviour and physiology. For instance, sticklebacks develop dif-
2002, 2009). Individuals also have many mechanisms to reduce ferences in not only behaviour, but also morphology, when reared
‘realized’ environmental variation. Habitat and resource prefer- in benthic and limnetic conditions (Shaw et al. 2007; Wund et al.
ences are ubiquitous among animals. In particular, animals often 2008, 2012).
prefer habitats that are similar to those they have experienced Activational behavioural plasticity is also important for survival
before, through natal habitat preference induction (Stamps 1995; in new environments because it can allow immediate adjustment
Davis & Stamps 2004; Mabry & Stamps 2008) or phenotypee to novel environments. For example, stringent preferences for
habitat matching (Edelaar et al. 2008; Karpestam et al. 2012). mates or resources are often adjusted when resources or mates are
Realized environmental variation may also be reduced through less common. Mate preferences are often relaxed when organisms
diapause or hibernation (Tauber & Tauber 1976), habitat modifi- are reared in low-density environments (Bailey & Zuk 2008;
cations (Frederickson et al. 2005), or sensory biases that result in Fowler-Finn & Rodriguez 2012), which might facilitate survival of
ignoring stimuli (Wcislo & Tierney 2009). populations in new environments where population density is low.
The various methods that reduce environmental variation could Similarly, butterflies in an environment without innately preferred
potentially turn fine-grained variation into coarse-grained varia- host plants will start exploring (and accepting) alternate hosts
tion, favouring the evolution of developmental plasticity (or no (Bergstrom et al. 2004; Snell-Rood & Papaj 2009). Thus, plasticity in
variation, favouring the evolution of specialization). Indeed, the acceptance thresholds based on environmental conditions repre-
reduced neural costs and increased range of phenotypes associated sents differential development of activational behavioural plasticity
with developmental behavioural plasticity (see above) may result that may facilitate survival in new environments.
in selection for such plasticity through mechanisms that decrease The importance of both forms of behavioural plasticity for sur-
lifetime environmental variation. In this way, many ‘generalist’ vival in new environments is emphasized by the range of studies
species that cope with a wide range of environments may be linking brain size to survival in cities and after invasions (reviewed
composed of individual specialists that have developed behavioural in Wright et al. 2010). Both birds and mammals with greater neural
and morphological traits suited for particular resources or micro- investment (and behavioural innovation rate) do better after
environments (Fox & Morrow 1981; Bolnick et al. 2003). introduction into novel environments (Sol et al. 2005, 2008). Sim-
A niche choice or niche construction perspective also reveals the ilarly, bird species with relatively larger forebrains are more likely
possibility for complex evolutionary feedbacks (Kylafis & Loreau to thrive in urban environments or rural environments with
2008). For instance, an increase in learning or cognition may lead increasing human presence (Carrete & Tella 2011; Maklakov et al.
to exploring and surviving in a wider range of environments, 2011; Pocock 2011). Behavioural plasticity may facilitate novel,
resulting in an increase in realized environmental variation, innovative behaviour that allows survival in novel environments
selecting for greater plasticity. This positive feedback loop would (Sol et al. 2002; Møller 2009), such as learning to avoid or ignore
eventually be offset by the costs of plasticity (Fig. 5). novel predators (Levey et al. 2009) or adjusting signals in the face of
noise (Halfwerk & Slabbekoorn 2009).
CONSEQUENCES OF BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY IN NOVEL Behavioural plasticity, in particular developmental plasticity,
ENVIRONMENTS also has major impacts on evolutionary diversification (Price et al.
2003; West-Eberhard 2003; Lande 2009; Pfennig et al. 2010).
