Sunteți pe pagina 1din 128

VOLUME 79

JCD
JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT
NUMBER 3 SUMMER 2001

PRACTICE & THEORY


259 Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and Failures: Eight Personal Perspectives
Stephen G. Weinrach, Albert Ellis, Catharine MacLaren, Raymond DiGiuseppe,
Ann Vernon, Janet Wolfe, Ruth Malkinson, and Wouter Backx

269 Metaphor and Change in Counseling


William J. Lyddon, Alison L. Clay, and Cheri L. Sparks

275 FOCUS on the Family Caregiver: A Problem-Solving Training Intervention


Monica F. Kurylo, Timothy R. Elliott, and Richard M. Shewchuk

282 Where Has Our Theory Gone? Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention
Sterling Gerber

RESEARCH
292 Clinical Judgment in Case Conceptualization and Treatment Planning Across Mental
Health Disciplines
Janet E. Falvey

304 Counselors’ Attribution of Responsibility, Etiology, and Counseling Strategy


Jerry L. Kernes and J. Jeffries McWhirter

314 Risk Factors for Suicidality Among Clients With Schizophrenia


Robert C. Schwartz and Benjamin N. Cohen

320 Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful: A Statewide Study of the Impact
of Comprehensive Guidance and Counseling Programs
Richard T. Lapan, Norman C. Gysbers, and Gregory F. Petroski
331 High School Students’ Career-Related Decision-Making Difficulties
Itamar Gati and Noa Saka

341 Assessing the Impact of a Prejudice Prevention Project


Michael Salzman and Michael D’Andrea

347 Training AIDS and Anger Prevention Social Skills in At-Risk Adolescents
Melbourne F. Hovell, Elaine J. Blumberg, Sandy Liles, Linda Powell, Theodore C. Morrison,
Gabriela Duran, Carol L. Sipan, Susan Burkham, and Norma Kelley

356 Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity


Hardin L. K. Coleman, Sherry B. Casali, and Bruce E. Wampold

365 Rational Suicide: An Empirical Investigation of Counselor Attitudes


James R. Rogers, Christine M. Gueulette, Jodi Abbey-Hines, Jolynn V. Carney, and
James L. Werth, Jr.

373 The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents


Linda L. Viney, Rachael M. Henry, and Joanne Campbell

TRENDS
382 Finding Happiness for Ourselves and Our Clients
Geri Miller

© 2001 by the American Counseling Association. All rights reserved.


PRACTICE & THEORY
Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and
Failures: Eight Personal Perspectives

Stephen G. Weinrach, Albert Ellis, Catharine MacLaren, Raymond DiGiuseppe, Ann Vernon, Janet
Wolfe, Ruth Malkinson, and Wouter Backx

Eight experts in Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) provided personal examples of their own successes and failures in
applying REBT to themselves. The experts actively talked to themselves both rationally and irrationally. Understandably, there
were far more shoulds, oughts, musts, and have to’s in the narratives in which the experts described when they failed to use REBT
than when they succeeded in using REBT. Rational self-talk was more prevalent in the examples of how REBT was successfully
used by the experts.

T
o its followers, Rational Emotive Behavior The Committee comprised experts in REBT from around
Therapy (REBT) is an excellent theory of men- the world. The committee, of which REBT’s founder Albert
tal health and philosophy of life. If anyone could Ellis is a member, has an international membership with repre-
be expected to have mastered its finer nuances, sentatives from Australia, Germany, Israel, the Netherlands, and
and thus be less prone to overreacting emo- the United States. Former members have come from Canada,
tionally or behaviorally, it would be REBT experts. Would England, and France. The committee serves several functions,
it not be reasonable to expect REBT experts to “practice including the approval of REBT training programs around the
what they preach?” Yet common sense dictates that even world. Current and past committee members are among the
experts are unlikely to be exempt from overreacting emo- world’s experts on REBT.
tionally or behaviorally some of the time. After all, even Specifically, each committee member was asked to do the
experts are fallible human beings. The purpose of this following:
project was to provide a panel of REBT experts with the
opportunity to reveal where they succeeded and where they Describe in 400 words or less, one example of where you have
failed at applying REBT to themselves. successfully invoked REBT in your own life. Include, where
applicable, rational and irrational self-talk. Label your feelings.
Indicate how long it took you to be successful. Indicate the
GUIDELINES extent to which your use of REBT in this situation has been
generalized to similar situations (elegant solution). [The term
In April 1999, the 11 members of the Albert Ellis Institute’s “elegant solution” refers to the search for profound philosophic
International Training Standards Committee were invited to changes in the client’s thinking.] If you had help in applying
write two essays each. In the first essay, committee members REBT to yourself (as opposed to using REBT on yourself with-
out seeking help), so indicate. What did the helper do that you
were asked to provide a personal example of how they had
found helpful. Please be very specific. Do not write in general
been successful in using REBT on themselves. In the second terms about how REBT has changed your life.
essay, committee members were asked to provide an example
of how they failed in using REBT on themselves. and,

Stephen G. Weinrach is a professor of counseling and human relations at Villanova University, Villanova, Pennsylvania, and a Fellow of the Albert Ellis
Institute in New York. Albert Ellis is the founder and president, Catharine MacLaren is a training and special projects coordinator, and Janet Wolfe is the
executive director, all at the Albert Ellis Institute in New York. Raymond DiGiuseppe is a professor of psychology at St. John’s University, Jamaica, New York.
Ann Vernon is the graduate coordinator for the School Counseling Program at the University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls. Ruth Malkinson is a counselor
at the Israeli Center for REBT, Rehovot, Israel. Wouter Backx is a counselor at the Institute voor RET, Haarlem, Netherlands. Correspondence regarding this
article should be sent to Stephen G. Weinrach, Department of Education and Human Services, Villanova University, 800 Lancaster Avenue, Villanova, PA
19085-1699 (e-mail: stephen.weinrach@villanova.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 259


We i n r a c h , E l l i s , M a c L a r e n , D i G i u s e p p e , Ve r n o n , Wo l f e , M a l k i n s o n , a n d B a c k x

Describe in 400 words or less, one example of where you tried but and Wolfe succeeded in using REBT in the area of uncondi-
were unsuccessful in a timely manner to invoke REBT in your own tional other-acceptance, yet Malkinson and Weinrach failed
life. Include, where applicable, rational and irrational self-talk. La-
bel your feelings. Fully describe the dysfunctional or irrational be-
in managing their issues of unconditional self-acceptance.
havior. Indicate the obstacles that you encountered in using REBT. DiGiuseppe cited low frustration tolerance (LFT) as an
What were you continually telling yourself? If you sought help in example of how he succeeded (and also failed), whereas
applying REBT to yourself (as opposed to using REBT on yourself Ellis and Wolfe cited LFT as an area where they both failed.
without seeking help), so indicate. How come counseling didn’t Backx’s narrative about how he was successful in applying
help, either? Please be very specific. Do not write in general terms.
REBT to himself was not classified because of its general
Between May and December, 8 committee members gen- nature. The assignment of narratives into specific themes
erated 15 responses. Three committee members chose not was not precise because some of the narratives could have
to participate. Most essays were returned to their respec- been assigned to more than one theme. The classification
tive authors with requests for revisions by the first author, of narratives by theme was made primarily on the basis of
who also served as compiler and editor. All essays were copy self-talk as opposed to the experts’ description of the sur-
edited so that there was some semblance of uniformity and rounding circumstances.
consistency across essays. Some key words, phrases, and con- Walen, DiGiuseppe, and Dryden (1992) subsumed Ellis’s
cepts, commonly used by REBT counselors have been itali- irrational beliefs into four categories, which they referred
cized so as to sensitize readers to rational self-talk as well as to as “core irrational beliefs.” The categories are “demands,
irrational self-talk. Word limits were not strictly enforced. awfulizing, low frustration tolerance, and global evaluations
The order of authorship, and therefore the order in which of human worth” (p. 17). This four-category model was
the responses are presented, as follows, was based on the applied only to the narratives in which the experts described
order in which the first drafts were received. In the past, their failures at applying REBT to themselves. (It was not
then current members of the committee have participated applied to the narratives wherein the experts succeeded
in writing two other articles (Weinrach, 1996; Weinrach et because there was considerably less evidence of irrational
al., 1995). This is the third such article. thinking in these examples.) Two of the experts (Weinrach
and Malkinson) dealt primarily with issues of global evalu-
REVEALED PERSPECTIVES: COMMONALITIES AND DIFFERENCES ations of human worth, and three of the experts (Ellis,
DiGiuseppe, and Wolfe) experienced LFT, primarily.
There is considerable emotional energy found in many of MacLaren’s narrative about depression was not classified
the narratives. These are not the narratives of individuals because its origin seems to have fallen primarily outside of
with very low or flat affect. To the contrary, some of the REBT’s sphere. It serves as a cogent reminder that relying
experts displayed considerable anger and frustration in those solely on one approach, in this case REBT, may not always
areas where they failed to apply REBT to themselves as be in the client’s best interest.
well as satisfaction and contentment where they succeeded. Finally, more than one core irrational belief was found
The experts used many of the same words and phrases within most of the narratives, which is often the case with
common to REBT. They actively talked to themselves both clients. For example, Weinrach dealt with both global evalu-
rationally and irrationally. It is interesting that none cited verba- ations of (his) human worth and the demand that his col-
tim, or even close to verbatim, the well-known irrational league offer greater respect for the importance of his work.
beliefs that are found in most standard counseling theory Ellis displayed classic LFT and demanded that the airline
textbooks. An example is “It is a dire necessity for adult hu- treat him better. DiGiuseppe experienced LFT (by display-
mans to be loved or approved by virtually every significant ing an intolerance for discomfort) and demanded that he
other person in their community” (Ellis, 1994, p. xx). Rather, should not have to use an appointment book. Vernon de-
the experts, who presumably have been practicing REBT on manded a certainty of the outcome of her husband’s sur-
themselves for many years, have adapted the standard irratio- gery as well as “awfulized” about what life would be like
nal beliefs to fit their own idiosyncratic belief systems or just for her son if her husband were to die. Wolfe displayed
shortened them for the sake of convenience. Understandably, LFT along with global evaluations of human worth of her
there were far more shoulds, oughts, musts and have to’s in the employees; Malkinson dealt with global evaluation of (her)
narratives in which the experts described when they failed to human worth and demanded that she be treated better.
use REBT than in those describing when they succeeded. Ra-
tional self-talk was more prevalent in the examples of how SUCCESSES IN APPLYING REBT
the experts succeeded in using REBT—the type of rational
self-talk that contributes to positive counseling outcomes. In the following section, the experts provided examples of
There are several themes that are found in both the nar- how they succeeded in applying REBT to themselves.
ratives in which the experts succeeded in applying REBT
to themselves and those in which they failed. One expert’s Anxiety Reduction (Stephen G. Weinrach)
area of success was often another expert’s area of failure. In August of 1998 I had coronary bypass surgery. It is com-
For example, Weinrach succeeded in using REBT to man- mon for bypass surgery patients to complain more about
age his anxiety, whereas Vernon failed to manage hers. Ellis all of the tubes in their mouth and nose than about pain

260 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and Failures

from the surgery itself. The tubes and oxygen mask are tests were far more painful than anything I had experi-
suffocating. Although the surgery went well, there were enced previously and at times I would cry afterwards. In
serious problems with my respiration immediately there- all three situations, I was successful in calming myself
after. Pulmonary therapists have this funny habit of want- down to the point where I was merely appropriately ap-
ing their patients to breathe on their own, and I was not prehensive. I was able to tell myself “I may not like it, but
doing a very good job of it. At one point, my pulmonary I can stand it. I have stood it before so I know that I can
therapist, whom I shall call Debbie, after trying to moti- stand it again.”
vate me to breathe more independently, calmly informed
me that if my output did not improve significantly within Unconditional Other-Acceptance (Albert Ellis)
the next hour, she would have no choice but to insert a
nasotracheal tube. The tube is inserted in one’s nose and I used to be quite angry at people who acted stupidly or
wends its way down to one’s lungs. Once in place, mu- immorally and at horrible world conditions. My use of REBT
cous is suctioned out of the lungs. on myself, when I started to apply it to others in January
I fully understood Debbie’s directions about inhaling and 1955, convinced me that my angry feelings were largely
exhaling, and I was willing but unable to comply. There created by my absolutistic demands that people must treat
was a cognitive disconnect between comprehending and me fairly and considerately and were rotten people when
acting. For example, I was unable to imitate someone show- they did not. I taught many of my clients unconditional
ing me how to cough. Debbie, in her own charming and other-acceptance (UOA), to accept the sinner and not his
nonthreatening way, calmly made it clear to me that I was or her sins, and thereby refuse to feel angry.
not following her directions. I fully understood the impli- I got a good chance to put my unconditional other-
cations of her threat to insert the tube—something I did acceptance into practice in 1993, when I was celebrating
not want to take place. I was not particularly frightened my 80th birthday. For several years before that, I had
that I was not breathing adequately because I was unaware been collaborating on a Dictionary of Sexological Terms
of its serious implications. I was frightened that inserting with B., with whom I had maintained a close friendship
the tube would be very painful. since our college days. I gave him considerable material
As the hour elapsed, I found myself remarkably and un- that I had already gathered for the Dictionary, and he
characteristically calm about the prospect of the tube be- promised to complete it in, at most, a year and by doing
ing inserted. I was alert enough to talk to myself rationally. I so get some credit on his PhD in philology. B. did little
told myself that “I was powerless to change whatever the work on the Dictionary, kept lying to me and our pub-
ultimate outcome was” (tube or no tube) and that “were lisher, and completed the work he had to do on his PhD.
the tube to be inserted, it would take only a minute or two The publisher rightly canceled the contract for the book
and that I could stand a lot of pain for that short amount of after 3 years of B.’s abysmal procrastination; the Dictio-
time. That was the worst thing that could realistically hap- nary was never published.
pen.” The prospect of dying had never occurred to me. Under ordinary conditions, before I began to use REBT
Throughout the hour, Debbie coached and encouraged me on myself, I would have been incensed for several reasons:
to inhale and exhale to no avail. At the appointed hour, she (a) B. was slothfully procrastinating, (b) he kept lying about
told me to swallow once she got the tube halfway down. I the work he was doing, (c) he had me write a special letter
relaxed, swallowed, and the tube was painlessly inserted. I to his Philology Department saying that he was working
was very excited that I could invoke REBT under the cir- hard on the Dictionary, and (d) I had agreed to forgive him
cumstances. Its use helped me handle a situation that I had $8,000 he owed me when he would finish his work on the
found very threatening. At the time, I remember being more Dictionary. But after the publisher canceled our contract,
elated that I could rely on invoking REBT while lying in he invented an outrageous lie to the effect that I had for-
the cardiothoracic intensive care unit than I was upset with given the debt just for his starting to work on the Dictio-
the necessity to insert the tube. I may have forgotten how nary. So he canceled the debt.
to breathe, but I did not forget my REBT. Instead of making myself incensed at B., I used REBT to make
A month after my initial discharge from the hospital I myself feel very displeased with his behavior but not angry
was readmitted for 2 more weeks, during which time I with him. How? By telling myself, “Too bad that B. has lied,
had two additional surgeries and the temporary insertion cheated, and procrastinated, but that rotten behavior doesn’t
of a port in my arm so that intravenous antibiotics could make him a bad person.” I no longer viewed him as a close
be self-administered 3 times a day for the next 45 days at friend but was not angry at him. I invited him to my 80th
home. For this procedure, I was not sedated and was even birthday party in 1993, and we talked in a pleasant manner,
more anxious than before, with some justification. Then, reminisced about some of our mutual experiences over the
every few weeks, I had to have blood tests to determine if years, and semihumorously mentioned our monetary differ-
the antibiotics were damaging my liver. The blood needed ences. No, no feelings of anger, rancor, or resentment on my
to be drawn from the top of my hands because I had an part. Using REBT and its philosophy and practice on myself, I
artery removed from my left arm for the bypass surgery accepted B. with his poor behavior and I rarely think any more
and I had the temporary port in my right arm. These blood of the highly immoral way he treated me.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 261


We i n r a c h , E l l i s , M a c L a r e n , D i G i u s e p p e , Ve r n o n , Wo l f e , M a l k i n s o n , a n d B a c k x

Former “Love Slob” (Catharine MacLaren) Low Frustration Tolerance (Raymond DiGiuseppe)
This is the confession of a former love slob. I used to be- Presently I have two young children, ages 20 and 36 months,
lieve that I could feel positive about myself only if almost and many writing obligations. Since the birth of these chil-
everyone else on the planet approved of me. It did not matter dren, I almost never have enough time to do my profes-
what it cost me. I now know that attempting to attain that sional writing activities. Almost every time I sit down to
goal is not only exhausting but impossible and only leads to write, I am distracted by one or both of them, or I have
frustration, resentment, and poor self-care. Unfortunately, family activities that preclude my writing. But I remind
it took me a little time and a lot of REBT to figure that out. myself that “things don’t have to go my way, I don’t always
In my late adolescence and early 20s, I noticed that I was get what I want, and I can tolerate not having my own way.”
often trying to “fix” things for people so that they would Now I can be pretty happy and content and not upset.
not have to deal with them. If I was incapable of “making it I did, however, spend about 6 months being upset, vacil-
better,” or, worse yet, happened to do something that con- lating between being angry and depressed. When I felt angry,
tributed to someone’s stress (even if it was in my best inter- I was telling myself that “I should be able to do whatever I
est), then I would berate myself for days or months and feel want to do, and my wife should do the child care. It’s unfair
miserable and guilty. Phrases like I’m a terrible friend (per- to me and I’ve got to have my way.” And I felt depressed when
son) and I don’t deserve to (fill in the blank) would flow I thought, “It’s awful that I’ll never get what I want done.
through my mind endlessly. Child care is an unfair restriction on my personal choice.
In my mid-20s, I started to practice REBT. I would listen Everyone will forget who I am because I’m not publishing
to clients at the beginning of my Fellowship at the Albert anything any more, and I’ll just be a mediocre nobody.”
Ellis Institute and think, “that sounds a little bit like me.” I disputed these beliefs by telling myself “I don’t have to
Then one night in group, after a member had been lament- have what I want! There may be other things I can enjoy.
ing the sacrifices she was making for a friend, Al Ellis turned There’s more than one way to enjoy your life, and there’s no
to her and said, “You’re a love slob of the worst sort and one absolute way you have to live your life. If I never have a
you’d better strongly work to get over it and start accepting professional publication again, I’ll still enjoy Thomas and
you for you instead of for what you can do for other people.” Anna. And I guess I won’t be a mediocre nobody.”
Something clicked for me.
I poured over all the information I could find on love Regret (Ann Vernon)
slobbism, guilt, and global self-rating. I cut out particularly
relevant small sections of resources and pasted them into I employed REBT effectively in a situation that occurred
my planner as daily reminders that looking out for myself this past year. My 21-year-old niece has a very serious case
was acceptable and only made me self-interested, not self- of Crohn’s Disease and was hospitalized at the Mayo Clinic
ish. I monitored my constant internal monologue by set- for more than 3 months, during which time she had several
ting up cues at home and in my office. When I noticed operations, including a colostomy. While she was in the hos-
these cues (small familiar articles placed in innocuous pital, my family and I visited several times, called regularly,
places), I stopped myself immediately and “rewound” the and sent cards and gifts to show our support.
tape in my head to pinpoint unhelpful thinking. About 4 months after the colostomy, my niece returned
Once I identified the negative messages I was sending to college, and we began to exchange e-mail. After my ini-
myself (without really being mindful of them), I disputed tial inquiry about how she was feeling and adjusting to be-
them with REBT self-help forms and reviewed the forms ev- ing back in school, I shared my thoughts about all the
ery day. I developed and consistently practiced coping state- changes she had been through recently and specifically asked
ments like I am allowed to say “No” and It doesn’t make me a how she was adjusting to the colostomy. I asked this ques-
bad person if I can’t accommodate everyone. I deliberately tion out of concern, assuming that there would be signifi-
pushed my comfort limits in terms of asking for things from cant physical as well as psychological ramifications.
others to prove that I could do it and still maintain my rela- The next e-mail was from my sister who was furious that
tionships with them. Slowly, over a period of several months I had asked such a direct question. She adamantly stated
and with lots of hard work, I began to experience a shift and that my niece wanted no contact with me for a long time
feel more comfortable asking for what I wanted (and did not and that what I had done was extremely insensitive and
want) instead of trying to provide others with what I thought outrageous. I was dumbfounded and hurt by her response,
they wanted. My confidence in myself increased dramati- but I did not take the attack personally. I told myself that
cally and my relationships with others improved. I am still a they were overreacting because they had been through so
work in progress and have weak moments when I despair at much with this illness and that I was probably a scapegoat
disapproval from certain people, but I am quick to take ac- for their anger about the entire situation.
tion against that process and remind myself that others will I e-mailed an apology, expressing that I had not inten-
inevitably disapprove of me some of the time and that is O.K. tionally been insensitive but in fact felt that I was being
I now have significantly more energy to devote to other, more thoughtful by expressing concern about an adjustment that
productive endeavors. could be very difficult for a 21-year-old. I also shared that I

262 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and Failures

had asked similar questions of two clients who had had the friends, which has meant that my friendships have deep-
same operation and that they had appreciated such a direct ened and grown rather than tapered off. And I also have a
inquiry because it helped them talk honestly about the tremendous amount of time alone in the beautiful space I
issues. My sister’s response to this message was that I must have created because my partner conveniently spends most
be a terrible therapist. of his waking hours in his office. And because he recog-
Once again, I did not take this personally. I was very dis- nizes our home as largely my space (because he mainly sleeps
appointed by her comments but had not intended to be in- or grabs a quick meal there), I am probably one of the few
sensitive and realized I could not control their reaction. I women who has been able to hang 40 yards of Victorian
knew I was not a terrible therapist, nor was I a rotten person lace around her bed without a peep of objection from her
for making a comment that was misinterpreted. I did not mate! During the 10–15 minutes a day we spend in the
get angry, thinking that my sister shouldn’t react as she did, same space we giggle, mime, occasionally discuss profes-
even though I would have preferred a more mature response sional matters, or say nothing at all, and generally have a
without her personal attacks. By thinking rationally, I was fine time.
able to stop this from escalating into a bigger argument. My success in this area is not completely due to my hav-
Because I stayed calm, I could assertively request that she ing reframed something in my head from awful to highly
accept my apology and that we put this issue behind us so advantageous; it is also because I discovered empirically that
as not to jeopardize our relationship. Although I do not the way relationships are traditionally supposed to be is, in
think I made a mistake by asking such a direct question, I the last analysis, really not the kind of relationship that is
am not perfect and certainly had no way of predicting their best for my own growth and happiness. However, were it
response. I can regret my behavior but not beat myself up not for my having done a lot of disputing, I probably would
about it, thanks to REBT. not have stayed around long enough to find this out.

Unconditional Other-Acceptance (Janet Wolfe) Anxiety Reduction and Shame Attacking (Ruth Malkinson)
Having been brought up with the quaint notion that when Two years ago, I was on my way to a memorial service for
I got mated, I would have someone with whom to walk on my parents. I went with my son who drove our car, and we
moonlit beaches, travel, and do other activities, it was quite stopped at a nearby plant nursery to get some flowers and
jolting for me to realize at age 25 that I had cast my lot plants. The nursery was located next to a gas station where
with a “workaphile” with just about zero desire for socializ- my son checked the air in the car’s tires. On my way back
ing and who is too busy for nature, culture that does not to the car, holding the plants in my hands, I slipped on an
come piped in through the radio or television, or other oil spill and fell into a ditch. I was in great pain. My son,
things most so-called normal couples do together. In fact, who saw me approaching the car and falling, ran quickly to
he and his former wife separated for that very reason, he help but realized that I was badly hurt. Although I was in
pointed out before I moved in with him some 30 years ago. great pain, I kept telling myself that I could stand the pain
For the first few years—although happy when in his com- and that it was not awful.
pany and generally pleased with a lot of his behavior (for I was hospitalized with a broken elbow and had to un-
example, his tremendous encouragement of my personal dergo surgery. During those few days in the hospital I could
and career goals and his utter lack of male chauvinism)—I not move, felt miserable, and was in great pain. Also, for the
would periodically find myself in tears because every time first time in my life I was going to have surgery under gen-
I wanted to travel or do something with him, I would wind eral anesthesia, of which I was terribly afraid. I heard my-
up having to recruit someone else or go by myself. I believe self saying that I couldn’t stand the pain and I was miserable
that my mate should want to spend time with me, and it is for having to go through the discomfort of being in bed
really awful that he does not (and, if he does not, I must not with a broken elbow. I immediately told myself that I could
be very worthwhile). Although busy with my own work, in- stand the pain, in spite of the discomfort of being in bed
terests and friends, I would periodically feel depressed, self- and unable to function. I found that rational self-talk was
pitying, and angry and perorate at him for being so selfish very helpful in dealing with my pain and discomfort.
and unloving (to which he replied, to my further furor, But the great moment of which I knew very little was yet
“that’s the way I am—now what are you telling yourself to to be experienced. I was unaware of the effect of the anes-
get yourself so upset?”). thesia, especially its aftereffect. I remember hearing some-
Many years later, I find myself with just the opposite feel- one call my name over and over again and not understand-
ings. Barely a day goes by that I do not congratulate myself ing why I was being shouted at. I felt very “frightened and
for really having the best of both worlds, the single and the angry” with whomever it was who was shouting at me. I
mated. As an independent agent, I can go where I want, remember refusing to respond to the voice that called my
when I want, and with whom I want (with no flack from name. Shortly afterward, I opened my eyes and felt a com-
my mate, because he is happily working away in his own forting touch—my daughter was standing next to me and
space). Unlike most women who are more traditionally explaining that the nurse had been rather harsh in awaken-
coupled, I spend nice chunks of prime time with close ing me. I felt very embarrassed for what seemed to me a

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 263


We i n r a c h , E l l i s , M a c L a r e n , D i G i u s e p p e , Ve r n o n , Wo l f e , M a l k i n s o n , a n d B a c k x

noncompliant behavior on my part and proceeded to put FAILURES IN APPLYING REBT


myself down for behaving childishly. But not for very long,
as I told myself that I have never before experienced any- In the following section, the experts provided examples of
thing similar to that and there is nothing wrong in reacting how they failed at applying REBT to themselves.
the way I did. Also, my anger toward the nurse subsided
when I reminded myself that she was only doing her job, Need for Unconditional Self-Acceptance
even though it was not in a particularly nice way. “It is defi- (Stephen G. Weinrach)
nitely not the end of the world,” I told myself. I was able to
accept myself as a fallible human being. The individual faculty teaching load, where I work, is largely
controlled by the recommendations of a faculty committee to
the dean. The standard teaching load is four courses per
Self-Help Group (Wouter Backx)
semester. For many years I have been granted a one-course-
Some 8 years ago, two very good friends of my partner and per-semester reduction in load because of my scholarly
me wanted to have some psychological support but did not productivity. In 1997, I decided to apply for a two-course-
know how to proceed. When we discussed their problems, per-semester reduction in teaching load, which is rarely granted.
it became clear that their problems were rather similar to At the time, I had just completed an article titled “Publication
those that my partner and I sometimes experienced. Some Patterns of the Personnel and Guidance Journal/Journal
of the difficulties my female partner had and the woman in of Counseling and Development—1978 to 1993” (Weinrach,
the other couple had were particularly similar, which was Lustig, Chan, & Thomas, 1998). My productivity not only
quite understandable because they are sisters. placed me in the top 1% of contributors for the 15-year
We started to explore solutions very tentatively. We came period of the study, but it had even increased appreciably
up with the idea of forming a self-help group. We wondered between 1993 and 1997.
how to create an egalitarian group even though, initially, I My application to the committee included evidence of
was the one who had the expertise in REBT. The four of us past performance and an outline of what I planned to do in
developed a set of rules whose purpose was to prevent us the future. It is impossible to reconstruct all of the discus-
from becoming a group of three clients around one thera- sion that took place at my meeting with the committee,
pist—me. I was convinced that the other three group mem- and I was not privy to their private deliberations. However,
bers would be able to learn enough REBT to make it pos- while discussing my level of scholarly output, one member
sible to have three (nonprofessionally trained) therapists of the committee, after I referred to my being in the top
working with one client, every time. Even better yet would 1% as measured by the study I had conducted, moved his
be three clients working on the problem of a fourth client. thumb and index finger a few inches apart and stated: “But
We aspired to replicate the traditional model of REBT groups at Villanova University, your performance is only average.”
in which members help each other. Aside from the statement being patently inaccurate, I was
The beginning was difficult because we did not know irrationally angry that this committee member disparaged
what would happen and how it would go. We made some my work, and indirectly me. It reflected a condescending
mistakes like first having a nice dinner together followed attitude, not uncommon among some faculty toward col-
by our group session. That did not work. It was better to leagues in a different department.
separate our social activities strictly from the therapy. For about a year, I spent an inordinate amount of time
But at some point the group really began to click. All unsuccessfully trying to dispute my irrational anger at the
four of us brought to the group our problems with our person who made this statement. I was far more angry at
respective partner or more often with somebody or the inaccurate characterization of my life’s work than I was
something outside the group. In the beginning, every- upset about not having my teaching load further reduced. I
body took turns, and we came together on a regular basis. know that my worth is unrelated to my productivity. I know
But later, when we were more used to it, one person that his saying something does not make it so. I know that life
could just ask for a session with the group for some is not fair. I know that I was acting like a big baby—demand-
problem that was bothering him or her. As a result we ing that others think highly of me and my work (approval).
began to meet less often. I know. I know. I know. But for the longest time I continued
Our group has been functioning this way for the past 7 to ruminate about it just the same.
years. It has effectively helped to resolve personal prob- Then one day my anger mysteriously disappeared. I freely
lems and marital crises. As a group, we have become very tell myself now that “people make inaccurate and insensi-
sophisticated in applying REBT. As such, the group has tive statements all of the time, and it is up to me whether I
become a very safe place to bring in my problems and work choose to dwell on them or not.” I do not believe my being
on them, a luxury therapists do not always have. The group able to let go was primarily a function of my finally using
has also served to prevent problems before they arise. By REBT on myself (at least at any conscious level) because
applying what we do in the group in problematic situa- my anger vanished weeks before my rational self-talk
tions we are able to reduce much of the unnecessary suffer- emerged. Even if REBT could be appropriately credited with
ing we would experience otherwise. my finally letting go, letting go took a very long time, which

264 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and Failures

when I think about it, is not all that uncommon for me. was the hotel third-rate, but about a hundred small ants
Maybe had I sought help from an REBT counselor, I would kept falling on the floor of my bathroom and I spent a good
have been able to let go sooner. A case could be made that deal of the night killing them.
I really have not yet fully let go because I am publicly bring- Fortunately, I called the people for whom I was giving
ing attention to this matter, which could conceivably cause the workshops in Colorado Springs; they got on the phone
the object of my discomfort some discomfort himself. The to the airlines, and I was able to get the 6:30 AM flight for
issue that I have presented is not a rare event; there are Denver and then on to Colorado Springs. With only about
other unresolved examples that, if space permitted, I could 3 hours of sleep on Thursday night, I arrived at my confer-
have used. ence at 10:30 AM. I still missed some of my workshop time,
but at least I gave most of my presentations.
Low Frustration Tolerance (Albert Ellis) To make matters still worse, I was supposed to return on
Friday night, after my workshops, to New York on the same
Normally, I have fairly high frustration tolerance (HFT), delinquent airline that had unethically given me so much
which I have increased over the years by using REBT on trouble on Thursday. The airline only allowed 45 minutes
myself. Witness my publishing over 65 books and some 700 for me to connect in Denver with my New York flight,
articles, giving over 80 professional and public workshops a however my flight to Denver was a half hour late. I had to
year in New York and throughout the world, regularly su- run a fantastic distance at the Denver airport to catch my
pervising 10 interns and therapists in REBT, and having ses- New York flight. I and my bedraggled luggage finally made
sions every week with about 70 individual and group the New York flight, just as they were closing the doors to
therapy clients. Pretty good for an old man! In fact, in this— take off.
my 86th year—I shall probably publish 4 or 5 new books I was convinced that this famous American airline was
and a dozen or more articles. So I hardly give in to my reprehensibly responsible for my frustration, because (a)
potential low frustration tolerance and procrastination. my original plane got unexplainably lost; (b) we were there-
Nonetheless, I still at times suffer from abysmal LFT— fore 2 hours late in starting; (c) we consequently sat on the
and my use of REBT almost always removes it. Take, for field another hour waiting for a thunderstorm to stop; (d)
example, what happened to me in August 1999. I was sched- we were 3 hours late to Dallas; (e) they unethically can-
uled to give workshops from 10:00 AM to 5:00 PM in Colo- celed our duly scheduled flight to Colorado Springs—for
rado Springs. To make sure I arrived in time, I planned on no good reason that I could see; (f) their agent lied to me
taking one of the largest airlines from New York directly to about there being no early flight Friday morning to my des-
Colorado Springs the afternoon before. We were scheduled tination; and (g) they put me up for the night in a “flea-
to leave New York at 5:00 PM, stop over briefly in Dallas, bag” hotel, which had hundreds of ants in my bathroom.
and then arrive at our destination at 9:00 PM. I deliberately Frankly, I was incensed. I obsessively awfulized. I tried to
took this flight because it was the only one that went, with use my best REBT—but it did not work. I foolishly vowed
an interim stop, directly to Colorado Springs. So, presum- never to use that infamous airline again. My usual high frus-
ably, I could not miss getting there Thursday night, in good tration tolerance failed me, and for 2 days, including my
time to be fresh for my workshops on Friday. trip back to New York and the following day, I inwardly
No such luck. The airline never told us passengers what seethed. As soon as I returned, I wrote the airline a scathing
happened to our plane, but after 2 hours of its getting lost letter and demanded monetary remuneration.
somewhere, they finally got us on a substitute plane that Finally, I went over the dismal events of the trip, saw that
was to leave LaGuardia airport at 7:00 PM. A thunderstorm the airline was wrong and its unethical agent was highly
occurred while we were about to take off. All plane traffic fallible, but that those kinds of mistakes were not horrible
was halted, and we finally left New York at 9:00 PM—a and sometimes inevitably occur. I then got back some mea-
good 3 hours late. sure of my HFT. I damned the behavior of the airline and its
The pilots did their best, but by the time we arrived in agent, but stopped blaming the airline itself or the
Dallas, it was 12:00 AM local time, so the airline—quite personhood of the agent. At last, my REBT began to work
unethically, I thought—canceled our continuing flight to again. I have even traveled on that “wrongheaded” airline
Colorado Springs and we were stuck for the night in Dal- again since that time—with, fortunately, much better re-
las. I explained to the airline agent that I had to start my sults. My ill-fated trip was indeed bad—but I could stand it,
workshop in Colorado Springs at 10:00 AM the next morn- learn from it, and still survive. My high frustration tolerance
ing and therefore had to have an early flight to that city. was, for a while, rudely interrupted. But not forever.
Whereupon he lied to me, said there was no early morning
flight, and that I would have to take an 11:00 AM flight
Depression (Catharine MacLaren)
and arrive, at the earliest, at 12:15 PM—long after my work-
shop was scheduled to begin. Actually there was a 6:30 AM I moved to New York from Colorado in the summer of
flight from Dallas to Colorado Springs, but it was on a rival 1995. I knew that my active, outdoor lifestyle would have
airline and he wrongly told me it did not exist. So he put to change somewhat to accommodate city living, but by
me up at a “flea-bag” hotel for the night in Dallas. Not only December I was having serious difficulties getting out of

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 265


We i n r a c h , E l l i s , M a c L a r e n , D i G i u s e p p e , Ve r n o n , Wo l f e , M a l k i n s o n , a n d B a c k x

bed in the morning. A once 7- to 8-hours-a-night sleeper, I Despite the doctor’s assurance that there was no reason to
began logging 10 hours a night and still feeling run down think this was anything more serious, I became extremely
the next day. My energy level was at an all-time low, I anxious and was certain they would discover he had pros-
stopped engaging in most social activities, and it was diffi- tate cancer. My first husband had died from lymph cancer
cult for me to generate even a spark of excitement about 15 years earlier, and even though I tried to remind myself
any aspect of my life. By mid-January I realized that some- that these two situations were not similar and that the doc-
thing was going to have to change. tor had practically guaranteed us that this was not cancer, it
I read Overcoming Depression (Hauck, 1973) cover to cover did not stop me from assuming the worst. I attempted to
several times. I tried REBT Self-Help forms but had difficulty control the situation by demanding that the surgery be per-
identifying core irrational beliefs to dispute. On a practical level, formed immediately because I couldn’t stand the anxiety.
I started paying closer attention to my exercise and eating On the day of the surgery I took plenty of paperwork to
habits. I stepped up my exercise regime and tried to cut “un- the hospital, hoping to distract myself. It did not work. I
helpful” foods out of my diet. The effects were minimal and kept thinking that if this was cancer, I wouldn’t be able to
I experienced myself spiraling further down. Hindsight be- stand it. We had a young son, and it would be awful for him
ing 20/20, it comes as no surprise that I finally realized that to grow up without his father. I did not think I could live
there was probably a biological component to my experi- through watching someone I loved die, because I had al-
ence. My mother had been diagnosed several years before ready had to do that as a 1st-year bride. The fact that I had
with seasonal affective disorder, and it seemed that my de- lived through it never entered my mind. I was also very
pressive mood had started sometime in November. Colo- angry: it was not fair that this was happening to us.
rado being one of the sunniest states in the country, it was When the doctor finally came to report that as a matter
possible that I had unwittingly avoided seasonal downturns of routine they would biopsy the stones and we should
by spending so much time outside throughout the year. My know the results in a few days, I fell apart, saying that “I
apartment in New York did not afford me even one window couldn’t stand the anxiety of waiting and wondering.” He
in my bedroom, so it was pitch black at all times of the day was very unempathic, reminding me that there was prob-
without a light on. My work schedule was fairly rigorous, so ably nothing to worry about, but even if there was cancer,
I spent most of my time indoors or on the subway, and much it would be very treatable. He was probably attempting to
less time outside on the weekends than I had in previous balance my irrationality with rationality, but I did not appre-
years. I was even doing the majority of my exercise inside. ciate it. I burst into tears, saying that he did not understand
I read Winter Blues (Rosenthal, 1993) and got down to what I had already been through with my first husband, and
work. I replaced all the light bulbs at home and in my of- that if this was cancer it would be awful.
fice with full-spectrum bulbs. I obtained a light box and I know the doctor wanted to get rid of me, so he promised
started waking up 30–50 minutes early to sit in front of it to rush the results. When the results came back negative, my
and read, effectively extending the light in my day. Within anxiety quickly dissipated. However, when I was caught up
3 weeks I was feeling much better. REBT was helpful in in the actual experience, I wasn’t able to think rationally. I
terms of dealing with the frustration of having to take those had not even considered the fact that I had survived this
extra steps, but ultimately it was the additional full- before and no doubt could again if in fact it was cancer.
spectrum light that seemed to make the real difference. Had the time frame been longer, I might have sought coun-
seling, although I was the only REBT practitioner in the
Impulsivity (Raymond DiGiuseppe) area. So in addition to my other irrationalities, I was engag-
One area in which REBT has not helped me (yet) is control- ing in self-downing: “I should have been able to be more
ling my impulsivity. I tend to take on too many tasks because rational about this. What a hypocrite I am, teaching others
I enjoy doing them all and often do not write them down. The to be rational and not applying it to myself.”
consequences are that I wind up being double-booked at times
and inconveniencing myself and the people with whom I work. Low Frustration Tolerance (Janet Wolfe)
And I become very stressed when I have to reschedule things I was brought up with a strong work ethic and sense of
or have a huge amount of work piling up. My irrational belief personal responsibility. These values have helped me be the
is that “I shouldn’t have to take the time to fill in the appoint- person I am, but probably have also helped contribute to
ment book. It’s too much of a pain and I can’t stand having to my main REBT “failure”: my tendency to ruminate and
do it.” I’ve attempted to think to myself, “Just take the time to awfulize about people who have been contracted to do work
write stuff in your appointment book.” But no matter how for me or my organization and either ignore it, screw it up,
many times I have tried to challenge this, it still has not gotten or take three times as long to do it as they should have.
me to keep an up-to-date appointment book. Although the majority of the time these situations occur
with outside contractors (repair people, professional con-
Anxiety (Ann Vernon)
sultants, etc.), they can occasionally happen with various
One instance when REBT did not work for me was when members of my own staff. And no matter how much I try
my spouse had to have surgery to remove prostate stones. to tell myself, “They’re just being FFHB’s (fallible, fouled-up

266 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Successes and Failures

human beings), I find myself enraged, depressed, and rumi- army is embedded in the history of contemporary Israeli
nating for days on end. Typically, my self-talk is of the same society and is regarded as an important transitional period
ilk as that of my clients’ daily expostulations: “I can’t believe for the individual and society.
I put in 10 hours of work setting up this project for her, left Feeling bad and hoping for the best was all I could do.
her a small task, and she’s taken 2 months to mail it out!” Despite my training in REBT, I was telling myself “I must
“How could someone possibly sign a contract, take our hard- be at my best” and “they must like REBT.” Following a short
earned money, then so grossly inconvenience us by taking 5 introduction, I asked participants about their past experi-
months longer than they said they would?” “It’s awful—I can’t ence with REBT and their expectations for the workshop.
stand it—this person is an incredibly (stupid) (lazy) (devious) They had heard about Ellis and REBT. Their expectations
(incompetent) (crooked) (despicable) human being!” were essentially neutral—neither high nor low. How awful.
My friends and colleagues with whom I share my latest In spite of their lack of enthusiasm, I did not feel terrible
horror story do all the right things—empathize, validate all the time, but just part of the time. Toward the end of the
that the person has indeed screwed up—and then try to workshop, the head of the clinic shared a traumatic event
help me see that it is a hassle, not a horror, that humans will that he had experienced, which was an opportunity for me
be human, and all those other rational beliefs I try to instill to demonstrate the application of REBT under such cir-
in my clients when their significant others act unfairly or cumstances. As a result of the demonstration, the “client”
incompetently. This may help for a few hours, but left alone reported a significant change of his feeling of anxiety to a
(especially on holiday weekends) laboring at the task that level of merely “deep concern.” I felt very proud. My use of
this other person has already been paid a good deal of money REBT with him had obviously had the desired outcomes.
to do, I usually manage fairly quickly to resume my rumi- However, I was profoundly angry, disappointed and frus-
nations—along with considerable anger—not only while I trated when the “client’s” reported change of cognitions and
am working but well into my now quite-diminished (be- emotions were interpreted by his colleagues as a “classic pri-
cause of others’ delinquency) leisure time. And although I mary death anxiety,” which had nothing to do with my help-
am grateful for the many rational shoulders I have to cry ing him to change his thinking. I felt as though I was a com-
on, the grim fact remains that it is usually me with whom plete failure. I was saying to myself, “his cognitive change
the buck stops. I hope I can “lick this one” some day, but I was an excellent ‘proof’ of the effectiveness of REBT and
am only guardedly optimistic. all of the participants must be convinced. If they were not,
that would make me a complete failure as a teacher.”
Need for Unconditional Self-Acceptance (Ruth Malkinson)
In Israel, the dominant school of psychotherapy is still psy- CONCLUSIONS
chodynamic, even though REBT, like other cognitive models,
has been empirically proved to be an effective therapy. There- It should come as no surprise that several experienced REBT
fore, an invitation to give a workshop on REBT to a team of experts have both succeeded and failed in applying REBT
professionals who adhere to traditional therapies is always a to themselves. The implication for practitioners who use
challenge. Such was the case when I was invited by a senior REBT with their clients and perhaps on themselves, as well,
social worker, who practices cognitive therapy himself, in one is clear: The successful use of REBT requires considerable
of the public mental health clinics, to conduct a one-day work- and continuous hard work. REBT is not easy. REBT is not
shop on REBT for the staff, consisting of psychiatrists, social applicable to every situation. Nor is it realistic to expect
workers, psychologists, and occupational therapists. The only REBT, or any other approach, to work 100% of the time.
counselors in Israel are school counselors. That said, it has been used successfully in the personal lives
I told myself that the workshop must be a success and of those experts who contributed to this article. It could be
that the participants must be convinced of the effective- argued that in those cases in which the experts failed, it
ness of REBT. I prepared handouts, video vignettes, role may have been a function of their fallibility as human be-
plays, a list of references, and more. I was experiencing anxi- ings. For example, those who struggle with LFT probably
ety before meeting the group. On my arrival, I was greeted get frustrated trying to reduce their LFT. In the final analy-
by my host, a social worker, who then proceeded to apolo- sis, Paul (1967) got it right. The ultimate question is, of
gize for not participating in the workshop because of an course, “What treatment, by whom is most effective for this
unexpected personal commitment (a military parade to individual with that specific problem under which set of
mark the completion of his son’s training). Understandably, circumstances?” (p. 111).
participating in the parade of one’s son is a special event
not to be missed—a point that I failed to accept because I REFERENCES
told myself that he must not leave me alone in the battle-
field to fight for cognitive psychotherapy. I felt deserted. Ellis, A. (1994). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy (Rev. ed.). New
York: Carol Publishing Group.
How could he leave me all by myself in the “lion’s den?” It Hauck, P. (1973). Overcoming depression. Philadelphia: Westminster Press.
should be noted that military service in Israel is compul- Paul, G. L. (1967). Strategy of outcome research in psychotherapy. Jour-
sory for boys and girls reaching the age of 18 years. The nal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 109–118.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 267


We i n r a c h , E l l i s , M a c L a r e n , D i G i u s e p p e , Ve r n o n , Wo l f e , M a l k i n s o n , a n d B a c k x

Rosenthal, N. (1993). Winter blues: Seasonal affective disorder, what is it Weinrach, S. G., Ellis, A., DiGiuseppe, R., Bernard, M., Dryden, W.,
and how to overcome it. New York: Guilford Press. Kassinove, H., Morris, G. B., Vernon, A., & Wolfe, J. (1995). Rational
Walen, S. R., DiGiuseppe, R., & Dryden, W. (1992). A practitioner’s Emotive Behavior Therapy after Ellis: Predictions for the future.
guide to rational emotive therapy (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 17, 413–427.
University Press. Weinrach, S. G., Lustig, D., Chan, F., & Thomas, K. R. (1998). Publi-
Weinrach, S. G. (1996). Nine experts describe the essence of rational- cation patterns of The Personnel and Guidance Journal/Journal of
emotive therapy while standing on one foot. Journal of Counseling & Counseling & Development—1978 to 1993. Journal of Counseling &
Development, 74, 326–331. Development, 76, 427–435.

