Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

DOI 10.1007/s10826-017-0800-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

Early Feeding, Child Behaviour and Parenting as Correlates of


Problem Eating
Michelle Adamson1 Alina Morawska2

Published online: 15 June 2017


© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Abstract Mealtimes are a common source of stress for (Behaviour: F(1187) = 42.36, p < .001; Setting: F(1187) =
families. Examining factors related to problem eating may 10.64, p = .001). Evidence of feeding issues in infancy may
provide markers by which to identify families requiring support early detection of and intervention for later problem
assistance and salient targets for treatment. The current eating. The significance of broader child behaviour is less
study investigated parenting practices and cognitions, gen- clear. Parent factors, particularly those specific to meal-
eralisation of child behavioural issues, and early feeding times, and cognitive in nature (including mealtime parent-
history as they relate to problem eating in typically devel- ing self-efficacy) clearly differentiated the groups, and
oping young children. We compared a community sample represent important targets for intervention.
of 105 parents of 1.5–6-year-old children via survey and
observation with 96 parents seeking treatment for their Keywords Feeding Child Parent Mealtimes
● ● ● ●

child’s problem eating. History of problems with breast- Behaviour


feeding, χ2(1) = 3.88, p = .049, and the transition to solids,
χ2(1) = 7.27, p = .007, were more common among problem
eaters than comparisons. Problem eaters had a greater
number of problem behaviours outside of mealtimes,
F(1181) = 10.88, p = .001, though not more frequently than
comparisons and not to clinical levels, F(1181) = 1.81, Introduction
p = .181. Parents of problem eaters reported more unhelpful
mealtime parenting strategies, F(1155) = 22.59, p < .001, Problem eating is common during early childhood (de Moor
yet general parenting style was similar by group, F(1187) = et al. 2007b). Refusing to eat a reasonable range or amount
0.42, p = .527. Parents’ cognitions about mealtimes, of food, or to perform behaviour necessary for eating (such
F(1155) = 119.81, p < .001, including mealtime-specific as self-feeding and manipulating food and utensils appro-
self-efficacy, F(1155) = 171.30, p < .001, were poorer priately) can cause significant stress for parents (Greer et al.
amongst problem eaters, and were the only factors to predict 2008), and may result in physical and health detriments
problem eating in the total sample. General parenting self- (e.g., de Moor et al. 2007b) and long-standing eating issues
efficacy was poorer in parents of problem eaters (Marchi and Cohen 1990). Identification of effective treat-
ments is therefore important, and should be guided by
factors thought to cause and maintain feeding issues. Var-
ious medical, developmental and oral/motor factors have
* Michelle Adamson been implicated in the development and maintenance of
michelle.adamson@usq.edu.au
feeding problems (e.g., Johnson and Harris 2004; Williams
1
School of Psychology and Counselling, University of Southern et al. 2008). However, feeding problems often persist
Queensland, Toowoomba, QLD, Australia beyond remission of (de Moor et al. 2005) or independent
2
Parenting and Family Support Centre, School of Psychology, The of these biological or developmental factors (e.g., de Moor
University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia et al. 2007b; Lewinsohn et al. 2005).
3168 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

Various antecedents and consequences in the mealtime attributions about feeding may act as a distal mechanism in
environment, in particular those administered by parents, the maintenance of feeding difficulties by determining
have been associated with feeding problems (Blissett et al. parental responses to child mealtime behaviour (Berlin et al.
2011; de Moor et al. 2005). Aversive parenting strategies 2009; Chavira et al. 2000). Research has linked childhood
such as berating, threatening and force feeding have been feeding issues with parents’ causal inferences for feeding
associated with fussy eating (Crist and Napier-Phillips (e.g., due to temperament, or being a good or bad parent;
2001; de Moor et al. 2007b) and food refusal (Orrell- Bramhagen et al. 2006) and concern about childhood obe-
Valente et al. 2007), and the use of physical punishment, sity (Pugliese et al. 1987) though, like the parent cognition
criticism and negative social attention with child non- literature more generally, research into parent cognitions in
compliance (Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993). More permissive the specific areas of feeding is limited (Farrow and Blissett
strategies like coaxing, providing alternate meals and 2006). Likewise, despite a wealth of anecdotal evidence
allowing food between meals have been associated with suggesting parents lack confidence at the task of feeding
selective eating (Crist and Napier-Phillips 2001; Williams (e.g., de Moor et al. 2007a), we found only two studies that
et al. 2008). Little or poorly timed praise, more frequent yet have investigated parental self-efficacy amongst parents of
less specific instruction (Sanders et al. 1997) and offering problem eaters (Dunne et al. 2007; Sanders et al. 1997).
alternate food have been shown to differentiate parents of This research did not find differences in parental self-
picky eaters (Carruth et al. 1998) and those with broader efficacy between parents of problem eaters and compar-
feeding issues from comparisons (Crist and Napier-Phillips isons, though each measured parents’ perceptions of their
2001; Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993). These are thought to competence in the parenting role broadly; it remains to be
result from negative cycles of interaction common between seen whether self-efficacy is lower in relation to the task of
problem eaters and their parents, whereby problem eating feeding specifically.
invites increasingly coercive responses from parents to Further study is thus needed of the correlates of problem
force the child to eat, resulting in even more avoidant and eating, particularly among young children who are devel-
disruptive behaviour by the child (Sanders, Patel, et al. oping normally (de Moor et al. 2007b; Nicholls et al. 2001),
1993). Parenting practices are a significant predictor of child to better inform assessment and intervention. The present
feeding problems (Johnson and Harris 2004; Sanders, Patel, study investigated particular child and parent factors as they
et al. 1993), and suggested as more powerful than satiety relate to problem eating in a sample of 201 parents of
mechanisms in regulating children’s intake (Mrdjenovic and typically developing young children, 96 of whom were
Levitsky 2005). Behavioural interventions based on mod- concerned about and seeking assistance for their child’s
ifying such mechanisms have been shown to reduce pro- eating. Of primary interest were mealtime-specific parenting
blem eating (Adamson et al. 2013; Morawska et al. 2014). practices, cognitions and self-efficacy: specifically, parents
While there is clear evidence for the effects of parenting of problem eaters were predicted to report greater use of
on child mealtime problems, the existing literature on maladaptive mealtime parenting strategies, and greater
parent–child interactions at mealtimes has tended to focus endorsement of unhelpful parental cognitions about meal-
on children with chronic illness or disability (Crist and times than comparisons, in addition to lower self-efficacy
Napier-Phillips 2001), feeding disorder, or obesity (Wil- about parenting at mealtimes. Of secondary interest were
liams et al. 2008). While these are of great clinical impor- non-mealtime variables: we expected that self-efficacy
tance, limited conclusions may be drawn for children who related to the task of parenting generally would be similar
are otherwise healthy and typically developing, and who by group given earlier findings, though predictions related
represent a larger number of children (Lewinsohn et al. to general child behaviour (including external to mealtimes)
2005). Likewise, while general parenting style has been and how parents respond to child behaviour (parenting
modestly correlated with child mealtime behaviour (de style) by group were exploratory.
Moor et al. 2007a), examination of this construct amongst
parents of problem eaters is relatively limited. Questions
also remain regarding whether problem eaters display Method
broader behavioural issues, with studies suggesting general
child behaviour issues among problem eaters (McDermott Participants
et al. 2008; Sanders et al. 1997) and improved general
behaviour following an 8-week mealtime intervention Participants were self-nominated parents of young Aus-
(Adamson et al. 2013) but not a single session intervention tralian children with and without feeding concerns drawn
(Morawska et al. 2014). from a broader research program, including an intervention
Of emerging interest are parental cognitions (Morawska trial for children with problem eating (Adamson et al.
and Sanders 2007). Parental beliefs, expectations and 2013). Both controls and problem eaters were sought via the
J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178 3169