Both types of behavioural plasticity impact evolution in novel Plastic populations have the potential to immediately shift to new
environments by increasing the probability of survival in that adaptive peaks, especially if phenotypes develop through learning-
environment. Developmental behavioural plasticity is particularly like processes that incorporate both sampling and environmental
relevant to survival in novel environments because trial-and-error feedback (Hinton & Nolan 1987; Frank 1996, 2011; Snell-Rood
processes such as learning, which include both phenotype sam- 2012). Once on these new selective peaks, the costs of plasticity,
pling and environmental feedback, have the ability to immediately which are especially pronounced for learning and similar mecha-
shift an entire population close to a novel adaptive peak (Hinton & nisms of plasticity, should select for more efficient production of
Nolan 1987; Snell-Rood 2012). Such plasticity can result in the the phenotype; in other words, reduced plasticity. Alternatively, if
the environment is still variable, there may be selection for habitat
choice, followed by reduced plasticity. Either way, genetic assim-
ilation may result, where an initially environmentally induced
Niche choice Costs
phenotype is constitutively produced, potentially leading to
diversification (see Pigliucci & Murren 2003; West-Eberhard 2003;
Pfennig et al. 2010; Bateson & Gluckman 2011).
Because developmental plasticity can simultaneously affect the
development of behaviour, ornaments and sensory systems, it may
be especially important for diversification because it can lead to
Environmental variation Behavioural plasticity immediate prezygotic isolation. It is well established that the
environment affects the development of signals: song structure is
often learned (Marler 1997; Doupe & Kuhl 1999) and then adjusted
in noisy environments (Gross et al. 2010; Luther & Derryberry
Survival in more environments 2012), and the development of ornamentation often depends on
diet (Hill 1992; Toomey & McGraw 2009). We also know that the
Figure 5. Feedbacks in selection on behavioural plasticity. Environmental variation development of sensory systems depends on the environment. For
tends to select for behavioural plasticity while the costs select against behavioural instance, visual systems develop quite differently in different
plasticity. Given that plasticity increases survival in a range of environments, increases lighting conditions (Cronin et al. 2001; Fuller et al. 2010). This
in plasticity may correspond to increases in realized environmental variation, selecting
further for plasticity in a positive feedback loop. Niche choice may offset this feedback
suggests that there may be simultaneous developmental shifts in
loop through a decrease in realized environmental variation, for instance through traits that affect survival in a novel environment and traits that
preference for habitats and resources experienced earlier in life. affect mate choice. Thus, there is potential for immediate
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011 1009

prezygotic isolation from populations using ancestral environ- discussions with Reuven Dukas and Daniel Sol and comments from
ments (Price 1998; Slabbekoorn & Smith 2002). two anonymous referees.

CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS


References
In conclusion, this review has discussed how distinguishing Aboitiz, F. 1996. Does bigger mean better? Evolutionary determinants of brain size
between different forms of behavioural plasticity can yield insights and structure. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 47, 225e245.
into the costs and consequences of plasticity. The general discus- Adams, C. E., Woltering, C. & Alexander, G. 2003. Epigenetic regulation of trophic
morphology through feeding behavior in Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus. Bio-
sion has highlighted several exciting and open areas of current logical Journal of the Linnean Society, 78, 43e49.
research. First, the neural costs associated with developmental and Attwell, D. & Laughlin, S. B. 2001. An energy budget for signaling in the grey
activational plasticity are hypothesized to be different, but we have matter of the brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 21, 1133e
1145.
no strict tests of this idea. The most straightforward way to initially
Badyaev, A. V. 2005. Stress-induced variation in evolution: from behavioral plas-
test this idea would be through neural network models that also ticity to genetic assimilation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 272, 877e886.
incorporated costs. But it could also be informative to contrast Bailey, N. W. & Zuk, M. 2008. Acoustic experience shapes female mate choice in
behavioural plasticity of genotypes (or species) that differ in the field crickets. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 2645e2650.
Barrickman, N. L., Bastian, M. L., Isler, K. & van Schaik, C. P. 2008. Life history
maintenance of neural investment from early in life to later in life. costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-
Second, developmental plasticity was highlighted as important term studies of primates in the wild. Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 568e590.
for integrated and potentially broad responses to novel environ- Bateson, P. & Gluckman, P. 2011. Plasticity, Robustness, Development and Evolution.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
ments. What are the physiological mechanisms that underlie such Bell, A. M. 2007. Future directions in behavioral syndromes research. Proceedings of
extreme and integrated responses? It is likely that stress will play the Royal Society B, 274, 755e761.
a major role in plastic responses to novel environments given that Bell, G. & Collins, S. 2008. Adaptation, extinction and global change. Evolutionary
Applications, 1, 3e16.