268 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Metaphor and Change in Counseling

William J. Lyddon, Alison L. Clay, and Cheri L. Sparks

Increased general interest among counselors in the way language, narratives, and stories influence clients’ personal and social
realities has drawn particular attention to metaphorical language and its facilitative role in counseling. In this article, the authors
suggest that metaphors and metaphorical knowing may play a significant role in facilitating at least 5 developmental change
processes in counseling: relationship building, accessing and symbolizing client emotions, uncovering and challenging clients’
tacit assumptions, working with client resistance, and introducing new frames of reference.

T
he emergence of the constructivist perspective which means to transfer or carry across, allows the user to
in counseling and psychotherapy (Neimeyer & transfer meaning from one domain to another by “giving to
Mahoney, 1995; Rosen & Kuehlwein, 1996; Sex- one thing a name or description that belongs . . . to some-
ton & Griffin, 1997) has highlighted a central thing else, on the grounds of some similarity between the
role for human language, narrative, and stories in two” (Leary, 1990b, p. 4). Kopp (1971) elaborated stating,
the creation of personal and social realities (Anderson & “a metaphor is a way of speaking in which one thing is ex-
Goolishian, 1988; Efran, Lukens, & Lukens, 1990; Hare-Mustin, pressed in terms of another, whereby this bringing together
1994; Russell, 1995; Shotter, 1993). It is interesting that one throws new light on the character of what is being described”
important consequence of this focus on language has been a (p. 17). For example, one person may describe marriage as
greater appreciation for the metaphorical features of human a journey, whereas another may view marriage in terms of
knowing and meaning creation (Carlsen, 1996). Indeed, meta- a competitive game or even a battle.
phors may be found across all domains of human thought, It is interesting to note that significant scientific develop-
whether philosophical and scientific or psychological and ments (or paradigm shifts) have not only been referred to
personal (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Leary, 1990a; Lyddon, metaphorically as scientific “revolutions” (Kuhn, 1962) but
1989). In the counseling context, metaphors may help to also have often been accompanied by the use of novel meta-
structure and facilitate client–counselor communications and phors (Leary, 1990a). For example, Gentner and Grudin
interactions (Berlin, Olson, Cano, & Engel, 1991). In addi- (1985) assembled a collection of metaphors used to describe
tion, transformations in the metaphors clients use to define the human mind from more than 80 years of articles in the
their lives and problems may also correspond with signifi- Psychological Review. They found that during the early years of
cant change in counseling (Carlsen, 1996; Goncalves, 1994; this journal, spatial and animate-being metaphors dominated,
Meichenbaum, 1995). Thus, metaphor is a vehicle both for whereas computer and systems metaphors dominated the later
communication and for change in counseling (Muran & years. Mahoney (1991) noted that several metaphorical shifts
DiGiuseppe, 1990). have occurred in the history of psychobiology, from hydraulic
In this article, we explore the use of metaphor in coun- “humours” to mechanical, electrical, and contemporary
seling with a particular focus on the role metaphorical lan- hormonal notions. Sternberg (1990) has drawn similar
guage may play in facilitating key developmental change conclusions about the history of the study of intelligence. He
processes. Toward this end, we first offer a brief historical has suggested that different theories of intelligence evolved
and theoretical overview of metaphor. from different metaphors of intelligence—geographic,
computational, biological, epistemological, anthropological,
HISTORICAL AND THEORETICAL BACKGROUND sociological, or systems metaphors.
On an individual level, metaphors are believed to aid in
The study of metaphor may be traced to the Greek phi- the organization of personal experience (Fox, 1989). Sub-
losopher Aristotle, who conceptualized metaphor as “a se- stantial linguistic evidence supports the metaphorical qual-
ries of words in which a comparison is being made between ity of human thought processes (Johnson, 1987; Lakoff &
two or more entities that are literally dissimilar” (Angus & Johnson, 1980), and research on epistemic cognitive style
Rennie, 1988, p. 552). Metaphor, from the Greek metaphera, posits “metaphorism” as a way of knowing apart from either

William J. Lyddon is a professor and director of training, and Alison L. Clay and Cheri L. Sparks are students, all in the Counseling Psychology Doctoral
Program at the University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to William J. Lyddon, Counseling
Psychology Program, University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS 39406-5025 (e-mail: william.lyddon@usm.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 269


L y d d o n , C l a y, a n d S p a r k s

rational or empirical knowing (Royce & Powell, 1983). Mills however, are the particular ways in which use of metaphor
and Crowley (1986) further suggested that metaphor is the functions to facilitate client change. In the following dis-
language of the right brain, a more fluid, imagistic, and cussion, we suggest that metaphor is central to at least five
emotional operational system than the left brain (Marlatt developmental change processes in counseling: (a) relation-
& Fromme, 1987). ship building, (b) accessing and symbolizing emotions, (c)
Lankton and Lankton (1989) broadly defined metaphor uncovering and challenging clients’ tacit assumptions, (d)
to include any form of communication or human activity working with client resistance, and (e) introducing new
that “speaks” to the individual. Thus, biblical parables, fairy frames of reference. What follows is a discussion of the role
tales, myths, stories, rituals, and motion pictures may all be of metaphor in each of these counseling processes.
seen as metaphorical vehicles able to facilitate change, growth,
and understanding. Through use of metaphor, individuals are Relationship Building
able to explore and expand present experience into previ-
ously unrecognized possibilities (Fox, 1989). Fairy tales The development of a safe and secure counseling relation-
stimulate the imagination, while engaging both intellect and ship, or working alliance (Bordin, 1976), is believed to be a
emotion to “see” unexpected solutions for existential significant component of successful counseling process and
problems (Bettelheim, 1975). Metaphor facilitates under- outcome (Lambert & Bergin, 1994). Counselor sensitivity
standing, in part, of that which defies understanding in to client-generated metaphors may help to convey under-
toto—feelings, experiences, moral, and spiritual perspectives standing of the client’s unique way of knowing and at the
(Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). same time contribute to the development of a shared lan-
Metaphor, as an experiential phenomenon, is part of an guage and collaborative bond between client and counselor
ongoing dialectic in the search for meaning (Angus & Rennie, (Fine, Pollio, & Simpkinson, 1973; Goncalves & Craine,
1988), a juxtaposing of two seemingly unrelated referents 1990). Although many counselors may generate their own
“producing semantic conceptual anomaly, the symptom of metaphors, Angus and Rennie (1988) encouraged metaphori-
which is usually emotional tension” (MacCormac, 1985, cal communications in which the counselor and client work
p. 5). It is interesting that the emotional tension or disequi- together in the process of “apprehending, articulating, and
librium that may accompany significant metaphorical shifts elaborating inner association to metaphors” (p. 555). For
(and hence, shifts in personal meaning) is viewed from a de- example, during her first counseling session a client described
velopmental constructivist perspective as a natural part of her experience of a romantic relationship as being like play-
the change process (Lyddon, 1990; Mahoney, 1991; Rosen, ing the childhood game of “Monkey in the Middle.” The
1996). Thus, as a transformative change agent, metaphor may client recounted her role in the relationship as being the
be particularly useful in the context of counseling. “monkey,” that is, the person jumping for the ball that is
usually thrown just out of her reach. The counselor en-
couraged the client to elaborate on her metaphor by dis-
METAPHOR AND COUNSELING cussing her perception of the different perspectives from
According to Mahoney (1991), which the various game participants view the “ball.” She
stated that the player who is throwing the ball behaves so
Our theories of change—which are our theories of personality, psy- as to diminish the object, which is casually tossed about
chopathology, lifespan development, identity, and systems—are most while she (the client), on the other hand, believes the ball
powerfully captured in the generativities and multiplicities that (relationship) to be valuable—something to be treasured
are afforded by metaphors. (p. 273)
and protected. As a result, the counselor suggested that,
Indeed, the use of metaphor permeates many facets of from the client’s perspective, the ball may be compared to
counseling theories as evidenced in the literature. Many a piece of Waterford crystal, which the client would cher-
authors have suggested a facilitative role for metaphorical ish rather than throw about as the other player does. In this
communication in counseling through stories, anecdotes, way, the counselor used the client’s metaphor to
metaphorical objects, and music (e.g., Barker, 1985; Jooste empathically communicate an understanding of the client’s
& Cleaver, 1992; Saari, 1986). Metaphorical strategies have language and subjective experience. Furthermore, the
been implemented with a variety of client populations, from counselor’s willingness to elaborate on the client’s meta-
children to adults, as well as across a wide range of pre- phor invited the client to either agree with the elaboration
senting concerns (cf. Adams & Chadbourne, 1982; Bowman, or further clarify her feelings in the collaborative context
1992; Cornille & Inger, 1992; Davis & Sandoval, 1978; Huss of the developing counseling relationship.
& O’Connor, 1995; James & Hazler, 1998; Kopp, 1971; Accessing and Symbolizing Emotions
Lankton & Lankton, 1989; Marlatt & Fromme, 1987;
McClure, 1987; Roberts, 1987; Saari, 1986; Schwartz- Emotional experience and exploration often play a pivotal
Borden, 1992). role in the change process by organizing the client’s self-
There is much support, therefore, for the notion that use experience and establishing “links between self and environ-
of metaphor in counseling provides an effective mode of ment” (Greenberg, Rice, & Elliott, 1993, p. 54). Metaphors
communication and meaning creation. What is not clear, may be useful tools for helping clients access, as well as

270 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Metaphor and Change in Counseling

symbolize, emotions that may have been previously unex- future. In this way, metaphor can condense and make vivid the
pressed, unexplored, or even unrecognized. Fox (1989), for person’s apparent view of his lot in life. Moreover, metaphor can
highlight the client’s active contribution to his lot and thereby imply
example, asserted that metaphors help to expand the client’s choices open to him for modifying that lot. (p. 147)
emotional awareness, increase emotional arousal, and lead to
the expression of a “felt-sense” and “nonliteral experience” The role of the counselor is not to interpret the client’s
(p. 234). Similarly, Carlsen (1996) noted that use of meta- metaphors or to directly modify the client’s belief system
phors might facilitate the counseling process by “translating but to facilitate, guiding the client’s exploration and mean-
the intangibles of emotion into some sort of verbal expres- ing in a supportive but challenging manner. For example, as
sion” (p. 350). For example, a female client who has under- the client becomes more aware of the tacit beliefs and as-
gone a hysterectomy may refer nonemotionally to her scar. sumptions that are no longer viable, the counselor may guide
The counselor, in following up, may inquire about the client’s the exploration of alternative metaphors (e.g., “What game
emotional meaning attached to the physical representation. In would you rather play than ‘Monkey in the Middle?’” or
response, the client may speak of an emotional “hole in her “Instead of feeling like a juggler, can you imagine another
middle” and the pain associated with the loss of part of her way of relating to work, school, and your wife and child?”).
body. Further exploration may focus more specifically not only Encouraging a client to explore alternative metaphors is a
on the emotional pain associated with her loss (i.e., grief work) way of questioning a point of view while at the same time
but also on the new metaphors that may emerge as she begins maintaining the client’s integrity (Billings, 1991).
to reconstruct her relationship to that which has been lost. Although metaphor is usually thought of in terms of nar-
By allowing clients to symbolize the “intangibles of emo- rative presentation, counselors should also be alert to the
tion,” metaphors act as vehicles within which clients may way that clients may reveal metaphors (and thus, tacit as-
construct new personal meanings of their experiences. sumptions) nonverbally (Modell, 1997). Indeed, as Fox
(1989) asserted, “However clients breathe, laugh, sigh and
The power of metaphor lies in its ability to touch an affective com-
ponent of the individual’s experience. Emotions are presumed to
move are metaphors” (p. 236). Sims and Whynot (1997)
make up “feeling memories” which in turn furnish individuals with agreed, adding: “Gesture . . . as well as language may have
a more or less automatic unconscious reaction repertoire to metaphorical structure” (p. 342). The client whose hand
affectively meaningful stimuli. By providing affectively meaning- gestures with index finger and thumb separated by a space
ful stimuli which are incongruous with the individual’s current ways of approximately 2 inches is indicating an amount of “some-
of seeing him/herself or situation, metaphor may necessitate struc-
tural changes in the person’s personal reality system. (Marlatt &
thing,” the identity of which may be discovered later in
Fromme, 1987, p. 22) counseling. Likewise, the client who gestures—hands in
front of chest, palms down, and making a downward push-
Thus, metaphors may not only help clients become more ing motion—may be expressing nonverbally what she may
aware of their emotions (“I realize that I feel very frus- not be able to express in words. Careful attention to such
trated playing ‘Monkey in the Middle.’”) but may also help nonverbal metaphorical expressions may provide the coun-
them to symbolize personal experiences in meaningful ways seling relationship with rich content for exploration.
(“I was so happy to just be asked to play the game that I
didn’t even stop to realize I wasn’t enjoying it.”). Working With Client Resistance
Helping clients identify and work through their resistance,
Uncovering and Challenging Tacit Assumptions or self-protective fear of change, is often an important
For many clients, becoming more aware of previously tacit aspect of the counseling process (Bugental & Bugental,
or unconscious beliefs about self and world that influence 1984). As Fox (1989) has pointed out, metaphors often
current behavior and choices constitutes an important help clients indirectly access new information about them-
change process in counseling (Ecker & Hulley, 1996). Meta- selves, arousing their ambivalence while protecting them
phors may play an important role in facilitating such aware- against the potential adverse impact of full discovery. As it
ness and expression (e.g., “I now understand that for a long turned out, the client who offered the “Monkey in the
time I have simply believed that life is a big party, and I’m Middle” metaphor for describing a relationship had been
not invited” or “I realize that I’ve always felt like an actor in unable to express her feelings regarding the ending of this
a tragic play and that I will never get what I want most— relationship. By examining the component parts of the
happiness.”). Through metaphor, clients’ tacit beliefs are metaphor, the client was able to recognize and express her
unveiled as the unspeakable is uttered, the taken for granted sense of frustration and also to identify the power differen-
or the unimagined is expressed and explored (Schnitzer, tial inherent in the relationship. In addition, the counselor’s
1993). Furthermore, by recognizing their tacit assumptions, elaboration of the metaphor to examine the nature of the
clients are in a better position to change or modify them. “ball” allowed the client to gain awareness of the discrepant
As Lenrow (1966) suggested, valuations of the relationship being made by the “players.”
Thus, metaphorical communication may allow clients to
Metaphors can highlight the client’s unspoken assumptions about explore what is “them” by talking about what is “not them.”
his capacities for influencing his surroundings effectively in the In other words, metaphors may function as a safe mediator

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 271


L y d d o n , C l a y, a n d S p a r k s

between what is and is not currently threatening to the ries and understandings but also that metaphorical knowing is
client (Billings, 1991; Fox, 1989; Jooste & Cleaver, 1992). fundamental to the very personal theories and narratives
Caruth and Ekstein (1966) referred to metaphor as the people construct about their lives, relationships, and per-
client’s alibi, “a conscious allusion which is a way of imply- sonal problems. In particular, we have suggested that in the
ing what he wants to communicate without actually com- counseling context, counselors’ sensitivity to and use of
mitting himself, a way of simultaneously keeping and re- client-generated metaphors may play a facilitative role in
vealing a secret” (p. 38). Indeed, the counselor’s ability to at least five developmental change processes important to
hear and participate in the creation and elaboration of meta- successful counseling outcomes. First, a counselor’s ability
phor may provide an alternative vehicle within which to to hear and attend to client metaphors not only enlivens
express and examine painful client feelings and experiences the communication but also facilitates the development of
in a manner that bypasses rigid defenses and facilitates rap- a collaborative counseling relationship, or working alliance.
port (Billings, 1991; Fox, 1989). From this perspective, empathic understanding may even
be viewed as a process of metaphorical mapping, whereby
Introducing New Frames of Reference the empathizer (counselor) is able to generate a metaphor
that not only results in the communication of verbal un-
Significant, or second-order, change in counseling often derstanding but also expresses an understanding of the
involves a dramatic shift in a client’s perspective or frame client’s emotional state. A second facilitative role of meta-
of reference (Lyddon, 1990). It is not unusual for this type phor concerns the ways in which metaphors help clients
of change to be organized around a new or novel meta- access and symbolize emotions. In essence, metaphorical
phor. As previously noted, metaphors as a device for discovery language offers clients a figurative medium for expressing
may facilitate awareness of aspects of the individual or emotional understandings that are often difficult (or some-
relationship that have previously been out of view (Cirillo times impossible) to express in literal terms. Third, in addi-
& Crider, 1995). When this information becomes available, tion to emotional expressions, metaphors may be useful
however, it sets the stage for the exploration of alternative vehicles for the identification and clarification of clients’
perspectives and possibilities. The client who explores the long held tacit beliefs and assumptions about self and world.
“Monkey in the Middle” game, for example, may be able Exploration of these beliefs provides clients with the oppor-
to examine the options she has either to continue playing or tunity to modify those beliefs that are no longer functional
to refuse to play. She may evaluate the discrepancy between and to construct alternative, more viable life metaphors
the level of enjoyment afforded to the people on the outside (Goncalves, 1994). A fourth facilitative role concerns the
compared with her position and eventually decide that simply way metaphorical expressions may help some clients work
being accepted as a powerless player in the game is no longer more effectively with their ambivalence and fear of change
desirable. Therefore, rather than playing “Monkey in the by allowing them to indirectly express certain feelings that if
Middle,” the client may speak of playing chess (a game in dealt with directly would engender strong resistance. In this
which both players use strategies of anticipating the other’s way, metaphors may function as a safe bridge between the
moves) or eventually transform the metaphor from a game part of the client that wants to change and the part that does
of competition to a game of cooperation and support. not. Fifth, as clients begin to both reevaluate previously held
The use of metaphor thus allows clients the opportunity beliefs and explore alternative, more viable frames of refer-
to create new possibilities and to modify their conceptual ence, novel metaphors can often play an important organiz-
framework within which new solutions may be explored ing role for these new developmental possibilities. One cli-
and adopted (Billings, 1991). Within the counseling rela- ent described this personal metamorphosis:
tionship, counselor and client collaborate in examining
these new frames of reference, with the client choosing to I am becoming real . . . I’m taking off this big coat that I have been
discard those that are no longer useful and adopt those lugging around for so long . . . I carried a belief—from the media,
that are more viable. As a consequence, the client’s sense from the culture. Now I am shedding it, getting in touch with the
of possibility may be expanded, and new meanings for genuine. (cited in Carlsen, 1996, p. 339)
emotions, thoughts, and behaviors are considered. Davies
(1988), for example, described how the use of the fairy By using their individual creative styles, counselors have
tale Sleeping Beauty allowed one client to view her eating the opportunity of speaking metaphorically with clients in
disorder from the perspective of being in a deep sleep. As a very personal and meaningful ways. Whether through dis-
result of this reframing, the client explored the possibility cussion of a current movie or by reference to musical lyrics
of “waking up” and taking a more active role in counseling or a child’s book such as Williams’s The Velveteen Rabbit,
and in her life. the environment is “replete with opportunity for therapeutic
suggestion” (Billings, 1991, p. 5). As clients become more
SUMMARY AND CONCLUDING REMARKS aware of their metaphorical experiences and expressions,
they become empowered to critically examine them.
In this article, we have suggested that metaphors not only Cirillo and Crider (1995) encouraged counselors to take
play a significant role in the generation of scientific theo- a “fluid view” (p. 518) of metaphorical communication not-

272 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Metaphor and Change in Counseling

ing that meaning is constructed in the process of dialogical Huss, E., & O’Connor, T. (1995). The broken balloon: A metaphor for
interaction between speakers. In our view, metaphor may divorce. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 23, 211–223.
James, M. D., & Hazler, R. J. (1998). Using metaphors to soften resis-
be seen as an all-terrain vehicle carrying the therapeutic tance in chemically dependent clients. Journal of Humanistic Educa-
process off the paved roads of prior meaning structures out tion and Development, 36, 122–133.
into uncharted territories where new meanings are yet to Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. Chicago: University of
be created. Within this mode of travel, client and counselor Chicago Press.
may come to concur with Sims and Whynot (1997), who Jooste, E. T., & Cleaver, G. (1992). Metaphors and metaphoric objects.
Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 23, 136–148.
suggested that “meaning is not a destination. It is a Kopp, S. B. (1971). Guru: Metaphors from a psychotherapist. Palo Alto,
process” (p. 343). CA: Science and Behavior Books.
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: Univer-
REFERENCES sity of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Univer-
Adams, C. H., & Chadbourne, J. (1982). Therapeutic metaphor: An approach sity of Chicago Press.
to weight control. The Personnel and Guidance Journal, 60, 510–512. Lambert, M. J., & Bergin, A. E. (1994). The effectiveness of psycho-
Anderson, H., & Goolishian, H. (1988). Human systems as linguistic therapy. In A. E. Bergin & S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psycho-
systems. Family Process, 27, 371–393. therapy and behavior change (pp. 143–189). New York: Wiley.
Angus, L. E., & Rennie, D. L. (1988). Therapist participation in meta- Lankton, C. H., & Lankton, S. R. (1989). Tales of enchantment: Goal-
phor generation: Collaborative and noncollaborative styles. Psycho- oriented metaphors for adults and children in therapy. New York: Brunner/
therapy, 25, 552–560. Mazel.
Barker, P. (1985). Using metaphors in psychotherapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Leary, D. E. (1990a). Metaphors in the history of psychology. Cambridge,
Berlin, R. M., Olson, M. E., Cano, C. E., & Engel, S. (1991). Metaphor England: Cambridge University Press.
and psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 45, 359–367. Leary, D. E. (1990b). Psyches muse: The role of metaphor in the history of
Bettelheim, B. (1975). The uses of enchantment. New York: Knopf. psychology. In D. E. Leary (Ed.), Metaphors in the history of psychology
Billings, C. V. (1991). Therapeutic use of metaphor. Issues in Mental Health (pp. 1–78). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Nursing, 12, 1–8. Lenrow, P. B. (1966). Uses of metaphor in facilitating constructive behav-
Bordin, E. S. (1976). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept ior change. Psychotherapy: Theory Research, and Practice, 3, 145–148.
of the working alliance. Psychotherapy, 16, 252–260. Lyddon, W. J. (1989). Root metaphor theory: A philosophical frame-
Bowman, G. (1992). Using therapeutic metaphor in adjustment coun- work for counseling and psychotherapy. Journal of Counseling and De-
seling. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 86, 440–442. velopment, 67, 442–448.
Bugental, J. F. T., & Bugental, E. K. (1984). A fate worse than death: The Lyddon, W. J. (1990). First- and second-order change: Implications for
fear of changing. Psychotherapy, 21, 543–549. rationalist and constructivist cognitive therapies. Journal of Counsel-
Carlsen, M. B. (1996). Metaphor, meaning-making, and metamorphosis. ing & Development, 69, 122–127.
In H. Rosen & K. T. Kuehlwein (Eds.), Constructing realities: Meaning- MacCormac, E. R. (1985). A cognitive theory of metaphor. Cambridge,
making perspectives for psychotherapists (pp. 337–367). San Francisco: England: Bradford Books.
Jossey-Bass. Mahoney, M. J. (1991). Human change processes: The scientific founda-
Caruth, E., & Ekstein, R. (1966). Interpretation within the metaphor. tions of psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
American Academy of Child Psychiatry Journal, 5, 35–45. Marlatt, G. A., & Fromme, K. (1987). Metaphors for addiction. Journal
Cirillo, L., & Crider, C. (1995). Distinctive therapeutic uses of meta- of Drug Issues, 17, 9–28.
phor. Psychotherapy, 32, 511–519. McClure, B. A. (1987). Metaphoric illumination in groups. Small Group
Cornille, T., & Inger, C. (1992). The armor metaphor in marital and fam- Behavior, 18, 179–187.
ily therapy. Journal of Family Psychotherapy, 3, 27–42. Meichenbaum, D. (1995). Changing conceptions of cognitive behavior
Davies, E. (1988). Reframing, metaphors, myths and fairy-tales. Journal modification: Retrospect and prospect. In M. J. Mahoney (Ed.), Cog-
of Family Therapy, 10, 83–92. nitive and constructive psychotherapies (pp. 20–26). New York: Springer.
Davis, J. M., & Sandoval, J. (1978). Metaphor in group mental health Mills, J. C., & Crowley, R. J. (1986). Therapeutic metaphors for children
consultation. Journal of Community Psychology, 6, 374–381. and the child within. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (1996). Depth-oriented brief therapy. San Fran- Modell, A. H. (1997). The synergy of memory, affects, and metaphor.
cisco: Jossey-Bass. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 42, 105–117.
Efran, J. S., Lukens, M. D., & Lukens, R. J. (1990). Language, structure, Muran, J. C., & DiGiuseppe, R. A. (1990). Towards a cognitive formulation
and change: Frameworks of meaning in psychotherapy. New York: of metaphor use in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 69–85.
Norton. Neimeyer, R. A., & Mahoney, M. J. (Eds.). (1995). Constructivism in psy-
Fine, H. J., Pollio, H. R., & Simpkinson, C. H. (1973). Figurative lan- chotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
guage, metaphor, and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research Roberts, S. (1987). Therapeutic metaphors: A counseling technique. Jour-
and Practice, 10, 87–91. nal of the Academy of Rehabilitative Audiology, 20, 61–72.
Fox, R. (1989). What is meta for? Clinical Social Work Journal, 17, 233–244. Rosen, H. (1996). Meaning-making narratives: Foundations for
Gentner, D., & Grudin, J. (1985). The evolution of mental metaphors in psy- constructivist and social constructionist psychotherapies. In H. Rosen
chology: A 90-year retrospective. American Psychologist, 40, 181–192. & K. T. Kuehlwein (Eds.), Constructing realities: Meaning-making per-
Goncalves, O. F. (1994). Cognitive narrative psychotherapy: The herme- spectives for psychotherapists (pp. 3–51). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
neutic construction of alternative meanings. Journal of Cognitive Psy- Rosen, H., & Kuehlwein, K. T. (Eds.). (1996). Constructing realities: Mean-
chotherapy: An International Quarterly, 8, 105–125. ing-making perspectives for psychotherapists. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Goncalves, O. F., & Craine, M. H. (1990) The use of metaphors in cogni- Royce, J. R., & Powell, A. (1983). Theory of personality and individual
tive therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quar- differences: Factors, systems, and processes. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
terly, 4, 135–149. Prentice-Hall.
Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating emotional Russell, R. L. (Ed.). (1995). Discourse narrative in psychotherapy. Wash-
change: The moment-by-moment process. New York: Guilford. ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hare-Mustin, R. T. (1994). Discourses in the mirrored room: A Saari, C. (1986). The use of metaphor in therapeutic communication
postmodern analysis of therapy. Family Process, 33, 19–35. with young adolescents. Child and Adolescent Social Work, 3, 15–25.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 273


L y d d o n , C l a y, a n d S p a r k s

Schnitzer, P. K. (1993). Tales of the absent father: Applying the “story” Shotter, J. (1993). Conversational realities: Constructing life through lan-
metaphor in family therapy. Family Process, 32, 441–458. guage. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Schwartz-Borden, G. (1992). Metaphor: Visual aid in grief work. Omega, Sims, P. A., & Whynot, C. A. (1997). Hearing metaphor: An approach to
25, 239–248. working with family-generated metaphor. Family Process, 36, 341–355.
Sexton, T. L., & Griffin, B. L. (Eds.). (1997). Constructivist thinking in coun- Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind: Conceptions of the nature of
seling practice, research, and training. New York: Teachers College Press. intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

274 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


FOCUS on the Family Caregiver: A Problem-Solving
Training Intervention

Monica F. Kurylo, Timothy R. Elliott, and Richard M. Shewchuk

Problem-solving interventions have documented effectiveness in treating distress among a variety of clientele. The authors dis-
cuss the application of training in social problem solving with family caregivers of persons who have incurred severe physical
disabilities. Specifically, the authors outline training procedures (i.e., Project FOCUS) that enable counselors to assist family
caregivers in developing effective problem-solving skills that may translate to increased wellness for each caregiver, and by
extension, their care recipients.

“I’ve taken all the responsibility for the patient.” functioning over time (Kiecolt-Glaser, Dura, Speicher, Trask,
“I want to get over feeling responsible to keep him busy.” & Glaser, 1991; King, Oka, & Young, 1994; Vitaliano, 1997).
“I have feelings of guilt.”
“It is a challenge to find new ways to do old things, and in dealing
Finally, caregivers may receive little positive reinforcement
with new everyday solutions.” for what they do, and they are often forced to neglect their
own needs, leisure pursuits, and personal interests (Quittner,
—Statements made by caregivers in a focus group convened to Opipari, Regoli, Jacobsen, & Eigen, 1992). Simply stated,
address caregiver issues (cf. Elliott & Shewchuk, 2000). the ability of the caregiver to provide support and assis-
tance to their loved one may be reduced if the health and

T
hese statements provide a window to under- well-being of the caregiver erodes over time.
standing the concerns of today’s family caregiver. Caregivers of persons (adults and children) with acquired
The new role of caregiver may be unexpected, severe physical disabilities, such as spinal cord injury (SCI),
particularly in the case of a sudden onset illness face many unique circumstances that complicate the
or injury (e.g., traumatic brain injury or stroke), caregiving experience. When a family member acquires an
and the concomitant duties may at times be overwhelm- SCI, for example, the normal pattern of life events in the
ing. Wellness of the caregiver, as well as the care recipient, family system may be disrupted. A young person with SCI
is obviously affected. Research examining caregivers of older may be forced to return to the parental home or may be
individuals, particularly those with Alzheimer’s disease or unable to be “launched” from it (Whiting, Terry, & Strom-
other dementing illnesses, has shown that caregivers dem- Hendriksen, 1984). Although most states have programs to
onstrate higher levels of distress than noncaregivers on di- promote personal independence after disability, cutbacks
verse measures of depression, anxiety, well-being, and physi- in public funds in many states have limited the availability
cal health (Haley et al., 1995). Family caregivers may be of vocational rehabilitation and independent living programs
particularly distressed when their care recipients endure for many persons with severe physical disabilities (Elliott
chronic and uncontrollable bouts of pain and distress & Shewchuk, 1998). Thus, they are compelled to reside
(Miaskowski, Kragness, Dibble, & Wallhagen, 1997). Emo- with family members who then provide assistance and per-
tional distress and physical symptoms experienced by sonal care. Furthermore, it is important to note that with
caregivers may be due in part to the subjective appraisal of most severe physical disabilities, there is no subtle, devel-
caregiving as onerous and unrewarding (Chwalisz, 1996; opmental course that may aide family members in plan-
Haley, Levine, Brown, & Bartolucci, 1987; Haley et al., 1996). ning for care. Instead, role changes and lifestyle changes
In addition to emotional distress, caregivers often develop occur abruptly and almost instantaneously with the onset
problems in cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and immune of the disability. The role changes and lifestyle changes re-

Monica F. Kurylo is a postdoctoral fellow, and Timothy R. Elliott is an associate professor, both in the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
and Richard M. Shewchuk is an associate professor in the Department of Health Services Administration, all at the University of Alabama at Birmingham.
Monica F. Kurylo is the project coordinator and Timothy R. Elliott and Richard M. Shewchuk are codirectors for Project FOCUS. This article was supported
in part by the National Center for Injury Prevention and Control and the Disabilities Prevention Program, National Center for Environmental Health Grant
R49/CCR412718-01 and the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research Grant #H133B980016A. The contents of this article are solely
the authors’ responsibility and do not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to
Timothy R. Elliott, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, SRC 530 1717 6th Ave S, Birmingham, AL
35249-7330 (e-mail: telliott@uab.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 275


Kurylo, Elliott, and Shewchuk

quired in these circumstances are necessarily related to long- ing (a) worry and concern for their loved one’s recovery
term adaptation given that persons with such physical dis- and medical care; (b) problems dealing with money, trans-
abilities are likely to live for many years with a disability portation, and making necessary home modifications; (c)
that is typically not terminal or life threatening. having to take time off work, make changes in their work
The importance of the family caregiver role has been rec- schedule, make a job change, or even quit working to ac-
ognized by professionals interested in initiating interventions commodate their role of caregiver; and (d) serving as moni-
to assist the family caregiver (Myers, 1989). As practitioners tor for the individual’s daily programs to maintain good
of the wellness paradigm, counselors are uniquely qualified health (e.g., medication management, exercise, diet, appoint-
to provide interventions to family caregivers. For counselors ments). This means that while caregivers typically want to
offering assistance to newly appointed family caregivers, a give love and support, they must also take on additional
focus on wellness provides an excellent backdrop for explora- duties geared toward adequate family care and assume mul-
tion of interventions focused toward maximizing physical, tiple roles (e.g., financial manager, health care supervisor).
psychological, and spiritual health (Myers, 1991, 1992). In an effort at reducing distress and maximizing the
According to counseling researchers in the area, wellness caregiver’s (and in turn, care recipient’s) well-being, these
involves the integration of aspects of health (social, mental, concerns can be addressed through a focused intervention
emotional, spiritual, and physical components) and is invested in assisting the caregiver at arriving at solutions to
maximally achieved when all components are in balance problems they face in their multiple roles. The purpose of
(Myers, 1991, 1992; Witmer & Sweeney, 1992). Wellness does this article is to describe this problem-solving intervention
not necessitate the absence of illness (as defined by health) for family caregivers (Project FOCUS), and discuss how
but instead involves enhanced functioning in all areas of life counselors can use this intervention with caregivers of in-
and throughout the life span (Ginter, 1999; Greenberg, 1985; dividuals with a severe acquired physical disability.
Myers, 1991). As such, wellness is a proactive lifestyle in
which a person is responsible for choices of self-care, self- PROJECT FOCUS
sufficiency, and empowerment. The goal of counseling, then,
is to promote wellness among clients. Given the lifestyle Project FOCUS is a problem-solving training program we
change presented by an acute-onset severe physical disabil- have developed specifically for family caregivers of persons
ity, caregivers often experience an imbalance in at least one, with spinal cord injury. Family caregivers are encouraged
if not several, of the aforementioned areas of health and would to build on existing problem-solving tools and apply these
likely benefit from intervention. tools to their new situation of providing care for a family
member with a severe physical disability. This commu-
FOSTERING CAREGIVER WELLNESS nity-based program currently serves an approximately
equal distribution of African American and Caucasian
Interventions stemming from the wellness paradigm to date individuals. These individuals in our program come pre-
have included a focus on providing respite care and social dominantly from lower to lower-middle socioeconomic
support. Although some studies have shown benefits of status backgrounds. FOCUS is an acronym associated with
caregiver support groups and social networks that offer the problem-solving process (Facts, Optimism, Creativity,
assistance, respite care, and other buffering functions (e.g., Understanding, Solve), which assists the caregivers in recall-
Haley et al., 1996; Myers, 1989), evidence indicates that such ing the problem-solving techniques. The concept within
support erodes over time (Quittner, Glueckauf, & Jackson, each step of the process is briefly covered in a handout (see
1990). This suggests that informal efforts to intervene with Appendix) and is explained in greater detail later in this
family caregivers have not been highly successful or long last- article. This handout is used as a guide for the caregivers to
ing. In addition, it may be difficult to address individual follow during training.
caregiver needs within group situations or a respite network, Before training in problem-solving skills is initiated, as-
and this may lead the caregiver to feel alone in his or her sessment of the caregiver’s problem orientation and prob-
concerns. Therefore, individual interventions are necessary. lem-solving skills is performed. Then, card-sorting tasks are
A cognitive-behavioral intervention approach that has used to determine what specific problems are of concern
been proven effective when used in other populations (i.e., to each individual caregiver. After the assessment and card-
adults with depression, caregivers of cancer patients) is train- sorting tasks are complete, the caregiver is introduced to
ing in social problem solving. The social problem-solving the project and problem-solving training. A brief descrip-
intervention can benefit family caregivers because the tech- tion of the theoretical background of problem solving fol-
nique addresses each caregiver’s unique needs and demands lows, along with a detailed explanation of the assessment
within an individual counseling context. Skills learned in and training process.
training provided by counselors can augment caregiving,
help decrease caregiver distress, and maximize wellness in Theoretical Overview
the caregiver.
Particularly during their first year of caregiving, family According to contemporary models of social problem solv-
caregivers contend with several competing demands, includ- ing (D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1990,

276 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


FOCUS on the Family Caregiver

1999; Nezu & D’Zurilla, 1989), a person’s orientation to his caregivers of individuals with cancer (Houts, Nezu, Nezu,
or her problems determines the manner in which a person & Bucher, 1996).
processes information about the self, the environment, and
problematic situations encountered in everyday life. Problem Measuring Social Problem Solving
orientation, or attitude toward problem solving, involves the
ability to (a) ward off negative emotions (e.g., anxiety, de- Before beginning the intervention, we first determine each
pression, and anger) that hamper problem-solving efforts, caregiver’s problem orientation and specific problem-solving
(b) promote positive emotions and a sense of competency skills using the Social Problem Solving Inventory–Revised
that facilitate problem solving, and (c) motivate an individual (SPSI–R; D’Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu-Olivares, 1995). A
toward solving problems (D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1990; D’Zurilla caregiver rates the extent to which each of 52 statements is
& Sheedy, 1991; Nezu & D’Zurilla, 1989). Persons with a true of him or her on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all
positive problem orientation often resolve everyday prob- true of me, to 4 = extremely true of me). The measure has five
lems without much effort, preventing these problems from scales: (a) Positive Problem Orientation (e.g., “Whenever I
exacerbating. Consequently, a sense of competency and have a problem, I believe that it can be solved”); (b) Nega-
self-efficacy is reinforced as the individual recalls these tive Problem Orientation (e.g., “When my first efforts to solve
successful experiences when more complex problems are a problem fail, I get discouraged and depressed”); (c) Ratio-
encountered. In contrast, a negative problem orientation is nal Problem-Solving Skills (e.g., “Before I try to solve a prob-
associated with ineffectual coping, negative emotional lem, I set a specific goal so that I know exactly what I want
experiences under general and stressful conditions, and to accomplish”); (d) Impulsivity/Carelessness (e.g., “When I
more critical views of the self and personal abilities (Elliott, have a decision to make, I do not take the time to consider
Herrick, MacNair, & Harkins, 1994; Elliott, Sherwin, the pros and cons of each option”); and (e) Avoidance (e.g.,
Harkins, & Marmarosh, 1995), which tend to reinforce a “I wait to see if a problem will resolve itself first before try-
negative orientation and impair problem-solving efforts ing to solve it myself”) (D’Zurilla & Chang, 1995). Scores on
(Nezu, 1987; Nezu & D’Zurilla, 1989). A negative problem all five scales are then examined as a profile by which the
orientation is also associated with more health complaints clinician can understand the caregiver’s attitude toward prob-
(Elliott & Marmarosh, 1994). Problem-solving skills encom- lem solving and tendencies when problem solving.
pass the specific, goal-directed strategies by which individuals
Card Sort Procedures
define problems, gather facts about problems, generate alter-
natives, decide on solutions, and implement and monitor After measuring each caregiver’s problem-solving orienta-
problem-solving strategies, according to the original model tion and skills, two unique card-sorting tasks are used to
of D’Zurilla and Goldfried (1971). determine which areas may be problems for the caregiver
Effective problem solving has been related to greater use and his or her loved one. The card-sorting (or Q-sort) task
of instrumental problem-focused coping in times of stress used in our intervention is based on 24 problems identified
(MacNair & Elliott, 1992) and an assertive interpersonal by a focus group of 7 caregivers (4 Caucasian women, 2
style (Elliott, Godshall, Herrick, Witty, & Spruell, 1991). African American women, and 1 African American man;
Effective problem-solvers might experience fewer health specific information and procedures concerning development
problems because they may have a more proactive, consci- of these cards may be obtained from the second author or
entious style of coping. Furthermore, care recipients who the following publications: Elliott & Shewchuk, 1999, 2000).
are with caregivers who possess many impulsive and careless A single problem is listed on a card for a total of 24 cards.
problem-solving tendencies have more difficulty accepting Each caregiver in our project is asked to examine the deck
their condition and are at risk for developing preventable of 24 cards and place all 24 cards into different piles based
health complications over time (Elliott, Shewchuk, & on their perceived similarities. The caregiver is instructed
Richards, 1999). In addition, when caregivers of persons to separate them into as many or as few piles as desired
with recent-onset SCI have a negative orientation toward and to move cards into different piles as necessary until
problem solving, they have more problems with depres- satisfied with how the cards are arranged. The caregiver is
sion, anxiety, and ill health throughout the first year of then asked to give each group a name so that the counselor
caregiving, regardless of the actual demands of caregiving can understand the unique theme of each group and what
or level of physical impairment of the care recipient (Elliott, separates each group from the others. This technique en-
Shewchuk, & Richards, in press). courages the caregiver to think about how problems might
The effectiveness of problem-solving training has been overlap or share similar characteristics. Understanding the
documented in treatment of clinical depression among similarities between problems may be helpful for generat-
adults (Nezu & Perri, 1989) and elderly individuals (Arean ing possible solutions that may have been used successfully
et al., 1993), suicidal ideation (Lerner & Clum, 1990), dis- in other similar situations.
tress among persons with chronic illness (Roberts et al., A second method of card sorting is also used in which
1995), and in improving self-management skills of under- each caregiver is asked to rank order on a continuum from
graduates (Richards & Perri, 1978). This model has also 1 (most problematic) to 7 (least problematic) the 24 cards as
been successful in alleviating distress among family they apply to the caregiver’s unique situation. All 24 cards

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 277


Kurylo, Elliott, and Shewchuk

are used, and the caregiver is instructed to place a certain concrete terms, separate facts from assumptions, and dif-
number of cards at each level of the continuum (2 cards ferentiate relevant from irrelevant information.
under #1 and #7, 3 cards under #2 and #6, 4 cards under #3 Within this phase of training, the caregiver is assisted in ar-
and #5, and the remaining 6 cards under #4). This forced- ticulating a specific, attainable goal in an effort to overcome
choice format is used to determine which item(s) is (are) the identified problem. The counselor encourages realistic goals
the most problematic and may need to be explored further and discourages “perfect” goals. The caregiver is assisted in
during the problem-solving training. This method of card determining potential obstacles to meeting the established
sorting has many advantages. It requires a new caregiver goal—solutions to these obstacles will also be generated in the
to consider problems that may be outside immediate creativity phase. If the problem situation is complex (i.e., in-
awareness or may help the caregiver to label a particular volves several problem areas that are subsumed under one
experience as a problem. This technique also allows the large problem situation, such as several smaller interpersonal
caregiver to talk with the counselor about problems that conflicts that have an effect on one’s relationship with an-
he or she may have been previously unwilling or unpre- other person), the caregiver is encouraged by the counselor to
pared to discuss (Brown, 1986; Elliott & Shewchuk, 2000). focus on one aspect of the situation at a time because smaller,
The counselor may use this approach to help the caregiver more discrete goals are typically easier to solve.
prioritize problems that need immediate attention from
those that are less urgent. This technique also allows the Optimism/Orientation
caregiver to consider problems that may arise in the future
and plan how they wish to prevent these problems from The primary goal in this segment in the problem-solving
occurring or lessen the problems’ potential impact. The training paradigm is to assist the caregiver in developing a
caregiver can then plan for potential consequences of and sense of optimism regarding his or her abilities to problem
obstacles to his or her goals. solve. This includes instilling a belief that one is sufficiently
skilled to solve the problem and instilling a sense of motivation
Problem-Solving Training to engage in the problem-solving process while simulta-
neously regulating emotional experiences to maintain a sense
Problem-solving training for the caregiver includes five of confidence. Often, when the counselor recognizes the im-
major components (adapted from the original problem-solving portance and breadth of the caregiver’s role, the caregiver
model proposed by D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971): (a) facts/ begins to feel a sense of pride in accomplishments to date,
problem definition, (b) optimism/orientation, (c) creativity/ which motivates continuation in this training process. The
generation of alternatives, (d) understanding/decision making, caregiver is asked to provide an example of recent per-
and (e) solve/implementation and verification. This training sonal successes in problem solving and to consider the mo-
protocol also parallels the protocol for problem-solving tivational characteristics of a personal role model who is
training for depression outlined in Nezu, Nezu, and Perri an effective problem solver. Furthermore, the caregiver
(1989). It is important to note that the five components can feel more optimistic regarding problem solving with
should be viewed as a continuous and interlinking process the recognition that (a) problems are a common (“nor-
rather than five stages to be followed in serial order. For mal”) part of everyone’s life, (b) many other caregivers
example, the counselor may decide to begin with the optimism have similar problems, (c) problems can be predicted and
component of the model when working with a caregiver prevented, and (d) there have been personal problem-solving
who is less motivated (as determined through the SPSI–R successes in his or her past. The idea that optimism must
assessment device) and continuously address the optimism be accompanied with realism is stressed. In other words,
component throughout the training process. the caregiver should have realistic expectations about the
time and effort that will be necessary to identify and use
THE FIVE COMPONENTS OF FOCUS chosen strategies, and the caregiver should not expect to
“move mountains.”
Facts/Problem Definition
In addition to the aforementioned, the individual is assisted
This component involves description of a problem that was in recognizing a problem when it occurs and thereby recog-
identified as such in the card-sorting procedures. The caregiver nizing when problem solving is applicable. One manner
is asked to articulate the specific problem and then break it in which this can be accomplished is through recognition
down into manageable parts. The caregiver is instructed to of emotional cues (e.g., anger, sadness, frustration, irritability)
seek all available facts about the problem in an effort to that a problem exists. We call these cues “red flags” or signs
answer who, what, when, where, why, and how of the situ- that the person should be problem solving. The caregiver is
ation. The caregiver is also instructed to ask experts for encouraged not to be impulsive when solving problems and
information regarding the problem situation if necessary. For not to avoid solving problems because both of these processes
example, if the problem situation concerns a constellation of usually complicate the initial problem. Specifically, we dis-
physical symptoms, the caregiver would be encouraged to cuss that impulsive attempts often result in careless mistakes.
contact the physician’s office. When describing the problem, The caregiver is encouraged to take the time necessary to
the caregiver is encouraged to give the facts in unambiguous, analyze situations, to tolerate discomfort when solutions are

