Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

471717

al of Attention DisordersWilliamson and Johnston


© 2013 SAGE Publications

Reprints and permission:


JADXXX10.1177/1087054712471717Journ

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Articles
Journal of Attention Disorders

Marital and Coparenting Relationships:


XX(X) 1­–11
© 2013 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permission:

Associations With Parent and Child sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/1087054712471717
http://jad.sagepub.com
Symptoms of ADHD

David Williamson1 and Charlotte Johnston1

Abstract
Objective: To examine relations between symptoms of ADHD and reports of marital and coparenting functioning.
Method: Parents of 8- to 12-year-old boys with (n = 26) and without (n =38) ADHD participated. Results: Although
mothers of children with ADHD had the highest levels of ADHD symptoms, their symptoms were typically not related
to measures of the couples’ functioning, particularly when controlling for family ADHD and comorbidities. Father ADHD
symptoms were related to their reports of couple functioning, and fathers’ ADHD symptoms were associated with their
negative attributions for their wives’ behavior, even with child and mother ADHD controlled. However, when fathers’
depression and hostility symptoms were controlled, these were more important predictors of the attributions than ADHD
symptoms. Conclusion: It is important to consider parental levels of ADHD symptoms in the context of other family
members’ symptoms as well as other forms of psychopathology. (J. of Att. Dis. 2012; XX(X) 1-XX)

Keywords
ADHD, family, marital, coparenting

ADHD is highly heritable and persistent across the life span dissolution (Barkley, Murphy, & Fischer, 2008; Eakin et al.,
(Bidwell et al., 2011) and is associated with impairments in 2004; Minde et al., 2003). In some reports, the association
numerous domains of life functioning (Barkley & Murphy, between adult ADHD symptoms and marital dissatisfaction
2011), including interpersonal relations (Overbey, Snell, & appears to be independent of comorbidities (Eakin et al.,
Callis, 2011). Although previous studies have reported lower 2004); however, these are not always controlled.
marital satisfaction among adults with ADHD symptoms In addition to the possible contributions of comorbidities
(Eakin et al., 2004), the mechanisms that might underlie this to the association between adult ADHD and marital func-
marital dissatisfaction, such as relationship attributions, tioning, the influence of child ADHD also sometimes
have not been examined. Similarly, although a recent review remains uncontrolled. This omission raises concerns
(Johnston, Mash, Miller, & Ninowski, 2012) highlighted because we know that ADHD often affects both parents and
links between adult ADHD symptoms and parenting diffi- children within a family (Biederman, Faraone, Mick, &
culties, most previous research has focused on parenting at Spencer, 1995), and child ADHD behavior is a risk factor
the individual level. Less is known about how ADHD symp- for marital dissolution (Wymbs et al., 2008). In addition,
toms in adults may be related to mothers’ and fathers’ abili- there is considerable evidence supporting the transactional
ties to cooperate as a couple. In this study, we examine nature of the relationship between marital functioning and
ADHD symptoms in mothers and fathers in relation to the child problems including both ADHD and oppositional
couples’ marital satisfaction, attributions for each other’s behavior (Jenkins, Simpson, Dunn, Rasbash, & O’Connor,
behavior, and their reports of coparenting. We also examine 2005; Wymbs & Pelham, 2010). Thus, it is important to
these relations within the context of child ADHD symptoms assess the extent to which adult ADHD symptoms are
and both parent and child comorbidities. related to marital satisfaction independent of the impact of
ADHD symptoms have been associated with difficulties
1
in interpersonal relationships in adults (Babinski et al., University of British Columbia,Vancouver, Canada
2011; Das, Cherbuin, Butterworth, Anstey, & Easteal, 2012;
Corresponding Author:
Overbey et al., 2011). Regarding marital relationships, stud- David Williamson, Department of Psychology, University of British
ies have reported that adults with ADHD symptoms experi- Columbia, 2136 West Mall,Vancouver, British Columbia,V6T 1Z4, Canada
ence less marital satisfaction and elevated rates of marital Email: dkwilliamson@psych.ubc.ca

