Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

The Ciliary Corona: Physical Model and Simulation of

the Fine Needles Radiating from Point Light Sources


Thomas J. T. P. van den Berg, Michiel P. J. Hagenouw, and Joris E. Coppens

PURPOSE. Most people see, around bright lights against dark particles of much smaller size.12 For backward directions,
backgrounds, a radiating pattern of numerous fine, slightly irregular reflections by the “zones of discontinuity” in the lens,
colored needles of light—the so called ciliary corona. The also play a role.12 Bettelheim7 has noted that the density of
purpose of this study was to try to explain this phenomenon. larger particles need not be high to explain the intensity of
METHODS. Recently, it has been shown that light-scattering in forward light-scattering. In fact, the experimentally determined
the eye, measured psychophysically and on human donor levels of light-scattering and stray light are consistent with
lenses, can be explained assuming the presence of specific particles that occupy only 0.000006 of the volume of the
distributions of small particles in the eye. Light entering the eye human eye lens.12 A morphologic search by Gilliland et al.13,14
is diffracted by these particles. Each such particle causes a led to the identification of candidate particles for forward
circular diffraction pattern on the retina of tens of degrees, light-scattering in human lenses.
much like the well-known Airy pattern. The optics of combin- However, with these positive results, a new question
ing many such diffraction patterns was modeled and the result- emerged. The scattering pattern of such particles consists of a
ant pattern simulated graphically. The simulations were com- dominating central disc of more or less uniform light, extend-
pared with observations on the ciliary corona, as seen by the ing over more than 10°. It resembles the well known Airy
natural eye. pattern that describes diffraction of light around a circular
object. However, what we subjectively see around a point
RESULTS. The diffraction discs originating from all the particles source of light does not resemble a smooth disc at all. Instead,
coherently superimposed on the retina. Because of phase dif- we perceive a very fine pattern of innumerable needles of light,
ferences this resulted in breaking the Airy-like discs into a fine fanning out from the point object over several degrees. It must
spotted pattern when monochromatic light was used. For be noted that the pattern at some angular distance from the
white (polychromatic) light, the spots line up to form the very source is considered here. At the site of the source itself, a
fine-line pattern seen in the ciliary corona. Details such as the white stellar shape with a small number of spokes is often
width and color of the needles follow from the theoretical observed, but this depends on the subject. With squeezing or
treatment and were demonstrated by simulations. cataract, the spokes can extend considerably. These effects
CONCLUSIONS. The details of the ciliary corona can be under- were not considered in this study. The pattern of innumerable
stood on the basis of polychromatic light-scattering by the needles is familiar to most people. Typically, it is seen around
particles predicted to be present in human eye lenses on the a (distant) white lamp against a dark sky at night or a halogen
basis of light-scattering studies of donor lenses. (Invest Oph- spot at home. The phenomenon is called ciliary corona,15
thalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46:2627–2632) DOI:10.1167/iovs.04- “cilia” being Latin for eyelashes (but also for eyelids). It was the
0935 purpose of the present study to try to understand this seeming
discrepancy and to explain the origin of the ciliary corona.