same recruitment materials advertising the research to par- Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of the Sample
ents at general practice clinics, childcare centres, kinder- Variable Comparisons Problem χ2 (df) p
gartens, schools and community health centres in regional N (%) eaters
and metropolitan Queensland and on a number of Aus- N (%)
tralian parenting websites. Parents of children aged Marital status 105 96 5.04 (4) .283
approximately 1.5–6 years, without complex medical or Single 1 (0.95) 3 (3.13)
developmental conditions, and not already receiving pro- Married 84 (80.00) 82 (85.42)
fessional assistance for behaviour and/or feeding were eli- Defacto 15 (14.29) 7 (7.29)
gible to participate. A total of 201 families participated Separated 5 (4.76) 3 (3.13)
including 96 parents who responded affirmatively to the
Divorced 0 1 (1.04)
question, Are you concerned about your child’s eating? and
Household 105 94 3.60(3) .308
were seeking assistance for this (problem eaters) and 105
Original 100 (95.24) 87 (92.55)
families who indicated no concern about their child’s
Step 0 1 (1.06)
feeding and/or were not seeking assistance (non-problem
Sole parent 5 (4.76) 6 (6.38)
eaters) and thus acted as comparisons. Data used in the
Education 102 93 4.41(4) .353
current study was cross-sectional, collected at baseline of
the intervention study, in order to compare parents of pro- Year 10/11 5 (4.90) 7 (7.53)
blem eaters with a control sample. Parents of problem eaters Year 12 8 (7.84) 9 (9.68)
progressed on to the intervention study so additionally were TAFE/College 18 (17.65) 15 (16.13)
living in the geographical areas where the intervention was Trade/Apprenticeship 0 3 (3.23)
offered (regional and metropolitan Queensland). Tertiary 71 (69.61) 59 (63.44)
Table 1 displays demographic data. Respondents were Work hours per week 102 94 7.16 (5) .209
mostly mothers (98.51%) within original two-parent None 33 (32.35) 33 (35.11)
families with 1–5 children, who had been able to meet <10 h 12 (11.76) 9 (9.57)
household expenses in the last 12 months. Male and female 10–20 h 17 (16.67) 10 (10.64)
children were approximately equally represented, with a 20–30 h 23 (22.55) 22 (23.40)
mean age of 2.81 years (SD = 1.01). Problem eaters were 30–40 h 15 (14.71) 14 (14.89)
older (M = 3.14 years, SD = 1.10) than comparisons (M = >40 h 2 (1.96) 6 (6.38)
2.52 years, SD = 0.81), t(173.33) = −4.52, p < .001. Pro- Able to meet expenses 103 94 0.00 (1) .975
blem eating ranged in duration from several months to Yes 93 (90.29) 85 (90.43)
years, and included refusing to self-feed, leaving the table, No 10 (9.71) 9 (9.57)
food refusal and selective eating. Most parents in this group
t = Independent samples t-test; χ = Pearson’s chi-square. Numbers in
2
were a bit (45.8%) or quite (36.5%) concerned about their each analysis vary due to missing data. TAFE technical and further
child’s eating on the above question. education

Procedure

Participants were screened via telephone, at which time covering child age, gender, family composition, parental
eligible and consenting parents were directed to online education and employment, early feeding history (Did you
measures and an observation was scheduled for the next experience any of the following in their 1st year: Difficulty
available opportunity. On completion of the measures, breastfeeding? Difficulty moving from breast/bottle to solid
parents in the comparison group were thanked for their foods?), parental concern about the child’s eating, and
participation by receiving general feedback and a movie/ whether the parent wanted assistance with mealtimes.
food voucher. Families in the problem eaters group pro- Child feeding behaviour, mealtime parenting strategies
gressed on to the intervention study to receive a free and parental cognitions were assessed via the 90-item Par-
mealtime intervention. The research was conducted with ent and Toddler Feeding Assessment (PATFA; Adamson
ethical approval from The University of Queensland. and Morawska 2016). Parents first rated 21 common child
feeding problems (e.g., spitting food out) in three ways: a)
Measures frequency, on a five-point scale from 1 (never) to 5 (almost
always); b) whether each was problematic (yes/no); and c)
Parents completed a suite of measures electronically. This their confidence in managing each on a 10-point scale
included a demographic questionnaire adapted from the (higher scores indicating greater self-efficacy). Parents then
Family Background Questionnaire (Sanders et al. 2001) rated how frequently they used 30 strategies at mealtimes
3170 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

