Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

Veterinary Quarterly

ISSN: 0165-2176 (Print) 1875-5941 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tveq20

Assessing the pathological extent of aggressive


behaviour in dogs. A review of the literature

C. Jacobs , T. De Keuster & P. Simoens

To cite this article: C. Jacobs , T. De Keuster & P. Simoens (2003) Assessing the pathological
extent of aggressive behaviour in dogs. A review of the literature, Veterinary Quarterly, 25:2, 53-60,
DOI: 10.1080/01652176.2003.9695145

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01652176.2003.9695145

Copyright Taylor and Francis Group, LLC

Published online: 01 Nov 2011.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 869

View related articles

Citing articles: 6 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tveq20

Download by: [97.32.128.54] Date: 08 July 2017, At: 12:48


53
1

Veterinary Quarterly 2003; 25(2): 53-60

Assessing the pathological extent of aggressive behaviour in dogs


A review of the literature

C. Jacobs", T. De Keuster2 and P. Simoensl

1Department of Morphology, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Ghent University, Belgium


20ostveldkouter 222, 9920 Lovendegem, Belgium
3Corresponding author:
Department of Morphology, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine,
Ghent University, Salisburylaan 133, 9820 Merelbeke, Belgium
E-mail: Christel.Jacobs@UGent.be

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Summary and keywords 54

Introduction 54

Aggression as a part of normal canine behaviour 54

Aggression promoting factors 54


Genetic predisposition 55
Environment 55
(Patho)physiology 55
Disease processes 55

Classification of aggressive behaviour 55


Descriptive classification 56
Functional classification 0

57
Other classification methods 57

Discussion 58

Conclusion 59

References 60
54

Assesessing the pathological extent of aggressive behaviour in dogs


A review of the literature

C. Jacobs, T. De Keuster and P. Simoens

SUMMARY
In this review the variety of parameters used for evaluating the pathological extent of aggressive
behaviour is summarised and the practical usefulness of each parameter is discussed. The selected
parameters are: the objective analytic description of the aggressive behaviour, the function of the
aggression, the presence of the three phases of a normal aggression sequence, the number of bites per
attack, the duration of the attack and the frequency of the aggressive behaviour. Other criteria such
as the appropriateness of the aggression in relation to the context, the predictability of the aggression
and the severity of the caused injury are biased because of the variation caused by numerous external
factors. The relevance of the most suitable parameters will be assessed in a further study in which the
distribution of aggression modulating neurotransmitter receptors will be determined.
Keywords: Aggression; Aggressive behaviour; Behavioural diseases; Bites; Dog behaviour; Dog diseases;
Dogs; Review.

Introduction repertory of ritualised behaviour. In this context


Canine aggression is the cause of many veterinary canine aggression is often considered to be a
consultations and is of current interest because of normal behavioural expression (5,20,26). The
the danger it involves for society. Askew (4) major criterion by which the normality of an
mentions that aggression towards humans forms aggressive incident is evaluated, is the
54% of the behaviour problems in dogs while appropriateness of the aggression to a certain
aggression towards other dogs represents 15% of situation (22).
all the cases. These facts have lead to an increa- An important note is the fact that dogs tempt to
sing interest in the etiology of aggresion. create a dominance hierachy in which it is
For scientific research in this context, it is determined which animal is allowed access to a
important to have the disposal of a reference group critical resource such as food, water, shelter and
of dogs with low aggressiveness and a test group to a mate without having to fight (11,12,33). Dogs
of dogs with pathological aggression. A fixed allow people to enter the hierarchy but maintain
protocol with unequivocal criteria is required for their repertory of behavioural patterns for the
the selection of dogs in order to obtain two communication with this other species. A wrong
homogeneous groups and reproducible test results. interpretation of interspecific behavioural signs
may lead to misunderstandings and cause aggressi-
A variety of parameters has been used in canine ve conflicts (26).
behavioural studies to classify aggressive
behaviour but these parameters are seldom linked Although aggression is a part of the normal
to an evaluation of the pathological extent of this behavioural pattern of dogs, Netto and Planta (20)
behaviour. Therefore a practical, but still strictly claim that the aggressiveness of some dogs is too
objective classification standard is required. high to be acceptable for the near environment and
for society. Such dogs can be distinguished by
In this review the concept of canine aggression assessing the aggression score by means of
will be approached from different angles, and the aggression tests. Aggression tests are based on the
importance and practical use of each point of view ease by which aggressive behaviour can be elicited
will be discussed.
in an extensive range of situations and contexts
(20).
Aggression as a part of normal canine
behaviour Aggression promoting factors
Dogs are social animals with an extensive Behaviour is influenced by a variety of factors
55

