Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

This article was downloaded by: [laure Coudrat]

On: 04 February 2014, At: 08:32


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Sports Sciences


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

Emotional pictures impact repetitive sprint ability test


on cycle ergometre
ab ac a a a a
Laure Coudrat , Majdi Rouis , Hamdi Jaafar , Elvis Attiogbé , Thierry Gélat & Tarak Driss
a
Centre de Recherches sur le Sport et le Mouvement (CeRSM), UFR STAPS, Université Paris
Ouest Nanterre La Defense, Nanterre, France
b
Laboratoire de Recherche Adaptations physiologiques à l’exercice et réadaptation à
l’effort, EA3300, UFR STAPS, Université de Picardie Jules Vernes, Amiens, France
c
Laboratoire d’Etudes de la Motricité Humaine, Faculté des Sciences du Sport et de l’EP,
Université de Lille, Ronchin, France
Published online: 30 Jan 2014.

To cite this article: Laure Coudrat, Majdi Rouis, Hamdi Jaafar, Elvis Attiogbé, Thierry Gélat & Tarak Driss , Journal of Sports
Sciences (2014): Emotional pictures impact repetitive sprint ability test on cycle ergometre, Journal of Sports Sciences, DOI:
10.1080/02640414.2013.865253

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.865253

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2013
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.865253

Emotional pictures impact repetitive sprint ability test on cycle


ergometre

LAURE COUDRAT1,2, MAJDI ROUIS1,3, HAMDI JAAFAR1, ELVIS ATTIOGBÉ1,


THIERRY GÉLAT1 & TARAK DRISS1
1
Centre de Recherches sur le Sport et le Mouvement (CeRSM), UFR STAPS, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Defense,
Nanterre, France, 2Laboratoire de Recherche Adaptations physiologiques à l’exercice et réadaptation à l’effort, EA3300, UFR
STAPS, Université de Picardie Jules Vernes, Amiens, France and 3Laboratoire d’Etudes de la Motricité Humaine, Faculté des
Sciences du Sport et de l’EP, Université de Lille, Ronchin, France

(Accepted 10 November 2013)


Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

Abstract
This study investigated the interaction between emotion-eliciting pictures and power output during a repetitive supra-
maximal task on a cycle ergometre. Twelve male participants (mean (±SD) age, height and weight: 28.58 ± 3.23 years, 1.78
± 0.05 m and 82.41 ± 13.29 kg) performed 5 repeated sprint tests on a cycle ergometre in front of neutral, pleasant or
unpleasant pictures. For each sprint, mechanical (peak power and work), physiological (heart rate) and perceptual (affective
load) indices were analysed. Affective load was calculated from the ratings of perceived exertion, which reflected the amount
of pleasant and unpleasant responses experienced during exercise. The results showed that peak power, work and heart rate
values were significantly lower (P < 0.05) for unpleasant pictures (9.18 ± 0.20 W ∙ kg−1; 47.69 ± 1.08 J ∙ kg−1; 152 ± 4 bpm)
when compared with pleasant ones (9.50 ± 0.20 W ∙ kg−1; 50.11 ± 0.11 J ∙ kg−1; 156 ± 3 bpm). Furthermore, the affective
load was found to be similar for the pleasant and unpleasant sessions. All together, these results suggested that the ability to
produce maximal power output depended on whether the emotional context was pleasant or unpleasant. The fact that the
power output was lower in the unpleasant versus pleasant session could reflect a regulatory process aimed at maintaining a
similar level of affective load for both sessions.

Keywords: power output, repetitive sprint ability test, affective load, emotion

Introduction demonstrated the existence of a pacing strategy


managing the total energy expenditure during
Sports performance depends not only on the abil-
exercise in order to ensure that the exercise bout
ity to produce a maximal or near-maximal effort
is completed with maximal performance and with-
but also on the ability to repeat it. These skills
out a catastrophic biological failure (Noakes, St
could be measured by cycling laboratory tests
Clair Gibson, & Lambert, 2005; St Clair Gibson
such as the repetitive sprint ability (i.e. RSA) test
& Noakes, 2004). In the central governor model
(Bishop, Spencer, Duffield, & Lawrence, 2001).
(CGM) (Noakes et al., 2005), this pacing strategy
Performance in such a test, estimated from the
is thought to be controlled by the brain and
power output, depends on several factors such as
selected according to internal (e.g. from peripheral
recovery patterns (Billaut & Basset, 2007), pre-
physiological systems) and external (e.g. environ-
vious fatiguing (Mendez-Villanueva, Hamer, &
mental conditions) information (Noakes et al.,
Bishop, 2007) or time of the day (Racinais,
2005; St Clair Gibson et al., 2006; St Clair
Connes, Bishop, Blonc, & Hue, 2005). For exam-
Gibson & Noakes, 2004). Among this information,
ple, in the latter study, the peak power developed
the conscious sensory perception of fatigue may
in the first sprint was higher in the evening than in
have a crucial role (Baron, Moullan, Deruelle, &
the morning. Moreover, it has been recently sug-
Noakes, 2011; St Clair Gibson & Noakes, 2004).
gested that power output is also affected by the
Interestingly, based on the hypothesis that the rat-
knowledge of sprint number (Billaut, Bishop,
ing of perceived exertion (RPE) has an affective
Schaerz, & Noakes, 2011). These findings
component (Baden, McLean, Tucker, Noakes, &