on the five-point scale above. This included strategies ≥3.2 was employed for the total score based on Arnold et al.
deemed helpful (e.g., eating with the child) and unhelpful (1993).
(e.g., force-feeding), with the former reverse-scored to give Parental self-efficacy was assessed by the Parenting
a measure of the frequency of maladaptive mealtime par- Tasks Checklist (PTC; Sanders and Woolley 2005). Parents
enting strategies. Finally, parents rated their level of rated their confidence in managing 28 challenging child
agreement with 39 statements related to cognitive and behaviours (e.g., tantrums) and settings (e.g., shopping) on
emotional aspects of feeding (e.g., I feel defeated by my a ten-point scale (10 being most confident). Scores were
child), including four statements relevant to partners (e.g., averaged into two subscales: Behavioural self-efficacy and
my child’s behaviour at mealtimes upsets my partner), on a setting self-efficacy, both with excellent internal con-
five-point scale, higher scores indicating stronger agree- sistency in the current study (α = .97 and .92 respectively).
ment. This included statements deemed helpful (e.g., per- The PTC has been shown to distinguish mothers seeking
ception of supports) and unhelpful (e.g., attributing feeding assistance for their child’s behaviour from controls (Sanders
issues to deliberate actions of the child), with the former and Woolley 2005). Community and clinic group means
reverse-scored to give a measure of agreement with from Sanders and Woolley 2005 were used to assess
unhelpful parental cognitions about mealtimes. Responses Behaviour and Setting scale scores.
were summed to provide six summary scores: the frequency
of child feeding problems, the number of child feeding Observations
problems, parent mealtime self-efficacy, the frequency of
maladaptive mealtime parenting strategies, agreement with An evening meal was filmed to capture parent and child
unhelpful parental cognitions about mealtimes, and agree- mealtime behaviour. Strategies to ensure authenticity of the
ment with unhelpful cognitions related to partners. The observations included filming in the home at the time
PATFA has good test–retest reliability (r = .68–.89) and is nominated by the parent without research assistants present,
well correlated with the total score of the Children’s Eating asking parents to provide a normal meal to their child and to
Behavior Inventory (CEBI; Archer et al. 1991) (r = .50 act as they usually would, and checking afterward with
−.72; Adamson and Morawska 2016). It has been shown to parents that the mealtime was typical of the child/family
differentiate problem eaters and non-problem eaters (Janicke Mitchell and Stark 2005). When mealtimes were
(Adamson and Morawska 2016) and capture post- not reported as typical, a second observation session was
intervention change (Adamson et al. 2013; Morawska attempted, and the more typical of the two sessions was
et al. 2014). The six summary scores had adequate to coded. Most problem eaters were filmed as this was part of
excellent internal consistency (α = .65–.97) in the current the intervention study (n = 74, 77.08%; a minority either
sample. declined observation or did not continue on to the inter-
The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI; Eyberg vention study and were not coded). One-third of non-
and Pincus 1999) assessed disruptive child behaviour. problem eaters (n = 33, 31.34%) were filmed as non-
Parents rated 36 behavioural issues (e.g., whines) on a problem eaters could reside anywhere in the state, this
seven-point scale as to how often their child demonstrates number reflected those consenting to filming who lived
this behaviour (Intensity), and whether it is a problem for locally. These families did not differ on demographic and
the parent (Problem) using a yes/no format. The ECBI is a clinical variables from comparison families not observed.
sensitive measure of change with regard to child behaviour Video footage was analysed by a coder blind to study
problems, with good internal consistency in the current hypotheses and the group membership of each family using
study (α = .93 and .91 for the Problem and Intensity scales the Mealtime Observation Schedule (MOS; Sanders, Le
respectively), and test–retest reliability (r = .86; Eyberg and Grice, et al. 1993). The MOS employs a partial interval time
Robinson 1982). Clinical cut-off scores of ≥131 and ≥15 sampling procedure to record the presence of 16 child
have been suggested for the intensity and problem scales, behaviours and 14 parent behaviours in consecutive 10-s
respectively (Eyberg and Pincus 1999). intervals (see Table 2). This yielded four summary measures
The Parenting Scale (PS; Arnold et al. 1993) measured regarding the percentage of intervals in which the child
general parenting style. Parents rated their usual response to demonstrated appropriate (e.g., eating; Child positive) or
child behaviour on 30 scenarios using a seven-point scale inappropriate behaviour (e.g., noncompliance; Child nega-
between two anchors (one representing an effective tive) and the parent displayed positive (e.g., praise; Parent
response and the other ineffective). Responses were then positive) or aversive behaviour toward the child (e.g.,
summed (higher scores indicating less effective parenting negative contact; Parent negative). The MOS is an estab-
style). The total score had good internal consistency in the lished method of coding that has been shown to have good
current study (α = .87) and has good test–retest reliability interrater reliability (mean k parent codes = .83; child
(r = .84; Locke and Prinz 2002). A clinical cut-off score of codes = .80) and reliably differentiate children with and
J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178 3171

Table 2 Observational codes for Mealtime Observation Schedule (general child behaviour) and parenting factors (mealtime
(Sanders, Le Grice, et al. 1993)
parenting practices, cognitions, and self-efficacy; general
Child codes Parent codes parenting style and self-efficacy) in predicting child feeding
difficulties.
Positive codes Praise
Appropriate verbal interaction Positive contact
Engaged activity Aversive contact
Results
Affection Positive specific instruction
Appropriate eating Aversive specific instruction
Missing data was minimal (not exceeding 15.42% for any
Request for food Positive vague instruction
item, with most items missing no or few values) and there
Negative codes Aversive vague instruction were no outliers. Violations of the homogeneity of
Non-compliance Positive social attention variance–covariance matrices assumption were addressed
Complaint Aversive social attention by equalising sample size via random deletion of cases
Aversive demand Affection (Tabachnick and Fidell 1996). Between 5 and 11 cases were
Physical negative Presentation of food deleted per MANCOVA, and did not change the outcomes
Oppositional Removal of Food of analyses.
Interrupt Non-interaction We first checked if children in the problem eaters group
Food refusal Blank parent showed more problems at mealtimes. Parents of problem
Vomit eaters rated a greater number and frequency of mealtime
Playing with food issues on the PATFA than comparisons, F(2173) = 199.36,
Leaving the table p < .001 (see Table 3). Likewise, child observational data
Blank child indicated significantly lower rates of positive behaviour at
mealtimes and more instances of negative behaviour among
problem eaters, F(2103) = 9.40, p < .001 (see Table 4).
without feeding difficulties (Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993).
Coders were trained over several sessions and met peri- Early Feeding History
odically throughout coding to minimise drift. Approxi-
mately 20% of tapes were randomly allocated to a second Two chi-square tests compared problem eaters to non-
coder and interrater reliability values were calculated based problem eaters on early feeding history. A history of
on correlations between raters in terms of the presence or breastfeeding problems was reported significantly more
absence of each code in each interval. Kappa was not often in problem eaters (38.30%, n = 36) than comparisons
appropriate as base rates of behaviour codes were low. (25.24%, n = 26), χ2(1) = 3.88, p = .049. Problem eaters
Interrater correlations for the summary codes were moderate were also more likely to have had problems when solids
to high (r = .45−.79). were introduced, χ2(1) = 7.27, p = .007, with 20% of pro-
blem eaters (n = 19) reported to have had early difficulties
Data Analyses compared to 6.9% (n = 7) of non-problem eaters.

