Sunteți pe pagina 1din 60

Effects of Bacopa monnieri Supplementation on the Learning and Short-Term Memory Retention

of Sleep-Deprived Drosophila melanogaster

Bradley Ashley G. Ong


Maria Clarice N. Villanueva

An Undergraduate Thesis
Submitted to the Department of Biology
College of Arts and Sciences
University of the Philippines Manila
Padre Faura, Manila

In partial fulfillment of the requirements


For the degree of
Bachelor of Science in Biology
May 2016
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Department of Biology
College of Arts and Sciences
University of the Philippines Manila
Padre Faura, Manila

Announcement of the
Undergraduate Thesis Presentation

BRADLEY ASHLEY GUE ONG


and
MARIA CLARICE NUQUI VILLANUEVA

Entitled
EFFECTS OF Bacopa monnieri SUPPLEMENTATION
ON LEARNING AND SHORT-TERM MEMORY RETENTION
OF SLEEP-DEPRIVED Drosophila melanogaster

For the degree of


Bachelor of Science in Biology

1:00 P.M., 13 April 2016


Room 115, Rizal Hall

THESIS ADVISER THESIS CO-ADVISER


Miriam P. de Vera, Ph.D. Paul Mark B. Medina, Ph.D.
Associate Professor Associate Professor
Department of Biology Department of Biochemistry
College of Arts and Sciences College of Medicine
University of the Philippines Manila University of the Philippines Manila

THESIS READER THESIS READER


Melody Anne B. Ocampo, M.S. Ma. Cristina A. Bordallo, M.P.H.
Assistant Professor Senior Lecturer
Department of Biology Department of Biology
College of Arts and Sciences College of Arts and Sciences
University of the Philippines Manila University of the Philippines Manila

Endorsed by: Authorized by:

Miriam P. de Vera, Ph.D. Elena M. Ragragio, MA Sci. Educ.


Chair Chair
Thesis Committee Department of Biology

ii
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Department of Biology
College of Arts and Sciences
University of the Philippines Manila
Padre Faura, Manila

ENDORSEMENT

The thesis attached hereto, EFFECTS OF Bacopa monnieri SUPPLEMENTATION


ON LEARNING AND SHORT-TERM MEMORY RETENTION OF SLEEP-
DEPRIVED Drosophila melanogaster, prepared and submitted by Bradley Ashley G. Ong
and Maria Clarice N. Villanueva in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Bachelor of Science in Biology, was successfully defended on 13 April 2016.

MIRIAM P. DE VERA, Ph.D. PAUL MARK B. MEDINA, Ph.D.


Thesis Adviser Thesis Co-Adviser

MELODY ANNE B. OCAMPO, M.S. MA. CRISTINA A. BORDALLO, M.P.H.


Thesis Reader Thesis Reader

This undergraduate thesis is hereby officially accepted as partial fulfillment of the


requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Science in Biology.

ELENA M. RAGRAGIO, MA Sci. Educ. LEONARDO R. ESTACIO Jr., Ph.D.


Chair Dean
Department of Biology College of Arts and Sciences

iii
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

BIOGRAPHICAL DATA
Bradley Ashley G. Ong

I. Personal Information
Full Name: Bradley Ashley Gue Ong
Nickname: Brad, Ash
Birthday: 22 February 1995
27 Doña Aurora Street Araneta Village
Permanent Address: Potrero, Malabon City
1476 NCR, Philippines
Mobile Number: +639175272823
Electronic Mail Address: bradleyashleyong@gmail.com
Father’s Name: Joselito Reyes Co Ong
Mother’s Name: Vicky Gue Ong

II. Educational Background


Primary Jubilee Christian Academy, Quezon City, Philippines

Secondary Jubilee Christian Academy, Quezon City, Philippines


(First Honorable Mention)
Collegiate University of the Philippines Manila, Manila, Philippines
(Bachelor of Science in Biology, Magna cum laude standing)

III. Collegiate Organizations


Member (2012-2014) and Vice President for External Affairs (2015-2016), Cerulean
Member (2013-2014), Lifebox
Member (2013–2014), UP One Earth (UPOE)
Member (2013–2014), P.R.O. Internals (2014-2015) and Vice President (2015-2016)
U.P. Biological Sciences Society (BSS)
Member (2013–2016), U.P. Biology Major’s Association (BIOMAS)
Member (2013-2016), Biology Majors Alliance of the Philippines (BMAP)
Externals Head (2014-2015) and Member (2015-2016), The Hope Project (THP)
Member (2014-2016), Registration Volunteer Corporation (RVC)

iv
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Member (2014-2016), U.P. Manila Biorhythm


Managing Editor (2015-2016), Aninag Editorial Board
Member (2015-2016), Phi Kappa Phi (PKP)
Member (2015-2016), Ugnayan ng Pahinungod
Member (2016-2017), Phi Sigma Biological Sciences Honor Society

BIOGRAPHICAL DATA

v
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Maria Clarice N. Villanueva

I. Personal Information
Full Name: Maria Clarice Nuqui Villanueva
Nickname: Claire, Cla
Birthday: 18 June 1996
Purok 6, Old Road, San Jose Village,
Permanent Address: City of San Fernando, Pampanga
2000 Region III, Philippines
Mobile Number: +639358663547
Electronic Mail Address: clrcvillanueva@yahoo.com
Father’s Name: Alvin Santos Villanueva
Mother’s Name: Imelda Nuqui Villanueva

II. Educational Background


Primary San Fernando Elementary School, Pampanga, Philippines
(Fourth Honorable Mention)
Secondary Pampanga High School, Pampanga, Philippines
(Fourth Honorable Mention)
Collegiate University of the Philippines Manila, Manila, Philippines
(Bachelor of Science in Biology)

III. Collegiate Organizations


Member (2012-2013), U.P. Biological Sciences Society (BSS)
Member (2013-2016) and Vice President for Finance (2014-2016), UP Manila
Indayog Dance Varsity

vi
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
The authors are formally indebted to a small group of individuals who made this
undergraduate thesis experience extremely endurable and worthwhile. Heartfelt gratitude is given
to the following:

 Dr. Miriam de Vera, the adviser, for her essential and valuable corrections, gentle prodding and
germane suggestions throughout the entire course of this endeavour,

 Dr. Paul Medina, the co-adviser, for his thorough guidance, unfaltering patience, surprising
amicability and generosity, in both time and resources, that provided a unique first-hand
perspective on the workings and potentials of our study,

 Prof. Melody Ocampo and Mrs. Cristina Bordallo, the readers, for their positive criticisms
and out-of-the-box insights leading to the expansion and improvement of this work,

 Mr. Ben Lopez, an L.U. IV student of the College of Medicine, for essentially and voluntarily
giving a crash course on Drosophila breeding, from food medium preparation to specimen
handling,

 Ms. Kim Cochon, presently an instructor at the Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics,
College of Public Health, for her invaluable assistance during the statistical analysis phase of the
research,

 National Institutes of Health, for generously providing funds for the completion of our study
from the first day until the very last,

 PCUM Robotics Trading, for supplying professional-level schematic drawings and the
construction of the improvised assay apparatus and sleep deprivation machine employed in this
study,

 the Ong and Villanueva families, together with their friends: Ashley, Chanel, Hanna, Lea,
Nathea, and Shaira, for the emotional, financial and moral support during this 10-month struggle,
and

 the Lord Almighty, for the strength and everything in between.

Bradley Ashley G. Ong Maria Clarice N. Villanueva

vii
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Title Page ................................................................................................................................ …..i


Announcement ............................................................................................................................. ii
Endorsement ............................................................................................................................... iii
Biographical Data ....................................................................................................................... iv
Acknowledgement ..................................................................................................................... vii
Table of Contents ...................................................................................................................... viii
List of Tables .............................................................................................................................. ix
List of Figures ...............................................................................................................................x
List of Appendices ...................................................................................................................... xi
Abstract ...................................................................................................................................... xii
Introduction ...................................................................................................................................1
Review of Related Literature ........................................................................................................4
Materials and Methods ................................................................................................................12
Results .........................................................................................................................................19
Discussion ...................................................................................................................................22
Conclusion ..................................................................................................................................27
Recommendations .......................................................................................................................28
Literature Cited ...........................................................................................................................29
Tables ..........................................................................................................................................36
Figures.........................................................................................................................................39
Appendix 1. Media......................................................................................................................43
Appendix 2. T-maze Setup for Aversive Phototaxic Suppression Assay (APS) ........................44
Appendix 3. Sleep Nullifying Apparatus (SNAP) ......................................................................45
Appendix 4. Methodology Flowchart .........................................................................................46
Appendix 5. Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) and Post Hoc Analaysis .....................................47

viii
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1. Difference on the Quinines Sensitivity Index of both non-slpD and slpD Drosophila….36

Table 2. Average Pass Rates for Learning and Memory Retention of non-slpD Drosophila…. 37

Table 3. Average Pass Rates for Learning and Memory Retention of slpD Drosophila…….....38

ix
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Survival rate of Drosophila melanogaster on the difference of 10-fold concentration


of Bacopa monnieri supplement over the course of seven days……………….….…..39
Figure 2. Comparison of the effect of velocity stressed caused by mechanical stimulation of the
machine on the learning and short-term memory retention of D. melanogaster……...39
Figure 3. Standardized average pass rates for learning and memory retention of non-sleep-
deprived and sleep-deprived D. melanogaster ………………………………….........40
Figure 4. Pass-Fail Map for Learning and STM retention of fruit flies………………………....41
Figure 5. Mean score of photonegative choices by non-sleep-deprived D. melanogaster on the
learning phase and short-term memory retention of different treatment groups….….42
Figure 6. Mean score of photonegative choices by sleep-deprived D. melanogaster on the
learning phase and short-term memory retention of different
treatment groups.………………………………………………………….…….….....42

x
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

LIST OF APPENDICES

Appendix 1. Media.................................……………………………………………………………43
Appendix 1a. Media Synthesis……………………………….……………………….........……...43
Appendix 1b. Summary of the Bacopa monnieri Memory Enhancing Supplement………...…..43

Appendix 2. T-Maze Setup for Aversive Phototaxis Suppression Assay…………………….….…..44

