Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/254377677

Cruise Passengers' Decision-Making Processes

Article  in  Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing · October 2007


DOI: 10.1300/J073v23n01_01

CITATIONS READS
61 1,747

3 authors, including:

James Petrick Xiang (Robert) Li


Texas A&M University Temple University
113 PUBLICATIONS   5,738 CITATIONS    107 PUBLICATIONS   2,084 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Resident Sentiment of Tourism: Construct and Model Development View project

Chinese perception and attitudes towards cruising as a new travel mode View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Xiang (Robert) Li on 16 October 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Cruise Passengers’ Decision-Making Processes
James F. Petrick
Xiang (Robert) Li
Sun-Young Park

ABSTRACT. While various models for understanding tourists’ decision-making processes have
been conceptualized (e.g., Crompton, 1992; Gursoy & McCleary, 2004; Huan & Beaman, 2003),
few studies have attempted to examine them. The current study, guided by Crompton’s choice set
conceptualization, utilized qualitative methods (focus groups) to investigate the role of choice sets
while examining the underlying reasons and social influences for passengers’ cruise vacation
choice. Findings of the study suggest the existence of two groups of cruisers: those who go through
complex decision making and those who are brand loyal. Further theoretical and managerial impli-
cations were discussed. doi:10.1300/J073v23n01_01 [Article copies available for a fee from The Haworth
Document Delivery Service: 1-800-HAWORTH. E-mail address: <docdelivery@haworthpress.com>
Website: <http://www.HaworthPress.com> © 2007 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved.]

KEYWORDS. Decision-making, choice sets, cruise, marketing

INTRODUCTION until one decides which goods/services to


purchase. Past research has revealed that this
The process that individuals go through process may be moderated by: a tourist’s famil-
when deciding on where to travel has received iarity with destinations (Gursoy & McCleary,
much attention within the tourism literature 2004; Sirakaya, Sonmez, & Choi, 2001), mari-
(Crompton, 1992; Crompton & Ankomah, tal roles (Ford, LaTour, & Henthorne, 1995;
1993; Dellaert, Borgers, & Timmermans, Mottiar & Quinn, 2004), gender (Vogt &
1997; Fodness & Murray, 1999; Vogt & Fesen- Fesenmaier), travel frequency (Morgan, 1991),
maier, 1998). The underlying purpose of the children (Thornton, Shaw, & Williams, 1997;
majority of this research is that by understand- Wang, Hsieh, Yeh, & Tsai, 2004), spouse
ing how tourists make decisions, service pro- (Madrigal, 1993; Zalatan, 1998), friends and
viders can more effectively market to, and sat- relatives (Gitelson & Kerstetter, 1994), lifecycle
isfy visitors. It has been argued that the (Decrop, 1999), culture (Caneen, 2003), cogni-
decision-making process involves the narrow- tive distance (Ankomah, Crompton, & Baker,
ing down of initial alternatives (Nicosia, 1966) 1996; Crompton & Kim, 2001), group pro-

James F. Petrick is Associate Professor, Department of Recreation, Park and Tourism Sciences, Texas A&M
University, 2261 TAMU, College Station, TX 77843-2261 USA (E-mail: jpetrick@tamu.edu). Xiang (Robert) Li is
Assistant Professor, School of Hotel, Restaurant, and Tourism Management, College of Hospitality, Retail, and
Sport Management, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208 USA (E-mail: robertli@sc.edu). Sun-
Young Park is Assistant Professor, School of Travel Industry Management, The University of Hawaii at Manoa,
2560 Campus Road, George Hall, Room 219, Honolulu, HI 96822 USA (E-mail: parksy@hawaii.edu).
Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, Vol. 23(1) 2007
Available online at http://jttm.haworthpress.com
Ó 2007 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1300/J073v23n01_01 1
2 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

cesses (Decrop, 2005), local “experts” (Rompf, Combined, these changes in the market have
DiPietro, & Ricci, 2005), and advertising made it integral for the cruise industry (in par-
(Johnson & Messmer, 1991). ticular the “high-end” markets) to understand
While various models for understanding the decision-making processes of their current
tourists’ decision-making processes have been passengers. Moreover, the lack of empirical
conceptualized (e.g., Crompton, 1992; Gursoy evidence related to tourism decision-making
& McCleary, 2004; Huan & Beaman, 2003; models, suggests that inductive (qualitative)
Sirakaya, McLellan, & Uysal, 1996), few stud- methods might be more appropriate for exam-
ies have attempted to empirically examine ining these processes than deductive (quantita-
them. By examining the processes that tourists tive). Thus, the current study will utilize quali-
utilize to make purchasing decisions, tourism tative methods (focus groups), guided by the
practitioners should be better equipped to mar- choice sets model, as conceptualized by
ket their offerings to both current and future Crompton (1992) to better understand the deci-
visitors. sion-making processes of passengers on a
One sector in need of a better understanding “high-end” cruise line.
of their markets is the cruise industry. Since
1980, the North American cruise market has en-
joyed an annual growth rate of 8.2% (CLIA, LITERATURE REVIEW
2005). This surge in revenues was rejuvenated
back into the industry by increasing fleet sizes Decision Making
and increasing cruise capacity by building new,
larger ships (Lois, Wang, Wall, & Ruxton, The concept of “choice sets” was first pro-
2004). CLIA (2006) reports that during the posed and elaborated in the consumer behav-
1980s 40 new ships were built, while in the ior literature in examining consumers’ pur-
1990s approximately 80 ships debuted, and at chase decisions by Howard (1963). The
least 69 new ships have debuted since 2000. concept purports that consumers make a final
They argue that the industry’s commitment to choice from gradually reduced groups of alter-
expanding guest capacity is because the indus- natives among finite numbers of potential op-
try has tremendous potential for growth, as
tions. The concept has since been adopted and
only 16% of adults in the United States have
tested across several disciplines (Thill &
ever taken a cruise. This growth in berths has
made it imperative for the industry to not only Wheeler, 2000). Particularly, the concept has
attract a larger percentage of potential cruisers, been applied to the tourists’ destination choice
but to also retain its current clientele in order to context (e.g., Um & Crompton, 1990; Wood-
maintain current occupancy rates. side & Lysonski, 1989). The literature suggests
The cruise industry has also seen a change in that decisions which are perceived to have
the demographics of their cruise passengers higher levels of risk are more likely to include
over the past few decades. Data published by higher levels of information search and evalu-
CLIA (2005) reveals that passengers are taking ation of alternatives (Crompton & Ankomah,
shorter cruises, as the overall percentage of 1993). To date, the line of research on choice
growth since 1980 for short (2-5 day) cruises sets has grown to be an important branch of
(724.5%) is much higher than that of 6-8 day travel decision-making studies (Jeng & Fesen-
(497.4%), 9-17 day (425.3%) and 18 or more maier, 2002). Further, Sirakaya and Woodside
day (82.4%) cruises. Additionally, cruise pas- (2005, p. 828), in their review of tourism deci-
sengers are younger (average age is 50 years sion-making theories, purport that “the choice
old) than they have been since they first started sets approach offers a rather simple and practical
collecting data in 1975 (including corrections perspective to understanding the travelers’ deci-
for inflation). This change in demographics sion-making process” and that the Crompton
suggests that “high-end” cruise lines may be (1992) “choice sets approach provides practi-
losing a share of the market, and value oriented cal advantages” to other types of models. Thus,
(i.e., shorter cruises for younger persons) cruise the Crompton will be the model utilized to
lines may be gaining a competitive edge. guide the present study.
Petrick, Li, and Park 3