L ight scattering in the eye’s optical media causes a veil of


stray light over the retina.1–3 This stray light leads to dele-
terious visual effects, such as glare while driving at night or METHODS
haziness of vision, which increases with age.1,3,4 The proteins
Theory
in the eye lens have long been considered an important source
of light-scattering, especially when aggregates form.5– 8 Exam- The basis for the explanation was sought in the fact that retinal stray
ination of optical scattering from donor lenses has been used to light is the summation of scattered light from many particles. Figure 1
estimate the proportion of light-scattering sources within the gives a schematic representation of light-scattering by three particles in
lens (0.000006) and the size of these sources (⬃0.7-␮m ra- the lens, illuminated by an infinite point source of monochromatic
dius).9 –12 The forward scatter seen from donor lenses was also light. Simpson15 has pointed out that the phenomenon of the ciliary
consistent with the perceived scatter in vivo (i.e., retinal stray corona bears subjective resemblance to what is perceived when look-
light).10,12 Backward directions of scattering are dominated by ing through a glass powdered with very fine lycopodium powder. The
data on human donor lenses predict a median value for the particle size
of 0.724 ␮m radius and a median volume fraction of 0.000006 for these
particles.12 If the particles are assumed to be spherical, each would
From The Netherlands Ophthalmic Research Institute, The Neth-
erlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. occupy a volume of 4/3␲r3 ⫽ 1.6 ␮m3, and many thousands of
Supported by the Dutch Foundation for Technical Sciences particles would occupy a 4-mm diameter pupil. Each of these particles
(STW). diffracts light so as to project an Airy-like disc on the retina when the
Submitted for publication August 3, 2004; revised December 22, eye looks at a distant point source (Fig. 2A). Please refer to textbooks,
2004, and March 24, 2005; accepted April 2, 2005. such as that by van de Hulst,11 for background about light-scattering by
Disclosure: T.J.T.P. van den Berg, None; M.P.J. Hagenouw, small particles. The diffraction disc is centered around the direct
None; J.E. Coppens, None retinal projection of the point source. Because this is true for all the
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page particles, many diffraction discs overlap. Particles with larger sizes
charge payment. This article must therefore be marked “advertise-
project smaller discs compared with particles with smaller sizes. The
ment” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Corresponding author: Thomas J. T. P. van den Berg, The Neth- radius (the first 0) of an Airy disc is 1.22 ␭/2r radian, with ␭ the
erlands Ophthalmic Research Institute, The Netherlands Academy of wavelength of the incident light, and r the radius of the particle. One
Arts and Sciences, Meibergdreef 47, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Nether- radian corresponds to 180/␲°. The Airy pattern describes diffraction
lands; t.j.vandenberg@ioi.knaw.nl. around a more or less opaque circular disc. The particles in the human

Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7
Copyright © Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2627
2628 van den Berg et al. IOVS, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7

FIGURE 1. Representation of light-


scattering by three particles in the
lens, illuminated by an infinite point
source of monochromatic light. Each
of the three scattered waves is cen-
tered around the primary image on
the retina. Because the waves arrive
on the retina from different direc-
tions, phase differences as a function
of retina location occur, giving rise to
interference.

eye lens can be assumed to be more or less transparent. Only the index corona, because the difference between the two approximations af-
of refraction is assumed to be different from its surroundings. The true fects only the intensity distribution, not the phase differences between
scattering characteristic of such particles (the Rayleigh-Gans-Debeye the scattered waves.
approximation11) is somewhat different from the Airy type, with a When the eye is looking at an ideally small point of light, different
radius of 1.43 ␭/2r. For a wavelength of 0.56 ␮m in air, ␭ ⫽ 0.56 positions in the pupil of the eye are illuminated coherently (complete
␮m/1.336 (1.336 is the index of refraction of the vitreous), and the spatial coherence). True sources, of course, are never ideally small, but
radius of the diffraction disc on the retina using this formula would be if the size is limited to the visual resolution limit of the eye, a high
24° for r ⫽ 0.724 ␮m. The Rayleigh-Gans-Debeye approximation was degree of coherence is obtained all over the pupil plane. In this way,
actually used in the present and previous studies. This bears virtually all particles in the eye lens are illuminated with coherent light, and, as
no connection, however, to the present discussion on the ciliary a consequence, all their retinal diffraction discs are coherent as well.

FIGURE 2. Illustration of coherent


combination of light-scattering by 1
(A), 2 (B), 3 (C), and 30 (D) particles.
For clarity, the illustration shows un-
realistically small particle distances
compared with particle sizes. True
particle distances/particle sizes are
larger by a factor of 300, meaning
that the fringes and grain in (B), (C),
and (D) should be smaller by a factor
of 300.
IOVS, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7 The Ciliary Corona 2629