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


2 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

child ADHD symptoms. Similarly, it is important to control levels of ADHD symptoms have higher rates of comorbid
for child oppositional behavior, given its relations to both internalizing (e.g., anxiety, depression) and externalizing
marital satisfaction and child ADHD. Finally, given the (e.g., substance use, antisocial behavior) problems (Miller,
contextual nature of each partner’s experience of marital Nigg, & Faraone, 2007). Therefore, we tested whether the
functioning, we argue it is also important to account for the associations between parents’ ADHD symptoms and mari-
partner’s level of ADHD symptoms when examining cor- tal functioning and marital attributions were specific to
relations between adult ADHD symptoms and measures of ADHD symptoms or might be better accounted for by
marital functioning. comorbid symptoms of depression or hostility.
Most existing studies of adult ADHD symptoms and mari- In families, couples function not only as marital partners
tal distress have focused on adults with extreme or diagnostic but also as parents of offspring and must coordinate their
levels of ADHD symptoms (e.g., Eakin et al., 2004). parenting efforts. This ability to coparent is related to marital
However, given the recognized limitations to the diagnostic functioning and child behavior problems (Teubert &
criteria for adult ADHD (Barkley et al., 2008; Kessler et al., Pinquart, 2010). Parenting alliance, one aspect of coparent-
2010), it is possible that impairments in social functioning ing, reflects each parent’s views of the cooperation and feel-
may also appear at subdiagnostic levels of symptoms. We ings of mutual respect in the parenting team (Konold &
examine marital satisfaction as it relates to a dimensional Abidin, 2001). To extend our understanding of how adult
measure of ADHD symptoms reported by mothers and ADHD symptoms may be related to functioning within cou-
fathers of children with varying levels of ADHD symptoms ples, we included assessment of the coparenting alliance.
and oppositional behavior. We predicted that higher levels of Although couples’ childrearing disagreements have been
ADHD symptoms in either parent would be related to less linked to child problems in families of children with ADHD
marital satisfaction as reported by both parents, even account- (Johnston & Behrenz, 1993), the relation of coparenting to
ing for child ADHD. We also predicted that this relationship adult ADHD, to our knowledge, has not been examined.
would survive control for child oppositional behavior. Given that adult ADHD is associated with reduced marital
Moving beyond marital satisfaction, we know less about satisfaction and interpersonal communication deficits
the aspects of marital functioning that are linked to ADHD (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2006), we expected
symptoms. Studies of marital functioning consistently dem- that adult ADHD symptoms would be negatively associated
onstrate that attributions of blame or intent for negative with coparenting alliance. As with the marital measures, we
spouse behavior predict lower and less stable levels of satis- hypothesized that parenting alliance would be related to both
faction in the marriage (Bradbury, Fincham, & Beach, 2000). self and partner ADHD symptoms, and that these relations
Although various types of psychopathology within each would survive control for comorbid conditions.
spouse are associated with both attributions and marital sat- In summary, we examined marital satisfaction, marital
isfaction (Fincham & Bradbury, 2004), possible relations attributions, and parenting alliance as reported by both
between ADHD symptoms and marital satisfaction and attri- mothers and fathers of children with and without ADHD,
butions have not been examined. ADHD symptoms can be and predicted associations between these variables and the
hypothesized to influence marital attributions in at least two level of ADHD symptoms in each parent and their partner.
ways. First, ADHD symptoms may lead partners to give less We also examined whether these associations were unique
attention to situational influences on behavior, to expend to ADHD symptoms or were accounted for by comorbid
less effort when considering alternate causes of negative family problems.
events, and/or to have more impulsive reactions to the part-
ner’s negative behavior (Knouse et al., 2008), such that there
is a positive relationship between the perceiver’s ADHD Method
symptoms and negative attributions for the spouse. Participants
Alternately, it is also possible that ADHD symptoms in a
spouse result in more negative attributions for his or her Mothers and fathers of 64 boys aged 8 to 12 years with
behavior by the partner (Canu, Newman, Morrow, & Pope, (41%) and without (59%) ADHD participated in this study.
2008). For example, partners with ADHD symptoms may Participants were recruited from both community and clini-
act in ways that have negative consequences for their spouse, cal sources. All mothers, and 81% of fathers, were the bio-
and such behaviors may be interpreted as intentional. Thus, logical parents of their child. Demographic information can
we predicted that ADHD symptoms in mothers and fathers be found in Table 1. Child ADHD symptoms were assessed
would be related to more negative attributions, both for their with mother and other informant reports on the ADHD
partners and as made by their partners about them. Rating Scale–IV (ADHD-IV; DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos,
As noted above, marital functioning and attributions are & Reid, 1998). Other informants were the children’s teach-
related to child ADHD and oppositional behavior, and to ers (78%), coaches, day care workers, or relatives. The
other adult psychopathologies. In addition, adults with high ADHD-IV is an 18-item scale that reflects Diagnostic and

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


Williamson and Johnston 3

Table 1. Family, Child, and Parent Characteristics

Child has ADHD (n = 26) Control child (n = 38)

Variable % %
Maternal ethnicity
  Euro North American 65 55
 Asian 19 37
 Other 12 5
 Missing 4 3

  M (SD) M (SD)
Family SES 2.01 (0.77) 1.82 (0.72)
Number of years married 11.16 (9.59) 13.21 (5.71)
Child age in months 117.23 (14.38) 114.74 (11.98)
Comorbidities
  Child ODD 1.37 (.57) 0.69 (0.40)
  Mother Father Mother Father
a
Depressive symptoms 0.60 (0.63) 0.50 (0.68) 0.34 (0.42) 0.47 (0.50)
Hostility symptomsa 0.92 (0.48) 0.81 (0.60) 0.58 (0.42) 0.53 (0.45)
Note: SES = socioeconomic status; ODD = oppositional defiant disorder.
a
Brief Symptom Inventory: High scores indicate greater symptom endorsement.

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; Parent Measures


American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for inatten-
tive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms on a 4-point rat- Parent ADHD symptoms. Mothers and fathers each com-
ing scale from 0 (never or rarely) to 3 (very often). The pleted the Current Symptoms Scale (CSS; Barkley & Mur-
ADHD-IV has demonstrated good psychometric properties phy, 2006), an 18-item measure assessing adult ADHD
including a replicable factor structure, interrater reliability, symptoms on a scale ranging from 0 (never or rarely) to 3
and convergent validity (DuPaul et al., 1998). In this study, (very often). The CSS has shown good reliability (Gomez,
the internal consistency on the Inattention and Hyperactive- 2011; Ladner, Schulenberg, Smith, & Dunaway, 2011), con-
Impulsive subscales were, respectively, .80 and .87 for struct validity (Gomez, 2011; Murphy & Barkley, 1996),
mothers, and .91 and .91 for other informants. and convergent validity (Ladner et al., 2011). Parent ADHD
Children were considered to have ADHD if they had symptoms were calculated as the average level of endorse-
been diagnosed by a health professional, symptoms were ment across all items. Internal consistencies were .93 and
present prior to age 7, and either the mother or the infor- .89 for mothers and fathers, respectively.
mant rated the child as 2 or 3 for at least six inattentive or Relationship quality. The short form of the Dyadic Adjust-
hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. In addition, either the ment Scale (DAS-7; Hunsley, Best, Lefebvre, & Vito, 2001;
mother or teacher must have rated the child as impaired by Spanier, 1976) was administered to each parent to measure
his symptoms. Of the 26 boys with ADHD, 16 (62%) were marital satisfaction. The DAS-7 has seven items, and the
taking medication at the time of the study. Parents were total score averaged across items rated from 0 to 5 or 6 was
asked to complete all measures of child behavior, thinking used. The DAS-7 total score has shown good criterion
of the child as off medication. validity, convergent validity, and consistently high internal
The Total Problems Score of the Strengths and Difficulties consistency (Hunsley et al., 2001). In this study, mothers’
Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 2001) was used to ensure ratings of marital adjustment were internally consistent, α =
that the control children did not have high levels of behavior .83, as were fathers’ α = .81.
problems. The SDQ has good psychometric properties Spousal attributions. The Relationship Attribution Mea-
(Goodman, 2001), and in this study, α = .89 for mothers and sure (RAM; Fincham & Bradbury, 1992) was administered
α = .85 for fathers. All children in the control group scored to mothers and fathers to assess attributions spouses make
below the 90th percentile on the Total Problems Score for their partner’s behavior. The RAM has six scenarios, four
(youthinmind, 2004). Children with pervasive developmen- of which are negative and two of which are positive. For
tal disorders were excluded from both groups. each scenario, participants use a 6-point scale (1 = strongly