The Model of Destination Choice Sets When building the destination choice sets
model, Crompton (1992) and Crompton and
Crompton (1992) integrated relevant re- Ankomah (1993) cautioned that the notion of
search findings by consumer behavior scholars choice sets is applicable only when the task of
and extant yet sparse findings from tourism purchasing requires non-routinized decision-
studies, and proposed a model (Figure 1) on the making and high level of involvement. In
structure of tourists’ choice sets. According to other words, three main types of deci-
this model, tourists’ decision-making process sion-making behaviors suggested by con-
goes through three stages, whereby all destina- sumer behavior literature (i.e., brand loyalty:
tions that they are aware of are funneled down high involvement, routinized; limited deci-
to reach a final choice. The three stages are: ini- sion-making: low involvement, non-routin-
tial consideration set, containing all destina- ized; and inertia: low involvement, routinized)
tions considered by tourists as “possible to visit (Assael, 2004) may not fit in this model. Yet, it
within a period of time” (Crompton, p. 423); has been argued that since the tourism product
late consideration (evoked) set (termed by reflects the “unique characteristics of services”
Howard (1963)), containing destinations con- (i.e., intangible, inseparable, etc.) that deci-
sidered by tourists as “probable to visit within a sions related to tourism purchase are all rela-
period of time” (Crompton, p. 424); and final tively highly involved (Sirakaya & Woodside,
choice decision which is the final destination 2005). Moreover, it was explicitly pointed out
tourists choose to visit. Simply put, this model by Crompton (p. 432) that the taxonomy of
delineates the process used by potential tourists choice sets is “an analytical tool” rather than
to reduce the number of destinations through “an explanatory model,” “because it does not
three stages of alternative sets before reaching a explain the roles of internal and external forces
final selection. that shape the choices.”
In a follow-up study, Crompton and Ankomah More recently, a series of studies reported by
(1993) developed a total of nine propositions Crompton and his associates (Botha, Crompton,
(two for the first stage; three for the second & Kim, 1999; Crompton, Botha, & Kim, 1998;
stage; and four for the final stage) based on this Kim, Crompton, & Botha, 2000) have provided
model. For example, for the first stage, it was empirical support to the model as well as certain
proposed that the probability for a destination propositions. For instance, Crompton et al. ver-
to be chosen as a final choice depends on the ified the predicted results of the propositions
level of tourists’ awareness of the destination in for the late consideration set as stated above.
the early consideration set. Moreover, the num- Their findings supported the claim that the
ber of destinations in the early consideration set number of destinations in the decision maker’s
within a given geographical area was postu- late consideration set is typically under four,
lated to be related to the tourists’ prior visitation and this number is about 60% to 90% of its
to that area, and the distance of their residence counterpart in their early consideration set.
from that area. As for the late consideration set, However, the authors failed to identify a consis-
fewer than four destinations were estimated to tent relationship between perceived impor-
be included in this stage, and the ratio between tance and perceived risk with the late consider-
the first consideration set and this set was hy- ation set size. Nor did they confirm the
pothesized to range from .6 to .9. In the final proposed positive relationship between re-
stage, various decision strategies and criteria spondents’ preference rankings of destinations
(decision rules) such as the relative merits of and the order of the destinations being men-
destinations, perceived constraints, the extent tioned in unaided recall questions. Interest-
of reliance on information, and so forth were ingly, while their findings supported the propo-
proposed for a final choice. The authors ac- sition that destinations in which people invest
knowledged that most of these propositions more information-seeking effort are more
were “transplanted” from the consumer behav- likely to be included in the late consideration
ior field. Thus, it seems that the applicability set, it was also reported that for a familiar desti-
and relevance of these propositions in the tour- nation (such as Sun/Lost City to most respon-
ism context remained to be investigated. dents in their study), there could be a different
4 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

FIGURE 1. Structure of Vacation Destination Choice Sets (Crompton, 1992, p. 421)

All Potential Destinations

Awareness Set Unawareness Set

Initial Excluded
Stage 1 Consideration Set
Set
Unpleasant Negative
Foggy Hold
Personal External
Set Set
Experience Feedback
Set Set

Inert Set Inept or Reject Set

Stage 2 Late Consideration


(Evoked) Set

Action Set Inaction Set

Interaction Set Quiet Set

Stage 3 Final Selected


Destination

Used with Permission: Elsevier Limited.

situation. Thus, their results revealed that a fa- The first trend can be characterized as inte-
miliar destination could become the ultimate gration of the choice sets model. These studies
choice even though search effort on it is much integrated the model as part of their conceptual
less than on some alternatives. basis for explaining tourists’ decision-making
processes (e.g., Klensosky & Gitelson, 1998;
Ramifications of the Model Sonmez, 1998; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998;
Zalatan, 1996). For example, factors like safety
Since this “classic” model of tourist destina- (Sonmez & Graefe) or destination characteris-
tion choice was proposed, tourism studies on tics (Zalatan) were included as part of tourists’
decision-making process have extended to not decision-making processes.
only destination choice (e.g., Prentice & Ander- The second trend is an extension of the
sen, 2000; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998), but to the choice set conceptualization to different situa-
contexts of tourist behavior (e.g., Huang & tions from the high-involvement and non-rou-
Tsai, 2003; Kozak, 2001); tourist motivation tinized scenario specified by Crompton (1992).
(e.g., Heung, Qu, & Chu, 2001; Kozak, 2002); These studies examined the influence of previ-
and travel agents’ destination recommendation ous experiences (Huang & Tsai, 2003; Kozak,
(Klenosky & Gitelson, 1998). There seems to 2001; Oppermann, 1998) and familiarity (Pren-
be at least three trends in the application of the tice & Andersen, 2000) on destination choice,
choice sets conceptualization: integration, ex- which could make the decision-making situa-
tension, and amplification. tion more routinized and less involved. For ex-
Petrick, Li, and Park 5