such as genetic predisposition, environment spayed females than in intact bitches (7,22,33).
including experience and learning processes, The role of the male sex hormone testosteron in
(patho)physiology and disease processes (8,12,32). these gender-related behavioural differences cannot
The wide range of aggression promoting factors be ignored (22).
illustrates the low specificity of the term Hoerlein (13) however states that castration of
aggression (6). male dogs only affects the intermale aggression
type while territorial and fear-induced aggression
Genetic predisposition remain unaltered.
The significantly higher incidence of behaviour According to Voith and Borchelt (33) the higher
problems involving aggression in certain canine incidence of aggression in neutered females could
breeds indicates a genetic background. Blackshaw have three different causes. First, neutered bitches
(7) and Roll and Unshelm (27) mention that the are present in increasingly higher number in our
German Shepherd dog and the Bull Terrier are modern society. A second explanation for the
most frequently involved in aggressive incidents. higher aggressiveness in spayed females could be
Sherman et al. (29) and Uchida et al. (31) add the the fact that sterilisation is often the first therapeu-
Doberman Pinscher and the Lhasa Apso to this tical approach to behaviour problems involving
list. Ushida et al. (31) mention the English aggression. A third explanation could be that some
Springer Spaniel additionally. bitches are androgenised in utero and spaying of
Voith and Borchelt (33) perceive a higher these animals may facilitate the expression of their
aggressiveness in pure-bred dogs compared to masculine behaviour (22,33).
mixed breed dogs. In contrast, Beaver (6) descri- The expression of masculine behaviour in neutered
bes a higher incidence of aggressiveness in mixed females is more pronounced when sterilisation is
breed dogs. performed before the age of 12 months (19,26).
Other aspects with aggression promoting
Environment characteristics are neurochemical dysfunctions (23)
An important environmental factor in the genesis and aging, especially when allied to a loss of
of canine aggressive behaviour is a lack of early visual and hearing acuity (19).
experience during the socialisation period (3,12).
Adult dogs are more aggressive towards species Disease processes
with which they have had little contact in early Medical conditions that may affect aggressiveness
life, especially 3 to 12 weeks after birth (12). include neoplasia of the central nervous system,
Another environmental factor with a considerable infectious diseases (canine distemper, rabies),
influence on the development of aggression is the epilepsy, developmental disorders (lissencephaly,
teaching of positive and negative behaviour by the hydrocephaly), metabolic (hyper- or hypothyroidy)
owner or by certain training programs. Aggressi- and cardiovascular disorders, trauma and pain
veness can for example be taught intentionally to processes (6,12,13,32). Disorders of the central
fighting dogs, whereas behavioural therapy may nervous system can induce aggressive behaviour in
have an appeasing effect on hyperreactive dogs several species, especially when located in the
(12). frontal cortex, the hypothalamus, the thalamus, the
amygdaloid body, the medial mamillary nucleus,
Other potential aggression promoting factors are the habenular nuclei, the hippocampus and the
the restraint from exerting breed-specific tasks caudate nucleus (9,14,16,17,21,24). These regions
such as hunting or herding, or food scarcity during are illustrated in figures 1 through 4. The specific
gestation (10). involvement of the frontal cortex (1),
hypothalamus (15) and amygdaloid body (2,13) in
(Patho)physiology behavioural changes in dogs has been established.
The higher aggressiveness presented by male dogs The behaviour modula-ting properties of the
is very suggestive for the importance of the caudate nucleus in dogs can be assumed because of
physiological state in the expression of behaviour its connections with the frontal cortex (30).
(12,26,31,33). Furthermore, aggression is often
more pronounced in intact males compared to Classification of aggressive behaviour
castrated dogs and, although in a lesser amount, in There are different ways to classify aggressive
56

Figure 1. Medial view of the right brain half of a dog Figure 2. Rostrolateral view of the canine brain. The
showing important brain regions involved in aggression. left frontal lobe has been removed to visualise the
caudate nucleus (9).

