Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 2015, 23, 653  -658

http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/japa.2014-0044
© 2015 Human Kinetics, Inc. Original Research

Efficacy of Brain Gym Training on the Cognitive Performance


and Fitness Level of Active Older Adults: A Preliminary Study
José M. Cancela, Mª Helena Vila Suárez, Jamine Vasconcelos, Ana Lima, and Carlos Ayán

This study evaluates the impact of Brain Gym (BG) training in active older adults. Eighty-five participants were assigned to four training
groups: BG (n = 18), BG plus water-based exercise (n = 18), land-based exercise (n = 30), and land plus water-based exercise (n = 19). The
effects of the programs on the attention and memory functions were assessed by means of the symbol digit modality test. The two-min step and
the eight-foot up-and-go tests were used to evaluate their impact on fitness level. No program had a significant influence on the participant’s
cognitive performance, while different effects on the sample’ fitness levels were observed. These findings suggest that the effects of BG on the
cognitive performance and fitness level of active older adults are similar to those obtained after the practice of a traditional exercise program.
Whether BG is performed in isolation or combined with other exercise programs seems to have no influence on such effects.

Keywords: cognition, physical exercise, fitness, attention, memory

The rapidly-growing literature strongly suggests that physical on healthy older adults. In this regard, Brain Gym, a movement-
activity (PA) can have a positive impact on brain aging (Lauten- based program originally designed for children with learning
schlager, Cox, & Cyarto, 2012). Indeed, the findings of several disabilities on the assumption that physical movement enhances
randomized trials of the effect of PA on cognition in healthy older learning ability (Dennison & Dennison, 1994), is an interesting
adults have confirmed the existence of potential benefits, particularly field of research. Brain Gym is a structured nonaerobic physical
on executive function, attention, and processing speed (Smith et al., exercise intervention in which a combination of specific patterns
2010). This positive effect of exercise in cognition has been related of crossing movements of the head, eyes, and extremities are
to an increment of hippocampal volume (a structure sensitive to performed together with brain and breathing exercises. According
exercise-induced change via neurogenesis and cell proliferation) to its founders, the regular performance of Brain Gym results in
and level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (involved in stimulation and integration of different parts of the brain, espe-
cell growth and survival and memory promotion) (Hauer, Schwenk, cially the corpus callosum, which, in the long term, makes com-
Zieschang, Essig, Becker, & Oster, 2012). Regarding the above- munication between the two hemispheres faster and promotes a
mentioned, studies involving animal models have shown that PA more integrated form for high-level reasoning (Dennison, 1985).
can stimulate angiogenesis, brain perfusion, and neurovascular However, there is little evidence to support the claims made
integrity (Swain et al., 2003). Finally, it has been suggested that about the benefits of Brain Gym since scientific research has
a higher level of fitness through increased physical activity might failed to support the assertion that this educational kinesiology
have a positive effect on neuropathologic substrates, and it might therapy improves academic learning (Hyatt, 2007). Moreover,
lead to decreased whole-brain and medial temporal lobe atrophy its theoretical underpinning has also been subject to criticism by
in older adults (Makizako et al., 2011). neuroscientists and by educators with expertise in neuroscience
Most of the studies in this regard have focused on analyzing the and cognition (Stephenson, 2009).
effects of typical PA interventions (Ahlskog, Geda, Graff-Radford, In spite of all this, it has been suggested that Brain Gym can
& Petersen, 2011). For instance, it has been observed that aerobic be considered as a useful physical therapy strategy for older adults,
exercise, mainly brisk walking and/or jogging, has been associ- since it can have a positive impact on brain functioning (Drabben-
ated with modest improvements in attention, memory, processing Thiemann, Hedwig, & Kenklies, 2002). In this regard, Yágüez,
speed, and executive function among seniors (Smith et al., 2010). Shaw, Morris, and Matthews (2011) conducted a small random-
Similarly, scientific evidence has shown that muscular resistance ized controlled trial which showed that the performance of Brain
training can lead to improvements in memory (short- and long-term) Gym can enhance cognitive performance, specifically attention and
and verbal reasoning, while strength and balance training exercises memory, in older adults with dementia (Yágüez et al., 2011). Simi-
can enhance fluid intelligence in this population (Liu-Ambrose & larly, Sidiarto, Kusumoputro, Munir, and Nugroho (2003) carried
Donaldson, 2009). out a quasi-experimental study involving a group of healthy older
It has been observed that although some evidence exists, adults who improved their cognitive performance after taking part
cognitive benefits may be obtained by means of alternative PA in an intervention based on the performance of specific patterns of
training programs (Forte et al., 2013).There is still a need for movements and brain exercises, very similar to the ones described in
studies aimed at identifying the effects of such exercise therapies the Brain Gym manual. Nevertheless, regarding the benefits of Brain
Gym in the cognitive performance and fitness level of community-
dwelling older individuals, scientific evidence is scarce. Moreover,
Cancela, Vila Suárez, and Ayán are with the Faculty of Education and Sports no research has compared the effects of this therapy with traditional
Science, University of Vigo, Pontevedra, Spain. Vasconcelos and Lima are exercise interventions or has analyzed its potential benefits when
with the HealthyFit Research Group, University of Vigo, Pontevedra, Spain. it is included as part of a more comprehensive physical exercise
Address author correspondence to Mª Helena Vila Suárez at evila@uvigo.es. program. This study aims to fill this gap.
653
654  Cancela et al.