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


4 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

agree to 6 = strongly disagree) to rate the cause of the behav- Procedure


ior on six items that correspond to the attributional dimen-
sions of locus, stability, globality, intentionality, selfish This study was conducted in a research lab at the University
motivation, and whether the behavior is blameworthy. Only of British Columbia and was approved by our university’s
the four negative questions are utilized in scoring. The RAM ethics board. After families indicated interest in participat-
has shown adequate test–retest reliability, as well as good ing, the study was described and informed consents were
internal consistency (Tonizzo, Howells, Day, Reidpath, & obtained. Mothers and fathers completed questionnaires
Froyland, 2000) and convergent and criterion validity separately, with mothers completing the measures during a
(Fincham & Bradbury, 1992). In this study, RAM scores lab visit with their son, and fathers completing the mea-
were averaged within and across scenarios. Internal consis- sures at home and returning them by mail.1 Parents received
tency was α = .94 for mothers and α = .91 for fathers. an honorarium, and each child received a t-shirt.
Although higher ratings indicate less negative attributions,
for ease of interpretation, inversed scores were used such that
higher RAM scores indicate more negative attributions. Results
Parenting alliance. Each parent completed the Parenting A total of 65 two-parent families completed all questionnaires
Alliance Measure (PAM; Abidin & Konold, 1999) reporting required to be included for analysis. During preliminary data
their cohesiveness as a parenting team. The PAM has analysis, an outlier was discovered. This mother responded
20-items rated on a 5-point scale from strongly agree to inconsistently across and within questionnaires. In addition,
strongly disagree. The PAM has high internal consistency, she rated herself very low on acculturation to Canadian cul-
test–retest reliability, concurrent validity, criterion validity ture. This family’s data were omitted from analysis, resulting
(Abidin & Konold, 1999), and construct validity (Abidin & in 64 families. As a subset of participants had missing data on
Konold, 1999; Hughes, Gordon, & Gaertner, 2004). In addi- one or more variables, we used multiple imputation in all
tion, the factor structure of the PAM indicates that it is a analyses. A total of 20 imputations were conducted for each
valid measure for use with both mothers and fathers (Konold analysis, and each data set was analyzed separately; the
& Abidin, 2001). In this study, the internal consistency was results were then combined using the procedures outlined in
α = .95 for maternal and α = .94 for paternal parenting alli- Barnard and Rubin (1999). This procedure allows the full use
ance. PAM scores were calculated by averaging across the of all available data without limiting inferences to those par-
items on the scale, and higher scores indicate greater ticipants with complete data. In addition, due to several
alliance. assumption violations, all reported statistics were boot-
Parental comorbidity. Both parents completed the Hostil- strapped, and bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) boot-
ity and Depression subscales of the Brief Symptom Inven- strapped confidence intervals were calculated via resampling.
tory (BSI; Derogatis, 1993). The BSI is a self-report
measure of psychological symptoms rated on a 5-point
scale (0 = not at all to 4 = extremely). The Depression sub- Correlations Among Variables
scale consists of six items, and the Hostility subscale has Bivariate correlations among variables can be found in
five items. The BSI has demonstrated good psychometric Table 2. As expected, DAS and PAM scores correlated
properties (Boulet & Boss, 1991; Derogatis & Melisaratos, positively with each other, and RAM scores generally cor-
1983), including validity when used in families of children related negatively with DAS and PAM scores. In addition,
with ADHD (Seipp & Johnston, 2005). Scores were calcu- the comorbidities correlated with DAS, PAM, and RAM
lated by averaging items on each subscale. Internal consis- scores in the expected directions. Looking specifically at
tencies for mothers’ ratings were α = .71 for hostility and parental ADHD symptoms, for mothers, there was one mar-
α = .87 for depression. For fathers, α = .76 for hostility and ginal association of ADHD symptoms with lower PAM
α = .87 for depression. scores. For fathers, their ADHD symptoms were associated
Child comorbidity. Child oppositional behavior was with less marital satisfaction (a marginal association with
assessed on the Oppositional Defiant Disorder Rating Scale mother reports and a significant association with father
(ODDRS; Hommersen, Murray, Ohan, & Johnston, 2006). reports), and with lower PAM and higher RAM scores.
The ODDRS is an eight-item scale keyed to DSM-IV criteria
with items ranged from 0 (not at all) to 3 (very much). The
ODDRS has demonstrated high internal consistency and Families of Children With and
interrater reliability as well as good test–retest reliability and Without ADHD
convergent validity (Hommersen et al., 2006). The internal Comparing families of children with and without ADHD
consistencies for mother and father ratings on the scale were on demographic variables and child comorbidities, only
both .91. Scores for each child were calculated as averages child oppositional symptoms differed significantly, with
across items, and across mother and father ratings. parents of children with ADHD rating their children

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


Williamson and Johnston 5

Table 2. Correlations Between Predictor and Criterion and Between Covariate and Criterion Variables

Child Mother Father

  ODD DAS PAM RAM Depression Hostility DAS PAM RAM Depression Hostility
Mother
 DAS −.15 1.00 −.24+ −.12 .40* .29* −.09 −.28* −.26*
 PAM −.40* .74* 1.00 −.29* −.15 .43* .53* .02 −.24* −.32*
 RAM .07 −.51* −.45* 1.00 .31* .09 −.28* −.24* −.10 .16 .16
 ADHD .22+ −.02 −.24+ .16 .24+ .41* −.16 −.17 −.01 .18 .28*
Father
 DAS −.30* .40* .43* −.28* −.09 −.02 1.00 −.31* −.32*
 PAM −.33* .29* .53* −.24* −.19 −.08 .70* 1.00 −.40* −.37*
 RAM .14 −.09 .02 −.10 −.22 −.10 −.42* −.30* 1.00 .40* .27*
 ADHD .08 −.23+ −.20 .11 .03 .09 −.25* −.30* .34* .64* .52*
Note: ODD = oppositional defiant disorder; DAS = Dyadic Adjustment Scale; PAM = Parenting Alliance Measure; RAM = Relationship Attribution Measure.
+
.05 < p < .10. *p < .05.