Preliminary analyses compared the problem and non- Mealtime-Specific Parenting Variables
problem eater groups on demographic variables to ensure
sample equivalency. The presence of early feeding pro- The four parent summary scores on the PATFA were ana-
blems was examined by group using chi-square procedures. lysed via a between-groups MANCOVA with child age as
A series of multivariate analyses of covariance (MAN- covariate. A significant main effect was found, F(4152) =
COVA) compared problem eaters to comparisons on the 53.16, p < .001, and univariate analyses indicated sig-
outcome measures in conceptually-similar groups: nificant differences on all four scores (see Table 3). Namely,
mealtime-specific parenting variables (parenting practices, parents of problem eaters reported more frequent use of
cognitions and self-efficacy, as measured by the PATFA), unhelpful mealtime strategies, F(1155) = 22.59, p < .001,
general parenting variables (parenting style and self-effi- greater agreement with more unhelpful cognitions at meal-
cacy, as measured by the PTC and PS), and general child times, F(1155) = 119.81, p < .001, including those related
behaviour (as measured by the ECBI). Child age was to partners specifically, F(1155) = 12.28, p = .001, and
included as a covariate given it differed by group. Follow- significantly lower mealtime self-efficacy than parents of
up of significant effects was done via univariate analyses of non-problem eaters, F(1155) = 171.30, p < .001.
covariance (ANCOVA). Hierarchical regression analyses Individual PATFA parent strategy and cognition items
then examined the relative importance of these child were compared by group via a series of individual t-tests,
3172 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

Table 3 Problem eaters and


Measure Comparisons Problem eaters F (df) p Cohen’s d
comparisons on parent-report
measures N M (SD) N M (SD)

PATFA
Frequency of child feeding 88 39.80 (7.36) 88 62.87 (9.79) 268.64 (1173) <.001 2.74
problem
Number of child feeding 88 1.86 (2.37) 88 11.21 (4.36) 269.91 (1173) <.001 2.78
problems
Parent mealtime self- 79 172.08 (25.99) 79 104.19 (34.73) 171.30 (1155) <.001 2.22
efficacy
Unhelpful parent mealtime 79 62.56 (9.10) 79 69.17 (12.22) 22.59 (1155) <.001 0.64
strategies
Unhelpful parent mealtime 79 79.51 (11.35) 79 99.44 (10.91) 119.81 (1155) <.001 1.80
cognitions
Unhelpful cognitions 79 8.66 (2.70) 79 10.48 (2.81) 12.28 (1155) .001 0.67
related to partners
ECBI
Intensity scale 92 103.11 (22.97) 92 114.13 (27.79) 1.81 (1181) .181 0.47
Problem scale 92 4.64 (5.37) 92 9.27 (7.46) 10.88 (1181) .001 0.77
PS
Total score 95 2.66 (0.57) 95 2.82 (0.74) 0.42 (1187) .527 0.25
PTC
Behaviour 95 8.69 (1.16) 95 6.95 (1.94) 42.36 (1187) <.001 1.09
Setting 95 8.94 (0.85) 95 8.45 (1.38) 10.64 (1187) .001 0.43
Numbers in each analysis vary due to missing data and equalisation of sample size in response to breaches of
the homogeneity of variance–covariance assumption.

Table 4 Problem eaters and


Observed behaviour (% of Comparisons Problem eaters F (df) p Cohen’s d
comparisons on observational
intervals)
measures
N M (SD) N M (SD)

Child positive 33 81.72 (13.62) 74 64.19 (22.54) 15.93 (1104) <.001 0.87
Child negative 33 18.33 (13.87) 74 33.90 (22.63) 12.80 (1104) .001 0.77
Parent positive 33 70.53 (19.84) 74 70.51 (17.81) 0.00 (1105) .995 0.00
Parent negative 33 0.50 (1.57) 74 0.67 (1.26) 0.35 (1105) .557 0.13

using a stringent p value of .01 and t-values for which equal likely to attribute feeding issues to deliberate actions of the
variances were not assumed where homogeneity of variance child, to worry about preservatives and additives, to feel
was breached. The descriptive and inferential statistics for alone and ineffective in managing mealtime issues, and to
items that differed by group are detailed in Tables 5 and 6. perceive fewer supports.
Among these, parents of problem eaters reported more No significant main effect was found for observed
frequently distracting, promising food or tangible rewards, mealtime parent behaviour, F(2104) = 0.17, p = .842 (see
and coaxing their child to encourage eating, and were more Table 4). Mean rates of each composite were very similar in
likely to offer alternative food and/or milk after the meal if each group, and observed negative behaviours rare.
the child ate poorly. Parents in this group reported eating
with their child, using mealtime rules, or providing appro- General Parenting Variables
priate food choices less often than controls, and more fre-
quently berating and force-feeding. In terms of cognitions, A significant main effect was found for the MANCOVA
parents of problem eaters more often endorsed items related comprising general parenting style (PS) and self-efficacy
to mealtime stress, fears about the consequences of poor (PTC), F(3185) = 16.35, p < .001. Follow-up univariate
eating (e.g., on weight, health, development), and parental comparisons indicated no significant differences between
disagreement over mealtimes. These parents were also more the groups on general parenting style, yet lower general
J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178 3173

Table 5 Parent strategy items on the PATFA that differed by group (p < .01)
Item Comparisons Problem eaters t (df)
(N = 103) (N = 96)
M (SD) M (SD)