Figure 3. Rostrolateral view of the canine brain. The Figure 4. Medial view of the right hemisphere with a
rostral part of the left hemisphere has been removed by lateral view of the dissected left hippocampus (8).
a transversal incision through the piriform lobe (10).
The position of the amygdaloid body (5) has been tinted
for better visualisation.

Legend to figures 1-4:


1. Frontal cortex 2. Cingulate gyms 3. Hypothalamus 4. Thalamus 5. Amygdaloid body
6. Mamillary body 7. Habenula . 8. Hippocampus 9. Caudate nucleus 10. Piriform lobe .

behaviour patterns. First a distinction is made systems, in aggression is classified


which
between intra- and interspecies aggression based according to the form or the context, respectively,
on the species, dog and human respectively, that is are discussed underneath.
the target of the aggressive insult (6,12).
In contrast to other authors, Askew (4) defines Descriptive classification
canine aggression directed towards dogs and The descriptive or empirical-operative classifica-
humans as intraspecies aggression when the dog tion system aims to describe the various
accounts his target as a member of his own behavioural patterns objectively in order to avoid
species, whereas interspecies aggression is directed premature conclusions based on the observer's
to a target that is not considered as belonging to early intuitive table 1 the
impressions. In
his kind. The objectivity of this classification is behavioural components involved in aggressive
challenged however, since the species perception behaviour are summarised. It shows that a single
of the individual dog is difficult to be estimated act can be the expression of several behavioural
unambiguously. ,7?
patterns. Biting and growling for ,example are
The descriptive and the functional classification expressed during both aggression and play.
57

Table 1. Descriptive classification of aggression warning phase (growling, exhibiting teeth,


(according to Borchelt and Voith, 1982) posture), an action phase (biting) and a stop signal
(appeasing signal). Fear induced aggression is
Behaviour Possible etiologies often devoid of a clear warning phase, but facial
Biting (snap, nip) Aggression expression and/or posture and autonomic nervous
Play reactions such as trembling, mydriasis, urination
and defecation lead to the diagnose of this type of
Growling Aggression aggression.
Play The sixth or descriptive class is identified as
Baring teeth (lift lip) Aggression instrumentalised aggression and is characterised by
Barking (whine, howl) Aggression
a loss of the normal aggression sequence. The
warning phase and/or the stop signal disappear(s)
Separation behaviour
Play
and only the action phase is constantly present.
Each of the five functional classes may evolve to
Reaction to exciting stimuli
this sixth class of instrumentalised aggression (5).
Social facilitation
Fear
The altered aggression sequence indicates that the
aggression has reached a pathological level. The
Functional classification prognosis worsens in proportion to the loss of
sequence integrity (5).
The functional classification of aggressive Instrumentalised aggression has also been
behaviour is the most commonly used method. It described by Moyer (18) who defined it as a
is based on the emotional/motivational incentives learning process with an increased tendency for
of the aggression. With the functional classification exhibiting aggressive behaviour in response to
scheme an attempt is made to deduce the function certain stimuli.
of the aggression from its context and from the
relationship between stimulus and response (8). Other classification methods
Moyer (18) has introduced the functional classifi-
Reisner et al. (25) mention other features that can
cation scheme in the conviction that the different
be used for the classification of aggression. One of
aggression types have a specific neurological and
endocrine background. Many authors have
these features is 'the relative degree of inhibition
extended or adjusted this initial classification with
or disinhibition' by which a distinction is made
between attacks that are limited to a single bite and
the consequence that the aggression types
attacks with multiple bites. The predictability of
acknowledged by the different authors display a the aggression could also be used as a
considerable diversification (table 2). Possessive
classification parameter (25).
aggression for example is considered as a separate
entity by Borchelt and Voith (8) and Overall (22), Reisner (26) and Sacks et al. (28) describe a
while other authors include it within the classification based on the severity of the caused
dominance aggression type. injury. The duration of the attack is another
possible criterion proposed by Reisner (26) for
Each type of aggression is attended with a
assessing the pathological extent of aggression.
description of the specific behavioural pattern and The frequency of aggressive insults is used as an
the possible situations in which this particular form
additional parameter by Blackshaw (7) and Reisner
of aggression can occur (8). et al. (25).
A remarkable approach of aggression classification
is given by Beata (5) who combined the descrip- Discussion
tive and functional classification systems. This The precise evaluation of aggression in dogs is
author describes five functional classes supplemen- difficult because of the large number of factors
ted by a sixth descriptive class. The functional that have to be considered. Aggression is part of
classes are: predatory aggression, irritation the normal canine behaviour and there is a
aggression, territorial and maternal aggression manifest difference of aggressiveness between
(grouped into 1 class), fear aggression and hierar- several subpopulations of dogs. In the scientific
chical aggression. search for disorders that can affect the mechanisms
A normal aggression sequence always exhibits a of aggressive behaviour it is desirable to select
58