278 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


FOCUS on the Family Caregiver

not immediate, and to withhold judgment until the problem- caregiver is instructed to think of as many solutions to the
solving process is complete. problem as possible and write each one down on a worksheet.
After discussing the aspects of a positive problem orienta- When the caregiver reaches an impasse, the counselor sug-
tion at length with the caregiver, the counselor leads the gests solutions. It is crucial that judgments are withheld at this
caregiver in an exercise designed to increase motivation for point in the process, which means that “silly” or “impossible”
solving problems. One exercise we use is a reversed role-play solutions count equally as potential solutions. The caregiver is
scenario. Specifically, the counselor adopts a maladaptive prob- reinforced for deferring judgment and for giving as many al-
lem orientation, and the caregiver is asked to successfully ternative strategies as possible. The caregiver is encouraged to
counter-argue the attitudes presented by the counselor (e.g., be as specific and concrete as possible in generating strategies.
“I don’t have any problems at all,” “Only someone who is The following techniques are offered to increase the number
experiencing the exact same problem can be helpful to me; of items generated: (a) combining different solutions together
no one else can understand,” “I should avoid making decisions to make another solution, (b) changing a solution slightly to
no matter what the consequences”). By counter-arguing the create more solutions (adding or subtracting from it), (c) imag-
attitudes, the caregiver can develop skills to use to counter- ining oneself and his or her role model(s) in the actual
argue personal attitudes that may decrease motivation and situation(s) and considering what each would do and say in
impede problem solving at a later time. the situation, and (d) recalling potential solutions to other simi-
When working with individuals who impulsively solve prob- lar problems that were evaluated during the card-sorting pro-
lems (as assessed with the SPSI–R), a different technique is cedure. Although there is no required number of solutions,
used. The caregiver is instructed to recognize the occurrence the caregiver is instructed that having more options will
of a problem situation and label the situation as a problem. increase optimism about his or her ability to solve problems.
This can help deter the tendency to react automatically. First,
the caregiver is asked to generate a list of areas in life in which Understanding/Decision Making
problems occur (caregiving, relationships, job, children and
After having generated several options, it is now time for
education, etc.). Then the caregiver creates a second list that
the caregiver to integrate all the information gathered to
includes specific current problems as well as predicted areas
this point and consider which solutions to implement. Be-
of vulnerability unique to the caregiver.
fore deciding on a solution, the caregiver is encouraged to
For those individuals who are typically unassertive when
consider the potential outcomes of the chosen solutions
problems arise, we use a method to help the caregiver to
and weigh the costs and benefits of each. This is done by
recognize “red flags” as cues to the existence of problems.
rating each alternative as it relates to feasibility, achieving
In this exercise, the caregiver and counselor generate a list
goals, perceived fit between the option and the problem at
of feelings and explore how the feelings serve as cues for
hand, and coping with obstacles. This step is the essence of
the presence of a problem. The caregiver is encouraged to
the “stop and think” paradigm, in which the problem solver
stop and think when these emotions are detected and to
weighs the pros and cons of each solution alternative be-
avoid reacting automatically.
fore proceeding with implementation. Finally, the caregiver
Once the caregiver is familiar with the problem-solving
rates the likelihood that he or she will implement each so-
process, the person is asked to provide a brief written de-
lution alternative (0 = not at all likely, 1 = somewhat likely, 2
scription of the aforementioned recent problem situation
= very likely). Recall of the facts identified earlier in the
that was articulated in the Facts/Problem Definition seg-
process may be beneficial in processing information during
ment. This exercise emphasizes how the first two segments
this segment. Of course, maintaining a positive problem
of the model intersect. Then the caregiver is instructed to
orientation is also crucial during this phase because the
indicate how he or she initially reacted to the situation (both
caregiver will be most likely to implement those solutions
emotionally and behaviorally) and describe how those re-
about which he or she feels most optimistic.
actions compare with a positive problem orientation. This
illustrates to the caregiver how negative emotions can be Solve/Implementation and Verification
used as a cue that a problem exists. It also provides an op-
portunity for the caregiver to understand what aspects of a The final phase in the problem-solving training process is
positive problem orientation will be most useful in main- the act of solving the problem and then systematically review-
taining a positive outlook through the problem-solving pro- ing the outcome to determine how the solution worked and
cess, given the available facts of the problem situation. the degree to which the actual outcome approximates the
expected one. Because the counselor is not typically present
during this stage, the caregiver is requested to provide feed-
Creativity/Generation of Alternatives
back at the next meeting with the counselor. The caregiver is
This portion of the problem-solving training involves actively asked to evaluate how effective his or her solution was in
brainstorming multiple solutions to the identified problem. solving the problem and to identify the actual effects on each
Because a caregiver may believe that there is only one correct individual involved in the problem situation and solution. This
answer or may tend to solve the problem with the first idea self-monitoring component is crucial to promote under-
that comes to mind, training in this area is critical. First, the standing about what made the chosen solution effective

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 279


Kurylo, Elliott, and Shewchuk

or ineffective and how to implement similar or alternative problems of a caregiver. The caregiver is empowered with
solutions in future problem situations. This component builds self-care skills and the ability to handle situations that arise
self-esteem, self-efficacy, and self-empowerment. in an effective fashion; the person seeks more information
when needed and takes a proactive role in all aspects of the
FINAL POINTS OF CONSIDERATION caregiver experience. These skills may promote indepen-
dence from formal health care systems and empower
This five-phase problem-solving training process requires caregivers and their family members to achieve a greater
approximately 2 to 3 hours to complete during an initial level of wellness and self-sufficiency.
meeting with a caregiver. Manuals are available that de-
scribe the use of this intervention with other groups (e.g., REFERENCES
terminal cancer patients and their family members) and
Arean, P. A., Perri, M., Nezu, A., Schein, R., Christopher, F., & Joseph, T.
provide informative examples (D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1999;
(1993). Comparative effectiveness of social problem-solving therapy
Nezu, Nezu, Friedman, Faddis, & Houts, 1998; Nezu, Nezu, as treatments for depression in older adults. Journal of Consulting and
& Perri, 1989). In the Project FOCUS model, our training Clinical Psychology, 61, 1003–1010.
with caregivers also includes (a) regular telephone contacts Brown, S. R. (1986). Q-technique and method: Principles and procedures. In W.
with the caregivers emphasizing and reinforcing effective D. Berry & M. S. Lewis-Beck (Eds.), New tools for social scientists: Advances and
applications in research methods (pp. 57–76). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
problem solving each month during the year; (b) regular Chwalisz, K. (1996). The perceived stress model of caregiver burden:
mailings of informative and instructive materials over the Evidence from the spouses of persons with brain injuries. Rehabilitation
year to the caregiver; and (c) follow-up evaluations and Psychology, 41, 91–114.
“booster sessions” at 6 months and 12 months to reassess D’Zurilla, T. J., & Chang, E. C. (1995). The relations between social prob-
problem-solving orientation and skills, and level of distress lem solving and coping. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 547–562.
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. (1971). Problem solving and behavior
(i.e., depressed mood, anxiety), and to retrain and reinitiate modification. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 104–126.
the caregiver as necessary. These components reinforce use D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1990). Development and preliminary evalu-
of effective problem-solving principles throughout the year. ation of the Social Problem Solving Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 2,
The caregiving career seems to follow a dynamic course 156–163.
over the first year after an injury to a family member D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1999). Problem-solving therapy: A social
competence approach to clinical intervention (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
(Shewchuk, Richards, & Elliott, 1998). Therefore, we dis- D’Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (1995). Manual for
tribute problem-solving training over the duration of a year the Social Problem Solving Inventory–Revised. Unpublished manuscript,
rather than over the course of several weeks, as suggested State University of New York at Stony Brook.
by other social problem-solving researchers (Nezu & Perri, D’Zurilla, T. J., & Sheedy, C. (1991). Relation between social problem-
solving ability and subsequent level of psychological stress in college
1989). In addition, our approach considers the immediate
students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 841–846.
and specific problems reported by each individual caregiver Elliott, T., Godshall, F., Herrick, S., Witty, T., & Spruell, M. (1991). Prob-
and matches the intervention with the problems unique to lem-solving appraisal and psychological adjustment following spinal cord
each caregiver (Elliott & Shewchuk, 2000). This can pro- injury. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 387–389.
mote self-empowerment among caregivers and foster a sense Elliott, T., Herrick, S., MacNair, R., & Harkins, S. (1994). Personality cor-
relates of self-appraised problem-solving ability: Problem orientation
of pride and accomplishment. and trait affectivity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 489–505.
We believe that family caregivers of persons who receive Elliott, T., & Marmarosh, C. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal, health
training in problem solving will have greater well-being, will complaints, and health-related expectancies. Journal of Counseling &
be more likely to use rational problem-solving skills, and will Development, 72, 531–537.
report less distress. By extension, persons with severe physi- Elliott, T., Sherwin, E., Harkins, S., & Marmarosh, C. (1995). Self-appraised
problem solving ability, affective states, and psychological distress. Jour-
cal disabilities whose caregivers are trained in problem solv- nal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 105–115.
ing may likely have fewer secondary complications in the Elliott, T., & Shewchuk, R. (1998). Recognizing the family caregiver: Inte-
first year following the injury and thus require less medical gral and formal members of the rehabilitation process. Journal of Voca-
attention. We intend to promote health, well-being, and in- tional Rehabilitation, 10, 123–132.
dependence among caregivers and their family members. Elliott, T., & Shewchuk, R. (1999). Using focus groups to understand the
experience of family caregivers and care recipients. Manuscript submitted
for publication.
CONCLUSION Elliott, T., & Shewchuk, R. (2000). Problem-solving therapy for family
caregivers of persons with severe physical disabilities. In C. Radnitz
Counselors are uniquely qualified to assist caregivers in (Ed.), Cognitive-behavioral interventions for persons with disabilities
(pp. 309–327). New York: Aronson.
maximizing their health and well-being through individu- Elliott, T., Shewchuk, R., & Richards, J. S. (1999). Caregiver social problem-
alized cognitive-behavioral intervention provided by the solving abilities and family member adjustment to recent-onset physical
Project FOCUS model. The model illustrates that caregivers disability. Rehabilitation Psychology, 44, 104–123.
can benefit from problem-solving training that enhances Elliott, T., Shewchuk, R., & Richards, J. S. (in press). Family caregiver social
problem-solving abilities and adjustment during the initial year of the
their ability to regulate emotional experiences when en-
caregiving role. Journal of Counseling Psychology.
countering problems and provides them with the cogni- Ginter, E. J. (1999). David K. Brooks’ contribution to the developmen-
tive-behavioral skills for effective problem solving. In using tally based life-skills approach. Journal of Mental Health Counseling,
this model, counselors consider the immediate and specific 21, 191–202.

280 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


FOCUS on the Family Caregiver

Greenberg, J. S. (1985). Health and wellness: A conceptual differentiation. Nezu, A. M., & D’Zurilla, T. (1989). Social problem solving and negative
Journal of School Health, 55, 403–406. affect. In L. P. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinc-
Haley, W. E., Levine, E., Brown, S. L., & Bartolucci, A. (1987). Stress, ap- tive and overlapping features (pp. 285–315). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
praisal, coping and social support as predictors of adaptational outcome Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., Friedman, S. H., Faddis, S., & Houts, P. S. (1998).
among dementia caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 2, 323–330. Helping cancer patients cope: A problem-solving approach. Washington, DC:
Haley, W. E., Waff, D. L., Coleton, M. I., Ford, G. R., West, C. A. C., Collins, American Psychological Association.
R. P., & Isobe, T. L. (1996). Appraisal, coping, and social support as mediators Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., & Perri, M. G. (1989). Problem-solving therapy for
of well-being in Black and White family caregivers of patients with Alzheimer’s depression: Theory, research, and clinical guidelines. New York: Wiley.
disease. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 121–129. Nezu, A. M., & Perri, M. G. (1989). Social problem-solving therapy for uni-
Haley, W. E., West, C. A. C., Wadley, V. G., Ford, G. F., White, F. A., Barrett, J. polar depression: An initial dismantling investigation. Journal of Consulting
J., Harrell, L. E., & Waff, D. L. (1995). Psychological, social, and health and Clinical Psychology, 57, 408–413.
impact of caregiving: A comparison of Black and White dementia family Quittner, A. L., Glueckauf, R. L., & Jackson, D. (1990). Chronic parenting
caregivers and non-caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 10, 540–552. stress: Moderating versus mediating effects of social support. Journal of
Houts, P. S., Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., & Bucher, J. A. (1996). The prepared Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1266–1278.
family caregiver: A problem-solving approach to family caregiver educa- Quittner, A. L., Opipari, L., Regoli, M., Jacobsen, J., & Eigen, H. (1992). The
tion. Patient Education and Counseling, 27, 63–73. impact of caregiving and role strain on family life: Comparisons between
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Dura, J. R., Speicher, C. E., Trask, O. J., & Glaser, R. mothers of children with cystic fibrosis and matched controls. Rehabilita-
(1991). Longitudinal changes in immunity and health. Psychosomatic Medi- tion Psychology, 37, 275–290.
cine, 53, 345–362. Richards, C. S., & Perri, M. G. (1978). Do self-control treatments last? An evalu-
King, A. C., Oka, R. K., & Young, D. R. (1994). Ambulatory blood pressure ation of behavioral problem solving and faded counselor contact as treat-
and heart rate responses to the stress of work and caregiving in older women. ment maintenance strategies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25, 376–383.
Journals of Gerontology, 49, 239–245. Roberts, J., Brown, G. B., Streiner, D., Gafni, A., Pallister, R., Hoxby, H.,
Lerner, M. S., & Clum, G. A. (1990). Treatment of suicidal ideators: A prob- Drummond-Young, M., LeGris, J., & Meichenbaum, D. (1995). Problem solv-
lem-solving approach. Behavior Therapy, 21, 403–411. ing counseling or phone-call support for outpatients with chronic illness:
MacNair, R. R., & Elliott, T. (1992). Self-perceived problem-solving ability, Effective for whom? Canadian Journal of Nursing Research, 27, 111–137.
stress appraisal, and coping over time. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, Shewchuk, R. M., Richards, J. S., & Elliott, T. (1998). Dynamic processes in
150–164. health outcomes among caregivers of patients with spinal cord injuries.
Miaskowski, C., Kragness, L., Dibble, S., & Wallhagen, M. (1997). Differences Health Psychology, 17, 125–129.
in mood states, health status, and caregiver strain between family caregivers Vitaliano, P. P. (Ed.). (1997). Physiological and physical concomitants of
of oncology outpatients with and without cancer-related pain. Journal of caregiving: Introduction to special issue. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19,
Pain and Symptom Management, 13, 138–147. 75–77.
Myers, J. E. (1989). Adult children and aging parents. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt. Whiting, R. A., Terry, L., & Strom-Hendriksen, H. (1984). From home
Myers, J. E. (1991). Wellness as the paradigm for counseling and development: to college, from college to home: An interactional approach to treat-
The possible future. Counselor Education and Supervision, 30, 183–193. ing the symptomatic disabled college student. In J. C. Hansen (Ed.),
Myers, J. E. (1992). Wellness, prevention, development: The cornerstone of Families with handicapped members (pp. 30–43). Rockville, MD:
the profession. Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 136–139. Aspen.
Nezu, A. M. (1987). A problem solving formulation for depression: A litera- Witmer, J. M., & Sweeney, T. J. (1992). A holistic model for wellness and
ture review and proposal of a pluralistic model. Clinical Psychology Review, prevention over the life span. Journal of Counseling & Development,
7, 121–144. 71, 140–148.

APPENDIX
Project FOCUS
Facts: Get the facts! First, define the problem. When you you can solve the problem (both old and new ways). You
have determined what the specific problem is, your goal will be determining later which ideas may work best, but
becomes solving that problem. In order to determine the for now, think of as many as you can.
necessary steps leading to your goal (i.e., solving the prob- Understanding: Do you understand what the problem is?
lem), you will need to decide what information (facts) you Understanding the problem, the facts about the problem,
already have to assist you in solving the problem, and de- and the possible solutions to the problem will assist you in
cide what else you need to solve the problem. You may solving the problem. It is also important to know and
need to ask an expert (e.g., nurse, physical therapist, men- understand the possible consequences (both positive and
tal health practitioner, physician) in order to get the neces- negative) for each of the ideas of how to solve the problem
sary information (facts). Utilize the experts. We are here to that you thought of previously. Finally, it will also be
assist you. No question is a dumb question. important to have an understanding of the difficulties, or
Optimism: Be optimistic, but realistic. You will feel better potential obstacles, involved in reaching your goal.
about yourself and your ability to problem-solve when you Solve: Solving the problem is the final step. Given the
are hopeful and positive. Think of a role model who handles preceding information (facts, ideas of how to solve the prob-
problems effectively (either someone you know personally— lem, and understanding), you can develop a plan of action
a friend or relative—or know about—president, hero). How (e.g., steps), which will lead to your goal, as you outlined
would they feel in this situation? Think: “I CAN do this!” above. Then solve the problem and see the outcome. Does
Creativity: Be creative in your ideas! What other ways the outcome match your expectations, your goals? If not,
can you solve the problem that you have not thought of go back to your list of ideas and try another idea. Try not to
before? Brainstorm! Think of as many ways as possible that be impulsive. Stop and Think.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 281


Where Has Our Theory Gone? Learning Theory
and Intentional Intervention

Sterling Gerber

Reflecting on the principle that every effective counseling intervention works to the extent that it incorporates sound learning
principles, this article emphasizes intentional selection of intervention strategies with reference to 4 learning models: association,
reinforcement, cognition-perceptual, and cognition-rational/linguistic. Three contexts—developmental, social, and spiritual—are
described that require modification of intervention strategy, and a case example is given.

C
ounselor educators and practitioners who were theory). The number and complexity of theories provided a
educated in the 60s will remember the tri- rich array of positions to work within. To the neophyte,
chotomy of choices relative to philosophical selecting a preferred position from among them was nearly
and theoretical foundations. There existed the impossible. As a result, many counselors resolved their pro-
“directive counseling” school of thought, fessional identity crisis by adopting the eclectic label and
whose major proponent was E. G. Williamson (1939, 1950). assumed that they could extract methods and procedures from
This position rested firmly in the guidance tradition of any source and integrate them into a workable practice.
defining the client as one who was deficient or limited Early in my professional formation, I received two impera-
regarding some knowledge, skill, or wisdom in which the tives that made it difficult to follow the zeitgeist and become
counselor was proficient. The task of the counselor was to eclectic. One was the Gestalt maxim that “meaning comes
ascertain that deficiency and to prescribe or direct infor- from context” and, as an extension of that, the power of any
mation or procedures to rectify the problem. counseling method (now named intervention procedure or
The “nondirective counseling” school was strongly in strategy) comes from its theoretical context. The second was
vogue, due largely to the work of Carl Rogers (1942, 1951, the directive given by an influential professor, “Do not be-
1961). Built on the premise that clients are basically ca- come eclectic. Believe in something!” If eclecticism involved
pable and socialized and that the source of their self-defeat taking various methods out of their theoretical context, and
lies largely in perceptual distortion or “defensiveness,” the indeed this is what Thorne prescribed, then they existed in a
procedure of choice was to create an atmosphere of uncon- diluted, weak, or powerless form. If the power rests in the
ditional positive regard. As the client would disclose infor- theoretical context, then it is conceivable that two methods
mation of a potentially risky nature and be met with a taken from opposing contexts would cancel each other.
nonjudgmental response, he or she was helped to broaden The apparent dilemma was to choose from a restricted
the perceptual frame of reference and attain a more realis- number of theory-based counseling approaches that seemed
tic, nondefensive view of reality. Such expansion of per- to be limited in their scope of application or to become
ception freed the client to do those things that were neces- eclectic, which was “forbidden.” This forced a third option,
sary and acceptable to the client and generally were already one that was not readily apparent. It is possible to use vari-
existent within his or her repertoire. ous intervention strategies in their appropriate context—
The third option was called “eclectic counseling” and was theory pure—by selecting from among extant theory bases
put forth by Frederic Thorne (1950). It grew out of the the particular one that shows the closest match to client
observation that rigid adherence to any theoretical position circumstance and style. It will have a higher probability of
precluded the flexibility to fit the broad range of client successful resolution of client concerns than one that does
personalities and problems that were encountered by most not fit and higher than an amalgamation of methods that is
practitioners. Counselor education curricula typically had applied out of context. The awareness of strength in match-
several components including developmental theories, per- ing intervention to specific client characteristics is not new.
sonality theories, learning theories, counseling theories, and Over the past several years a trend has developed, that of
assessment procedures (which rested heavily on psychometric integrative metaframeworks (see for example Mueller,

Sterling Gerber is a professor in the Counseling, Educational, and Developmental Psychology Department at Eastern Washington University, Cheney. Corre-
spondence regarding this article should be sent to Sterling Gerber, Department of Counseling, Educational, and Developmental Psychology, Martin Hall 135,
Eastern Washington University, Cheney, WA 99004-2437 (e-mail: Sterling.Gerber@mail.ewu.edu).

282 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention

Dupuy, & Hutchins, 1995; Preston, 1998). What is new with


this model is its reliance on learning theory as the basis for Class Family Context
making decisions regarding intervention strategies.
One Stage
An immediate cry arises, “It is highly impractical if not Association Selection of
impossible to become proficient in all the theory-based basic
approaches. There are just too many of them. If I select a Reinforcement intervention
Two Stage
few or several, what are the chances that those I select will Cognition–
strategy
match my client population?” according to
Perceptual which family
There is an answer to that concern. Although it requires client circum-
that counselors learn more than one theory approach (thus Cognition– stance and
Rational/
avoiding the limitations of theory-pure discipleship), it per- Linguistic
style fit.
mits avoidance of the theory-poor practice of eclecticism. Many
professional counselors, accustomed to labeling themselves as
eclectic, bristle at the implication of “less than okay” proce-
dures, yet their uses of intervention strategies have evolved, Modifications to the
from experience or intent, to incorporate those that tend to application of the Developmental
be complementary. The intent is not to criticize what may be strategy result from
aspects of client Social
a workable frame or to quibble over the meaning of the term contexts.
eclectic. Rather it is to explicate what is seen as a cognitive Spiritual
structure that will enhance intentionality and effectiveness.
In addition to the premise that the power of an interven-
tion comes from its theoretical context, it is necessary to FIGURE 1
recognize that all client change comes as the result of learn-
ing. Another way of phrasing this principle is that all coun- Selecting Intervention Strategy to Match Client
seling interventions work because they exist in the context Circumstance and Style
of a learning theory. Given this observation, the number of
positions needed to deal with all clients is reduced from all
counseling, personality, development and learning theories The second and confounding contemporary force is the
to “just” all learning theories. Even so, this task is more than diminution of emphasis on theory, particularly learning
daunting. Although possible, it still remains impractical. theory, in counselor preparation programs. Referring to an
early draft on revision of the Council for Accreditation of
Counseling and Related Educational Programs (CACREP;
A LEARNING THEORY BASE FOR INTERVENTION 1998) standards, there were eight curricular areas: Human
A careful study of learning theories and of counseling in- Growth and Development, Social and Cultural Foundations,
terventions interpreted from their learning theory under- Helping Relationships, Groups, Career and Lifestyle De-
pinnings results in a blending of many seemingly diverse velopment, Appraisal, Research and Program Evaluation,
positions into two classes, four families, and three contexts and Professional Orientation. The two areas that might be
(see Figure 1). The two classes are one-stage and two-stage related to theory, Human Growth and Development and
models. The four families are association, reinforcement, cog- Helping Relationships, did not specify learning theory.
nition-perceptual, and cognition-rational/linguistic. The con-
Human Growth and Development—studies that provide an under-
texts are developmental, social, and spiritual. It is possible standing of the nature and needs of individuals at all developmental
for a counselor to learn at least one approach within each levels. Studies in this area include, but are not limited to the follow-
of these four families, with awareness of modifications to ing: a. theories of human development across the life-span; b. major
fit the contexts, and integrate them into a repertoire rich theories of personality development; c. human behavior including
an understanding of developmental crises, disability, psychopa-
enough to match essentially all of his or her clients.
thology, and cultural factors as they affect both normal and abnormal
Two contemporary forces in counselor education create a behavior.
major inconsistency and challenge. The first is a societal press Helping Relationships—studies that provide an understanding
for accountability that has spawned an emphasis on inten- of counseling and consultation processes. Studies in this area in-
tionality in the selection and carrying out of intervention clude, but are not limited to the following: a. helping skills, and
counseling and consultation theories including coverage of relevant
strategies (Gerber, 1999). It is only through knowing, in research and factors considered in applications; b. counselor or con-
advance, the unique aspects of the client concerns, patterns sultant characteristics and behaviors that influence helping processes
of self-defeating behavior, and preferred patterns of reacting including gender and ethnicity differences, verbal and nonverbal
(client circumstance and style) that an appropriate interven- behaviors and personal characteristics, orientations, and skills; and
tion can be selected. Matching the client circumstance and c. client or consultee characteristics and behaviors that influence
helping processes including gender and ethnicity differences, ver-
style to a particular family of learning-theory-based interven- bal and nonverbal behaviors and personal characteristics, traits,
tion permits rational and intentional selection of methods capabilities, life circumstances, and developmental level. (CACREP,
that have a high probability of working. 1998, p. 4)

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 283


Gerber

Subsequent iterations of the accreditation standards restored are inferred from systematic observation of differences in SàR
some reference to learning theory. That it was ever in question trials. It can be said that S causes R, or that S cues the person
is instructive. Another factor in this equation is the experi- to give the predictable R. This occurs as a unitary process, a
ence of professors in finding textual materials for teaching single stage, triggered by the stimulus. Another model says
that subject. Many, if not most texts, communicate a narrow- that the probability of a particular response is enhanced or
ing focus to one of research and academic interest (see diminished by conditions in the person, SàOàR. This also is
Hergenhahn & Olson, 1993). The problem of translating theory a single-stage model. The button is pushed and the machine
to practice is aggravated by teaching materials and program responds. Even though the response might be one of lowered
guidelines that treat learning theory as an esoteric, if necessary intensity because of “dirty bearings” or “frayed connections,” it
at all, component of counselor training. still is a specific response to a particular stimulus.
Frequently the profession is in the “schizoid” position of The probability of a specific stimulus eliciting a particu-
telling its trainees to be more accountable and sending them lar response can sometimes be affected by what follows
out without the cognitive and perceptual preparation to the response. This formula is SàRàS. The change in stimu-
permit them to do the job correctly (see Gladding, 1996; lus condition, subsequent to and consequent on the re-
Lawless, Ginter, & Kelly, 1999). One mark of a professional sponse, acts to increase the probability of recurrence of the
is the ability to explicate the rationale for his or her proce- response on another presentation of the original stimulus.
dures (Ginter, 1999). Those who prepare professionals re- This also is a single-stage dynamic because the relationship
ally ought to equip them for the task. The remainder of between S and R is a one-step, highly predictable one. Dif-
this article provides an introduction to the classes, families, ferences across observations of different people in similar
and contexts of learning-based, intentional intervention. A stimulus circumstances and in the same individual from
more complete treatment can be found in Enhancing Coun- time to time are explained by organismic changes that in-
selor Intervention Strategies: An Integrational Approach hibit or enhance normal response or by changes in conse-
(Gerber, 1999). quential stimulus conditions that affect the probability of
One problem with learning theory approaches in the past responding, an external locus of control.
is that they were based on a definition of learning that was Another approach for looking at the dynamics across
restrictive or inclusive of only a small portion of human an SàOàR event is to recognize that the person (O) is
learning dynamics. Obviously it is difficult to explain cog- an active agent. This usually requires allowing for the
nitive or perceptual change in associational or reinforce- person’s ability to perceive, think, judge, and decide. This
ment terms and vice versa. Of necessity, the definition of is a two-stage process. The first stage is the occurrence
learning must be broader or integrational in nature. For the of a stimulus and its perceptual impact on the person
purposes of this treatise, learning is considered to be a con- (SàO). What does the person perceive and how clear or
struct, meaning that it is not accessible to direct empirical accurate is that perception? This explains variations in
assessment and, hence, is implied or inferred from observed factors such as attention, sensory receptor differences,
change. This involves changes in behavior, cognition, or both and perceptual set across individuals and in the same in-
in ways of thinking (process) and in results of thinking dividual over time. In effect, the same physical event can
(products)—perception, or affect, including feelings, atti- be perceived at a variance from its physical science prop-
tudes, and values. The definition excludes changes due to erties. The second stage is the decision and action pro-
drugs, fatigue, illness, instinct, and maturational changes cess (OàR). The person takes an action based not on
in readiness. Learning occurs following or as a result of the stimulus but, rather, on the decision he or she has
experience, direct or vicarious; thinking; or insight. It is made. Stage one is stimulus and perception. Stage two is
relatively permanent, usually enhanced by practice, and is decision and action. Differences in individuals’ responses
purposeful for self-maintenance, survival, or prediction or across similar stimulus circumstances and in the same
anticipation of future conditions. Often, there are physi- individual to repetitions of a similar stimulus situation
ological correlates: Learning either causes or results from result from decision, an internal locus of control.
changes in the central nervous system.
Families of Learning Models
Classes of Learning Models
The two classes give rise to four families, two of which are
The symbols SàR are often used to indicate a cause and one-stage models and two are two-stage models. For refer-
effect relationship between stimulus and response. Some ence in this context and as fairly common descriptors in the
theories frame the human as a biological machine that “runs” area of learning theory, these are referred to as association,
according to which “buttons are pushed.” To elicit a certain reinforcement, cognition-perceptual, and cognition-rational/
behavior, you must press a specific button. linguistic. Each is described briefly, including an abbreviated
Sometimes the person is included in the formula: set of directions for application.
SàOàR. This recognizes organismic qualities or charac- Association. Association theories, such as those of Pavlov
teristics that might affect the response. Because the inner and of Guthrie (Sahakian, 1976), have the following com-
workings of a person are closed to direct observation, they mon dynamics:

284 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention

• The stimulus event occurs prior to or at the same time as the 1. Identify the target behavior in terms of discrete units
response and either elicits or cues the response. (countable instances of each or time intervals that
• A connection or bond is established between the stimulus and
response (afferent-efferent neural events) or between two stimuli
include their occurrence)
and a reflexive response (afferent-afferent-efferent neural events). 2. Decide if the target behavior is to occur in a restricted
• Contiguity—the simultaneous or overlapped, or closely proximate context that includes signals (discriminated stimuli)
occurrence of the stimulus and response—is the single and pow- for the target response
erful condition for the bond or association of the two to be made. 3. Establish a baseline (the number of discrete occur-
• Teaching or conditioning is a process of arranging stimulus and
response events to occur contiguously to one another. (Gerber, rences within a specified time)
1999, p. 44; see Endnote) 4. Identify reinforcers (generally small objects or to-
kens that can be managed easily and delivered readily
Two applied approaches that are based in association on occurrence of the target response)
theory are those of Wolpe (1969) and of Bandler and 5. Run the learning trials (repetitions of the Sd à R à
Grinder (1975; Neuro-Linguistic Programming). The inter- Sreinfpattern)
vention will vary depending on which of the two basic as- 6. Perform a criterion test (repetition of the procedure
sociation approaches best fits the client situation. used to establish the baseline).
Generally, it is best to use a Pavlovian-based approach if
the response to be modified or to be brought under differ- Cognition-perceptual. Perception theories, such as
ential stimulus control is reflexive. For a mature client, a Wertheimer (1959), Köhler and Koffka (as cited in Sahakian,
vicarious strategy may be appropriate (à la Wolpe). Some 1976), Tolman (1949), and Bandura (1969, 1974), have the
reflexive retraining can be accomplished by use of a following common dynamics:
Guthrian “threshold” method. If the response is not reflex-
ive, a Guthrian-based approach is preferred. There are two • What a person perceives varies with the properties of physical
stimulation.
applications of Pavlovian-based interventions (see Appen- • The perceptual variations or distortions follow patterns or “rules.”
dix A): (a) bringing an existing reflex under stimulus con- • Perception and understanding are molar events that cannot be
trol and (b) overriding an undesirable conditioned response reduced without altering their meaning.
by substituting a more desirable one to an existing stimu- • Learning is a process of varying perceptual frames until one is
lus. The intitial determination in Guthrian-based interven- accomplished that corresponds to a universal structure.
• Perceptual learning is not incremental; it happens in an all-or-
tions (see Appendix B) is whether (a) a new behavior is to none fashion and is experienced as a revelation.
be conditioned to a specific cue or (b) an existing behavior • Past perceptual units or Gestalts are stored in memory and be-
is to be replaced in a prescribed context. come data for future decisions.
One caveat of an integrational approach is that although • Teaching or conditioning is a process of managing experiences,
it may be possible to explain learning and to manage change real or vicarious, until the learner accomplishes insight or per-
ceptual “homeostasis.” (Gerber, 1999, p. 46)
from more than one family of models, the preferred inter-
vention is the one that is most parsimonious in application. Applied models based in perceptual learning theory include those
It creates the highest probability for change at the least
of Bruner (1966), Seligman (1975), Perls (1969), Watzlawick
expense of time and resources.
(1978), and Rogers (1951). Maslow (1962) also conforms with
Reinforcement. Reinforcement theories, like that of Skinner
these theorists, as do the objects-relations theorists.
(Sahakian, 1976), have the following common dynamics:
Although it may be an oversimplification, applications of cog-
• Learning is a process of the contiguous occurrence of three re-
nitive-perceptual learning can be placed into just two categories:
lated or contingent events: the stimulus, the response, and the framing and reframing. When a client has no prior experience to
consequent stimulus or reinforcer. draw on in a given circumstance (perceptual deficit), the creation
• Specific stimuli that are intended to become controls for spe- of a frame is called for. If the client has a self-defeating frame
cific responses must be discriminated by the learner from among from prior experience (perceptual surplus), that frame needs to
other similar but not identical stimuli.
• The desired response or target behavior must be caused to occur
be loosened and a more functional frame created.
contiguously to the discriminated stimulus; this may be accom- Basic to a perceptual intervention is an awareness of the
plished by selective reinforcement or by the conditioning of suc- client’s circumstance and style. The client’s behavior will flow
cessive approximations of the target response. from his or her unique perceptual awareness, hence it is im-
• A consequent stimulus is any event subsequent to the response portant to discover that frame of reference. Through the use
that has the effect of increasing the probability of the discrimi-
nated stimulus calling out the target response. of active listening, several questions must be answered:
• The consequent stimulus must be caused to occur contiguously
to the target response. • What is the client doing or not doing that is self-defeating?
• Teaching or conditioning is a process of managing the contiguous • What sense do you make of the client’s behavior in the context
relationship of the three contingent events. (Gerber, 1999, p. 45) of his or her situation?
• Is there a perceptual deficit? Is the client trying to do something
for which he or she has no previous experience, either direct or
Although the intricacies of a reinforcement theory strat-
vicarious?
egy can be complex, most can be reduced for analysis and • Does the client’s behavior make sense from the client’s distorted or
management to six steps: incorrect perception? Is the client working from a misleading frame?

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 285


Gerber

• From a perception of the client’s style, what kind(s) of expe- Is the rationale loose, weak, or strong and well-defended? If
rience, direct or vicarious, would be effective in creating or weak, treat the problem as a cognitive deficit. If strong, use a
helping the client to replace a perceptual frame? (Gerber, 1999,
p. 112)
cognitive surplus approach. Work to loosen the structure by
using cognitive dissonance principles. Introduce dissonant
After answering the questions, the counselor selects a strat- information (doubt) thereby producing an “I’m not so sure”
egy that will fit the client’s circumstance and style. In in- state that will be receptive to a new structure—particularly if
stances in which the client is using an ineffective perceptual the new structure eliminates the dissonance.
frame, it is necessary to loosen that frame before introducing The second part of cognitive restructuring requires in-
a preferred perception. This is done through techniques such troduction of a competing structure, the use of which rules
as role reversal, splits, and figure-ground shifts (Gerber, 1999). out the initial structure (mutually exclusive where possible),
Cognition-Rational/Linguistic. Festinger (1957), Försterling and that will work for the client in the particular context.
(1986), and Rokeach (1973) are primary theorists, whereas The pattern of cognitive self-defeat tends to be general-
Ausubel (1963), Ellis (1958, 1962), Beck, Rush, Shaw, and ized across many client responses. Some approaches to
Emery (1979), and Berne (1961) have intervention strategies cognition-rational/linguistic intervention include a teach-
based in cognition-rational/linguistic learning theory. The works ing phase to help the client learn and use the intervention
of all of these have the following dynamics in common: process in his or her own behalf. Instruction and practice in
identifying defeating self-statements or cognitive distor-
• Experience becomes coded and remembered; humans rely to a tions, confronting the illogicality, and substituting better
great extent on language codes. structures are provided.
• Language permits labeling of referents and of experience; it also
permits progressively higher levels of abstraction resulting in Conditions of cognition-rational/linguistic deficit seem
organization or structure of knowledge. unusually easy to remedy. They involve a relatively simple
• People can modify or combine structures to arrive at new and process of teaching the client what is missing. It may be
creative knowledge. that the difficulty comes in recognizing that a deficit occurs.
• Reasoning skills permit analysis of past and present experiences
Because the intervention is so simple, in many situations it is
and prediction of future outcomes.
• Thought precedes action; most behavior results from cognitive used at the outset. If it works, then the deficit has been
analysis of data and from decision. filled. If it does not work, then the problem is more assur-
• Teaching or conditioning is a process of providing (a) standard- edly a cognition-rational/linguistic surplus or an affective
ized labels and/or descriptions for experiences and referents; (b) or behavioral one.
organizational structures of abstractions to enhance memory re-
trieval, reasoning, and classification of new experiences; and (c)
supplanting of inefficient or self-defeating structures with new CONTEXTS FOR APPLICATION OF LEARNING MODELS
ones. (Gerber, 1999, p. 47)
It is relatively easy to identify cognitive deficits or surpluses
Because both cognition-rational/linguistic and cognition- or behavioral deficits or surpluses and apply integrally sound
perceptual approaches are two-stage models that involve strategies to resolve identified problems. There are, how-
mental functions, they have similar patterns of application ever, confounding variables that make the process more
dynamics. Deciding on an appropriate cognition-rational/ complex. People learn their way through life, including both
linguistic intervention will include the following steps. First, successful coping responses and self-defeating or inappro-
in answering the following questions: priate ones. Intervention techniques are teaching techniques
geared to produce self-enhancing learned responses to
• What is the client doing or not doing that is self- replace problematic ones.
defeating? So far, this article has described four major types of learn-
• What is the experiential context of the undesirable ing families, each with different dynamics and each particu-
response? larly useful for producing specific types of response. Because
• What explanation does the client give for his or her people are different one from another in many ways, it is
response? necessary to consider characteristics of individual clients that
require modifications of intervention strategies. These char-
This last question is misleading in its simplicity. In most acteristics may block client receptiveness to a given approach,
cases, the client will not verbalize clearly or accurately the or they may facilitate acceptance of another approach. They
underlying rationale. It has to be deduced by the counselor can be categorized for discussion; however, it is important
from an analysis of the context and the response. The coun- to recognize that each client can be better served when
selor may ask himself or herself, “What rationale logically operating from an understanding of his or her unique cir-
precedes the response in this context?” cumstance and style and his or her context and response to
Is the rationale ill-defined, whimsical, or nonsensical? If that context. Three categories of individual differences—
so, treat the problem as a cognitive deficit and provide a developmental, social, and spiritual dynamics—are considered
cognitive structure that will direct self-enhancing behavior. as contextual issues, things that need to be taken into account
Nonexistent and weak structures are easily replaced by when personalizing an intervention strategy for each particu-
strong, contextually appropriate and logical structures. lar client. Contextual issues are client variables that mandate

286 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention

modification of learning-based intervention strategies. They customs, and traditions create limits on the accessibility of
further define the circumstance and style of the client. client or system to intervention. They also indicate the types
of counselor-initiated activities that are acceptable. That
Developmental Context social context is a critical variable is attested to by Sue, Ivey,
and Pedersen (1996):
As people develop through the various ages and stages of life,
they meet challenges or crises (Gilligan, 1982; Ginter, 1999; A person’s identity is formed and continually influenced by his or
Havighurst, 1949; Levinson, 1978; Sheehy, 1976). Preventive her context. Working effectively with clients requires an under-
mental health is concerned with anticipating and teaching for standing of how the individual is embedded in the family, which in
turn requires an understanding of how the family is affected by its
such transitions. Counseling provides assistance for people who place in a pluralistic culture. . . . Virtually all clients who come for
have difficulty moving through these challenges. help have cultural issues underlying their concerns. (pp. 15–16)
Developmental norms are useful for identifying the prob-
lem dynamics encountered by people at various stages of Consideration of the social context is a critical compo-
progress. They are helpful, too, in clarifying anomalies of nent to effective counseling. This is supported with studies
development that may typify a given client. It is helpful to of group influence, including peer group dominance for
classify the kinds of abilities available to people of different adolescents, family dynamics (Ansbacher & Ansbacher,
ages. Piaget (1952) has shown that there are qualitative dif- 1956), groupthink (Janis, 1967), and the Abilene paradox
ferences in the cognitive abilities of children as they grow (Harvey, 1974). The family is seen by Adler (Ansbacher &
from very young to at least adolescence. Such data support Ansbacher, 1956) to be the primary and most influential
my contention that the intervention of choice for children group for comparison and for establishment of lifestyle.
up to 8 years of age (plus or minus 2 years) and of people Other groupings are of major importance to the identity of
with corresponding mental capacity is either some form of clients. They include gender (Baber, 1992; Beall & Sternberg,
operant conditioning or perceptual framing. 1993; Gilligan, 1982; Minton, 1992; Notman & Nadelson,
In addition to the obvious variation in intervention strat- 1991), racial, cultural (Aponte, Rivers, & Wohl, 1995;
egy dictated by a client who is at the concrete operations Axelson, 1993; Crane, 1997; Dunn & Griggs, 1995; Wehrly,
stage in contrast to the formal operations stage of Piaget 1995), regional (Good & Good, 1986), and socioeconomic
(Ivey & Ivey, 1998; Phillips, 1969), there are similar indica- classifications. Some knowledge of major identity charac-
tors in other models. Consider the shift in motivation and teristics common to such groups is helpful in working with
attentional focus between clients identified in Erickson’s (1980) a particular client.
identity and intimacy stages. The kind of self-statements
invoked in a cognitive restructuring paradigm would be In the past, both psychologists and other people assumed that indi-
likely to differ along the egocentric versus interpersonally viduals’ behavior and achievements reflected their stable, internal
personality traits. In contrast, current research shows that behavior
expansive lines. Likewise, clients identified in the Perry
and achievement, and even our personality traits, are heavily influ-
Scheme (Moore, 1992), a developmental model incorpo- enced by other’s beliefs, perceptions, expectations, and treatment,
rating both cognitive and value dimensions, as operating in as well as by our knowledge of expectations for members of our
a dualistic mind-set, are markedly different from those work- sex. . . . . Both men’s and women’s behavior, achievements, and
ing in contextual relativism. The extent to which they can personality traits are far more quickly responsive to social situ-
ational forces than was previously supposed. (Beall & Sternberg,
be expected to engage in self-directed experiences or to 1993, pp. 33–34)
which they rely on an external source of knowledge and
validation will dictate differences in application of inter- Some readily apparent examples that illustrate the impor-
ventions in each of the families of learning theory. tance of social context sensitivity are clients presenting a “not-
Within any of these developmental models, there are okay” self-image (Harris, 1967) from early family socializa-
adjustments required in the use of in vivo and vicarious or tion, the contrast between self-views and worldviews of fe-
fantasy generated experience and in determining whether male cohorts of age 50 and age 30, and the difference in
a client needs step-by-step modeling and practice of an iden- cultural structure evidenced by a Western focus on individual
tified experience or if he or she can carry out the prescribed responsibility and Eastern or Native American emphases. It
activity after receiving simple instructions. must be remembered that reliance on stereotypical charac-
teristics may blur what is uniquely important about the cli-
Social Context ent. It is necessary to attend to the client’s disclosure and
definition of what his or her affiliations mean to him or her.
Systems theorists contend that it is counterproductive to
isolate the “identified client” for treatment and then intro-
Spiritual Context
duce that client back into an unchanged context (Bateson,
1958; Minuchin, 1974; Minuchin & Fishman, 1981; Ruesch Another aspect of client context is spirituality. This may or
& Bateson, 1968). The system is the client. From the empha- may not include religion, although a large proportion of
sis on multiculturalism (Gazda, Ginter, & Horne, 2001; Sue clients either belong to a church or identify with a group
& Sue, 1999) comes the awareness that group morés, ethics, for whom spirituality is of high interest.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 287


Gerber

Consideration of two major emphases seems warranted: (a) The ex- standing. For more complete explanations and for numer-
tent to which clients define themselves relative to religious prin- ous case examples, the reader is referred to Gerber (1999).
ciples, indoctrination, and practices as evidenced in self-descriptions
such as, “I am a devout Christian” or “I am not a very good Methodist”
One case, paraphrased from the reference text, will serve
or “I am an avid Buddhist” or “My parents/priest would be disappointed as an example of client circumstance and style and of two
in me;” and (b) The reliance on a relationship with a higher power, optional intervention strategies, each based on learning
informally as a support or more formally through prayer or medi- theory and each being internally consistent.
tation. (Gerber, 1999, p. 183) The client is a 10-year-old girl, diagnosed as having a learn-
ing disability. She attends a combination of special education
Membership in a church does not necessarily accompany a and regular classes. Teachers report that she is disruptive,
high degree of religiosity or of spirituality. Sometimes member- nonattentive, and noncompliant. Her personal history indi-
ship may apply more to a social context than a spiritual one.
cates that she was born to a single parent with whom
The large portion of the world population projected to be church she spent the first 8 years. Her mother has a history of
affiliated is sufficient cause to recognize and to legitimize religious substance abuse and had been reported on numerous
or spiritual factors either as determinants or as modifiers of human occasions to Child Protective Services for alleged child
thought, attitude, emotion, and behavior. Even people who profess neglect. At age 9 she began a series of placements in foster
anti-religious or anti-spiritual sentiment are defined, at least in part,
homes, ending with assignment to her maternal grandpar-
by the religious context they oppose. (Gerber, 1999, pp. 183–184)
ents who now have permanent custody. She has a history
If clients do not exist in a spiritual or religious vacuum of malnutrition, although at present her health is normal.
and if their experiences and manifest behavior are influ- In classroom observations, her behavior was generally age
enced by spiritual or religious factors, then it is necessary appropriate. She typically hesitated on introduction to new
that therapists allow for and account for such factors. From activities then became fully invested. Her class disruptions
a counseling standpoint, less so from the scientist’s view- bordered on being “cute,” never so bad as to cause major
point in which it is important to verify the existence of problems and yet too obvious to ignore. Variations in her
spiritual entities and causal relationships between religious attention were random. Noncompliance occurred mostly
practices and results, it is important to recognize the power when she was challenged with misbehavior or when experi-
for direction within the client’s phenomenal experience and encing difficulty with a task. The special education teacher
from the client’s cognitive structure. Whether or not they used standard operant procedures for most tasks. The client
have been empirically isolated and their veracity and power was inconsistent in her responses, on some occasions jump-
demonstrated, spiritual beliefs have significance for clients ing right into active involvement with moderate success
(Bergin, 1991; Fukuyama & Sevig, 1997; Ingersoll, 1997; and on other occasions showing either daydreaming or off-
Paloutzian, 1996; Richards & Potts, 1995). task activity. One peculiar response of note was her insist-
Although many topics may have intense spiritual meaning ing to sit close to her regular classroom teacher during story
for a particular client, there are some that often have reli- time—sitting at the feet of the teacher and often leaning
gious or spiritual implications. These include suicide, extra- against the teacher’s leg. Another observational vignette
marital sex, criminality, homosexuality, poor self-image, showed her not being aware of which problems she was to
questions of self-worth, coping with death, rebellion against answer in class during oral math review yet, when oriented
and feelings of disrespect for parents, and family break-up. to a specific item, giving marginally correct answers.
These may be apparent in clients who claim a degree of spiri- An interview with the grandparents disclosed the follow-
tuality without religious affiliation. The conceptualizations ing concerning her transition into their home. At first, she
may be ill defined and vague (religion puts words on them, ate more than necessary, almost as though she did not trust
with the risk of distorting and being dogmatic). Because the that there would be more. She lied about insignificant things
client is the resident expert on himself or herself, it is not such as what she was watching on television. The grandpar-
necessary to have an exhaustive knowledge of his or her ents had received direction in setting ground rules, chart-
professed religious affiliation or spiritual philosophy. Such ing responses, and contracting for privileges. This approach
knowledge provides a stereotypical and dogmatic structure had some moderating effect but was not working consis-
of his or her abstract spiritual nature. Of more importance is tently. The grandmother was quite concerned about the cli-
the meaning and experience of the client’s spiritual context ent. The grandfather tended to say, “She’ll be okay. Just give
from his or her phenomenal viewpoint. her time.” He especially enjoyed having her follow him
around the farm as he did his evening chores.
CASE EXAMPLE In an interview with the client, most responses and ac-
tions were typical for her age. She answered questions halt-
The intent of this article was to give a relatively brief over- ingly and watched the interviewer intently as she answered.
view of a complex process for personalizing counseling. For Rephrases of the same question elicited varying responses.
that reason, the segments of the model were described in When given an invitation to freely describe a school expe-
simple, relatively uncomplicated ways. Examples of each rience, she started readily then hesitated, changing her story
aspect of the model and of various integrations of model frequently, again watching the interviewer intently as though
dynamics would be helpful in gaining a complete under- for guidance or affirmation of her story.