Correspondence: Laure Coudrat, Centre de Recherches sur le Sport et le Mouvement (CeRSM), UFR STAPS, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Defense,
Nanterre, France. E-mail: laure.coudrat@wanadoo.fr

© 2014 Taylor & Francis


2 L. Coudrat et al.

St Clair Gibson, 2005; St Clair Gibson et al., 2006), et al.’s study, an extended exposure to unpleasant
Baron et al. (2011) recently proposed an affective pictures (i.e. 8 s) decreased the movement accuracy
loading scale computed on the basis of RPE. (i.e. greater error). This result illustrated the deleter-
Affective loading here is defined as the difference ious effect of unpleasant stimuli on performance,
between negative and positive affects arising from resulting from the defensive motivational system
exercise intensity and represent internal information activation.
that contributes to the regulation of pacing strategy. To our knowledge, no study has investigated the
In this framework, the authors suggested that the role of external emotional information during supra-
more positive the affective responses during exercise, maximal exercise performed in varied emotional
the greater the desire to maintain or to increase the contexts. The potential role of emotional informa-
exercise intensity. In contrast, the more negative the tion in changing power output during supra-maxi-
affective responses, the lesser this desire, resulting in mal exercise might have strong implications on
a performance decrease. The role of this emotion- training.
motivational process has been studied under condi- The aim of the present study was to explore the
tions where changes in emotional state stemmed effect of continued emotional information on a non-
from exercise intensity and not from external sensory directional movement: a repetitive supra-maximal
information (Smith & O’Connor, 2003; Tian & task on a cycle ergometre. Emotional context was
Smith, 2011). For example, affective valence of the modulated by the display of neutral (i.e. control
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

experienced exercise was less pleasant during a high- condition), pleasant and unpleasant pictures, tradi-
intensity exercise (i.e. 105% of ventilatory threshold) tionally used to induce emotion. Based on Baron’s
compared with a low-intensity (i.e. 85% of ventila- model and the results of Coombes, we hypothesised
tory threshold) cycling bout, suggesting that affective that peak power and work should be greater in a
responses were less pleasant and the desire to main- pleasant context, when compared with an unpleasant
tain the exercise was lower during a high-intensity one.
exercise (Kilpatrick, Kraemer, Bartholomew,
Acevedo, & Jarreau, 2007). From another point of
view, other studies showed that unpleasant emotions Methods
such as anxiety and anger might be used for enhan- Participants
cing performance based on emotion-regulation stra-
tegies (Lane, Beedie, Jones, Uphill, & Devonport, Twelve active and healthy men practicing recrea-
2012). tional sporting activities for about 4–5 h a week
Furthermore, it has been hypothesised that emo- (jogging, soccer) volunteered for this study. None
tional motivation, such that involved by appetitive of them was a cyclist. Their mean (±SD) age, height
and defensive motivational systems (Lang, Bradley, and weight were 28.58 ± 3.23 years, 1.78 ± 0.05 m,
& Cuthbert, 1997), was activated by pleasant and and 82.41 ± 13.29 kg, respectively. Each participant
unpleasant external sensory information (e.g. emo- signed an informed consent before the experimental
tional pictures), respectively, contrasting with emo- procedures. The study complied with the Helsinki
tional motivation arising from effort intensity. In this Declaration for human experimentation and was
framework, many studies have shown that emotional approved by the Institutional Review Board of the
information could have some beneficial or deleter- University.
ious effects on many goal-directed movements such
as those involved in button press tasks (Pereira et al.,
Experimental design
2006), arm movement (Chen & Bargh, 1999;
Coombes, Cauraugh, & Janelle, 2006) and gait Three days after experiencing a familiarisation ses-
initiation (Gélat, Coudrat, & Le Pellec, 2011; Stins sion, participants completed 3 test sessions in a ran-
& Beek, 2011). In these studies, the impact of emo- domised order: a neutral, a pleasant and an
tion on motor performance, measured in terms of unpleasant one (2 days apart). Thus, 6 possible com-
reaction time, accuracy and amplitude of motor binations were performed: neutral-pleasant-unplea-
responses, usually depended on the congruency sant, neutral-unpleasant-pleasant, pleasant-neutral-
between the direction of the considered movement unpleasant, pleasant-unpleasant-neutral, unplea-
(i.e. approach, avoidance) and the valence of stimuli. sant-neutral-pleasant and unpleasant-pleasant-neu-
To the extent that supra-maximal exercises are often tral, with 2 participants per combination. Each
produced without any approach-avoidance direction, session consisted of a warm-up and a RSA cycle test
one study appears to be particularly interesting as (Bishop et al., 2001).
emotional effects were analysed on a non-directional
arm movement such as a square-tracing task Warm-up. The standardised warm-up consisted of
(Coombes, Janelle, & Duley, 2005). In Coombes 5 min of cycling at 80 W, followed by 3 min of rest
Interaction between emotion and power output 3