Methods to participate in. They were also informed both aspects would be
taken into account to include them in one of our programs, with the
Participants purpose of motivating the participants and getting a good adherence
level during the study. According to their time availability and PA
Eligible individuals were community-dwelling older adults who preferences, participants were allocated to two groups, the Brain
had been taking part in a PA program during the previous five Gym (BG) or traditional exercise (TG) program. BG program par-
years. This program, which was offered by the council of their local ticipants took part in a 1-hr weekly session, performing 15 exercises
town every year, was aimed at improving the functional capacity focusing on fine motor involvement, balance, and hand-eye coordi-
of the participants by means of the performance of water-based nation. TG program participants attended 1-hr weekly sessions of
exercise, however, it did not include Brain Gym’s realization or land-based training, which included callisthenic exercises aimed at
similar exercises destined to ameliorate cognitive performance. improving dynamic balance, coordination, and aerobic capacity. The
For the purpose of the current study, inclusion criteria were older possibility of adding a 1-hr weekly session of water-based exercise
adults between ages 65–80 with absence of a clinical diagnosis to the program was offered to participants in both groups. Thus,
of dementia and independent ambulation. People who presented four different groups were set up: BG, BG + water-based exercise,
any comorbidities or any acute illness that would make training land-based exercise, and land + water-based exercise. The total
inappropriate, individuals who had already participated in Brain intervention duration was 16 weeks. Water-based and land-based
Gym sessions, or those who were taking part in any other physical sessions were monitored by a specialist with previous experience
exercise or memory training program different to the one offered administering physical exercise programs for older adults. He also
in the present research were excluded. recorded attendance and dropouts. BG sessions were monitored
Downloaded by University of California on 09/18/16, Volume 23, Article Number 4