stress responses are incredibly environmentally sensitive and affect Bell, G. & Gonzalez, A. 2009. Evolutionary rescue can prevent extinction following
the activation and development of a suite of traits. Understanding environmental change. Ecology Letters, 12, 942e948.
the intersection between stress and plasticity in the context of Bergstrom, A., Nylin, S. & Nygren, G. H. 2004. Conservative resource utilization in
the common blue butterfly: evidence for low costs of accepting absent host
novel and rapidly changing environments is an exciting area of plants? Oikos, 107, 345e351.
current and future research (McEwen & Wingfield 2003; Wingfield Bolnick, D. I., Svanback, R., Fordyce, J. A., Yang, L. H., Davis, J. M., Hulsey, C. D. &
2003; Badyaev 2005). Forister, M. L. 2003. The ecology of individuals: incidence and implications of
individual specialization. American Naturalist, 161, 1e28.
Third, discussions throughout this review have highlighted
Bromham, L. 2011. The genome as a life-history character: why rate of molecular
the importance of habitat choice and niche construction in the evolution varies between mammal species. Philosophical Transactions of the
evolution of plasticity. In particular, habitat choice may be one Royal Society B, 366, 2503e2513.
mechanism by which organisms reduce the costs of plasticity and Bruni, N. C., Young, J. P. & Dengler, N. G. 1996. Leaf developmental plasticity of
Ranunculus flabellaris in response to terrestrial and submerged environments.
maximize the benefits of developmental plasticity. This hypoth- Canadian Journal of Botany-Revue Canadienne de Botanique, 74, 823e837.
esis predicts that many generalist species may actually be acting Brzek, P., Kohl, K., Caviedes-Vidal, E. & Karasov, W. H. 2009. Developmental ad-
as specialists at the level of individuals, in terms of behavioural, justments of house sparrow (Passer domesticus) nestlings to diet composition.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 1284e1293.
morphological and physiological differentiation. Incorporating Burghardt, G. M. & Krause, M. A. 1999. Plasticity of foraging behavior in garter
niche construction into models of plasticity, diversification and snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis) reared on different diets. Journal of Comparative
personality is also an area ripe for research (Stamps & Groothuis Psychology, 113, 277e285.
Byers, J. A., Wiseman, P. A., Jones, L. & Roffe, T. J. 2005. A large cost of female mate
2010). Studies of behaviour are poised to add empirical views to sampling in pronghorn. American Naturalist, 166, 661e668.
theoretical studies linking niche construction to the evolution of Cahu, C. L., Infante, J. L. Z., Peres, A., Quazuguel, P. & Le Gall, M. M. 1998. Algal
cognition (Kerr & Feldman 2003; Kerr 2007) and the main- addition in sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax) larvae rearing: effect on digestive
enzymes. Aquaculture, 161, 479e489.
tenance of variation within populations (Hui & Yue 2005; Han
Carrete, M. & Tella, J. L. 2011. Inter-individual variability in fear of humans and
et al. 2006). relative brain size of the species are related to contemporary urban invasion in
Finally, predicting how species will respond to novel and rapidly birds. PLoS One, 6, 8.
Catania, K. C. & Kaas, J. H. 1997. Somatosensory fovea in the star-nosed mole:
changing environments is an important focus of future research,
behavioral use of the star in relation to innervation patterns and cortical rep-
not just for conservation reasons, but also for understanding gen- resentation. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 215e233.
eral evolutionary responses of populations to major environmental Changizi, M. A. 2003. Relationship between number of muscles, behavioral rep-
changes. The costs of learning suggest that species with pro- ertoire size, and encephalization in mammals. Journal of Theoretical Biology,
220, 157e168.
nounced developmental or plastic responses to novel environ- Chevin, L. M., Lande, R. & Mace, G. M. 2010. Adaptation, plasticity, and extinction in
ments may have more ‘slow’ or K-selected life history traits. On the a changing environment: towards a predictive theory. PLoS Biology, 8, 8.
other hand, species with short generation times and high fecundity Cronin, T. W., Caldwell, R. L. & Marshall, J. 2001. Sensory adaptation: tunable
colour vision in a mantis shrimp. Nature, 411, 547e548.
are thought to have rapid evolutionary responses to novel envi- Davis, J. M. & Stamps, J. A. 2004. The effect of natal experience on habitat pref-
ronments (Lande 1998; Reznick & Ghalambor 2001; Bromham erences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 411e416.