288 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention

One interpretation of this behavior just described is that relationships, and the other succeeds by avoiding contin-
the client has been conditioned by an unpredictable reality to gency dynamics. Which is better? The one that works is. At
manifest externally cued survival responses. At the same time, this juncture, it is too early to judge. The selection of strat-
she did not accomplish a repertoire of socially acceptable egy is one of the professional decisions inherent in the func-
responses. Because of the developmental level of the student tion of counselor. Having a rationale and a theory base that
(i.e., 6–10 years of age) and taking the diagnosis of learning permits efficient management of therapeutic dynamics per-
disability into account, I suggest it is likely that she will mits the counselor to begin with the strategy chosen and to
respond best to an operant intervention strategy. Inconsistencies evaluate its immediate results. Obviously if it does not work
in her responses at school and home are due to three problems as expected, selecting an alternative strategy is indicated.
in application of operant procedures: (a) inconsistency on This particular case example has obvious and strong impli-
the part of teachers and grandparents, (b) absence of system cations for tailoring the intervention in response to develop-
adjustments to maintain progress in target response, and mental and social contexts. It does not, however, include refer-
(c) lack of working programs for important target responses ence to the spiritual context. If there existed strong religious
such as staying on task, appropriately responding to adults, opposition from those involved to the use of extrinsic rein-
increased production between reinforcements, monitoring forcers or if there were moralistic intrusions regarding the
or “staying with” class assignments, and being ready to respond rightness or wrongness of single parenting, or conceptions of
at her turn. The intervention strategy that follows from this the client being “bad” or “evil,” perhaps some variations in ap-
involves identifying one or two unwanted responses in each proach would be necessary. There is nothing inherent in this
setting—regular classroom, special education classroom, and approach that requires searching for obscurities in the client’s
grandparents’ home—and building a behavior management circumstance. It works from a phenomenological base of an
program for each. As the client consistently shows desired expanding counseling-mediated perception shared by the
target response, additional wanted and not present responses client and counselor (Stewart, 1983).
will be programmed for reinforcement.
Here is an alternate set of assumptions and a second REPRISE AND CONCLUSION
intervention strategy that logically follows. The client has
some major developmental deficiencies due to a lifetime The profession of counseling (i.e., training programs) has
of near-noncontingency relationships between activity and suc- moved away from its theoretical foundations (Weinrach,
cess. She has been striving to earn acceptance in a world of 1991). A rudderless ship is an appropriate metaphor. It might
changing rules. She was neither valued nor accepted as a per- be supposed that the movement away from learning theory
son, no matter what her condition and no matter what her within the profession was purposeful and probably resulted
response. From the cognition-perceptual family of learning from the previous assessment of learning theory as being
theory, it can be asserted that the client is in a deficit state, not ineffectual. A functional solution is possible.
having had the experiences that provide a basis for normal, A careful study of learning theories and of counseling
socialized responses. Her inconsistency in attention, variability interventions interpreted from their learning theory under-
in responses—both acceptable and unacceptable—to the pinnings results in a blending of many seemingly diverse
same situation, seeking of personal closeness and physical positions into two classes, four families, and three contexts.
contact in the nonthreatening story context, and her fitting The two classes are one-stage and two-stage models. The
in as a companion in farm chores all indicate a need for four families are association, reinforcement, cognition-
personal validation. perceptual, and cognition-rational/linguistic. The contexts
One strategy that fits this frame requires the creation of are developmental, social, and spiritual. It is possible for a
periods of time and activities for which quantity or quality counselor to learn at least one approach within each of these
of performance does not matter. The critical factor is for the four families, with awareness of modifications to fit the
client to receive adult attention and acceptance, no matter contexts, and integrate them into a repertoire rich enough
what her actions have been. One example is already in place— to match virtually all of his or her clients.
her accompanying her grandfather on his chores during which Whether or not a new model is particularly appealing to
time he focuses on her as an active participant with no stan- counselor educators and practitioners depends on their re-
dards for acceptable performance. With the development of ceptiveness and the model’s salience. This particular model
a feeling of security and worth in these settings, the despera- represents an important set of dynamics, a means to the
tion to be okay in other situations will subside and she will end of truly being intentional and accountable in the prac-
naturally pay attention more consistently and perform more tice of the counseling profession.
often within the limits of acceptability. From a Maslovian
perspective, once she has learned that she is okay, fulfilled
her love and belonging needs, and accomplished personal REFERENCES
validation, she will be able to function at the higher level of
Ansbacher, H., & Ansbacher, R. (1956). The individual psychology of Alfred
contingency based, self-esteem activities. Adler. New York: Basic Books.
The two strategies just outlined operate on mutually ex- Aponte, J., Rivers, R., & Wohl, J. (1995). Psychological interventions and
clusive principles. One uses strict adherence to contingency cultural diversity. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 289


Gerber

Ausubel, D. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning: An Ingersoll, E. (1997). Teaching a course on counseling and spirituality.
introduction to school learning. New York: Grune & Stratton. Counselor Education and Supervision, 36, 224–232.
Axelson, J. (1993). Counseling and development in a multicultural soci- Ivey, A., & Ivey, M. (1998). Reframing DSM-IV: Positive strategies from
ety (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. developmental counseling and therapy. Journal of Counseling & De-
Baber, A. (1992). Naked at gendergap: A man’s view of the war between velopment, 76, 334–350.
the sexes. New York: Carol Publishing Group. Janis, I. (1967). Victims of groupthink: A psychological study of foreign
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1975). The structure of magic. Palo Alto, CA: policy decisions and fiascos. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Science and Behavioral Books. Lawless, L., Ginter, E., & Kelly, K. (1999). Managed care: What mental
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, health counselors need to know. Journal of Mental Health Counseling,
Rinehart, and Winston. 21, 50–65.
Bandura, A. (1974). Behavior theory and the models of man. American Levinson, D. (with Darrow, C., Klein, M., & McKee, B.). (1978). The
Psychologist, 29, 859–869. seasons of a man’s life. New York: Knopf.
Bateson, G. (1958). Naven, a survey of the problems suggested by a com- Maslow, A. (1962). Toward a psychology of being. Princeton, MA: Van
posite picture of a New Guinea tribe drawn from three points of view. Nostrand.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Minton, H. (Ed.). (1992). Gay and lesbian studies. New York: Haworth.
Beall, A., & Sternberg, R. (1993). The psychology of gender. New York: Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA:
Guilford Press. Harvard University Press.
Beck, A., Rush, A., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of Minuchin, S., & Fishman, H. (1981). Families therapy techniques. Cam-
depression. New York: Guilford Press. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bergin, A. (1991). Values and religious issues in psychotherapy and Moore, W. (1992). Student and faculty epistemology in the college class-
mental health. American Psychologist, 46, 397–403. room: The Perry scheme of intellectual and ethical development. Un-
Berne, E. (1961). Transactional analysis in psychotherapy. New York: published manuscript.
Grove Press. Mueller, R., Dupuy, P., & Hutchins, D. (1995). A review of the TFA
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. New York: Norton. counseling system: From theory construction to application. Journal
Council for Accreditation of Counseling and Related Educational Pro- of Counseling & Development, 72, 573–577.
grams. (1998). CACREP standards 2000, second draft. Unpublished Notman, M., & Nadelson, C. (Eds.). (1991). Women and men: New perspec-
manuscript. tives on gender differences. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Crane, R. (1997, February 10). Counseling multicultural clients. Ad- Paloutzian, R. (1996). Invitation to the psychology of religion. Boston:
vance: For Speech-Language Pathologists & Audiologists, 7(6), 7. Allyn & Bacon.
Dunn, R., & Griggs, S. (1995). Multiculturalism and learning style: Teach- Perls, F. (1969). Gestalt therapy verbatim. Moab, UT: Real People Press.
ing and counseling adolescents. Westport, CT: Praeger. Phillips, J. (1969). The origin of intellect: Piaget’s theory. San Francisco:
Ellis, A. (1958). Rational psychotherapy. Journal of General Psychology, Freeman.
59, 35–49. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Inter-
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle national Universities Press.
Stuart and Citadel Press. Preston, J. (1998). Integrative brief therapy. San Luis Obispo, CA:
Erickson, M. (1980). The collected papers of Milton H. Erickson on hyp- Impact.
nosis (E. Rossi, Ed.). New York: Irvington. Richards, P., & Potts, R. (1995). Using spiritual interventions in psycho-
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: therapy: Practices, successes, failures, and ethical concerns for Mor-
Stanford University Press. mon psychotherapists. Professional Psychology Research and Practice,
Försterling, F. (1986). Attributional conceptions in clinical psychol- 26, 163–170.
ogy. American Psychologist, 41, 275–285. Rogers, C. (1942). Counseling and psychotherapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Fukuyama, M., & Sevig, T. (1997). Spiritual issues in counseling: A Rogers, C. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
new course. Counselor Education and Supervision, 36, 224–232. Rogers, C. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gazda, G., Ginter, E., & Horne, A. (2001). Group counseling and group Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
psychotherapy: Theory and application. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Ruesch, J., & Bateson, G. (1968). Communication: The social matrix of
Gerber, S. (1999). Enhancing counselor intervention strategies: An psychiatry. New York: Norton.
integrational viewpoint. Philadelphia: Accelerated Development/ Sahakian, W. (1976). Introduction to the psychology of learning. Chicago:
Taylor & Francis. Rand McNally.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and
development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. death. San Francisco: Freeman.
Ginter, E. (1999). David K. Brooks’ contribution to the developmen- Sheehy, G. (1976). Passages: Predictable crises of adult life. New York:
tally based life-skills approach. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, Bantam Books.
21, 191–202. Stewart, J. (1983). Interpretive listening: An alternative to empathy.
Gladding, S. (1996). Counseling: A comprehensive profession (3rd ed.). Communication Education, 32, 379–391.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Merrill. Sue, D., Ivey, A., & Pedersen, P. (1996). A theory of multicultural coun-
Good, B., & Good, M. (1986). The cultural context of diagnosis and seling and therapy. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
therapy: A view from medical anthropology. In M. Miranda & H. Kitano Sue, D., & Sue, D. (1999). Counseling the culturally different: Theory and
(Eds.), Mental health research and practice in minority communities: practice (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Development of culturally sensitive training programs (DHHS Publica- Thorne, F. C. (1950). Principles of personality counseling: An eclectic view-
tion No. ADM 86-1466, pp. 1–8). Rockville, MD: National Institute point. Brandon, VT: Journal of Clinical Psychology.
of Mental Health. Tolman, E. (1949). Purposive behavior in animals and men. Berkeley:
Harris, T. (1967). I’m OK—you’re OK: A practical guide to transactional University of California Press.
analysis. New York: Harper & Row. Watzlawick, P. (1978). The language of change: Elements of therapeutic
Harvey, J. (1974). The Abilene paradox: The management of agreement. communication. Palo Alto, CA: Basic Books.
Organizational Dynamics, 3, 63–80. Wehrly, B. (1995). Pathways to multicultural counseling competence: A
Havighurst, R. (1949). Developmental tasks and education. Chicago: Uni- developmental journey. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
versity of Chicago Press. Weinrach, S. (1991). Selecting a counseling theory while scratching
Hergenhahn, B., & Olson, M. (1993). An introduction to theories of learn- your head: A rational-emotive therapist’s personal journey. Journal of
ing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Mental Health Counseling, 13, 367–378.

290 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Learning Theory and Intentional Intervention

Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking, enlarged edition. New York:


Harper.
Endnote. The following copyright information applies to this
Williamson, E. G. (1939). How to counsel students: A manual of techniques for and subsequent quotations for this work. From Enhancing
clinical counselors. New York: McGraw-Hill. Counselor Intervention Strategies: An Integrational Viewpoint,
Williamson, E. G. (1950). Counseling adolescents. New York: McGraw-Hill. by S. Gerber, 1999. Copyright 1999 by Taylor & Francis, Inc.,
Wolpe, J. (1969). The practice of behavior therapy. New York: Pergammon http://www.routledge-ny.com. Reprinted with permission.
Press.

APPENDIX A APPENDIX B

Two Applications of Pavlovian-Based Interventions Intitial Determination in Guthrian-Based Interventions

a. existing reflex—new stimulus. a. new response.


1. Identify the US [unconditioned stimulus] for the 1. Identify or create obvious contextual elements that
existing reflex (UR). consistently occur and can be used as cues.
2. Select a substitute stimulus (CS) to which the re- 2. Cause the desired response to occur in the presence
flex is to be conditioned. of the cues, making certain that the cues are viable
3. Pair the US and the CS contiguously over many tri- and are attended to by the learner.
als until presentation of the CS alone elicits the re- 3. Repeat step two numerous times so that the various
flex that is now the CR. elements of the complex cues can be associated with
b. existing stimulus—undesirable conditioned response. the desired response.
1. Identify the US for that undesired reflex (UR1) that 4. Change the situation so that no subsequent response
has become a conditioned response (CR1) elicited is associated with the same cues.
by the existing stimulus (CS1). b. replace response.
2. Determine a desired reflexive response (UR2) that 1. Determine a response that is to replace the undesir-
is mutually exclusive of the undesired reflex (UR1). able response. Note that non-response, e.g., remain-
3. Find the stimulus (US2) that elicits the desired ing quiet and still, is an option as a replacement
reflex (UR2). response for inappropriate activity.
4. Pair US2 with several previously neutral stimuli 2. Identify the situation that cues the undesirable
(CS2) so as to gain stimulus control over UR2. response.
5. Present CS2 to elicit CR2 at the same time as a 3. In the presence of those cues, cause the substitute
diluted form of CS1, resulting in only the elicitation response to occur. This can be done by direct ma-
of CS2. nipulation or by using the fatigue or incompatible
6. Gradually increase the intensity of CS1 while CS2 response approaches to habit change.
is eliciting CR2 until CS1 becomes an eliciting stimu- 4. Change the situation so that no subsequent re-
lus only for the desired reflex, now CR2. (Gerber, sponse is associated with the same cues. (Gerber,
1999, p. 66) 1999, p. 66)

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 291


RESEARCH
Clinical Judgment in Case Conceptualization and
Treatment Planning Across Mental Health Disciplines

Janet E. Falvey

This study compared clinical judgment on case conceptualization and treatment planning (TP) tasks among psychologists,
mental health counselors, and social workers using Clinical Treatment Planning Simulations depicting anxiety and affective
disorders. Orientation and work setting did not significantly influence judgment; experience exerted a “modest” curvilinear
effect. Mental health counselors scored higher than social workers and similar to psychologists on both tasks. The best predic-
tors of TP scores for the anxiety disorder were interventions addressing stress management and family dynamics; strong
predictors for the affective disorder included focus on family dynamics and the therapeutic relationship. Implications of findings
for training are discussed.

O
ver the past decade, the managed care in- pare conceptualization, interventions, and treatment costs
dustry has become the new gatekeeper of among psychologists and doctoral-level interns with vary-
mental health practice (Lawless, Ginter, & ing theoretical orientations, finding significantly distinct
Kelly, 1999). Access to service delivery has patterns of conceptualization and intervention across
increasingly shifted from state licensing orientations. Houts (1984) also found significant differences
boards to managed care provider panels, where profes- across orientation for client responsiveness and treatment
sionals competing for contracts submit credentials such prognosis among doctoral-level trainees. Butcher and Scofield
as education, licenses, work setting, experience, and client (1984) used a standardized simulation and process-tracing
utilization patterns (Shueman, 1997; Stromberg, Ratcliff, approach to assess judgment in case review and treatment
& Scheutze, 1997). However, empirical links between these planning among 15 practitioners with 2–5 years postdegree
practitioner variables and treatment outcomes are equivo- experience, finding evidence that either advanced education
cal at best (Dawes, 1994; Garb, 1998). The use of such or increased experience enhanced judgment on these tasks.
criteria as benchmarks of clinical competency thus remains Falvey and Hebert (1992) found significant differences in treat-
poorly validated. The impetus for these professions to ment planning among a sample of 137 clinical mental health
specify factors that enhance clinical practice has therefore counselors and 62 graduate trainees reviewing standardized
intensified, fueled by health policy issues and consumer case simulations.
protection interests. Other findings, however, conclude that clinician demo-
In support of common clinical opinion, some research graphics do not seem to significantly influence treatment
suggests that demographics such as profession, orientation, decisions (Dawes, 1989; Faust et al., 1998; Rock, Bransford,
degree, work setting, and experience inform diagnosis, case Maisto, & Morey, 1987; Turner & Kofoed, 1984; Worthington
conceptualization, and treatment planning (Bishop & Richards, & Atkinson, 1993) or that they do not usefully distinguish
1984; Falvey, 1992b; Garb, 1989; Gil-Ali & Newman, 1984; how clinicians think and process client data (Spengler &
Plous & Zimbardo, 1986). For example, Lambert and Strohmer, 1994; Strohmer & Spengler, 1993). For example,
Wertheimer (1988) found that diagnostic accuracy in Clavelle and Turner (1980) compared paraprofessionals,
reviewing clinical case histories improved significantly with social workers, and psychologists on judgments regarding
relevant education and experience among graduate students the need for hospitalization and medication using two simu-
and paraprofessionals. Kopta, Newman, McGovern, and lated intake interviews, finding no greater consensus in de-
Sandrock (1986) used two 10-page case vignettes to com- cision making among professionals than paraprofessionals.

Janet E. Falvey is an associate professor of counseling in the Department of Education at the University of New Hampshire and a practicing psychologist.
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Janet E. Falvey, Morrill Hall Room 207, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH 03824.

292 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

O’Donohue, Fisher, Plaud, and Curtis (1990) used a struc- Thomsen, 1988; Turk & Salovey, 1988; Tutin, 1993). Judgment
tured interview and retrospective self-report method to is frequently influenced by heuristics such as availability
review three recently terminated cases of each of 25 clini- (ease of recall of relevant information), representativeness
cians from a community mental health center to assess their (similarity to other cases or theoretical models), illusory
assessment methods, treatment goals and methods, finding correlation (judgment based on personal belief rather than
the lack of any systematic decision process for these ac- standard criteria), and overconfidence (increasing confidence,
tions in over 90% of the 75 cases reviewed. Turner and but not judgment accuracy, with additional case informa-
Kofoed sampled five disciplines and three work settings tion) (Faust, 1986; Garb, 1998; Turk & Salovey, 1988).
among 75 mental health practitioners reading an inpatient Cognitive complexity, the ability of an individual to inte-
case report and selecting treatment options, finding that grate large amounts of multidimensional information, clearly
work setting had no impact and only social workers showed influences clinical decision making (Spengler & Strohmer,
indication of any “set” related to treatment planning. 1994). However, there are few empirical studies of the cog-
Research to date thus remains inconclusive regarding nitive processes of clinicians engaged in treatment planning
the impact of therapist demographics on common clinical (Garb, 1998). One related line of inquiry has examined
judgment tasks. Equally disturbing are conflicted findings patterns of case review: the sources, quantity, and sequence
regarding whether experienced or even expert clinicians of clinical data gathering. Findings suggest that the order in
outperform trainees or lay persons on diagnostic, assess- which case material is accessed and how much of it is
ment, and treatment planning tasks (Berman & Norton, reviewed may affect treatment decisions (Butcher & Scofield,
1985; Christensen & Jacobson, 1994; Dawes, 1994; Falvey 1984; Butcher, Scofield, & Baker, 1985; Chapman, Bergus, &
& Hebert, 1992; Garb, 1989; Kleinmuntz, 1990; Mahoney, Elstein, 1996; Ellis, Robbins, Schult, Ladony, & Banker, 1990;
1988; Shaw & Dobson, 1988). Given that such demograph- Elstein et al., 1978; Friedlander & Stockman, 1983). Experts
ics often dictate access to professional practice, credentialing, tend to review less but more relevant data than do less experi-
and reimbursement, it seems critical to extend this research. enced clinicians in the assessment process. There is also some
Case conceptualization and treatment planning are uni- evidence that clinical judgment may be case specific; that is,
versal clinical judgment tasks that lend themselves to such performance on one case may not predict performance on
efforts. From intake to termination, clinicians must gather another case (Elstein et al., 1978; Falvey & Hebert, 1992;
and analyze case information, formulate hypotheses, and Luborsky et al., 1986; O’Donohue et al., 1990).
implement treatment decisions. Given the quantity and Given lack of empirical consensus on the influence of
ambiguity of information presented, this is a daunting task. professional variables on clinical judgment to date, the cur-
Complex clinical data will not “speak for itself”; it must be rent study replicates and extends previous research by ex-
interpreted (Kleinmuntz, 1990; O’Donohue et al., 1990; ploring the impact of practitioner demographics, case re-
Turk & Salovey, 1988). Providing the framework for subse- view processes, and clinical focus on case conceptualization
quent interventions, conceptualization and treatment plan- and treatment planning across two common mental health
ning go beyond simple diagnostic matching to consider the disorders. Participants from three professions responded to
symptoms, context, and history of a problem; the demand standardized case simulations representing anxiety and af-
characteristics of the therapeutic environment; client goals fective disorders. Performance was evaluated on two dimen-
and motivation; and anticipated process dynamics in treat- sions: case conceptualization (CC), the ability to identify
ment (Makover, 1996; Mordock, 1994; O’Donohue et al., important signs, symptoms, and dynamics of a client; and
1990; Tillett, 1996). Moreover, these processes are linked. treatment planning (TP), the ability to synthesize this
To understand how clinicians make treatment decisions, one information into a viable intervention strategy. These skills
needs to understand how they formulate case histories are widely recognized as critical case management compe-
(Garb, 1998). Efforts to identify cognitive strategies and tencies for all mental health providers (Garb, 1998; Makover,
biases that characterize these judgment tasks have yielded 1996; Mordock, 1994; O’Donohue et al., 1990; Strupp &
intriguing findings to date. Butler, 1990; Tillett, 1996; Waddington, 1997).
First, limited human information-processing capacity affects This study was designed to explore four research questions
how practitioners collect, synthesize, and recall information regarding performance on these standardized simulations:
in complex judgment tasks (Faust, 1986; Garb, 1998; (a) Is clinical judgment consistent or case specific across
Kleinmuntz, 1990). Cognitive shortcuts (heuristics) and two distinct disorders? (b) What is the impact of therapist
judgment biases are common. A considerable body of research demographics (profession, work setting, clinical experience,
concurs that mental health clinicians (including experts) and orientation) on CC and TP scores? (c) What factors
selectively screen client data, generate few alternative hypoth- in the case review process affect performance on these
eses early in the assessment process, retain initial diagnoses judgment tasks? and (d) What areas of clinical focus
even in the presence of subsequent refuting evidence, and enhance performance on these judgment tasks? The study
tend to select treatments that confirm (rather than test) their also assessed content validity of the Clinical Treatment
initial judgments (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992; Elstein, Planning Simulations (CTPS; Falvey, 1992b) for assess-
Shulman, & Sprafka, 1978; Garb, 1998; Hogarth, 1987; ing clinical judgment among master’s- and doctoral-level
Kleinmuntz, 1990; Schwartz & Griffin, 1986; Snyder & therapists.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 293


Falvey

METHOD notes). Information in these reports was generated from


criteria identified and ranked by national panels of inter-
Participants disciplinary experts on affective disorders (N = 56) and
anxiety disorders (N = 50). These experts included research-
One hundred sixty-eight participants included 49 psycholo-
ers and practitioners from six professions, averaging 15 years
gists and 59 social workers from a tristate New England
of clinical, and 7 years of research experience with these
region and 60 clinical mental health counselors from East
disorders, and documenting an average of four refereed pub-
Coast states. Demographics were obtained from the one-
lications and seven national/international presentations ad-
page biodata form submitted by participants. All profes-
dressing these disorders (Falvey, 1992b). The CTPS cases pro-
sionals were state licensed or certified, reporting a mean of
vide standard stimuli for assessing clinical judgment in case
13.7 years of clinical experience (SD = 4.1, range 2–46
conceptualization, diagnostic, and treatment planning tasks.
years). The sample comprised 86 women and 82 men, with
Interrater coding reliability for these cases has been reported
a mean age of 45.4 (SD = 9.4) and a range of 29 to 74 years.
as high (.88), and the simulations have demonstrated con-
Ethnic representation was 155 Caucasian (92%), 9 African
tent and discriminant validity across experts, experienced
American (5%), and 4 Hispanic (2%) participants. Primary
practitioners, and trainees (Falvey & Hebert, 1992).
clinical orientations were reported as psychodynamic (n =
Two scores are derived from written responses to each
67, 40%), cognitive-behavioral (n = 52, 31%), family sys-
CTPS simulation. A Case Conceptualization (CC) score
tems (n = 28, 17%), and humanistic (n = 21, 13%). Al-
consists of the following information:
though most cited various work settings during their ca-
reers, nearly half (n = 79, 47%) of these professionals were
1. Initial assessment sources—specific interviews, reports
currently working in independent practice, whereas 50
and referrals reviewed or requested to gain familiarity
(30%) worked in agencies, 19 (11%) in inpatient settings,
with the case (e.g., intake interview, medical records)
and 20 (12%) in academic settings. In self-reporting areas
2. Relevant history and status—symptoms, client presen-
of clinical expertise, 70 clinicians (42%) reported expertise
tation, and aspects of current or developmental his-
in treating anxiety disorders, and 106 (63%) cited expertise
tory identified as salient in the written case summary
in treating affective disorders, the two diagnoses represented
in this research. All participants reported membership in
The Treatment Planning (TP) score includes the follow-
the primary professional organization of their discipline (e.g.,
ing information:
American Psychological Association [APA]; American
Counseling Association [ACA]; National Association of
1. Clinical considerations—recommendations regarding
Social Workers [NASW]), and most listed additional pro-
format of therapy, its expected frequency and dura-
fessional affiliations.
tion, prognosis, and anticipated process dynamics (e.g.,
Chi-square analyses were computed to examine poten-
resistance, motivation)
tial distinctions across the three groups of professionals
2. Treatment interventions—clinical or medical interven-
participating in this study. There were no significant differ-
tions, or both, and referrals identified as relevant in
ences in years of experience across groups. Psychologists
the treatment protocol
differed significantly in work setting from both master’s-
3. Treatment goals—changes in target areas of intrapsy-
level groups (χ2 = .002, p < .01); they more frequently re-
chic and interpersonal functioning specified in the
ported independent practice (61%) rather than agency
treatment protocol
(10%) employment. By contrast, more mental health coun-
selors and clinical social workers were employed in agency
Written responses are coded and computer scored using weights
settings (33% and 43%, respectively). Significant differences
assigned by the experts who developed CC and TP criteria for
in two primary clinical orientations were also distinguished
each CTPS case. Further information on coding and scoring
across professions (χ2 = .0009, p < .001). Twenty-four psy-
procedures is described in Falvey and Hebert (1992).
chologists (49%) reported a cognitive-behavioral orienta-
To assess content validity of the CTPS cases, all participants
tion, whereas 32 clinical social workers (54%) reported a
were asked to complete a 10-item CTPS Evaluation Form at
psychodynamic orientation. Among clinical mental health
the conclusion of each of the two cases. A Likert scale (1 = not
counselors, 22 (37%) reported each of these orientations.
at all, 2 = slightly, 3 = moderately, 4 = significantly, 5 = extremely)
was used to rate the format (clarity of instructions, clarity of
Instruments
material presentation, task adequate to reflect their clinical
Two CTPSs (Falvey, 1992b) consist of standardized case experience); fidelity (degree of realism of case file, compre-
simulations representing anxiety and affective disorders. hensiveness of case as an initial evaluation, reflects actual
Each CTPS case includes eight sealed reports containing intake procedures); and utility (usefulness for teaching/
information commonly found in mental health files (i.e., training programs, for self-assessment by clinicians, in com-
emergency services contact, request for services, intake petency assessment for credentialing purposes) of each
evaluation, previous treatment records, medical history, CTPS case. Participants were also asked to estimate the
couples’ interview, current medical report, and progress degree of difficulty and time spent completing the case.

294 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

Procedure RESULTS
An informational mailing was sent to all current APA and Coding and Scoring
NASW members in a tristate New England region and to
all certified clinical mental health counselors (CCMHCs) Two trained raters independently coded all CTPS responses
in Eastern coastal states, describing the project as an inter- (N = 323) for the three professions represented in this study.
disciplinary study of clinical decision making and inviting Raters were both licensed psychologists and counselor edu-
their participation if currently a mental health provider. cators; one also held national counselor certification. One
No additional incentive or reimbursement for participa- rater had 26 years experience as an educator/researcher and
tion was offered. A total of 358 professionals expressed 13 years of clinical experience. The other rater had 12 years
interest by returning an enclosed biodata form on which experience as an educator/researcher and 16 years of clini-
they summarized graduate training, licenses/certifications, cal experience. Both raters were trained in content analysis
years of clinical experience, areas of expertise, clinical work and had participated in pilot testing the coding keys. The
setting(s), orientation(s), and professional affiliation(s). On coding key for each disorder consists of a one-page listing
receipt of the biodata forms, these practitioners were assigned of all CC and TP categories of information identified by
a three-digit ID number and mailed the following research the expert panel. Raters marked all categories identified by
materials: two CTPS case simulations representing adults a respondent in their essay response to the CC and TP tasks.
with an anxiety and an affective disorder, respectively; Raters had no information regarding individual respondents
response forms with instructions for reviewing each CTPS during the coding process.
case file and writing a case conceptualization and treat- Interrater reliability for these raters using the point-by-
ment plan; a 10-item CTPS Evaluation Form for each case, point statistic (Kelly, 1977) ranged from .85 to .89 across
and a stamped manila envelope for returning all materials cases, with overall reliability at .87. A computer program
to the researcher on completion. scored all coded responses, deriving a CC and TP score for
From among this pool of practicing psychologists, social each case based on expert rankings of the relevance/impor-
workers, and mental health counselors, 168 professionals tance of each possible category. Table 1 presents the range
(47%) subsequently completed and returned the research and mean scores for respondents completing each disorder.
materials. Given the large number of nonparticipants (n =
190) from among those initially interested, chi-square analy- CTPS Content Validity
ses were computed across available demographic variables
(age, sex, experience, expertise) of the two groups. One CTPS format (clarity of instructions and case materials)
significant finding emerged, distinguishing years of experi- was rated highly by psychologists across both cases (Ms =
ence (χ2 = 8.91, p < .01). Nonparticipating clinicians aver- 4.1), and only slightly less so for social workers (Ms = 4.0)
aged less clinical experience (M = 10.3 years) than did par- and mental health counselors (M = 3.9; M = 3.8). Fidelity
ticipants in this study (M = 13.7 years). of the cases in representing actual clinical judgments tasks
In reviewing the CTPS cases, participants were con- was rated similarly, with means between 3.9 and 4.2 across
fronted with two judgment tasks: analysis—abstracting professions for both disorders. Utility, the usefulness of these
relevant client data into a case conceptualization; and simulations for specific purposes, yielded slightly more di-
synthesis—assembling this information into a viable treat- verse ratings. For teaching and training purposes respon-
ment plan (Smith, 1983). Instructions were printed on dents rated the CTPS cases as quite useful, with mean rat-
the response forms in the sequence in which they were ings between 4.1 and 4.4 across professions and cases. Their
followed. First, participants recorded which reports they potential as professional credentialing tools was considered
chose to review in each CTPS case file and the order in good; mental health counselors rated them highest (Ms =
which they were chosen. When they determined that they 4.0), and social worker and psychologist ratings for this pur-
had enough information about the client, they were instructed pose averaged between 3.7 and 3.9.
to write a case conceptualization (CC) including salient Although time-consuming, completing these materials
client demographics, symptoms, clinical presentation, psy- was not particularly difficult for experienced clinicians. Chi-
chosocial history and their clinical and diagnostic impres- square analyses revealed no significant differences for time
sions. The second task was to write a treatment plan for on task or degree of difficulty across the disciplines. Psy-
that client using whatever format they chose. Third, partici- chologists and social workers found the CTPS tasks to be
pants were requested to complete the CTPS Evaluation somewhat easier (ratings between 2.3 and 2.6) than did
Form included with each case and return all materials to mental health counselors (Ms = 2.9). Psychologists took
the researcher. less time to complete each case (averaging 66–72 minutes)
Thirteen participants (8%) returned only one CTPS than did social workers or counselors (averaging 78–90 min-
case; they were included in all analyses except for the utes), with all professionals requiring more time to complete
assessment of judgment consistency. Each participant’s the anxiety disorder than the affective disorder. The CTPS
ID number was recorded on all returned materials to as- thus seems to sample task domains that are familiar and rou-
sure anonymity of responses. tine for both master’s- and doctoral-level practitioners.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 295


Falvey

TABLE 1

Means and Standard Deviations of Case Conceptualization (CC) and Treatment Planning (TP) Scores
Across Profession, Experience, Work Setting, and Clinical Orientation

Anxiety Disordera Affective Disorderb


CC TP CC TP
Variable n M SD M SD M SD M SD

Profession
Psychologist 49 1898.7 509.4 1889.2 537.9 2390.3 390.9 1527.0 530.9
Social worker 59 1813.9 485.3 1753.6 483.3 2339.1 508.8 1263.4 392.0
Mental health counselor 60 2110.3 397.4 1907.5 595.8 2514.4 555.1 1749.3 672.7
Experience (years)
Less than 5 24 1980.0 428.3 1875.8 450.3 2422.7 507.8 1520.7 500.0
5–15 90 2048.3 436.4 1919.4 552.5 2432.7 505.4 1583.6 632.6
More than 15 54 1760.4 515.0 1721.0 549.6 2373.2 486.0 1415.6 523.8
Work setting
Inpatient 19 1997.6 472.0 1881.9 588.5 2499.7 304.8 1617.6 558.4
Agency/CMHC 50 1979.6 517.4 1833.5 599.1 2488.5 572.4 1539.3 591.4
Private practice 79 1921.7 473.4 1840.2 506.1 2361.2 485.3 1503.1 548.4
School/university 20 1887.6 421.6 1880.8 523.6 2386.1 502.1 1445.4 721.4
Clinical orientation
Psychodynamic 67 1992.5 472.2 1779.6 490.1 2460.2 475.6 1379.4 497.7
Cognitive-behavioral 52 1977.2 417.5 1915.9 601.4 2465.2 433.3 1708.0 683.5
Humanistic 21 1899.5 506.3 1923.4 365.7 2421.1 550.1 1438.6 546.1
Family systems 28 1802.6 562.9 1827.2 653.0 2227.0 596.1 1560.9 507.5

Note. CMHC = community mental health center.


a
Possible range of scores: CC = 0–3128; TP = 0–5923. bPossible range of scores: CC = 0–3604; TP = 0–5115.

Judgment Consistency tal health counselor), clinical experience (< 5 years, 5–15
years, > 15 years), work setting (inpatient, agency, indepen-
Some research has concluded that judgment is case spe- dent practice, or academic), and orientation (psychody-
cific; that is, performance on one case does not predict per- namic, cognitive-behavioral, family systems, or humanistic)
formance on another case (Elstein et al., 1978; O’Donohue on performance, analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were com-
et al., 1990). This assertion infers that clinical decisions re- puted across CC and TP scores for each case. Alpha was set
flect knowledge about specific clients or disorders rather at .01 to control for four multiple comparisons. Table 1
than representing characteristic thinking processes of prac- presents score means and standard deviations across demo-
titioners (Hogarth, 1987). The first research question, there- graphic variables of interest in this study.
fore, addressed whether participant performance on these ANOVA revealed significant CC effects on the anxiety dis-
judgment tasks was consistent within and across cases or order for profession, F(2, 116) = 3.99, p < .01, and experience,
whether their decision outcomes were largely independent F(2, 116) = 5.94, p < .01, with no interactions. Mental health
of one another. counselors scored significantly higher (M = 2110.3) than so-
Pearson product–moment correlations were computed for cial workers (M = 1813.9), and all professionals in the study
CC and TP scores within and across the two CTPS cases. who reported between 5 and 15 years of experience scored
Findings revealed a modest relationship between CC and significantly higher (M = 2048.3) than did those with more
TP within cases (anxiety disorder r = .37, affective disorder than 15 years in the field (M = 1760.4). CC scores on the
r = .34) and a moderately strong relationship between CC affective disorder yielded no main effect or interactions.
scores (r = .64) and TP scores (r = .51) across cases. All Across TP scores, ANOVA revealed no main effects or
correlations were significant at the .001 level. Although interactions for the anxiety disorder. For the affective dis-
there was considerable group variability on these tasks, per- order, a main effect was found for profession, F(2, 111) =
formance outcomes were thus relatively consistent for in- 7.66, p < .001, with no interactions. Mental health counse-
dividual participants. lors again scored significantly higher (M = 1749.3) than
did social workers (M = 1263.4).
Demographic Variables
Case Review Process
The second research question addressed the impact of thera-
pist demographics on clinical judgment. To examine the The third research question examined behavioral indices
influence of profession (psychologist, social worker, men- to determine which factors in participants’ review of these

296 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

cases were the strongest predictors of performance. Multi- their case reviews and what treatment recommendations were
variate regression analyses were computed across case re- the strongest predictors of performance among these profes-
view variables to assess their contribution to CC and TP sionals? In order to develop concise models to predict outcome
scores, with the alpha coefficient set at .01 to control for scores, intercorrelations were first computed across predictor
experiment-wise error rate across four multiple tests. The and criterion variables. The CC score was found to be signifi-
regression models accounted for a small but significant pro- cantly related to all predictor variables across both cases. For
portion of the variance in CC and TP scores for both cases. the anxiety disorder, intercorrelations among the independent
Table 2 summarizes these analyses. variables were found between client presentation and medical
Using CC as the criterion, predictor variables included history (r = .24, p < .01) and substance abuse history (r = .25,
the number of reports reviewed, use of formal (Diagnos- p < .01), and between substance abuse and medical histories
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM) diag- (r = –.26, p < .01). For the affective disorder, intercorrelations
nosis, diagnostic accuracy, reported task difficulty, reported among independent variables were found between current
expertise with this disorder, and time on task. For the symptoms and psychosocial history (r = 1.00) and between
anxiety disorder, significant predictors, R2 = .12, F(6, 146) = suicide risk and current support system (r = –.22, p < .01).
3.19, p < .01, included the use of a DSM diagnosis (p < .01) Across both cases, TP scores were also found to be sig-
and longer time on task (p < .01). Significant predictors for nificantly related to each predictor variable. For the anxi-
the affective disorder, R2 = .23, F(6, 139) = 6.87, p < .0001, ety disorder, 4 among 11 treatment considerations and in-
included reviewing more reports (p < .0001) and longer time terventions were found to be highly intercorrelated with
on task (p < .001). several other variables and were thus eliminated from the
Using TP scores as the criterion, predictor variables included model. For the affective disorder, 5 among 13 treatment
time on task, degree of difficulty, treatment format (struc- considerations and interventions were similarly highly
tured vs. unstructured), brief treatment approach (specified intercorrelated and eliminated from that model. Multivari-
as 6 months or less), use of DSM diagnosis, and reported ate stepwise regression analyses were computed for the re-
expertise with this disorder. For the anxiety disorder, signifi- maining clinical focus variables related to CC and TP for
cant predictors, R2 = .17, F(6, 146) = 4.83, p < .001, included each case, with alpha level set at .01 to control for experi-
longer time on task (p < .001), DSM diagnosis (p < .01), and ment-wise error rate across the four multiple tests. Table 3
use of a structured treatment format (p < .01). For the affec- presents results of these analyses.
tive disorder, significant predictors, R2 = .22, F(6, 139) = 6.49, Using CC as the criterion, six predictor variables entered
p < .0001, included longer time on task (p < .0001) and a into the regression equation for the anxiety disorder accounted
structured treatment format (p < .0001). for 59% of the variance in case conceptualization scores.
Client presentation accounted for nearly one third of the
Clinical Focus variance in Step 1 of the model (adj R2 = .31, F = 74.4, p <
The final research question examined areas of clinical focus for .0001). Medical history, added in Step 2, was also a strong
their impact on CC and TP scores. Which areas of attention in predictor, accounting for 15% of CC score variance.