Appendix 3. Sleep Nullifying Apparatus………..…………………………………………….....45

Appendix 4. Methodology flowchart…………………………………………………………….46

Appendix 5. ANOVA Results and Post Hoc Results…………………………………………….47


Appendix 5a. Two way ANOVA results for Velocity Stress Test…………………………….47
Appendix 5b. Two way ANOVA results for the Learning Phase……………………………….47
Appendix 5c. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions between the control and
treatment groups for the Learning Phase……………………………………………………..47
Appendix 5d. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions for the Learning Phase
Using Bonferroni Method…………………………..............…………………………………….48
Appendix 5e. Two way ANOVA results for the Memory Retention Phase..................................49
Appendix 5f. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions between the control and
treatment groups for the Memory Retention Phase…....................................................................49
Appendix 5g. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions for the Memory Retention
Phase Using Bonferroni Method………………………………………………………………….49
Appendix 5h. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) results between Average Pass Rates of
Learning and Memory Retention…………………………………………………………………50

xi
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

ABSTRACT

Aversive phototaxic suppression (APS) is a behavioral assay in which photopositive fruit


flies (Drosophila melanogaster) are trained to avoid light using quinine as an aversive stimulus.
This conditioned behavior has been found to be impaired by sleep deprivation in D. melanogaster.
This study tested whether supplementation with Bacopa monnieri can counter this impairment.
Three sublethal and effective concentrations of B. monnieri supplement were given to test groups
of non-sleep-deprived (slp-) and sleep-deprived (slp+) flies subjected to the APS assay. The extent
of learning and short-term memory (STM) retention were based on average pass rates (APR)
defined as the number of trials that each fly exhibited a photonegative response before and after
conditioning. Results showed that B. monnieri supplement improved the APR of both slp- and slp+
flies as compared to the unsupplemented flies. These effects were found to be dose-dependent
from the low to the mid-level (4.62x10-4g/mL to 4.62x10-3g/mL) concentrations of the supplement
on D. melanogaster learning and STM retention.

xii
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

INTRODUCTION

Background of the Study

Nootropics, commonly known as memory enhancers, are compounds that act on cognitive

functions. These type of supplements are purportedly used to treat cognitive deficits such as in

Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or ADHD

(Barkley, 2006). The claims of the manufacturers of these dietary supplements posed a problem

due to the lack of verifiable therapeutic effects that is verified by industrial entities (Hathcock,

2001). Nonetheless, these off-label and non-prescription supplements have been increasingly

commonplace for non-medical situations like shift-works, jet lags and active military personnel

(Sahakian and Morein-Zamir, 2007). These situations are related to sleep deprivation and fatigue

leading to decrements in cognition as well as in psychomotor vigilance response speed or the

reaction time to sustained-attention to visual stimulus (Belenky et al., 2003). Sleep deprivation can

lead to significant changes in hippocampal activity during episodic memory encoding, and results

in impaired subsequent retention (Yoo, 2007).

The fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, has long been utilized as a neurophysiological-

behavioral model to understand various human cognitive deficits for many reasons. The organism

can be reliably trained to learn and remember association between stimuli through both classical

and operant conditioning (Tully and Quinn, 1985).

Statement of the Problem

Does Bacopa monnieri supplementation have an effect on the learning capacities and short-

term memory (STM) retention of non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived Drosophila

melanogaster?

1
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Research Objectives

The general objective of this study is to test the potential effects of B. monnieri supplement

on the learning capacities and STM retention in non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived D.

melanogaster subjected to the aversive phototaxic suppression (APS) assay. The specific

objectives are: (1) to demonstrate if the photopositive behavior of D melanogaster can be altered

upon conditioning with quinine; (2) to test if sleep deprivation can impair the learning capacities

and STM retention of D. melanogaster, and (3) to compare learning capacities and STM retention

among non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived D. melanogaster with supplementation.

Significance of the Study

For more than a century and particularly in the last 40 years (Vosshall, 2007), behavioral

assays designed for D. melanogaster have been successful in elucidating facets of the basis of

learning and memory in this model organism. This study contributes in these efforts by evaluating

and expanding the potential of APS assay in neurobehavioral research, particularly in assessing

pharmacological effects on Drosophila learning and STM using this simple assay format.

Conclusive confirmation of the experimental reliability of this tool will also strengthen support for

mainstreaming the use of APS assay in fly learning and memory studies. This would have positive

economic and practical implications in Drosophila learning and memory research worldwide.

The behavioral performance of D. melanogaster individuals in the assay can also provide

information on the effects of B. monnieri supplementation on non-sleep-deprived and sleep-

deprived Drosophila. Future efforts on the animal model may then relate the results obtained to

better evaluate the effects of these nootropics among humans in conditions like jet lag, sleep

deprivation, and night shift-requiring jobs.

2
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Scope and Limitations of the Study

The study was limited to the assessment of learning and STM retention capacities in seven-

day old adult wild-type female D. melanogaster individuals, with disrupted sleeping patterns that

are fed with the different concentrations of B. monnieri supplement, based on their collective

average pass rates in the APS assay. Neither biochemical nor physiological mechanism as basis

for the results were experimentally elucidated in this study, and such processes were only

predicted, inferred and related to the collected data through literature.

3
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE

Behavioral Learning Assays

Positive phototaxis is one of the earliest neurobiological aspects which was studied

extensively in Drosophila. As interest in genetics has been the main historical driving force in fruit

fly research, initial inquiries into this feature all attempted to link this apparently plastic behavior

with the inherited genotype and the surrounding environment (Hadler, 1964b). Hadler (1964a),

Hirsch and Boudreau (1958) and Médioni (1963) demonstrated that Drosophila phototaxis can be

altered when subjected to experimental behavioral-genetic methods. Such procedures include

quantitative screening, mathematical analyses of heritability and selective breeding performed

over several fly generations. Combining these techniques, Benzer (1967) created one of the first

Drosophila behavioral assays. In his first foray into this research field, he devised a method that

was able to “fractionate” D. melanogaster populations fed with the mutagen ethyl methane

sulfonate based on their relative phototaxic preferences on repeated trials. This approach facilitated

the isolation of non-phototaxic Drosophila behavioral mutants in just one offspring generation. As

this discovery effectively posited that behavior is genetically-controlled, the official acceptance of

the science of neurogenetics followed suit (Vosshall, 2007).

Research attention in phototaxis, however, gradually faded in favor of olfactory-based

memory, which was perceived to have greater potential in understanding neurobehavior in this

organism. Two landmark procedures concerned in the study of olfactory memory, by Quinn et al.

(1974) and Tully and Quinn (1985), are among the most replicated in Drosophila learning and

memory for more than 30 years. Olfactory-based learning in most insects, including Drosophila,

can be traced to structures in the nervous system called mushroom bodies. It is a cluster of about

2500 neurons known as Kenyon cells. The elucidation of the critical role of mushroom bodies in
4
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

olfactory-based memory involved a series of experiments: chemical ablation, induced genetic

mutation and radiofrequency ablation (de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994).

It was long assumed that the mushroom bodies has the same level of importance in other

observable behaviors in Drosophila (Waddell and Quinn, 2001). However, Wolf et al. (1998)

demonstrated that the ablation of the organ did not hamper the performance of the flies in simple

motor, tactile and visual tasks. Supporting this report in visual-based learning is the observation

that the presence of input from the optic lobes to mushroom bodies is non-existent (Heisenberg,

2003). According to van Swinderen (2011), visual-based learning studies in Drosophila has a

functional significance in higher and more complex tasks. This finding implies that visual

information reaches mushroom bodies and that genetic or chemical deactivation of this structure

still allows stimuli sensing and response and learning acquisition. However, visual information

hampers the ability to form associations and retrieve memories to the subtraction of unreliable

background cues from relevant ones to use the learned behavior in a partially different set of

circumstances (Waddell and Quinn, 2001). This organ complex is also shown to be used in

resolving contradictory visual cues. In mushroom bodies-disrupted flies, for instance, preference

of certain cues is attributed to pertinent cues instead of learning (Heisenberg, 2003; Liu et al.,

1999; van Swinderen, 2011). Modulation of selective fixation behavior based on the strength of

cues is associated to this structure as well (Xi et al., 2008). Brembs and Wiener (2006) suggests

that the use of mushroom bodies in visual-based memory is only for the simple stimulus processing

and not for cognitive and higher-order learning. Based on these uncertainties, molecular and neural

underpinnings of learning and memory through vision in Drosophila are arguably and relatively

less known than those through olfaction (van Swinderen, 2011).

5
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

While olfactory-based behavior of Drosophila is continually expounded through the

knowledge of mushroom bodies, the physiological basis of visual-based behavior is poorly

understood. Nevertheless, a wide range of assays have been designed for investigating this feature

in Drosophila. One of the earliest visual-based learning protocols was devised by Quinn et al.

(1974). It was utilized in many studies that followed. It consists of an operant group conditioning

apparatus forking into two arms: one transmitted light at a wavelength of 450 nm (blue) and the

other at 610 nm (red). Flies were conditioned to avoid the arm and the light that contained a bitter-

tasting compound known as quinine. Ever since this successful conditioning, quinine has been

used in several experiments on neurobiological behaviors of Drosophila. Quinine is used as a

potential stimulus for conditioning since it does not lead to stimulus sensitization or habituation. \

In aversive phototaxic suppression (APS) assay, an individual fly is operantly conditioned

to avoid light by associating it with quinine. This conditioning occurs when the fly exits the maze

through the lighted tube, where it encounters a quinine-soaked filter paper lining. The tendency to

avoid the lighted tube and to prefer the dark vial instead increases significantly over repeated

training (Le Bourg and Buecher, 2002; Le Bourg, 2005; Perisse et al., 2007; Seugnet et al., 2008;

Seugnet et al., 2009).

Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Behavior

The influence of sleep deprivation is well studied in Drosophila. In the review of sleep

deprivation-related research, Seugnet et al. (2008) showed that sleep loss impairs learning and

short sleep mutants impairs memory. Moreover, waking experience affects sleep needs in

Drosophila (Ganguly-Fitzgerald, 2006). All these studies show that sufficient sleep is important

in learning and memory.

6
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Resting Drosophila is defined as a long period of immobility and an increased arousal

threshold at a particular time during the circadian day (Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000).

Young flies tend to sleep extensively than older flies. Thus, sleep in the latter is modulated by

hypnotics and stimulants. The brain activity of Drosophila is reliably correlated with activity state,

and local field potential fluctuations can be reliably recorded from the brains of awake, moving

fruit flies (Nitz et al., 2002).