The research protocol followed the guidelines provided by the by a physiotherapist experienced in applying this type of physical
Helsinki Statement about biomedical research for humans (18th therapy. The characteristics of the sessions can be seen in Table 1.
Medical Assembly, 1964; revised 1983 in Italy and 1989 in Hong
Kong) at the international level, and the Arrangement for Preserva-
tion of Human Rights and Dignity with Respect to Biological and Data and Statistical Analysis
Medical Applications (IR1999; B.O.E. 251, 1999) at the national Means and standard deviations for the whole group were calculated
level. The study was approved by the local ethics committee, and for all continuous variables. Discrete variables were described by
written informed consent was provided by all participants. percentage. All continuous variables were tested for normality by
Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistics. With the aim of analyzing intra-
Measurements group behaviors of each one of the variables, a variability analysis
was realized by confidence interval and coefficient of variation
Academic Background and Medication.  An ad hoc self-com- calculus in every group and during the two moments of the analysis.
pletion questionnaire was administered to obtain information about Paired-sample tests were also used to assess changes from preinter-
the educational attainment of the sample and the type of medication vention to postintervention (16 weeks) in each one of the programs.
that individuals were taking. A repeated-measures MANOVA test was conducted to analyze the
Self-Perceived Physical and Cognitive Health.  A Likert-type effect of the intervention programs in the cognitive performance and
scale was used to assess the perceived fitness level and the existence fitness level of the sample. The Bonferroni multiple comparisons test
of memory problems (range 1–5 points; lower scores indicate worse was used to compare specific means. Also, 2 × 4 mixed MANOVAs
fitness level and greater memory problems). (moment × program) were used, with a moment factor (pretest and
posttest) and program factor (BG, BG + water exercise, land-based
Mental Processing, Attention, and Memory Functions.  These exercise, land + water-based exercise), considering dependent
cognitive variables were assessed by means of the Spanish-validated variables as: SDMT, 2MST, and 8FGT. Pearson’s correlation coef-
version of the symbol digit modality test (SDMT) (Smith, 2002). ficient (r) was accomplished with the aim of determining degrees
This test involves filling nonsense symbol blanks with matching of association between the ad hoc questionnaire and the objective
numbers to a specific code. The scoring ranged from 0–110, with tests employed during the study. Significance was accepted at p <
higher scores representing better performance. .05. All data were analyzed using Statistical Package for the Social
Fitness Level.  The two-min step test (2MST) (Rikli & Jones, 1999) Sciences 17.0 (IBM, Inc., Chicago, IL).
was used to assess aerobic endurance. This test determines the rep-
etitions a person can do by stepping in place alternately raising the Results
knees to a height halfway between the iliac crest and middle of the
patella in 2 min. Agility, power, dynamic balance, and speed were A total of 85 individuals volunteered and finished the study. The
assessed by means of the eight-foot up-and-go test (8FGT) (Rikli, characteristics of the participants, which were distributed depend-
& Jones, 1999). This test measures the time it takes to get out of a ing on the program in which they took part, can be seen in Table 2.
chair, walk eight feet to and around a cone, and return to the chair. Baseline assessment showed that the group distribution was
The assessment was carried out by two physiotherapists and two homogeneous. The variability analysis accomplished by means of
physical exercise specialists, who were blind to group allocation, the confidence interval (CI) and the coefficient of variation (CV)
one week before and one week after the intervention. indicated that intragroup variability of the physical analyzed vari-
ables was low (CV < 32) while cognitive variables were elevated
Intervention (CV > 62). According to the results obtained, none of the four
programs had a significant effect on mental processing, attention,
People were told in advance the types of programs which were or memory function. However, the performance of BG seemed to
going to be developed as well as the exact date in which they were improve significantly the participants’ self-perceived cognitive
going to take place. Then they were asked about scheduled prefer- health (Table 3). The difference between pre- and postintervention
ences to assist with the study as well as the program they would like scores obtained in the 8FGT showed that patients significantly
JAPA Vol. 23, No. 4, 2015
Efficacy of Brain Gym Training   655

Table 1  Exercise Interventions


Land-Based Exercise Water-Based Exercise Brain Gym
Warm-up exercises (10 min) Warm-up exercises (10 min) Warm-up exercises (10 min)
Gentle walking Gentle walking Gentle walking
–Walking different directions –Walking different directions –Walking different directions
–Walking with long deliberate steps –Walking while shifting support (heels, on –Walking with long deliberate steps
–Walking with small deliberate steps tiptoe) –Walking with small deliberate steps
–Walking while raising knees –Marching (raising knees-heels/legs apart- –Walking while raising knees
crossed/arms in-out of the water)
Active joint motion range Active joint motion range
Gentle stretching
General exercises (45 min) General exercises (45 min) General exercises (30 min)
Aerobic dance exercises (25 min) Group games (15 min) –Belly breathing (1 min)
–Low impact aerobic movements (120 beats –Passing a ball among the participants –Cross-lateral walking in place (2 min)
per min) –Throwing balls into dispersed hoops in the –Lazy eight (2 min)
General motor coordination (10 min) water –Elephant (2 min)
Downloaded by University of California on 09/18/16, Volume 23, Article Number 4