ample, Oppermann contended that because of between cognitive distance and choice sets.
previous visits, some repeat visitors may ex- They found that cognitive distance estimates to
perience a unique pattern of choice sets: their destinations in the late consideration set were
early, late, and final consideration sets could significantly more accurate than estimates to
consistently contain only one option. Prentice destinations in the reject set. Further, they
and Andersen acknowledged that the choice found that respondents tended to underestimate
sets model is limited in understanding choices cognitive distance for destinations in the late
made without much problem-solving pro- consideration set, but the level of underestima-
cesses. They argued that in alternative situa- tion varies in different subsets of the late set,
tions, specifically, when “familiarity is a driver with the action subset higher than inaction set.
for imagined affective associations about desti- Kozak (2001) found that the determinant fac-
nations, evoked opportunities, and prefer- tors for intention to revisit were the level of
ences” (Prentice & Andersen, p. 493), familiar- overall satisfaction and the number of previous
ity could become the ultimate determinant of visits. In addition, tourist motivations have
visiting propensity. According to their concep- been found to be different based on two factors:
tualization, familiarity, as opposed to extensive nationality and destinations visited (Kozak,
information processing postulated in the 2002). By identifying the antecedents and con-
Crompton’s model, becomes the mechanism of sequences of choice set formation, this stream
choice selection. of research has attempted to amplify the model,
Research has also extended the classic by making it more explanatory by including
choice sets model by examining tourists’ de- various factors influencing choice-decision.
cision-making characteristics in multipur- In comparison to the numerous studies inte-
pose trips (versus one single pleasure-seeking grating, extending, or amplifying the choice
trip) (Oppermann, 2000), and by examining sets model (which is arguably beyond the
multi-facet travel decisions (choice of the com- model’s initial purpose), there has been little ef-
bination of several travel components versus fort made in testing the original model. Fur-
simply destination choice) (Dellaert et al., 1997; thermore, although Crompton and Ankomah
Dellaert, Ettema, & Lindh, 1998). Dellaert et al. (1993) estimated that the concept of choice sets
(1998) suggested that both the timing of travel- could be generalizable to many facets of tour-
ers’ choices on different components of one va- ism, no studies have examined this model in
cation and their constraints affect the overall other sectors of the tourism industry (outside of
process of travel decision-making. Further, the destinations). While this model has possibly
timing of those parts differed in varying de- been the most often cited and utilizedin the field
grees until the actual travel takes place, and of tourism, few studies have empirically exam-
constraints were found to be the determinant ined it. Recent research has generated a better
factor for travel decisions. Overall, this stream understanding of the phenomena from different
of research has attempted to extend the applica- perspectives.
bility of the concept of choice sets. Looking be-
yond the specific choice analysis context, these Recent Tourist Decision-Making Research
researchers associated the notion of choice sets
with a broader picture of travel decision-mak- Most of the recent literature related to tour-
ing situations from various angles. ists’ decision-making processes have exam-
A third trend is the amplification of the ined how tourists search for information when
choice sets model by adding additional factors making travel decisions (Gursoy & McCleary,
to the model. Additional factors which have 2004; Huan & Beaman, 2003; Maser &
been added to the model include: cognitive dis- Weiermair, 1998; Vogt & Fesenmaier, 1998).
tance (Ankomah et al., 1996), image (Heung et Vogt and Fesenmaier (p. 552) suggested that
al., 2001; Sonmez & Sirakaya, 2002), satisfac- the underlying premise of this research is “that
tion (Kozak, 2001), motivational differences individuals are goal-directed; that is, they are
(Kozak, 2002), and the tourist’s role (Jiang, attempting to answer a specific question as to
Havitz, & O’Brien, 2000). For instance, which product to buy or how to spend time.”
Ankomah et al. investigated the relationship They examined information requesters, to a
6 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

Midwest destination in the United States and friends (in comparison to couples/families) are
were able to develop a reliable and valid scale more likely to: involve all members, take lon-
for measuring multiple information needs of ger, and are more likely to be ineffective. Yet,
tourists. Results revealed that most information decisions made with friends are less likely to be
is collected for functional reasons, though in- frustrating as friends are more willing to com-
formation is also gathered to fulfill: innovative, promise (“sacrifice”), and group adhesion is a
hedonic (entertainment) and aesthetic/visual major goal.
needs. They thus suggest that when developing Similar to the Crompton (1992) model,
communication materials, destination managers Woodside and Dubelaar (2002) and Woodside
should incorporate these needs. and King (2001) postulated a purchase con-
Gursoy and McCleary (2004) conceptual- sumption system (PCS) which sequences the
ized a more holistic model of tourists’ informa- steps in which consumers utilize to buy and use
tion search behavior, though the model has yet products. Their sequential model helps to
to be empirically examined. Their model postu- explain how different variables (i.e., demo-
lated that familiarity and expertise are the key graphics, choices/alternatives, pre-planning is-
moderating variables between situational fac- sues, key selection drivers, etc.) in the deci-
tors (previous visits, involvement, intentional sion-making process affect each other. Their
learning and incidental learning) and both in- behavioral model (as opposed to choice set
ternal and external searching behavior. Their models; see Sirakaya and Woodside (2005) for
model further suggested that the relationships a full-review of both types of models) includes
between familiarity and expertise, and internal ten propositions related to the affects of one de-
and external search were mediated by the per- cision behavior on another (see Woodside,
ceived costs of both the internal and external in- MacDonald, & Burford, 2004, for a full review
formation search. These proposals suggested of the propositions).
that travel decision-making was typically a Also related to the current study is the work
highly involved and complex process. of Sirakaya, McLellan, and Uysal (1996) who
Another model of decision-making was pro- utilized a behavioral decision theory to model
posed by Huan and Beaman (2003). They sug- college students’ decision-making processes.
gested that not including tourist type or treating They revealed that for college students, deci-
multiple step decisions as one compensatory sions are individual specific and that destina-
choice invalidates decision-making research. tion attractiveness and the cost of the trip are
They further suggested that examining deci- the most important factors for predicting final
sion-making at the individual level is flawed, as choice. This finding suggests that decisions
the process of choosing a destination is also a related to choosing a cruise may be related to
social process (influenced by others). Thus, the attractiveness and perceived value of the
they argued that decision-making is a social- product.
psychological construct.
Similarly, Decrop and Snelders (2005) pro- Purpose of the Study
posed a decision-making typology based on so-
cial-psychological processes. They examined Since it has been suggested that understand-
the decision-making processes of 25 Belgian ing tourists’ decision-making is one of the keys
households choosing a vacation and identified to marketing success (Gursoy & McCleary,
six different types of vacationers: habitual (re- 2004; Johnson & Messmer, 1991) and the
peaters), rational (risk avoiders), hedonic (plea- cruise industry is becoming increasingly com-
sure seekers), opportunistic (non-planners), petitive (CLIA, 2005, 2006), it would seem im-
constrained (have contextual inhibitors) and portant for cruise management to better under-
adaptable (flexible). While these categories are stand their visitors’ decision-making processes
not mutually exclusive, they do offer insight in a practical manner. Since the Crompton
into the decision-making processes of vaca- (1992) model has been suggested to offer a
tioners from various social-psychological “rather simple and practical perspective to un-
backgrounds. From this same data set, Decrop derstanding the travelers’ decision process”
(2005) further found that decisions made with and has been argued to have “practical advan-
Petrick, Li, and Park 7