Xinjian et al. (2009) experimented on sleep deprivation and found that sleep deprivation

specifically impaired one-hour memory in wild type Canton-S flies. Flies were also tested on

alternative stresses like heat stress, oxidative stress, starvation and rotation stress and resulted in a

similar effect, leaving the memory of the flies intact. Mechanistic studies demonstrated that flies

with either silenced transmission of the MB during sleep deprivation or down-regulated cAMP

levels in the mushroom body demonstrated no sleep deprivation-induced one hour memory

impairment (Xinjian et al., 2009). In line with the expansion of knowledge and studies about the

effects of sleep deprivation on learning and memory, an emerging class of supplements called

nootropics are designed to enhance cognitive function (Dimond and Brouwers, 1976).

Nootropic Effects

Nootropics are part of a broader category of drugs known as performance and image

enhancing drugs which are used for enhancement of memory and cognition, sexual performance,

athletic performance or musculature (Stäubli et al., 1994). Dimond and Brouwers (1976) reported

that nootropics improved verbal learning, short-term memory, and performance in normal adults.

Nootropics are chemically similar to gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) which is the major

excitatory neurotransmitter in the brain. It has been suggested that these supplements act on the

7
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

cerebral cortex and related telencephalic structures and thereby enhance higher cortical functions

without apparent stimulant effects (Giurgea, 1973; Union Chimique Beege, 1976; Simeon et al.,

1980). Among other natural nootropics which claims to enhance learning and memory retention

is the plant Bacopa monnieri.

The plant Bacopa monnieri, also referred to as Herpestis monniera, is known to be

powerful antioxidants, and may also have anti-inflammatory properties (Jain et al., 1994; Tripathi

et al., 1996; Bhattacharya et al., 2000). Compounds responsible for the pharmacological effects of

B. monnieri include alkaloids, saponins and sterols. In the 1950s and 60s research by chemists at

the Central Drug Research Institute (CDRI) of India identified a variety of saponins. The main

active chemical constituents of Bacopa are the dammarane-type triterpenoid saponins (Garai et al.,

1996a, 1996b; Mahato et al., 2000) with jujubogenin and pseudojujubogenin as the aglycones

(Deepak and Amit, 2004). The saponins consist of numerous subtypes designated as bacosides,

bacopasides and bacopasaponins. Detailed investigations first reported the isolation of the alkaloid

“brahmine” from B. monnieri (Bose and Bose, 1931). In 1963, two active memory-enhancing

molecules were isolated and identified as Bacosides A and B.

Morgan and Bone (1999) stated that in herbal extracts standardised to bacoside A content,

it is likely that bacoside A collectively refers to the overall content of dammarane saponins rather

than a solitary saponin. Dammarane-type saponins have been shown to promote memory

acquisition, retention and retrieval in rats (Ma and Yu, 1993; Ma, Yu and Chen, 1991) and survival

of experimentally damaged neurones in chicken and rat cerebral cortex (Himi et al., 1989).

Commercially prepared nootropics that contains B. monnieri were developed to improve

learning and provide a feeling of well-being (Singh and Dhawan, 1982). Learning ability in rats

8
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

has been significantly enhanced by Bacopa extract as it facilitated acquisition, consolidation and

retention of three newly learned behavioural responses at an oral dosage of 40 milligram for three

times daily (Singh and Dhawan, 1982). Additionally, this study showed that the bacosides caused

enhanced levels of protein kinase activity and increases in protein levels in the hippocampus,

cerebral cortex and hypothalamus regions of the brain. The findings indicated positive implications

for improved neurotransmission and repair of damaged neurons via enhanced regeneration of

nerve synapses.

Placebo-controlled double-blind phase II clinical trials were held by the CDRI on children

with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Pal and Sarin, 1992). Additional studies were done

for the elderly with age-associated memory impairment. Both tests showed that B. monnieri

supplement is safe to use. At the Swinburne University in Australia, double-blind randomized

placebo-controlled studies were conducted. Tests results showed that Brahmi significantly

improved visual information processing (Shanker and Singh, 2000).

9
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Test Animals

Wild type Drosophila melanogaster (Oregon-R strain) were obtained from the Department

of Biology, University of the Philippines in Baguio. The flies were reared based on the protocol

by Medina et al. (2015) at room temperature on a sweet potato medium. A bottle containing one-

day old flies from hatched pupa were immediately transferred to two separate bottles containing

freshly prepared sweet potato medium. The reproduction of flies were allowed to obtain sufficient

number of flies. The breeding bottles were maintained at 24°C to 28°C and in a 12-hour light: 12-

hour dark cycle, with the 12-W fluorescent light. The progeny of these flies was considered for for

the test group of this study. However, only female flies were used since male flies spend

considerable time sleeping during both day and night, with the amount of sleep during the day

often comparable to that at night. Although mature females sleep as much as their male

counterparts during the night, they exhibit sustained activity during day and sleep much less during

this period (Huber et al., 2004). Thus, females were seen to be more consistent models for the

study (Dalke, 2002).

Carbon dioxide gas was used to put the flies to sleep while segregation according to sex

was conducted. Sex combs were used to distinguish male from the female flies since the structure

is present in male Drosophila only.

To ascertain that all flies that were used in the experiment are seven days old, separating

the progeny in a stock bottle with respect to the day of juvenile emergence was necessary. If, for

the first time, the newly-emergent flies were observed in a stock bottle in the morning (0700 hrs),

transfer to a new bottle, with medium corresponding to that in their current bottle, was performed

10
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

before midnight (<2330 hrs). All transferred flies were then considered as one-day old adults the

next day.

Test Chemicals

The Bacopa monnieri supplement, with brand name Memo Plus Gold, that was used, the

range of dosages employed, and the proposed mechanism of action were summarized in Appendix

1b. Flies were raised in the media containing a specific dose of the supplement (g/mL) at 25°C.

The supplement was only added into the media at 60°C before it solidifies. A lethality test was

conducted to estimate what concentrations are sublethal and effective to produce measurable

effects on the learning capacities and STM retention of D. melanogaster. The baseline dosages of

the supplements for Drosophila were calculated using the equation by Hong et al. (2002) for

conversion from human dosages. A five-point series of B. monnieri concentrations starting from

4.62 in g/mL and decreased by ten-fold reaching 4.62 x 10-4 g/mL were used for the lethality test.

Three concentrations were chosen for the experiment proper and were labeled as low, mid and

high, respectively. Female flies were allowed to feed on the food medium with corresponding B.

monnieri concentration previously defined from the first day after eclosion till the seventh day

when learning and conditioning commenced. There was constant feeding of the supplement during

the sleep deprivation process and resting period.

To prepare the quinine solution used as an aversive stimuli in the “lighted” tube, 1.98 grams

of quinine hydrochloride was dissolved in 50 mL distilled water to prepare a 0.10 M stock solution.

Sleep Deprivation Procedure

11
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Flies were sleep-deprived using an improvised Sleep Nullifying Apparatus (SNAP)

adapted from Shaw et al. (2002). They were individually put inside test tube-containing-media.

The SNAP travels vertically such that sleeping flies were displaced during the dropping movement

of the machine once a distance of five centimeters was reached for 15 times every minute. The

whole sleep deprivation process lasted for ten hours. Flies were sleep-deprived using the SNAP

from zeitgeber time ZT 12 to ZT 22. Flies were confirmed to be kept awake by their constant

moving inside the tubes.

Pre-tests

1. Quinine Sensitivity

This pre-test was conducted using the T-maze. It consisted of a center column with the trap

door and two independent chambers, a dark chamber and a lighted chamber. Two 15 mL plastic

centrifuge tubes were used to prepare it. One tube was shone with light to serve as the lighted

chamber. The other tube was wrapped with aluminum foil to serve as the dark chamber. A 180 μL

of either distilled water or quinine solution were added to filter paper and placed in the lighted

chamber, two centimeters from the opening. These two chambers were screwed on each side of

the center column of the T-maze with the trap door in the middle closed (Appendix 2).

Sensitivity to quinine was evaluated as in Le Bourg and Buecher (2002) with the following

modifications: 30 flies were individually placed in the dark tube of the T-maze that was maintained

in a horizontal position after the introduction of the animal. Each half of the apparatus contained

separate pieces of filter paper that could be wetted with quinine or kept dry. The quinine sensitivity

index (QSI) was determined by calculating the time in seconds that the fly spent on the dry side of

the tube when the other side had been wetted with quinine, during a five minute period. In the

12
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

absence of quinine when both sides of the tube are dry, flies quickly move to the side opposite

their point of entry and stay there. Indeed, under dry conditions, flies never reside for more than

one minute on the side of the chamber where they were placed initially (Le Bourg and Buecher,

2002). However, in the presence of quinine, flies briefly cross from the dry end of the tube into

the quinine portion of the apparatus and then quickly come back to the dry side of the chamber (Le

Bourg and Buecher, 2002).

2. Velocity Stress Test

Seven-day old flies experienced jolting using the SNAP, 30 times per minute for ten hours

from ZT 12 to ZT 22, and then were given 30 minutes to recover before being subjected to the

APS assay.

Average Pass Rates

Incidence of flies from originally the dark chamber that goes through the lighted chamber

was recorded and tallied per trial except during photopositive screening. A score of “1” was given

for a fly exhibiting a photonegative choice (remained in the dark chamber). A score of “0” was

noted for a fly entering the lighted chamber. The trials that consecutively scored with 1 up to the

last four trials were summed and divided by total number of trials to get the pass rate. The average

pass rate (APR) or the mean of all the pass rates of the 30 flies per treatment group was then

obtained together with its standard deviation and error. These values were then used to represent

the percent learning and memory retention in the descriptive statistical methods that were

performed.

13
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Assay Proper

The aversive phototaxic suppression assay (LeBourg et al., 2002) takes advantage of the

positive phototactic behavior of flies to train them to associate light with an aversive odor, in this

case quinine.

The following were the treatment groups used in the experiment: (1) non-sleep-deprived

and fed without supplement or slp+/Bm-, (2) non-sleep-deprived and fed with the supplement with

three sub-setups or slp+/Bm+ low, slp+/Bm+ mid and slp+/Bm+ high, (3) sleep-deprived and fed

without the supplement or slp-/Bm-, and (4) sleep-deprived and fed with the supplement with three

sub-setups or slp-/Bm+ low, slp-/Bm+ mid and slp-/Bm+ high. A sample size of 30 flies were tested

per setup/sub-setup.