2011). These interactions between plastic and evolutionary re- DeWitt, T. J., Sih, A. & Wilson, D. S. 1998. Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity.
Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 77e81.
sponses suggest that both empirical and theoretical work on re- Dingemanse, N. J. & Wolf, M. 2010. Recent models for adaptive personality dif-
sponses to rapid environmental change should more explicitly ferences: a review. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 3947e
incorporate a life history perspective into predictions of which 3958.
Doupe, A. J. & Kuhl, P. K. 1999. Birdsong and human speech: common themes and
species will thrive in these novel environments (e.g. Sol 2013). mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567e631.
Duckworth, R. A. 2010. Evolution of personality: developmental constraints on
Acknowledgments behavioral flexibility. Auk, 127, 752e758.
Dukas, R. 1998. Evolutionary ecology of learning. In: Cognitive Ecology: the Evolu-
tionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision Making (Ed. by R. Dukas),
Many thanks to Susan Foster and Andy Sih for organizing the pp. 129e174. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
symposia linking behavioural plasticity to evolution and coping Edelaar, P., Siepielski, A. M. & Clobert, J. 2008. Matching habitat choice causes
with rapid human-induced environmental change. Thanks to the directed gene flor: a neglected dimension in evolution and ecology. Evolution,
62, 2462e2472.
Animal Behavior Society and the National Science Foundation for Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T. & Teschromer, C. 1993. The role of deliberate practice
funding the symposium. This manuscript was improved based on in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363e406.
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

1010 E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011

Eurich, C. W., Schwegler, H. & Woesler, R. 1997. Coarse coding: applications to the Lande, R. 1998. Anthropogenic, ecological and genetic factors in extinction and
visual system of salamanders. Biological Cybernetics, 77, 41e47. conservation. Researches on Population Ecology, 40, 259e269.
Fowler-Finn, K. D. & Rodriguez, R. L. 2012. Experience-mediated plasticity in Lande, R. 2009. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phe-
mate preferences: mating assurance in a variable environment. Evolution, 66, notypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22,
459e468. 1435e1446.
Fox, L. R. & Morrow, P. A. 1981. Specialization: species property or local phe- Laughlin, S. B., van Steveninck, R. R. D. & Anderson, J. C. 1998. The metabolic cost
nomenon. Science, 211, 887e893. of neural information. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 36e41.
Frank, S. 1996. The design of natural and artificial adaptive systems. In: Adaptation Laverty, T. M. & Plowright, R. C. 1988. Flower handling by bumblebees: a com-
(Ed. by M. Rose & G. Lauder), pp. 451e505. New York: Academic Press. parison of specialists and generalists. Animal Behaviour, 36, 733e740.
Frank, S. A. 2011. Natural selection. II. Developmental variability and evolutionary Lefebvre, L. & Sol, D. 2008. Brains, lifestyles and cognition: are there general
rate. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24, 2310e2320. trends? Brain Behavior and Evolution, 72, 135e144.
Frederickson, M. E., Greene, M. J. & Gordon, D. M. 2005. ‘Devil’s gardens’ Lefebvre, L., Marino, L., Sol, D., Lemieux-Lefebvre, S. & Arshad, S. 2006. Large
bedevilled by ants. Nature, 437, 495e496. brains and lengthened life history periods in odontocetes. Brain Behaviour and
Fuller, R. C., Noa, L. A. & Strellner, R. S. 2010. Teasing apart the many effects of Evolution, 68, 218e228.
lighting environment on opsin expression and foraging preference in bluefin Levey, D. J., Londono, G. A., Ungvari-Martin, J., Hiersoux, M. R., Jankowski, J. E.,
killifish. American Naturalist, 176, 1e13. Poulsen, J. R., Stracey, C. M. & Robinson, S. K. 2009. Urban mockingbirds
Ghalambor, C. K., McKay, J. K., Carrol, S. P. & Reznick, D. N. 2007. Adaptive versus quickly learn to identify individual humans. Proceedings of the National Academy
non-adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the potential for contemporary adap- of Sciences, U.S.A., 106, 8959e8962.
tation in new environments. Functional Ecology, 21, 394e407. Levins, R. 1968. Evolution in Changing Environments: Some Theoretical Explorations.