and then a 10-s maximal sprint test on cycle ergo- According to the IAPS model, valence of each
metre. Upon completion of the 10-s test, partici- image category was chosen by the experimenter as
pants rested for 5 min before performing the RSA significantly different from each other (P < 0.05).
cycle test. Importantly, participants were required Sports images (6.9) were rated as more pleasant
to gaze horizontally during the 10-s maximal sprint than neutral people images (5.1), which were rated
test. This instruction served as training the parti- as more pleasant than mutilation ones (2.2).
cipants to keep their gaze horizontally during the However, while the arousal of sports and mutilation
subsequent RSA cycle test, for which a projected pictures were not significantly different (P = 0.480),
picture had to be viewed during each 6-s sprint. neutral people images were rated as less arousing
The participants were informed that if their head (P < 0.0001) than both sport and mutilation images
got down during the RSA test, then the trial would (neutral people: 3.2, sport: 6.1, mutilation: 5.8).
be discarded. IAPS image numbers were (i) neutral, 2102, 2191,
2383, 2513, 2570; (ii) pleasant, 8060, 8090, 8200,
RSA cycle test. Each RSA cycle test consisted of 5 × 8210, 8260; (iii) unpleasant, 3015, 3051, 3062,
6-s sprints separated each by 24-s of passive recovery 3068, 3100.
period on a cycle ergometre (Monark 894E, Immediately following the completion of all ses-
Stockholm, Sweden). Load was set at 60 g ∙ kg−1 of sions, a computerised 9-point version of the self-
body mass on the basis of previous results which assessment manikin (SAM) (Lang et al., 2005) was
showed that a load between 50 and 75 g ∙ kg−1 of
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

used to obtain subjective ratings of valence and


body mass is suitable for the determination of max- arousal of all images viewed during the sessions.
imal power in active adults (Doré et al., 2000). In this system, ratings of valence are indicated by
Participants were seated on the cycle ergometre 5 graphical representations of facial expressions
placed at a distance of 2.50 m in front of a white ranging from a severe frown (most unpleasant) to
wall of the laboratory on which visual stimuli was a broad smile (most pleasant). For the arousal, the
presented. The temperature was 22°C. manikin varies from a state of low to high agitation.
At trial onset, adjustments of saddle height, foot Participants may select any of the 5 figures, or
position on pedals and upper body position were boxes in between.
made according to each participant’s satisfaction.
Toe clips and heel straps were used to prevent the
feet from slipping. The optimal riding position was
maintained identical throughout the study. The start Variables
and end of each sprint was performed according to
Mechanical indices. The Monark cycle ergometer
the onset and extinction of a single visual stimulus,
interfaced with a microcomputer was used to collect
respectively. Participants were instructed (1) to
the data and to calculate the power and the work
assume the ready position while awaiting the start
performed during each sprint.
visual signal, (2) to pedal as soon as and as fast as
The peak power output (PPeak) elicited during
possible after stimulus onset, (3) to perform an all-
each sprint was calculated from the relationship
out effort throughout the duration of the stimulus
between the peak pedal velocity and the braking
presentation and (4) to look at the stimulus the
force as described by Vandewalle, Pérès, Heller,
entire time it was on the wall, i.e. do not get the
and Monod (1985). The mechanical work (W) per-
head down during the 6-s sprint test.
formed during each sprint was calculated by the
integral of the power versus time curve.
Stimuli presentation. SuperLab Pro v.2 was used to
Two decrement values were calculated by con-
control the presentation of visual stimuli for each
sidering all sprints: the decrement in peak power
sprint which began with a 4-s preparatory period,
output (PPdec) and the decrement in work done
characterised by the display of a fixation cross, fol-
(Wdec) using the following equations (Bishop
lowed by a 6-s visual stimulus (95 cm × 160 cm).
et al., 2001; Fitzsimmons, Dawson, & Ward,
During the familiarisation session, the visual stimu-
1993):
lus was a white square. During the 3 test sessions, it
was a neutral, pleasant or unpleasant image. Thus,  
15 images were selected from the International sum of PPeak
PPdec ð%Þ ¼ 1  100
Affective Picture System (IAPS) according to their 5  best PPeak
normative ratings for male participants (Lang,
Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2005). These images repre-
sented neutral people for the neutral session, sports  
total W
images for the pleasant session and mutilation for the Wdec ð%Þ ¼ 1  100
unpleasant session. 5  highest W
4 L. Coudrat et al.