TABLE 2

Multiple Regression With Case Review Variables as Predictors of Case Conceptualization (CC) and
Treatment Planning (TP) Scores

Predictor Variables
No. Reports Formal Diagnostic Degree of Area of Time on Brief TX Treatment
Reviewed Diagnosis Accuracy Difficulty Expertise Task Approach Format
Criterion
Variable B t B t B t B t B t B t B t B t F R2

Anxiety
disorder
CC .11 1.38 .21 2.70* .01 .08 .11 1.34 –.05 –0.59 .17 2.12* 3.19* .12
TP .19 2.37* .04 0.44 .09 1.22 .29 3.66** .10 1.30 .15 1.85* 4.83** .17
Affective
disorder
CC .30 3.97*** .12 1.62 .03 .45 .09 1.20 –.03 –0.45 .26 3.44** 6.87*** .23
TP .10 1.34 .11 1.41 –.01 –0.13 .30 4.00*** .11 1.41 .30 3.99*** 6.49*** .22

Note. TX = treatment.
*p < .01. **p < .001. ***p < .0001.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 297


Falvey

TABLE 3

Multiple Regression Models With Clinical Focus Variables as Predictors of Case Conceptualization and
Treatment Planning Scores

Predictor Variable B SE B β t p F R2 Adj R2

Case conceptualization
Anxiety disorder 40.4* .61 .59
Substance abuse history 185.9 51.1 .19 3.6 .0004
Intake interview 881.7 220.7 .20 4.0 .0001
Medical history 306.3 50.8 .32 6.0 .0000
Current symptoms 906.0 308.7 .15 2.9 .0038
Psychological testing 201.6 61.9 .17 3.3 .0014
Client presentation 410.1 51.3 .43 8.0 .0000
Affective disorder 55.1* .72 .71
Substance abuse history 343.8 51.3 .30 6.7 .0000
Current symptoms 927.2 276.0 .15 3.4 .0010
Prior treatment records 411.8 72.0 .26 5.7 .0000
Medical history 269.5 44.2 .27 6.1 .0000
Intake interview 800.7 273.2 .13 2.9 .0039
Suicide assessment 466.1 47.6 .44 9.8 .0000
Current support system 291.7 46.1 .29 6.3 .0000
Treatment plan
Anxiety disorder 32.6* .59 .58
Cognitive restructuring 291.4 63.6 .25 4.6 .0000
Psychoeducation 187.9 60.9 .17 3.1 .0024
Explore client history 180.7 57.1 .17 3.2 .0019
Realign family relationships 402.2 58.5 .36 6.9 .0000
Build rapport 191.6 56.9 .17 3.4 .0010
Stress management 375.9 59.7 .35 6.3 .0000
Socialization 289.1 77.3 .19 3.7 .0003
Affective disorder 42.1* .69 .68
Cognitive restructuring 211.8 58.5 .17 3.6 .0004
Coping skills training 278.4 73.7 .18 3.8 .0002
Psychoeducation 288.4 60.5 .23 4.8 .0000
Suicide precautions 262.3 70.9 .17 3.7 .0003
Realign family relationships 494.2 53.9 .42 9.2 .0000
Medication consultation 153.9 58.4 .12 2.6 .0093
Building rapport 411.9 53.2 .35 7.7 .0000
Socialization 442.9 75.8 .27 5.8 .0000

*p < .0001.

For the affective disorder, seven predictor variables entered building therapeutic rapport contributed 15% of score variance
into the regression equation accounted for 71% of CC score in Step 2. Socialization activities (9%) and cognitive restructur-
variance. Suicide assessment accounted for one third of the ing (8%) were also relatively strong TP score predictors.
variance in Step 1 (adj R2 = .33, F = 77.9, p < .0001). It was
followed by substance abuse history, which increased adj R2 DISCUSSION
by 14% in Step 2. Medical history contributed an additional
8% of the variance in Step 3 of this model. The rapidly shifting landscape of mental health service pro-
Using TP as the criterion, seven predictor variables en- vision demands that clinical training programs, credentialing
tered into the regression model for the anxiety disorder bodies, and continuing education venues identify the spe-
accounted for 58% of treatment planning score variance. cific skills and training associated with competent service
By far, the best single predictor (adj R2 = .26, F = 58.2, p < delivery. Although public discourse about the qualifications
.0001) was stress management. It was followed by realign- of various provider groups to conduct psychotherapy
ing marital and family relationships, which increased adj R2 abounds in the professional journals, it is largely guild driven
by 13% in Step 2 of the equation. rather than empirically grounded. Until a body of research
Eight predictor variables entered into the regression model establishes which variables are associated with best clinical
for the affective disorder accounted for 68% (adj R2) of TP score practices and develops methodologies for their assessment,
variance. The best single predictor (adj R2 = .24, F = 51.9, p < provider screening criteria will remain largely at the discre-
.0001) was realigning marital and family relationships, whereas tion of the managed care industry.

298 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

Assessment of practitioner competencies is admittedly by averaging these experienced clinician ratings has been
quite complex. Defining relevant practice domains, devel- cited as a viable method to establish reliability and validity
oping assessment tools, and establishing valid competency of the CTPS cases (Garb, 1998).
criteria require considerable empirical rigor. Several decades Client symptoms in each case met all DSM criteria for the
of clinical judgment research have been marked by contro- target diagnosis. The largely unstructured response format
versy and criticism over the lack of consensus regarding minimized cueing effects and simulated real-life decision
how judgment and decision making can be learned, taught, contexts in which clinicians must generate options under
and measured (Garb, 1998). The large dispersion of scores conditions of uncertainty. This increased the fidelity of the
in the current study reaffirms that even within cohorts of instrument, although it required complex coding and scor-
profession, experience, work setting, and orientation, men- ing procedures. High validity ratings by practitioners in this
tal health providers are a diverse group. Clinical judgment and other studies support the viability of CTPS simulations
is a highly individual cognitive process, even among those as a strategy for evaluating clinical judgment on these tasks.
one would assume to be similarly trained or credentialed.
Notwithstanding these limitations, this study demon- Implications for Training and Practice
strates a multidisciplinary approach to evaluating two widely
recognized clinical judgment tasks common to all mental Four analyses were conducted to delineate how participants
health practitioners. A self-selecting bias favored participa- approached these tasks, how they performed in relation to
tion by more experienced clinicians over less experienced one another, and clinical variables that contributed to their
ones in the recruitment process, and this demands caution CC and TP scores. How findings can be linked to strategies
in interpreting these results. Given previous research that for improving clinical training and practice is the focus of
suggests that clinicians gain confidence (although not nec- the following discussion.
essarily competence) with experience (Dawes, 1989; Garb, The finding that clinical judgment was consistent among
1989), one may speculate that less experienced clinicians these clinicians is encouraging. Individual CC and TP scores
may have felt more intimidated about having their skills were modestly yet significantly correlated within and across
compared with professionals from other disciplines on a both cases. Given that less than half of these clinicians re-
largely unstructured judgment task. ported expertise with anxiety disorders and fewer than two
Both a limitation and a strength come from the experi- thirds reported expertise with affective disorders, this finding
enced sample participating in this study. The majority of challenges earlier conclusions that judgment simply reflects
research on clinical judgment in mental health has focused knowledge about specific disorders. It instead lends support
on trainees or new professionals, whereas this study attracted to other empirical evidence of internally consistent reasoning
predominantly “mid-career” professionals. With 86% of par- styles among experienced clinicians (Falvey, 1992a; Turner &
ticipants reporting more than 5 years of clinical experience Kofoed, 1984) and suggests that there may be distinct cogni-
in predominantly outpatient settings (including 32% report- tive patterns that characterized their decision processes.
ing more than 15 years experience), the following findings The significant difference found across professions on CC
are most applicable to that population. scores for the anxiety but not the affective disorder may be
explained by the lower level of expertise with anxiety dis-
orders (42%) that this sample reported. Conversely, the
Addressing the Criterion Issue
higher percentage of expertise with affective disorders
Given the centrality of case conceptualization and treat- (63%) may account for less variability among those scores.
ment planning in most psychotherapy models, efforts to That is, variability in scores could be expected to decrease
define current best practices seem worthwhile (Nathan, as overall expertise increases. Does expertise also enhance
1998). However, to do so requires some valid way of as- performance? Falvey and Hebert (1992) reported signifi-
sessing these clinical judgment skills. Against what criteria cantly higher TP scores on CTPS cases among experts as
should individual performance be measured? compared with experienced clinicians completing these
As in the recent development of clinical practice guide- cases, lending some support to this speculation. However,
lines, it is advisable to use independent subject-matter ex- other research remains equivocal on whether experience
perts with both clinical and research expertise to develop improves performance (Garb, 1998; Hogarth, 1987).
criterion-referenced standards of performance (Baker, Although several trends across disciplines were suggested
Lichtenberg, & Moye, 1998; Edmunds, 1996; Friedman, by the data, it is not clear from this research what heuris-
Prywes, & Benbassat, 1989; Loveland, 1985). CTPS simu- tics were involved in the case review process and how they
lations in this study relied on large multidisciplinary expert may have influenced decision making. A follow-up study,
panels to identify and weight all case information as well as using a process-tracing approach in which another sample
all case conceptualization and treatment planning criteria. of psychologists, social workers, and mental health coun-
These experts represented all three professions sampled selors “think aloud” while reviewing and responding to a
in this study and three of the four clinical orientations CTPS case, is examining this line of inquiry further (Falvey
(the humanistic orientation was cited as influential but & Bray, 2001). To the extent that specific cognitive styles
not primary among experts). Weighting criterion scores and their associated decision outcomes can be identified,

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 299


Falvey

preprofessional supervision and in-service training may be Work setting and clinical orientation did not distinguish
targeted to correct problematic heuristics and enhance performance on either CTPS case. Setting was also nonsig-
clinical judgment. nificant in another interdisciplinary study of clinical deci-
The second analysis examined the impact of practitioner sion making (Turner & Kofoed, 1984) and thus seems to
demographics on performance. Among the master’s-level have little impact on these judgment tasks. The finding for
professionals included in this study (N = 119), mental health orientation was unexpected and failed to support previous
counselors scored higher than clinical social workers on all evidence that this variable does influence judgment (Beutler,
judgment tasks. This supports other findings that profes- Crago, & Arizmendi, 1986; Bishop & Richards, 1984; Gil-
sion does influence clinical judgment (Clavelle & Turner, Ali & Newman, 1984; Houts, 1984; Kopta et al., 1986).
1980; Falvey, 1992b; Turner & Kofoed, 1984) and suggests Some research has suggested that the impact of orientation
that certified mental health counselors are at least compa- may diminish over time, affecting the performance of train-
rable with their clinical social work counterparts on these ees and entry-level professionals more than experienced
case management tasks. clinicians (Berven, 1985; Falvey & Hebert, 1992). Although
It is interesting that CC and TP scores for mental health this study found no interaction between orientation and
counselors were also slightly higher than those of psycholo- experience, it primarily sampled experienced professionals
gists across both cases, although not significantly. Given that (e.g., 86% reporting over 5 years in the field).
most CCMHCs receive their graduate training from psy- Several other factors may have contributed to this finding.
chologist faculty members, they could be expected to ap- In the current study, two primary orientations (psychody-
proach decision-making tasks similarly. It is also possible namic and cognitive-behavioral) accounted for more than
that CCMHCs were highly motivated on these tasks due two thirds of the participants in each profession. This may
to recent efforts of that group to achieve parity with other reflect a self-selection bias among those choosing to partici-
mental health professionals. However, participants were pate because humanistic and family systems clinicians are
unaware of specific comparison groups used in the study, less likely to rely on the formal clinical assessment proce-
thus partially controlling for rivalry and expectancy effects. dures that these tasks represent (Bellack & Hersen, 1990). In
These findings suggest that training and experience at the addition, in noting their primary orientation, participants had
master’s level is sufficient for completing the clinical judg- the option of ranking other orientations that significantly
ment tasks of case conceptualization and treatment plan- influenced their work. Most did; in fact, only 23 (14%) listed
ning. Clinical mental health counselors performed as well just one orientation, and well over 50% of the participants
as clinical psychologists across both tasks and both cases, from each profession listed three or more orientations as in-
although counselors found the tasks slightly more difficult fluential. This finding supports evidence of multiple orienta-
than did psychologists. Previous studies concluded that tions among psychologists (Zook & Walton, 1989) and sug-
master’s-level practitioners spend the majority of their pro- gests that pragmatic eclecticism rather than theoretical alle-
fessional time in direct service delivery (Robiner, Arbisi, & giance was the mode among these practitioners.
Edwall, 1994; Trent, 1993; Watkins, Campbell, & McGregor, From the aforementioned evidence, one could speculate
1989) and thus could be expected to have competency in that orientation has more impact on clinicians’ decision
the case management skills assessed in this study. For com- making early in their careers (as assessed in the earlier re-
mon outpatient disorders, then, client assessment and treat- search) when a limited knowledge base favors theoretical
ment planning seems well within the scope of master’s- allegiance. With increased experience, practitioners tend to
level clinicians. The degree to which these skills are related become more pragmatic and theoretically eclectic, thus the
to subsequent successful treatment outcomes is a recom- impact of orientation is probably muted to a large degree.
mended future research direction. The finding that experience significantly affected CC
Using the CTPS cases as standard stimuli, results of this scores on the anxiety disorder suggests that experience
study also suggest that psychology-based training is more does have some influence on judgment. Further inspec-
beneficial than social work training on these clinical judg- tion of group means suggests that this is a “modest” cur-
ment tasks. Psychologists and mental health counselors vilinear relationship. Previous research concluding that
scored higher than social workers on both tasks across both clinicians do not seem to learn from experience (Dawes,
cases. This was particularly evident in treatment planning 1989; Garb, 1998) may be clarified by this finding. In
in which social workers considered fewer interventions and this study, midcareer professionals having 5 to 15 years
goals, relied less on cognitive-behavioral strategies that are of experience (n = 90) performed better than either
empirically supported for these disorders, and achieved con- newly trained (n = 24) or more senior practitioners (n =
sistently lower scores than the other practitioners. Given 54) on CC and TP scores. It seems that perhaps we do
the lack of interaction between profession and experience, learn from our clinical experience, up to a point. With
work setting, or orientation, and considering that these were experience comes the tendency to rely more heavily on
experienced and credentialed professionals with similar con- cognitive heuristics, commonly experienced as clinical wis-
tinuing education requirements, this finding seems robust. dom (Brenner & Howard, 1976; Clavelle & Turner, 1980;
Social work training may thus benefit from an increased fo- Turk & Salovey, 1988). One might hypothesize that this
cus on goal-setting and cognitive-behavioral interventions. “wisdom” provides so many templates that it interferes with

300 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

the ability to consider unique aspects of a case. Alternately, An important finding regarding patterns of case review
it is possible that practitioners trained prior to the 1980s among these clinicians was that the use of a structured format
did not receive extensive training in case conceptualization for treatment planning resulted in significantly higher TP scores
and treatment planning. However, managed care utiliza- across both disorders. Treatment format was considered struc-
tion reviews and professional continuing education require- tured if it included a problem list, a sequence of recommended
ments over the past decade would be expected to update interventions, and targeted goals of these interventions. This
the skills of those professionals. finding supports the utility of focused treatment plans as com-
The third set of analyses addressed aspects of the case pared with general treatment discussions. More than half of
review process that influenced case conceptualization and the psychologists and social workers submitted structured
treatment planning scores. Taking time to review more of the treatment plans for both cases, whereas less than one third
available reports, reliance on DSM diagnostic taxonomy, and of CCMHCs used structured formats for each case. This also
use of structured treatment plans predicted better performance seems to be an area of needed training for mental health
than did other variables. These findings suggest that there are counselors because it contributed to the wide standard de-
diminishing returns in emphasizing efficiency over thorough- viation among TP scores for that group.
ness on these tasks and support the value of formal documen- The final set of analyses in this study examined variables
tation systems typically required by third parties. of clinical focus for their impact on CC and TP scores. Case
Spending more time on these tasks predicted higher CC conceptualization was clearly enhanced by focusing on the
and TP scores. Half of the participants (49%) reported taking client’s current clinical presentation, which accounted for
1 hour or less to complete each case. Scores improved as time nearly one third of score variance. Medical history for both
increased, with professionals taking 2 hours or longer averag- cases and substance abuse history for the affective case were
ing the highest CC and TP scores for each case. The number also important predictors. Formal psychological testing did
of reports reviewed also predicted CC scores for the affective not emerge as an important predictor. This finding supports
disorder in which 33 participants (21%) who reviewed fewer other evidence that current symptoms and client status are
than half of the eight possible reports averaged CC scores critical variables to consider in intake assessments (Bellack
more than 300 points lower than those who reviewed more & Hersen, 1990; Greenblatt & Kleinmuntz, 1984).
case material. This challenges previous findings that adding Realigning marital and family relationships emerged as
incremental data during case review increased confidence but an important predictor of treatment planning scores across
not judgment accuracy (Garb, 1998; Hogarth, 1987). both cases. Although only 17% of these participants reported
Identification of a formal (DSM) diagnosis was also associ- a family systems orientation, those who were most effec-
ated with higher scores across both cases and significantly tive on the treatment planning task clearly were thinking
higher CC scores for the anxiety disorder. Most participants systemically in developing an intervention plan. This em-
did record DSM diagnoses: 80% of psychologists, 75% of so- phasis was reiterated in the affective disorder, in which at-
cial workers, and 58% of mental health counselors. It is curi- tention to improving interpersonal relationships (through
ous that the accuracy of diagnoses was not a significant pre- therapeutic rapport, in the family, and in the client’s social
dictor across either disorder. A probable explanation for this is network) emerged as significantly stronger predictors than
“fine tuning”; that is, incorrect or unclear diagnoses often tar- either cognitive-behavioral or insight-oriented strategies in
geted signal symptoms of the disorders (e.g., anxious, depressed treatment planning. Intrapsychic interventions thus seem
mood) but did not adhere to formal taxonomy. Despite limi- less prominent than an interpersonal focus in successfully
tations of the DSM (Strupp & Butler, 1990), this classification conceptualizing treatment for these disorders.
system does seem to enhance case conceptualization.
It was notable that considerably fewer mental health Implications for Clinical Assessment
counselors used DSM diagnoses and structured treatment
plans than did the other groups. This helps to explain the Practitioners sampled in this study concurred that the CTPS
high variability in counselor scores on these tasks. Mental cases portray realistic and comprehensive outpatient files
health counselors recorded more extreme scores at both similar to what they encounter in their clinical work envi-
ends of the range and thus seem to constitute a more di- ronments. They found the cases to be particularly suited
verse group than did the other professions. In a relatively for preservice and in-service training, as well as having po-
recent nationwide survey of CCMHCs, less than two thirds tential for use in professional credentialing. These findings
of respondents (64%) believed that they were adequately support the viability of the CTPS for competency-based
trained in use of the DSM (Mead, Hohenshil, & Singh, assessment in the allied health professions (Falvey & Hebert,
1997). This study unfortunately did not assess which 1992; Loveland, 1985; McLeod, 1992). Standardized simu-
CCMHCs were trained in CACREP-approved programs lations have been widely used for training and credentialing
(that require competency in the DSM diagnostic system) in medical specialties (Friedman et al., 1989; Krahn &
versus other training programs; that may account for the Blanchaer, 1986; Melnick, 1990; Schwartz & Griffin, 1986)
wide variability in scores. Clearly, this finding supports and have demonstrated good potential for use in counsel-
the inclusion of course work on formal diagnostic systems ing and psychology (Butcher et al., 1985; Hile, Campbell,
in graduate counseling curricula. & Ghobary, 1994; Smith, 1983; Turk & Salovey, 1988). The

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 301


Falvey

current study demonstrates the potential of such simula- sessed. This may have influenced findings because those
tions to discern decision processes and outcomes. who conduct case conceptualizations and prepare treatment
When used for teaching purposes, the ability of trainees plans regularly could be expected to differ from those who
to receive immediate feedback on their performance as only infrequently do so. Future research may benefit from
compared with a criterion group provides a valuable learn- evaluating this variable in conjunction with performance.
ing tool (Friedman et al., 1989). The CTPS coding system In this era of managed care, standardized simulations such
is formatted such that CC and TP criteria identified by the as the CTPS can become very useful in providing empirical
experts can be viewed, along with weightings for each cri- support for professional accountability. To the extent that we
teria. Trainees can see which aspects of a case they con- can identify and assess those practitioner variables associated
curred with the experts on and where they may have missed with successful performance on common clinical judgment
important aspects of case conceptualization or treatment tasks, the mental health disciplines will be well positioned to
planning. Training for a particular disorder could focus on regain clinical authority over our professional practice.
(for example) identifying target goals, specifying the length
of treatment, selecting adjunct services or referrals, or in- REFERENCES
cluding specific relational and behavioral interventions as
needed based on expert feedback. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as
predictors of interpersonal consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 111,
The use of CTPS simulations for credentialing is some- 256–274.
what less promising. The simulations were rated by practi- Baker, R. B., Lichtenberg, P. A., & Moye, J. (1998). A practice guideline for
tioners in this study as offering a high fidelity representa- assessment of competency and capacity of the older adult. Professional
tion of their actual clinical practice. Their usefulness for Psychology: Research and Practice, 29, 149–154.
Bellack, A. S., & Hersen, M. (1990). Handbook of comparative treatments for
credentialing purposes was also rated highly across all
adult disorders. New York: Wiley.
groups. However, the large standard deviations found across Berman, J. S., & Norton, N. C. (1985). Does professional training make a
most variables of interest reveal that there is considerable therapist more effective? Psychological Bulletin, 98, 401–407.
individual variation in performance on these tasks. This may Berven, N. L. (1985). Reliability and validity of standardized case manage-
limit their utility for standard competency assessments such ment simulations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 397–409.
Beutler, L. E., Crago, M., & Arizmendi, T. G. (1986). Research on thera-
as licensure or certification. pist variables in psychotherapy. In L. S. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.),
Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 257–310). New
Caveats and Conclusions York: Wiley.
Bishop, J., & Richards, T. (1984). Counselor theoretical orientation as related
This investigation suggests that there are many similarities to intake judgments. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 398–401.
Brenner, D., & Howard, K. (1976). Clinical judgment as a function of experi-
and some important distinctions across professionals in re- ence and information. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 721–728.
sponding to universal case management tasks. It also dem- Butcher, J. E., & Scofield, M. (1984). The use of a standardized simulation and
onstrates a methodology for evaluating clinical decision pro- process tracing for studying clinical problem-solving competence. Counse-
cesses and outcomes. However, although a more realistic lor Education and Supervision, 24, 70–84.
assessment of clinical skill than traditional written exams, Butcher, J. E., Scofield, M., & Baker, S. (1985). Clinical judgment in
mental health treatment planning: An empirical investigation. Ameri-
simulations do not replicate face-to-face interactions be- can Mental Health Counselors Association Journal, 7, 116–126.
tween practitioner and client (Frederikson, 1986). It is thus Chapman, G. B., Bergus, G. R., & Elstein, A. S. (1996). Order of information
not clear that how one responds to a written simulation affects clinical judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 201–211.
will accurately reflect their behavior with actual clients. The Christensen, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1994). Who (or what) can do psycho-
use of case simulations may therefore best be incorporated therapy: The status and challenge of nonprofessional therapies. Psychologi-
cal Science, 5, 8–14.
into a testing battery that includes theoretical knowledge Clavelle, P. R., & Turner, A. D. (1980). Clinical decision-making among profes-
(assessed with traditional written exams), clinical knowl- sionals and paraprofessionals. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 833–838.
edge (evaluated through performance on standardized simu- Dawes, R. M. (1989). Experience and validity of clinical judgment: The
lations), and applied skill (demonstrated through work illusory correlation. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 7, 457–467.
Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built
samples of actual psychotherapy practice).
on myth. New York: Free Press.
Major findings of this research suggest that mental health Edmunds, M. (1996). Clinical practice guidelines: Opportunities and
professionals are consistent in their performance on case implications. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 18, 126–132.
conceptualization and treatment planning tasks across diag- Ellis, M. V., Robbins, E. S., Schult, D., Ladony, N., & Banker, J. (1990).
noses, that professional training and experience seem to have Anchoring errors in clinical judgments: Type I error, adjustment, or
mitigation? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 343–351.
some impact on these outcomes, and that it is possible to Elstein, A. S., Shulman, L. S., & Sprafka, S. A. (1978). Medical problem
identify specific aspects of case review and treatment plan- solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
ning that enhance performance on these judgment tasks. University Press.
Perhaps most encouraging about this methodology is its Falvey, J. E. (1992a). Science or serendipity? What we know about
potential to provide trainees and professionals with direct clinical judgment in mental health. Journal of Mental Health Coun-
seling, 14, 458–470.
feedback about their performance. Falvey, J. E. (1992b). From intake to intervention: Interdisciplinary per-
Although all participants in the study were practicing spectives on MH treatment planning. Journal of Mental Health Counsel-
clinicians, their weekly client contact hours were not as- ing, 14, 471–489.

302 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C l i n i c a l J u d g m e n t i n C a s e C o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n a n d Tr e a t m e n t P l a n n i n g

Falvey, J. E., & Bray, T. (2001). How therapists think while making clinical judg- Melnick, D. E. (1990). Computer-based clinical simulation: State of the art.
ments: A multidisciplinary process-tracing study. Unpublished manuscript. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 13, 104–120.
Falvey, J. E., & Hebert, D. J. (1992). Psychometric study of the Clinical Mordock, J. B. (1994). Treatment planning in counseling. In J. L. Ronch,
Treatment Planning Simulations (CTPS) for assessing clinical judgment. W. Van Ornum, & N. C. Stilwell (Eds.), The counseling sourcebook: A
Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 14, 490–507. practical reference on contemporary issues (pp. 227–233). New York: Cross-
Faust, D. (1986). Research on human judgment and its application to clini- road Publishing.
cal practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17, 420–430. Nathan, P. E. (1998). Practice guidelines: Not yet ideal. American Psycholo-
Faust, D., Guilmette, T. J., Hart, K., Arkes, H. R., Fishburne, F. J., & Davey, L. gist, 53, 290–299.
(1998). Neuropsychologists’ training, experience, and judgment accu- O’Donohue, W., Fisher, J. E., Plaud, J. J., & Curtis, S. D. (1990). Treatment
racy. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 145–163. decisions: Their nature and their justification. Psychotherapy, 3, 421–427.
Frederikson, N. (1986). Construct validity and construct similarity: Meth- Plous, S., & Zimbardo, P. (1986). Attributional biases among clinicians: A
ods for use in test development and test validation. Multivariate Behav- comparison of psychoanalysts and behavior therapists. Journal of Con-
ioral Research, 21, 3–28 sulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 568–570.
Friedlander, M., & Stockman, S. (1983). Anchoring and publicity effects in Robiner, W. N., Arbisi, P. A., & Edwall, G. E. (1994). The basis for the
clinical judgment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 637–643. doctoral degree for psychology licensure. Clinical Psychology Review,
Friedman, M., Prywes, M., & Benbassat, J. (1989). Variability in doctors’ 14, 227–254.
problem-solving as measured by open-ended written patient simulations. Rock, D. L., Bransford, J. D., Maisto, S. A., & Morey, L. (1987). The study of
Medical Education, 23, 270–275. clinical judgment: An ecological approach. Clinical Psychology Review, 7,
Garb, H. N. (1989). Clinical judgment, clinical training, and professional 645–661.
experience. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 387–396. Schwartz, S., & Griffin, T. (1986). Medical thinking: The psychology of medi-
Garb, H. N. (1998). Studying the clinician: Judgment research and psychologi- cal judgment and decision making. New York: Springer-Verlag.
cal assessment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Shaw, B. F., & Dobson, K. S. (1988). Competency judgments in the training
Gil-Ali, D., & Newman, F. L. (1984). Patient information and its use by thera- and evaluation of psychotherapists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
pists differing in theoretical orientation. Psychotherapy, 21, 479–482. Psychology, 56, 666–672.
Greenblatt, R. L., & Kleinmuntz, B. (1984). Statistical modeling of clinical Shueman, S. A. (1997). Confronting health care realities: A reply to Sank
intake decisions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 1528–1537. (1997). Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, 555–558.
Hile, M. G., Campbell, D. M., & Ghobary, B. (1994). Automation for clini- Smith, I. L. (1983). Use of written simulations in credentialing programs.
cians in the field: The validity of a performance support system. Behavior Professional Practice of Psychology, 4, 21–50.
Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 26, 205–208. Snyder, M., & Thomsen, C. J. (1988). Interactions between therapists and
Hogarth, R. (1987). Judgment and choice (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. clients: Hypothesis testing and behavioral confirmation. In D. Turk & P.
Houts, A. (1984). Effects of clinician theoretical orientation and patient Salovey (Eds.), Reasoning, inference, and judgment in clinical psychology
explanatory bias on initial clinical judgments. Professional Psychology: (pp. 124–152). New York: Free Press.
Research and Practice, 15, 284–293. Spengler, P. M., & Strohmer, D. C. (1994). Clinical judgment biases: The
Kelly, M. B. (1977). A review of the observational data-collection and reli- moderating roles of counselor cognitive complexity and counselor client
ability procedures reported in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. preferences. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 8–17.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 97–101. Strohmer, D. C., & Spengler, P. M. (1993). Studying mental health counse-
Kleinmuntz, B. (1990). Why we still use our heads instead of formulas: lor clinical judgment: A response to Falvey and colleagues. Journal of
Toward an integrative approach. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 296–310. Mental Health Counseling, 15, 465–474.
Kopta, S. M., Newman, F. L., McGovern, M., & Sandrock, D. (1986). Psy- Stromberg, C., Ratcliff, R., & Scheutze, J. (1997). Surviving the
chotherapeutic orientations: A comparison of conceptualizations, inter- minefield of managed care contracting. The Psychologist’s Legal Up-
ventions, and treatment plan costs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- date (#9). Washington, DC: National Register of Health Service
chology, 54, 369–374. Providers in Psychology.
Krahn, C. G., & Blanchaer, M. C. (1986). Using an advanced organizer to Strupp, H. H., & Butler, S. F. (1990). Psychotherapy. In A. S. Bellack & M.
improve knowledge application by medical students in computer-based Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of comparative treatments for adult disorders (pp.
clinical simulations. Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 13, 71–74. 3–16). New York: Wiley.
Lambert, L. E., & Wertheimer, M. (1988). Is diagnostic ability related to Tillett, R. (1996). Psychotherapy assessment and treatment selection. Brit-
relevant training and experience? Professional Psychology: Research and ish Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 10–15.
Practice, 19, 50–52. Trent, J. T. (1993). Issues and concerns in master’s-level training and em-
Lawless, L. L., Ginter, E. J., & Kelly, K. R. (1999). Managed care: What ployment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 586–592.
mental health counselors need to know. Journal of Mental Health Coun- Turk, D. C., & Salovey, P. (Eds.). (1988). Reasoning, inference, and judgment
seling, 21, 50–65. in clinical psychology. New York: Free Press.
Loveland, E. H. (1985). Measurement of competence: Some definitional Turner, J., & Kofoed, L. (1984). Decision-making, professional discipline,
issues. Professional Practice of Psychology, 6, 129–143. and program affiliation: Selection of an inpatient treatment alternative.
Luborsky, L., Crits-Christoph, P., McLellan, A. T., Woody, G., Piper, W., Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 858–865.
Liberman, B., Imber, S., & Pilkonis, P. (1986). Do therapists vary much in Tutin, J. (1993). The persistence of initial beliefs in clinical judgment. Jour-
their success? American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 501–502. nal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 319–335.
Mahoney, M. J. (1988). Rationalism and constructivism in clinical judg- Waddington, L. (1997). Clinical judgment and case formulation. British
ment. In D. Turk & P. Salovey (Eds.), Reasoning, inference, & judgment Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 309–311.
in clinical psychology (pp. 155–181). New York: Free Press. Watkins, C. E., Campbell, V. L., & McGregor, P. (1989). APA-affiliated
Makover, R. B. (1996). Treatment planning for psychotherapists. Washington, master’s-level counselors. Counseling Psychologist, 17, 289–300.
DC: American Psychiatric Press. Worthington, R. L., & Atkinson, D. R. (1993). Counselors’ responsibil-
McLeod, J. (1992). What do we know about how best to assess counsellor ity and etiology attributions, theoretical orientations, and counseling
competence? Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 5, 359–372. strategies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 295–302.
Mead, M. A., Hohenshil, T. H., & Singh, K. (1997). How the DSM system is Zook, A., & Walton, J. M. (1989). Theoretical orientations and work
used by clinical counselors: A national study. Journal of Mental Health settings of clinical and counseling psychologists: A current perspec-
Counseling, 19, 383–401. tive. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 20, 23–31.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 303


Counselors’ Attribution of Responsibility, Etiology, and
Counseling Strategy

Jerry L. Kernes and J. Jeffries McWhirter

This study surveyed 167 counselors working at university counseling centers on their etiology and responsibility attributions and
models of helping. Participants responded to vignettes describing either a male or female client experiencing symptoms of either
an identity or adjustment problem. Counselors endorsed all of P. Brickman et al.’s (1982) models of helping for both problem types.
Predictions concerning etiology attributions were partially supported. Counselors selected attributions logically consistent with
an internal cause for the identity problem. However, counselors did not make external attributions for the adjustment problem. No
significant results were observed for the influence of client sex.

C
ounselors constantly seek to understand the disorders were found to play a role in diagnostic decisions
behavior of their clients. In the process of at- (Strohmer, Biggs, Keller, & Thibodeau, 1984). Specifically,
tempting to understand their clients, counse- the researchers found that attributions significantly affected
lors observe clients’ behaviors and the context diagnostic discriminations between bipolar and unipolar
in which those behaviors are imbedded and disorders.
make inferences concerning what could have produced such Additional evidence for the influence of counselors’ attri-
behavior. These inferences may indict dispositional characteris- butions on counseling decisions is provided by Batson (1975)
tics of the client, environmental forces, or a combination of both and Murdock and Freemont (1989). Batson compared the
(Heider, 1958). Diagnostic decisions, symptom recognition, and attributions and treatment recommendations of professional
predictions concerning treatment response and outcome can and nonprofessional helpers in a simulated referral agency.
be influenced by counselors’ explanations for the cause of In general, participants in the study tended to match treat-
clients’ presenting problems (Lopez & Wolkenstein, 1990). ment referrals to attributions. Clients whose problems
The role attributions play in counselor decision making were attributed to personal factors (mental disorder) were
has not been sufficiently explored (Murdock & Freemont, given referrals to agencies concerned with personal change
1989). The majority of clinical attributional research has (mental hospital, residential treatment center), whereas
focused on questions of self-perception (see Harvey & clients whose problems were viewed as stemming from
Galvin, 1984, for review). Few studies of clinical attribution situational factors (unemployment, lack of job skills) were
have focused on person perception (Lopez & Wolkenstein, given referrals to social change agencies (state employment
1990). Studies that have been conducted in this area have agency, social services). Murdock and Fremont, likewise, had
produced mixed results. For example, Strohmer, Haase, counselors rate clients along four attributional dimensions
Biggs, and Keller (1982) examined counselors’ attributions (locus, stability, globality, and controllability). Counselors
along three dimensions: the degree to which the client were then asked to make decisions on treatment urgency,
could control difficulties (controllability), whether the duration of the problem, and on ideal treatment modality.
problem was long term or temporary (stability), and the Attributions regarding the stability of the cause and dura-
degree to which the problem pervaded the client’s life tion of the problem best predicted treatment decisions
(globality). Counselors were then asked to predict the likeli- (Murdock & Freemont, 1989).
hood of the client’s progress in counseling. The researchers If attributions play a significant role in counselors’ decision-
concluded that attributions might not significantly influ- making processes, issues of responsibility are one important
ence counselor decision making. In a later study, counselors’ set of attributions to consider. People tend to hold others
attributions of controllability in diagnosis of affective more responsible for their situations if they perceive those

Jerry L. Kernes is a doctoral candidate, and J. Jeffries McWhirter is a professor, both in the Counseling Psychology Program at Arizona State University,
Tempe. This article is based on the master’s thesis completed by Jerry L. Kernes under the direction of J. Jeffries McWhirter. The research was funded, in part,
by a grant from the Research Support Program at Associated Students of Arizona State University, Graduate College and Vice President for Research. An
earlier version of this article was presented in August 1998 at the 106th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association in San Francisco. The
authors thank Richard T. Kinnier and Barbara Kerr for their comments and assistance in conducting this study. Correspondence regarding this article should
be sent to Jerry L. Kernes, c/o J. Jeffries McWhirter, Counseling Psychology Program, Division of Psychology in Education, Arizona State University, Box
870611, Tempe, AZ 85287-0611 (e-mail: Jerry.Kernes@asu.edu).

304 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C o u n s e l o r s ’ A t t r i b u t i o n o f R e s p o n s i b i l i t y, E t i o l o g y, a n d C o u n s e l i n g S t r a t e g y

individuals as having been in control of their previous behav- clients are seen as responsible for both causing and solving
iors (see Weiner, 1993). In addition, people are less likely to their problems. Clients view themselves, or are viewed by oth-
want to help others whom they perceive to be responsible for ers, as creating their problems through a deficit in moral char-
their situations (Schmidt & Weiner, 1988). According to acter (i.e., laziness, stubbornness, lack of willpower) and are
Feinberg (1970), the question of moral responsibility in- expected to strive to overcome problems. Helpers are essen-
volves two separate issues—blame and control. Blame is tially viewed as motivators or coaches. The advantage of this
attributed to individuals if they are seen as responsible model is that clients are recognized as being totally respon-
for creating their problems. Control is attributed to people sible for their lives and are likely to be more motivated to
if they are held responsible for changing or influencing produce changes. The drawback of this model is that it can be
events in their lives. These dimensions undoubtedly influ- taken to the extreme to believe that victims are responsible
ence counselors’ clinical decisions. Specifically, counselors’ for their own victimization (Brickman et al., 1982).
attributions concerning clients’ responsibility for causing
and solving their problems may affect treatment decisions Compensatory Model
and helping behavior.
Brickman et al. (1982) provided a theoretical framework This model holds clients responsible only for solving their
for classifying models of helping and coping according to problems but not for causing them. Clients are seen as suf-
attributions of responsibility for the cause and solution of a fering from the failure of their social environments to meet
client’s problems. Their analysis yielded four fundamentally their needs. The relationship between client and helper is a
different orientations to the world said to exist in the minds partnership, with the helper assuming a subordinate role.
of helpers, aggressors, and recipients of help or aggression. Helpers are expected to be teachers and provide educa-
Brickman and his colleagues contended that people might tion, skill building, and opportunities. Examples of this
be unaware of the assumptions they hold regarding respon- model include community action programs and rehabilita-
sibility for the causes and solutions of problems but that tive therapy. Brickman et al. (1982) have claimed that the
“they cannot, as social actors, avoid making such advantage of this model is that it actively involves clients in
assumptions”(p. 370). Karuza, Zevon, Rabinowitz, and finding solutions to their problems while discounting the
Brickman (1982) pointed out that in a clinical setting the clients’ past failures. The potential drawback of this model,
assumptions implicit in these orientations might color the however, is that it may make clients feel undue pressure at
diagnosis, treatment choice, intervention strategy, and be- having to continually solve problems they did not create
havior of helpers. Brickman et al.’s model of helping and and may foster a negative view of the world.
coping has been applied to a number of settings and clini-
Enlightenment Model
cal concerns including cancer patients’ beliefs about their
cancer, prevention of suicidal behavior, interventions with This model holds clients responsible for causing their prob-
the elderly, drug addiction, congruence in beliefs between lems but not for solving them. In this model, clients are seen
client and counselor, alcoholism, and cross-cultural counsel- as guilty individuals whose lives are out of control. The aim
ing (see Avants, Margolin, & Singer, 1993; Jack & Williams, of this model is to provide clients with enlightenment about
1991; Karuza, Zevon, Gleason, Karuza, & Nash, 1990; Morojele the nature of their problems and the difficulty inherent in
& Stephenson, 1992; Tracey, 1988; West & Power, 1995; Young solving them. Clients are expected to submit to the will of
& Marks, 1986, respectively). the authority figure or helper who knows the course of ac-
tion to follow. The advantage of this model is that it provides
FOUR ORIENTATIONS OF THE MODEL clients with a sense of relief that their problems are beyond
their control and a sense of shared community and suffering.
Medical Model A disadvantage of this model is that it may lead clients to
In this model, clients are not held responsible for either structuring their entire life around the source of authority.
the cause of their problem or its solution. Clients are seen In addition to the role played by attributions of responsi-
as suffering from illness they did not cause and must accept bility, counselors’ etiology attributions also seem important
the interventions of trained experts in order to improve. in their influence on counselor decision making and help-
Helpers are to use their training to identify the client’s prob- ing behavior. According to Dumont (1993), the formation
lems and provide the necessary services for change. According of an etiology assessment “inevitably implicates a particu-
to Brickman et al. (1982), the advantage of this model is lar vision of the development of a disorder and the treat-
that it allows clients to seek and accept help without being ment of choice for remediating it” (p. 197). Most of the
blamed for their weakness. However, the model also promotes research in this area has focused on larger assessments of
a sense of client dependency. causality. For example, Weiner (1979) identified three di-
mensions along which attributional judgments may vary:
locus of cause (internal vs. external), the pervasiveness of
Moral Model
the cause (global vs. specific), and the degree to which the
This model can be seen as the converse of the medical model cause is seen as changing over time (stable vs. unstable).
and is best typified by self-help movements. In this model, Likewise, Hansen (1980) distinguished between three gen-

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 305


Kernes and McWhirter

eral types of explanations observers can make for a behavior: selors varied their helping orientation according to the type
They may attribute the behavior to dispositions of the person of disorder presented. Specifically, counselors subscribed
(traits, personality, etc.); they may attribute the behavior to to a compensatory model of helping for the adjustment disor-
characteristics of the stimulus with which the person is inter- der and were equally divided between the compensatory and
acting; or they may attribute the behavior to the circumstances the moral models of helping for the identity disorder. Coun-
in which the behavior is taking place. Finally, research has iden- selors did not endorse the medical or the enlightenment mod-
tified a number of expectancies and biases (e.g., dispositional, els for either disorder type. Worthington and Atkinson also
representative, availability) implicated in determining where examined the etiology attributions the counseling center coun-
a client’s problem lies (see Batson, O’Quin, & Pych, 1982; selors made for these disorders. In general, counselors selected
Morrow & Deidan, 1992). It is surprising that very little re- etiology attributions logically consistent with the assumption
search has examined specific etiology attributions. Most of of an internal cause for an identity disorder and an external
the research that has been conducted on specific etiology as- cause for an adjustment disorder. Specifically, counselors ranked
sumptions has looked only at clients’ assumptions about the lack of self-understanding as the primary cause of the iden-
nature of their problems. tity disorder (86%) and specific trauma as the primary cause
Townsend (1975) studied cross-cultural beliefs of mental of the adjustment disorder (55%). Moreover, specific trauma
health clients and found that American mental health clients was never endorsed as a primary etiology attribution for
tended to believe their disorders were due to environmental the identity disorder, whereas lack of self-understanding
factors, whereas German mental health clients tended to be- received only minimal endorsement as the primary cause
lieve both that mental disorders are reactions to psychological of adjustment disorder.
events and are curable and that mental disorders are inherited One limitation Worthington and Atkinson (1993) ac-
and cannot be cured. Foulks, Persons, and Merkel (1986) at- knowledged in their investigation was the examination of
tempted to link beliefs about causes of psychological prob- counseling center counselors’ attributions for only female
lems to therapeutic process. They developed a 47-item inven- clients. They suggested that attributions of responsibility
tory of psychiatric patients beliefs about their own illnesses. and etiology and recommended counseling strategy might
They found that patients who endorsed medical-model causes vary as a function of client sex, and they called for addi-
for their illness and rejected nonmedical-model causes made tional research investigating counselors’ attributions of male
more visits to psychiatric clinics than patients who endorsed clients. A long line of research on gender bias and stereo-
a nonmedical model of causation and rejected a medical types indicates that gender may play an important part in
model. Similarly, Pistrang and Barker (1992) developed an attributions made by mental health professionals. Becker
inventory of clients’ beliefs about the causes and treatments and Lamb (1994); Broverman, Broverman, Clarkson,
of their own psychological problems. Beliefs about cause and Rosenkrantz, and Vogel (1970); O’Malley and Richardson
treatment were strongly associated (e.g., clients who saw their (1985); Robertson and Fitzgerald (1990); and Swenson and
problems as resulting from psychodynamic issues such as Ragucci (1984) all spoke of differential evaluations made
early childhood problems also endorsed a psychodynamic of male and female clients. Much of this research indicated
type of treatment). that women are perceived in more negative terms than are
Still, little is known about the specific etiology beliefs and men and that psychotherapy may function to reinforce
treatment recommendations of mental health professionals. traditional roles for women (see Abramowitz, Abramowitz,
Atkinson, Worthington, Dana, and Good (1991) investigated Jackson, & Gomes, 1973; American Psychological Associa-
clients’ and counselors’ beliefs about the etiology of psycho- tion Task Force, 1975). Other research suggested that
logical problems and preferences for counseling orientations. counseling and psychotherapy might also function to
They used an empirical procedure to produce a list of spe- reinforce traditional roles for men (see Costrich, Feinstein,
cific beliefs about the causes of psychological problems (ir- Kidder, Marcek, & Pascale, 1975; Fitzgerald & Cherpas, 1985;
rational concerns, career or academic difficulties, physical ill- Robertson & Fitzgerald, 1990). Still other research reported
ness, trauma or pain, lack of social skills, genetics, and bad that mental health professionals might not hold differential
luck). Results indicated that the majority of both clients and views of men and women (see Phillips & Gilroy, 1985; Poole
counselors ranked irrational concerns as the cause of psy- & Tapley, 1988; Smith, 1980).
chological problems. The major focus of the study, however, The present study seeks to build on the findings of
was on the etiology attributions made by clients, therefore Worthington and Atkinson (1993) and Brickman et al.
providing little data on the attributions of counselors. In ad- (1982) regarding counselors’ attributions of responsibility
dition, the study examined etiology attributions of psycho- and etiology and to extend these findings by adding client
logical problems in general rather than focusing on attribu- sex as an independent variable. The purpose of the present
tions made for specific disorders. study is to investigate the relationship between disorder
Worthington and Atkinson (1993) applied the Brickman type and client sex on counselors’ ratings of client respon-
et al. (1982) model of responsibility attribution to the at- sibility, problem etiology, and recommended counseling
tributions made by counseling center counselors of clinical strategy. On the basis of the findings of Worthington and
vignettes describing a student with an adjustment disorder Atkinson, we predicted that the majority of counselors
or an identity disorder. Among their findings was that coun- would endorse a compensatory model of helping for an ad-