During the experimental procedure, flies were individually screened for positive

phototaxis, subjected to SNAP (depending on assigned treatment group), trained to suppress their

preference for light and then tested for the retention of the conditioned behavior. They underwent,

a general sequence of steps comprising an experimental trial which is the basic unit of the assay

procedure.

1. Photopositive Fly Screening

The dark chamber was unscrewed from the T-maze to allow transfer of a fly into it then

screwed back to the T-maze immediately after. The lights in the room were turned off and the red

light was turned on. Acclimatization of the flies in the dark chamber were allowed for 30 seconds

before slowly turning on the light source illuminating the lighted chamber. The trap door

separating the two chambers were then slowly opened. If the fly walked towards the lighted

chamber within ten seconds, it was considered as positively phototactic and qualified for the assay.

14
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

This single-trial phase was conducted to qualitatively verify the presence of the positive phototoxic

reflex in the fly. If the fly did not show this behavior, it was not considered as photopositive

therefore, not used in the experiment since APS conditioning relies on the photopositive trait to

induce learning in the first place. Furthermore, as this behavior is essentially a behavioral reflex,

only one screening trial was performed. Doing this trial more than once might inadvertently alter

tube preference in the succeeding blocks and irrespective of the effect of the conditioning

procedure.

Fruit flies are known to be generally photopositive, hence they would prefer most of the

time to travel to the lighted tube. However, it is also observed that variation in the level of

preference to light exists even among wild-type fly groups, from highly photopositive to

practically photonegative (Hadler, 1964a; Benzer, 1967; Markow, 1975).

2. Learning Phase

For the APS assay proper, a fly that demonstrated positive phototaxis was tapped back to

the dark chamber. The trap door was closed and the lights were turned off. Thirty seconds were

allowed for acclimatization but during this time, a filter paper with quinine solution was put inside

the lighted chamber. Then, the trap door was slowly opened as well as the lights. The fly was given

10 seconds to walk on the quinine coated lighted chamber. This was repeated for nine times.

Usually, wild type flies avoid after three to five training trials (Ali et al., 2011). Failure to walk to

the lighted chamber was recorded as "Pass", which is equivalent to "task learned through

reinforcement".

15
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

The right-left (RL) double alternation of the tubes was executed in an RRLLRRLLRR

manner, switching every two trials. This alternation was performed to exclude sustained

directional bias as a source of the behavioral response (Le Bourg and Buecher, 2002).

3. Short-Term Memory Retention Phase

To assess STM retention in flies, each fly was placed back into its original food tube and

kept aside for three hours. Nootropic feeding was resumed during this resting period. Three hours

post training, each fly was put in the T-maze again in the same way as before, and the number of

times the fly avoids (pass) or goes into (fail) the lighted chamber was recorded. A total of five

trials were conducted to test STM retention. Alternation of the tubes was performed after every

trial. This phase was conducted to assess the span of effective retention, handling and retrieval of

the memory of the conditioned behavior in the flies during the one-hour STM period (Ramaswami,

2003; Tamura et al., 2003).

Statistical Analysis
To detect if differences in obtained average pass rates were significant due to Bacopa

monnieri supplement, sleep deprivation and the interaction of the two, a two-way analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was utilized. In addition, pairwise comparisons for each treatment group were

performed using Bonfferoni method. The level of significance (α) was set at 0.05.

16
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

RESULTS

Lethality Test

The group with no Bacopa monnieri supplementation had a 100% survival rate of flies.

The three lowest among the five tested concentrations showed the highest survival rate next to the

no supplement treatment group. Although notable mortality rates were still observable within one

week. The lowest B. monnieri concentration had a survival rate of 96.60% of the fruit flies. The

mid-level concentration, which is ten-fold higher, had a 90% survival rate. Lastly, 86.67% of the

fruit flies survived in the treatment group with the highest concentration of the supplement. The

last two setups with the concentrations 0.462 g/mL and 4.62 g/mL, had a survival rate of 60.00%

and 53.33%, respectively. It should also be noted that pupae formation from the two highest

concentrations were approximately 50% less compared to the three lowest concentrations

suggesting that at higher doses may have negative effects on fertility.

Quinine Sensitivity

There was no significant difference on the quinine sensitivity indices (QSI) between non-

sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived fruit flies (p<0.05). All flies in both treatment groups displayed

QSI scores well within ranges that permit normal learning together. Data were recorded alongside

17
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

the standard deviation (SD). Indeed, under dry conditions with filter paper only, flies did not reside

for more than one minute on the side of the chamber where they were placed initially (Le Bourg

and Buecher, 2002). However, in the presence of quinine, all flies in both treatment groups briefly

cross from the dark tube to the quinine on the lighted chamber of the apparatus and then quickly

go back to the dry side of the chamber.

Velocity Stress Test

The average pass rate of the fruit flies that had undergone the stress test were similar with

the APR values of the slp-/Bm- treatment group. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) results

indicated that the increased rotations per minute of the sleep deprivation machine had no

significant difference on the learning and memory retention capacity of the fruit flies in comparison

with the standard rotations per minute.

Learning Capacity

The APR values of the slp-/Bm- treatment group was 45.92%. APR values of all sleep-

deprived treatment groups decreased in comparison with the APR values of non-sleep-deprived

groups. All treatment groups with B. monnieri supplementation had increased APR values in

comparison to the no supplementation group (p<0.001). The slp+/Bm+ mid treatment group

obtained the highest APR of 81.48% among all treatment groups. The APR values of the slp+/Bm+

high group was 80.37%. The APR values of the two highest concentrations were higher compared

to that of the lowest (p<0.05). There was no significant difference between the APR values of the

two highest concentrations (p>0.05). The APR values obtained in the slp-/Bm+ groups were lower

18
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

compared to the APR values of the slp+/Bm+ groups (p>0.05). There was no interaction between

these two factors that may affect the resulting rates (p<0.01).

Short-term Memory Retention

After three hours post-training, the APR values of the slp-/Bm- treatment group was

47.33%. APR values of all sleep-deprived treatment groups decreased in comparison with those

of not sleep-deprived groups. There was an increase in the APR values of the groups slp+/Bm+

low, slp+/Bm+ mid and slp+/Bm+ high in comparison to slp+/Bm- (p<0.001). Similarly, the APR

values of the slp-/Bm+ group were higher in comparison to that of slp-/Bm- (p<0.001). The STM

retention rates of the two highest concentrations were higher compared to that of the lowest

(p<0.001). There was no significant difference between the APR of the two highest concentrations

(p>0.05). The APR values of the sleep-deprived flies with the mid and low concentrations were

not significantly different with the non-sleep-deprived counterparts (p>0.05). There was a decrease

in the APR values of the group slp-/Bm+ low in comparison to its non-sleep-deprived counterpart.

Interaction between two factors had non-existent effect and insignificant to the obtained short-term

memory retention rates (p>0.05).

19
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

DISCUSSION

As an inexpensive and simple assay, aversive phototaxis suppression (APS) assay may be

utilized to evaluate learning, short-term memory (STM) retention and response inhibition in

relation to genetic and environmental manipulations (Seugnet et al., 2009; van Swinderen, 2011).

With the exclusion of the positive phototaxis screening trial and the quinine sensitivity index (QSI)

test, the assay procedure is divided into two sequential phases: the learning phase and the STM

retention phase. Removing quinine during the APS assay also removes a contextual cue, humidity,

which is required for recall. Thus, in the STM retention phase, filter paper soaked in water was

used as a recall cue for the flies.

The presence study utilized quinine as the aversive stimulus for conditioning the avoidance

of light by D. melanogaster. Quinine sensitivity was tested in order to assess its induction of

avoidance behavior in both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies. It was observed that when

quinine was used in the T-maze test, flies briefly cross from the dark chamber of the tube into the

quinine portion of the lighted chamber and then quickly come back to the dry side of the chamber.

Moreover, quinine sensitivity of flies did not differ in non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived

20
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

conditions suggesting that contact chemosensation of the bitter tasting compound is not

significantly altered by sleep deprivation.

Since then, sleep deprivation experiments have been inherently problematic in that it is

frequently difficult to test whether an observed outcome is because of the lack of sleep or the

methods used to keep the organism awake (Rechtschaffen and Bergmann, 2002). (Insert studies).

Statistically, the APR values of learning and STM retention of the control and the one subjected

to velocity stress were similar indicating that the reduction of learning capacity and STM retention

is specifically imparted by the sleep deprivation alone. In other fly experiments concerning

neurobiological a study by Shaw et al. (2002) have already demonstrated that the SNAP used to

keep animals awake does not activate stress-response genes such as those involved in metabolic

stress, chemical stress, and humoral stress.

From the data gathered, a ten hour sleep deprivation decreases both the learning ability and

the STM retention of the flies. Low motivation is an unlikely explanation for the impairment of

learning since the number of trials it takes for a specific fly to learn was statistically lower from

the non-sleep-deprived flies that were not fed with B. monnieri supplement. Sleep deprivation also

did not alter the QSI which indicates that learning impairment is due to sleep deprivation and not

due to sleep deprivation-induced alteration in sensory thresholds. The decline of STM retention

in sleep-deprived flies may be attributed to the deficit in mushroom body plasticity (Seugnet et

al., 2008) during episodic memory encoding, resulting in impairment of subsequent retention

(Talamini et al., 2008).

Learning was observed in all non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived D. melanogaster as

confirmed by the consistent avoidance of a fly from the light in the last four trials of the learning

phase. This indicates that effective conditioning was qualitatively observed. The APR values

21
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

recorded for the non-sleep-deprived flies fed with the different B. monnieri supplement

concentrations were statistically similar to the sleep-deprived flies fed with the same B. monnieri

concentrations. In fact, even at low concentrations used in the study, B. monnieri supplement

increases learning in sleep-deprived flies at the same level as their non-sleep-deprived

counterparts. The results demonstrate that B. monnieri supplement compensates for deficiency of

learning caused by sleep deprivation. Comparisons of the three concentrations of the B. monnieri

supplement on both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies occurred at a dose–dependent

manner, reaching a statistical plateau starting on the middle dosage. Moreover, the treatment

groups with B. monnieri supplement had a faster rate in the first phase, learning as early as Trial 2

or 3, indicative of better learning capacities, than the one without B. monnieri supplement.