Physiological and perceptual responses to exercise. The measures on the 2 factors. PPdec and Wdec bias
heart rate (HR) was continuously measured during scores were analysed using a one-way repeated-mea-
each trial using a heart rate monitor with 1-s fre- sures ANOVA (emotional pictures: pleasant,
quency (Polar RS400, Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, unpleasant).
Finland) strapped to the chest wall directly above the The difference between the ratings of self-reported
heart. The peak of heart rate was obtained for each valence and arousal of each image category was ana-
sprint. lysed using one-way repeated-measures ANOVA
For each 6-s sprint, participants were asked to rate (image categories: neutral, pleasant and unpleasant).
their subjective perceived exertion (RPE) using the Post hoc analyses were conducted using the
6–20 point Borg scale (Borg, 1982). The end of each Bonferroni test. For all analyses, the probability
6-s sprint corresponded to the offset of a picture. At was set at P < 0.05. Effect sizes were calculated as
this time, the RPE scale appeared on the wall and partial eta-squared (η2p ) to estimate the meaningful-
participants had to give one number from 6 to 20 ness of significant findings.
after each 6-s sprint. According to Baron et al.
(2011) and considering affect as bipolar, RPE scores
allowed to calculate the affective load defined as the Results
difference between the coexisting negative and posi- Pictures ratings and affective load bias score
tive affective responses associated to the exercise. As
The one-way ANOVA performed on the ratings of
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

RPE score indicates a physical discomfort, it directly


reflects the amount of unpleasant affective (UA) self-reported valence (SAM procedure) revealed a
responses, which cannot be lower than 6 (very very main effect of image category (F2,22 = 156.45, P <
light) and greater than 20 (very very hard). Pleasant 0.0001, η2p = 0.93). Post hoc test showed that valence
affective (PA) responses could be thus deduced from for pleasant image (6.3 ± 0.4) was significantly
the amount of UA responses. For instance, if the higher (P = 0.01) than neutral images (5.3 ± 0.2),
RPE score is equal to 6, then PA responses will be the latter being significantly higher (P < 0.0001) than
equal to 14. In this case, affective load computed as unpleasant ones (1.2 ± 0.1).
the difference between UA and PA responses, will be The one-way ANOVA performed on the ratings of
equal to −8. At the opposite, if the RPE score is self-reported arousal also revealed a main effect of
equal to 20, then PA responses will be equal to 0. image category (F2,22 = 93.81, P < 0.0001, η2p =
In this case, affective load will be equal to 20. A 0.89).
negative affective load score indicates the dominance In contrast with normative arousal ratings (IAPS),
of PA responses, whereas a positive score indicates post hoc test demonstrated that pleasant images (4.5
the dominance of unpleasant ones. ± 0.5) were rated as less arousing (P < 0.0001) than
unpleasant ones (7.8 ± 0.3). Neutral images (2.3 ±
Emotional bias scores. In order to assess the positive or 0.4) were rated as less arousing (P < 0.0001) than
negative effect of emotional pictures on perfor- pleasant and unpleasant images.
mance, 2 indexes were created for each dependent Regarding the affective load bias score, the 2-way
variable: a pleasant and an unpleasant effect bias ANOVA revealed no main effect of image (P =
score. They were calculated as the value associated 0.088) despite a trend indicating that the affective
to the pleasant or unpleasant context, respectively, load value was lower for pleasant pictures (0.43 ±
minus the value associated to the neutral one (Fox, 0.50), compared with unpleasant ones (1.73 ± 0.58)
2002; Naugle, Joyner, Hass, & Janelle, 2010). A (Table I). No main effect of sprint and no interac-
positive score indicates greater values during an tion were found on this variable (P > 0.05).
emotional relative to a neutral context, while a nega-
tive score indicates reduced values during the emo-
Mechanical and physiological bias scores
tional relative to a neutral context. The bias scores
served as the bases for all statistical analyses. Table I presents summary data for all these depen-
dent variables.
The 2-way ANOVA (image × sprint) showed a
Statistical analysis
significant main effect of image for the PPeak (F1,11
Analyses were performed using Statistica 7.1 soft- = 21.71, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.66), W (F1,11 = 56.02, P
ware for Windows (StatSoft, France). Data distribu- < 0.0001, η2p = 0.84), and HR (F1,11 = 6.71, P =
tion normality was confirmed with the Kolmogorov- 0.025, η2p = 0.38). Exposure to pleasant pictures led
Smirnov distance test. Bias scores for PPeak, W, HR to significantly greater peak power (Figure 1a), work
and affective load were analysed using a 2-way (emo- (Figure 1b) and heart rate (Figure 2c) following
tional pictures: pleasant, unpleasant) x 5 (sprints) picture onset, as compared with unpleasant ones.
analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated However, the 2-way ANOVA (image x sprint)
Interaction between emotion and power output 5