Eating with your child 4.52 (0.74) 4.11 (1.06) 3.15 (168.85)
Offering something else to your child if he/she doesn’t eat the food 2.24 (1.03) 2.79 (1.25) −3.39 (197)
provided
Promising your child a food reward (e.g., a treat/dessert/favourite food) if 2.17 (1.10) 3.11 (1.31) −5.49 (197)
he/she isn’t eating well
Distracting your child (e.g., with games, TV, toys) to get him/her to eat 1.91 (1.08) 2.51 (1.26) −3.59 (187.65)
Promising your child a toy or activity (e.g., game, TV time, or outing) if 1.62 (0.91) 2.35 (1.27) −4.64 (170.79)
he/she isn’t eating well
Giving your child a bottle or milk after an unfinished meal to make sure 1.68 (0.97) 2.24 (1.28) −3.46 (177.03)
he/she gets enough
Coaxing and pleading with your child to eat more 2.18 (0.96) 3.50 (1.14) −8.77 (185.89)
Setting a goal with your child for how much they will eat 2.13 (1.24) 2.78 (1.39) −3.49 (197)
Threatening your child with consequences for not eating or misbehaving at 1.93 (0.99) 2.45 (1.24) −3.23 (182.04)
mealtimes
Allowing your child to decide when he/she has eaten enough 4.08 (1.06) 3.58 (1.06) 3.28 (197)
Providing your child with appropriate food choices at mealtimes 4.50 (0.87) 4.11 (0.92) 3.04 (197)
Yelling at or scolding your child for not eating or misbehaving at mealtimes 1.89 (0.83) 2.29 (1.08) −2.90 (177.36)
Having a set of rules around what behaviours are expected at mealtimes 3.97 (1.23) 3.47 (1.38) 2.67 (190.48)
Physically forcing food into your child’s mouth if he/she refuses to eat 1.15 (0.41) 1.42 (0.87) −2.83 (132.73)
Scale: 1 (Never)–5 (Almost Always); t = independent samples t-test

parenting self-efficacy among parents of problem eaters. Table 3 shows the means for both groups were within
Excluding two eating-specific items from the PTC Beha- normal limits.
viour scale (Your child refuses to eat their food, Your child
takes too long when eating) in order to limit the influence of Predictors of Feeding Difficulties
problem eating on the overall parenting self-efficacy score
did not alter findings, differences between the groups on the A hierarchical regression was conducted on the total sample
PTC Behaviour scale remaining significant, F(1187) = to examine the relative importance of child and parenting
20.26, p < .001. Means on the PTC Behaviour scale were factors in predicting child feeding difficulties. The overall
close to clinic means for the feeding difficulties group, model, significantly predicted problem eating, F(1146) =
while PS scale means for both groups were within normal 33.22, p < .001, accounting for 69.50% of the variance in
limits and PTC Setting scale means close to community child feeding difficulties. Results are displayed in Table 7.
means. Child age was mildly correlated with feeding concerns
(r = .373, p < .001) and predicted problem eating in the
General Child Behaviour overall model, R2 = .14, F(1155) = 25.05, p < .001. Parent
mealtime self-efficacy and cognitions likewise contributed
ECBI scores were also found to differ by group, F(2180) = significantly to prediction, ΔR2 = .39, F(3149) = 63.34,
5.73, p = .004. Follow-up ANCOVA indicated significant p < .001, with more frequent feeding issues associated
differences on the Problem scale, with parents of problem with more unhelpful cognitions about mealtimes (r = .740,
eaters reporting a greater number of disruptive child beha- p < .001) and lower parental self-efficacy about feeding
viours than non-problem eaters, though the reported fre- (r = −.742, p < .001). General disruptive child behaviour
quency of these behaviours (Intensity scale) did not differ (r = .384, p < .001) and maladaptive parenting mealtime
by group. Subtracting items relevant to eating (Dawdles or strategies (r = .297, p < .001) were each mildly correlated
lingers at mealtimes, Has poor table manners, Refuses to with and predicted problem eating at their respective steps
eat food presented) resulted in a non-significant omnibus but did not remain significant predictors in the overall
MANCOVA, F(2180) = 2.52, p = .083, though a similar model. General parenting style and self-efficacy failed to
pattern of significant group differences on the Problem but contribute significantly to the prediction of feeding issues,
not Intensity scale was suggested by univariate analysis. ΔR2 = .00, F(3146) = 0.47, p = .701.
3174 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

Table 6 Parent cognition items on the PATFA that differed by group (p < .01)
Item Comparisons Problem eaters t (df)
(N = 104) (N = 96)
M (SD) M (SD)

It is a struggle to get my child to eat 1.75 (0.86) 3.85 (1.04) −15.40 (198)
If I didn’t guide or regulate my child’s eating, he/she would eat the wrong 3.37 (1.02) 4.16 (0.87) −5.87 (196.84)
amount or types of foods
My child would eat a lot better if I just knew how to manage him/her at 2.02 (0.83) 3.55 (1.00) −11.64 (184.81)
mealtimes
Feeding my child is much harder than I thought it would be 2.18 (0.96) 3.99 (0.84) −14.01 (196.84)
I feel stressed out during mealtimes with my child 1.99 (0.89) 3.73 (1.08) −12.36 (184.19)
I enjoy mealtimes with my child 4.01 (0.84) 2.89 (0.96) 8.77 (189.53)
When my child misbehaves at mealtimes, I feel like he/she is doing it on 2.31 (0.89) 2.69 (1.03) −2.79 (198)
purpose
I feel anxious when my child does not finish their meal/snack 2.17 (0.97) 3.29 (1.01) −7.96 (198)
I feel like I am the only parent facing these problems 1.55 (0.68) 2.03 (1.00) −4.02 (198)
As long as my child eats something, I don’t care what it is 1.67 (0.72) 1.96 (0.79) −2.67 (198)
I feel defeated by my child 1.68 (0.77) 3.04 (1.09) −10.09 (168.61)
I worry that my child is underweight 1.77 (0.88) 2.52 (1.23) −4.89 (171.26)
I feel like I spend the whole meal so focused on what and how my child 2.02 (0.89) 3.31 (1.06) −9.30 (186.40)
eats, I don’t get to enjoy it
I feel confident that my child eats enough 4.20 (0.93) 2.83 (1.04) 9.77 (190.69)
I worry about my child not growing or developing properly 1.88 (1.01) 2.91 (1.14) −6.68 (190.01)
I worry about my child’s health 2.36 (1.04) 3.38 (1.10) −6.74 (198)
I am unsure what to do when my child refuses to eat 2.20 (0.90) 4.13 (0.68) −17.13 (191.41)
I have to be careful what I feed my child because of all the preservatives 3.31 (1.23) 2.82 (1.26) 2.75 (198)
and additives in food nowadays
I worry that my child’s diet lacks variety 2.37 (0.95) 4.05 (1.01) −12.18 (198)
If my child does not eat well, I feel like I’m a bad parent 2.27 (1.02) 3.18 (0.96) −6.45 (198)
I have friends and family I can ask for advice on feeding 3.89 (0.68) 3.13 (1.09) 5.94 (157.23)
My child’s behaviour at mealtimes upsets my partner# 2.38 (1.08) 3.49 (1.22) −6.58 (184)
I get upset with the way my partner deals with our child at mealtimes# 2.26 (0.98) 2.68 (1.07) −2.85 (184)
Scale: 1 (strongly disagree)–5 (strongly agree)
Analyses for these items were based on N = 98 and N = 88 respectively due to missing data t = Independent samples t-test
#