Table 2. The functional classification system of aggression types


Moyer (18) Borchelt and Hart and Blackshaw (7) Beaver (6) Overall (22) Beata (5)
Voith (8) Hart (12)

Dominance Dominance Dominance Dominance Dominance Hierarchical


Fear-induced Fear-induced Fear-induced Fear-induced Fear-induced Fear-induced Fear-induced
Territorial Protective Territorial Territorial Protective Territorial Territorial +
,
, maternal
Maternal Maternal Maternal Maternal Maternal . Territorial +
maternal
Predatory Predatory Predatory Predatory Predatory Predatory Predatory
Irritation Irritation
Instrumentalised Instrumentalised
Intermale Intermale/ Intermale Intermale Intrasexual Intercanine
interfemale
Learned Learned
Medical .

Pain-induced Pain-induced Pain-induced Pain-induced


Play Play
Redirected Redirected Redirected
Sex related
Idiopathic Idiopathic Idopathic
Sibling rivalry
Possesive - Possesive
Punishment-elicited
Competitive
- Food related

dogs that most likely exhibit a pathological form strongly dependent on public opinion and on the
of aggression, defined as a form that does not extent to which the environment is familiar with
comply with the normal behaviour code. To the behavioural patterns of dogs in general. This
determine the pathological state of the aggression a perception is liable to a subjectivity that needs to
specific set of criteria should be applied for each be avoided in scientific research. This does not
subpopulation. For example, aggression of a dog exclude the fact that functional classification
belonging to a more aggressive breed or strain schemes are highly valuable in this context for
-
may not be evaluated with equal measures as the practical diagnostics where therapy and prognosis
same degree of aggression performed by a dog are of prior interest.
from a less aggressive breed or strain.
It is obvious that the predictability of aggressive
Therefore numerous factors such as breed, gender,
behaviour, which is suggested as a criterion for the
early experience and the ability to perform breed
classification of aggression by Reisher et al. (25),
specific tasks need to be taken into account for
suffers from the same shortcomings as mentioned
each individual dog. For example, aggression by a
for the appropriateness of aggressive behaviour.
hunting dog should be considered less severe when
Aggression will be more predictable to a person
the dog is not used for this purpose.
who is familiar with canine behaviour patterns in
Another issue is the appropriateness of the general and who can distinguish subtle signals.
aggression in relation to the context. The tolerance The considerable amount of subjectivity undermi-
for a certain aggressive behaviour pattern is nes the reliability of this parameter.
59