–Bouncing a ball while walking Balance and coordination (15 min) –Cross-lateral walking in place (2 min)
–Throwing a rolling ball to a partner while –Standing and holding the pool wall, wide –Neck rolls (1 min with eyes open/1 min
walking stance, with support, and shift weight left with eyes closed)
–Throwing a ball in the air to a partner while and right, forward and backward as far as
possible –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
walking
–Standing on one leg while the other leg –The owl (2 min)
Balance (10 min)
pushes and pulls a “noodle” (long float) –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
–Walking between cones:
–Riding on a noodle and maintaining balance –Arm activation (1 min for each arm)
Steady cadence (zig-zag movements)
–Standing with one leg on a noodle –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
Different walking cadences (changing the
direction) –Standing with both legs on the noodle and –Foot flex (1 min for each foot)
maintaining
Mild jogging between cones –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
–Standing with wide stance while being
pushed by a partner in different directions –Calf-pump (1 min for each leg)
Relaxation (15 min) –The grounder (1 min for each leg)
–Standing and holding the pool wall, wide –Gravity glider (1 min)
stance, with support, inserting the head in the –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
water (breathing out underwater) –Brain buttons (1 min)
–Lying supine in the water –Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
–Slow swimming –Earth buttons (1 min)
–Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)
–Balance buttons (1 min)
–Cross-lateral walking in place (1 min)

Cooling-off activities (5 min) Cooling-off activities (5 min) Cooling-off activities (5 min)


General stretching exercises General stretching exercises General stretching exercises

increased their agility, power, dynamic balance, and speed regard- (F[3,77] = 4.031, p = .010, η2 = .136). There were not differences
less of the program to which they were allocated. The results for SDMT (F[3,77] = 0 .342, p = 0 .795, η2 = .013). Bonferroni
obtained in the 2MST showed that only those who participated in multiple comparison tests indicated the absence of differences in
programs including land-based exercise sessions showed a signifi- the programs on the object study variables (SDMT, 2MST, 8FGT).
cant improvement in their aerobic level. Correlational analyses accomplished by means of Pearson’s (r)
Table 4 shows the results obtained due to the compara- correlation indicated the absence of association between the ad
tive study accomplished by 2 × 4 mixed MANOVA (moment hoc questionnaire of cognitive health perception and the SDMT
× program), which showed that there was a difference between (r = .113; p = .356). This analysis showed the existence of sig-
intervention groups in the analyzed dependent variables. Invariant nificant associations between the ad hoc questionnaire of physical
tests also indicated that there was intervention effect differences on health perception and the 2MST (r = .378; p = .001) and 8FGT
individual 2MST (F[3,77] = 4.433, p = .06, η2 = .147) and 8FGT (r = –.206; p = .042).
JAPA Vol. 23, No. 4, 2015
656  Cancela et al.

Table 2  Characteristics of the Sample (n = 85)


Brain Gym Brain Gym + Water Land-Based Land + Water-Based
Variable (n = 18) Exercise (n = 18) Exercise (n = 30) Exercise (n = 19)
Age, M (SD) 71.94 (7.13) 69.89 (6.91) 71.19 (5.06) 67.95 (4.98)
Physical activity program
Years in the previous physical activity program, M (SD) 4.50 (3.78) 5.06 (3.13) 4.82 (3.54) 4.00 (3.54)
Sex
Women, n (%) 16 (88.8) 11 (61.1) 23 (76.6) 13 (68.4)
Medication
Medication to improve memory, n (%) 5 (26.7) 6 (33.3) 10 (33.3) 5 (26.3)
Other pathologies, n (%) 18 (100) 18 (100) 30 (100) 19 (100)
Academic background
No education, n (%) 6 (33.3) 4 (22.2) 2 (6.6) 1(5.2)
Primary education, n (%) 11 (61.1) 13 (72.2) 14 (46.6) 18 (94.7)
Secondary education, n (%) 1 (5.5) 1 (5.5) 1 (3.3) –
Downloaded by University of California on 09/18/16, Volume 23, Article Number 4