Discussion Valente et al. 2007). Parent mealtime strategies further


predicted child problem eating when introduced in the
The current study identified a number of factors differ- regression, though did not remain a significant predictor
entiating children and families with mealtime difficulties once other variables were introduced.
from comparisons. Of primary interest were parent factors, Only parent mealtime cognitions, including mealtime-
including parent mealtime practices given the role of par- specific parental self-efficacy, predicted child feeding in the
enting in problem eating (Crist and Napier-Phillips 2001; final model, which included a number of child and parent
Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993). We found parents of problem factors. This suggests that cognitions may be particularly
eaters to report less adaptive mealtime practices, consistent salient in explaining child problem eating, and may repre-
with predictions. Parents of problem eaters reported more sent key targets for intervention (Morawska and Sanders
frequent use of unhelpful strategies overall, and in particular 2007). In the current study, this included concerns about the
distracting, coaxing, berating, force-feeding, and offering potential impact of poor eating on the child, attributions of
alternative foods more often, and less frequently eating with problem eating to being a ‘bad parent’ and/or deliberate
the child, using mealtime rules and providing food choices, actions of the child, and feeling alone in managing mealtime
strategies which have been associated with problem eating issues. This extends the emerging literature on how parents
in the literature (e.g., Crist and Napier-Phillips 2001; Orrell- think and feel about mealtimes.
J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178 3175

Table 7 Hierarchical multiple


Predictor β 95% Confidence interval for β t p r sr2
regression analysis predicting
child feeding difficulties from Lower bound Upper bound
child and parent variables
Step 1
Child age 0.37 0.23 0.52 5.01 <.001 .373 .139
Step 2
Child age 0.24 0.08 0.39 2.90 .004 .373 .139
ECBI Intensity scale 0.21 0.02 0.40 2.12 .036 .384 .147
ECBI Problem scale 0.12 −0.07 0.32 1.26 .211 .362 .131
Step 3
Child age 0.31 0.15 0.46 3.91 <.001 .373 .139
ECBI Intensity scale 0.15 −0.03 0.33 1.62 .107 .384 .147
ECBI Problem scale 0.10 −0.08 0.28 1.06 .292 .362 .131
Parent mealtime strategies 0.31 0.17 0.44 4.34 <.001 .297 .088
Step 4
Child age 0.20 0.09 0.30 3.68 <.001 .373 .139
ECBI Intensity scale 0.02 −0.10 0.15 0.33 .741 .384 .147
ECBI Problem scale −0.03 −0.15 0.10 −0.41 .683 .362 .131
Parent mealtime strategies 0.02 −0.08 0.12 0.43 .669 .297 .088
Parent mealtime cognitions 0.40 0.27 0.53 6.00 <.001 .740 .548
Parent mealtime self- −0.39 −0.52 −0.26 −5.97 <.001 −.742 .551
efficacy
Cognitions about partners 0.06 −0.05 0.17 1.11 .268 .456 .208
Step 5
Child age 0.21 0.10 0.33 3.60 <.001 .373 .139
ECBI Intensity scale 0.03 −0.10 0.17 0.46 .650 .384 .147
ECBI Problem scale −0.01 −0.15 0.12 −0.21 .835 .362 .131
Parent mealtime strategies 0.03 −0.08 0.14 0.59 .554 .297 .088
Parent mealtime cognitions 0.40 0.27 0.54 5.90 <.001 .740 .548
Parent mealtime self- −0.41 −0.56 −0.26 −5.25 <.001 −.742 .551
efficacy
Cognitions about partners 0.06 −0.05 0.17 1.16 .249 .456 .208
PS Total score −0.03 −0.15 0.09 −0.46 .645 .196 .038
PTC Behaviour scale 0.08 −0.09 0.25 0.90 .368 −.471 .222
PTC Setting scale −0.05 −0.19 0.09 −0.73 .465 −.228 .052
β beta, t t-test

Ratings of parental self-efficacy related to feeding were estimate of behaviour (Bandura 1997). This would suggest
poorer among parents of problem eaters. This is consistent that parents of problem eaters feel less effective not only at
with anecdotal evidence suggesting parents of problem mealtimes but at various other parenting tasks. We did not
eaters feel ineffectual in feeding their children (e.g., de control for levels of problematic child behaviour by group
Moor et al. 2007a), and established this empirically. Parents when comparing self-efficacy, though our problem eaters
of problem eaters also rated themselves as less competent in group did not appear to have more problematic behaviour
managing other child behaviours (e.g., tantrums, interrupt- generally—while parents of problem eaters did report a
ing) and settings (e.g., bathing, shopping). This finding is in greater number of child behaviours as problematic, these
contrast to previous research which found general parental were not more frequent than comparisons nor were scores
self-efficacy similar among parents of problem eaters and above clinical cut-offs. Other research has found more
controls (Dunne et al. 2007; Sanders et al. 1997). We behavioural difficulties amongst children with feeding
applied a task-specific measure of parental self-efficacy issues (e.g., Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993; Sanders et al. 1997)
rather than using a tool which seeks more global ratings of though in each case, as in the current study, disruptive
parenting self-efficacy, which likely provided a closer behaviour was below clinical levels. This would suggest
3176 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