The severity of the caused injury as proposed by behaviour repertory and are capable to correctly
Reisner (26) and Sacks et al. (28) is also a dubious interpret subtle signals. This is in particular the
parameter for assessing the pathological extent of case for fear-induced aggression which is
aggressive behaviour, because it is strongly characterised by a limited warning phase and could
influenced by various additional factors such as the therefore be falsely defined as instrumentalised
strength of the attacking dog and of the victim, the aggression. However, the warning phase of
predictability of the assault, the relative degree of fear-induced aggression can be recognised by
inhibition or disinhibition of the dog, and the facial and/or postural expressions and autonomic
duration of the attack. nervous reactions.
The descriptive classification is essential for an The number of bites per attack as suggested by
unbiased evaluation of animal behaviour in order Reisner et al. (25) could be a reliable parameter
to avoid false determination of different behaviour because it yields information about the relative
patterns as aggression. It forces the evaluator to degree of bite inhibition of the dog. Since some
set up a clear and objective description of the dog strains are genetically selected for a reduced
behaviour patterns before any interpretation is bite inhibition, one may assume a considerable
made. Therefore this classification should be
genetical determination of this parameter.
included in every evaluation protocol.
Other factors, such as the duration of the attack as
There is also a strong motivation for determining mentioned by Reisner (26) and the frequency of
the function of aggressive behaviour using a aggressive behaviour as suggested by Blackshaw
functional classification because the various (7) and Reisner et al. (25), could also be related to
functional aggression types are suggested to have a the pathological extent of aggressive behaviour and
specific neurological and/or endocrine basis (18). should therefore be taken into account for research
Because many factors affect
purposes. However, the frequency of the assaults
aggression, a
simplified approach is desired for the practical is often difficult to determine because some dogs
evaluation of the pathological extent of aggressive are euthanised after the first aggressive incident
behaviour. A suitable method is given by Beata (5) while in other cases attempts are made to solve the
who describes a functional classification system problem by behaviour therapy and/or medication.
combined with a descriptive class of
instrumentalised aggression which is characterised Conclusion
by the absence of a warning phase and/or stop Several criteria described in the literature are
signal. Each functional class can evolve to this suitable for the practical classification of
instrumentalised aggression. A major advantage of aggressive behaviour. Some of these parameters
this approach is the fact that it offers for all the could give information about the pathological
functional classes a general threshold, namely the extent of this behaviour. They include the objecti-
loss of the aggression sequence integrity, by which ve description of the behaviour pattern, the deter-
the pathological level of the aggression can be mination of the function of the aggression, the eva-
determined and a prognosis can be made. luation of the presence of the three phases of the
The aggression sequence can be altered either at normal aggression sequence, and the assessment of
the first aggressive assault or after an evolutionary the degree of inhibition or disinhibition. Additional
process in which a dog that previously exhibited a parameters include the duration of the attack and
normal aggression sequence undergoes a gradual the frequency of the aggressive behaviour.
reduction of the warning phase and/or the stop The presence of a normal aggression sequence is
signal over a certain period of time until these the only parameter of which a direct link to the
phases become absent. pathological level of the aggression is mentioned.
The approach of Beata (5) has the additional Other features such as the appropriateness of the
advantage that canine aggressiveness can be aggression in relation to the context, the
assessed under various conditions (different predictability of the aggressive assaults and the
breeds, sexes, environmental factors), since dogs severity of the caused injury interfere with a
with a high intrinsic aggressiveness are still uniform classification because they are strongly
expected to exhibit the normal aggression dependent on several external factors.
sequence. Further neurochemical research is planned for
It is important, however, that the classification of studying the link between the various parameters
dogs with instrumentalised aggression is performed and the pathological extent of aggressive behaviour
by experts who are familiar with the normal canine in dogs.
60