Glendenning, K. K. & Masterton, R. B. 1998. Comparative morphometry of mam- Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
malian central auditory systems: variation in nuclei and form of the ascending Losos, J. B., Creer, D. A., Glossip, D., Goellner, R., Hampton, A., Roberts, G.,
system. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 51, 59e89. Haskell, N., Taylor, P. & Ettling, J. 2000. Evolutionary implications of phenotypic
Gonzalez-Lagos, C., Sol, D. & Reader, S. M. 2010. Large-brained mammals live plasticity in the hindlimb of the lizard Anolis sagrei. Evolution, 54, 301e305.
longer. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 23, 1064e1074. Luo, L. Q. & O’Leary, D. D. M. 2005. Axon retraction and degeneration in devel-
Greenberg, R. 1983. The role of neophobia in determining the degree of foraging opment and disease. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 127e156.
specialization in some migrant warblers. American Naturalist, 122, 444e453. Luther, D. A. & Derryberry, E. P. 2012. Birdsongs keep pace with city life: changes
Greenberg, R. 1989. Neophobia, aversion to open space, and ecological plasticity in in song over time in an urban songbird affects communication. Animal Behav-
song and swamp sparrows. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne de iour, 83, 1059e1066.
Zoologie, 67, 1194e1199. Mabry, K. E. & Stamps, J. A. 2008. Dispersing brush mice prefer habitat like home.
Gross, K., Pasinelli, G. & Kunc, H. P. 2010. Behavioral plasticity allows short-term Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 543e548.
adjustment to a novel environment. American Naturalist, 176, 456e464. Mace, G. M. & Eisenberg, J. F. 1982. Competition, niche specialization and the
Halfwerk, W. & Slabbekoorn, H. 2009. A behavioral mechanism explaining noise- evolution of brain size in the genus Peromyscus. Biological Journal of the Linnean
dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Animal Behaviour, 78, 1301e1307. Society, 17, 243e257.
Hampson, S. 1991. Generalization and specialization in artificial neural networks. McEwen, B. S. & Wingfield, J. C. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and
Progress in Neurobiology, 37, 383e431. biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 2e15.
Han, X. Z., Li, Z. Z., Hui, C. & Zhang, F. 2006. Polymorphism maintenance in Maklakov, A. A., Immler, S., Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Ronn, J. & Kolm, N. 2011. Brains
a spatially structured population: a two-locus genetic model of niche con- and the city: big-brained passerine birds succeed in urban environments.
struction. Ecological Modelling, 192, 160e174. Biology Letters, 7, 730e732.
Herman, B. H. & Nagy, Z. M. 1977. Development of learning and emmory in mice Margulies, C., Tully, T. & Dubnau, J. 2005. Deconstructing memory in Drosophila.
genetically selected for differences in brain weight. Developmental Psychobiol- Current Biology, 15, R700eR713.
ogy, 10, 65e75. Marler, P. 1997. Three models of song learning: evidence from behavior. Journal of
Hill, G. E. 1992. Proximate basis of variation in carotenoid pigmentation in male Neurobiology, 33, 501e516.
house finches. Auk, 109, 1e12. Mery, F. & Burns, J. G. 2010. Behavioral plasticity: an interaction between evolution
Hinton, G. & Nolan, S. 1987. How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems, 1, and experience. Evolutionary Ecology, 24, 571e583.
495e502. Mery, F. & Kawecki, T. J. 2003. A fitness cost of learning ability in Drosophila
Hoffmann, A. A. & Sgro, C. M. 2011. Climate change and evolutionary adaptation. melanogaster. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 2465e2469.
Nature, 470, 479e485. Mery, F. & Kawecki, T. J. 2004. An operating cost of learning in Drosophila mela-
Hopfield, J. J. 1982. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective nogaster. Animal Behaviour, 68, 589e598.
computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., Mery, F. & Kawecki, T. J. 2005. A cost of long-term memory in. Drosophila. Science,
79, 2554e2558. 308, 1148.