Higher education, n (%) – – – –

Discussion values published about a population from the same country and of
similar age (Peña-Casanova et al., 2009). Although the people who
This research was aimed at determining the effect of Brain Gym volunteered for this study did not present a clinical diagnosis of
on the cognition and fitness level of healthy older adults, as well dementia, it may be possible that they presented some type of mild
as finding out whether its effects were more significant that those cognitive decline, considering the low mean score obtained in the
produced by a traditional exercise program. Obtained results indicate SDMT. This has been already proposed in other studies which have
that none of the four exercise programs had significant effects on used this test as a cognitive screening tool (Cherbuin, Sachdev, &
the cognitive functions related to mental processing, attention, and Anstey, 2010). Under these circumstances, it may be reasonable to
memory as assessed by means of the SDMT. Thus, it seems that speculate the possibility of the Brain Gym program having a protec-
Brain Gym practice, whether it is the only exercise or it is combined tive effect, somehow hindering or hampering the aforementioned
with a water-based program, is not more effective than classical mild cognitive decline. This idea could also help to explain why the
physical exercise programs when improving cognitive performance. traditional exercise program had no significant effect on the cognitive
This contradicts the results of previous investigations. For instance, variables assessed by the SDMT, despite the fact that it produced
Drabben-Thiemannet al. (2002) reported beneficial effects of using some improvements in the aerobic level. Nevertheless, the lack of an
Brain Gym on people who had been diagnosed with dementia, and inactive control group makes this idea just a mere hypothesis, thus
Yágüez et al. (2011) found significant improvements in sustained it cannot be considered as an evident result in this study.
attention and visual memory in people with Alzheimer’s-type In relation to the effects of the different interventions, which
dementia after applying this therapy. Two different reasons may were carried out on the fitness level of the sample, it was observed
explain the fact that no effects were found in our research. First, that the participants in the groups with land-based exercise were
it could be thought that the reduced weekly frequency of the the only ones who improved their aerobic level. This may be a con-
Brain Gym program may not be enough to enhance the cognitive sequence of the training session’s content because the Brain Gym
performance of the sample. However, Yágüez et al. (2011) found program had no aerobic component and the water-based exercise
some benefits after applying the program once per week for only program focused on relaxing and toning up. Indeed, only land-based
six weeks. These differences could have appeared because that sessions included aerobic exercise.
program included longer sessions (90 min) and longer exercises Finally, all the programs had similar effects on agility, balance,
(2–3 min, which suggest that the volume of Brain Gym sessions speed, power, and self-perceived physical health. This was some-
accomplished by people in this study could not have been enough how the expected result, as their sessions included postural control
to incite changes in cognitive performance. exercises and tasks aimed at enhancing coordination, balance, and
Secondly, taking into account the characteristics of the sample motor ability in general.
from the two previously-mentioned studies, it could be hypothesized The fact that no differences were found regarding the effects
that the effect of Brain Gym depends on the individual’s cognitive on cognitive performance of both Brain Gym and traditional train-
state. Thus, healthy older adults would not obtain the same benefit ing may be considered an original contribution of this research. As
as older people suffering from dementia. Nevertheless, Sidiarto et to the authors’ knowledge, no comparative study on the effects of
al. (2003) informed that mentally healthy older populations may the Brain Gym has been published to date. Regarding this aspect of
experience significant improvements in different cognitive functions investigation, it is necessary to point out that although it is true that
after Brain Gym practice. Brain Gym practice had effects on the cognitive health perceived by
In relation to this, it is important to point out that the initial mean participants, this was evaluated with an ad hoc questionnaire, which
values obtained from the sample of this study in the SDMT may be was not validated and whose obtained results did not correlate with
classified as too low (Sheridan et al., 2006). The sample would be the ones obtained in the SDMT test. This is the reason why this result
between percentile 19 and percentile 28, according to the reference might be due to an information slant instead of the intervention per se.
JAPA Vol. 23, No. 4, 2015
Downloaded by University of California on 09/18/16, Volume 23, Article Number 4