306 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C o u n s e l o r s ’ A t t r i b u t i o n o f R e s p o n s i b i l i t y, E t i o l o g y, a n d C o u n s e l i n g S t r a t e g y

justment disorder and would be “equally” divided in their cognitive-behavioral, humanistic-existential, family systems,
endorsement of a moral model and a compensatory model eclectic, and other) because of Worthington and Atkinson’s
of helping for an identity disorder. We also predicted that finding that few respondents subscribed to some of the theo-
counselors would select etiology attributions logically con- ries listed on the CPPS.
sistent with an internal cause for identity problems and an The third and fourth parts of the CPPS consist of two
external cause for adjustment problems. vignettes taken from the DSM-III Case Book (Spitzer,
In addition to these replication hypotheses, based on re- Skodol, Gibbon, & Williams, 1981) and a series of ratings
search on dispositional biases in counseling, we predicted that addressed client responsibility for causing and resolv-
that counselors would attribute greater internal causality, ing problems, counselor etiology beliefs, and recommended
greater stability, and greater controllability to the symptoms counseling strategy. One vignette describes a female client
of clients described as exhibiting an identity problem than exhibiting symptoms of an adjustment problem, and the
to clients described as exhibiting an adjustment problem. other describes a female client exhibiting symptoms of
Likewise, we predicted that counselors would report being an identity problem. Worthington and Atkinson (1993)
more likely to engage in interventions aimed at changing the developed the vignettes to test Brickman et al.’s (1982)
client’s environment or activities for clients exhibiting an assumption that counselors would vary their model of
adjustment problem and would be more likely to engage in helping based on the client’s responsibility for causing and
interventions aimed at changing the client’s thoughts or feel- resolving the problem and on Furman and Ahola’s (1989)
ings for clients exhibiting an identity problem. assumption that counselors would vary their attributions
of etiology based on the type of problem presented. Two
METHOD additional vignettes were created in the present study by
altering the sex description of the clients in the original
Participants vignettes to include descriptions of male clients with adjust-
ment and identity problems.
Participants were 167 counselors (85 women and 82 men)
After reading the vignettes, participants rated a client’s
from university counseling centers in the United States and
responsibility for causing and resolving their problems on
Canada. Participants ranged in age from 24 to 65 years, with
separate 6-point scales that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 6
a mean age of 46.2 years and standard deviation of 9.15
(completely). Participants also rank ordered the top 3 causes
years. The participants held advanced degrees in the fol-
of the client’s problems from a list of 12 causes of psycho-
lowing specialties: counseling psychology (81), clinical psy-
logical problems (genetics, unresolved feelings, specific
chology (41), counselor education (24), social work (11),
trauma, social isolation, stress, lack of self-understanding,
and other (10). The participants held the following posi-
dysfunctional family, sick society, maladaptive learning, bio-
tions: center director (90), senior staff (52), assistant direc-
logical imbalances, physical illness, irrational thinking) taken
tor/training director/program coordinator (14), staff (5), and
from existing etiology models (see Atkinson et al., 1991;
other (6). Years of professional experience beyond highest
Daws, 1967; Foulks et al., 1986; Kedric, 1985; Maloney,
degree ranged from 1 to 36, with a mean of 12.5 years and
1985; Robertson & Fitzgerald, 1990). Finally, participants
standard deviation of 9.58 years.
were asked to indicate their likelihood of engaging in coun-
The following theoretical orientations were represented among
seling strategies aimed at changing the client’s environment,
the participants: psychodynamic (34), cognitive-behavioral
changing the client’s interactions, and changing the client’s
(37), humanistic-existential (18), eclectic (69), family systems
thoughts and feelings. Participants were asked to rate their
(4), and other (5). The ethnic (self-reported) representation of
likelihood of engaging in each strategy on separate 6-point
the participants was Native American (3), African American
scales that ranged from 1 (extremely unlikely) to 6 (extremely
(5), European American/White (154), Asian American (1),
likely). No reliability or validity data is available for the CPPS.
Chicano/Latino/Hispanic (1), multiethnic (1), and other (2, who
Helping-Coping Attribution Scale. The therapist version of
did not indicate ethnicity).
the Helping-Coping Attribution Scale (HCAS; Tracey, 1988)
Instruments was used to measure counselors’ helping and coping attribu-
tions toward vignette clients. Tracey developed the HCAS to
Counselor Preferences and Practices Survey. A revised form measure the blame and control dimensions of Brickman et
of the Counselor Preferences and Practices Survey (CPPS; al.’s (1982) model of responsibility attribution. The HCAS
Worthington & Atkinson, 1993) was used in the present contains four items. Two of the items measure attribution of
study to assess attributions of responsibility and etiology the cause of the client’s current difficulty (e.g., “this client is
and to examine recommended counseling strategies. The responsible for his or her current problem” and “this client’s
CPPS consists of four parts. The first part requests basic problem is more a result of the situation he or she is in rather
demographic information (age, sex, ethnicity) of the par- than his or her own inability to cope”). The remaining two
ticipants. The second part requests information regarding items were designed to measure attributions regarding the
years of professional experience, type of degree, and theo- client’s responsibility for resolving the problem (e.g., “solving
retical orientation. In the present study, theoretical orien- the client’s problem is more the client’s responsibility than
tation was collapsed into six categories (psychodynamic, mine” and “this client would not be able to change without

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 307


Kernes and McWhirter

my or others’ aid”). Counselors rated each of the four items randomly to each counseling center director listed in
using a 7-point Likert-type scale that ranged from 1 (very Gallagher’s (1995) National Survey of Counseling Center
strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree). The items on the Directors in the United States and Canada. In the cover let-
two scales were summed to produce two scores (blame and ter, directors were asked for their assistance in gathering
control). High scores on the Blame scale indicated the client data by passing the materials on to an experienced staff
was viewed as responsible for the cause of the problem, and member. Specifically, they were informed that the condi-
high scores on the Control scale indicated the client was tions of the study necessitated responses from experienced
viewed as responsible for the solution to the problem. mental health professionals with ongoing caseloads of cli-
Tracey (1988) reported 1-week test–retest reliability for ents. Directors were invited to participate themselves if they
the Blame and Control subscales of the therapist version of believed they met the criteria.
the HCAS as .71 and .68, respectively. Internal consistency Participants read packets consisting of one of four versions
of the subscales was estimated using the Spearman-Brown of the CPPS (male identity problem vignette with male ad-
Prophecy formula to be .45 for the Blame subscale and .50 justment problem vignette, male identity problem vignette
for the Control subscale. Tracey also reported that the Blame with female adjustment problem vignette, female identity
and Control subscales were relatively independent of each problem vignette with female adjustment problem vignette,
other with correlations between scales at –.15. or female adjustment problem vignette with male identity
Causal Dimension Scale. A revised version of Russell’s problem vignette). Vignettes were counterbalanced accord-
(1982) Causal Dimension Scale (CDS) was used to assess ing to problem type and client sex to prevent any potential
counselors’ attributions of the locus of causality, stability, and order effects. Three hundred thirty packets were mailed in
controllability of clients’ problems. The CDS was originally the initial mailing. Six packets from this initial mailing were
designed to assess how an individual perceived the causes returned as nondeliverable. A follow-up letter was mailed 3
they have stated for an event. The CDS contains nine items weeks after the initial mailing. A second follow-up letter was
with three questions each measuring the subscales of Locus mailed 3 weeks after the first reminder. This procedure re-
of Causality, Stability, and Controllability. Responses were sulted in the return of 177 questionnaires, which represented
measured on 9-point semantic differential scales. A total score a 53.6% return rate. Ten of these questionnaires were re-
for each subscale was arrived at by summing the responses jected due to incomplete or missing data.
for each scale (Locus of Causality items = 1, 5, 7; Stability
items = 3, 6, 8; Controllability items = 2, 4, 9). High scores RESULTS
on these scales indicated a cause was perceived as internal,
stable, and controllable, whereas low scores indicated a cause Endorsement of Model
was perceived as external, unstable, and uncontrollable.
Russell investigated the validity of the individual semantic Ratings on the attributions of responsibility for causing and
differential scales by subjecting each item to separate analy- solving the problems described in the vignettes were summed
ses of variance. For each item, the largest main effect was across the two measures assessing responsibility (CPPS and
observed for the dimension the item was intended to mea- HCAS) and recorded as low (ratings of 1–10) or high (rat-
sure. The subscales were found to be only moderately re- ings of 11–20). Each model in Brickman et al.’s (1982)
lated to one another, with correlations ranging from .19 to conceptualization posits a different degree of responsibil-
.28. Russell found the three scales to be internally consis- ity for causing and solving the problem at hand. Briefly, the
tent. Alpha coefficients for the Locus of Causality, Stability, medical model does not hold clients responsible for either
and Controllability subscales were .87, .84, and .73, respec- causing or solving their problems. The moral model holds
tively. Similarly, Vallerand and Richer (1988) reported alpha clients responsible for both the cause and the solution of
coefficients for the Locus of Causality, Stability, and Con- their problems. The compensatory model holds clients re-
trollability scales at .80, .73, and .50. Abraham (1985) tested sponsible only for solving their problems, whereas the en-
the usefulness of the CDS as a tool for measuring attribu- lightenment model holds them responsible only for caus-
tions made about the mental health of others. Alpha coeffi- ing their problems. Cross-tabulations were developed to
cients for the Locus of Causality, Stability, and Controllabil- assess these models and the results presented in Table 1.
ity subscales were .68, .90, and .88. The CDS was modified Table 1 indicates that participants subscribed to all four
for use in the present study by changing the wording of the of the models for both problem types presented. For the
items from self-attributional statements (e.g., “Is the cause adjustment problem, counselors were relatively evenly di-
something that reflects an aspect of yourself?”) to vided in their endorsements of a compensatory model
attributional statements made of clients (e.g., “Is the cause (48.80%) and a moral model (44.64%). Counselors also
something that reflects an aspect of the client?”). endorsed medical and enlightenment models (4.16% and
2.38%, respectively). For the identity problem, counselors
generally favored a moral model (51.20%) over a compen-
Procedure
satory model (37.95%). Again, small percentages of coun-
A packet consisting of a cover letter, the CPPS, the HCAS, selors also endorsed medical and enlightenment models
the CDS, and a self-addressed stamped envelope was mailed (4.81% and 6.02%, respectively).

308 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C o u n s e l o r s ’ A t t r i b u t i o n o f R e s p o n s i b i l i t y, E t i o l o g y, a n d C o u n s e l i n g S t r a t e g y

TABLE 1

Counselors’ Endorsement of Model by Problem Type and Client Sex

Problem Type Client Sex

Brickman et al.’s (1982) Model Type Adjustment % Identity % Male % Female %

Medical 7 4.16 8 4.81 5 2.90 10 6.17


Moral 75 44.64 85 51.20 76 44.18 84 51.85
Compensatory 82 48.80 63 37.95 83 48.25 62 38.27
Enlightenment 4 2.38 10 6.02 8 4.65 6 3.70
Note. Percentages reflect rounding error.

To determine if counselors altered their model of help- etiology attributions varied across the two disorders. For the
ing to match the disorder presented, a 2 (disorder type) by identity disorder, the majority of counselors (81.32%) clearly
4 (helping model) cross-tabulation was submitted to a chi- selected etiology attributions consistent with an internal cause
square analysis. The chi-square value was nonsignificant, (lack of self-understanding). The internal attributions of ir-
χ2(3, 334) = 5.741, p = .125, thus indicating that there was rational thinking and unresolved feelings accounted for 5.42%
no association between model endorsement and type of and 4.21% of etiology attributions, respectively. Thus, nearly
problem presented. 91% of counselors’ attributions reflected a belief in an inter-
Table 1 also indicates that counselors endorsed all four nal cause for identity problems.
models of helping for both male and female clients. For male For the adjustment problem, counselors selected more
clients, counselors were relatively evenly divided in their internal than external attributions. The internal attributions
endorsements of a compensatory model and moral model of of unresolved feelings, lack of self-understanding, and irra-
helping (48.25% and 44.18%, respectively). A small percent- tional thinking accounted for nearly 55% of counselors re-
age of counselors also endorsed enlightenment and medical sponses (38.09%, 8.92%, and 7.73%, respectively). The ex-
models of helping (4.65 and 2.90%, respectively). For female ternal attribution of specific trauma accounted for only
clients, counselors generally favored a moral model of helping 33.92% of counselors’ attributions.
(51.85%) to a compensatory model (38.27%). A small per- A 2 (problem type) by 12 (etiology attribution) was sub-
centage of counselors also endorsed medical and enlighten- jected to chi-square analysis to determine if counselors
ment models (6.17% and 3.70%, respectively). altered their attributions based on problem type presented.
Chi-square results were significant, χ2(9, 334) = 199.171,
Etiology Attributions p = .005. Thus, counselors’ attributions were associated with
problem type presented. A 2 (client sex) by 12 (etiology
The prediction that counselors would select etiology attri- attribution) cross-tabulation was performed to determine
butions logically consistent with an internal cause for an whether counselors made differential etiology attributions
identity problem and an external cause for an adjustment based on client sex. As can be seen in Table 2, counselors
problem was partially supported. As can be seen in Table 2, selected similar etiology attributions for both male and

TABLE 2
Counselors’ Etiology Attributions by Problem Type and Client Sex

Problem Type Client Sex

Etiology Attribution Adjustment % Identity % Male % Female %

Genetics 1 0.59 1 0.60 1 0.58 1 0.61


Sick society 0 0.00 0 0.00 0 0.00 0 0.00
Social isolation 2 1.19 1 0.60 3 1.74 0 0.00
Dysfunctional family 0 0.00 1 0.60 1 0.58 0 0.00
Biological imbalance 2 1.19 2 1.20 1 0.58 3 1.85
Lack of self-understanding 15 8.92 135 81.32 74 43.02 76 46.90
Unresolved feelings 64 38.09 7 4.21 39 22.67 32 19.75
Stress 8 4.76 6 3.61 8 4.65 6 3.70
Specific trauma 57 33.92 1 0.60 28 16.27 30 18.51
Physical illness 0 0.00 0 0.00 0 0.00 0 0.00
Maladaptive learning 6 3.57 3 1.80 5 2.90 4 2.46
Irrational thinking 13 7.73 9 5.42 12 6.97 10 6.17
Note. Percentages reflect rounding error.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 309


Kernes and McWhirter

female clients. Specifically, counselors selected lack of self- (Λ = .976 F = 2.74, p = .043, η = .02). A follow-up one-way
understanding as the primary etiology attribution for both ANOVA indicated a significant difference between groups for
men and women (43.02% and 46.90%, respectively). A large restructuring the client’s environment, F(1, 332) = 7.620,
number of counselors also selected the internal attribution p = .006. In general, counselors endorsed this intervention as
of unresolved feelings for both men and women (22.67% more appropriate for clients with an adjustment problem (M =
and 19.75%, respectively). Specific trauma was identified 3.48) than for clients with an identity problem (M = 3.09).
as the most important external etiology attribution for both
men and women (16.27% and 18.51%, respectively). DISCUSSION
Ratings on the Causal Dimension Scale Predictions concerning counselors’ endorsement of theo-
retical models were not supported. No significant relation-
A 2 × 2 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) (disor- ship was detected between endorsement of theoretical
der type by client sex) was conducted for the three ratings of model and disorder type. Counselors endorsed all of
the Causal Dimension Scale (locus of causality of the client’s Brickman et al.’s (1982) models of helping for both disor-
problem, stability of the client’s problem, and controllability der types. These results are in contrast to Worthington and
of the client’s problem). Atkinson’s (1993) findings that counseling center counse-
There was a significant main effect of type of problem lors endorsed only the compensatory and moral models of
(Λ = .852 F = 19.067, p = .001, η = .15). A follow-up one- helping across disorder types. The number of counselors
way analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated significant endorsing medical and enlightenment models of helping in
differences between groups for ratings of locus of causality, the present study was quite small, however. For the adjust-
F(1, 332) = 47.319, p = .001. In general, clients described ment disorder, 4.16% of counselors endorsed a medical model
as experiencing an identity problem were rated as having and 2.38% endorsed an enlightenment model. For the iden-
greater internal locus of causality than those clients described tity disorder, 4.81% of counselors endorsed a medical model
as experiencing an adjustment problem. A follow-up one- and 6.02% endorsed an enlightenment model. A more chal-
way ANOVA also indicated a significant difference between lenging finding is that counselors “equally” endorsed a com-
groups for ratings of problem stability, F(1, 332) = 10.140, pensatory and moral model for helping clients with an
p = .002. Counselors ascribed more permanence to the adjustment disorder and favored a moral model for helping
symptoms of clients described as having an adjustment clients with an identity disorder. These findings contrast with
problem than to clients described as having an identity Worthington and Atkinson’s finding that counseling center
problem. A follow-up one-way ANOVA also revealed counselors favored a compensatory model for helping cli-
significant differences between groups for ratings of the con- ents with an adjustment disorder and were equally divided
trollability of the problem, F(1, 332) = 30.845, p = .001. in their endorsement of moral and compensatory models for
Counselors ascribed more control over their problems to helping clients with an identity disorder.
clients with an identity problem than to clients with an Although no significant relationship was observed be-
adjustment problem. Means and standard deviations are tween endorsement of theoretical model and disorder type,
presented in Table 3. clearly the majority of college counseling center counse-
lors adhere to either a moral or compensatory model of
Ratings on Specific Interventions helping (91.3%). These findings, although not as encourag-
A 2 × 2 MANOVA (disorder type by client sex) was ing as those of Worthington and Atkinson (1993), should
be viewed favorably if one accepts Brickman et al.’s (1982)
conducted for ratings of the three specific interventions of
assumption that “models in which people are held respon-
restructuring the client’s environment, changing the client’s
sible for solutions (the compensatory and moral models)
interactions, and modifying the client’s thoughts or feelings.
are more likely to increase people’s competence than mod-
There was a significant main effect of type of problem
els in which they are not held responsible for solutions (the
medical and enlightenment models)” (p. 375).
TABLE 3 Although counselors in the present study generally favor
the moral and compensatory models of helping, they do
Counselors’ Ratings on the Causal not seem to distinguish between these models in their
Dimension Scale conceptualization of client problems. Both the moral and
compensatory models view clients as responsible for find-
Causal Dimension Scale ing solutions to their problems. The issue of responsibility
Locus of Causality Stability Controllability for causing those problems differentiates these models, with
the moral model holding clients responsible for causing their
Variable M SD M SD M SD
problems and the compensatory model avoiding discussion
Adjustment 16.68 4.69 11.32 4.77 11.64 4.41
of causal responsibility. It is possible that the scenario prob-
Identity 19.95 3.95 9.84 3.62 14.21 4.05 lems were too innocuous to enlist any significant differences
between endorsement of these two model types. Moreover,

310 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C o u n s e l o r s ’ A t t r i b u t i o n o f R e s p o n s i b i l i t y, E t i o l o g y, a n d C o u n s e l i n g S t r a t e g y

if these problems were too innocuous, causal responsibility tion. Certainly Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
might have been distorted. Further research should examine Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric
whether counselors endorse different model types when the Association, 1994) criteria for the diagnosis of adjustment
problems experienced by clients are more severe. and identity problems indicates different loci of client re-
Predictions concerning etiology attributions were partially sponsibility in contributing to their problems. There is a
supported. Counselors clearly selected attributions logically question as to whether it is biased for clinicians to assume
consistent with an internal cause for the identity problem. that clients presenting with different problems are differ-
Specifically, lack of self-understanding accounted for 81.32% entially responsible for the cause of those problems. Lopez
of the responses. This finding is consistent with Worthington (1989) has suggested there is a conceptual difference be-
and Atkinson (1993). Attributions were less supportive of tween a clinician who errs in adjusting diagnostic norms
predictions for the adjustment problem. In fact, counselors and one who errs because of prejudicial attitudes. It is pos-
more frequently endorsed internal etiology attributions than sible that counselors may make an error by not considering
external attributions. Although specific trauma did account individual differences in shaping their interventions and sim-
for a large percentage of counselors’ attributions (33.92%), ply evaluating client needs based on categorization into such
this is less than Worthington and Atkinson’s finding of 55% of gross membership as “adjustment problem” or “identity
counselors endorsing specific trauma. problem.” However, this error differs from one made be-
As seen in Table 2, lack of self-understanding, the most cause of the influence of prejudicial attitudes based on cli-
frequently endorsed etiology attribution for the identity ent background.
problem, received only minimal endorsement for the ad- A similar question arises regarding responsibility for over-
justment problem (8.92%). Likewise, specific trauma and coming one’s problems. If a client’s difficulties lie with the
unresolved feelings, the most frequently endorsed etiol- fact that there is something “about the client” (i.e., person-
ogy attributions for the adjustment problem, received only ality traits or characteristics), counselors are less likely to
minimal endorsement for the identity problem (.60% and hold that client responsible for overcoming their difficul-
4.21%, respectively). These results indicate that counse- ties than they are clients whose problems stem from a re-
lors do make differential etiology attributions based on action to an event. That is, clinicians may view dispositional
disorder type presented. characteristics as less amenable to change. Again, the prob-
Predictions concerning the three dimensions of the Causal lems presented in the current study were rather innocuous.
Dimension Scale (causality, stability, controllability) were It is quite possible that a different pattern of responsibility
generally supported. As predicted, the symptoms of clients attribution would emerge if the scenarios had presented
with an identity problem were rated as having greater in- more severe forms of identity and adjustment problems.
ternal locus of causality than were symptoms of clients with Certainly, one can logically argue that counselors should
an adjustment problem. Similarly, the prediction that coun- hold those who are contributing to the cause of their prob-
selors would attribute greater controllability to the symp- lems as more responsible for the resolution of those prob-
toms of clients with an identity problem was also supported. lems than they would clients whose problems are largely
These findings suggest that counselors view identity problems reactions to significant events. The answers to those ques-
as more dispositional in nature than adjustment problems. tions lie outside the scope of the present study.
However, the prediction that identity problems would be rated Predictions for specific interventions were supported for
as more stable than adjustment problems was not supported. changing the client’s environment. Counselors viewed mak-
In fact, counselors ascribed more permanence to the symp- ing changes in a client’s environment as more appropriate for
toms of adjustment problem clients. This finding is curious. clients with an adjustment problem than for clients with an
Perhaps it is possible that counselors paid attention to the identity problem. These results are generally consistent with
larger context of clients’ symptoms (i.e., reaction to a mother’s Royce and Muehlke’s (1991) findings that internal attribu-
death for the adjustment problem and a general sense of not tions of clients’ problems were associated with treatment aimed
knowing who one is for the identity problem). Erikson’s (1980) at changing the person, whereas stable attributions were linked
personality theory suggests that identity confusion, although to strategies aimed at changing behaviors and systems. It is
pervasive, lasts only for a circumscribed period from adoles- unlikely that given such limited information as case vignettes,
cence through early adulthood. In this sense, a mother’s death counselors were able to form a complete treatment plan.
is a permanent event, whereas identity confusion is tempo- However, the fact that any specific interventions were en-
rary. This explanation is tenuous. Future research might ex- dorsed indicates how quickly such a plan can be set in mo-
plore how variations in descriptions of disorder types might tion. Meehl (1960) suggested that clinicians formulate a
affect ratings of symptom permanence. Further research would client’s problems in a few sessions and that these formula-
be more informative if it allowed counselors to provide a ra- tions remain largely unchanged over the course of therapy.
tionale for their responses. For example, counselors could be Client sex seemed to play an insignificant role in the
asked to provide commentary at the end of the vignettes de- present study. No significant results were observed for the
scribing their working hypotheses. influence of client sex in counselors’ endorsement of theo-
Whether these findings reflect a “dispositional bias” or retical models or in etiology attributions. Likewise, client
are merely good clinical judgment remains an open ques- sex failed to influence ratings on the CDS and ratings

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 311


Kernes and McWhirter

concerning specific interventions. This seems in opposition Atkinson, D. R., Worthington, R. L., Dana, D. M., & Good, G. E. (1991).
to claims made for differential attributions based on client Etiology beliefs, preferences for counseling orientations, and counsel-
ing effectiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 258–264.
sex (see Phillips & Gilroy, 1985, for an overview). One plau- Avants, S. K., Margolin, A., & Singer, J. (1993). Psychological interven-
sible explanation for this finding is that counselors may be tions and research in the oncology setting. An integrative framework.
trained to pay particular attention to a client’s sex when Psychotherapy, 30, 1–10.
making clinical decisions in order to avoid any potential “gen- Batson, C. D. (1975). Attribution as a mediator of bias in helping. Jour-
der biasing.” In an “innocuous” situation such as presented nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 455–466.
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., & Pych, V. (1982). An attribution theory analy-
here, client sex is not seen as a salient attribute in making sis of trained helpers’ inferences about clients’ needs. In T. A. Wills
diagnostic and treatment decisions. At the same time, how- (Ed.), Basic processes in helping relationships (pp. 59–80). New York:
ever, counselors are inextricably linked to their own gender Academic Press.
role socialization and hence may make decisions based on Becker, D., & Lamb, S. (1994). Sex bias in the diagnosis of borderline
personality disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. Professional
such. Although a client’s sex may not play much of a role in
Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 55–61.
clinical decisions, counselors cannot escape entirely the in- Brickman, P., Rabinowitz, V. C., Karuza, J., Jr., Coates, D., Cohn, E., Kid-
fluence their own sex may have in such decisions. Further der, L. (1982). Models of helping and coping. American Psychologist, 37,
research should examine the interplay of client sex, counse- 368–384.
lor sex and presenting issue when a client’s sex might be Broverman, I. K., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F. E., Rosenkrantz, P., &
Vogel, S. R. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgments of
germane (pregnancy, impotence, etc.). In addition, future mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 1–7.
research should more precisely investigate the potential role Costrich, N., Feinstein, J., Kidder, L., Marcek, J., & Pascale, L. (1975).
differences in counselor training (i.e., specialty areas) might When stereotypes hurt: Three studies of penalties of sex-role rever-
play in counselors’ attributions. Further research must also sals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 520–530.
expand the restrictive nature of the definition of bias and of Daws, P. (1967). The aetiology of mental disorder. Bulletin of the British
Psychological Society, 20, 45–47.
the biased clinician (Lopez, 1989). For example, research Dumont, F. (1993). Inferential heuristics in clinical problem formula-
must investigate not only instances of “overpathologizing” of tion: Selective review of their strengths and weaknesses. Professional
women but also explore the existence of “minimizing bias” Psychology: Research and Practice, 24, 196–204.
in the evaluation of feminine gender role symptomatology. Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Norton.
In addition to some of the limitations already discussed, Feinberg, J. (1970). Doing and deserving: Essays in the theory of responsibility.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
the present study is limited by threats to internal and exter- Fitzgerald, L. F., & Cherpas, C. C. (1985). On the reciprocal relationship
nal validity common to analogue studies. Although reliabil- between gender and occupation. Rethinking the assumptions concerning
ity and validity data are available for some of the instruments masculine career development. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 27,
used in the present study, the lack of reliability and validity 109–122.
Foulks, E. F., Persons, J. B., & Merkel, R. L. (1986). The effect of patients’
data for the CPPS and the marginal reliability of the HCAS
beliefs about their illnesses on compliance in psychotherapy. American
limits the conclusions that can be drawn. However, the CPPS Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 340–344.
does use some of the suggestions Worell and Robinson (1994) Furman, B., & Ahola, T. (1989). Adverse effects of psychotherapeutic
make for improving analogue research methods, and the beliefs: An application of attribution theory to the critical study of
CPPS and HCAS represent the only measures specifically psychotherapy. Family Systems Medicine, 7, 183–195.
Gallagher, R. (1995). National survey of counseling center directors [mono-
designed to test Brickman et al.’s model of responsibility graph]. Alexandria, VA: International Association of Counseling Services.
attribution. The development of more reliable and valid mea- Hansen, R. D. (1980). Commonsense attribution. Journal of Personality
sures is clearly needed. Results of this study cannot be and Social Psychology, 39, 996–1009.
directly generalized to college counseling centers. Although Harvey, J. H., & Galvin, K. S. (1984). Clinical implications of attribution
this study did employ university counseling center counse- theory and research. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 15–33.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.
lors as participants, judgments made of fictional “clients” may Jack, R., & Williams, J. (1991). Attribution and intervention in self-
only remotely parallel diagnostic and treatment decisions poisoning. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 64, 345–358.
made with actual student clients. Logically, additional research Karuza, J., Jr., Zevon, M. A., Gleason, T. A., Karuza, C. M., & Nash, L.
needs to include counselor evaluations of actual clients. (1990). Models of helping and coping, responsibility attributions, and
well-being in community elderly and their helpers. Psychology and
Aging, 5, 194–208.
REFERENCES Karuza, J., Jr., Zevon, M. A., Rabinowitz, V. C., & Brickman, P. (1982).
Attribution of responsibility by helpers and recipients. In T. A. Wills
Abraham, I. L. (1985). Causal attributions of depression: Reliability of (Ed.), Basic processes in helping relationships (pp. 107–129). New York:
the Causal Dimension Scale in research on clinical inference. Psycho- Academic Press.
logical Reports, 56, 415–418. Kedric, H. C. (1985). Attitudes toward the etiology of psychopathology
Abramowitz, S. I., Abramowitz, C., Jackson, C., & Gomes, B. (1973). in members of a fundamentalist Christian church. Dissertation Ab-
The politics of clinical judgment. What nonliberal examiners infer stracts International, 47, 1703B. (University Microfilms No. 86-14914)
about women who do not stifle themselves. Journal of Consulting and Lopez, S. R. (1989). Patient variable biases in clinical judgment: Con-
Clinical Psychology, 41, 385–391. ceptual overview and methodological considerations. Psychological
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Bulletin, 106, 184–203.
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Lopez, S. R., & Wolkenstein, B. H. (1990). Attributions, person perception, and
American Psychological Association Task Force. (1975). Report of the clinical issues. In S. Graham & V. S. Folkes (Eds.), Attribution theory: Applica-
task force on sex bias and sex-role stereotyping in psychotherapeutic tions to achievement, mental health, and interpersonal conflict (pp. 103–121).
practice. American Psychologist, 30, 1169–1175. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

312 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


C o u n s e l o r s ’ A t t r i b u t i o n o f R e s p o n s i b i l i t y, E t i o l o g y, a n d C o u n s e l i n g S t r a t e g y

Maloney, H. N. (1985). Theories of psychopathogy [Monograph]. Pasadena, Spitzer, R. L., Skodol, A. E., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W.
CA: Fuller Theological Seminary. (1981). DSM-III casebook. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Meehl, P. (1960). The cognitive activity of the clinician. American Psy- Association.
chologist, 15, 19–27. Strohmer, D. C., Biggs, D. A., Keller, K. E., & Thibodeau, J. R. (1984).
Morojele, N., & Stephenson, G. (1992). The Minnesota model in the Clinical judgment and affective disorders. Journal of Counseling Psy-
treatment of addictions: A social psychological assessment of changes chology, 31, 99–103.
in beliefs and attributions. Journal of Community and Applied Social Strohmer, D. C., Haase, R. F., Biggs, D. A., & Keller, K. E. (1982). Process
Psychology, 2, 25–41. models of counselor judgment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29,
Morrow, K. A., & Deidan, C. T. (1992). Bias in the counseling process: 597–606.
How to recognize and avoid it. Journal of Counseling & Development, Swenson, E. V., & Ragucci, R. (1984). Effects of sex-role stereotypes and
70, 571–577. androgynous alternatives on mental health judgments of psychothera-
Murdock, N. L., & Freemont, S. K. (1989). Attributional influences in coun- pists. Psychological Reports, 54, 475–481.
selor decision making. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 417–422. Townsend, J. M. (1975). Cultural conceptions, mental disorders and so-
O’Malley, K. M., & Richardson, S. (1985). Sex bias in counseling: Have cial roles: A comparison of Germany and America. American Socio-
things changed? Journal of Counseling and Development, 63, 294–299. logical Review, 40, 739–752.
Phillips, R. D., & Gilroy, F. D. (1985). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical Tracey, T. J. (1988). Relationship of responsibility attribution congru-
judgments of mental health: The Broverman’s findings reexamined. ence to psychotherapy outcome. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy-
Sex Roles, 12, 179–193. chology, 7, 131–146.
Pistrang, N., & Barker, C. (1992). Client’s beliefs about psychological Vallerand, R. J., & Richer, F. (1988). On the use of the causal dimension
problems. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 5, 325–335. scale in a field setting: A test with confirmatory factor analysis in suc-
Poole, F. D., & Tapley, A. (1988). Sex roles, social roles, and clinical judg- cess and failure situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ments of mental health: The Broverman’s findings reexamined. Sex 54, 704–712.
Roles, 12, 265–272. Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experi-
Robertson, J., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1990). The (mis)treatment of men: ences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.
Effects of client gender role and life style on diagnosis and attribution Weiner, B. (1993). On sin versus sickness: A theory of perceived respon-
of pathology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 3–9. sibility and social motivation. American Psychologist, 48, 957–965.
Royce, W. S., & Muehlke, C. V. (1991). Therapists’ causal attributions of West, R., & Power, D. (1995). Alcoholic’s beliefs about the responsibil-
clients’ problems and selection of intervention strategies. Psychologi- ity for, and recovery from, their condition. Drug and Alcohol Review,
cal Reports, 68, 379–386. 14, 55–62.
Russell, D. (1982). The Causal Dimension Scale: A measure of how in- Worell, J., & Robinson, D. (1994). Reinventing analogue methods for
dividuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, research with women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 463–476.
42, 1137–1145. Worthington, R. L., & Atkinson, D. R. (1993). Counselors’ responsibil-
Schmidt, G., & Weiner, B. (1988). An attribution-affect-action theory of ity and etiology attributions, theoretical orientations, and counseling
behavior: Replications of judgments of help-giving. Personality and strategies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 295–302.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 610–621. Young, R. A., & Marks, S. E. (1986). Understanding attributional pro-
Smith, M. L., (1980). Sex bias in counseling and psychotherapy. Psycho- cesses in cross-cultural counseling. International Journal for the Ad-
logical Bulletin, 87, 392–407. vancement of Counseling, 9, 319–330.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 313


Risk Factors for Suicidality Among Clients With Schizophrenia

Robert C. Schwartz and Benjamin N. Cohen

Among clients with schizophrenia, suicidality is associated with extreme personal distress, an increased number of inpatient
hospitalizations, increased health care expenditures, and early mortality. This study attempted to identify risk factors for current
suicidality in clients diagnosed with schizophrenia (N = 223). Results indicated that severity of depressive symptoms most strongly
correlated with degree of suicidality. Younger age and recent traumatic stress each significantly predicted suicidality independent
of depressive symptoms. Stepwise regression procedures showed that the combination of depression, younger age, and trau-
matic stress might provide a general prediction model for suicidality among clients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Counseling
implications of these findings are outlined.

S
chizophrenia is a common mental disorder with Sheehy, & Baker, 1995; K. J. Kaplan & Harrow, 1996,
international prevalence rates of approximately 1999) reported that increased hallucinations and delu-
1%. In the United States, prevalence rates range sions were indicative of suicidal risk. Therefore, severe
from .6% to 1.9% (World Health Organization, psychotic symptoms may increase depression, leading to
1995). Symptoms of schizophrenia are usually a heightened risk of suicide (Fenton, McGlashan, Victor,
chronic and pervasive, leading to over $16 billion in annual & Blyler, 1997; K. J. Kaplan & Harrow, 1999).
health care expenditures (American Psychiatric Association Rossau and Mortensen (1997) asserted that men are
[APA], 1997). Costly hospitalizations and early mortality significantly more likely to complete suicide than are women,
are also common results of schizophrenic symptoms. One indicating that demographic characteristics may correlate with
of the most common reasons for premature death among suicidal risk. Other investigators have proposed that no clear
clients with schizophrenia is suicide (Black & Fischer, 1992). relationship exists between suicidal behavior and sex (Amador
Estimates of suicidal ideation (i.e., thoughts of killing oneself) et al., 1996; S. Cohen et al., 1994). As J. Cohen, Test, and Brown
range from 60% to 80%, and approximately 20%–42% of these (1990) explained, age may be more highly correlated with
clients actively attempt suicide (Drake, Gates, Whitaker, & suicidality than sex. There may be an inverse relationship be-
Cotton, 1985). Studies also indicate that approximately tween age and suicidal risk (Breier & Astrachan, 1984; Langley
10%–15% of clients with schizophrenia complete suicide & Bayatti, 1984). That is, younger clients tend to become more
(Black, Warrack, & Winokur, 1985; Landmark, Cernovsky, & suicidal more often. However, S. Cohen et al. (1994) and Amador
Mersky, 1987; Miles, 1977; Roy, Mazonson, & Pickar, 1984). et al. (1996) failed to link age with suicidal behavior. Therefore,
For example, Caldwell and Gottesman (1990) found that further research is necessary to explore the relationship between
50% of clients with schizophrenia attempt suicide, and up demographic characteristics and suicidality among clients with
to 13% complete the act. Given the significance of this prob- schizophrenia.
lem, it is important to identify risk factors associated with It has also been hypothesized that severe life stressors
suicidality among clients with schizophrenia. may elicit suicidality among clients with schizophrenia.
Prior research suggests that certain psychotic symptoms However, only one study found to date has attempted to
may correlate with suicidality. In a retrospective autopsy correlate life stressors with suicidality in clients with schizo-
study, Heila, Isometsa, Henriksson, and Heikkinen (1997) phrenia. In this study, Weiden and Roy (1992) asserted that
reported that depression and severe psychosis were related a combination of psychological stressors and poor overall
to suicides. These findings support prior research indicating functioning may lead to suicides. Perhaps severe life stres-
that depressive symptoms (e.g., extreme hopelessness) cor- sors combined with poor global functioning and inadequate
relate with suicides among clients with schizophrenia (S. coping mechanisms result in suicidal behaviors (Weiden &
Cohen, Lavelle, Rich, & Bromet, 1994; Drake & Cotton, 1986; Roy, 1992).
Milch, 1990; Roy, 1982). Regarding psychotic symptoms, As Heila et al. (1997) explained, many of the studies cited
several authors (S. Cohen et al., 1994; Gallagher, Dinan, earlier are hampered by methodological limitations. These

Robert C. Schwartz is an assistant professor and coordinator of the Addiction Counseling Certification Program in the Department of Counseling and Special
Education at The University of Akron, Akron, Ohio. Benjamin N. Cohen is a doctoral student in the Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and
Special Education at Michigan State University, East Lansing. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Robert C. Schwartz, Department of
Counseling and Special Education, The University of Akron, 127 Carroll Hall, Akron, OH 44325-5007 (e-mail: rcs@uakron.edu).