Table 3. Parental ADHD Symptoms and Relationship Variables as a Function of Child ADHD

Child has ADHD (n = 26) Control child (n = 38)

  M (SD) M (SD)

Variable Mother Father Mother Father


ADHD symptoms 0.98 (0.64) 0.63 (0.52) 0.42 (0.30) 0.48 (0.36)
Marital adjustmenta 3.28 (0.78) 3.03 (1.18) 3.58 (0.84) 3.42 (0.71)
Parenting allianceb 3.88 (0.65) 4.04 (0.94) 4.37 (0.55) 4.22 (0.59)
Spousal attributionsc 3.48 (0.77) 3.79 (1.59) 3.35 (0.90) 3.37 (0.76)
a
Dyadic Adjustment Scale: Higher scores indicate greater marital adjustment.
b
Parenting Alliance Measure: Higher scores indicate greater coparenting alliance.
c
Relationship Attribution Measure: Higher scores indicate more negative relationship attributions.

higher, t(62) = 5.58, p < .001, 95% confidence interval = mothers of children without ADHD. Fathers of children with
[.44, .93], d = 1.38 (see Table 1).Within-between ANOVA ADHD did not differ significantly from fathers of children
compared parental depressive and hostility symptoms across without ADHD in their reports of ADHD symptoms. In
families of children with and without ADHD and across addition, among parents of children with ADHD, mothers
mothers and fathers within families. Hostility symptoms self-reported significantly more ADHD symptoms than
were significantly higher in parents of boys with ADHD, fathers. However, among parents of children without ADHD,
F(1, 62) = 12.81, p < .001, η2 = .09 (see Table 1), and this mothers and fathers did not significantly differ in their self-
effect remained significant even when child ODD symptoms reports of ADHD symptoms (see Table 3). The interaction
were covaried (p = .011). No other effects were significant. between child ADHD status and parent sex remained signifi-
To test whether parent and child ADHD symptoms were cant even when child ODD symptoms, parental depression,
associated, an ANOVA was conducted comparing parental and parental hostility were covaried (p < .001).
ADHD symptoms across families of children with and Finally, ANOVAs were conducted comparing the rela-
without ADHD and within mothers and fathers (see Table 3). tionship variables across families of children with and with-
Consistent with our hypotheses, parents of sons with out ADHD and within mothers and fathers (see Table 3).
ADHD were significantly more likely to report ADHD Although we expected parents of children with ADHD to
symptoms of their own. This main effect was qualified by differ on all of these measures, the only significant differ-
a significant interaction between child ADHD status and ence was on the PAM; parents of sons with ADHD reported
parent sex, F(1, 62) = 11.55, p = .001, η2 = .07. Post hoc lower parenting alliance than parents of control children,
analyses indicated that mothers of children with ADHD F(1, 62) = 9.71, p = .003, η2 = .10. When child ODD was
self-reported significantly more ADHD symptoms than covaried, this difference was no longer significant (p = .102).

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


6 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

Table 4. ADHD Symptoms Predicting Marital and Coparenting Variables in Mothers and Fathers

RAM PAM

  Mothers Fathers Mothers Fathers

  β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI


Child ADHD −.01 0.07 .945 [−.24, .29] .14 −0.88 .385 [−.34, .14] −.34 −2.21 .031 [.03, .64] −.24 −1.55 .127 [−.06, .55]
Mother ADHD .14 1.14 .259 [−.08, .42] −.14 −0.96 .340 [−.36, .15] −.04 −0.21 .835 [−.42, .30] .003 0.02 .986 [−.31, .31]
Father ADHD .09 0.66 .515 [−.18, .34] .36 3.05 .003 [.10, .45] −.15 −1.17 .245 [−.40, .11] −.27 −1.81 .075 [−.56, .04]
Note: RAM = Relationship Attribution Measure; PAM = Parenting Alliance Measure; CI = confidence interval.
Child ADHD: 0 = control child, 1 = child with ADHD.

Table 5. Psychopathology Predicting Negative Relationship Attributions and Parenting Alliance in Mothers and Fathers
RAM PAM

  Mother Father Mother Father

  β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI β t p 95% CI

Child ODD .07 0.44 .659 [−.23, .42] .13 0.75 .459 [−.18, .53] −.30 −1.94 .056 [−.59, .003] −.30 −1.61 .112 [−.63, .09]
Mother depression .32 2.51 .015 [.10, .62] −.26 −1.98 .052 [−.53, −.004] −.24 −1.50 .140 [−.62, .04] −.16 −1.27 .207 [−.46, .05]
Mother hostility −.02 −0.10 .918 [−.35, .27] −.01 −0.03 .973 [−.31, .31] .04 0.29 .772 [−.25, .34] .03 0.29 .777 [−.23, .26]
Father depression .09 0.44 .661 [−.29, .52] .37 2.32 .023 [.04, .64] −.18 −1.09 .279 [−.51, .13] −.35 −1.76 .082 [−.72, .05]
Father hostility .12 0.65 .521 [−.26, .47] −.06 −0.36 .717 [−.42, .21] −.15 −0.89 .375 [−.47, .19] −.12 −0.63 .534 [−.51, .27]
Child ADHD −.11 0.61 .545 [−.23, .48] .14 −0.92 .362 [−.44, .17] −.12 0.68 .497 [−.24, .48] −.05 0.24 .808 [−.32, .42]
Mother ADHD .08 0.56 .581 [−.23, .37] −.12 −0.75 .459 [−.46, .18] −.01 −0.04 .968 [−.44, .35] .04 0.24 .812 [−.27, .37]
Father ADHD −.02 −0.11 .909 [−.42, .39] .14 0.83 .411 [−.22, .45] .04 0.25 .805 [−.29, .31] .01 0.08 .933 [−.29, .42]

Note: RAM = Relationship Attribution Measure; PAM = Parenting Alliance Measure; CI = confidence interval; ODD = oppositional defiant disorder.