Table 3  Effects of the Different Exercise Programs on the Cognitive Variables


SDMT Cognitive Health
Program Pre M (SD) 95% CI CV Post M (SD) 95%CI CV t student Pre M (SD) 95% CI CV Post M (SD) 95%CI CV t student
t = –.689 t = –3.063
Brain Gym 13.53 (8.47) 9.17–17.88 62.60 14.12 (7.78) 10.11–18.11 55.09 3.78 (0.81) 3.30–4.10 21.42 4.22 (0.73) 3.80–4.55 17.29
Sig = .501 Sig = .007
Brain Gym + t = –.507 t = –2.915
19.29 (9.17) 14.57–24.00 47.53 19.94 (8.23) 15.71–24.17 41.27 4.00 (0.84) 3.55–4.44 21.00 4.33 (0.68) 3.94–4.64 15.70
water exercise Sig = .619 Sig = .010
Land-based t = .376 t = 1.852
16.17 (6.70) 14.24–21.86 41.43 15.87 (7.00) 11.82–19.00 44.10 3.47 (1.07) 2.92–4.01 30.83 3.29 (1.10) 2.72–3.86 33.43
exercise Sig = .710 Sig = .083
Land + water- 18.06 (7.66) 12.79–19.09 42.41 18.94 (7.86) 15.01–22.86 41.49 t = –.829 3.84 (1.21) 3.65–4.45 31.51 3.94 (0.80) 3.54–4.34 20.30 t = 1.458
based exercise Sig = .419 Sig = .163
Abbreviations: SDMT = symbol digit modalities test; CI = confidence interval; CV = coefficient of variation.

JAPA Vol. 23, No. 4, 2015


Table 4  Effects of the Different Exercise Programs on the Physical Variables
2MST 8FGT Physical Health
Pre M Post M Pre M Post M Pre M Post M
Program (SD) 95% CI CV (SD) 95%CI CV t stud. (SD) 95% CI CV (SD) 95% CI CV t stud. (SD) 95% CI CV (SD) 95%CI CV t stud.
Brain Gym 79.76 70.96– 20.02 76.71 69.69– 14.44 t = .938 6.78 6.03– 16.96 5.91 5.00– 12.52 t = 6.963 3.78 3.37– 21.42 4.16 3.91– 12.25 t = –2.364
(15.97) 88.53 (13.93) 83.30 (1.15) 7.62 (0.74) 6.42 (0.81) 4.17 (0.51) 4.42
Sig = .362 Sig = .001 Sig = .030
Brain Gym + 87.89 80.93– 23.17 85.33 77.10– 31.66 t = .987 6.38 5.71– 15.51 5.28 4.62– 29.35 t = 7.432 4.11 3.73– 18.49 4.38 4.13– 11.41 t = –2.051
water exercise (20.37) 98.95 (27.02) 97.36 (0.99) 6.97 (1.55) 5.91 (0.76) 4.48 (0.50) 4.63
Sig = .331 Sig = .001 Sig = .046
Land-based 84.13 76.17– 20.83 90.13 82.57– 16.67 t = –2.632 6.42 5.94– 14.79 5.77 5.04– 18.02 t = 4.912 3.71 3.19– 26.68 4.11 3.80– 14.59 t = –2.915
exercise (17.53) 91.09 (15.03) 97.68 (0.95) 6.89 (1.04) 5.96 (0.99) 4.21 (0.60) 4.42
Sig = .013 Sig =.001 Sig = .010
Land + water- 83.89 76.92– 15.43 94.89 83.87– 16.53 t = –3.113 6.22 5.75– 13.98 5.16 4.21– 8.91 t = 6.111 4.16 3.82– 16.58 4.47 4.22– 11.40 t = –2.051
based exercise (12.95) 90.18 (15.69) 102.79 (0.87) 6.67 (0.46) 5.08 (0.69) 4.48 (0.51) 4.72
Sig = .006 Sig = .001 Sig = .045
Abbreviations: 2MST = two-min step test; 8FGT = eight-foot up-and go test; CI = confidence interval; CV = coefficient of variation; t student = t student.