314 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Risk Factors for Suicidality Among Clients With Schizophrenia

include small sample size, the use of nonstandardized as- ployed. Average days worked during the previous month
sessment instruments, and retrospective (e.g., autopsy) were 1.9 (SD = 6.0). These characteristics are representa-
investigations that did not evaluate current suicidality. tive of clients with schizophrenia and thus reflect the
In addition, we noted that the assessment of important broader population (APA, 1997).
variables (e.g., medical/physical problems, family relationship All clients in this study met the diagnostic criteria for
problems, and work/school problems) were not included in schizophrenia according to the Diagnostic and Statistical
prior research designs. It is crucial to discriminate immediate Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; APA,
risk factors if eventual suicidal behaviors are to be forestalled. 1994). Diagnoses were based on the Structured Clinical
If immediate risk factors for suicidality are identified, clini- Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams,
cians may be better equipped to preempt suicidal gestures 1995). Clients with psychotic disorders other than schizo-
(Sullivan, Young, & Morgenstern, 1997). This study attempted phrenia (e.g., schizoaffective disorder or delusional disor-
to expand on prior research by investigating the relationship der) were excluded from participation in this study because
between current suicidality and demographic characteristics, the symptoms and course of other psychotic disorders dif-
psychological and emotional symptoms, life stressors, and fer significantly from those of schizophrenia (APA, 1994).
overall functioning in a large sample of clients with schizo-
phrenia. We hypothesized that a combination of current Instruments
symptoms, life stressors, and demographic characteristics
would predict suicidal risk. Specifically, we hypothesized that The Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) Scale (APA,
as life stressors and symptoms of illness intensified, suicidality 1994) is a 100-point instrument used to rate clients’ cur-
would become more severe among clients with schizophrenia rent overall functioning based on a standard global mental
who had certain demographic characteristics. health–illness continuum. The GAF is a widely used in-
strument with good validity and reliability that matches
clients’ self-reported functioning (Jones, Thornicroft, Coffey,
METHOD & Dunn, 1995; Piersma & Boes, 1995). To obtain interrater
reliability estimates for this study, 20% (45) of evaluations
Participants
were conducted simultaneously by two clinicians. Pearson
Participants for this study were selected from a crisis stabili- product–moment correlations of GAF scores showed an
zation unit (CSU) at a large, 12-county community agency interrater agreement of r = .91.
in Florida. The CSU is an emergency mental health The Functional Assessment Rating Scale (FARS; Ward &
facility designed to assess symptoms (including suicidal Dow, 1994) is a 17-item instrument used to assess emotional
risk), diagnostic criteria, social problems, global functioning, symptoms and psychosocial impairments in clients with
and treatment needs of clientele presenting with emotional severe mental illnesses, especially those with psychotic
crises. All clientele participate in a comprehensive evalua- disorders. Each item is scored independently after a struc-
tion conducted by trained mental health personnel. Then, tured clinical interview. Items are scored according to how
standardized instruments are completed, and a diagnosis (if severe a certain symptom or area of impairment is for that
any) is given. Archival data on all adult clients diagnosed client. A standardized 9-point rating system is used for all
with schizophrenia (any subtype) over a continuous 6-month items (1 = absent, 3 = mild, 5 = moderate, 7 = severe, 9 =
period were included in this study (N = 223). Participants extreme). In this study, the FARS was used to assess the
were not discriminated based on income, age (if over 18 following symptoms and psychosocial impairments: severity
years), severity of symptoms, physical disabilities, current of suicidality, depression, cognitive impairments, disturbed
or prior treatment history, or living status (i.e., homeless, thought processes, recent traumatic stress, medical/physical
supported housing, or independent living environments). problems, work/school problems, self-care deficits, and family
The only inclusion criterion for this study was a confirmed relationship problems. Severity of suicidality served as the
diagnosis of schizophrenia. Although clients gave written dependent variable, and all other items were used as indepen-
informed consent for participation in clinical evaluations, dent variables. All variables were specifically operationalized
consent for participation in this study was not obtained in behavioral terms, and all clinicians were trained to use the
because archival data were used from medical records. The FARS before clinical evaluations commenced.
identity of participants was protected at all stages of data According to Ward and Dow (1994), the FARS has good
collection and analysis. interrater reliability, good stability reliability, and good con-
Participants were between the ages of 18 and 79 years struct validity. In a more recent test of the instrument’s
(M = 38.8, SD = 10.1); 135 (61%) were women and 88 psychometric properties, Schwartz (1999) reported mean
(39%) were men. On average they evidenced a long-standing interrater reliability correlations of r = .88, mean stability
treatment history (M years of treatment = 13.2, SD = 8.8). reliability correlations of r = .86, mean concurrent reliabil-
Regarding race, 117 (52%) were Caucasian and 106 (48%) ity correlations of r = .89, and good construct validity. To
were African American. Participants were generally from a test interrater agreement on FARS items used in this study,
lower socioeconomic class; average monthly income was 20% (45) of evaluations were conducted simultaneously
$460 (SD = $302). In general, participants were unem- by two clinicians. Pearson product–moment correlations

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 315


Schwartz and Cohen

revealed the following statistics on interrater agreement: mentation included demographic information, a medical his-
traumatic stress (r = .95), depression (r = .93), suicidality tory, prior treatment history, a social history, and a family
(r = .92), cognitive impairments (r = .92), disturbed thought history. Clients then participated in two standardized clini-
processes (r = .90), medical/physical problems (r = .89), cal interviews, the Structured Clinical Interview for the Func-
self-care deficits (r = .88), family relationship problems tional Assessment Rating Scale (SCI-FARS; Ward et al., 1995)
(r = .80), and work/school problems (r = .61). and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et
al., 1995). Total interview duration averaged approximately
FARS Items 90 minutes. After each individual interview, clinicians
diagnosed the client (if appropriate) according to DSM-IV
Degree of suicidality was the dependent variable in this
criteria. All clinical interviews were conducted by master’s-
study. Suicidality was defined as the severity of reported
or doctoral-level clinicians specializing in the assessment of
and observed dangerousness to self. Severity of suicidality
mental disorders, and all diagnoses were confirmed by a board-
was operationalized as the degree to which the client mani-
fested suicidal ideation, intent to harm oneself, lethality of a certified psychiatrist. Then, each client was rated using the
current plan (if any), and opportunity or means of complet- GAF scale (APA, 1994) and the FARS (Ward & Dow, 1994).
All raters were blind to the protocol and existence of this
ing that plan. Clinicians specifically assessed the frequency
study at the time of evaluations. The University of Florida
and nature of suicidal thoughts, whether and to what degree
Institutional Review board, the Idaho State University Hu-
the client wanted and intended to commit suicide, whether
man Subjects Committee, and the Meridian Behavioral
they had a current plan (and if so how organized and lethal
the plan was), and how achievable the plan would be given Healthcare Risk Management Department approved the
their current psychological symptoms and their physical re- research design and methodology for this study.
sources. Only current suicidality was evaluated because past
suicidal ideation or gestures could confound results. RESULTS
The independent variables in this study were separated into
First, frequency distributions were obtained for all FARS
symptoms of illness and social problems or impairments.
items and for the GAF. Then, a multiple regression omni-
Regarding symptoms of illness, depression was measured
bus test was conducted to determine whether the combi-
according to degree of reported and observed hopelessness,
nation of symptoms, social problems, global functioning,
helplessness, depressed mood, and current neurovegitative
and demographic characteristics significantly correlated with
symptoms (e.g., disturbed sleep or appetite patterns). Cogni-
ratings of suicidality. Eleven independent variables were
tive impairments included cognitive disorganization, dis-
included in the omnibus test: 3 symptom variables (sever-
orientation, memory problems, poor abstract thought, and
ity of depression, cognitive impairments, and disturbed
poor attention or concentration. These impairments were mea-
thought processes), 5 social variables (traumatic stress,
sured using a mental status examination. Disturbed thought
medical/physical problems, work/school problems, self-care
processes referred to psychotic symptoms, such as delusions,
deficits, and poor family relationship), 1 measure of global
hallucinations, paranoia, and grandiosity. Medical/physical
functioning (the GAF score), and 2 demographic variables
problems included any current impairing physical condition,
(age and gender).
such as injuries or illnesses. Medical/physical problems were
Follow-up multiple regression tests were used to evalu-
rated according to self-report or clinician observation.
ate the predictive significance of each independent vari-
In terms of social problems or impairments, traumatic
able while controlling for the influence of other indepen-
stress was operationalized as upsetting memories, night ter-
dent variables. That is, specific tests were used as follow-up
rors, hypervigilance, and flashbacks related to a traumatic
measures to determine which independent variable(s) sig-
event (e.g., physical or sexual abuse). Work/school prob-
nificantly contributed to the overall prediction model. Fi-
lems were operationalized as the inability to cope with the
nally, a stepwise regression procedure was conducted to sta-
demands of work/school, poor performance on the job or
tistically determine the most effective overall prediction
with school grades, being terminated from work or expelled
model regarding suicidality. An alpha level of p < .01 was
from school, or inability to maintain consistent employ-
used for all statistical tests. A power analysis revealed that,
ment or registration status. Self-care deficits were defined
given a predicted moderate effect size and an alpha level of
as a reduced ability to independently perform basic adult
p < .01, a power of at least .80 could be expected using the
daily living skills, such as personal hygiene, cooking, clean-
sample size obtained in this study (J. Cohen, 1992; J. Cohen
ing, or money management. Family relationship problems
& Cohen, 1983).
referred to arguments at home, conflicted relationships with
Results indicated that, on average, participants manifested
parents or siblings, lack of emotional or social support, or
a mild degree of suicidality (M = 1.8, SD = 1.7), although a
lack of desired contact with family members.
full range of suicidality was observed in the sample popula-
tion (Range = 1–9). On average, clients also exhibited se-
Procedure
vere impairments in overall functioning as measured by the
On unsolicited presentation at the CSU, the psychosocial GAF scale (M = 42.5, SD = 15.3). The most severe symp-
history of each participant was documented. This docu- toms and psychosocial problems in this sample included

316 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Risk Factors for Suicidality Among Clients With Schizophrenia

disturbed thought processes (M = 4.7, SD = 2.5), cognitive TABLE 2


impairments (M = 4.4, SD = 2.1), and work/school prob-
lems (M = 4.3, SD = 2.7), respectively. These results are Results of Follow-Up Regression Tests for
consistent with a population of clients with schizophrenia Predictor Variables
presenting for psychological evaluation during a period of
crisis (APA, 1997). Table 1 displays a frequency distribu- Standardized
tion for all FARS items assessed in this study. FARS Item Beta Weights t value p value
Results showed that the omnibus regression model was sta-
Depression .39 7.40 *p = .001
tistically significant (F = 10.8, df = 11,211, p < .0001). For the Age –.04 –4.40 *p = .001
omnibus multiple regression model, the proportion of shared Traumatic stress .17 3.00 *p = .002
variance equaled .44, suggesting that 44% of the variance in Gender .41 2.00 p= .04
ratings of suicidality were accounted for by the 11 independent Disturbed thoughts .07 1.40 p= .17
Medical/physical
variables included in the model. Specific follow-up multiple re- problems .08 1.20 p = .23
gression tests showed that three variables were significantly and Self-care deficits .06 1.10 p = .24
independently associated with ratings of suicidality. These Work/school problems –.04 –1.10 p = .26
Cognitive impairments .05 0.84 p = .40
tests indicated that, when the influence of all other vari- GAF Scale score –.01 –0.81 p = .41
ables were controlled, severity of depression (t = 7.4, p = .001), Poor family relationship .01 0.29 p = .77
age (t = –4.4, p = < .001), and traumatic stress (t = 3.0, p = .002)
were significantly related to ratings on suicidality. Table 2 Note. See Table 1 Note. GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning.
*p < .01.
shows results of specific regression tests, including standardized
beta weights (parameter estimates) for all variables.
Finally, a stepwise regression procedure was used to obtain spent approximately one third of their life in treatment.
the most effective overall prediction model for suicidality. They were from a low socioeconomic group (based on
Results suggest that depressive symptoms alone accounted monthly income) and were generally unemployed. Most
for 28% of the variance in ratings of suicidality. When age was participants lived independently, with family members, or
added to the prediction model as a second variable, the two- in supported housing units in the community. Thus, we
variable combination accounted for 33% of the variance in believe that empirical results should adequately general-
ratings of suicidality. After traumatic stress was entered into ize to a population of clients with schizophrenia who are
the prediction equation, the three-variable model accounted receiving treatment for their illness.
for 38% of the variance in ratings of suicidality. The addition Our research hypothesis was supported by results obtained
of other independent variables added only minimally (and in- from multiple regression analyses, expanding the knowledge
significantly) to the prediction model. Therefore, although base regarding risk factors for suicidality among clients with
depressive symptoms were found to most strongly associate schizophrenia. First, severity of depression was found to
with ratings of suicidality, the most significant combination of significantly positively correlate with degree of suicidality.
risk factors was the three-variable model including depressive As depressive symptoms (e.g., hopelessness, helplessness,
symptoms, age, and recent traumatic stress. neurovegitative symptoms) increased, suicidal ideation and
intent also increased. Other investigators (S. Cohen et al.,
DISCUSSION 1994; Drake & Cotton, 1986; Heila et al., 1997; Milch, 1990;
Rossau & Mortensen, 1997; Roy, 1982; Taiminen, 1994) have
In general, the participants selected for this study manifested found this correlation as well. Moreover, it is well documented
characteristics common to clients with schizophrenia (APA, in clinical training manuals that depression is a dysphoric
1997). Participants were middle-aged and, on average, had mental state that may induce suicidal ideation or culminate
in suicide if not properly treated (H. I. Kaplan & Sadock,
TABLE 1 1995). Thus, the combination of severe depression and
schizophrenia may constitute a psychiatric emergency (Roy,
Frequency Distribution for FARS Ratings 1995). For example, King (1994) recently found that over a
7-year period 84% of persons who completed suicide had
FARS Item M SD symptoms of schizophrenia or severe depression, or both,
Disturbed thoughts 4.7 2.5 immediately preceding the suicidal act. Therefore, assessing and
Work/school problems 4.3 2.7 monitoring depressive symptoms is extremely important for
Poor family relationship 3.1 2.0 professionals serving clients with schizophrenia. We propose
Traumatic stress 2.2 1.8
Suicidality 1.8 1.7 that future research should attempt to ascertain which
Cognitive impairments 4.4 2.1 specific aspects of depression lead to increased suicidality in
Self-care deficits 4.0 2.4 this client population. If specific aspects of the depressive
Depression 2.6 1.9
Medical/physical problems 1.9 1.5
syndrome can be identified and linked to suicidal potential,
then clinicians can be better prepared to intervene with
Note. FARS = Functional Assessment Rating Scale. clients manifesting these symptoms.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 317


Schwartz and Cohen

Two other variables—age and traumatic stress—were COUNSELING IMPLICATIONS


each independently correlated with ratings of suicidality
in this sample. Age was negatively correlated with degree of Many of the findings reported in this study have direct clini-
suicidality; that is, younger clients tended to evidence more cal implications. Most important, practitioners should be
lethality than older clients. This finding has also been aware that a combination of severe depressive symptoms
previously reported in the literature (APA, 1997; Breier and recent traumatic stress among younger clients with
& Astrachan, 1984; J. Cohen et al., 1990; Langley & schizophrenia may indicate a substantially increased risk of
Bayatti, 1984), lending additional support to the hypoth- suicidality. Severity of depression and traumatic stress can
esis that younger clients with schizophrenia should be be directly and reliably assessed by counselors (e.g.,
monitored more conscientiously for suicidality, especially Zimmerman, 1994), and we strongly advise that clinicians
when depressed (Gupta, Black, Arndt, Hubbard, & evaluate these areas regardless of whether or not they are
Andreasen, 1998). However, research has yet to deter- presented by clients as primary treatment concerns. Treat-
mine why younger clients with schizophrenia tend to ment for depression and traumatic stress are both widely
manifest increased suicidality. Perhaps they are more available and therapeutically effective if used in a timely
profoundly affected by psychosocial stressors due to un- manner. Therefore, if counselors conscientiously monitor
derdeveloped coping mechanisms. Or possibly they dis- symptoms associated with an increased risk of suicidality
play more severe psychotic symptoms related to the early among clients with schizophrenia, preventive interventions
age of onset of this disorder, resulting in more severe au- can be used more effectively. In addition, it is important
ditory command hallucinations or persecutory delusions. for counselor training to include suicide assessment, in
A third hypothesis is that younger clients with schizo- general, as well as the impact on the counselor both emo-
phrenia may become more depressed and discouraged tionally and professionally of a client’s suicide (Foster &
about their future due to the long-term and pervasive McAdams, 1999).
nature of this disorder. As Schwartz and Petersen (1999) Should a mental health professional confront a client
explained, increased insight may lead to heightened with schizophrenia who manifests suicidality, standard
suicidality particularly among younger clients. Future re- crisis management procedures exist that serve as general
search should attempt to ascertain the primary factor(s) guidelines for appropriate interventions (see Gilliland &
resulting in increased suicidality in younger clients with James, 1997). Practitioners should first assess the client’s
schizophrenia. The early age of onset of schizophrenia lethality (ideation, intent, plan, and means). Then, symptoms
coupled with an increased risk of suicide among younger specifically associated with suicidal thoughts (e.g., depres-
clients necessitates heightened preventive interventions sion and traumatic stress) should be evaluated. Next, the
focused on this client population. extent and availability of social support networks (e.g., friends,
Recent traumatic stress was shown to positively corre- family members, case managers) should be ascertained.
late with suicidality in this sample of clients with schizo- Finally, treatment options should be discussed and clinical
phrenia. As severity of traumatic stress increased, suicidal interventions should be initiated immediately if necessary.
ideation and intent also increased. To our knowledge, the Thus, it is important to investigate biological, psychologi-
impact of recent traumatic stress on suicidality has not been cal, and social aspects of suicidality with clients who have
studied previously with this population of clients. The afore- schizophrenia (Harkavy-Friedman & Nelson, 1997). Treat-
mentioned finding is intuitively sensible, however, because ment options may range from more intensive outpatient
one’s intrapsychic perceptions and coping abilities in rela- counseling to the addition of new psychotropic medica-
tion to extreme stressors certainly affects one’s overall out- tions to inpatient hospitalization, depending on the client’s
look of life and death (Roy, 1995). If a client who is schizo- potential danger to self (APA, 1997). The optimum treat-
phrenic confronts a traumatic event, the intensity of that ment strategy should use the least restrictive environment
event, as well as the degree to which the client is able to necessary to provide increased personal safety and improved
manage the resulting affect, may have a direct impact on psychological functioning.
danger to self. Degree of suicidality may also be a function It should be noted, however, that this study did not inves-
of what type of traumatic event occurred, the duration of tigate past suicidal behaviors or suicides. Thus, results may
traumatic stress, and whether or not therapeutic interven- be generalizable only to those clients reporting current
tions were received. These specific factors were not included suicidality. In addition to the fact that current suicidality
in the data collection for this study. Clearly, further investi- does not always lead to suicidal gestures, there were several
gation concerning the relationship between traumatic stress other limitations to this study. Even though most indepen-
and suicidality is warranted because appropriate clinical dent variables (e.g., medical/physical problems) were not
interventions may preempt suicidal behaviors if the critical found to associate with suicidality in this study or in prior
aspects of the traumatic experience were known. Until ad- research, psychosocial variables not included in this inves-
ditional empirical research is conducted, we suggest that tigation may ultimately demonstrate an impact on
practitioners conscientiously monitor (and possibly increase suicidality. For example, substance abuse has been shown
therapeutic interventions with) clients with schizophrenia to correlate with increased suicidality in patients with
reporting recent traumatic stress. schizophrenia (Heila et al., 1997; Roy, 1995). Finally, the

318 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Risk Factors for Suicidality Among Clients With Schizophrenia

independent variables that were assessed in this study were Heila, H., Isometsa, E. T., Henriksson, M. M., & Heikkinen, M. E.
global areas of functioning. As stated previously, we did not (1997). Suicide in schizophrenia: A nationwide psychological au-
topsy study on age and sex-specific clinical characteristics of 92
evaluate which specific subcomponents of each variable suicide victims with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry,
(e.g., depression and traumatic stress) accounted for our 154, 1235–1242.
statistical results. Given the complex nature of suicidality Jones, S. H., Thornicroft, G., Coffey, M., & Dunn, G. (1995). A brief men-
among clients with schizophrenia, we recommend that in- tal health outcome scale: Reliability and validity of the Global Assess-
vestigators and clinicians use a biopsychosocial approach ment of Functioning. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 654–659.
Kaplan, H. I., & Sadock, B. J. (1995). Comprehensive textbook of psychia-
when conducting research or implementing interventions. try (6th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
We agree with Harkavy-Friedman and Nelson (1997) that Kaplan, K. J., & Harrow, M. (1996). Positive and negative symptoms as
ongoing assessment and clinical interventions must be used risk factors for late suicidal activity in schizophrenics versus
with clients with schizophrenia to reduce the likelihood of depressives. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 26, 105–121.
Kaplan, K. J., & Harrow, M. (1999). Psychosis and functioning as risk
suicidality. In addition, further empirical research in this
factors for later suicidal activity among schizophrenia and
complex area is clearly warranted. schizoaffective patients: A disease-based interactive model. Suicide
and Life-Threatening Behavior, 29, 10–24.
King, E. (1994). Suicide in the mentally ill: An epidemiological sample and
REFERENCES implications for clinicians. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 658–663.
Landmark, J., Cernovsky, Z. Z., & Mersky, H. (1987). Correlates of sui-
Amador, X. F., Friedman, J. H., Kasapis, C., Yale, S. A., Flaum, M., & Gorman, cide attempts and ideation in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psy-
J. M. (1996). Suicidal behavior in schizophrenia and its relationship to chiatry, 151, 18–20.
awareness of illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1185–1188. Langley, G., & Bayatti, N. (1984). Suicide in Exe Vale Hospital. British
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 463–467.
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Milch, W. E. (1990). The change of symptomatology in hospitalized sui-
American Psychiatric Association. (1997). Practice guidelines for the cidal patients. Crisis, 11, 44–51.
treatment of patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psy- Miles, C. P. (1977). Conditions predisposing to suicide: A review. Jour-
chiatry, 154(Suppl.), 1–63. nal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 164, 231–246.
Black, D. W., & Fischer, R. (1992). Mortality in DSM-III-R schizophre- Piersma, H. L., & Boes, J. L. (1995). Agreement between patient self-
nia. Schizophrenia Research, 7, 109–116. report and clinician rating: Concurrence between the BSI and the
Black, D. W., Warrack, G., & Winokur, G. (1985). The Iowa record-linkage GAF among psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51,
study, I–III. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 71–88. 153–157.
Breier, A., & Astrachan, B. (1984). Characterization of schizophrenic patients Rossau, C. D., & Mortensen, P. B. (1997). Risk factors for suicide in
who commit suicide. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 206–209. patients with schizophrenia: Nested case-control study. British Jour-
Caldwell, C. B., & Gottesman, I. I. (1990). Schizophrenics kill themselves too: nal of Psychiatry, 171, 355–359.
A review of risk factors for suicide. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 16, 571–589. Roy, A. (1982). Suicide in chronic schizophrenia. British Journal of Psy-
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. chiatry, 141, 171–177.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analy- Roy, A. (1995). Psychiatric emergencies. In H. I. Kaplan & B. J. Sadock
sis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Eds.), Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (6th ed., pp. 1739–1752).
Cohen, J., Test, M. A., & Brown, R. L. (1990). Suicide and schizophrenia: Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Data from a prospective community treatment study. American Journal Roy, A., Mazonson, A., & Pickar, D. (1984). Attempted suicide in chronic
of Psychiatry, 147, 602–607. schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 303–306.
Cohen, S., Lavelle, J., Rich, C. L., & Bromet, E. (1994). Rates and correlates Schwartz, R. C. (1999). Reliability and validity of the Functional As-
of suicide attempts in first-admission psychotic patients. Acta Psychiatrica sessment Rating Scale. Psychological Reports, 84, 389–391.
Scandinavica, 90, 167–171. Schwartz, R. C., & Petersen, S. (1999). The relationship between insight
Drake, R. E., & Cotton, P. G. (1986). Depression, hopelessness and suicide and suicidality among patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous
in chronic schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 554–559. and Mental Disease, 187, 376–378.
Drake, R. E., Gates, C., Whitaker, A., & Cotton, P. G. (1985). Suicide among Sullivan, G., Young, A. S., & Morgenstern, H. (1997). Behaviors as risk
schizophrenics: A review. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 26, 90–100. factors for rehospitalizations: Implications for predicting and prevent-
Fenton, W. S., McGlashan, T. H., Victor, B. J., & Blyler, C. R. (1997). ing admissions among the seriously mentally ill. Social Psychiatry and
Symptoms, subtype, and suicidality in patients with schizophrenia Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 185–190.
spectrum disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 199–204. Taiminen, T. (1994). Depression among schizophrenic patients: A selec-
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1995). Struc- tive review. Psychiatrica Fennica, 25, 185–194.
tured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV disorders. New York: State Ward, J. C., & Dow, M. G. (1994). The Functional Assessment Rating
Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department. Scale (Florida Mental Health Institute). Tampa: University of South
Foster, V. A., & McAdams, C. R. (1999). The impact of client suicide in Florida, Department of Community Mental Health.
counselor training: Implications for counselor education and supervi- Ward, J. C., Dow, M. G., Saunders, T., Penner, K., Halls, S., & Sachs-
sion. Counselor Education and Supervision, 39, 22–33. Ericsson, N. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for the Functional
Gallagher, A. G., Dinan, T. G., Sheehy, N. P., & Baker, L. J. V. (1995). Assessment Rating Scale (Florida Mental Health Institute). Tampa:
Chronic auditory hallucinations and suicide risk factors in schizophre- University of South Florida, Department of Community Mental
nia. Irish Journal of Psychology, 16, 346–355. Health.
Gilliland, B. E., & James, R. K. (1997). Crisis intervention strategies (3rd Weiden, P., & Roy, A. (1992). General versus specific risk factors for
ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. suicide in schizophrenia. In D. Jacob (Ed.), Suicide and clinical prac-
Gupta, S., Black, D. W., Arndt, S., Hubbard, W. C., & Andreasen, N. C. tice (pp. 75–100). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
(1998). Factors associated with suicide attempts among patients with World Health Organization. (1995). Schizophrenia: Epidemiology of men-
schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 49, 1353–1355. tal disorders and psychosocial problems. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Harkavy-Friedman, J. M., & Nelson, E. A. (1997). Assessment and interven- Zimmerman, M. (1994). Interview guide for evaluating DSM-IV psychiat-
tion for the suicidal patient with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Quarterly, 68, ric disorders and the mental status examination. Philadelphia: Psych
361–375. Press Products.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 319


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful:
A Statewide Study of the Impact of Comprehensive
Guidance and Counseling Programs

Richard T. Lapan, Norman C. Gysbers, and Gregory F. Petroski

The researchers examined the relationships between statewide implementation of comprehensive guidance and counseling
programs and indicators of safety and success for seventh graders. Hierarchical linear modeling was used to analyze data from
22,601 seventh graders attending 184 Missouri schools and 4,868 middle school teachers. After researchers controlled for differ-
ences between schools due to socioeconomic status and enrollment size, students attending middle schools with more fully
implemented comprehensive programs reported (a) feeling safer attending their schools, (b) having better relationships with their
teachers, (c) believing that their education was more relevant and important to their futures, (d) being more satisfied with the
quality of education available to them in their schools, (e) having fewer problems related to the physical and interpersonal milieu
in their schools, and (f) earning higher grades.

S
chools and school counselors in the United States on their perceived personal satisfaction with the quality of
today face a daunting array of challenges gener- education available in their schools. The objective evalua-
ated from profound social, cultural, and economic tion of students’ QOL focuses on their personal assessment
changes that occurred in the last half of the twen- of the physical and the interpersonal contexts that shape
tieth century and that will continue into the learning opportunities and social interactions within their
twenty-first century. The unprecedented number of vio- schools. To help students satisfy their individual needs, in-
lent acts in public schools helped motivate the Congress of crease the likelihood that they will experience some con-
the United States to consider legislation that would pro- trol over their school environments, be successful academi-
vide schools with money to hire more elementary school cally, and have real opportunities to make choices in their
counselors. Keeping students safe has become a primary current and future learning, schools and school counselors
focus for everyone involved in America’s schools. School must find ways to improve the QOL available to students.
counselors have an important role in promoting and main- In addition, public schools are charged with the task of
taining student safety. promoting the development of future generations of citizens
At a point in American history when schools are searching who can constructively participate in a democratic society
for effective ways to respond to issues of violence, drugs, that is competing in a constantly changing, increasingly
dropouts, teenage pregnancy, adolescent suicide and depres- technologically sophisticated global marketplace. Federal
sion, the quality of life (QOL) available to students in schools legislation (e.g., the School-to-Work Opportunities Act
has become a critical educational issue. In the rehabilitation of 1994) and reports (e.g., Secretary’s Commission on
counseling literature, enhanced QOL has been linked to Achieving Necessary Skills, 1991) have reflected wide-
expanded opportunities individuals have to promote spread concern regarding the preparation needed today
personal growth, fulfillment, and self-esteem (Kosciulek, by young people to attain real economic opportunity. Full
1999; Pain, Dunn, Anderson, Darrah, & Kratochvil, 1998). and equitable membership in such an economic future
QOL has been defined as an overall assessment, based on requires sophisticated and advanced academic preparation,
both a subjective and objective evaluation, of one’s emo- particularly in the areas of mathematics, science, and tech-
tional, physical, social, and material well-being (Felce & Perry, nology. The stakes are high, both for individuals and the
1996). The subjective evaluation of students’ QOL centers nation as a whole.

Richard T. Lapan is an associate professor, Norman C. Gysbers is a full professor, and Gregory F. Petroski is a doctoral student, all in the Department of
Educational and Counseling Psychology at the University of Missouri at Columbia. This research was supported in part by a grant from the Missouri
Department of Elementary and Secondary Education. Special thanks are extended to Missouri’s School-to-Careers State Management Team. In particular, the
authors thank Doug Sutton, Pam Spires, Robert Ruble, Donna Schulte, and Michelle Corcoran for their continued support, encouragement, and feedback.
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Richard T. Lapan, 16 Hill Hall, University of Missouri at Columbia, Columbia, MO 65211 (e-mail:
LapanR@missouri.edu).

320 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful

Finding sustainable and systematic ways to motivate stu- In a survey of Missouri school counselors K–12, Gysbers,
dents to achieve academic success continues to be a major Lapan, and Blair (1999) found that school counselors who
task facing school practitioners, educational researchers, and were more fully implementing their state’s comprehensive
policy makers. In the educational psychology literature, guidance and counseling program model reported substantial
research is accumulating that highlights the important role increases over time in the delivery of the kinds of guidance
of the relationships between teachers and students in mo- and counseling services suggested in recent national legis-
tivating academic achievement and social competencies for lative initiatives. These activities included (a) having school
elementary and middle school students (Wentzel, 1999). counselors conduct more structured group and classroom ac-
Teacher–student relationships that are characterized by tivities; (b) spending more time with students; and (c) being
warmth, an absence of conflict, and open communication more extensively used by teachers, administrators, and
promote better school adjustments. Students benefiting from parents as resources and consultants. School counselors reported
such relationships with teachers display a stronger sense (a) becoming more active in the school and local community,
of community in the school, more competent social be- (b) spending less time in their offices and more time deliv-
haviors, and improved academic performance (Schaps, ering developmental counseling services, and (c) spending
Battistich, & Solomon, 1997). In the counseling literature, less time on clerical and administrative tasks.
academic gains have been found when curriculum activities Support for the positive and preferred roles that school
forge relevant, interesting connections to possible educational counselors should, and often do, perform is also to be found
and career futures for students (e.g., Baker & Taylor, 1998; in studies conducted independent of research on comprehen-
Evans & Burck, 1992; Hoyt, 1998; Lapan, Gysbers, Hughey, sive guidance and counseling programs. For example, Schneider
& Arni, 1993). Although they found significant gains across and Stevenson (1999) published a comprehensive study of
the K–12 years, Evans and Burck (1992) argued that infu- the dramatic rise in educational and occupational ambitions
sion of career education guidance activities into the for adolescents across the United States. Relying on the
academic curriculum led to even greater gains for pre– extensive national data set provided by the Alfred P. Sloan
high school students. Study of Youth and Social Development, these authors
reported that high schools that did not encourage and
SCHOOL COUNSELOR IMPACT ON STUDENT DEVELOPMENT emphasize student planning for the future were more likely
to have students with misaligned ambitions. Misaligned
There is a growing body of empirical research highlighting ambitions were defined as a significant mismatch between
the positive impact school counselors have on overall student the amount of education an adolescent expects to attain
development K–12, including academic development and the education required for those occupations the adoles-
(e.g., Borders & Drury, 1992; Gerler, 1992; Macdonald & cent wants to enter.
Sink, 1999; Paisley & Borders, 1995; Whiston & Sexton, 1998). In conducting in-depth analyses of high schools that more
Evidence for this has emerged from research conducted both effectively help adolescents develop aligned ambitions,
inside the field of school counseling and from sources out- Schneider and Stevenson (1999) pointed to the important
side of the discipline. For example, Lapan, Gysbers, and Sun role of school counselors. The most effective high school in
(1997) found in a statewide sample that high school stu- their study had a school counselor to student ratio of 225 to
dents who attended schools where more fully implemented 1. School counselors were expected to work closely with
comprehensive guidance and counseling programs were in both students and their parents to (a) help develop and
place reported more positive school experiences. Across 236 implement educational and career plans, (b) focus on stu-
small, medium, and large size high schools, surveys of a total dent behavior, and (c) deal with personal problems that
of 22,964 students found positive relationships between students might come to experience during their high school
guidance program implementation and (a) higher self- years. Schneider and Stevenson concluded that this emphasis
reported grades, (b) student perceptions that their high on planfulness by school counselors greatly assisted stu-
school education was better at preparing them for the dents to develop a sense of personal agency and encour-
future, and (c) student perceptions that their schools were aged students to take responsibility for both their educa-
making more career and college information available. In tion and decisions they made about their future.
addition, positive associations were found between guid- Blustein, Phillips, Jobin-Davis, Finkelberg, and Roarke
ance program implementation and indicators of school climate (1997) conducted a theory-building investigation of fac-
that assessed QOL (e.g., student feelings of belonging and tors that promote more successful school-to-work transi-
safety in school, perceptions that classes were less often tions for individuals who enter the workforce directly after
disrupted, and that peers were better behaved). Similarly, leaving high school. Individuals who had more positive ex-
Nelson and Gardner (1998) found that students in Utah periences with high school counselors expressed much
high schools with more fully implemented comprehen- greater levels of satisfaction with their current jobs than
sive guidance and counseling programs rated their overall individuals who had more negative experiences with coun-
education as better, took more advanced mathematics and selors. Blustein et al. reported that more satisfied workers
science courses, and had higher scores on the ACT College indicated that their school counselors had been proactive
Entrance Examination. in reaching out to them and had provided both emotional

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 321


Lapan, Gysbers, and Petroski

and instrumental support for them in high school. In con- (a) perceptions of safety in school, (b) satisfaction with their
trast, individuals who were more dissatisfied with their jobs education, (c) grades, (d) perceptions of their relationships
thought that school counselors had too many students to with teachers, and (e) perceptions of the importance and
take care of and did not provide them with the kind of indi- relevance of education to their future. Comprehensive guid-
vidualized attention that could have been most helpful. ance and counseling programs have been conceptualized and
designed to positively affect each of these factors. This study
MISSOURI COMPREHENSIVE GUIDANCE PROGRAM (MCGP) examined whether or not such relationships exist for seventh-
grade students because the seventh grade is a critical time in
In 1971, the University of Missouri at Columbia was awarded individual development and in the transition process from
a U.S. Department of Education grant to assist all states with late elementary to middle and high school years.
the development and implementation of career guidance, Missouri’s database, generated from the statewide school
counseling, and placement programs in their schools. On the district accreditation process, was used to explore relation-
basis of the model developed in this project, Missouri school ships between teacher ratings of the extent to which the
counselors, state guidance officials from the Missouri MCGP was being implemented in their schools and the
Department of Elementary and Secondary Education (1999), student factors described above. One limitation of some of
the leaders of the Missouri School Counselor Association, the earlier research on the impact of comprehensive guid-
and university faculty who train school counselors in ance and counseling programs was a reliance on school coun-
Missouri have worked together to develop and implement selor self-report data (Lapan et al., 1997). It was predicted
the MCGP (see Gysbers et al., 1999, as well as Lapan et al., in the present study that findings from earlier research with
1997, for more extensive reviews of this history). The MCGP high school students that used counselor self-report data
has three structural elements that integrate (a) program would be replicated at the seventh-grade level, using teach-
content, (b) an organizational plan and delivery system for ers as observers of the level of implementation of recom-
guidance and counseling services, and (c) the political and mended MCGP guidance activities in their schools.
human resources needed to successfully carry out the pro-
gram. School counselor time is differentially apportioned across RESEARCH QUESTIONS
the K–12 years to implement the four program components
of the MCGP: (a) Guidance Curriculum, (b) Individual The following research questions were examined in this
Planning, (c) Responsive Services, and (d) System Support study:
(Gysbers, Starr, & Magnuson, 1998).
Starting in the 1984–85 school year and ending during 1. Are student indicators of safety and success posi-
the 1997–98 school year, school counselors and adminis- tively related to student characteristics (i.e., sex,
trators from 441 of the 525 school districts across Missouri minority status, objective assessment of the quality
received extensive training in the development and imple- of life in one’s school, and amount of time spent on
mentation of the MCGP in their home school districts. The homework)?
MCGP has become the state mandated training model for 2. After controlling for differences between schools due
school counselors. The extent of its implementation is one to enrollment size and socioeconomic level, are more
of the criteria on which all school districts are evaluated in fully implemented comprehensive guidance and
Missouri’s mandatory school district accreditation process, counseling programs related to each of the five indi-
that is, the Missouri School Improvement Program (MSIP). cators of student safety and success?
Because of this statewide attempt to implement a com- 3. Do school enrollment size, socioeconomic level, and
mon program model, Missouri provides a unique oppor- level of implementation of the school counseling pro-
tunity to study the effects of comprehensive guidance and gram significantly affect the regression slopes be-
counseling programs on student development. In addition tween student characteristics and student indicators
to Missouri, this training model has been adopted, in whole of safety and success?
or in part, in many states and school districts across the
country. Results from statewide studies in Missouri have
clear relevance and applicability for school counselors, state METHOD
officials, policy makers, and university faculty in other Sample
states who are engaged in the difficult process of devel-
oping and implementing comprehensive guidance and MSIP seventh-grade student and middle school teacher survey
counseling programs. data collected from 1992 to 1996 were analyzed in this study.
For each of these years, MSIP collected stratified random samples
PURPOSE OF THE STUDY representative of all schools in Missouri. The data set analyzed
in the present study included 22,601 seventh graders and 4,868
The purpose of the present study was to examine, on a teachers from 184 schools. Approximately 50% of the
statewide basis, the impact of more fully implemented com- students were girls (n = 11,344) and 16% of the total sample
prehensive guidance and counseling programs on student were minority students (n = 3,682). Approximately 69%

322 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful

of these middle school teachers were women (94% were not included in the analyses. The present study focused on
Caucasian American and 5% were African American). Ap- student attitudes of those minority groups at greater risk for
proximately 52% of these teachers had bachelor’s degrees, school failure and lower academic achievement.
with 46% having master’s degrees. Safety and relationships with teachers. Students were asked
The sample was representative of the diversity of schools to rate on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly
in Missouri. The state classified the locations of the 184 agree) the item “I feel safe at school” (M = 3.42, SD = 1.21).
schools in the following ways: approximately 12% were des- On the same 5-point scale, students were asked to rate the
ignated as large cities, 8% as midsize cities, 32% as suburban following four items referring to their relationships with
areas, 27% as towns, and 22% as rural locales. The number of teachers: (a) “There is good communication between teach-
schools sampled each year was as follows: 33 in 1992, 33 in ers and students,” (b) “If a student has a problem there are
1993, 41 in 1994, 42 in 1995, and 35 in 1996. teachers who will listen and help,” (c) “In my school every-
one is given a chance to succeed in class,” and (d) “Students
Missouri’s School Improvement Program (MSIP) and teachers often work together on projects in class.” A prin-
cipal component factor analysis was performed on these four
The MSIP (Missouri Department of Elementary and Sec- items. Only one factor was extracted because it was the only
ondary Education, 1999) is an extensive and mandated factor to have an eigenvalue greater than 1.0. Loadings of
statewide school district evaluation and accreditation pro- the four items on this factor ranged from a minimum of .68
cess. Each year an MSIP external accreditation team evalu- to a maximum of .78. The estimate of internal consistency
ates a statewide representative sample of school districts, across these four items was adequate for research purposes
assuring that all districts in the state are assessed on a 5-year (coefficient alpha = .74). Therefore, a summed scale score
cycle. Quality standards and indicators for all educational across all four items was calculated for each student and used
programs, including guidance, have been developed and in all subsequent analyses (M = 10.13, SD = 3.23).
implemented statewide. Before an MSIP accreditation team Relevance of education to one’s future. Two items (using the
visit, school personnel, school board members, parents, and same 5-point Likert scale described earlier) were combined
students complete surveys. Of particular interest for the to measure this variable: (a) “The kind of education I’m get-
present study, classroom teachers are asked several behav- ting here will help me later on” and (b) “What I study in
iorally specific questions about the extent to which the school seems important to me.” Summative scores across both
counseling activities of the MCGP are being implemented items were calculated and used in all subsequent analyses to
in their schools. Teachers are in a key observer role because measure the perceived importance of one’s education and
they have direct knowledge of the extent and kinds of coun- its connection to one’s future (M = 7.82, SD = 1.60).
selor involvement in their classrooms and with their stu- Subjective estimate of QOL. One item asked students
dents. Questions answered by teachers were taken directly to make a global assessment of the quality of education
from the four program components of the MCGP. available to them in their school. A 5-point scale was
All students in a school district that is being evaluated by used to measure these student perceptions (1 = poor to 5 =
MSIP are expected to complete a questionnaire assessing excellent; M = 3.77, SD = .93).
critical aspects of their school experiences. The seventh- Objective estimate of QOL. Four items asked students to
grade survey covers a wide range of factors critical to student rate the environmental conditions and interpersonal milieu
perceptions of safety and success in school. Data gathered from in their schools on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to
MSIP student questionnaires have become a valuable source 5 = strongly agree). These items were (a) “It is often too hot
of insight into student attitudes and behaviors across Missouri or too cold in this building,” (b) “There is often so much
(e.g., see Sun, Hobbs, & Elder, 1994). In the present study, noise that it is hard to study,” (c) “Our classes are often
principal investigators identified survey items that assessed interrupted,” and (d) “At least half the time in school is
critical aspects of student safety and school success. All wasted.” A principal component factor analysis was per-
selected items have clear face validity with the issues of formed on these four items. Only one factor was extracted
student safety and success central to the present study. In the because it was the only factor to have an eigenvalue greater
following sections, all items analyzed in the present study are than 1.0. Loadings of the four items on this factor ranged
listed with their corresponding student-level or school-level from a minimum of .60 to a maximum of .74. The estimate
variables. of internal consistency across these four items was adequate
for research purposes (coefficient alpha = .59). Therefore,
Student-Level Variables
summed scale scores were calculated for each student and
Sex and minority status. A dichotomous variable was created used in all subsequent analyses (M = 8.09, SD = 3.16).
to distinguish between girls and boys (girls = 0, boys = 1). A Grades. Students were asked to pick one of eight categories
dichotomous variable was created to distinguish between that best described their grades in school. An 8-point scale
majority Caucasian American students and minority students was created to assign values to student self-reported grades.
(i.e., African Americans, Hispanic Americans, and Native These values were as follows: mostly A = 8, half A and B = 7,
Americans; majority = 0, minority = 1). The percentage of mostly B = 6, half B and C = 5, mostly C = 4, half C and
Asian American students in the sample was small and was D = 3, mostly D = 2, and below D = 1 (M = 6.09, SD = 1.77).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 323


Lapan, Gysbers, and Petroski

Homework. One item asked students to rate how much time was adequate for research purposes (coefficient alpha = .95).
they usually spent on homework each day. A 6-point scale was A summed scale score across these seven items was calcu-
constructed to assign values to each of six possible student lated for each teacher (M = 26.32, SD = 3.55). To compute a
responses. These values ranged from “I don’t usually have it school-level variable assessing the extent to which the guid-
assigned” = 1 to “More than 2 hours” = 2 (M = 2.63, SD = 1.16). ance program was being implemented in that school, each
individual teacher’s summed score was then averaged across
School-Level Variables all middle school teachers in that school who responded to
the MSIP survey. The mean number of middle school teach-
Enrollment. MSIP provides information on the actual num- ers completing the survey in each school was 25 (SD = 15).
ber of students attending each school that was sampled.
In the present study, mean student enrollment was 552 Data Analysis
seventh-grade students (SD = 262) per school. The mini-
mum number of students was 97 and the maximum was The three research questions addressed in this study were
1,245. The total number of students attending each middle tested using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM). Bryk and
school was used as a covariate to control for effects due to Raudenbush (1992) developed HLM to address the nesting
school enrollment size. problem in which research participants are simultaneously
Socioeconomic status (SES) level. MSIP also provides data embedded within several social layers. For example, students
on the percentage of students in each school who receive come from various family backgrounds and participate in
free and reduced lunch. In the present study, the percent- classrooms and school districts that are very different from
age of students receiving free and reduced lunch varied be- each other. As illustrated in Figure 1, HLM uses multiple
tween a minimum of approximately 4% to a maximum of linear regression to predict outcomes for group members
almost 98%. The percentage of students receiving free and that are a function of both the characteristics of the group to
reduced lunch in each school was used as a covariate to which they belong and the characteristics of the individuals
control for effects due to varying SES levels between schools. themselves (Arnold, 1992). Starting at the individual
By using school enrollment size and SES in this way, the
present study was able to statistically control for two of the
most important factors that discriminate between urban,
Characteristics of schools Between-school models:
suburban, and rural schools. By first removing the effects Covariates: Student attitudes are due
of enrollment and SES on student attitudes and school ex- Enrollment size to factors that vary
periences, we were able to study the possible impact of Socioeconomic level between schools.
Independent Predictor: (Research Question 2)
more fully implemented guidance programs on student
Teacher ratings of the
safety and success independent of these potentially con- extent to which the 4
founding factors. program components of ➤
Guidance. MSIP requires classroom teachers to rate seven the MCGP are being
implemented in their
items assessing the extent to which counseling activities school.
Student indicators of safety
and success
outlined in the four program components of the MCGP Safety
have been carried out in their classrooms and schools. These Relationship with teachers
Between-school models:
items are (a) “The guidance program provides classroom Regression slopes Relevance of education to
activities for students on a regular basis,” (b) “Counselors between student one’s future
Satisfaction (Subj. QOL)
work with the students to assist them with their personal characteristics and
Grades
student attitudes are
concerns,” (c) “Counselors assist students with their educa- influenced by factors that
tional and career plans,” (d) “Counselors consult with staff vary between schools.

and parents concerning students’ personal and academic (Research Question 3)


progress,” (e) “Counseling is provided for individuals and
small groups,” (f) “Students are referred to other professional Characteristics of
students Within-student models:
resources in the school and community as appropriate,” and

Sex Student attitudes are due


(g) “Counselors inform staff and community about the aims Minority status to characteristics of
and purposes of the guidance program.” Obj. QOL students that vary within
Time spent on schools.
A 5-point scale was provided to teachers to rate the extent (Research Question 1)
homework
to which each of these activities was actually being carried
out in their schools (1 = strongly disagree/never to 5 = strongly
agree/always). A principal component factor analysis was FIGURE 1
performed on these seven items. Only one factor was Relating Student Outcomes to Between-School
extracted because it was the only factor to have an eigen- Differences in the Implementation of Comprehensive
value greater than 1.0. Loadings of the seven items on this School Counseling Programs
factor ranged from a minimum of .69 to a maximum of .80.
The estimate of internal consistency across these seven items Note. See Table 1 Note.

324 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful

student level, HLM calculates regression equations for out- dent indicators of safety and success (Research Question
comes that are a function of characteristics of students 3). HLM computes coefficients and standard t statistics to
within each school. For each of the 184 schools participat- test each relationship for statistical significance (Bryk &
ing in the present study, separate regression equations were Raudenbush, 1992).
calculated for the five indicators of student safety and suc- Before performing the HLM analyses, scatter plots of the
cess (i.e., perceived safety, relationships with teachers, rel- dependent variables in relation to the ordinal independent
evance of education to one’s future, satisfaction with edu- variables were examined. No evidence was found to suggest
cation available to one, and self-reported grades). curvilinear relationships between variables. In addition, plots
Each of these indicators was predicted to be a function of residuals did not indicate the existence of polynomial
of four student characteristic variables (i.e., sex, minority relationships. Only statistically significant HLM findings are
status, objective assessment of quality of life in one’s school reported in the Results section. Additional data are avail-
building, and time spent on homework). These equations able from the principal author on request.
produce an intercept value and beta weight for each of
the five indicators of student success and safety for every
school in this study. The intercept is the average value for RESULTS
each of the four indicators in every school. The beta Correlations Between Student Characteristics and
weights estimate the association between student indica- Indicators of Safety and Success
tors and student characteristics in each school. HLM tests
these relationships for statistical significance and reports Correlations between student characteristics and student
the results as within-school unconditional models (Research indicators of safety and success are reported in Table 1.
Question 1). Unconditional models summarize student- Minimal gender and minority differences were found. How-
level regression results across all schools. They are defined ever, seventh-grade girls reported earning higher grades than
as unconditional models because the potential effects of boys did, and nonminority students indicated that they were
between-school predictors on these values have not been earning higher grades than minority students. For all stu-
taken into account. dents, a better relationship with teachers was strongly re-
Second, HLM uses between-school factors to predict the lated to (a) feeling safer in school, (b) being more satisfied
variability in these intercepts and beta weights across with their education and feeling that it was more relevant
schools. This second set of regression equations attempts to and important to their future, (c) earning higher grades and
explain these distributions of scores as a function of factors doing more homework, and (d) attending a school where
that vary between schools. Three school-level predictors there were fewer problems with the interpersonal and physi-
(i.e., school enrollment size, socioeconomic level, and level cal environment. Believing that one was safer in school was
of implementation of the school counseling program) were related to (a) earning higher grades, (b) believing that one’s
hypothesized to significantly affect each of the five indica- education was more relevant to one’s future, and (c) being
tors of student success and safety (Research Question 2). more satisfied with both the subjective and the objective
In addition, each of the school-level predictors was hypoth- components assessing the QOL available in one’s school. The
esized to have a significant influence on the relationships objective assessment of the QOL in one’s school was related
(i.e., beta weights) between student characteristics and stu- to all student-level variables except sex and minority status.