Parent and Child ADHD Symptoms attributions either individually or together, despite our
as Predictors of Marital Adjustment, expectation of positive associations, R2adj = −.02, F(3, 61) =
.65, p = .585 (see Table 4). However, as expected, paternal
Relationship Attributions, and Coparenting
negative relationship attributions were significantly pre-
Regressions were conducted to assess the relationships dicted by the full model, R2adj = .10, F(3, 61) = 3.21, p =
between parent ADHD symptoms and measures of marital .029, as well as by paternal ADHD symptoms in particular
functioning and coparenting. Analyses were conducted (see Table 4). When other parent and child psychological
separately for mothers’ and fathers’ DAS, RAM, and PAM symptoms were included in the regression, the overall
scores as dependent variables. At Step 1, mother and father model remained significant, R2adj = .17, F(8, 55) = 2.57, p =
ADHD symptoms and child ADHD status were entered, .019, but now both maternal and paternal depression sig-
and at Step 2, an interaction term between mother and nificantly predicted paternal relationship attributions, and
father ADHD symptoms was added. This interaction term paternal ADHD symptoms were no longer significant. This
was not significant in any model and is not discussed fur- suggests that depressive symptoms account for much of the
ther. Models that significantly predicted marital and copar- relationship between paternal ADHD symptoms and nega-
enting variables were reanalyzed: covarying child ODD tive relationship attributions (see Table 5).
symptoms, parental depression, and parental hostility. For Similar regressions were calculated with family ADHD
marital adjustment, contrary to our expectations of a nega- symptoms predicting parenting alliance. The model was
tive relationship between dyadic adjustment and parent significant for maternal reports of parenting alliance, R2adj =
ADHD symptoms, the overall models were not significant .13, F(3, 61) = 4.09, p = .010, with child ADHD status as
for mothers, R2adj = .04, F(3, 61) = 1.95, p = .132, or fathers, the only significant predictor (see Table 4). This model
R2adj = .03, F(3, 61) = 1.72, p = .173. remained significant when other parent and child psycho-
For the RAM, parent and child ADHD symptoms did logical symptoms were included, R2adj = .21, F(8, 55) =
not significantly predict maternal negative relationship 3.14, p = .005. However, child ADHD status was no longer

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


Williamson and Johnston 7

a significant predictor, and child ODD symptoms became mother DAS reports, but these relations were reduced when
marginally significant (see Table 5), suggesting that ODD placed in the context of mother and child ADHD symptoms,
symptoms are largely responsible for the relationship suggesting that the relationship between a father’s ADHD
between child ADHD and parenting alliance. Contrary to symptoms and marital satisfaction may be explained by
predictions, parents’ own ADHD symptoms did not signifi- ADHD symptoms in other members of the family. The dif-
cantly predict mothers’ reports of parenting alliance in the ferences between our results and previous findings may be
regression analysis. However, consistent with our predic- due to our control of other family ADHD symptoms, as well
tion, the results for fathers’ reports of parenting alliance as due to our focus on mothers and fathers separately.
were significant, R2adj = .10, F(3, 61) = 3.36, p = .024, and Consistent with the results for marital satisfaction, mother
father ADHD symptoms were marginally predictive of ADHD symptoms did not significantly predict mother-
paternal parenting alliance (see Table 4). When other family reported relationship attributions, nor the attributions her
psychological symptoms were included into the equation, partner made for her. Father ADHD symptoms did predict
the overall model remained significant, R2adj = .21, F(8, 55) his own negative relationship attributions about his wife,
= 3.16, p = .005. But again, paternal ADHD symptoms were and this effect survived the inclusion of other family mem-
no longer a predictor, and paternal depressive symptoms bers’ ADHD symptoms but not other comorbidities. For the
instead became marginally significant (see Table 5), sug- coparenting relationship, both mother and father ADHD
gesting that parental psychological symptoms, and paternal symptoms related to their own reports of difficulties in this
depression in particular, are largely driving the relationship alliance, but these relationships disappeared in the context
between paternal ADHD and paternal parenting alliance. of other family members’ ADHD symptoms, particularly
child ADHD status. These results are consistent with previ-
ous findings that child ADHD symptoms contribute to
Discussion interparental conflict (Wymbs & Pelham, 2010).
Contrary to expectations, our results indicated that adult Consistent with previous research, ADHD symptoms
ADHD symptoms were rarely related to measures of family were associated with both depression and hostility for
functioning. For the relations that did exist, including mothers and fathers, and child ADHD was significantly
ADHD or comorbid psychological symptoms of child or associated with ODD symptoms (Johnston et al., 2012;
parents, reduced the associations between parental ADHD Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman, & Meltzer, 2004).
and family functioning. The reasons why our primary These comorbid conditions in both parents and children
hypotheses were not fully supported are difficult to deter- were correlated with family functioning in the directions
mine. Perhaps the parents with high levels of ADHD symp- that would be expected, and when entered into the regres-
toms in our sample had developed compensatory strategies sion equations, it was often these comorbid conditions that
that reduced the impact of their symptoms, or had strategi- accounted for the relationships that had previously been
cally selected partners who were tolerant of ADHD symp- attributed to ADHD symptoms. For example, the effect of
toms, or the influence of child ADHD symptoms in other child ADHD status on maternal parenting alliance became
family members affected the impact of each parent’s symp- nonsignificant when child ODD symptoms were included
toms. Although our results regarding adult ADHD symp- and depressive symptoms seemed to be driving the rela-
toms and family functioning are unexpected and null when tionships between parent ADHD symptoms and negative
examined in the context of family members’ psychological marital attributions. Together, these results suggest that
symptoms, the intercorrelations among measures of family ADHD-specific behaviors may not be the most strongly
functioning and the bivariate associations between parent predictive of impairments in marital and parental function-
ADHD symptoms and functioning were often consistent ing, but, instead, that comorbid parental and child psycho-
with our expectations. In particular, intercorrelations pathologies are most important.
between mother and father reports of marital adjustment, Several important sex differences were observed in our
relationship attributions, and parenting alliance were gener- study. Among parents of children with ADHD, mothers
ally significant in the expected directions. reported, on average, higher levels of ADHD symptoms
With regard to the links between adult ADHD symptoms than fathers, although there were more significant correla-
and family functioning, previous research has found that tions between father ADHD symptoms and measures of
spousal and self-reports of dyadic adjustment are lower in family functioning. Using norms for adult ADHD symp-
adults with ADHD compared with control adults (Eakin toms provided by Barkley (2011), no more than 16% of
et al., 2004), even with child ADHD status controlled parents in control families and fathers in ADHD families
(Minde et al., 2003). In contrast to these results, we found scored above the 90th percentile. However, 38% of moth-
that maternal ADHD symptoms were not related to self- or ers in the ADHD group reported symptoms above the 90th
partner reports of dyadic adjustment. There were small cor- percentile. In short, although mothers reported more
relations of father ADHD symptoms with both father and ADHD symptoms than fathers, fathers appeared to be more