657
658  Cancela et al.

In any case, the absence of different effects between the two Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(8), 723–733. PubMed doi:10.1097/
modalities, as assessed by objective and previously-validated tests, JGP.0b013e3181cdecf1
seems to be in line with the findings obtained by other authors who Dennison, P. (1985). Whole Brain Learning for the Whole Person. Ventura,
have carried out similar studies. Thus, Forte et al. (2013) observed CA: Edu-Kinesthetics Inc.
Dennison, G., & Dennison, P. (1994). Brain Gym: Teacher’s edition revised.
that multicomponent training (simultaneous performance of cogni-
Ventura, CA: Edu-Kinesthetics, Inc.
tive tasks and physical exercises) and traditional training (muscular Drabben-Thiemann, G., Hedwig, D., & Kenklies, M. (2002). The effect of
resistance exercises) had the same effect on different cognitive Brain Gym on cognitive performance of Alzheimer’s patients. Brain
variables in healthy older adults. Gym Journal, 16, 3–4.
Similarly, Barnes et al. (2013) observed that the combination Forte, R., Boreham, C.A., Leite, J.C., De Vito, G., Brennan, L., Gibney,
of mental training and physical exercise in a sample of older adults E.R., & Pesce, C. (2013). Enhancing cognitive functioning in the
with cognitive complaints did not produce greater effects on the elderly: multicomponent vs. resistance training. Clinical Interventions
cognitive performance than those produced by a physical exercise in Aging, 8, 19–27. PubMed doi:10.2147/CIA.S36514
program which lacked this component. Hauer, K., Schwenk, M., Zieschang, T., Essig, M., Becker, C., & Oster, P.
The scarce existing studies on the effects of Brain Gym show (2012). Physical training improves motor performance in people with
dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Geriat-
different methodological weaknesses, such as the absence of a con-
rics Society, 60(1), 8–15. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03778.x
trol group (Drabben-Thiemannet al., 2002; Sidiarto et al., 2003) or Hyatt, K.J. (2007). Brain Gym: building stronger brains or wishful think-
the small sample size (Yágüez et al., 2011). An attempt to overcome ing? Remedial and Special Education, 28(2), 117–124. doi:10.1177
these weaknesses has been made in this study. However, this research /07419325070280020201
also has certain limitations of its own, especially those related to the
Downloaded by University of California on 09/18/16, Volume 23, Article Number 4