The results of the STM retention phase showed that B. monnieri supplement also enhances

neural processes involving memory retention in a dose-dependent manner. For both non-sleep-

deprived and sleep-deprived flies, significant increase of STM retention was observed in the two

highest concentrations. No additional functional and temporal benefit on the neural processes for

STM retention will be observed after the middle dosage has been reached. In this study, a threshold

concentration for the positive pharmacological impact of the compound for STM retention exists,

which is between lowest dosage and middle dosage. A statistical difference can be seen between

the non-sleep-deprived and the sleep-deprived flies that was fed with the lowest dosage of B.

monnieri. This finding suggests that if little experiential information was formed in the first place

during learning phase of the sleep-deprived flies, then the recent learned behavior that can be

retained and retrieved for use in future encounters with the same trial conditions would be much

smaller. The effect of B. monnieri supplement on the differential and increasing STM retention

trends were likely manifestations of (1) enhancement of B. monnieri supplement on memory phase

22
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

that following a dose dependent manner, and (2) the natural events outlining the extinction of the

memory of the conditioned behavior. In the context of the APS assay, the more frequently a trained

fly avoids the light, the more likely the behavior will be retained. In fact, it parallels to the cellular

basis of learning and memory retention (McGuire et al., 2005). This retention is an indicator of the

potentiation of the STM. The persistent strengthening of synapses of the test flies will lead to long-

term potentiation (LTP). Memory formation is dependent on changes in synaptic efficiency that

permit strengthening of associations between neurons (Heisenberg, 2003).

A plausible mechanism of mode action of this nootropic relates the effect of the bacosides

in neuron by increasing levels of serotonin when present in the nervous system in corresponding

rate of Ca2+ influx during the ionotropic pathway of LTP (Singh and Dhawan, 1997). The increase

of serotonin levels saturate the binding sites of serotonin receptors and allows the passage of more

Ca2+ ions into the mushroom body which activates protein kinase C and calmodulin kinase, causing

substrate level phosphorylation and insertion of additional serotonin receptors (McGuire et al.,

2005). The addition of more serotonin receptors to the membrane causes the post-synaptic cell to

respond more strongly to future releases of serotonin to a certain extent until all receptors are

saturated with serotonin. The synapse is thus strengthened and potentiation has occurred.

Strengthening of these synapses due to nootropic agents are demonstrable in changes of

insect behavior. In the context of fruit fly lifestyle, plant-derived nootropic compounds obtained

through feeding can have an effect on memory requiring tasks for these insects such as courtship

behavior in the wild. For example, male Drosophila show a suppression in courtship that can last

for three hours after encountering a mated female (Ackerman and Siegel, 1982). If such STM

retention observed in the study were to occur in the males through nootropic consumption, memory

of such vital details will be enhanced, decreasing the chance that the male fly to again attempt

23
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

courting the same females that avoided him. This event will lead to a conservation of energy and

an increase in reproductive success over time (Siegel and Hall, 1979; Griffith and Ejima, 2009).

The implication of the inclusion of natural nootropic in a daily-based diet for other species

may improve mental performance, without the risks of side effects. Wide ranges of natural

nootropics range from artichoke leaves to the Chinese club moss plant and even amino acids like

tryptophan which is found in poultry products and theanine that naturally occurs in green tea can

be included in a daily dietary plan. In dietary science, proper combination of natural nootropics,

known as nootropic stacking, synergistically enhances the efficacy of each active ingredient of the

nootropic (Spencer, 2009). Hence, having a diet which includes a natural nootropic may be able

to deter failures of mental performances including sleep deprivation.

These observations imply that the enhancing effects of B. monnieri supplement on learning

and STM retention in D. melanogaster follows a dose-dependent manner, with the optimal level

between the lowest and the middle dosage. In addition, having a natural nootropic diet may be able

to deter failures of mental performances including sleep deprivation.

24
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

CONCLUSION

The effects of Bacopa monnieri supplementation enhances both the learning capacities and

short-term memory retention of both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived seven-day old female

fruit flies. In addition, advantageous effects were dose-dependent from the low, 4.62x10-4g/mL to

the mid, 4.62x10-3g/mL concentrations for both learning and short-term memory retention with

middle dosage being the optimal concentration among the three concentrations used.

25
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

RECOMMENDATIONS

The independence of the nootropic on the sleep-deprived condition signifies possible

similar effects to other neurobiological conditions. Future efforts are hence suggested to more

accurately test the different neurobiological conditions on humans with measurable doses in its

optimum condition. Comprehensive studies following suit may soon be able to use the information

from Drosophila research in identifying the biologically-equivalent doses for learning and memory

retention in humans.

To test whether the observed behavior to alterations in certain biological structures are due

to the effects of the nootropic, biochemical and anatomical screenings of nervous tissue regions

are also recommended. These techniques may prove useful in monitoring developmental stage-

and age-specific effects of B. monnieri supplement intake. Furthermore, such approach can be of

help in investigating the impact of the nootropic in natural Drosophila activities such as courtship

and foraging.

The APS assay is also recommended to be used in conjuction with pharmacological effects

(i.e. other drugs and compounds) on learning and STM retention. This can be done by evaluating

the impact of other compounds believed or known to alter neurophysiological and behavioral

capacities, such as alcohol and caffeine. Finally, further attempts to improve the APS protocol

developed and tested here are urged to be conducted, since the increase in the quality and relevance

of the obtained data are dependent on such efforts.

26
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

LITERATURE CITED

Abrilla, A., Mangulabnan, M., and de Vera, M. 2013. Evaluation of short-term memory in
monosodium glutamate-fed fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) using aversive
phototaxic suppression. University of the Philippines Manila.
Ackerman, S. L. and Siegel, R. W. 1982. Chemically reinforced conditioned courtship in
Drosophila: responses of wildtype and the dunce, amnesiac and don giovanni mutants. J.
Neurogenet.
Alonso, W. J. and Schuck-Paim, C. 2006. The ‘ghosts’ that pester studies on learning in
mosquitoes: guidelines to chase them off. Med. Vet. Entomol. 20, 157-165.
Ali, Y. O., Escala, W., Ruan, K., and Zhai, R. G. 2011. Assaying Locomotor, Learning, and
Memory Deficits in Drosophila Models of Neurodegeneration. J. Vis. Exp.
Barkley, R. A. 2006. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and
treatment 3rd edition. Gulliford, NY: 2006.
Belenky, G., Wesensten, N.J., and Thorne, D.R. 2003. Patterns of performance degradation and
restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: A sleep dose response study.
J Sleep Res 2003; 12:1–12.
Benzer, S. 1967. Behavioral mutants of Drosophila isolated by countercurrent distribution. Proc.
Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 58: 1112-1119.
Bhattacharya S.K., and Ghoshal, A. 1998. Anxiolytic activity of a standardized extract of Bacopa
monnieri: An experimental study. Phytomed., 1998, 5: 95-100.
Bhattacharya, S.K., Bhattacharya, A., Kumar, A., and Ghosal, S. 2000. Antioxidant activity of
Bacopa monnieri in rat frontal cortex, striatum and hippocampus. Phytotherapy Research,
14: 174-179.
Blumenthal, M.E. 2003. The American Botanical Council (ABC) Clinical Guide to Herbs. Austin,
Texas: The American Botanical Council.
Bose, K.C. and Bose, N.K. 1931. Observations on the actions and uses of Herpestis monnieri. J
Ind Med Assoc 1:60
Breed, M. D., Garry, M. F., Pearce, A. N., Hibbard, B. E., Bjostad, L. B. and Page, R. E. 1995.
The role of wax comb in honey bee nestmate recognition. Anim. Behav. 50, 489-496.

Brembs, B. 2009. Mushroom bodies regulate habit formation in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 19: 1351-
1355.

Brioni, J.D., Hock, F.J., McGaugh, J.J. 2007. Drug effects on learning and memory. In: Vogel GH,
Vogel WH, editors. Drug discovery and evaluation: pharmacological assays. New York:
Springer, 1997. p. 335 – 6.

Bushey, D., Huber, R., Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. 2007. Drosophila Hyperkinetic mutants have
reduced sleep and impaired memory. J Neurosci.

27
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Butler, C. G., Fletcher, D. J. C. and Watler, D. 1969. Nest-entrance marking with pheromones by
the honeybee Apis mellifera L., and by a wasp, Vespula vulgaris L. Anim. Behav. 17, 142-
147.

Das A., Shanker G., Nath C., Pal R., Singh, S. and Singh, H.K. 2002. A comparative study in
rodents of standardized extracts of Bacopa monnieri and Ginkgo biloba: Anticholinestrase
and cognitive enhancing activities. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 2002.

de Belle, J. S. and Heisenberg, M. 1994. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by


chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science. 263: 692-695.

Deepak, M., and Amit, A. 2004. The need for establishing identities of 'bacoside A and B', the
putative major bioactive saponins of Indian medicinal plant Bacopa monnieri.
Phytomedicine, 11: 264-268.

Dey, C. D., Koley, P. N., and Dutta, C. P. 1964. Chemical and pharmacological properties of
Brahmi. Journal of Experimental Medical Sciences, 8: 1-13.

Dimond, S. J. and Brouwers, E. Y. M. 1976. Increase in the power of human memory in normal
man through the use of drugs. Psychopharmacol 49, 307–309.

Galizia, C. G., Kunze, J., Gumbert, A., Borg-Karlson, A. K., Sachse, S., Markl, C. and Menzel, R.
2004. Relationship of visual and olfactory signal parameters in a food-deceptive flower
mimicry system. Behav. Ecol. 16, 159-168.

Ganguly-Fitzgerald, I., Donlea, J., and Shaw, P.J. 2006. Waking experience affects sleep need in
Drosophila. Science.

Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K., and Yamasaki, K. 1996a. Dammarane-type triterpenoid
saponins from Bacopa monnieri. Phytochemistry, 42: 815-820.

Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K. and Yamasaki, K. 1996b. 'Bacopasaponin D- a


pseudojujubogenin glycoside from Bacopa monnieri. Phytochemistry, 43: 2447- 2449.