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


8 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

affected by their ADHD symptoms. Eakin and colleagues these long-acting medications altered our pattern of results,
(2004) speculated that husbands may be more upset by suggesting that the influence of recall bias is minimal.
their wives’ ADHD symptoms than the reverse; however, Although we do not have measures of current or past
this is contrary to our findings, which generally do not sup- treatment for marital difficulties, scores for the families in
port links between adult ADHD symptoms and spousal our sample were often below clinical cutoffs on measures of
reports of family functioning. However, looking at other family functioning. For example, the DAS scores were con-
psychological symptoms, we did find some cross-partner sistent with those from previous studies of adults with
effects. For example, although mothers’ depression was ADHD (Eakin et al., 2004), but predictably higher than
related to their own but not their husbands’, perceptions of what has been found in samples of adults with more severe
family functioning, fathers’ depression, and hostility were psychological difficulties (Hunsley et al., 2001). Scores on
related to their own and their wives’ reports. These rela- the RAM in our sample were similar to scores from typical
tions are generally consistent with previous results show- couples in previous studies (e.g., McNulty, O’Hara, &
ing that depression and anxiety are associated with both Karney, 2008). Similarly, 20% of mothers and 44% of
self- and spousal reports of marital adjustment (Whisman, fathers of children with ADHD scored below the 20th per-
Uebelacker, & Weinstock, 2004). Sex differences were centile on the measure of parenting alliance in comparison
also found in the relations within measures of family func- with 5% of mothers and 18% of fathers of control children
tioning. Wives’ negative attributions about husbands were (Abidin & Konold, 1999). Overall, although the families in
related to the husbands’ reports of dyadic adjustment and our sample likely overestimate the level of functioning in
parenting alliance, but husbands’ attributions about their many families of children with ADHD, perhaps due to cur-
wives were not related to any of the wives’ reports of fam- rent and past treatment, they were still significantly more
ily functioning. This is consistent with previous research impaired than controls in parenting alliance, and although
finding that wives’ attributions are more related to marital not significantly different, mean levels on the DAS and
satisfaction than husbands’ attributions (Bradbury, Beach, RAM also suggested greater difficulty in families with
Fincham, & Nelson, 1996; Sanford, 2005). Our results sug- ADHD compared with controls.
gest that, compared with men, women’s experiences of
their families are more strongly related to their partner’s
views and to psychological symptoms. Limitations
In interpreting our findings, it is important to consider Our study is limited by several factors. First, the relatively
the nature of the sample. Although not referred for clinical small sample size limits our power to detect relationships
services, the families in our study are representative of fam- among the variables. This is evidenced by a number of
ilies who seek treatment. All children in the ADHD group bivariate correlations and betas that are in the expected
had been diagnosed by a mental health professional, and direction, but they fail to reach traditional levels of signifi-
most had sought clinical services. For example, 63% of cance. Our study was powered to determine medium to
children with ADHD were taking medication, and more large effects, and although it is true that smaller effects may
than half of their parents reported using specialized parent- be obscured by our sample size, effects of such a size are
ing techniques such as reward system charts. However, the unlikely to be clinically significant. Nevertheless, our con-
sample is restricted to two-parent families where both par- clusions are tentative and require replication and elabora-
ents were willing and able to participate in research. Such tion. In addition, although one outlier was removed as there
families may be better functioning than many families with was an identifiable reason for the abnormal responses, two
a child with ADHD, including single-parent families. other outliers were maintained in the analysis as there were
Furthermore, it is possible that families’ treatment experi- no apparent reasons for their extreme responses. However,
ence may have limited our ability to find significant effects we note that our use of bootstrapping and multiple imputa-
between groups. This concern was alleviated somewhat by tion would minimize the effect of these outliers and that
the fact that rerunning the analyses controlling for child results remained the same even under the constraints of
medication status and use of behavior therapy did not alter robust regression. Nevertheless, a larger data set would
our pattern of results. However, further research would be reduce the impact of such outliers and add confidence to the
necessary to examine the possible influence of parental obtained results.
treatments. Our instruction to parents to imagine their child Our study is also limited by the uncertainty associated
off of medication when completing questionnaires also with the measurement of ADHD symptoms in adults.
introduces the possibility of recall bias. However, more DSM-IV criteria were written for children, and it is currently
than 80% of our families with children with ADHD consis- unknown the extent to which these criteria are applicable to
tently interacted with their child when he or she was not adults (Barkley et al., 2008). In this study, we relied on a
medicated. Neither excluding families whose children were dimensional approach to measuring adult ADHD; however, it
never off of medication nor controlling for the presence of is possible that different results might be obtained with a