Differences in depth of the particles are of little consequence with this method involves presenting a flickering ring to the subject. Be-
respect to coherence, because it is the total optical distance to the cause of light-scattering in the eye, part of the flickering light from this
retina that counts. However, because all particles are at different ring also reaches the center of the retinal projection of this ring.
locations, phase differences between the diffraction discs occur. This Because of that, the subject perceives a (faint) flicker in the center of
difference is illustrated in Figure 1 by the scattered wavefronts emitted the ring. With counterphase modulating light added to the center, this
from the particles. Only at the center of the retinal projection do the stray-light flicker can be silenced. The amount of counterphase-modu-
wavefronts coincide. Away from the center, they do not coincide, and lating light needed for silencing directly corresponds to the strength of
phase differences occur. retinal stray light in the respective individual. This approach was
Let us consider the case of two particles. The situation is then more implemented in a noncommercial instrument,16 as used in the present
or less comparable to the situation when using a retinometer. Two study, but recently a market instrument was manufactured by Oculus
points in the pupil plane project a coherent light disc on the retina. GmbH (Wetzlar, Germany). Main subjects were the three authors and
Because of the phase differences on the retinal plane, interference one other member of the group. The stray-light values are given as the
fringes arise (Fig. 2B). For a larger distance between the points in the log stray-light parameter (log(s)) for 10° of visual angle4: subject MH,
pupillary plane, the fringes are narrower and vice versa. Two particles age 25, glasses OD sphere (S): ⫺7.0, cylinder (C): ⫺0.75, OS S: ⫺6.75,
at distance (D) project two overlapping discs with a phase difference C: ⫺2.25, log(s) ⫽ 0.8; subject AR, age 31, glasses OD S: ⫺1.75, OS S:
equal to 2␲␪ D/␭, with ␪ the visual angle in radians and ␭ the wave- ⫺1.5, log(s) ⫽ 0.9; subject JC, age 31, glasses OD S: ⫺4.25, OS S:
length of the incident light. For two particles at a distance of 2 mm in ⫺4.25, log(s) ⫽1.0; subject TB, age 54, no correction (OD S: ⫺0.75, OS
the pupillary plane and a wavelength of 0.56 ␮m in air, the fringes of S: ⫺0.37), log(s) ⫽ 1.1.
the resultant interference pattern would have a period of approxi-
mately 0.0002 radians (0.7 minutes of arc). This compares with the
finest detail the human eye can resolve. In Figure 2B, the situation is RESULTS
exemplified for two particles with a distance (D) of 3.5 times their
radius (r). When three particles combine, interference would result in Figure 2 shows the patterns simulated for 1, 2, 3, and 30
three overlapping sets of such fringes (Fig. 2C), corresponding to the particles. The 30-particle simulation is exemplified in Figure 2D
three possible pairs of particles, and so forth. With many particles for the (unrealistic) situation that the 30 particles are homoge-
distributed over a 4-mm diameter pupil, the result would be a more or neously distributed over an area with a diameter of 17 times
less random pattern of very fine dark and light spots, with spot sizes at their radius. This is unrealistic because, in fact, the particles are
the limit of visual resolution (Fig. 2D). Thus, because of interference, very tiny compared with the pupillary diameter, and, as a
the smooth Airy-like disc breaks up in spots. This is the case with consequence, their diffraction discs are very large (24° for a
monochromatic light. It can be argued that at a different wavelength, particle of 0.724 ␮m) compared with the fine grain caused by
the whole spot pattern would be identical, apart from a scale factor interference. The size of the grain is as fine as the central part
corresponding to the ratio between the two wavelengths. The details of the retinal point-spread function (for the pupil size used).
of the spot pattern are dictated by the precise distribution of all Recall that in the Methods section the interference fringes for
scattering centers. To go further, with white light, the spots for the two particles at a 2-mm distance were shown to be 0.7 minutes
different colors that constitute white light would line up and form line apart. This grain size does not depend on the number or size of
segments. Many line segments would overlap, and it is no longer easy the particles. The grain in Figure 2D would have been virtually
to argue what the resultant pattern would look like. the same for different particle sizes or numbers, as long as the
sizes were small compared with the pupillary diameter, and the
Study Protocol particles were distributed more or less homogeneously over
In the present study this summation of coherent Airy-like discs on the the pupil. We must note that, in fact, diffraction discs of very
retina was studied by computer simulation MatLab software (The different sizes are combined because the particles in the eye
MathWorks, Natick, MA). The study adhered to the guidelines of the vary considerably in size.12 Because of this, the typical Airy-like
Declaration of Helsinki for research in human subjects. The outcome structure consisting of a central disc surrounded by rings gets
was compared to the subjective appearance of actual ciliary coronas. lost. The combined effect is a monotonic decrease of the
Subjects were asked to tell us about the details that were observed in scattered light intensity with angular distance. The same de-
the ciliary corona of their own eyes. For this, a bare 100-W halogen crease with angular distance was found with psychophysical
lamp was used, viewed on its smallest side (1.8 ⫻ 1.0-mm filament stray-light measurement.9 Because of the low intensities in-
size), located 4 meters from the subject against a black background. volved, subjectively we perceive the ciliary corona only over a
The viewing condition was otherwise unrestricted. The intensity of few degrees, depending on the strength of the light source.
this light was relatively easily to bear, with no adverse effects such as Simpson15 concluded from the limited size of the ciliary corona
frequent blinking or excessive tearing. Approximately 20 individuals that the (in his time, hypothetical) particles should be smaller
were asked for more global descriptions (appearance of “very fine than 10 ␮m in diameter. Subjectively, the maximum extent of
needles,” presence of “weak colorations,” global extent) while wearing the ciliary corona in our laboratory was approximately 8° for
their habitual correction. Four ophthalmically normal subjects partic- the four subjects (MH 6.4, AR 8.6, JC 9.0, TB 10), whereas with
ipated in a more elaborate process and systematic comparison. In these psychophysical measurement techniques stray light can be
four subjects habitual correction was used, and also best correction assessed far beyond that.1,2,16 Note that these values for the
with trial lenses, but this made no difference. Natural as well as dilated subjective extent of the ciliary corona increase with increasing
pupils were used. We tried to make the observations on the subjective stray light of the subjects (and age), as might be expected.
appearance as quantitative as possible. This was possible with respect Figure 3 gives the simulation for an area of 4.6° ⫻ 4.6°, a
to the estimation of maximum extent of the corona (⬃8°, see the pupil size of 0.2-mm diameter and particles with a wide size
Results section) and the ratio between inner and outer end of the line distribution around 0.724 ␮m. In fact, the precise size distri-
segments seen in the corona (⬃0.7, see the Results section). The other bution is of little consequence for the characteristics shown in
observations remained qualitative (e.g., “bluish” end “reddish” end this figure. Figure 3A gives the simulation for 450-nm wave-
regions, needles being “very fine,” but “coarser” for smaller pupils. See length light, and Figure 3B gives the same for 650 nm. As can
the Results section). Their retinal stray-light values were measured be seen, wavelength has only a scaling effect. Precisely the
quantitatively using a psychophysical approach, “the direct compen- same pattern is observed in Figures 3A and 3B, only sized
sation method” described in the literature.2,16 Their measured stray- differently. The scaling is in exact proportion to the wave-
light values are in the normal range for the respective ages.4 In short, length size—in this case, in a ratio of 450:650. This scaling
2630 van den Berg et al. IOVS, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7