Table I. Trends of peak power, heart rate and affective load during the 5 sprints and according to the neutral, pleasant and unpleasant
pictures. Values are mean ± SD.

Sprint 1 Sprint 2 Sprint 3 Sprint 4 Sprint 5


−1
Peak power (W ∙ kg ) Neutral 10.18 ± 0.24 9.84 ± 0.26 9.44 ± 0.23 9.10 ± 0.24 8.75 ± 0.22
Pleasant 10.34 ± 0.24 9.88 ± 0.22 9.43 ± 0.22 9.06 ± 0.19 8.79 ± 0.18
Unpleasant 9.66 ± 0.26 9.42 ± 0.23 9.29 ± 0.22 8.93 ± 0.21 8.62 ± 0.20
P *** ** NS NS NS
Work (J ∙ kg−1) Neutral 53.07 ± 1.17 50.97 ± 1.27 49.45 ± 1.17 47.59 ± 1.37 46.25 ± 1.11
Pleasant 54.33 ± 1.29 51.84 ± 1.15 49.42 ± 1.25 48.19 ± 1.09 46.78 ± 0.91
Unpleasant 50.05 ± 1.51 48.70 ± 1.29 48.04 ± 1.03 46.67 ± 0.97 44.99 ± 1.17
P *** *** NS NS NS
Hear rate (bpm) Neutral 139.5 ± 4.0 154.2 ± 4.1 161.2 ± 4.3 164.8 ± 4.3 165.8 ± 4.4
Pleasant 139.4 ± 3.5 155.2 ± 3.5 .0 ± 3.9 163.1 ± 3.8 164.0 ± 3.8
Unpleasant 136.2 ± 4.3 149.1 ± 3.9 155.7 ± 4.4 159.4 ± 4.4 159.8 ± 4.8
P * * * * *
Affective load Neutral −1.17 ± 1.29 1.33 ± 1.19 3.67 ± 1.30 6.67 ± 1.33 9.00 ± 1.17
Pleasant −0.83 ± 1.29 1.67 ± 1.37 4.50 ± 1.35 6.83 ± 1.44 9.50 ± 1.46
Unpleasant 1.0 ± 1.00 3.33 ± 0.96 5.67 ± 0.95 7.83 ± 1.29 10.33 ± 1.47
P NS NS NS NS NS
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

Note: P: significance level for the difference between emotional bias scores; ***: P < 0.001, **: P < 0.01, *: P < 0.05, NS: not significant.

1.0
(a) (b) *
0.10 *
0.5
0.05
W bias score (J.kg )

0.00 0.0
–1
Ppeak bias score (W.kg )
–1

–0.05
–0.5
–0.10
–1.0
–0.15

–0.20 –1.5

–0.25
–2.0
–0.30
–2.5
–0.35

2.0
(c) 2.0 (d) pleasant

1.5 * 1.5 * unpleasant

1.0 1.0 neutral


PPdec bias score (%)

Wdec bias score (%)

0.5 0.5

0.0 0.0

–0.5 –0.5

–1.0 –1.0

–1.5 –1.5
–2.0 –2.0

Figure 1. Mean (and standard error) bias scores for (a) PPeak, (b) W, (c) PPdec and (d) Wdec across trials. *Significant (P < 0.05) difference
between emotional bias scores.