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


Williamson and Johnston 9

diagnostic approach. Similarly, it is possible that ADHD present, the clinician should consider treating comorbid
symptoms as currently written function differently in adult psychological symptoms instead of or in addition to the
men compared with adult women. Finally, in this sample, ADHD symptoms. Our results suggest that comorbid
parents’ scores on the Inattention and Hyperactivity/ depressive and hostility symptoms are related to coparent-
Impulsivity subscales of the CSS were highly correlated, ing, marital attributions, and marital satisfaction, and it
r(62) = .74 and r(62) = .60 for mothers and fathers, respec- would be beneficial for future research to explore how
tively, and so were collapsed into a total score in our analyses other kinds of psychopathology (e.g., parental anxiety)
to avoid difficulties in interpretation of highly colinear pre- may be related to measures of family functioning.
dictors and to minimize the number of variables considered
in the regressions. It will be important for future research to
consider the role that the two symptom dimensions may play Conclusion
in family functioning. Among the children with ADHD, 77% This is the first study to investigate marital attributions and
were rated highly on both inattentive and hyperactive symp- coparenting as they relate to ADHD symptoms in adults in
toms by either their mother or another informant; the remain- a larger context that included child ADHD and family
ing children were split equally between predominantly comorbidities. Results highlighted the importance of con-
inattentive and predominantly hyperactive/impulsive symp- sidering ADHD symptoms in the broader family milieu, as
tom endorsement. Although this distribution of child ADHD well as the value of including both mothers and fathers
suggests that the effect of child subtype would be minor, future when investigating how ADHD is related to family mem-
studies are needed to more fully examine this question. bers’ perceptions of one another.
It is likely that genetic effects are contributing to our
results. In particular, our finding that parents of children Acknowledgment
with ADHD are more likely to have ADHD symptoms is We thank the families who generously gave their time and our lab
likely reflective of a genetic relationship. Specifically, our colleagues who offered comments.
results suggest that mothers’ ADHD symptoms might have
a stronger relationship to their child’s ADHD than fathers’ Declaration of Conflicting Interests
ADHD symptoms, although the presence of more nonbio- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
logical fathers than mothers in this sample limits the respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
strength of this interpretation. In addition, the specificity of article.
any genetic effects is difficult to determine due to the sig-
nificant relations among comorbid conditions and family Funding
functioning. Future research using genetically informed The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial sup-
designs would be useful in sorting out how genetic and port for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
environmental factors work together within families with article: This research was supported by a grant from the Social
members with ADHD. Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to the
second author.

Clinical Implications Note


It is important to consider both parent and child ADHD 1.  The data used for this study are a subset of a larger study of
symptoms in the context of other psychological difficulties. parent and child relationship attributions.
We found that when ADHD symptoms appear to be related
to important domains of family functioning, other psycho- References
logical difficulties often contribute to or were wholly Abidin, R. R., & Konold, T. (1999). Parenting alliance measure:
responsible for the relation with ADHD. Furthermore, any Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
one family member’s ADHD symptoms need to be under- Resources.
stood in the context of other family members’ ADHD and American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
comorbid psychological symptoms. Parents’ perceptions of tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
family functioning are often meaningfully affected by the Author.
symptoms of other family members, and what appears to be Babinski, D. E., Pelham, W. E., Jr., Molina, B. S. G., Gnagy, E. M.,
the effect of one person’s ADHD may actually reflect a Waschbusch, D. A., Yu, J., & Karch, K. M. (2011). Late adoles-
combination of their own and other family members’ ADHD cent and young adult outcomes of girls diagnosed with ADHD
and comorbid symptoms. As a result, in a clinical setting, it in childhood: An exploratory investigation. Journal of Atten-
is important to not assume that the presence of ADHD tion Disorders, 15, 204-214. doi:10.1177/1087054710361586
symptoms in a parent necessarily implies difficulties in Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV
marital or family relationships, and if such difficulties are (BAARS-IV). New York, NY: Guilford.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