FIGURE 3. Simulations for a 0.2-mm


diameter pupil, realistic particle dis-
tribution, and monochromatic light.
Simulation parameters: coherent
combination of light-scattering by
1000 particles uniformly distributed
over a 0.2-mm diameter area in the
pupillary plane for monochromatic
light of 450-nm wavelength (A) and
650 nm wavelength (B). Figure sizes
are 4.6° ⫻ 4.6° of visual angle.

effect forms the basis for the line pattern perceived in the the overlap does not disturb the expected fine line appearance.
ciliary corona. Realize that the ciliary corona is observed when It is expected to changes the coloration, since differently
using white (broad-band) light. Each wavelength contained in colored portions are summed together. In fact it is expected
that light projects a pattern like that in Figures 3A or 3B on the that the coloration is very much desaturated compared with a
retina. All the spots in this pattern line up for the different true wavelength spectrum. To view the exact nature of these
wavelengths, and an abundance of radiating lines can be ex- lines, and of the whole pattern of lines, computer simulation is
pected. Each line would be colored according to the wave- helpful. The simulation is shown in Figure 4 for equal-energy
length spectrum, blue on the inside to red on the outside. white light. The similarity to the subjectively perceived ciliary
However, many lines would overlap, since the spots from corona is striking. The simulation presented in the figure may
which they originate are very close together. Because overlap- differ from the actual corona, in saturation and contrast, be-
ping lines all point in precisely the same direction (the center), cause of printing and observation differences.