Grüter, C., Acosta, L. E. and Farina, W. M. 2006. Propagation of olfactory information within the
honeybee hive. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 60, 707-715.

Hadler, N. M. 1964a. Genetic influence on phototaxis in Drosophila melanogaster. Biol. Bull.


126: 264-273.

Hadler, N. M. 1964b. Heritability and phototaxis in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 50: 1269-
1277.

Hathcock, J. 2001. Dietary supplements: how they are used and regulated. Journal of Nutrition
(3s) 1114- 1117.
Heberlein, U. 2000. Genetics of alcohol-induced behaviors in Drosophila. Alcohol Res. Health.
24: 185-188.

28
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Heisenberg, M. 2003. Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4:266-
275.

Hendricks, J.C., Finn, S.M., and Panckeri, K.A. 2000. Rest in Drosophila is a sleeplike state.
Neuron 2000.

Hendricks, J.C., Williams J.A., and Panckeri, K. 2001. A non-circadian role for
cAMP signaling and CREB activity in Drosophila rest homeostasis. Nat Neurosci
2001.

Himi, T., Saito, H., and Nishiyama, N. 1989. Effect of Ginseng saponins on the survival of cerebral
cortex neurons in cell cultures. Chemical and Pharmaceutical Bulletin, 37(2), 481-484.

Hofferberth, B. 1994. The efficacy of EGb 761 in patients with senile dementia of the Alzheimer
type, a double blind, placebo controlled study on different levels of investigation. Human
Psychopharmacology, 9, 215-222.

Hong, Y.K., Park, S.H., Lee, S. 2011. Neuroprotective effect of SuHeXiang Wan in Drosophila
models of Alzheimer’s disease. J Ethnopharmacol. 2011;134:1028–1032

Huber, R., Hill, S. L., Holladay, C., Biesiadecki, M., Tononi, G., Cirelli, C. 2004. Sleep
homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep 27, 628–63.
Ishikawa, A., Kanayama, Y., Matsumura, H., Tsuchimochi, H., Ishida, Y., and Nakamura, S. 2006.
Selective rapid eye movement sleep deprivation impairs the maintenance of long-term
potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 24: 243-8.

Jain, P., Khanna, N.K., Trehan, T., Pendse, V.K., and Godhwani, J.L. 1994. Anti-inflammatory
effects of an Ayurvedic preparation, Brahmi rasayan, in rodents. Indian Journal of
Experimental Biology, 32, 633-636.

Jansen, R. L. M., Brogan, B., Whitworth, A.J., and Okello, E.J. 2014. Effects of Five Ayurvedic
Herbs on Locomotor Behaviour in a Drosophila melanogaster Parkinson’s Disease Model.

Kayser, M.S., Mainwaring, B., Yue, Z., and Sehgal, A. 2015. Sleep deprivation suppresses
aggression in Drosophila. eLife.

Kelly, W.E., Kelley, K.E., and Clanton, R.C. 2001. The relationship between sleep length and
grade-point average among college students. Coll Stud J. 35:84–86.

Kishore, K. and Sing, M. 2005. Effect of bacosides, alcoholic extract of Bacopa monnieri Linn.
(Brahmi) on experimental amnesia in mice. Indian J Exp Biol.

Klug, W. S., Cummings, M. R. and Spencer, C. A. 2007. Essentials of Genetics (6th ed.). Pearson
Education South Asia Pte Ltd. Jurong, Singapore.

29
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Koltermann, A., Hartkorn, A., Koch, E., Fürst, R., Vollmar, A. M., and Zahler, S. 2007. Ginkgo
biloba extract EGb 761 increases endothelial nitric oxide production in vitro and in vivo.
Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 64, 1715–1722.

Koch, E. 2005. Inhibition of platelet activating factor (PAF)-induced aggregation of human


thrombocytes by ginkgolides: considerations on possible bleeding complications after oral
intake of Ginkgo biloba extracts. Phytomedicine.

Kopp C., Longordo, F., Nicholson, J.R., and Luthi, A. 2006. Insufficient sleep reversibly alters
bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. J Neurosci 26: 12456-65.

LeBars, P.L., Katz, M.M., Berman, N., Turan, M., Freedman, A.M., and Schatzberg, A.F. 1997.
A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of Gingko biloba for
dementia. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278(16), 1327-1332.

Le Bourg, É. and Badia, J. 1995. Decline in photopositive tendencies with age in Drosophila
melanogaster. J. Insect Behav. 8: 835-845.

Le Bourg, É. and Buecher, C. 2002. Learned suppression of photopositive tendencies in


Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Learn. Behav. 30: 330-341.

Le Bourg, É. 2004. Effects of aging on learned suppression of photopositive tendencies in


Drosophila melanogaster. Neurobiol. Aging. 25: 1241-1252.

Le Bourg, É. 2005. Humidity as an aversive stimulus in learning in Drosophila melanogaster.


Learn. Behav. 33: 265-276.

Li, X., Yu, F., and Guo, A. Sleep deprivation specifically impairs short-term olfactory memory in
Drosophila. Sleep 2009.

Liu, W., Guo F., Lu, B., and Guo, A. 2008. Amnesiac regulates sleep onset and maintenance in
Drosophila melanogaster. Biochem 2008 372:798-803.
Liu, L., Wolf, R., Ernst, R. and Heisenberg, M. 1999. Context generalization in Drosophila visual
learning requires the mushroom bodies. Nature. 400: 753-756.

Lyons, L.C., Rawashdeh, O., Katzoff, A., Susswein, A.J., Eskin, A. 2005. Circadian modulation
of complex learning in diurnal and nocturnal Aplysia. Proc Natl Acad Sci 102: 12589–
12594.
Lyons, L.C., Rawashdeh, O., Eskin, A. 2006. Non-ocular circadian oscillators and photoreceptors
modulate long-term memory formation in Aplysia. J Biol Rhythms 21: 245–255.
Ma, T.C., Yu, Q.H., and Chen, M.H. 1991. Effects of ginseng stem-leaves saponins on one way
avoidance behaviour in rats. Zhongguo Yao Li Xue Bao, 12: 403- 406

30
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Ma, T.C., and Yu, Q.H. 1993. Effect of 20-(S)-Ginsenoside-R2 and cyproheptadine on two-way
active avoidance learning and memory in rats. Arzneim-Forsch/Drug Research, 43: 1049-
1052.
Mahato, S.B., Garai, S., and Chakravarty, A.K. 2000. Bacopasaponins E and F: two jujubogenin
bisdesmosides from Bacopa monnieri. Phytochemistry, 53: 711- 714.
Margulies, C., Tully, T. and Dubnau, J. 2005.Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr. Biol.
15: 700-713.
Markow, T. A. 1975. A genetic analysis of phototactic behavior in Drosophila melanogaster.
Genetics. 79: 527-534.
McCall, P. J. and Kelly, D. W. 2002. Learning and memory in disease vectors. Trends Parasitol.
18, 429-433.
McDermott CM, Hardy MN, Bazan N.G., and Magee JC. 2006. Sleep deprivation induced
alterations in excitatory synaptic transmission in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus. J
Physiol 570:553-65
McGuire, S. E., Deshazer, M. and Davis, R. L. 2005. Thirty years of olfactory learning and
memory research in Drosophila melanogaster. Prog. Neurobiol. 76: 328-347.
Medina, P.M., Cabaccan, J.S. and Asis, J. L. 2015. Effect of natural and artificial sweeteners on
the hemolymph glucose level (HGL) in Drosophila melanogaster. International Journal of
Biosciences. 07:119-131.
Menzel, R. and Shmida, A. 1993. The ecology of flower colours and the natural colour vision of
insect pollinators: the Israeli flora as a study case. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 68, 81-
120
Morgan, M., and Bone, K. 1999. Bacopa. Modern Phytotherapist, September, 21-27.
Nitz D.A., van Swinderen, B., Tononi G., and Greenspan, R.J. 2002. Electrophysiological
correlates of rest and activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol 200212:1934-40.
Pall R. and Sarin, J.P.S. 1992. Quantitative determination of bacosides by UV-Spectrophotometry.
Ind. J. Pharm. Sci.
Perisse, E., Portelli, G., Le Goas, S., Teste, E. and Le Bourg, É. 2007. Futher characterization of
an aversive learning task in Drosophila melanogaster: intensity of the stimulus, relearning,
and use of rutabaga mutants. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 193: 1139-1149.
Ramaswami, M. 2003. Specifying the age-specific component of a short-term memory. Neuron.
40: 877-879.
Rechtschaffen, A., and Bergmann, B.M. 2002. Sleep deprivation in the rat: An update of the 1989
paper. Sleep 25, 18–24.
Sahakian, B. and Morein-Zamir, S. 2007. Professor's little helper. Nature 450 (7173):1157-9.
Schulz, V., Hubner, W.D., and Ploch, M. 1997. Clinical trials with phytopsychopharmacological
agents. Phytomedicine, 4: 379-387.

31
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Seugnet, L., Suzuki, Y., Stidd, R. and Shaw, P. J. 2009. Aversive phototaxic suppression:
evaluation of a short-term memory assay in Drosophila melanogaster. Genes Brain Behav.
8: 377-389.

Shanker G. and Singh H.K. 2000. Anxiolytic profile of standardized Brahmi extract. Ind. J.
Pharmacol.

Shaw, P.J., Cirelli, C., Greenspan, R.J., and Tononi, G. 2000. Correlates of sleep and waking in
Drosophila melanogaster. Science 2000; 287:1834-7.

Shaw, P.J., Tononi. G., Greenspan, R.J., and Robinson, D.F. 2002. Stress response genes
protect against lethal effects of sleep deprivation in Drosophila. Nature 417:287-91.

Siegel, R. W. and Hall, J. C. 1979. Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and
mutant Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 76: 3430-3434.

Singh, H.K., and Dhawan, B.N. 1982. Effect of Bacopa monnieri Linn. (Brahmi) extract on
avoidance responses in rats. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 5: 205- 214.

Singh, H.K., and Dhawan, B.N. 1997. Neuropsychopharmacological effects of the ayurvedic
nootropic Bacopa monnieri linn. (brahmi). Indian Journal of Pharmacology, 29: 359-s365.

Smith, P.F., Maclennan, K., and Darlington, C.L. The neuroprotective properties of the Gingko
biloba leaf: a review of the possible relationship to platelet activating factor (PAF). J
Ethnopharmacol 1996, 50:131 – 139.