10 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006). Eakin, L., Minde, K., Hechtman, L., Ochs, E., Krane, E., Bouffard, R.,
Young adult outcome of hyperactive children: Adaptive & Looper, K. (2004). The marital and family functioning of
functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American adults with ADHD and their spouses. Journal of Attention Dis-
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 192-202. orders, 8, 1-10.
doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2 Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1992). Assessing attribu-
Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (2006). Attention-deficit hyper- tions in marriage: The Relationship Attribution Measure.
activity disorder: A clinical workbook (3rd ed.). New York, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 457-468.
NY: Guilford. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.3.457
Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (2011). The nature of executive Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (2004). Marital satisfaction,
function (EF) deficits in daily life activities in adults with depression, and attributions: A longitudinal analysis. In
ADHD and their relationship to performance on EF tests. R. M. Kowalski & M. R. Leary (Eds.), The interface of
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, social and clinical psychology: Key readings (pp. 129-146).
137-158. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Fischer, M. (2008). ADHD in Gomez, R. (2011). Item response theory analysis of adult self-
adults: What the science says. New York, NY: Guilford. ratings of the ADHD symptoms in the Current Symptoms
Barnard, J., & Rubin, D. B. (1999). Small sample degrees of free- Scale. Assessment, 18, 476-486.
dom with multiple imputations. Biometrika, 86, 948-955. Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths
Bidwell, L. C., Willcutt, E. G., McQueen, M. B., DeFries, J. C., and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Acad-
Olson, R. K., Smith, S. D., & Pennington, B. F. (2011). A family emy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337-1345.
based association study of DRD4, DAT1, and 5HTT and contin- Hommersen, P., Murray, C., Ohan, J. L., & Johnston, C. (2006).
uous traits of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Oppositional Defiant Disorder Rating Scale: Preliminary evi-
Genetics, 41, 165-174. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9437-y dence of reliability and validity. Journal of Emotional and
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Mick, E., & Spencer, T. (1995). High Behavioral Disorders, 14, 118-125.
risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children Hughes, F. M., Gordon, K. C., & Gaertner, L. (2004). Predict-
of parents with childhood onset of the disorder: A pilot study. ing spouses’ perception of their parenting alliance. Journal of
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 431-435. Marriage and Family, 66, 506-514.
Boulet, J., & Boss, M. W. (1991). Reliability and validity of the Hunsley, J., Best, M., Lefebvre, M., & Vito, D. (2001). The seven-
Brief Symptom Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Jour- item short form of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale: Further
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 433-437. evidence for construct validity. American Journal of Family
Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R. H., Fincham, F. D., & Nelson, G. N. Therapy, 29, 325-335.
(1996). Attributions and behavior in functional and dysfunc- Jenkins, J., Simpson, A., Dunn, J., Rasbash, J., & O’Connor, T. G.
tional marriages. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- (2005). Mutual influence of marital conflict and children’s
ogy, 64, 569-576. behavior problems: Shared and nonshared family risks. Child
Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (2000). Development, 76, 24-39. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00827.x
Research on the nature and determinants of marital satisfac- Johnston, C., & Behrenz, K. (1993). Childrearing discussions in
tion: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, families of nonproblem children and ADHD children with
62, 964-980. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00964.x higher and lower levels of aggressive-defiant behavior. Cana-
Canu, W. H., Newman, M. L., Morrow, T. L., & Pope, D. L. W. dian Journal of School Psychology, 9, 53-65. doi:10.1177/
(2008). Social appraisal of adult ADHD: Stigma and influences 082957358500900106
of the Beholder’s Big Five personality traits. Journal of Atten- Johnston, C., Mash, E. J., Miller, N., & Ninowski, J. E. (2012).
tion Disorders, 11, 700-710. doi:10.1177/1087054707305090 Parenting in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., & Easteal, S. der (ADHD). Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 215-228.
(2012). A population-based study of attention deficit/hyperac- Kessler, R. C., Green, J. G., Adler, L. A., Barkley, R. A., Chatterji, S.,
tivity disorder symptoms and associated impairment in mid- Faraone, S. V., & Van Brunt, D. L. (2010). Structure and diag-
dle-aged adults. PLoS One, 7, e31500. doi:10.1371/journal. nosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Analysis
pone.0031500 of expanded symptom criteria from the adult ADHD clinical
Derogatis, L. R. (1993). BSI Brief Symptom Inventory. Administra- diagnostic scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 1168-1178.
tion, scoring, and procedures manual (4th ed.). Minneapolis, Knouse, L. E., Mitchell, J. T., Brown, L. H., Silvia, P. J., Kane, M. J.,
MN: National Computer Systems. Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2008). The expression of
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The Brief Symptom adult ADHD symptoms in daily life: An application of experi-
Inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, ence sampling methodology. Journal of Attention Disorders,
13, 595-605. 11, 652-663. doi:10.1177/1087054707299411
DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. R., & Reid, R. (1998). Konold, T. R., & Abidin, R. R. (2001). Parenting alliance: A
ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and interpretation. multifactor perspective. Assessment, 8, 47-65. doi:10.1177/
New York, NY: Guilford. 107319110100800105

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015


Williamson and Johnston 11

Ladner, J. M., Schulenberg, S. E., Smith, C. V., & Dunaway, M. H. Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales
(2011). Assessing AD/HD in college students: Psychometric for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Jour-
properties of the Barkley self-report form. Measurement and nal of Marriage and Family, 38, 15-28.
Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 44, 215-224. Teubert, D., & Pinquart, M. (2010). The association between copa-
Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Messer, J., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. renting and child adjustment: A meta-analysis. Parenting: Sci-
(2004). Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in ence and Practice, 10, 286-307. doi:10.1080/15295192.2010
a national sample: Developmental epidemiology. Journal of .492040
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 609-621. Tonizzo, S., Howells, K., Day, A., Reidpath, D., & Froyland, I.
McNulty, J. K., O’Hara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008). Benevo- (2000). Attributions of negative partner behavior by men who
lent cognitions as a strategy of relationship maintenance: physically abuse their partners. Journal of Family Violence,
“Don’t sweat the small stuff” . . . But it is not all small stuff. 15, 155-167.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 631-646. Whisman, M. A., Uebelacker, L. A., & Weinstock, L. M. (2004).
Miller, T. W., Nigg, J. T., & Faraone, S. V. (2007). Axis I and II Psychopathology and marital satisfaction: The importance of
comorbidity in adults with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psy- evaluating both partners. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
chology, 116, 519-528. Psychology, 72, 830-838.
Minde, K., Eakin, L., Hechtman, L., Ochs, E., Bouffard, R., Wymbs, B. T., & Pelham, W. E., Jr. (2010). Child effects on
Greenfield, B., & Looper, K. (2003). The psychosocial func- communication between parents of youth with and without
tioning of children and spouses of adults with ADHD. Journal attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 637-646. Psychology, 119, 366-375. doi:10.1037/a0019034
Murphy, K., & Barkley, R. A. (1996). Prevalence of DSM-IV Wymbs, B. T., Pelham, W. E., Jr., Molina, B. S. G., Gnagy, E. M.,
symptoms of ADHD symptoms in adult licensed drivers: Wilson, T. K., & Greenhouse, J. B. (2008). Rate and predic-
Implications for clinical diagnosis. Journal of Attention Dis- tors of divorce among parents of youths with ADHD. Jour-
orders, 1, 147-161. nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 735-744.
Overbey, G. A., Snell, W. E., Jr., & Callis, K. E. (2011). Subclini- doi:10.1037/a0012719
cal ADHD, stress, and coping in romantic relationships of youthinmind. (2004, January 30). Normative SDQ data from the
university students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 67-78. USA. Retrieved from http://www.sdqinfo.com/USNorm.html
doi:10.1177/1087054709347257
Sanford, K. (2005). Attributions and anger in early marriage: Bios
Wives are event-dependent and husbands are schematic. Jour- David Williamson is a graduate student pursuing an MA in psy-
nal of Family Psychology, 19, 180-188. chology at the University of British Columbia.
Seipp, C. A., & Johnston, C. (2005). Mother-son interactions in
families of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder Charlotte Johnston is a professor in clinical psychology at the
with and without oppositional behaviour. Journal of Abnormal University of British Columbia. Her research focuses on adult and
Child Psychology, 33, 87-98. child ADHD.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at Northeastern University on November 14, 2015

S-ar putea să vă placă și