showed no significant main effect of sprint for PPeak, Discussion


W and HR. There was a significant interaction
According to our hypothesis, greater peak power and
(sprint × image) effect for PPeak (F4,44 = 6.52, P <
work were obtained in the pleasant context, com-
0.001, η2p = 0.37) and W (F4,44 = 5.05, P < 0.01, η2p
pared with the unpleasant ones. Specifically, peak
= 0.31). Post hoc tests revealed that the image effect
power and work bias scores were positive for plea-
was only observed during the 2 first sprints (P <
sant trials and negative for unpleasant ones (Figure
0.001) (Figure 2a, b).
1). These results demonstrated that emotional pic-
The one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect
tures affected power output during a supra-maximal
of image for PPdec (F1,11 = 10.03, P < 0.01, η2p =
task depending on whether their affective valence
0.48) and Wdec (F1,11 = 5.49, P = 0.039, η2p = 0.33),
was pleasant or unpleasant. While pleasant pictures
which were greater during exposure to pleasant (8.07
increased performance, unpleasant ones impaired it.
± 0.71% and 7.69 ± 0.64%) images (Figure 1c, d),
In line with Baron et al.’s model, the performance
compared with unpleasant (5.99 ± 0.74% and 5.97
improvement observed in pleasant trials accounted
± 0.93%) ones.
6 L. Coudrat et al.

0.3 *
(a)
*
0.1

Ppeak bias score (W.kg–1)


–0.1

–0.3

–0.5

–0.7
(b)

2.0
*
*
1.0
W bias score (J.kg )
–1

0.0

–1.0
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

–2.0

–3.0

–4.0 (c)

3
* * * *
*
1
Heart rate bias score (bpm)

–2

–4

–6

–8

–10

unpleasant
(d)
4.5 pleasant

neutral
3.5
Affective load bias score

2.5

1.5

0.5

sprint 1 sprint 2 sprint 3 sprint 4 sprint 5


–0.5

Figure 2. Mean (and standard error) bias scores across the picture categories and sprints for (a) PPeak, (b) W, (c) HR and (d) AL.
*Significant (P < 0.05) difference between emotional bias scores. Negative bias score indicates a lower value for the emotional trial (pleasant
or unpleasant) compared with the neutral trial.

for their associated positive affective responses, performance. Indeed, in agreement with normative
which tended to increase the motivation to produce valence ratings (Lang et al., 2005), SAM results
greater exercise intensity. In contrast, negative indicated that, in comparison with neutral people
affects experienced during unpleasant trials tended pictures, sport and mutilation ones induced pleasant
to reduce the motivation to produce a high exercise and unpleasant emotion, respectively. Thus, emo-
intensity, which resulted in a decrease in tional pictures took an active part in modifying the
Interaction between emotion and power output 7

environmental affective content, likely explaining produced an exercise intensity lower in the unplea-
why power output during repeated sprints was sant context than in the pleasant one may be inter-
affected. preted as the effect of a regulatory process to
In the present study, emotional impact on power experience the exercise as less unpleasant, enabling
output could be explained by appetitive and defen- thus the affective load score to not increase.
sive motivational systems activated by the viewing of Accordingly, if this regulatory process were reliable,
pleasant and unpleasant pictures, respectively (Lang it would be then relevant during training sessions to
et al., 1997). Indeed, these motivational systems use a pleasant context to be able to produce high
were considered to mediate action in order to pro- exercise intensity.
mote individual survivals (Bradley, Codispoti, Moreover, emotional effects on performance were
Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001; Lang et al., 1997). A observed for the only 2 first sprints since emotional
large body of the literature demonstrated that moti- peak power and work bias scores were not signifi-
vational system activations gave rise to physiological cantly different from each other, from the third
changes, mediated by the somatic and autonomic sprint to the end of the exercise. These results sug-
nervous system (Bradley et al., 2001; Bradley, gested that emotional effects present at the begin-
Codispoti, & Lang, 2006). For example, while sitting ning of the exercise were only transient. During RSA
or standing, the passive viewing of highly unpleasant tests, fatigue manifests as a decrease in peak power
pictures (e.g. mutilation) led to a significantly larger or work, which generally occurs after the first sprint
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