Spencer, J. P. 2009. Flavonoids and brain health: multiple effects underpinned by common
mechanisms. Genes Nutr. 4, 243–250.

Stäubli U., Rogers G., and Lynch, G. 1994. Facilitation of glutamate receptors enhances memory.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:777–781.

Talamini, L.M., Nieuwenhuis, I.L., Takashima, A., and Jensen, O. 2008. Sleep directly following
learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. Learn Mem.

Tamura, T., Chiang, A.S., Ito, N. 2003. Aging specifically impairs amnesiac dependent memory
in Drosophila. Neuron 2003; 40:1003-11.

Tamura, T., Chiang, A.-S., Ito, N., Liu, H.-P., Horiuchi, J., Tully, T. and Saitoe, M. 2003. Aging
specifically impairs amnesiac-dependent memory in Drosophila. Neuron. 40: 1003-1011.

Tan, M.S., Yu, J.T., Tan, C.C., Wang, H.F., Meng, X.F., and Wang, C. 2015. Efficacy and adverse
effects of Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015; 43: 589-603.

32
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Tripathi, Y.B., Chaurasia, S., Tripathi, E., Upadhyay, A., and Dubey, G.P. 1996. Bacopa monnieri
Linn. as an antioxidant: mechanism of action. Indian Journal of Experimental Biology,
34(6), 523-526.

Tang, F., Nag, S., Shiu, S. Y., and Pang, S. F. 2002. The effects of melatonin and Ginkgo biloba
extract on memory loss and choline acetyltransferase activities in the brain of rats infused
intracerebroventricularly with beta-amyloid. Life Sci. 71, 2625–2631.

Tully, T. and Quinn, W. G. 1985.Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant
Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 157: 263-277.

U. S. Food and Drug Administration and Center for Drug Evaluation and Research. 2006.
Dietary supplements: what you need to know. FDA public health advisory. Washington,
DC.
Waddell, S. and Quinn, W. G. 2001. What can we teach Drosophila? What can they teach us?.
Trends Genet. 17: 719-726.
Wolf, R., Wittig, T., Liu, L., Wustmann, G., Eyding, D. and Heisenberg, M. 1998. Drosophila
mushroom bodies are dispensable for visual, tactile and motor learning. Learn. Mem. 5:
166-178.
Vesper, J., and Hansgen, K.D. 1994. Efficacy of Gingko biloba in 90 outpatients with cerebral
insufficiency caused by old age: Results of a placebo-controlled double-blind trial.
Phytomedicine, 1, 9-16.
Vosshall, L. B. 2007. Into the mind of a fly. Nature. 450: 193-197.
Xia, S. Z., Liu, L., Feng, C. and Guo, A. K. 1997a. Memory consolidation in Drosophila operant
visual learning. Learn. Mem. 4: 205-218.
Xia, S. Z., Liu, L., Feng, C. and Guo, A. K. 1997b. Drug disruption of short-term memory in
Drosophila melanogaster. Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav. 58: 727-735.
Yamaguchi, S., Desplan, C., and Heisenberg, M. 2010. Contribution of photoreceptor subtypes to
spectral wavelength preference in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 107 (2010), pp.
5634–5639
Yoo, S.S., Hu, P.T., Gujar, N., Jolesz, F.A., Walker, M.P. 2007. A deficit in the ability to form
new human memories without sleep. Nat Neurosci 10:385-92.

33
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

TABLES

Table 1. Difference on the Quinines Sensitivity Index (QSI) of both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-
deprived D. melanogaster. All flies display QSI scores well within ranges that permit
normal learning. The final QSI is the average of the scores obtained for 30 flies ± standard
deviation (SD) values.

Fly Type ( n=30 ) Quinine Sensitivity Index

Non-Sleep-Deprived D. melanogaster 4:29 ± 0:13

Sleep-deprived D. melanogaster 4.34 ± 0:18

34
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Table 2. Average Pass Rates for Learning and Memory Retention of non-sleep-deprived
Drosophila

Fly Bm- Bm- Bm+ low Bm+ low Bm+mid Bm+ mid Bm+ high Bm+ high
number Learning MR Learning MR Learning MR Learning MR
1 44.44% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 77.78% 100.00% 88.89% 80.00%
2 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00%
3 44.44% 80.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 77.78% 60.00%
4 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00%
5 44.44% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 66.67% 100.00%
6 44.44% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 66.67% 100.00%
7 44.44% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 55.56% 100.00% 88.89% 80.00%
8 66.67% 60.00% 55.56% 60.00% 88.89% 60.00% 55.56% 80.00%
9 77.78% 80.00% 55.56% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 80.00%
10 55.56% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 60.00%
11 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 60.00%
12 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00%
13 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00% 77.78% 100.00% 77.78% 100.00%
14 55.56% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 60.00%
15 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 77.78% 100.00%
16 55.56% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 60.00%
17 66.67% 40.00% 88.89% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00%
18 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 40.00% 88.89% 100.00%
19 55.56% 60.00% 55.56% 60.00% 66.67% 100.00% 55.56% 60.00%
20 44.44% 60.00% 44.44% 60.00% 88.89% 40.00% 77.78% 80.00%
21 66.67% 40.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 77.78% 100.00%
22 77.78% 40.00% 88.89% 80.00% 66.67% 100.00% 55.56% 80.00%
23 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00% 55.56% 80.00% 66.67% 100.00%
24 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 40.00% 88.89% 80.00%
25 55.56% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 60.00%
26 55.56% 60.00% 66.67% 40.00% 88.89% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00%
27 66.67% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 60.00%
28 55.56% 40.00% 88.89% 60.00% 66.67% 100.00% 88.89% 100.00%
29 44.44% 60.00% 77.78% 40.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 60.00%
30 88.89% 40.00% 77.78% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 100.00%
Average
60.00% 60.00% 72.60% 68.67% 81.48% 80.67% 80.37% 79.33%
Pass Rate

35
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Table 3. Average Pass Rates for Learning and Memory Retention of sleep-deprived Drosophila
Fly Bm- Bm- Bm+ low Bm+ low Bm+mid Bm+ mid Bm+ high Bm+ high
number Learning MR Learning MR Learning MR Learning MR
1 55.56% 20.00% 77.78% 40.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 60.00%
2 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 55.56% 100.00%
3 55.56% 60.00% 88.89% 60.00% 77.78% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00%
4 44.44% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00% 77.78% 80.00% 88.89% 40.00%
5 44.44% 0.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00%
6 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 80.00% 77.78% 40.00%
7 55.56% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00% 55.56% 60.00%
8 66.67% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 55.56% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00%
9 44.44% 40.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 66.67% 80.00%
10 44.44% 40.00% 55.56% 20.00% 66.67% 80.00% 88.89% 60.00%
11 44.44% 60.00% 88.89% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 66.67% 40.00%
12 55.56% 60.00% 88.89% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 80.00%
13 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 40.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 100.00%
14 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00%
15 0.00% 0.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 77.78% 80.00%
16 44.44% 80.00% 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00%
17 44.44% 80.00% 55.56% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 80.00%
18 44.44% 60.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 100.00%
19 55.56% 80.00% 77.78% 80.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00%
20 44.44% 60.00% 44.44% 20.00% 66.67% 80.00% 77.78% 60.00%
21 44.44% 60.00% 44.44% 40.00% 88.89% 80.00% 77.78% 80.00%
22 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00%
23 44.44% 0.00% 66.67% 40.00% 66.67% 80.00% 66.67% 80.00%
24 44.44% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 60.00%
25 44.44% 60.00% 55.56% 40.00% 88.89% 80.00% 88.89% 100.00%
26 66.67% 40.00% 77.78% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 77.78% 100.00%
27 0.00% 0.00% 77.78% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00% 66.67% 80.00%
28 44.44% 40.00% 66.67% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 66.67% 60.00%
29 44.44% 40.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 100.00% 88.89% 100.00%
30 55.56% 40.00% 77.78% 60.00% 88.89% 80.00% 55.56% 80.00%
Average
45.92% 47.33% 71.11% 57.33% 77.78% 78.00% 76.30% 76.00%
Pass Rate

36
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

FIGURES

Figure 1. Survival rate of Drosophila melanogaster on the difference of 10-fold concentration of


Bacopa monnieri Supplement over the course of seven days.

Figure 2. Comparison of the effect of velocity stress caused by mechanical stimulation of the
machine on the learning and short-term memory retention of D. melanogaster (n=30). The
results indicate that stress did not affect both learning and memory (P<0.05).

37
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Standardized

Learning STM
Figure 3. Standardized average pass rates for learning and memory retention of non-sleep-
deprived and sleep deprived (slpD) D. melanogaster (n=30). Low dosage corresponds to 4.62 x
10-4 g/mL, middle dosage corresponds to 4.62 x 10-3 g/mL and high dosage corresponds to 4.62 x
10-2 g/mL. Error bars represent standard error (SE) values.

38
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016
TRIALS TRIALS
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 4 5

TRIALS
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

slp+/Bm-

slp+/Bm+ low

slp+/Bm+ mid

slp+/Bm+ high

slp-/Bm-

slp-/Bm+ low

slp-/Bm+ mid

slp-/Bm+ high

PASS
FAIL
Figure 4. Pass-Fail Map for Learning (left) and STM Retention (right) of D. melanogaster

39
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Figure 5. Mean score of photonegative choices (1 or 0) by non-sleep-deprived D. melanogaster


on the learning phase and short-term memory retention of different treatment groups
(n per group = 30)

Figure 6. Mean score of photonegative choices (1 or 0) by slpD D. melanogaster on the learning


phase and short-term memory retention of different treatment groups (n per group = 30)

40
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

APPENDICES

Appendix 1a. Media synthesis (adapted from the Medina et al., 2015).
1. Mix the following in a 2000-mL Erlenmeyer flask.
 Distilled water: 1000 mL
 Sweet Potato: 500 g
 Bacto ™ Agar: 15 g
 Baker’s yeast: 10 g
2. Boil the resulting mixture using autoclave.
3. Add 0.08% propionic when boiled media is cooled to 70°C.
4. Add yeast paste made with 0.08% propionic acid for optimum reproduction.
5. Place the content in the breeding bottles (approx. 450 mL per bottle).
6. Transfer the flies into the freshly prepared sweet potato medium.