Lautenschlager, N.T., Cox, K., & Cyarto, E.V. (2012). The influence of
sample characteristics. For instance, the entire sample was made up exercise on brain aging and dementia. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta,
of people willing to exercise and who were allowed to participate 1822(3), 474–481. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.010
in the preferred program. This facilitated adherence to the program, Liu-Ambrose, T., & Donaldson, M.G. (2009). Exercise and cognition in
but it limits generalization and it could provoke a self-selection bias. older adults: is there a role for resistance training programmes? Brit-
Similarly, the sample consisted of people who had been taking part ish Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(1), 25–27. PubMed doi:10.1136/
in physical exercise programs for five years. Consequently, a possible bjsm.2008.055616
Makizako, H., Shimada, H., Doi, T., Yoshida, D., Ito, K., Kato, T., . . .
ceiling effect on the variables under study must be taken into account. Suzuki, T. (2011). The association between decline in physical func-
Finally, the participants showed very low mean values in the SDMT, tioning and atrophy of medial temporal areas in community-dwelling
which implies that their cognitive level could be somewhat affected. older adults with amnestic and nonamnestic mild cognitive impair-
Therefore, the data may have been contaminated by the sampling ment. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 92(12),
cognitive profile. Regarding the above-mentioned, certain limitations 1992–1999. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.07.195
related to methodological design, such as the lack of an inactive control Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar,
group, the fact that group distribution was not randomized, and the M., Badenes, D., Molinuevo, J.L., . . . Blesa, R. (2009). Spanish
employment of ad hoc questionnaires which were not tested for valid- Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms
ity, must be acknowledged so as to interpret the results with caution. for verbal span, visuospatial span, letter and number sequencing, trail
Future randomized comparative studies should include sedentary making test, and symbol digit modalities test. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 24(4), 321–341. PubMed doi:10.1093/arclin/acp038
samples which consist of older adults who do not present any type of Rikli, R., & Jones, C. (1999). Development and validation of a functional
cognitive complain, so that the results here presented can be contrasted. fitness test for community-residing older adults. Journal of Aging and
Physical Activity, 7(2), 127–159.
Sheridan, L.K., Fitzgerald, H.E., Adams, K.M., Nigg, J.T., Martel, M.M., Put-
Conclusion tler, L.I., . . . Zucker, R.A. (2006). Normative Symbol Digit Modalities
Test performance in a community-based sample. Archives of Clinical
The results of this study indicate that the effects of Brain Gym on Neuropsychology, 21(1), 23–28. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.003
the cognitive performance and fitness level of community-dwelling Sidiarto, L.D., Kusumoputro, S., Munir, R., & Nugroho, W. (2003). The
older individuals are similar to those obtained after the practice of efficacy of specific patterns of movements and brain exercises on the
a traditional exercise program. Whether Brain Gym is performed in cognitive performance of healthy senior citizen in Jakarta. Medical
isolation or combined with other exercise programs seems to have Journal of Indonesia, 12(3), 155–161. doi:10.13181/mji.v12i3.107
no influence on the effects produced by this physical therapy. This Smith, A. (2002). Test de símbolos y dígitos. Madrid: TEA Ediciones S.A.
finding must be confirmed by investigations which include sedentary [Symbol and Digit Test].
older populations who do not present any type of cognitive complaint. Smith, P.J., Blumenthal, J.A., Hoffman, B.M, Cooper, H., Strauman, T.A.,
Welsh-Bohmer, S. …& Sherwood, A. (2010). Aerobic exercise and
neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized
References controlled trials. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(3), 239–252. PubMed
doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633
Ahlskog, J.E., Geda, Y.E., Graff-Radford, N.R., & Petersen, R.C. (2011). Stephenson, J. (2009). Best practice? Advice provided to teachers about
Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of the use of Brain Gym in Australian schools. Australian Journal of
dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 86(9), 876–884. Education, 53(2), 109–124. doi:10.1177/000494410905300202
PubMed doi:10.4065/mcp.2011.0252 Swain, R.A., Harris, A.B., Wiener, E.C., Dutka, M.V., Morris, H.D., Theien,
Barnes, D.E., Santos-Modesitt, W., Poelke, G., Kramer, A.F., Castro, C., B.E., . . . Greenough, W.T. (2003). Prolonged exercise induces angio-
Middleton, L.E., & Yaffe, K. (2013). The Mental Activity and eXer- genesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex
cise (MAX) trial: a randomized controlled trial to enhance cognitive of the rat. Neuroscience, 117(4), 1037–1046. PubMed doi:10.1016/
function in older adults. JAMA Internal Medicine, 173(9), 797–804. S0306-4522(02)00664-4
PubMed doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.189 Yágüez, L., Shaw, K.N., Morris, R., & Matthews, D. (2011). The effects
Cherbuin, N., Sachdev, P., & Anstey, K.J. (2010). Neuropsychological on cognitive functions of a movement‐based intervention in patients
predictors of transition from healthy cognitive aging to mild cog- with Alzheimer’s type dementia: a pilot study. International Journal of
nitive impairment: the PATH Through Life Study. The American Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(2), 173–181. PubMed doi:10.1002/gps.2510

JAPA Vol. 23, No. 4, 2015

S-ar putea să vă placă și