that, while broader behaviour may well be of concern to frequently emerge (de Moor et al. 2007b), and in fact 90%
parents, problem eating remains the primary challenge. of the sample were aged 4 years and under. That problem
Likewise, general parenting style did not differ by group eaters were on average older than controls in the current
with mean ratings within normal limits, suggesting that study may be intuitive as eating becomes more autonomous
parents of problem eaters did not identify more problematic with age, though extant research has suggested both more
styles of parenting than their non-problem eater counter- problematic feeding in the early years due to developmental
parts. The lack of generalisation of parent and child meal- constraints (Sanders, Patel, et al. 1993) and in older child-
time behaviour may support intervention with mealtime hood owing to the chronicity of concerns (Koivisto and
difficulties specifically rather than more broadly. In regards Sjoden 1996). The current study chose to control for age in
to the generalisation of mealtime-specific self-efficacy to conducting analyses, though feeding by age and the influ-
other tasks of parenting, it is possible that parents encoun- ence of parenting and other factors in this context requires
tering mealtime issues feel less effective at parenting more further research attention.
generally, consistent with the bidirectional nature of self- We acknowledge limitations in the use of parent self-
efficacy (Bandura 2001), though the direction of associa- report data generally, and particularly at a single time point
tions here is unclear. which raises concern of common method variance as an
In the current study, a greater proportion of problem explanation for relationships between variables. We
eaters had a history of difficulties with breastfeeding and the attempted to minimise this by using established ques-
transition to solids. This is consistent with evidence of a tionnaires to ensure construct validity, and various scale
trajectory between infant and later eating issues (e.g., Dahl types and formats to provide methodological separation.
and Sunderlin 1992; Farrow and Blissett 2012) and would The PATFA is a more recent measure, though child
support early detection of and intervention for feeding feeding on the PATFA was correlated with observed
problems. Data on the stability of eating behaviours over child mealtime behaviour. Use of the PATFA further
time is however generally sparse, and little is known about reflected the absence of a measure appropriate for measur-
how such chronicity relates to other constructs, for example, ing multiple domains (child and parent) related to mealtimes
parental self-efficacy. Clearly early feeding issues also need with younger children. The PATFA has now been used in
to be considered in conjunction with other factors, given several studies (e.g., Adamson et al. 2013; Morawska et al.
that this described less than half of problem eaters. 2014) with promising psychometric properties, and is the
subject of a paper in preparation (Adamson and Morawska
Limitations 2016).
Finally, observational data did not differentiate parents of
Several limitations of the study are apparent. First, the problem eaters from controls; aversive parent behaviour
sample was a reasonably homogenous group of educated was rare, and means for positive strategies similar by group.
mothers in intact marital relationships, who had volunteered This may have been the result of observer reactivity, despite
to participate. While various avenues of recruitment were research assistants not being present during filming. Fre-
used, only a small number of fathers, single parents and quency coding methods, such as the MOS, also provide
other family types participated. While this limits the con- limited information on how strategies are used (e.g., praise
clusions that can be drawn about fathering at mealtimes, the after good behaviour vs. pre-emptive praise). The interrater
absence of fathers in our sample is reflected in the child reliability obtained for the MOS was lower than reported in
development literature generally (Phares et al. 2005) and the other studies, though interrater correlations were moderate
feeding literature more specifically (Owen et al. 2010). This to high. Several of the videos for the problem eaters group
is despite evidence that fathers are increasingly involved in were rated as atypical even on the second observation
childrearing and mealtimes (Snethen et al. 2008), and the attempt; all were more typical on the second attempt though
unique and important influence of fathers on child weight and any divergence positive (e.g., the child behaving better
(Fraser et al. 2011). Further remains to be explored than usual) which may in fact provide a more conservative
empirically on the potentially differential role of mothers estimate of group differences. That all observations for the
and fathers in problem eating, and with a broader sample comparison group were rated as typical on the first attempt
generally, to ensure adequate generalisability. is interesting in itself, and perhaps suggests greater varia-
The focus on healthy, typically developing children was bility in the mealtimes of families with feeding difficulties.
consistent with the study’s aims in contributing to literature Notwithstanding these limitations, the current study pro-
on feeding issues in this population. Target children varied vides evidence for the role of parenting factors, particularly
in age from 1.5 to 6 years; while this may be considered a parental cognitions, in childhood problem eating and so
heterogeneous age range, it was intended to represent the directions for assessment and intervention.
preschool period, during which feeding difficulties
J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178 3177

Acknowledgements The Parenting and Family Support Centre is eating patterns of toddlers. Journal of the American College of
partly funded by royalties stemming from published resources of the Nutrition, 17(2), 180–186.
Triple P – Positive Parenting Program, which is developed and owned Chavira, V., Lopez, S. R., Blacher, J., & Shapiro, J. (2000). Latina
by The University of Queensland. Royalties from the program are also mothers’ attributions, emotions, and reactions to the problem
distributed to the Faculty of Health and Behavioural Sciences at UQ behaviors of their children with developmental disabilities.
and contributory authors of Triple P programs. Triple P International Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(2), 245–252.
(TPI) Pty Ltd is a private company licensed by Uniquest, Pty Ltd, a Crist, W., & Napier-Phillips, A. (2001). Mealtime behaviors of young
commercialization company of UQ, to publish and disseminate Triple children: A comparison of normative and clinical data. Devel-
P worldwide. The authors of this report have no share or ownership of opmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22(5), 279–286.
TPI. Dr Morawska has received royalties from TPI. TPI had no de Moor, J., Didden, R., & Korzilius, H. (2007a). Behavioural treat-
involvement in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation ment of severe food refusal in five toddlers with developmental
of data, or writing of this report. disabilities. Child: Care, Health and Development, 33(6),
670–676.
Author Contributions M.A. designed and executed the study, de Moor, J., Didden, R., & Korzilius, H. (2007b). Parent-reported
analysed the data, and wrote the paper in collaboration with A.M. feeding and feeding problems in a sample of Dutch toddlers.
Early Child Development and Care, 177(3), 219–234.
de Moor, J., Didden, R., & Tolboom, J. (2005). Severe feeding pro-
blems secondary to anatomical disorders: Effectiveness of beha-
Compliance with Ethical Standards vioural treatment in three school-aged children. Educational
Psychology, 25(2–3), 325–340.
Conflicts of Interest The authors declare that they have no com- Dahl, M., & Sunderlin, C. (1992). Feeding problems in an affluent
peting interests. society: Follow-up at four years of age in children with early
refusal to eat. Acta Paediatrica, 81, 575–579.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving Dunne, L., Sneddon, J., Iwaniec, D., & Stewart, M. C. (2007).
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Maternal mental health and faltering growth in infants. Child
the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Abuse Review, 16, 283–295.
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical Eyberg, S. M., & Pincus, D. (1999). Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory
standards. and Sutter-Eyberg Student Behavior Inventory—revised: Pro-
fessional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all indivi- Resources.
dual participants included in the study. Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Parent–child interaction
training: Effects on family functioning. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology, 11, 130–137.
Farrow, C., & Blissett, J. (2006). Maternal cognitions, psychopatho-
logic symptoms, and infant temperament as predictors of early
References infant feeding problems: A longitudinal study. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(2), 128–134.
Adamson, M., Morawska, A., & Sanders, M. R. (2013). Childhood Farrow, C., & Blissett, J. (2012). Stability and continuity of parentally
feeding difficulties: A randomised controlled trial of a group- reported child eating behaviours and feeding practices from 2 to 5
based parenting intervention. Journal of Developmental and years of age. Appetite, 58, 151–156.
Behavioral Pediatrics, 34(5), 293–302. Fraser, J., Skouteris, H., McCabe, M., Ricciardelli, L. A., Milgrom, J.,
Adamson, M. & Morawska, A. (2016). Parent and Toddler Feeding & Baur, L. A. (2011). Paternal influences on children’s weight
Assessment: Development of a measure of child feeding and gain: A systemic review. Fathering, 9, 252–267.
mealtime parenting constructs. Manuscript in preparation. Greer, A. J., Gulotta, C. S., Masler, E. A., & Laud, R. B. (2008).
Archer, L. A., Rosenbaum, P. L., & Streiner, D. L. (1991). The Caregiver stress and outcomes of children with pediatric feeding
Children’s Eating Behavior Inventory: Reliability and validity disorders treated in an intensive interdisciplinary program.
results. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 16, 629–642. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33, 612–620.
Arnold, D. S., O’Leary, S. G., Wolff, L. S., & Acker, M. M. (1993). Janicke, D. M., Mitchell, M. J., & Stark, L. J. (2005). Family func-
The Parenting Scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in tioning in school-age children with cystic fibrosis: An observation
discipline situations. Psychological Assessment, 5, 137–144. assessment of family interactions in the mealtime environment.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 179–186.
NY: WH Freeman and Company. Johnson, R., & Harris, G. (2004). A preliminary study of the predictors
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. of feeding problems in late infancy. Journal of Reproductive and
Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. Infant Psychology, 22(3), 183–188.
Berlin, K. S., Davies, W. H., Lobato, D. J., & Silverman, A. H. (2009). Koivisto, U.-K., & Sjoden, P.-O. (1996). Food and general neophobia
A biopsychosocial model of normative and problematic pediatric in Swedish families: Parent–child comparisons and relationships
feeding. Children’s Health Care, 38, 263–282. with serving specific foods. Appetite, 26, 107–118.
Blissett, J., Meyer, C., & Haycraft, E. (2011). The role of parenting in Lewinsohn, P. M., Holm-Denoma, J. M., Gau, J. M., Joiner, T. E.,
the relationship between childhood eating problems and broader Striegel-Moore, R., Bear, P., & Lamoureux, B. (2005). Proble-
behaviour problems. Child: Care, Health & Development, 37, matic eating and feeding behaviors of 36-month-old children.
642–648. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38(3), 208–219.
Bramhagen, A., Axelsson, I., & Hallstrom, I. (2006). Mothers’ Locke, L. M., & Prinz, R. J. (2002). Measurement of parental dis-
experiences of feeding situations - an interview study. Journal of cipline and nurturance. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 895–929.
Clinical Nursing, 15, 29–34. Marchi, M., & Cohen, P. (1990). Early childhood eating behaviors and
Carruth, B. R., Skinner, J., Houck, K., Moran, J., Coletta, F., & Ott, D. adolescent eating disorders. Journal of the American Academy of
(1998). The phenomenon of ‘picky eater’: A behavioral marker in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(1), 112–117.
3178 J Child Fam Stud (2017) 26:3167–3178