tages” over other models (Sirakaya & Wood- for a predominant cruise line (called ABC line
side, 2005), it was believed that the Crompton from here forward), utilizing focus groups. The
model would offer the best fit to the research ship utilized has been defined as a “premier
needed. ship” (Choosing Cruising, 2004) on a “premier
Yet, as argued by Crompton (1992), the line” (Cartwright & Baird, 1999). The ship has
choice sets model does not necessarily delin- 720 passenger staterooms which have been
eate the shaping forces of a choice. As stated by called “among the largest in the industry” with
Sirakaya and Woodside (2005, p. 829), choice 80% of all cabins having deluxe verandas
set models often “accept that other individuals (Cruise & Vacation Views, 2001). The ship’s
affect the decision-maker, but do not address itinerary included stops in St. Thomas, USVI;
active interaction with other individuals or Grand Cayman, Cayman Islands; Ocho Rios,
sources along the decision-making process. Jamaica and ABC’s private island.
Thus, it would further seem important to focus Potential participants were selected from
on the social influences on these decisions (as the cruise manifest (listing of all passengers,
per Huan & Beaman, 2003), and to examine the including relevant information) by the
underlying reasons why passengers chose the Ship’s Hotel Manager, and were stratified to
cruise they did, over all other vacations. ensure representation of different cruiser
Crompton’s (1992) choice set model was de- types (Alumni, suite, outside, and inside
veloped to examine the decision-making pro- cabin) were solicited. This was done by sorting
cesses related to choosing a vacation destina- passengers by cabin type and by number of
tion, and he argued that the process might be cruises, prior to systematically selecting them.
quite different for different types of vacations. Passengers selected (n = 152) were then ran-
It was thus postulated that the decision-making domly placed into one of 23 pre-planned focus
processes of persons purchasing a cruise vaca- groups. These passengers were then invited to
tion would be more complex than decisions re- participate by having a note explaining the
lated to choosing a destination, since it was be- study sent to their cabins, with an R.S.V.P. card
lieved the process would involve more steps. enclosed. The note informed the guests where
Similar to choosing a destination, cruise vaca- and when their focus group would take place.
tioners must decide: (a) whether or not to take a The note also asked them to indicate whether
vacation, and (b) where to go. Yet, cruise vaca- they would like to participate by returning their
tioners must also decide: (c) which cruise line to R.S.V.P. card to the Purser’s Desk.
travel on, and (d) which ship from that line to A total of 82 passengers agreed to participate
choose. via contacting the Purser’s Desk. Guests who
It has been suggested that for topics that have showed up for their focus groups (n = 72) were
a theoretical foundation which is not yet robust, interviewed in small groups (2 to 5 people) in
qualitative approaches are preferred (Dann & order to better hear individual opinions, com-
Phillips, 2001). Thus, since the Crompton ments and observances, while allowing for
(1992) choice sets model lacks empirical sup- group interaction. The interviews were all con-
port, it was determined that the most feasible ducted in the same conference room, and were
means for examining the purposes of the study recorded in order to better preserve the data.
was via focus groups of cruise passengers. The interviews were led by the primary investi-
Therefore, the current study utilized inductive gator of the current study, with one assistant in
reasoning (instead of hypotheses) as a guiding attendance to take additional notes to better as-
framework to investigate the role of choice sets sist with the reliability of responses. On aver-
while examining social influences and the age, the interviews lasted approximately 35
underlying reasons for final choice. minutes. As a gift for participating, guests were
given a small gift, compliments of the Hotel
Manager.
METHODS The questions asked were scripted in order to
be consistent, and multiple follow-up questions
Data for the current study was collected dur- were utilized as discussion warranted. The
ing a one week cruise on board the newest ship script was developed, following the Crompton
8 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

(1992) choice sets model, and included addi- suites and 14 guests who stayed in inside
tional inquiry into the role of others within the cabins.
decision-making process (as per Huan &
Beaman, 2003). Also, with the use of the inter- Initial Choice Set
viewing process, follow-up questions were in-
cluded to assist in understanding the underlying On average, guests started their decision-
causes of the decisions made. making process regarding which type of vaca-
Participants were initially informed that the tion to take 5.7 months prior to sailing. The vast
purpose of their focus group was: “to assist ‘the majority of initial decisions were made only by
cruise line’ in better serving them in the future, those who would be going on the vacation (i.e.,
and to determine why you chose this particular
couples discussed it together, or one member of
cruise and ship.” The initial set of questions
asked for feedback regarding participants’ a couple informed the other, or, if an individual,
cruising history (i.e., number of cruises they nobody influenced initial decisions). Of the 72
had taken, number with ABC, who else they participants, only four made initial decisions
had cruised). The second group of questions based on information received from ABC, and
asked respondents about their initial consider- only one was influenced by a travel agent. The
ation sets. Participants were asked: what types four persons who were influenced by ABC
of vacation they considered, who was involved were all Alumni, and contact made from their
in this process, when did this process start, were participation in the Alumni program is what
there multiple rounds of reducing choices (i.e., started their decision-making processes. The
was there a late consideration set), why/how did majority of responses mirrored the following:
they decide to take a cruise versus another type
of vacation. “Nobody, or no information influenced
The third group of questions asked partici- our decision to go on vacation. We go ev-
pants the main reasons why they chose ABC ery year, and as always, we just sat down
over other cruise lines and or vacations (late and and started to decide where we would go
final decision). Questions in this group in- this year. We make these decisions by
cluded: what other cruise lines/vacations were ourself.” (Male, Alumni visitor)
considered, who made the final decision, who/
what influenced the final decision, what were During this initial phase, only five inter-
the other possible choices, when was the final viewed guests considered a vacation other than
decision made, and why did they ultimately a cruise. These guests were either considering
choose the “ship.” Additional streams of ques-
flying to a single destination (n = 4) or driving to
tions (not included in this study) include: a
competitor analysis, the effect on the current see family (n = 1). While there were multiple
cruise of participating in a focus group, and reasons that participants gave for deciding to
future behavioral intentions. take a cruise instead of another type of vacation,
most were because cruising is perceived to be
more carefree than other vacations. Examples
RESULTS include:

Profile of Respondents “We think (cruising) is the best part of ev-


erything. You’ve got everything from food,
Respondents included 55 visitors who had live entertainment, and different ports
sailed ABC before and 17 first time ABC to go to. It’s a break away from doing
guests. Participants on average had taken a total nothing.”
of 13.7 cruises, with 8.3 (60.3%) of those
“You can unpack only once, and see dif-
cruises being taken on ABC (ranged from first
ferent places . . . it is convenient.”
time cruisers to 61 total cruises). Additionally,
the sample included 37 males, and 35 females, “We’ve done other vacations, been there,
and consisted of 18 guests who were staying in done that.”
Petrick, Li, and Park 9

“You don’t have to be anywhere, at any “We did research on all of the big cruise
specific time.” lines and narrowed it to Princess, Royal
(Caribbean) and ABC. Princess and
“You don’t have to deal with buses or ho- Royal (Caribbean) are less expensive,
tels. You have everything you need right but you don’t get as much as you do here.
here.” We decided that since it was our anniver-
While it was anticipated that there would be a sary that it was worth the extra money . . .
progressive funneling of multiple choices, Once we knew we were going on ABC,
down to a single choice, the vast majority of the ‘ship’ was our choice because we
participants (n = 56) knew that they were taking wanted to go to the Caribbean.”(First
an ABC cruise the second that they decided to Timer)
go on vacation. Of these, all but three were re-
peat ABC cruisers. Responses from the “Our last few cruises were all on Prin-
repeaters included: cess, but we wanted to sail somebody
new. We are able to cruise (Princess) for
“The only decision we make every year is a little less (money), but they aren’t
which ship we are going to go on.” worth it unless they are running a special
promotion. We checked out a lot of lines,
“We’ve tried other lines, but ABC is and even thought about Princess again . . .
where we belong . . . I don’t think we’ll We couldn’t find a super deal, so we
take a regular vacation again.” chose ABC because (the others) don’t of-
fer as high of quality overall . . . I really
“Why would we go through the hassle to wanted a newer ship (in response to why
try anything else? They (ABC) make us he chose the ‘ship’).”(First Timer)
happy and treat us like royalty . . . we “We knew it was going to be either Ce-
only sail ABC.” lebrity or ABC, and tried to see who
would offer us the best deal. Of all the
Responses from first time ABC (n = 3) cruis- lines these are our two favorites, and this
ers who knew they would be taking a cruise on time the decision seemed to be tougher
ABC immediately included: than normal. Both lines offered us won-
derful deals. We got a 45% discount for
“Our parents always swear by ABC and being Alumni members which made our
we just figured it was time for us to see decision much easier . . . we chose the
what it was all about . . . it was the only ‘ship’ because we wanted to be in a
vacation we considered.” warm itinerary.”(Repeater)
“The last cruise we went on, people told Late and Final Choice Set
us that ABC America was better . . . we
knew our next cruise would be with them, On average, respondents had decided to take
just didn’t know when.” their vacation on board the “ship” (final deci-
sion) 5.5 months prior to sailing. Thus, the time
The sixteen passengers who did not know from initially thinking about taking a vacation
that they would be sailing with ABC immedi- (5.7 months prior), to making a final decision
ately included two repeat ABC cruisers, and 14 took on average less than one week. For partici-
first timers. This group of respondents ap- pants who knew immediatelythat they were go-
peared to weigh the strengths of the different ing to take an ABC cruise (n = 56), they reduced
lines first, and then chose based on itinerary their choices down to the “ship” based on either
and/or ship. A single, consistent theme for why the destinations visited, or the ship itself. Ex-
they ended up cruising ABC seemed to be that it amples of quotations made by respondents who
offered better value. Quotes from this group based their final decision on destinations
include: visited include:
10 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

“Their itinerary and dates matched best DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS


with when and where we wanted to go.”
With the cruise industry becoming increas-
“This was the only ship ‘ABC’ has in the ingly competitive (CLIA, 2006), it is more im-
Caribbean which made our choice very portant than ever for cruise management to
easy.” understand their markets. Further, the under-
standing of tourists’ decision-making pro-
Responses from those who made their deci- cesses (Crompton, 1992), the various social in-
sion based solely on the ship include: fluences (Huan & Beaman, 2003) and the
underlying processes (Gursoy & McCleary,
“We have come to eliminate some of 2004; Johnson & Messmer, 1991) have been
(ABC’s) ships, the older ones, and wanted proposed as keys to marketing success. Thus,
it was the purpose of the current study to exam-
to cruise the newest one.”
ine cruise passengers’ choice sets (as per
Crompton), while focusing on the social influ-
“We heard that this ship had the best
ences on these decisions (as per Huan &
modern art.” Beaman), and examining the underlying rea-
sons why passengers chose the cruise they did.
As mentioned previously, the final decision for Results of the current study have both manage-
respondents who did not know that they would rial and theoretical implications. The Crompton
be going on ABC immediately was generally (1992) choice sets model suggested that deci-
made, based on value. sions related to choosing a vacation destination
During the final decision-making phase, re- go through three distinct stages, yet suggested
spondents were also more likely to seek infor- that this process could be very different for dif-
mation from others. Participants who knew im- ferent sectors of the tourism industry. It was
mediately that they were going to cruise with thus believed by the current researchers that the
ABC (n = 56), were most likely to seek informa- decision-makingprocesses of persons purchas-
tion from the Web (n = 33), though they also ing a cruise vacation would be more complex
contacted ABC directly for information (n = than decisions related to choosing a destination.
11) and also contacted travel agents for infor- Similar to choosing a destination, cruise vaca-
mation (n = 8). Participants (n = 16) who did not tioners must decide: (a) whether or not to take a
know that they were going to cruise ABC im- vacation, and (b) where to go. Yet, cruise vaca-
mediately were most likely to: go to the ABC tioners must also decide: (c) which cruise line to
Web site (n = 13), contact a travel agent (n = 9), travel on, and (d) which ship from that line to
ask friends/family for assistance (n = 6), or talk choose.
with a group leader for advice (n = 5). These re- Findings of the current study suggest the ex-
spondents as a whole were much more likely to istence of two groups of cruisers: those who go
use multiple sources of information to make a through complex decision-making (partici-
final decision, while the former group tended to pants who did not know that they were going to
cruise ABC immediately) and those who are
utilize only one source of information. brand loyal (participants who sailed ABC be-
Participants were also asked the main reason fore and knew immediately that they were go-
why they chose ABC over all other cruise lines. ing to cruise with ABC). The former went
These responses were systematically grouped through the funneling process before reaching
into themes by two researchers independently. their final decision, as suggested by Crompton
Once completed, the themes were compared and his associates (Crompton, 1992; Crompton
and negotiated. The resultant themes which in- & Ankomah, 1993; Crompton et al., 1998).
cluded three or more responses included: Supe- The latter went through a process that contra-
rior Product/Service (n = 19), Familiarity/Loy- dicted the choice sets model, by skipping earlier
alty (n = 11), Other Guests (n = 9), The Ship (n = stages. It was revealed that these passengers
8), Superior Crew (n = 6), Convenience (n = 6) seem to follow Langer’s (1978) notion of mind-
and Price (n = 4). lessness: they simplified their decision-making
Petrick, Li, and Park 11

with minimal processing of available informa- they retrieved this information internally, and
tion, and did not go through all three phases of their entire information search process stopped
the choice sets model. Crompton and Ankomah there (i.e., no more external information search
(1993) argued that the choice sets model would was necessary). Thus, internal information
only be effective for non-routinized and highly search preceded external information search in
involved decisions. While it can be argued that terms of both order and importance, which
the decisions of the current respondents were supports Bettman’s (1979) conceptualization.
highly involved (as per Assael, 2004), it ap- This finding seems to suggest that there is a
pears as if these decisions were routinized. This sequence of information search, such as in-
held especially true for repeat ABC travelers, ternal information based on one’s own past
which is similar to Oppermann’s (1998) postu- experienceÆinternal information based on
lation that for tourists who have previous expe-
rience, their entire set of choices may include previously collected, credible informationÆ
only one option. In this case, it was not that they internal information based on other memoriesÆ
had taken that specific vacation before (no par- external information search. As proposed by
ticipants had ever sailed on the “ship” before), Sirakaya and Woodside (2005, p. 827), “addi-
but they had purchased other products from the tional theoretical work and empirical reports
brand before (ABC). are needed to help understand heavy search be-
The above finding is in accordance with havior by visitors with extensive prior travel
Prentice and Anderson (2000) who suggested behavior experiences to the destination areas
that familiarity could become the ultimate de- that they are about to visit, as well as non-search
terminant of visitation, and with Crompton and behavior exhibited by some leisure first-time
Ankomah (1993) who postulated that decisions visitors to a given destination area.” From a
that involve familiar destinations would reduce managerial perspective, this finding suggests
the number of choices in the late consideration the importance of word-of-mouth advertising,
set. It is further possible that this finding is re- as it appears that for some visitors, just hearing a
lated to Decrop’s (1999) proposition that there confirmation that the product is a good one, jus-
are distinct differences between “brand loyal- tified their purchase.
ists” (those with intensive experience) and Results of the current study also validate por-
“variety seekers” (those with extensive experi- tions of the Decrop and Snelders’ (2005) deci-
ence). The role of prior experience in decision- sion-making typology. They proposed that de-
making is thus very complex, and certainly cision-makers can be classified as: habitual,
warrants further research. For cruise manage- rational, hedonic, opportunistic, constrained or
ment this finding reveals the importance of adaptable, and that these groups are not mutu-
maintaining customer loyalty, and that the use ally exclusive. Present results suggest that
of loyalty programs could be very beneficial.
many of the ABC cruisers are habitual cruisers,
Interestingly, the three first-time ABC cruis-
ers who knew that they wanted to take a ABC as they tended to make the same final decisions
cruise the second they decided to go on vacation that they have in the past (routinized). Addi-
demonstrated a new decision-making behavior tionally, some of the respondents could be clas-
that does not fall into traditional understanding: sified as rational (chose ABC as it was less
they had not been brand loyal yet (i.e., no previ- risky) and as opportunistic (i.e., went with a
ous experience with the brand), and did not uti- group that made the decisions for them). While
lize complex decision-making. In examining it would be assumed that many of the visitors
their decision-making processes, it was re- made decisions based on hedonic (for pleasure)
vealed that these respondents chose their vaca- reasons, the scope of the study did not reveal
tion, based on social influences (i.e., a family this as a motive. Additionally, the groups of
member, other cruisers, a travel agent). It ap- constrained and adaptable did not appear to be
pears as if these cruisers absorbed the informa- part of this sample. Future research should in-
tion from external sources perceived as credi- clude methods to examine whether or not these
ble and these perceptions became part of their groups of decision makers exist for cruise vaca-
memory. Once they decided to go on a vacation, tions. This knowledge could be very useful to
12 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