Hoverman, J. T. & Relyea, R. A. 2007. How flexible is phenotypic plasticity? Mettke-Hofmann, C., Winkler, H. & Leisler, B. 2002. The significance of ecological
Developmental windows for trait induction and reversal. Ecology, 88, 693e705. factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology, 108, 249e272.
Huerta, R., Nowotny, T., Garcia-Sanchez, M., Abarbanel, H. D. I. & Mettke-Hofmann, C., Lorentzen, S., Schlicht, E., Schneider, J. & Werner, F. 2009.
Rabinovich, M. I. 2004. Learning classification in the olfactory system of in- Spatial neophilia and spatial neophobia in resident and migratory warblers
sects. Neural Computation, 16, 1601e1640. (Sylvia). Ethology, 115, 482e492.
Hui, C. & Yue, D. X. 2005. Niche construction and polymorphism maintenance in Møller, A. P. 2009. Successful city dwellers: a comparative study of the ecological
metapopulations. Ecological Research, 20, 115e119. characteristics of urban birds in the Western Palearctic. Oecologia, 159, 849e858.
Hutcheon, J. M., Kirsch, J. W. & Garland, T. 2002. A comparative analysis of brain Moran, N. A. 1992. The evolutionary maintenance of alternative phenotypes.
size in relation to foraging ecology and phylogeny in the chiroptera. Brain American Naturalist, 139, 971e989.
Behavior and Evolution, 60, 165e180. Niven, J. E., Anderson, J. C. & Laughlin, S. B. 2007. Fly photoreceptors demonstrate
Huttenlocher, P. R. 1979. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: developmental energy-information trade-offs in neural coding. PLoS Biology, 5, 828e840.
changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195e205. Oppenheim, R. W. 1991. Cell death during development of the nervous system.
Iwaniuk, A. N. & Nelson, J. E. 2003. Developmental differences are correlated with Annual Review of Neuroscience, 14, 453e501.
relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis. Canadian Journal of Zoology- Padilla, D. K. & Adolph, S. C. 1996. Plastic inducible morphologies are not always
Revue Canadienne De Zoologie, 81, 1913e1928. adaptive: the importance of time delays in a stochastic environment. Evolu-
Iwaniuk, A. N., Nelson, J. E. & Whishaw, I. Q. 1999. The relationships between tionary Ecology, 10, 105e117.
brain regions and forelimb dexterity in marsupials (Marsupialia): a comparative Palumbi, S. R. 2001. Evolution: humans as the world’s greatest evolutionary force.
test of the principle of proper mass. Australian Journal of Zoology, 48, 99e110. Science, 293, 1786e1790.
Janz, N. & Nylin, S. 1997. The role of female search behavior in determining host Pfennig, D. W., Wund, M. A., Snell-Rood, E. C., Cruickshank, T., Schlichting, C. D.
plant range in plant feeding insects: a test of the information processing hy- & Moczek, A. P. 2010. Phenotypic plasticity’s impacts on diversification and
pothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 264, 701e707. speciation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 25, 459e467.
Jensen, C. & Fuller, J. L. 1978. Learning performance varies with brain weight in Pigliucci, M. & Murren, C. J. 2003. Genetic assimilation and a possible evolutionary
heterogenous mouse lines. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, paradox: can macroevolution sometimes be so fast as to pass us by? Evolution,
92, 830e836. 57, 1455e1464.
Kaelbling, L. P., Littman, M. L. & Moore, A. W. 1996. Reinforcement learning: Price, T. 1998. Sexual selection and natural selection in bird speciation. Philosophical
a survey. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 4, 237e285. Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 353, 251e260.
Karpestam, E., Wennersten, L. & Forsman, A. 2012. Matching habitat choice by Price, T. D., Qvarnström, A. & Irwin, D. E. 2003. The role of phenotypic plasticity in
experimentally mismatched phenotypes. Evolutionary Ecology, 26, 893e907. driving genetic evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 1433e1440.