McDermott, B. M., Mamun, A. A., Najman, J. M., Williams, G. M., Sanders, M. R., Le Grice, B., & Turner, K. M. T. (1993). Mealtime
O’Callaghan, M. J., & Bor, W. (2008). Preschool children per- Observation Schedule: An observer’s manual (rev. Ed.),.
ceived by mothers as irregular eaters: Physical and psychosocial Queensland: The University of Queensland. Unpublished tech-
predictors from a birth cohort study. Journal of Developmental nical manual.
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 29(3), 197–205. Sanders, M. R., Markie-Dadds, C., & Turner, K. M. T. (2001).
Morawska, A., Adamson, M., Hinchliffe, K., & Adams, T. (2014). Practitioner’s manual for Standard Triple P. Brisbane: Australian
Hassle Free Mealtimes Triple P: A randomised controlled trial of Academic Press.
a brief parenting group for childhood mealtime difficulties. Sanders, M. R., Patel, R. K., Le Grice, B., & Shepherd, R. W. (1993).
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 53, 1–9. Children with persistent feeding difficulties: An observational
Morawska, A., & Sanders, M. R. (2007). Concurrent predictors of analysis of the feeding interactions of problem and non-problem
dysfunctional parenting and maternal confidence: Implications for eaters. Health Psychology, 12(1), 64–73.
parenting interventions. Child: Care, Health and Development, Sanders, M. R., Turner, K. M. T., Wall, C. R., Waugh, L. M., & Tully,
33(6), 757–767. L. A. (1997). Mealtime behavior and parent–child interaction: A
Mrdjenovic, G., & Levitsky, D. A. (2005). Children eat what they are comparison of children with cystic fibrosis, children with feeding
served: The imprecise regulation of energy intake. Appetite, 44, problems, and nonclinic controls. Journal of Pediatric Psychol-
273–282. ogy, 22(6), 881–900.
Nicholls, D., Christie, D., Randall, L., & Lask, B. (2001). Selective Sanders, M. R., & Woolley, M. L. (2005). The relationship between
eating: Symptom, disorder or normal variant. Clinical Child maternal self-efficacy and parenting practices: Implications for
Psychology and Psychiatry, 6(2), 257–270. parent training. Child: Care, Health and Development, 31(1),
Owen, J., Metcalfe, A., Dryden, C., & Shipton, G. (2010). ‘If they 65–73.
don’t eat it, it’s not a proper meal’: Images of risk and choice in Snethen, J. A., Broome, M. E., Kelber, S., Leicht, S., Joachim, J., &
fathers’ accounts of family food practices. Health, Risk & Society, Goretzke, M. (2008). Dietary and physical activity patterns:
12, 395–406. Examining fathers’ perspectives. Journal for Specialists in
Orrell-Valente, J. K., Hill, L. G., Brechwald, W. A., Dodge, K. A., Pediatric Nursing, 13, 201–211.
Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2007). “Just three more bites”: An Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statis-
observational analysis of parents’ socialization of children’s eat- tics. (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Harper Collins.
ing at mealtime. Appetite, 48, 37–45. Williams, K. E., Hendy, H., & Knecht, S. (2008). Parent feeding
Phares, V., Lopez, E., Fields, S., Kamboukos, D., & Duhig, A. M. practices and child variables associated with childhood feeding
(2005). Are fathers involved in pediatric psychology research and problems. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
treatment? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30(8), 631–634. 20, 231–242.
Pugliese, M. T., Weyman-Daum, M., Moses, N., & Lifshitz, F. (1987).
Parental health beliefs as a cause of nonorganic failure to thrive.
Pediatrics, 80(2), 175–182.
Journal of Child & Family Studies is a copyright of Springer, 2017. All Rights Reserved.

S-ar putea să vă placă și