cardiac deceleration compared with the view of plea- (Girard, Mendez-Villanueva, & Bishop, 2011;
sant or neutral pictures (Azevedo et al., 2005; Racinais et al., 2005). Even if emotional pictures
Bradley, 2009). According to these previous studies, affected the ability to repeat sprints, as power output
heart rate was lower in front of unpleasant pictures, was also affected during the second cycling bout, it
when compared with pleasant ones. This physiologi- could be assumed that the fatigue generated imme-
cal change is reminiscent of the “fear bradycardia” diately after the first sprint has interfered with image
and reflects a common adaptive defensive behaviour effect.
like the behavioural phenomenon of “freezing” when Power and work decrement values were greater in
a potential threat is imminent (Bradley, 2009; pleasant than in unpleasant trials. This result was in
Fanselow, 1994). Thus, the freezing-like behaviour agreement with previous studies (Billaut et al., 2011;
associated with highly unpleasant pictures could Racinais et al., 2005; Racinais, Perrey, Denis, &
explain the lower power output observed during Bishop, 2010) according to which decrement values
unpleasant trials, compared with pleasant ones, as would depend on the highest peak power or work
this behaviour was not compatible with the task values. The greater these values were, the greater
requirements, i.e. supra-maximal effort generating were the decrement values. In our study, the highest
over a 6-s sprint. peak power and work were obtained for pleasant
Previous studies showed a link between exercise trial, for which decrement values were the greatest.
intensity and affective responses (Kilpatrick et al., Thus, the greater decrement values observed during
2007; Smith & O’Connor, 2003; Tian & Smith, pleasant did not necessarily mean a greater fatigabil-
2011). While PA responses dominate during moder- ity but could simply be the consequence of a higher
ate exercise intensity, they decreased during high peak power or work (Racinais et al., 2010).
intensity exercises for the benefit of UA responses
(Kilpatrick et al., 2007; Tian & Smith, 2011). In the
Conclusion
present study, the results of affective load scores
were consistent with these previous findings. The results of the present study showed that emo-
Affective load scores were positive from the second tional pictures affected power output during a supra-
sprint regardless of the image category (Table I), maximal repetitive sprint test when the emotional
indicating the dominance of UA responses during context was changed. Peak power and work were
the supra-maximal task. higher for pleasant pictures compared with unplea-
However, although affective load in the unpleasant sant ones. As exercise intensity has been shown to
context was greater than in the pleasant one, this impact affect during exercise, the lower exercise
difference was not significant. This lack of signifi- intensity in the unpleasant session compared with
cance could stem from the fact that peak power and the pleasant session might be viewed as the effect
work were significantly lower in the unpleasant con- of a regulatory process aimed at making less unplea-
text, compared with the pleasant one. If peak power sant the exercise. If this regulatory process were
and work would have been similar for the pleasant relevant, the ability of participants to produce max-
and unpleasant sessions, affective load should have imal power output would thus depend on whether
been higher in the unpleasant context, because of the emotional context is pleasant or unpleasant.
higher exercise intensity. The fact that participants These findings highlighted that emotional context
8 L. Coudrat et al.

should be taken into account in training and compe- Fitzsimmons, M., Dawson, B., & Ward, D. (1993). Cycling and
tition in which maximal power output is required. running tests of repeated sprint ability. Australian Journal of
Science and Medicine in Sport, 25, 82–87.
Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: The role
of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral
Contributorship statement Neuroscience, 2, 52–63.
Gélat, T., Coudrat, L., & Le Pellec, A. (2011). Gait initiation is
Each author contributed to the conception and affected during emotional conflict. Neuroscience Letters, 497,
design, analysis and interpretation of data, the draft- 64–67.
Girard, O., Mendez-Villanueva, A., & Bishop, D. (2011).
ing of the article or revisiting it critically for impor-
Repeated-sprint ability-Part I. Factors contributing to fatigue.
tant intellectual content and approved the final Sports Medicine, 41, 673–694.
version to be published. Hillman, C. H., Rosengren, K. S., & Smith, D. P. (2004).
Emotion and motivated behavior: Postural adjustments to
affective picture viewing. Biological Psychology, 66, 51–62.
Kilpatrick, M., Kraemer, R., Bartholomew, J., Acevedo, E., &
References Jarreau, D. (2007). Affective responses to exercise are depen-
Azevedo, T. M., Volchan, E., Imbiriba, L. A., Rodrigues, E. C., dent on intensity rather than total work. Medicine & Science in
Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L. F., … Vargas, C. D. (2005). A Sports and Exercise, 39, 1417–1422.
freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology, Lambert, E. V., St Clair Gibson, A., & Noakes, T. D. (2005).
42, 255–260. Complex systems model of fatigue: Integrative homoeostatic
Baden, D. A., McLean, T. L., Tucker, R., Noakes, T. D., & St control of peripheral physiological systems during exercise in
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