Kerr, B. 2007. Niche construction and cognitive evolution. Biological Theory, 2, Pocock, M. J. O. 2011. Can traits predict species’ vulnerability? A test with farmland
250e262. passerines in two continents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278, 1532e1538.
Kerr, B. & Feldman, M. W. 2003. Carving the cognitive niche: optimal learning Purves, D., White, L. E. & Riddle, D. R. 1996. Is neural development Darwinian?
strategies in homogeneous and heterogeneous environments. Journal of Theo- Trends in Neurosciences, 19, 460e464.
retical Biology, 220, 169e188. Reznick, D. N. & Ghalambor, C. K. 2001. The population ecology of contemporary
Kylafis, G. & Loreau, M. 2008. Ecological and evolutionary consequences of niche adaptations: what empirical studies reveal about the conditions that promote
construction for its agent. Ecology Letters, 11, 1072e1081. adaptative evolution. Genetica, 112, 183e198.
SPECIAL ISSUE: BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND EVOLUTION

E. C. Snell-Rood / Animal Behaviour 85 (2013) 1004e1011 1011

Roth, T. C. & Pravosudov, V. V. 2009. Hippocampal volumes and neuron numbers Stamps, J. 1995. Motor learning and the value of familiar space. American Naturalist,
increase along a gradient of environmental harshness: a large-scale compari- 146, 41e58.
son. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 401e405. Stamps, J. A. & Groothuis, T. G. G. 2010. Developmental perspectives on person-
Roth, T. C., LaDage, L. D. & Pravosudov, V. V. 2011. Variation in hippocampal ality: implications for ecological and evolutionary studies of individual differ-
morphology along an environmental gradient: controlling for the effects of day ences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 4029e4041.
length. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278, 2662e2667. Stephens, D. 1992. Learning and behavioral ecology: incomplete information
Sabat, P., Riverosa, J. M. & Lopez-Pinto, C. 2005. Phenotypic flexibility in the in- and environmental predictability. In: Insect Learning: Ecological and Evolu-
testinal enzymes of the African clawed frog Xenopus laevis. Comparative Bio- tionary Perspectives (Ed. by D. Papaj & A. Lewis), pp. 195e217. New York:
chemistry and Physiology A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 140, 135e139. Chapman & Hall.
Schlichting, C. D. & Pigliucci, M. 1998. Phenotypic Evolution: a Reaction Norm Tauber, M. J. & Tauber, C. A. 1976. Insect seasonality: diapause maintenance,
Perspective. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer. termination, and postdiapause development. Annual Review of Entomology, 21,
Shaw, K. A., Scotti, M. L. & Foster, S. A. 2007. Ancestral plasticity and the evolu- 81e107.
tionary diversification of courtship behaviour in threespine sticklebacks. Animal Toomey, M. B. & McGraw, K. J. 2009. Seasonal, sexual, and quality related variation
Behaviour, 73, 415e472. in retinal carotenoid accumulation in the house finch (Carpodacus mexicanus).
Sherry, D. F., Jacobs, L. F. & Gaulin, S. J. C. 1992. Spatial memory and adaptive Functional Ecology, 23, 321e329.
specialization of the hippocampus. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 298e303. Tuomainen, U. & Candolin, U. 2011. Behavioral responses to human-induced
Sih, A., Bell, A. & Johnson, J. C. 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and environmental change. Biological Reviews, 86, 640e657.
evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 372e378. Van Buskirk, J. & Steiner, U. K. 2009. The fitness costs of developmental canal-
Sih, A., Ferrari, M. C. O. & Harris, D. J. 2011. Evolution and behavioral responses to ization and plasticity. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22, 852e860.
human-induced rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications, 4, 367e387. Wainwright, P. C., Osenberg, C. W. & Mittelbach, G. G. 1991. Trophic polymor-
Slabbekoorn, H. & Smith, T. B. 2002. Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philo- phism in the pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus Linneaus): effects of
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 357, 493e503. environment on ontogeny. Functional Ecology, 5, 40e55.