FIGURE 4. Simulated ciliary corona


for a distant point source emitting
equal-energy white light. Simulation
parameters: uniform random distri-
bution over a 4-mm diameter pupil of
1000 light-scattering particles of
⬃0.724 ␮m radius. Figure size is 4.6°
⫻ 4.6° of visual angle.
IOVS, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7 The Ciliary Corona 2631

The details in the simulated patterns were visually com- small particles. Another entoptic phenomenon, of direct rele-
pared by many observers to what they observed around an vance to the present study, is that of the “lenticular halo.”15
actual point source of light (a halogen lamp at 4 m) on the basis The lenticular halo is a colored band, much like the rainbow,
of several criteria. All observers agreed that the simulation was, perceived surrounding a bright spot of light at a mean distance
in a general manner, similar to the actual ciliary corona. Such of 3° radius. The inference in this case is that it originates from
aspects include the fineness and abundance of the needles, the the fibrous structure of the eye lens.15 The model is that the
fact that they are made up of line segments, the weak colora- lens fibers form a diffraction grating, arranged in a circular
tion, and the extent as mentioned earlier. Differences could fashion, much like the spokes of a wagon wheel.15 The grating
include the level of saturation and contrast and the fact that constant derived from the angular distance of the lenticular
true cilia seem to move all the time. It must be noted here that halo corresponds neatly to the width (periodicity) of the lens
a comparison between entoptic phenomena and simulation fibers— hence, the inference.15 In our study, the lenticular
can never really succeed. An important problem is that the halo was a bit of a nuisance, because it can be quite bright
dynamic span of our visual system (i.e., the differences in compared with the ciliary corona. However the lenticular halo
luminance that the visual system can cope with more or less appears only with larger pupil sizes, depending on the subject.
reliably) is much greater than that of simulation devices From the model simulations presented in this article, it
(CRTs). In case the effect of some form of stimulation is to be seems clear that the detailed structure of the ciliary corona can
simulated, the stimulus itself must be left out of the simulation. be understood on the basis of coherent light-scattering by
In our case, the actual light source could not be present in the particles in the eye. The particle distributions may differ con-
simulation. Also, as explained earlier, the details of the corona siderably between individuals. In particular, age changes in the
depend on the precise particle distribution (their location), eye lens may have a significant effect on particle distribution.
which is different for each individual. Moreover, with the pupil One might have expected the appearance of the ciliary corona
being in (slight) motion all the time for each individual, chang- to be different between individuals and to change with age.
ing populations of particles contribute to the corona. This is Also, within one individual, differences can be expected with
the presumed explanation for the continuous movements ob- pupil size. Indeed, for any one individual the ciliary corona is
served by many subjects in the actual corona. At most, our not a very constant pattern. Its details seem to vary continually.
model for the particle distribution is a statistical one, not one In overall appearance though, it is constant according to the
that describes actual location details. Moreover, the (statistical) six characteristics listed earlier. Between individuals, there may
particle size distribution varies somewhat between individuals be differences in precise detail, but not in overall appearance.
as well. To gain more certainty about the true nature of the Light-scattering and retinal stray light intensifies with age, due
corona, the four main observers aged between 25 and 54 years to increases in light-scattering particles, but the coherent sum-
had to make some observations of a more physical nature on mation on the retina is such that the line structure of the ciliary
sizes and coloration. But the results were remarkably similar corona remains essentially the same.
between the different observers as well as between the observ- What do we know specifically about these particles and
ers on the one hand and the simulation (Fig. 4). their change with age? The forward light-scattering data on
Each of the four observers had to judge explicitly the fol- donor lenses12 do not allow true identification. These data are
lowing (in addition to the extent mentioned earlier), also using consistent with some particle distributions (size and number),
artificial pupils and dilation: coloration, width, and length of assumed to be protein particles. They already occur in young
the line segments (cilia). They all agreed on the following lenses, consistent with young individuals also experiencing the
points: (1) The cilia seem to consist of separate line segments ciliary corona.12 The modeling suggested that the number of
with weakly colored end regions. (2) The end regions closer to particles increases with age and the scattering increases per
the center are bluish; the outer end regions are reddish. (3) particle, because they grow larger (or maybe more dense), on
The bluish end regions are closer to the center by about a average.12 From the light-scattering data we are not in a posi-
factor of 0.7, compared with the reddish end regions. This is tion to speculate on other potential changes such as, changes
more or less proportional to the corresponding wavelengths in the protein packing or in the cell surface interdigitations. Of
(i.e., 450:650 nm). (4) Correspondingly, the line segments are particular interest in this respect is the morphologic search by
proportionally longer if they are farther away from the center. Gilliland et al.13,14 that led to the identification of candidate
(5) The cilia are very thin, close to the finest detail the eye can particles. They found these particles to be consistent with our
resolve. (6) Pupil size had the effect of the cilia being coarser theoretical analysis, since the transparent lenses were esti-
for smaller pupil sizes (not shown in a figure). All this can be mated to have approximately 500 particles per cubic millime-
recognized by most normal observers in the subjectively expe- ter in the nuclear core, a number that agrees with the analysis
rienced ciliary corona. Each of these six points was seen also in and sufficient to produce the phenomenon.
the simulations, suggesting correspondence to exist between One might expect that other small-sized disturbances in the
subjective observation and the proposed explanation of the eye also would add to the intensity of the corona, without
phenomenon. The readers are invited to judge for themselves changing its appearance. These disturbances could take the
points 1 to 5 in Figure 4. form of cells in the anterior chamber, structures in the anterior
One more observation was added. With monochromatic vitreous, deposits on the posterior cornea, and deposits on the
light, a spot pattern, instead of a line pattern, is to be expected. anterior lens, among others. To take this argument further, one
Looking through interference filters at the halogen lamp, in- might also expect that these or other small disturbances, on
deed a spot pattern is seen, more coarse for long-wave light, in their own, could give the appearance of a ciliary corona.
correspondence with Figure 3. This observation is easily con- Determining which small irregularities in the eye would be
firmed, by looking at a sodium lamppost at night. true candidates to add to the ciliary corona is a subject for
further study.
In conclusion, the simulated corona compared very well
DISCUSSION with what is actually perceived. The predicted patterns show
details that are recognized by most normal subjects observing
Forward light-scattering is important in the human eye and has a point source. The ciliary corona can be modeled on the basis
several sources. This study addressed the entoptic phenome- of coherent light-scattering by small particles in the eye (lens).
non of the ciliary corona as originating from light scattering by This finding strengthens the earlier conclusion10 that forward
2632 van den Berg et al. IOVS, July 2005, Vol. 46, No. 7