cruise management in determining target mar- limited by utilizing passengers on board only
kets, and their differences. one ship, for one cruise line. Since Tyrrell,
Results of the current study also revealed Countryman, Hong, and Cai (2001) have
that for decisions that were more complex, per- shown differences in decision-making pro-
ceptions of value tended to be the most impor- cesses for travelers to different destinations,
tant decision-making factor. This finding is more research is necessary prior to generalizing
similar to Sirakaya, McLellan, and Uysal the current results. The current study was fur-
(1996) who found that cost is one of the most ther limited to only one cruising season
important variables related to final choice. The (Spring). The demographics of cruise passen-
importance of value may be even a larger draw gers change greatly between seasons, and fur-
for ABC passengers, as ABC has been awarded ther research is necessary in order to determine
“World’s Best Cruise Value” for the past thir- if decisions made for vacations at this time of
teen years (CruiseNetwork, 2005). This sug-
the year are the same as they are during other
gests that ABC should utilize messages related
to value, as it appears to be a major pull factor to times (i.e., are children more involved during
their market. Future research should examine the summer months?).
whether this finding is similar across lines. While it is difficult to generalize the results
It was also found that initial decisions had a of the present study, it is believed that they offer
tendency to be made solely by those who would new insights into the decision-making pro-
be traveling (via internal information process- cesses of cruise passengers. As proposed by
ing), while final decisions involved external in- Sirakaya, McLellan, and Uysal (1996), deci-
formation (i.e., Web sites, literature), and addi- sion-making is very individualistic, making it
tional social influences (i.e., travel agent, unrealistic to develop a model which explains
friends) as suggested by Huan and Beaman all tourists’ behaviors. Yet, the current study
(2003) and Maser and Weiermair (1998). This generated a better understanding of when
finding reveals to cruise management the im- cruise passengers’ decisions are made, how/
portance of having accurate information avail- why decisions were made, and who/what influ-
able to decision makers during the final phase, enced those decisions. Since little empirical re-
and the importance of word-of-mouth advertis- search has been conducted in this area, it is fur-
ing. Future research should examine what types ther believed that the current results offer an
of information are desired by decision-makers initial conceptualization of the decision-mak-
during this phase, and ways to best present it. ing processes that cruise passengers on a pre-
A final finding of the current study is that mier line go through. With the use of this
current respondents not only had to make a de- knowledge, cruise management should be able
cision at the brand level (i.e., ABC), but also at to more effectively manage resources, as they
the sub-brand level (i.e., which ship to choose). should have a better understanding of their
This decision was most often based on the itin- clientele.
erary, or the ship (in most cases, ship was cho-
sen because it was new). This finding suggests
that for cruise lines which have ships that are REFERENCES
not doing as well as the rest of their fleet, it may
Ankomah, P. K., Crompton, J. L., & Baker, D. (1996).
be possible to increase interest by changing the Influence of cognitive distance in vacation choice.
itinerary to a more appealing one, and that Annals of Tourism Research, 23(1), 138-150.
changing out older ships for newer ones may be Assael, H. (2004). Consumer behavior: A strategic ap-
an effective strategy. Since the ship utilized in proach. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
the current study was the fleet’s newest ship, fu- Bettman, J. R. (1979). Information processing theory of
ture research should examine whether or not consumer choice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
“newness” is an important factor in cruise Botha, C., Crompton, J. L., & Kim, S.-S. (1999). Devel-
oping a revised competitive position for Sun/Lost
passengers’ decision-making processes. City, South Africa. Journal of Travel Research,
While it is believed that the methods utilized 37(4), 341-352.
in the current study were fundamentally sound, Caneen, J. M. (2003). Cultural determinants of tourist in-
the study still had limitations. The study was tention to return. Tourism Analysis, 8(2), 237-242.
Petrick, Li, and Park 13

Cartwright, R., & Baird, C. (1999). The development and Fodness, D., & Murray, B. (1999). A model of tourist in-
growth of the cruise industry. Oxford: Butterworth- formation search behavior. Journal of Travel Re-
Heinemann. search, 37(3), 220-230.
Choosing Cruising. (2004). Holland America, MS Zaandam. Ford, J. B., LaTour, M. S., & Henthorne, T. L. (1995).
Retrieved December 18, 2004, from http://www. Perception of marital roles in purchase decision pro-
choosingcruising.co.uk/Cruiseweb/ShpDetailsQuery. cesses: A cross cultural study. Journal of the Acad-
asp?nshp=229 emy of Marketing Sciences, 23(2), 120-131.
Crompton, J. L. (1992). Structure of destination choice Gitelson, R., & Kerstetter, D. (1994). The influence of
sets. Annals of Tourism Research, 19(3), 420-434. friends and relatives in travel decision-making. Jour-
Crompton, J. L., & Ankomah, P. K. (1993). Choice set nal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 3(3), 59-68.
propositions in destination decisions. Annals of Tour- Gursoy, D., & McCleary, K. W. (2004). An integrative
ism Research, 20(3), 461-476. model of tourists’ information search behavior. An-
Crompton, J. L., Botha, C., & Kim, S.-S. (1998). Testing nals of Tourism Research, 31(2), 353-373.
selected choice propositions. Annals of Tourism Re- Heung, V. C. S., Qu, H., & Chu, R. (2001). Relationship
search, 26(1), 210-213. between vacation factors and socio-demographic and
Crompton, J. L., & Kim, S. S. (2001). The influence of traveling characteristics: The case of Japanese leisure
cognitive distance in vacation choice. Annals of travelers. Tourism Management, 22(3), 259-269.
Tourism Research, 28(2), 512-515. Howard, D. (1963). Marketing management (2nd ed.).
Cruise Line International Association. (2005). CLIA: Homewood, IL: Irwin Publishing.
The overview Spring 2005. Retrieved February 15, Huan, T. C., & Beaman, J. (2003). Contexts and dynam-
2006, from http://www.cruising.org/press/overview/ ics of social interaction and Information search in de-
Spring2005.pdf cision making for discretionary travel. Tourism
Cruise Line International Association. (2006). Profile of Analysis, 8(2), 177-182.
the U.S. cruise industry. Retrieved February 15, Huang, L., & Tsai, H. T. (2003). The study of senior trav-
2006, from http://www.cruising.org/press/sourcebook eler behavior in Taiwan. Tourism Management,
2006/profile_cruise_industry.cfm 24(5), 561-574.
CruiseNetwork. (2005). Lucky number 13 for Holland Jeng, J., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2002). Conceptualizing
America. Retrieved February 10, 2006, from http:// the travel decision-making hierarchy: A review of re-
www.cruisenetwork.com/cruise-news-20050425.jsp cent developments. Tourism Analysis, 7(1), 15-32.
Cruise & Vacation Views. (2001). Holland America Jiang, J., Havitz, M. E., & O’Brien, R. M. (2000). Vali-
Line: Keeping tradition and elegance exciting. dating the international tourist role scale. Annals of
Retrieved February 10, 2006, from http://www. Tourism Research, 27(4), 964-981.
etravelnews.com/Subscribers/CruiseMarket/Cruise Johnson, R. R., & Messmer, D. J. (1991). The effect of
Market_ref_2001/Holland%20America%20Line/ advertising on hierarchical stages in vacation destina-
Holland_America_2001.htm tion choice. Journal of Advertising Research, 31(4),
Dann, G., & Philips, J. (2001). Qualitative tourism re- 18-24.
search in the late twentieth century and beyond. In B. Kim, S.-S., Crompton, J. L., & Botha, C. (2000). Re-
Faulkner, G. Moscardo, & E. Laws (Eds), Tourism in sponding to competition: A strategy for Sun/Lost
the twenty-first century (pp. 247-265). London: Con- City, South Africa. Tourism Management, 21(1),
tinuum. 33-41.
Decrop, A. (1999). Personal aspects of vacationers’ de- Klenosky, D. B., & Gitelson, R. E. (1998). Travel agents’
cision making processes: An interpretivist approach. destination Recommendations. Annals of Tourism
Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 8(4), Research, 25(3), 661-674.
59-68. Kozak, M. (2001). Repeaters’ behavior at two distinct
Decrop, A. (2005). Group processes in vacation deci- destinations. Annals of Tourism Research, 28(3),
sion-making. Journal of Travel & Tourism Market- 784-807.
ing, 18(3), 23-36. Kozak, M. (2002). Comparative analysis of tourist moti-
Decrop, A., & Snelders, D. (2005). A grounded typology vations by nationality and destinations. Tourism
of vacation decision-making. Tourism Management, Management, 23(3), 221-232.
26(2), 121-132. Langer, E. (1978). Rethinking the role of thought in so-
Dellaert, B. G. C., Borgers, A. W. J., & Timmermans, H. cial interactions. In J. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R.
J. P. (1997). Conjoint models of tourist portfolio Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution re-
choice: Theory and illustration. Leisure Sciences, search (pp. 35-58). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
19(1), 31-58. Associates.
Dellaert, B. G. C., Ettema, D. F., & Lindh, C. (1998). Lois, P., Wang, J., Wall, A., & Ruxton, T. (2004). For-
Multi-faceted tourist travel decisions: A con- mal safety assessment of cruise ships. Tourism Man-
straint-based conceptual framework to describe tour- agement, 25(1), 93-109.
ists’ sequential choices of travel components. Madrigal, R. (1993). Parents’ perceptions of family
Tourism Management, 19(4), 313-320. members’ relative influence in vacation decision
14 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

making. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, Thill, J. C., & Wheeler, A. (2000). Tree induction of spa-
2(4), 39-57. tial choice behavior. Transportation Research Record,
Maser, B., & Weiermair, K. (1998). Travel deci- 1719, 250-258.
sion-making: From the vantage point of perceived Thornton, P. R., Shaw, G., & Williams, A. M. (1997).
risk and information preferences. Journal of Travel Tourist group holiday decision-making and behav-
and Tourism Marketing, 7(4), 107-120. ior: The influence of children. Tourism Management,
Morgan, M. S. (1991). Travelers’ choice: The effects of 18(5), 287-297.
advertising and prior stay. Cornell Hotel & Restau- Tyrrell, B., Countryman, C., Hong, G. S., & Cai, L. A.
rant Administration Quarterly, 32(4), 41-49. (2001). Determinants of destination choice by Japa-
Mottiar, Z., & Quinn, D. (2004). Couple dynamics in nese overseas travelers. Journal of Travel and Tour-
household tourism decision making: Women as the ism Marketing, 10(2/3), 87-100.
gatekeepers? Journal of Vacation Marketing, 10(2), Um, S., & Crompton, J. L. (1990). Attitude determinants
149-160. in tourism destination choice. Annals of Tourism Re-
Nicosia, F. M. (1966). Consumer decision processes: search, 17(3), 432-448.
Marketing and advertising implications. Englewood Vogt, C. A., & Fesenmaier, D. (1998). Expanding the
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. functional information search model. Annals of Tour-
Oppermann, M. (1998). Travel horizon: A valuable ism Research, 25(3), 551-578.
analysis tool? Tourism Management, 19(4), 321-329. Wang, K. C., Hsieh, A. T., Yeh, Y. C., & Tsai, C. W.
Oppermann, M. (2000). Tourism destination loyalty. (2004). Who is the decision-maker: The parents or
Journal of Travel Research, 39(1), 78-84. the child in group package tours? Tourism Manage-
Prentice, R., & Andersen, V. (2000). Evoking Ireland: ment, 25(2), 183-194.
Modeling tourist propensity. Annals of Tourism Re- Woodside, A. G., & Dubelaar, C. (2002). A general the-
search, 27(2), 490-516. ory of tourism consumption systems: A conceptual
Rompf, P., DiPietro, R. B., & Ricci, P. (2005). Local’s framework and an empirical exploration. Journal of
involvement in travelers’ informational search and Travel Research, 41(2), 120-132.
venue decision strategies while at destination. Jour- Woodside, A. G., & King, R. (2001). Tourism consump-
nal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 18(3), 11-22. tion systems: Theory and empirical research. Journal
Sirakaya, E., McLellan, R. W., & Uysal, M. (1996). of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 10(1), 3-27.
Modeling vacation destination decisions: A behav- Woodside, A. G., & Lysonski, S. (1989). A general
ioral approach. Journal of Travel and Tourism Mar- model of traveler destination choice. Journal of
keting, 5(1/2), 57-75. Travel Research, 27(4), 8-14.
Sirakaya, E., Sonmez, S. F., & Choi, H. S. (2001). Do Woodside, A. G., McDonald, R., & Burford, M. (2004).
destination images really matter? Predicting destina- Grounded theory of leisure travel. Journal of Travel
tion choices of student travelers. Journal of Vacation & Tourism Marketing, 17(1), 7-39.
Marketing, 7(2), 125-142. Zalatan, A. (1996). The determinants of planning time in
Sirakaya, E., & Woodside, A. G. (2005). Building and vacation travel. Tourism Management, 17(2),
testing theories of decision making by travellers. 123-131.
Tourism Management, 26(6), 815-832. Zalatan, A. (1998). Wives’ involvement in tourism deci-
Sonmez, S. F. (1998). Tourism, terrorism, and political sion processes. Annals of Tourism Research, 25(4),
instability. Annals of Tourism Research, 25(2), 890-903.
416-456.
Sonmez, S. F., & Graefe, A. R. (1998). Influence of ter- SUBMITTED: February 28, 2006
rorism risk on foreign tourism decisions. Annals of
Tourism Research, 25(1), 112-144. FINAL REVISION SUBMITTED:
Sonmez, S. F., & Sirakaya, E. (2002). A distorted desti- October 24, 2006
nation image: The case of Turkey. Journal of Travel ACCEPTED: November 28, 2006
Research, 41(2), 185-196. REFEREED ANONYMOUSLY

doi:10.1300/J073v23n01_01
View publication stats

S-ar putea să vă placă și