Slagsvold, T. & Wiebe, K. L. 2007. Learning the ecological niche. Proceedings of the Wcislo, W. T. & Tierney, S. M. 2009. Behavioral environments and niche con-
Royal Society B, 274, 19e23. struction: the evolution of dim-light foraging in bees. Biological Reviews, 84,
Snell-Rood, E. 2012. Selective processes in development: implications for the costs 19e37.
and benefits of phenotypic plasticity. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 52, West-Eberhard, M. J. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. New York: Ox-
31e42. ford University Press.
Snell-Rood, E. C. & Papaj, D. R. 2009. Patterns of phenotypic plasticity in common Wheeler, D. E. 1986. Developmental and physiological determinants of case in
and rare environments: a study of host use and colour learning in the cabbage social hymenoptera: evolutionary implications. American Naturalist, 128, 13e34.
white butterfly Pieris rapae. American Naturalist, 173, 615e631. Wimberger, P. H. 1991. Plasticity of jaw and skull morphology in the neotropical
Snell-Rood, E. C., Papaj, D. R. & Gronenberg, W. 2009. Brain size: a global or cichlids Geophagus brasiliensis and G. steindachneri. Evolution, 45, 1545e1563.
induced cost of learning? Brain Behavior and Evolution, 73, 111e128. Wingfield, J. C. 2003. Control of behavioural strategies for capricious environments.
Snell-Rood, E., Van Dyken, J. D., Cruickshank, T., Wade, M. & Moczek, A. 2010. Animal Behaviour, 66, 807e815.
Toward a population genetic framework of developmental evolution: costs, Woo, K. J., Elliott, K. H., Davidson, M., Gaston, A. J. & Davoren, G. K. 2008. Indi-
limits, and consequences of phenotypic plasticity. BioEssays, 32, 71e81. vidual specialization in diet by a generalist marine predator reflects special-
Snell-Rood, E. C., Davidowitz, G. & Papaj, D. R. 2011. Reproductive tradeoffs of ization in foraging behavior. Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 1082e1091.
learning in a butterfly. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 291e302. Wright, T. F., Eberhard, J. R., Hobson, E. A., Avery, M. L. & Russello, M. A. 2010.
Sol, D. 2009. Revisiting the cognitive buffer hypothesis for the evolution of large Behavioral flexibility and species invasion: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis.
brains. Biology Letters, 5, 130e133. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 22, 393e404.
Sol, D. 2013. Behavioural flexibility for a life in the city. Animal Behaviour, 85,1101e1112. Wund, M. A., Baker, J. A., Clancy, B., Golub, J. L. & Foster, S. A. 2008. A test of the
Sol, D., Timmermans, S. & Lefebvre, L. 2002. Behavioural flexibility and invasion ‘Flexible stem’ model of evolution: ancestral plasticity, genetic accommodation,
success in birds. Animal Behaviour, 63, 495e502. and morphological divergence in the threespine stickleback radiation. American
Sol, D., Duncan, R. P., Blackburn, T. M., Cassey, P. & Lefebvre, L. 2005. Big brains, Naturalist, 172, 449e462.
enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings Wund, M. A., Valena, S., Wood, S. & Baker, J. A. 2012. Ancestral plasticity and allometry
of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 102, 5460e5465. in threespine stickleback reveal phenotypes associated with derived, freshwater
Sol, D., Szekely, T., Liker, A. & Lefebvre, L. 2007. Big-brained birds survive better in ecotypes. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 105, 573e583.
nature. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 763e769. Young, R. L. & Badyaev, A. V. 2010. Developmental plasticity links local adaptation
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M. & Lefebvre, L. 2008. Brain size predicts the success and evolutionary diversification in foraging morphology. Journal of Experimental
of mammal species introduced into novel environments. American Naturalist, Zoology B, 314, 434e444.
Supplement, 172, S63eS71. Zera, A. J. & Denno, R. F. 1997. Physiology and ecology of dispersal polymorphism in
Sporns, O., Tononi, G. & Edelman, G. M. 2000. Connectivity and complexity: the insects. Annual Review of Entomology, 42, 207e230.
relationship between neuroanatomy and brain dynamics. Neural Networks, 13, Zohary, E. 1992. Population coding of visual stimuli by coritcal neurons tuned to
909e922. more than one dimension. Biological Cybernetics, 66, 265e272.

S-ar putea să vă placă și