light-scattering by small particles in the eye lens is the domi- human cataractous lenses: relationships between different light
nant source of retinal stray light, especially at older age. scattering parameters. Exp Eye Res. 1985;41:19 –30.
9. van den Berg TJTP. Depth-dependent forward light scattering by
References donor lenses. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1996;37:1157–1166.
10. van den Berg TJTP. Light scattering by donor lenses as a function
1. Vos JJ. Disability glare: a state of the art report. Commission of depth and wavelength. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:
International de l’Eclairage Journal. 1984;3/2:39 –53. 1321–1332.
2. van den Berg TJTP. Importance of pathological intraocular light 11. van de Hulst HC. Light Scattering by Small Particles. New York:
scatter for visual disability. Doc Ophthalmol. 1986;61:327–333. Dover Publications Inc., 1981.
3. Elliott DB, Bullimore MA. Assessing the reliability, discriminative 12. van den Berg TJTP, Spekreijse H. Light scattering model for donor
ability, and validity of disability glare tests. Invest Ophthalmol Vis lenses as a function of depth. Vision Res. 1999;39:1437–1445.
Sci. 1993;34:108 –119. 13. Gilliland KO, Freel CD, Lane CW, Fowler WC, Costello MJ. Multi-
4. van den Berg TJTP. Analysis of intraocular straylight, especially in lamellar bodies as potential scattering particles in human age-
relation to age. Optom Vis Sci. 1995;72:52–59. related nuclear cataracts. Mol Vis. 2001;7:120 –130.
5. Benedek GB. Theory of the transparency of the eye. Appl Opt. 14. Gilliland KO, Freel CD, Johnsen S, Craig FW, Costello MJ. Distri-
1971;10:459 – 473. bution, spherical structure and predicted Mie scattering of multi-
6. Delaye M, Tardieu A. Short-range order of crystallin proteins ac- lamellar bodies in human age-related nuclear cataracts. Exp Eye
counts for eye lens transparency. Nature. 1983;302:415– 417. Res. 2004;79:563–576.
7. Bettelheim FA. Physical basis of lens transparency. In: Maisel E, ed. 15. Simpson GC. Ocular haloes and coronas. Br J Ophthalmol. 1953;
The Ocular Lens: Structure Function and Pathology. 1985. New 37:449 – 486.
York: Marcel Dekker Inc., 1985. 16. van den Berg TJTP, Ijspeert JK. Clinical assessment of intraocular
8. Bettelheim FA, Chylack LT Jr. Light scattering of whole excised stray light. Appl Opt. 1992;31:3694 –3696.

S-ar putea să vă placă și