Clair Gibson, A. (2005). Effect of anticipation during unknown humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 52–62.
or unexpected exercise duration on rating of perceived exer- Lane, A. M., Beedie, C. J., Jones, M. V., Uphill, M., &
tion, affect, and physiological function. British Journal of Sports Devonport, T. J. (2012). The BASES expert statement on
Medicine, 39, 742–746. emotion regulation in sport. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30,
Baron, B., Moullan, F., Deruelle, F., & Noakes, T. D. (2011). 1189–1195.
The role of emotions on pacing strategies and performance in Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral
middle and long duration sport events. British Journal of Sports assessment: Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H.
Medicine, 45, 511–517. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds.), Technology in mental health
Billaut, F., & Basset, F. A. (2007). Effect of different recovery care delivery systems (pp. 119–137). Ablex, NJ: Northwood.
patterns on repeated-sprint ability and neuromuscular Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion,
responses. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 905–913. attention and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–
Billaut, F., Bishop, D. J., Schaerz, S., & Noakes, T. D. (2011). 395.
Influence of knowledge of sprint number on pacing during Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated
repeated-sprint exercise. Medicine & Science in Sports & attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F.
Exercise, 43, 665–672. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting:
Bishop, D., Spencer, M., Duffield, R., & Lawrence, S. (2001). Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Hillsdale, NJ:
The validity of a repeated sprint ability test. Journal of Science Erlbaum.
and Medicine in Sport, 4, 19–29. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005).
Borg, G. A. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 14, 377–381. of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6.
Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: Orienting and Gainesville: University of Florida.
emotion. Psychophysiology, 46, 1–11. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O.
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and
(2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273.
reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298. Mendez-Villanueva, A., Hamer, P., & Bishop, D. (2007). Physical
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., & Lang, P. J. (2006). A multi- fitness and performance. Fatigue responses during repeated
process account of startle modulation during affective percep- sprints matched for initial mechanical output. Medicine &
tion. Psychophysiology, 43, 486–497. Science in Sports & Exercise, 39, 2219–2225.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic Naugle, K. M., Joyner, J., Hass, C. J., & Janelle, C. M. (2010).
evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach Emotional influences on locomotor behavior. Journal of
or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Biomechanics, 43, 3099–3103.
25, 215–224. Noakes, T. D., St Clair Gibson, A., & Lambert, E. V. (2005).
Coombes, S. A., Cauraugh, J. H., & Janelle, C. M. (2006). From catastrophe to complexity: A novel model of integrative
Emotion and movement: Activation of defensive circuitry alters central neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in
the magnitude of a sustained muscle contraction. Neuroscience humans: Summary and conclusions. British Journal of Sports
Letters, 396, 192–196. Medicine, 39, 120–124.
Coombes, S. A., Janelle, C. M., & Duley, A. R. (2005). Emotion Pereira, M. G., Volchan, E., De Souza, G. G. L., Oliveira,
and motor control: Movement attributes following affective L., Campagnoli, R. R., Pinheiro, W. M., & Pessoa,
picture processing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 425–436. L. (2006). Sustained and transient modulation of
Doré, E., Bedu, M., França, N. M., Diallo, O., Duché, P., & Van performance induced by emotional picture viewing. Emotion,
Praagh, E. (2000). Testing peak cycling performance: Effects of 6, 622–634.
braking force during growth. Medicine & Science in Sports & Racinais, S., Connes, P., Bishop, D., Blonc, S., & Hue, O.
Exercise, 32, 493–498. (2005). Morning versus evening power output and repeated-
Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defensive sprint ability. Chronobiology International, 22, 1029–1039.
behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin & Racinais, S., Perrey, S., Denis, R., & Bishop, D. (2010). Maximal
Review, 1, 429–438. power, but not fatigability, is greater during repeated sprints
Interaction between emotion and power output 9

performed in the afternoon. Chronobiology International, 27, skeletal muscle recruitment during exercise in humans. British
855–864. Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 797–806.
Smith, J. C., & O’Connor, P. J. (2003). Physical activity does not Stins, J. F., & Beek, P. J. (2011). Organization of voluntary step-
disturb the measurement of startle and corrugator responses dur- ping in response to emotion-inducing pictures. Gait & Posture,
ing affective picture viewing. Biological Psychology, 63, 293–310. 34, 164–168.
St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., Rauch, L. H. G., Tucker, R., Tian, Q., & Smith, J. C. (2011). Attentional bias to emotional
Baden, D. A., Foster, C., & Noakes, T. D. (2006). The role of stimuli is altered during moderate – but not high – intensity
information processing between the brain and peripheral phy- exercise. Emotion, 11, 1415–1424.
siological systems in pacing and perception of effort. Sports Vandewalle, H., Pérès, G., Heller, J., & Monod, H. (1985). All-
Medicine, 36, 705–722. out anaerobic capacity tests on cycle ergometers: A compara-
St Clair Gibson, A., & Noakes, T. D. (2004). Evidence for com- tive study on men and women. European Journal of Applied
plex system integration and dynamic neural regulation of Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 54, 222–229.
Downloaded by [laure Coudrat] at 08:32 04 February 2014

S-ar putea să vă placă și