REFERENCES 17. Martinez Martinez PFA. Neuro-anatomy. Development


1. Allen BD, Cummings JF and De Lahunta A. The effects and anatomy of the central nervous system, 1980.
of prefrontal lobotomy on aggressive behavior in dogs. Wetenschappelijke uitgeverij Bunge, Utrecht (in Dutch).
Cornell Veterinarian 1974; 64: 201-216. 18. Moyer KE. Kinds of aggression and their physiological
2. Anderson B and Olson K. Effects of bilateral amygdaloid basis. Communications in Behavioral Biology, Part A2,
1968. Pages 65-87,
lesions in nervous dogs. Journal of Small Animal Practice
1965: 6: 301-308. 19. Neilson JC, Hart BL and Ruehl WW. Selecting, raising,
3. Appleby D. The importance of early socialisation and and caring for dogs to avoid problem aggression. Journal
habituation in the dog and how to accomplish it. In: "A of the American Veterinary Medical Association 1997;
good start as prevention of behavioural problems in dogs" 210: 1129-1134.
("Een goede start als preventie van gedragsproblemen bij 20. Netto WJ and Planta DJU. Behavioural testing for
honden"). Symposium 19 October, 2002, Merelbeke. aggression in the domestic dog. Applied Animal
4. Askew HR. Introduction to aggression problems. Treat- Behaviour Science 1997; 52, 243-263.
ment of behavior problems in dogs and cats. A guide for 21. Nickel R, Schummer A. and Seiferle E. Textbook of the
the small animal veterinarian, 1996 Blackwell Science, Anatomy in Domestic Animals. Volume IV, 3rd ed.
Oxford. Pages 96-108. Nervous system, sense organs and endocrine glands,
5. Beata CA. Diagnosis and treatment of aggression in dogs 1992. Verlag Paul Parey. Berlin und Hamburg (in
and cats. In: Recent Advances in Companion Animal German).
Behavior Problems. Ed. by K.A. Houpt. International 22. Overall KL. Canine aggression. In: Clinical Behavioral
Veterinary Information Service, Ithaca, New York, 2001. Medicine for Small Animals, 1997. Ed. by K.L. Overall.
6. Beaver BV. Profiles of dogs presented for aggression. Mosby, St. Louis. Pages 88-137.
Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 23. Overall KL. Pharmacological treatment in behavioural
2001: 29: 564-569. , medicine: The importance of neurochemistry, molecular
7. Blackshaw JK. An overview of types of aggressive biology and mechanistic hypotheses. Veterinary Journal
behaviour in dogs and methods of treatment. Applied 2001; 162: 9-23.
Animal Behaviour Science 1991: 30: 351-361. 24. Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives
8. Borchelt PL and Voith VL. Classification of animal of Neurology and Psychiatry 1937; 38: 725-743.
behavior problems. Veterinary Clinics of North America. 25. Reisner IR, Hollis N and Houpt KA. Risk factors for
Small Animal Practice 1982; 12: 571-585. behavior-related euthanasia among dominant-aggressive
9. Brodal A. Neurological Anatomy in Relation to Clinical dogs: 110 cases (1989-1992). Journal of the American
Medicine, 1969, 2nd ed. Oxford University Press. Veterinary Medical Assiciation 1994; 205: 855-863.
10. Coopman F, Struelens E, Audenaert K, odberg FO, 26. Reisner IR. Assessment, management, and prognosis of
Peremans K, Jacobs K, Haegeman A, Pee !man L, Van canine dominance-related aggression. Veterinary Clinics
Zeveren A, Van Bree H, Dierckx K, and Verschooten F. of North America. Small Animal Practice 1997; 27:
Agression in the dog (Canis familiaris); problems related 479-495,
to genetic manipulation. Vlaams Diergeneeskundig 27. Roll A and Unshelm J. Aggressive conflicts amongst dogs
Tijdschrift 2001; 70: 459-474 (in Dutch). and factors affecting them. Applied Animal Behaviour
11. Dodman NH, Moon R and Zelin M. Influence of owner Science 1997; 52: 229-242.
personality type on expression and treatment outcome of 28. Sacks JJ, Lockwood R, Hornreich J and Sattin RW. Fatal
dominance aggression in dogs. Journal of the American dog attacks, 1989-1994. Pediatrics 1996; 97: 891-895.
Veterinary Medical Association 1996; 209: 1107-1109. 29. Sherman CK, Reisner IR, Taliaferro LA and Houpt KA.
12. Hart BL and Hart LA. Aggressive behavior in dogs. In: Characteristics, treatment, and outcome of 99 cases of
Canine and Feline Behavioral Therapy, 1985. Edited by aggression between dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour
B.L. Hart and L.A. Hart. Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. Science 1996; 47: 91-108.
Pages 29-55. 30. Tanaka D. Neostriatal projections from cytoarchitectoni-
13. Hoerlein BF. Canine Neurology. Diagnosis and cally defined gyri in the prefrontal cortex of the dog.
Treatment, 1978. W.B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia Journal of Comparative Neurology 1987; 261: 48-73.
- London - Toronto. 31. Uchida Y, Dodman N, DeNapoli J and Aronson L.
14. Jenkins TW. Functional Mammalian Neuroanatomy, Characterization and treatment of 20 canine dominance
1978: 2nd ed. Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. Pages aggression cases. Journal of Veterinary Medical Science
282-301. 1997; 59: 397-399. .

15. Kaada B. Brain mechanisms related to aggressive 32. Voith VL and Borchelt PL. Introduction to animal
behavior. Aggression and defense : neural mechanisms behavior therapy. Veterinary Clinics of North America.
and social patterns, 1967. Clemente & Lindsley, Small Animal Practice 1982; 12: 565-570.
University of California Press, California. 33. Voith VL and Borchelt PL. Diagnosis and treatment of
16. Lewin W. Observations on selective leucotomy. Journal dominance aggression in dogs. Veterinary Clinics of
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1961; 24: North America. Small Animal Practice 1982; 12:
37-44. 655-663.

S-ar putea să vă placă și