Appendix 1b. Summary of the Bacopa monnieri Memory Enhancing Supplement

Memory-Enhancing Concentration That Will Be Proposed Mode of Action


Supplement Used (g/mL)
Improves consolidation of
learned behavior and delays
Bacopa monnieria 4.62 x 10-4 (Low) its extinction by enhancing
protein kinase activity and
4.62 x 10-3 (Mid)
new protein synthesis
4.62 x 10-2 (High) (Bhattacharya and Ghoshal,
1998).
a
Taking into account that 500 mg of capsulated B. monnieri supplement is equivalent to 55±5% w/w of
dried herb.

41
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Appendix 2. T-Maze Setup for Aversive Phototaxis Suppression Assay. (A) The overall experimental
setup for the APSA experiment with the light source connected to the "lighted" falcon
tube, lined with filter paper and the "dark" tube on the left (covered in foil), separated by
a trap door. (b) During the training and testing phase, the fly is in the dark chamber and
the trap door opened after the light is turned on in the lighted chamber.

Trap Door

Light Chamber Dark Chamber

A.

Quinine Soaked
Filter Paper Red Light

Light Source

B.

42
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Appendix 3. Sleep Nullifying Apparatus (SNAP) rhythmically jolts flies up and down, displacing
sleepy flies fifteen times per minute. In each trial, flies will be sleep deprived using
the SNAP from zeitgeber time (ZT) 0 to ZT 10 (beginning of the light phase).

Speed Controller
Unit

43
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Appendix 4. Methodology flowchart. Flies were repeatedly introduced to the apparatus in


several trials.

TREATMENT GROUPS
Not Sleep-Deprived Group Sleep-Deprived Group
No No
Supplement w/ Bacopa monnieri w/ Bacopa manniera
Supplement
Supple Supple
Individual
fly

General Assay Procedure Pre-Tests


Velocity Quinine
Positive Phototaxis Screening Trial
Stress Test Sensitivity
immediately

Learning Phase
Aversive Phototaxic Suppression

3 h after

Memory Retention Phase

Per Treatment Group: Average Pass Rate Calculation

Statistical Analysis: Two-Way ANOVA, Bonferroni Method

44
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

APPENDIX 5
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) and Post-hoc Analysis Results

Appendix 5a. Two way ANOVA results for Velocity Stress Test

Source of Variation % of total P-value P-value Significant


variation summary
Interaction 1725e-14 > 0.9999 Ns No
Row Factor 1.580 0.1750 Ns No
Column Factor 2.527e-7 0.9996 Ns No

SS df MS F P-value
Interaction 3.638e-12 1 3.638e-12 2.033e-14 P > 0.999
Row Factor 333.3 1 333.3 1.863 P = 0.1750
Column Factor 5.329e-5 1 5.329e-5 2.97e-7 P = 0.9996
Residual 20759 116 179

Appendix 5b. Two way ANOVA results for the Learning Phase
N=240 R-squared = 0.5054
Root MSE = 11.6144 Adj R-squared = 0.4905

Factor Partial SS df MS F Prob > F


Bacopa monnieri 28693.6687 3 9564.55625 70.90 0.0000
Sleep deprivation 1671.70103 1 1671.70103 12.39 0.0005
Bacopa + sleep deprivation 1615.48719 3 538.49573 3.99 0.0850

Appendix 5c. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions between the control and
treatment groups for the Learning Phase
Factor
Bacopa monnieri conc. Contrast Standard Error t P > |t|
low vs. none 20.373 2.12049 9.61 0.000
low vs. mid 27.59467 2.12049 13.01 0.000
low vs. high 25.55783 2.12049 12.05 0.000
mid vs. none 7.221567 2.12049 3.41 0.005
mid vs. high 5.184833 2.12049 2.45 0.091
high vs. none -2.036833 2.12049 -0.96 1.000

Sleep Deprivation
slpD vs. normal -5.278417 1.499413 -3.52 0.001

45
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Appendix 5d. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions for the Learning Phase Using
Bonferroni Method

x1=B. monnieri treatment groups (0= none, 1= low, 2= mid, 3= high)


x2=sleep deprivation groups (0=normal, 1=slpD)

Treatment Groups (x1,x2) Contrast Std. Error t P > |t|


(0,1) vs. (0,0) -14.077 2.998826 -4.69 0.000
(1,0) vs. (0,0) 13.705 2.998826 4.57 0.000
(1,1) vs. (0,0) 12.964 2.998826 4.32 0.001
(2,0) vs. (0,0) 21.85233 2.998826 7.29 0.000
(2,1) vs. (0,0) 19.26 2.998826 6.42 0.000
(3,0) vs. (0,0) 20.371 2.998826 6.79 0.000
(3,1) vs. (0,0) 16.66767 2.998826 5.56 0.000
(1,0) vs. (0,1) 27.782 2.998826 9.26 0.000
(1,1) vs. (0,1) 27.041 2.998826 9.02 0.000
(2,0) vs. (0,1) 35.92933 2.998826 11.98 0.000
(2,1) vs. (0,1) 33.337 2.998826 11.12 0.000
(3,0) vs. (0,1) 34.448 2.998826 11.49 0.000
(3,1) vs. (0,1) 30.74467 2.998826 10.25 0.000
(1,1) vs. (1,0) -0.741 2.998826 -0.25 1.000
(2,0) vs. (1,0) 8.147334 2.998826 2.72 0.000
(2,1) vs. (1,0) 5.555 2.998826 1.85 0.001
(3,0) vs. (1,0) 6.666 2.998826 2.22 0.000
(3,1) vs. (1,0) 2.962667 2.998826 0.99 0.001
(2,0) vs. (1,1) 8.888334 2.998826 2.96 0.001
(2,1) vs. (1,1) 6.296 2.998826 2.10 0.001
(3,0) vs. (1,1) 7.407 2.998826 2.47 0.000
(3,1) vs. (1,1) 3.703667 2.998826 1.24 0.001
(2,1) vs. (2,0) -2.592334 2.998826 -0.86 1.000
(3,0) vs. (2,0) -1.481333 2.998826 -0.49 1.000
(3,1) vs. (2,0) -5.184667 2.998826 -1.73 1.000
(3,0) vs. (2,1) 1.111 2.998826 0.37 1.000
(3,1) vs. (2,1) -2.592333 2.998826 -0.86 1.000
(3,1) vs. (3,0) -3.703334 2.998826 -1.23 1.000

46
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

Appendix 5e. Two way ANOVA results for the Memory Retention Phase

N=240 R-squared = 0.3066


Root MSE = 17.579 Adj R-squared = 0.2857

Factor Partial SS df MS F Prob > F


Bacopa monnieri 27098.3333 3 9032.77778 29.23 0.0000
Sleep deprivation 3375 1 3375 10.92 0.0011
Bacopa + sleep deprivation 1231.66667 3 410.555556 1.33 0.2658

Appendix 5f. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions between the control and
treatment groups for the Memory Retention Phase
Factor
Bacopa monnieri conc. Contrast Standard Error t P > |t|
low vs. none 9.333333 3.209481 2.91 0.024
low vs. mid 25.66667 3.209481 8.00 0.000
low vs. high 24 3.209481 7.48 0.000
mid vs. none 16.33333 3.209481 5.09 0.000
mid vs. high 14.66667 3.209481 4.57 1.000
high vs. none -1.666667 3.209481 -0.52 0.000

Sleep Deprivation
slpD vs. normal -7.5 2.269446 -3.30 0.001

Appendix 5g. Pairwise Comparisons of Marginal Linear Predictions for the Memory Retention
Phase Using Bonferroni Method
x1=B. monnieri treatment groups (0= none, 1= low, 2= mid, 3= high)
x2=sleep deprivation groups (0=normal, 1=slpD)

Treatment Groups (x1,x2) Contrast Std. Error t P > |t|


(0,1) vs. (0,0) -12.66667 4.538891 -2.79 0.000
(1,0) vs. (0,0) 8.666667 4.538891 1.91 0.000
(1,1) vs. (0,0) -2.666667 4.538891 -0.59 1.000
(2,0) vs. (0,0) 20.66667 4.538891 4.55 0.000
(2,1) vs. (0,0) 18 4.538891 3.97 0.003
(3,0) vs. (0,0) 19.33333 4.538891 4.26 0.001
(3,1) vs. (0,0) 16 4.538891 3.53 0.014
(1,0) vs. (0,1) 21.33333 4.538891 4.70 0.000
(1,1) vs. (0,1) 10 4.538891 2.20 0.000
(2,0) vs. (0,1) 33.33333 4.538891 7.34 0.000

47
Effects of Bacopa monnieri…
Ong and Villanueva, 2016

(2,1) vs. (0,1) 30.66667 4.538891 6.76 0.000


(3,0) vs. (0,1) 32 4.538891 7.05 0.000
(3,1) vs. (0,1) 28.66667 4.538891 6.32 0.000
(1,1) vs. (1,0) -11.33333 4.538891 -2.50 0.001
(2,0) vs. (1,0) 12 4.538891 2.64 0.000
(2,1) vs. (1,0) 9.333333 4.538891 2.06 0.001
(3,0) vs. (1,0) 10.66667 4.538891 2.35 0.000
(3,1) vs. (1,0) 7.33333 4.538891 1.62 0.001
(2,0) vs. (1,1) 23.33333 4.538891 5.14 0.000
(2,1) vs. (1,1) 20.66667 4.538891 4.55 0.000
(3,0) vs. (1,1) 22 4.538891 4.85 0.000
(3,1) vs. (1,1) 18.66667 4.538891 4.11 0.002
(2,1) vs. (2,0) -2.666667 4.538891 -0.59 1.000
(3,0) vs. (2,0) -1.333333 4.538891 -0.29 1.000
(3,1) vs. (2,0) -4.666667 4.538891 -1.03 1.000
(3,0) vs. (2,1) 1.333333 4.538891 0.29 1.000
(3,1) vs. (2,1) -2 4.538891 -0.44 1.000
(3,1) vs. (3,0) -3.333333 4.538891 -0.73 1.000

Appendix 5h. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) results between Average Pass Rates of
Learning and Memory Retention

MR Learning
MR 1.000
Learning 0.4739 1.000

48

S-ar putea să vă placă și