TABLE 1

Intercorrelations Among Student-Level Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Safety — .46 .38 .38 –.33 .19 –.09 –.08 .09


2. Teachers — .56 .51 –.33 .14 –.03 –.05 .12
3. Relevance — .49 –.27 .22 .03 –.09 .18
4. Subj. QOL — –.32 .24 –.05 –.05 .12
5. Obj. QOL — –.15 .07 .04 –.09
6. Grades — –.18 –.19 .09
7. Minority status — –.01 –.01
8. Sex — –.11
9. Homework —

Note. For students: N = 22,601. “Teachers” refers to students’ perceived relationship with teachers. “Relevance” refers to students’ perceptions
of the relevance and importance of their education to their future. “Subj. QOL” refers to students’ subjective evaluation of their level of satisfaction
with the quality of education available to them in their schools. “Obj. QOL” refers to students’ objective evaluation of the physical and interpersonal
milieu in their schools.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 325


Lapan, Gysbers, and Petroski

Correlations Between School-Level Variables TABLE 3


Correlations between school-level variables are reported in
Table 2. These coefficients were calculated between mean Final Estimation of Significant Hierarchical Linear
school-level values for variables used in the present study Modeling Fixed Effects
(n = 184). More complete implementation of a comprehen-
sive guidance and counseling program was consistently and Fixed Effect Coefficient SE T ratio
positively associated with school-level indicators of student
safety and success. As zero order correlations, these coeffi- Dependent variable: Safety
cients suggest that level of school counseling program imple-
School-level predictor
mentation shared between 3% and 8% of its variance with SES –0.011 .001 –7.63***
these school-level indicators of student safety and success. Enrollment –0.018 .005 –3.62***
Lower school-level SES ratings were associated with Guidance 0.023 .007 3.23**
Student-level predictor
school-level means that indicated students (a) did not feel Obj. QOL slope intercept –0.130 .016 –8.44***
as safe in their schools, (b) were less satisfied with both the SES × Obj. QOL slope 0.001 .000 2.74*
subjective and the objective components of the QOL avail-
able to them in their schools, and (c) were earning lower Dependent variable: Relationship with teachers
grades. However, correlations between school-level means School-level predictor
suggested that students from poorer schools had equally Guidance 0.053 .020 2.61*
good relationships with teachers and believed that their Student-level predictor
education was just as relevant and important to their future as Homework slope intercept 0.202 .018 11.20***
Obj. QOL slope intercept –0.358 .050 –7.22***
did students from wealthier schools. Correlations between SES × Obj. QOL slope 0.002 .001 3.51**
school-level means also suggested that students from larger
schools were earning higher grades and feeling more satisfied Dependent variable: Satisfaction with the quality of education
with the education available to them than were students from in one’s school
smaller schools. School-level predictor
SES –0.005 .001 –4.03**
Enrollment 0.012 .004 2.76**
Research Question 1 Guidance 0.031 .006 5.14***
Statistically significant relationships between student char- Student-level predictor
Homework slope intercept 0.053 .006 8.55***
acteristics and indicators of student safety and success are Obj. QOL slope intercept –0.093 .013 –7.28***
reported in Table 3. Students who believed that their learn- SES × Obj. QOL slope 0.001 .000 3.23*
ing environment was not as encumbered by disruptions in
their interpersonal and physical milieu were more likely to Dependent variable: Relevance and importance of education
(a) feel safer (T = –8.44, p < .001), (b) have better relation- to one’s future
ships with teachers (T = –7.22, p < .001), (c) be more satis-
fied with their education (T = –7.28, p < .001), (d) see School-level predictor
Guidance 0.022 .008 2.63*
their education as being more relevant and important to Student-level predictor
their future (T = –5.87, p < .001), and (e) earn higher grades Homework slope intercept 0.183 .012 14.83***
(T = –8.00, p < .001). Students who reported doing more Obj. QOL slope intercept –0.138 .024 –5.87***
SES × Obj. QOL slope 0.001 .000 3.01*
homework indicated that they (a) had better relationships
Dependent variable: Grades
TABLE 2 School-level predictor
SES –0.011 .002 –6.12***
Correlations Between School-Level Variables Enrollment 0.014 .005 2.46*
Guidance 0.027 .008 3.27**
Student-level predictor
Variable Guidance Enrollment SES Sex slope intercept –0.450 .051 –8.90***
SES × Sex slope –0.006 .001 –4.11***
1. Safety .17* –.08 –.50** Minority slope intercept –1.040 .102 –10.23***
2. Teachers .18* .05 –.13 SES × Minority slope 0.010 .002 4.00***
3. Relevance .19* .10 .02 Homework slope intercept 0.077 .015 5.15***
4. Subj. QOL .29** .28** –.37** Obj. QOL slope intercept –0.132 .016 –8.00***
5. Obj. QOL –.18* .11 .32** SES × Obj. QOL slope 0.001 .000 4.05***
6. Grades .13 .28** –.62** Enrollment × Obj. QOL
slope 0.004 .001 4.06***
Note. See Table 1 Note. For schools, N = 184. “Guidance” refers to the
extent of implementation of the guidance program. “Enrollment” re-
fers to the number of students in each school. “SES” (socioeconomic Note . See Tables 1 & 2 Notes . Only statistically significant
status) refers to the socioeconomic level of each school. coefficients are reported in the table.
*p < .05. **p < .001. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

326 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful

with teachers (T = 11.20, p < .001), (b) were more satisfied that there was a relationship between more disruptions in
with their education and believed that their education school and lower grades (T = 4.06, p < .001).
was more relevant and important to their future (T = 8.55,
p < .001), and (c) were earning higher grades (T = 5.15, p <
.001). Girls reported earning higher grades than boys did
DISCUSSION
(T = –8.90, p < .001), and minority students indicated they Many writers, both within the United States and from the
were earning lower grades than majority students were international guidance community, have consistently high-
(T = –10.23, p < .001). lighted the need for rigorous program evaluation research
studies that would inform school counseling program reform
Research Question 2 and improvement efforts (e.g., Borders & Drury, 1992; Gerler,
Results from between-school HLM analyses testing relation- 1992; Gysbers, Hughey, Starr, & Lapan, 1992; Killeen,
ships between school-level predictors and student indicators Sammons, & Watts, 1999; Lapan & Kosciulek, 2001; Oomen,
of safety and success are also reported in Table 3. After con- Athanassoula-Reppa, Barnes, Petri, & Hautvast, 1997).
trolling for between-school differences in SES and enrollment Findings from the present study have both replicated and
size, more fully implemented school counseling programs sig- extended a growing body of evaluation research on guidance
nificantly predicted (a) student perceptions of being safer in program effectiveness and the work of school counselors
their schools (T = 3.23, p < .01), (b) better relationships across the K–12 years (e.g., Blustein et al., 1997; Gerler,
between students and teachers (T = 2.61, p < .05), (c) greater 1992; Gysbers et al., 1999; Lapan et al., 1993; Lapan et
satisfaction of students with the education they were receiv- al., 1997; R. S. Lee, 1993; Schneider & Stevenson, 1999;
ing in their schools (T = 5.14, p < .001), (d) perceptions that St. Clair, 1989). These studies have consistently shown
one’s education was more relevant and important to one’s that when school counselors are more fully engaged in
future (T = 2.63, p < .05), and (e) earning higher grades (T = implementing preferred work tasks outlined in frameworks
3.27, p < .01). Students from lower SES schools believed that for comprehensive guidance and counseling programs,
they (a) were not as safe in their schools (T = –7.63, p < .001), counselors move out of marginalized positions and into
(b) were not as satisfied with the quality of education avail- roles that more effectively promote essential educational
able to them in their schools (T = –4.03, p < .01), and (c) were and career objectives for students (Gysbers & Henderson,
earning lower grades (T = –6.12, p < .001). Students from 2000; Watkins, 1994).
larger schools believed that they (a) were not as safe in their The present study provided additional evidence that the
schools (T = –3.62, p < .001), (b) were more satisfied with implementation of comprehensive school counseling pro-
the quality of education available to them in their schools grams was consistently associated with important indica-
(T = 2.76, p < .01), and (c) were earning higher grades tors of student safety and success. School counselors who
(T = 2.46, p < .05). These findings mostly corroborate results were more fully engaged in providing students with a unique
found in the zero order correlations between variables that network of emotional and instrumental support services were
were reported in Table 2. However, the HLM procedure more likely to exert a more positive impact on students than
found a significant relationship between school counseling counselors who did not implement such activities. This range
program implementation and student self-reported grades of school counseling activities included (a) spending more
in which the zero order correlation between these variables time in classrooms, (b) assisting students with personal prob-
was nonsignificant. lems as well as educational and career plans, (c) consulting
with parents and school personnel, (d) providing individual
Research Question 3 and group counseling services, (e) referring students as
needed, (f) and communicating to others both within the
Results from between-school HLM analyses testing whether school and local community about the goals and aims of the
or not the between-school variables significantly affected guidance program. For both girls and boys, seventh graders
regression slopes between student characteristics and stu- in schools where these guidance activities were more avail-
dent indicators of safety and success are also reported in able reported having (a) better relationships with teachers,
Table 3. Guidance program implementation did not sig- (b) higher grades, (c) a belief that their education was more
nificantly influence any of these slope relationships. How- important to them and relevant to their future, (d) and more
ever, students in poorer schools were more negative in their enhanced subjective and objective perceptions of the QOL
ratings of the objective QOL available to them in their available to them in their schools.
schools. These lower QOL ratings were associated with
student responses that indicated that these students were
Improving Student–Teacher Relationships
(a) not feeling as safe in school (T = 2.74, p < .05), (b) hav-
ing poorer relationships with teachers (T = 3.51, p < .01), Middle school students’ motivation to pursue both social
(c) less satisfied (T = 3.23, p < .05), (d) understanding their goals (e.g., to behave appropriately in school) and task-related
education to be less relevant to their future (T = 3.01, p < goals (e.g., to learn and achieve academically) has been
.05), and (e) earning lower grades (T = 4.05, p < .001). strongly linked to the kinds of relationships that they
Responses from students attending larger schools suggested perceive they have with their teachers (Wentzel, 1999). Stu-

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 327


Lapan, Gysbers, and Petroski

dents who perceive their teachers to be caring and support- their school building was often too hot or too cold. These
ive, respectful, fair, holding positive expectations for them, students reported earning lower grades, doing less home-
and having more open communication make greater aca- work, and not feeling as safe in their schools.
demic gains and use more prosocial behaviors in classrooms Our results showed that, to some degree, a more fully
(Brophy, 1983; Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Feldlaufer, Midgley, implemented comprehensive school counseling program
& Eccles, 1998; Murdock, 1999; Raudenbush, 1984; was of benefit to seventh graders regardless of the socio-
Weinstein, 1989; Wentzel, 1997, 1998, 1999). Student per- economic level of their school. More fully implemented
ceptions of a lack of equity and fairness in their relationships guidance and counseling programs were related to greater
with teachers have been found to be a major correlate of student feelings of both safety and success in poorer as well
dropping out of school (Fine, 1986; Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). as in wealthier schools.
Findings from the present study corroborate the central im-
portance of student perceptions of their relationships with Limitations
teachers in promoting student safety and success.
Seventh graders who reported better relationships with Two potential limitations to this study need to be ad-
teachers were much more likely to indicate having more dressed. First, the present study relied on student and
positive experiences in school. These students felt safer in teacher self-report data. More objective measures of stu-
school, reported earning higher grades, and believed that dent achievement and behavior in school as well as trained
their education was more important to them and relevant raters to assess the extent of counseling activities would
to their future. Students strongly connected more positive add to the results. However, previous researchers have
relationships with teachers to their subjective and objec- found strong connections between student ratings of these
tive evaluation of the QOL available to them in their school. kinds of items and more objective measures of student
That is, they felt more satisfied with their education and performance and behavior in school (e.g., Wentzel, 1999).
experienced their learning environment to be less encum- Although teachers are not neutral observers, they do pro-
bered by negative distractions (e.g., students misbehaving vide a critically important perspective on the delivery of
and disrupting classes). school counseling services.
Students who attended schools where a more fully imple- In the present study, it is important to note that teachers
mented MCGP was in place consistently indicated having rated the occurrence of school counseling activities for
better relationships with teachers. When trying to understand which they had direct behavioral evidence. They were not
the connection between more effective student–teacher asked to rate the quality of school counseling services, only
relationships and more positive achievement-related and whether or not specific activities were occurring in their
prosocial behaviors, Wentzel (1999) speculated that middle classrooms and schools. Teachers are important partners in
school students may experience supportive relationships with comprehensive guidance and counseling programs and on
teachers as effective parenting practices that promote devel- rating scales, such as the one used in the present study, a
opmentally appropriate levels of autonomy, competence, valuable independent source of information on school coun-
and control (Baumrind, 1971, 1991). Developmentally seling program implementation.
appropriate levels of autonomy, competence, and control are Second, although the tests for school counseling pro-
enhanced for students (a) where interactions with teachers gram implementation were statistically significant, the
are respectful and democratic; (b) where clear performance effect sizes were small. However, it is important to note
expectations are set based on a recognition of individual that these relationships were found across 184 schools,
differences; (c) and where communication is open, construc- more than 22,000 seventh graders, and 4,868 middle school
tive, and nurturing (Wentzel, 1999). These results suggest teachers, and we used a measurement strategy that did
that school counselors who are more fully implementing com- not exploit common method variance that could inflate
prehensive guidance and counseling programs are more likely such correlations. Validity coefficients across a population,
to effectively engage larger numbers of students in activities as opposed to a sample, would be expected to be smaller
that would help to promote these kinds of relationships. than ratings that are more similar to reliability estimates.
In the present study, teachers rated the extent to which
Offsetting SES certain counseling activities were occurring in their schools
and classrooms. Students rated the quality of their school
Seventh graders in economically poorer schools believed experiences, not the counseling program specifically.
that school was important and relevant to their future and Teacher and student ratings were then aggregated within
reported having nurturing and fair relationships with teach- each school and analyzed. These procedures made it very
ers as frequently as did students from more financially difficult to find statistical significance and exploit chance
advantaged schools. However, these girls and boys, both variations due to similar measures used and data collec-
minority and majority students, did not feel as satisfied with tion procedures used. Teachers and students completed
the QOL available in their schools. They indicated that more their different forms at different times, under different
time was wasted, more classes interrupted, too much noise test administration procedures, with no preconceptions
occurred that made it harder for them to study, and that that such an investigation would take place.

328 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Helping Seventh Graders Be Safe and Successful

Implications Felce, D., & Perry, J. (1996). Exploring current conceptions of quality of
life: A model for people with and without disabilities. In R. Renwick,
Given the daunting array of challenges that face our nation I. Brown, & M. Nagler (Eds.), Quality of life in health promotion and
and our schools, policy makers need to clearly understand that rehabilitation (pp. 51–62). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Feldlaufer, H., Midgley, C., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Student, teacher, and
school counselors will be better able to help solve current
observer perceptions of the classroom before and after the transition
problems that plague America’s schools if they are employed to junior high school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 8, 133–156.
not merely to fill a position but to fully implement a compre- Fine, M. (1986). Why urban adolescents drop into and out of public
hensive guidance and counseling program in their schools high school. Teachers College Record, 87, 393–409.
(Gysbers & Henderson, 2000). Comprehensive guidance and Gerler, E. R. (1992). What we know about school counseling: A reaction to
Borders and Drury. Journal of Counseling & Development, 70, 499–501.
counseling programs have provided school counselors K–12 Gysbers, N. C., & Henderson, P. (2000). Developing and managing your
with the organizational structure to focus efforts, organize work school guidance program (3rd ed.). Alexandria, VA: American Counsel-
schedules, and allocate time necessary for implementing ing Association.
proactive school counseling activities and services that Gysbers, N. C., Hughey, K., Starr, M., & Lapan, R. T. (1992). Improving
promote critical aspects of student development. Continued school guidance programs: A framework for program, personnel, and
results evaluation. Journal of Counseling & Development, 70, 565–570.
implementation of comprehensive programs through the work Gysbers, N. C., Lapan, R. T., & Blair, M. (1999). Closing in on statewide
of school counselors can deliver these critical services to the implementation of a comprehensive guidance program model. Profes-
entire student body. Unlike results found in earlier studies sional School Counseling, 2, 357–366.
(e.g., V. E. Lee & Ekstrom, 1987), implementation of compre- Gysbers, N. C., Starr, M. F., & Magnuson, C. S. (1998). Missouri compre-
hensive programs will provide equitable access to guidance hensive guidance: A model for program development and implementation
(Rev. ed.). Jefferson City, MO: Missouri Department of Elementary
and counseling services for students from rural areas, economi- and Secondary Education.
cally disadvantaged homes, and minority families. Hoyt, K. B. (1998). Career education and the transition from schooling-
Results from the present study certainly do not suggest to-employment. Youth Policy, 15 & 16, 10–20.
that school counselors are the only, or even the primary, Killeen, J., Sammons, P., & Watts, A. G. (1999). The effects of careers educa-
tion and guidance on attainment and associated behavior. Cambridge,
means by which the United States can improve public
England: The National Institute for Careers Education and Counselling.
school education. However, results do suggest that school Kosciulek, J. F. (1999). The consumer-directed theory of empowerment.
counselors’ implementation of comprehensive guidance and Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 42, 196–213.
counseling programs can be a consistently positive piece of Lapan, R. T., Gysbers, N. C., Hughey, K., & Arni, T. J. (1993). Evaluating
the puzzle that needs to be assembled if we are to make a a guidance and language arts unit for high school juniors. Journal of
Counseling & Development, 71, 444–452.
genuine effort to help all of our nation’s children achieve Lapan, R. T., Gysbers, N. C., & Sun, Y. (1997). The impact of more fully
academically and attain their desired future. Full implemen- implemented guidance programs on the school experiences of high
tation of such school counseling programs is a sustainable, school students: A statewide evaluation study. Journal of Counseling &
cost-effective national strategy for assisting all students to Development, 75, 292–302.
feel safer and be more successful in school. Lapan, R. T., & Kosciulek, J. F. (2001). Toward a community career system
program evaluation framework. Journal of Counseling & Development,
79, 3–15.
REFERENCES Lee, R. S. (1993). Effects of classroom guidance on student achieve-
ment. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 27, 163–171.
Arnold, C. L. (1992). An introduction to hierarchical linear models. Mea- Lee, V. E., & Ekstrom, R. B. (1987). Student access to guidance counseling
surement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 25, 58–90. in high school. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 287–310.
Baker, S. B., & Taylor, J. G. (1998). Effects of career education interven- Macdonald, G., & Sink, C. A. (1999). A qualitative developmental analysis
tions: A meta-analysis. The Career Development Quarterly, 46, 376–385. of comprehensive guidance programmes in schools in the United States.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Develop- British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 27, 415–430.
mental Psychology Monograph, 4(1, Pt. 2). Missouri Department of Elementary and Secondary Education. (1999).
Baumrind, D. (1991). Effective parenting during the early adolescent Missouri School Improvement Program: Procedures handbook. Jefferson City,
transition. In P. A. Cowan & M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family transi- MO: Author.
tions (pp. 111–164). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Murdock, T. B. (1999). The social context of risk: Status and motivation
Blustein, D. L., Phillips, S. D., Jobin-Davis, K., Finkelberg, S. L., & Roarke, predictors of alienation in middle school. Journal of Educational Psy-
A. E. (1997). A theory-building investigation of the school-to-work chology, 91, 62–75.
transition. The Counseling Psychologist, 25, 364–402. Nelson, D. E., & Gardner, J. L. (1998). An evaluation of the comprehensive
Borders, L. D., & Drury, S. M. (1992). Comprehensive school counseling guidance program in Utah public schools. Salt Lake City, UT: The Utah
programs: A review for policymakers and practitioners. Journal of State Office of Education.
Counseling & Development, 70, 487–498. Oomen, A., Athanassoula-Reppa, A., Barnes, A., Petri, M., & Hautvast,
Brophy, J. (1983). Research on the self-fulfilling prophecy and teacher D. (1997). A comparative study of careers education and guidance in
expectations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 631–660. six schools in Greece, the Netherlands and the United Kingdom. Edu-
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: cation for Careers in the Union. APS, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Pain, K., Dunn, M., Anderson, G., Darrah, J., & Kratochvil, M. (1998).
Eccles, J., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit: Developmentally Quality of life: What does it mean in rehabilitation? Journal of Rehabili-
appropriate classrooms for young adolescents. In C. Ames & R. Ames tation, 64(2), 5–11.
(Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). San Paisley, P. O., & Borders, L. D. (1995). School counseling: An evolving
Diego, CA: Academic Press. specialty. Journal of Counseling & Development, 74, 150–153.
Evans, J. H., & Burck, H. D. (1992). The effects of career education Raudenbush, S. (1984). Magnitude of teacher expectancy effects of pupil IQ as
interventions on academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of a function of the credibility of the expectancy induction: A synthesis of the
Counseling & Development, 71, 63–68. findings from 18 experiments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 85–97.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 329


Lapan, Gysbers, and Petroski

Schaps, E., Battistich, V., & Solomon, D. (1997). School as a caring com- Wehlage, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out: How much do
munity: A key to character education. In A. Molnar (Ed.), Ninety- schools contribute to the problem? Teachers College Record, 87,
sixth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education (pp. 374–392.
127–139). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Weinstein, R. (1989). Perceptions of classroom process and student
Schneider, B., & Stevenson, D. (1999). The ambitious generation: America’s motivation: Children’s views of self-fulfilling prophecies. In C.
teenagers motivated but directionless. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol.
School-To-Work Opportunities Act, Pub. L. No. 103-289. (1994). 3, pp. 187–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Secretary’s Commission on Achieving Necessary Skills. (1991). What Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle school: The role of
work requires of schools: A SCANS report for America 2000. Washing- perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,
ton, DC: U.S. Department of Labor. 411–419.
St. Clair, K. L. (1989). Middle school counseling research: A resource for Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social support and adjustment in middle school:
school counselors. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 23, The role of parents, teachers, and peers. Journal of Educational
219–226. Psychology, 90, 202–209.
Sun, Y., Hobbs, D., & Elder, W. (1994). Multi-level analyses of television Wentzel, K. R. (1999). Social-motivational processes and interpersonal
viewing among high school students: A contrast between non- relationships: Implications for understanding motivation at school.
metropolitan, rural and other communities. Journal of Research in Rural Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 76–97.
Education, 10, 97–107. Whiston, S. C., & Sexton, T. L. (1998). A review of school counseling
Watkins, C. (1994). Whole-school guidance? British Journal of Guidance outcome research: Implications for practice. Journal of Counseling &
and Counselling, 22, 143–150. Development, 76, 412–426.

330 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


High School Students’ Career-Related
Decision-Making Difficulties

Itamar Gati and Noa Saka

The present study examined the construct of career-related decision-making difficulties among 1,843 Israeli adolescents: choos-
ing a high school (9th grade), choosing high school elective courses (10th grade), and deciding on a military job preference (11th
grade). Three versions of the Career Decision-Making Difficulties Questionnaire (CDDQ) were constructed to match the 3 deci-
sion situations. The structures of the 10 difficulty categories of the revised CDDQ were found similar to that proposed by I. Gati,
M. Krausz, and S. H. Osipow (1996). Boys reported higher difficulties than girls in external conflicts and dysfunctional beliefs.
Research and counseling implications are discussed.

P
eople typically make their first career-related de- dealing with various situations (Scott, Reppucci, & Woolard,
cisions during adolescence. Such decisions may 1995). Indeed, Taveira, Silva, Rodriguez, and Maia (1998)
have lifelong consequences for the individual’s found that adolescents reported that fairly high levels of
vocational future, psychological well-being, stress were associated with career exploration and decision-
health, and social acceptance (Mann, Harmoni, making activities in general. On the other hand, a certain
& Power, 1989). Adolescent students often have to make degree of decision-specific affective distress among adoles-
decisions concerning their choice of high school and their cents can also be adaptive because it increases their moti-
high school elective courses. These decisions affect the stu- vation to seek help and thus decreases the chances for poor
dents’ educational and vocational opportunities. In addi- or ill-informed decisions (Larson & Majors, 1998).
tion, most Israeli high school students, both boys and girls, To help students in making career decisions, school coun-
must make another career-related decision—the type of selors have to locate the difficulties the adolescents face
army service they prefer. In all three situations, the stu- and provide them with guidance on how to overcome, or
dents are given the option to choose an alternative. How- at least minimize, these difficulties. The goal of this study
ever, the final decision, in most cases, is in the hands of the was to characterize and categorize the different types of
involved officials, who also take into account factors over career-related decision-making difficulties faced by Israeli
which the students have no influence. Finally, parents and adolescents, relying on a general theoretical model for ana-
additional significant others explicitly or implicitly affect lyzing such difficulties recently proposed by Gati, Krausz,
the adolescent’s decision. and Osipow (1996).
Although some of the adolescents who are required to
make these early career decisions do so relatively easily, ADOLESCENTS AS CAREER DECISION MAKERS
many others face difficulties before or during the actual
process of decision making. These difficulties may lead them Research has shown that educational issues and future vo-
to attempt to transfer the responsibility for making the cational decisions are of great importance to adolescents
decision to someone else or to delay or even avoid making (Bibby & Postersky, 1985; Collins & Harper, 1974; Eme,
a decision. This may ultimately lead to a less than optimal Maisiak, & Goodale, 1979; Nicholson & Antill, 1981; Rutter,
decision. In addition, the way students handle these deci- 1980; Violato & Holden, 1988). Friedman (1991) investi-
sions may have an effect on the way they will deal with gated types of decisions with which Israeli adolescents (9th
future career decisions. Finally, the stress involved in the and 11th graders) are concerned and found that problems
process may affect various aspects of the adolescents’ daily involving educational issues concern adolescents most (43%
life. For adolescents, who are trying to clarify and consoli- of the problems revolved around studies and career). Among
date their world, the skill of decision making is central in the educational concerns, the most serious ones were choos-

Itamar Gati is a professor of psychology and education, and Noa Saka is a graduate student, both at the Hebrew University in Jerusalem. The authors thank
Michal Malka, Adina Orenstein, Dafna Sharav, and Miri Zarihan, for their contribution in various phases of the research, and Itay Asher, Liat Barkai-
Goodman, Naomi Goldblum, Tali Ever-Hadani, Liat Kibari, Elchanan I. Meir, Leah Israelevich, Gal Ram, Talia Sagiv-HaCohen, and Laura Naftali for
their comments on an earlier version of this article. This research was supported by a grant from the chief scientist of the Department of Education of Israel and
carried out at the NCJW Research Institute for Innovation in Education at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. Correspondence regarding this article should
be sent to Itamar Gati, Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem 91905, Israel (e-mail: msgati@mscc.huji.ac.il).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 331


Gati and Saka

ing high school elective courses (46%) and choosing a high TAXONOMY OF CAREER DECISION-MAKING DIFFICULTIES
school (26%).
The significant developmental process that takes place The taxonomy of difficulties in career decision making
during adolescence results in improved cognitive abilities, developed by Gati et al. (1996) was used as a theoretical
which enhance decision-making capabilities. Piaget and basis for the present research. This taxonomy is based on
Inhelder (1969), for example, described such improvements decision theory, which has come to play an important role
in terms of the transition from concrete cognitive processes in understanding the processes involved in career decision
to abstract ones and from subjective to objective thinking. making (e.g., Brown, 1990; Gati, 1986; Gati & Asher, 2001;
They also described such processes as acquiring confidence Jepsen & Dilley, 1974; Katz, 1966; Mitchell & Krumboltz,
in handling complex environments and relying more on facts 1984; Osipow, 1987; Osipow & Fitzgerald, 1996; Phillips,
and exploration. Lewis (1981) found a positive relation- 1994; Pitz & Harren, 1980; Walsh & Osipow, 1988). The
ship between adolescents’ age and their decision-making difficulties in the taxonomy were based on deviations from
capabilities. This included increased awareness of the risks the model of an “ideal career decision maker.” The ideal career
and implications involved in making a decision, seeking more decision maker is a person who is aware of the need to make
advice from adults or peers, and awareness of the implica- a career decision, is willing to make such a decision, and is
tions of receiving advice from someone with vested inter- capable of making the decision “right” (i.e., a decision that is
ests. Other studies have found cognitive changes, such as based on an appropriate process and is compatible with the
improvements in memory, organization of cognitive pro- individual’s goals and resources). Any deviation from the
cesses, and the ability to better process information and model of the ideal career decision maker is regarded as a
apply knowledge (Keating & Bobbitt, 1978; Sternberg & potential difficulty that may affect the individual’s decision-
Nigro, 1980; Sternberg & Rifkin, 1979). Consequently, ado- making process in one of two possible ways: (a) by preventing
lescents develop the ability to think of the universe of pos- the individual from making a career decision or (b) by leading
sibilities, to frame the alternatives, and to think in abstract to a less than optimal career decision.
terms (Friedman & Mann, 1993; Ormond, Luszcz, Mann, The proposed taxonomy (Gati et al., 1996) includes three
& Beswick, 1991; Steinberg, 1985). major difficulty categories, which are further divided into
Thus, as the need to make significant decisions arises dur- 10 specific categories of difficulty. The first major category,
ing adolescence, the ability to make them develops as well. Lack of Readiness, includes three categories of difficulties
Moreover, research has shown that it is possible to improve that may arise before the beginning of the career decision-
adolescents’ decision-making skills (e.g., Mann, Harmoni, making process: (a) lack of motivation to engage in the ca-
Power, Beswick, & Ormond, 1988; Owens, 1983; Silverman reer decision-making process; (b) general indecisiveness con-
& Wells, 1987). As alluded to earlier, the present study is cerning all types of decisions; and (c) dysfunctional beliefs,
aimed at improving career counselors’ understanding of the including irrational expectations (Nevo, 1987) concerning
various difficulties faced by adolescents in making their the career decision-making process (e.g., “I believe there is
educational career decisions. This should eventually con- only one ideal career for me”).
tribute to developing further interventions aimed at facili- The two other major difficulty categories, Lack of Infor-
tating adolescents’ career decision-making processes by re- mation and Inconsistent Information, include categories of
ducing potential difficulties. difficulties that may arise during the actual career decision-
making process. Lack of Information includes four catego-
CAREER INDECISION ries of difficulties: (a) lack of knowledge about the steps
involved in the process, (b) lack of information about the
The difficulties related to making career and educational deci- self, (c) lack of information about the various alternatives
sions have been associated in the past with the notion “career (i.e., occupations), and (d) lack of information about the ways
indecision.” Previous research has devoted much attention to of obtaining additional information. The major category
categorizing the types of problems related to indecision, Inconsistent Information includes three categories of diffi-
focusing on several theoretical views (Campbell, & Cellini, culty: (a) unreliable information, that is, difficulties related to
1981; Crites, 1978; Fouad, 1994; Osipow & Fitzgerald, unreliable or contradictory information (e.g., above average
1996; Rounds & Tinsley, 1984; Super, 1953). Much of this high school grades, but a low SAT score); (b) internal
discussion focused on categorizing the types of difficulties conflicts, that is, conflicts within the individual, such as con-
related to indecision and on formulating a typology of indi- tradictory preferences or difficulties concerning the need
viduals according to the problems and difficulties they face. to compromise; and (c) external conflicts, that is, conflicts
The empirical research has focused on the development of involving the influence of significant others. The structure
measures for examining individual differences in career inde- of the three major categories and the 10 categories of diffi-
cision (see reviews by Gati et al., 1996; Osipow, 1999; Slaney, culties is summarized in Figure 1.
1988; Spokane & Jacob, 1996). Some of these measures Further distinctions within each category were based on
emerged from the daily practice of career counseling and both theoretical considerations and expected practical sig-
they typically provide a global rather than a refined nificance. For example, within the category of lack of in-
assessment of the individual’s difficulties. formation about the self, a distinction was made between lack

332 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


High School Students’ Career-Related Decision-Making Difficulties

CAREER DECISION-MAKING DIFFICULTIES


Prior to the Process During the Process

Lack of Lack of Inconsistent


Readiness Information Information
due to about due to

Lack of Indeci- Dysfunc- The Self Occupa- Ways of Unreli- Internal External
Motivation siveness tional Career tions (i.e., Obtaining able Conflicts Conflicts
Beliefs Decision- alterna- Informa- Informa-
Making tives) tion tion
Process

FIGURE 1
A Taxonomy of Career Decision-Making Difficulties

of information regarding the individual’s preferences (“What (1999) research supported the construct validity of the
do I want?”) and capabilities (“What can I do?”). (For a more CDDQ and reported a large difference between decided and
detailed description of the taxonomy, see Gati et al., 1996.) undecided groups in the total CDDQ score.
To empirically test the proposed theoretical taxonomy,
Gati et al. (1996) constructed the Career Decision-Making THE GOAL OF THE PRESENT RESEARCH
Difficulties Questionnaire (CDDQ), in which each of the
difficulties in the 10 categories was represented by a state- In the present study, we adapted the taxonomy of difficul-
ment (e.g., “I usually try to avoid commitment”). Studying ties proposed by Gati et al. (1996) to match the difficulties
both American and Israeli samples of young adults, Gati et faced by Israeli adolescent students in three grades and de-
al. (1996) found that the empirical structure of the three cision situations: choosing a high school in the 9th grade
major categories and the 10 specific categories was highly (which is the last year of junior high school in Israel), choos-
similar to the theoretical structure presented in Figure 1. ing high school elective courses in the 10th grade, and de-
Osipow and Gati (1998) examined the construct and concur- ciding on a military job preference in the 11th grade. We
rent validity of the CDDQ. They analyzed the responses of report three studies corresponding to these three decision
403 college students from the United States and examined contexts. For each decision context, we first revised and
the empirical relations of the CDDQ with two measures adapted the original taxonomy (Gati et al., 1996) and then
associated with difficulties in career decision making—the constructed a questionnaire on the basis of the adapted tax-
Career Decision Scale (CDS; Osipow, Carney, & Barak, onomy. Using these questionnaires, we empirically exam-
1976; Osipow & Winer, 1996), which is a widely used in- ined the structure of the difficulties and compared it with
decision questionnaire (Betz, 1992; Slaney, 1988), and the the original theoretical taxonomy. Specifically, in each of
Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (CDMSE; the three decision situations we examined whether the
Taylor & Betz, 1983), which includes five distinct, theo- decision-making difficulties are adequately represented by
retically defined scales, corresponding to the five career-choice the 10 categories proposed by Gati et al. (1996) and whether
competencies suggested by Crites (1978). As hypothesized, the empirical relations among the categories resemble the
Osipow and Gati found that the CDDQ was positively cor- theoretical taxonomy (i.e., whether the 10 categories are
related with the CDS and negatively correlated with the indeed clustered into the three major categories—Lack of
CDMSE. In addition, the scores on the CDDQ indicated Readiness, Lack of Information, and Inconsistent Informa-
that decided students had significantly fewer difficulties than tion). In addition, we examined gender differences, differ-
undecided ones. Lancaster, Rudolph, Perkins, and Patten’s ences between “decided” and “undecided” students, and dif-

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 333


Gati and Saka

ferences among the various grades in the relative salience difficult for me to make a decision”). The participants were
of difficulties involving conflicts with significant others. asked to rate the degree to which each statement described
them on a 7-point scale (1 = does not describe me at all to 7 =
describes me well). At the end of the questionnaire, the students
STUDY 1 were asked to rate the overall severity of their difficulties in
Toward the end of junior high school (i.e., the end of the 9th making the decision and to list any additional difficulties.
grade), most students in Israel can apply to the high school Procedure. The students responded to the questionnaires
they prefer. In certain cases it is possible to make the transi- during a class. The questionnaires were handed out to the
tion from junior high to high school within the same school; students by one of the research assistants in the presence of
in other cases, the student must transfer to another school. a teacher or by the homeroom teacher, who was given the
This school transition allows students to choose a high school relevant instructions and explanation regarding the
specializing in areas of particular interest to them, such as questionnaire’s purposes and importance. The question-
art, music, science, or technology. Usually the students have naires were distributed and the students’ questions, if any,
several alternatives to select from, and, in the larger cities, were answered. No time limitation was given; the time taken
the number of alternatives can reach more than a dozen. It to fill in the questionnaire varied from 20 to 30 minutes.
should be noted, however, that the students’ grades, behav- Analyses. First, we computed the following scores for each
ior, and occasionally also aptitude test scores affect their participant: (a) the score of each of the 10 scales represent-
chances of acceptance to their preferred high school. ing the 10 difficulty categories (defined as the mean of the
items included in each scale), (b) the score of the three
Method major categories (defined as the mean of the respective
scales), and (c) an overall difficulty score (the mean of the
Participants. Five hundred and seventy-nine 9th-grade students 10 scale scores). Next, we computed Cronbach’s alpha re-
(297 girls, 266 boys, 16 did not report sex) from 22 classes (in liability for each of the 10 scales, the three major difficulty
6 junior high schools) in Israel participated in the study. In categories, and the overall difficulty score. Then, using the
this sample, 90% of the students were native Israelis. Pearson product–moment correlation, we computed the
Questionnaire. We revised the 44 statements comprising intercorrelations among all the scales. Finally, to examine
the original CDDQ (Gati et al., 1996) and adapted them the empirical structure of the 10 questionnaire scales, we
to the context of choosing a high school. As part of this used a cluster analysis computer program—ADDTREE
adaptation, we decided to combine into one some of the (Sattath & Tversky, 1977). ADDTREE represents the
statements that earlier were found to be highly correlated, intercorrelations among the scales in the form of an addi-
and we deleted a few other irrelevant statements. These tive or “path length” tree, in which the variables are divided
changes were based on the results of item analyses of the into clusters according to the size of the correlations be-
original CDDQ, using data collected prior to the current tween them, with highly correlated variables being located
study (see Gati et al., 1996; Gati, Osipow, Krausz, & Saka, in the same cluster.
2000; Osipow & Gati, 1998). In addition, 20 school counse-
lors and 100 ninth-grade students were asked to list difficul-
Results
ties involved in the choice of high school, and we examined
whether all the difficulties listed by the school counselors Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and
and the students, themselves, could be represented by the Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the 10 scale scores, the three
statements in the revised questionnaire. After several revi- major categories, and the overall score. As in previous stud-
sions, 35 statements were included in the final version of the ies (Gati et al., 1996; Gati et al., 2000; Osipow & Gati,
questionnaire. 1998), Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the 10 scale scores
The first page of the anonymous questionnaire included varied, ranging from .40 for the scale of Dysfunctional Be-
general background information such as sex, name of cur- liefs to .82 for External Conflicts; the median scale reliabil-
rent school, grade, number of children in the family, position ity was .68. Among the three major categories, Lack of
in birth order, birthplace, and year of immigration to Israel. Readiness had the lowest reliability (.62); the reliability of
Following this were two questions regarding whether the the two other major categories was much higher (.88 for
student had some alternative (i.e., a specific high school) in Lack of Information, and .87 for Inconsistent Information).
mind and what was his or her degree of confidence in that The reliability of the questionnaire as a whole was .91.
alternative (on a 5-point scale, 1 = not confident at all, 5 = The mean overall difficulty score of students who had
very confident). Next, the students were asked to report to already considered an alternative was significantly lower
what degree they believed their choice would affect their than that of students who had not yet considered one, t(570)
vocational future and to what degree they believed their own = 2.70, p < .01. Although the mean severity of difficulties
preferences would determine which high school they would reported by the students who had already considered an
finally attend (both on a 5-point scale). alternative (3.58) was, as expected, lower than that of those
The subsequent four pages included 35 statements, each who had not (3.68), the difference was relatively small and
corresponding to a particular difficulty (e.g., “It is usually not statistically significant, t(560) = 0.53, p > .05.

334 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


High School Students’ Career-Related Decision-Making Difficulties

TABLE 1 As can be seen in Figure 2, the empirical structure is simi-


lar, although not identical, to that of the theoretical model
Means, Standard Deviations, Reliabilities of the underlying the questionnaire (see Figure 1). As expected,
Scales of the CDDQ, and Their Correlation With the major category, Lack of Readiness, emerged separately,
Severity in Study 1 (N = 579) suggesting that there is a distinction between difficulties
Correlation arising prior to the career decision-making process and those
Number With arising during the actual process. Although the third major
Scale/Category of Items M SD α Severity category, Inconsistent Information, also emerged as ex-
pected, the second category, Lack of Information, seemed
Lack of Readiness
Lack of Motivation 3 2.80 1.43 .59 .14* less distinct. First, in contrast to the hypothesized struc-
Indecisiveness 4 3.92 1.20 .54 .26* ture, the scale Lack of Information About the Process ap-
Dysfunctional peared independently instead of being located under Lack
Beliefs 3 3.50 1.34 .40 .08
Lack of Information
of Information. This deviation may be attributed to the fact
About the that lack of knowledge about how to engage in a decision-
Process 3 3.09 1.53 .78 .44* making process can arise prior to the actual engagement in
About the Self 4 2.81 1.31 .73 .43* such a process as well as during the process. Second, the
About the
Alternatives 3 2.74 1.56 .60 .34* scale Lack of Information About the Self emerged sepa-
About Ways of rately from the other two scales of Lack of Information.
Obtaining This deviation reflects the relatively high correlations of
Additional
Information 2 2.31 1.48 .65 .33*
this scale with difficulties involving Unreliable Information
Inconsistant and Internal Conflicts (.57 and .58). The two scales of Lack
Information of Information, which represent objective information (Lack
Unreliable of Information About the Alternatives and Lack of Infor-
Information 3 2.99 1.59 .72 .44*
Internal Conflicts 6 3.21 1.41 .76 .50* mation About Ways of Obtaining Additional Information),
External Conflicts 4 2.63 1.52 .82 .42* were grouped together as expected. In addition, Internal
Lack of Readiness 10 3.41 0.92 .62 .22* and External Conflicts were included in the same subcluster,
Lack of Information 12 2.74 1.20 .88 .47* as expected.
Inconsistent
Information 13 2.94 1.27 .87 .53* Next, we computed the correlations of the overall subjec-
Total CDDQ 35 3.00 0.97 .91 .51* tive severity reported by the students with each of the 10
difficulty scales. These correlations are presented on the right-
Note. CDDQ = Career Decision-Making Difficulties Questionnaire.
*p < .001. hand side of Table 1. All correlations were positive and,
except for the scale of Dysfunctional Beliefs, statistically
significant (p < .001). The correlations were higher for the
We computed the intercorrelations among the 10 scale Lack of Information and Inconsistent Information major cat-
scores. The clustering structure derived from this egories, and lower for difficulties related to Lack of Readiness.
intercorrelation matrix (by ADDTREE) is presented in Figure
2. The distance between any pair of scales in this clustering STUDY 2
structure is represented by the sum of horizontal segments
on the shortest path connecting them. The variance linearly In the 10th grade, Israeli high school students have to decide
accounted for by the distances in the clustering structure is how many credit points (3–5) they prefer to take in each of
.96, indicating that the clustering structure in the figure ad- their obligatory courses (e.g., mathematics, English, Hebrew
equately summarizes the empirical relations among the scales. literature), which elective courses to take as advanced-level
courses (e.g., sciences: physics, chemistry, etc.; humanities:
history, languages etc.; social sciences: psychology, econom-
Lack of Motivation ics, etc.) and at what levels. The elective courses selected and
Indecisiveness the levels in both types of courses (reflected in the number
Dysfunctional Beliefs
of credit points) may affect future educational possibilities;
Lack of Information About the Process
for example, to study engineering, students must take at least
Lack of Information About Alternatives
one advanced science course in high school.
Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Information
Lack of Information About the Self
Method
Unreliable Information
Internal Conflicts Participants. Seven hundred and eighty-seven 10th-grade
External Conflicts students (465 girls, 300 boys, 22 did not report sex) in 23
FIGURE 2 classes (in 8 high schools) in Israel participated in the study.
In this sample, 87% of the students were native Israelis.
The Structure of the 10 Difficulty Scales in Procedure. We adapted the CDDQ to the context of choos-
Grade 9 (N = 579) ing high school elective courses using the same procedure as

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 335


Gati and Saka

in Study 1. The content of the items was revised to match not yet considered one, t(778) = 4.94, p < .001. In addition,
the specific decision; the revised and adapted questionnaire the mean severity rating of the former group (3.71) was
consisted of 36 questions. The data collection procedure and lower than that of the latter (5.03), t(758) = 5.46, p < .001.
analyses were similar to those of Study 1. The clustering structure derived from the intercorrelation
matrix (by ADDTREE) is presented in Figure 3. The vari-
Results ance linearly accounted for by the distances in the clustering
structure is .96. As can be seen in Figure 3, the empirical
Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, and structure is highly similar to that obtained for the 9th-grade
Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the 10 scale scores of the students and similar but not identical to the theoretical model
questionnaire, its three major categories, and its overall score. underlying the questionnaire (see Figure 1). As expected,
As can be seen in Table 2, Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of two of the major categories—Lack of Readiness and Incon-
the 10 scale scores show considerable variation. As in Study sistent Information—emerged, whereas the four scales of
1, the lowest reliability (.34) was observed for the scale of Lack of Information were less cohesive. First, Lack of Infor-
Dysfunctional Beliefs; the other scales had moderate to high mation About the Process appeared independently, as in the
reliabilities ranging from .57 for the scale of Lack of Moti- 9th-grade sample. Second, Lack of Information About Self
vation to .84 for External Conflicts (the median scale reli- appeared independently but close to the clusters of Lack of
ability was .65). Among the three major categories, Lack of Information About Alternatives and Lack of Information
Readiness had the lowest reliability (.58); the reliabilities About Ways of Obtaining Additional Information.
of the two other major categories were much higher (.88 Next, we computed the correlations between the subjective
for Lack of Information, and .86 for Inconsistent Informa- severity reported by the students and each of the 10 difficulty
tion). The reliability of the questionnaire as a whole was scales. These correlations are presented on the right-hand
.91, the same as that in Study 1. Again, the mean difficulty column of Table 2. The pattern of correlations was similar to
score of students who had already considered an alterna- that in Study 1, with a low correlation for Lack of Readiness.
tive was significantly lower than that of students who had
STUDY 3
TABLE 2
At the end of high school, by the age of 18, the majority of
Means, Standard Deviations, Reliabilities of the Israeli youth are drafted for mandatory military service (typi-
Scales of the CDDQ, and Their Correlation With cally, 3 years for young men and 2 years for young women).
Severity in Study 2 (N = 787) Although the actual military job and its location are deter-
mined by the army, according to the individual’s specific
Correlation skills and capabilities and the army’s needs, the adolescent
Number With can contribute to the army’s decision regarding his or her
Scale/Category of Items M SD α Severity
military job. During the second half of the 11th grade, stu-
Lack of Readiness dents are usually asked to indicate their military job prefer-
Lack of Motivation 3 3.18 1.42 .57 .24*
Indecisiveness 4 3.85 1.28 .59 .34*
ence. This is often done by ranking the options on a given
Dysfunctional list of possible military jobs, which is compiled by the army
Beliefs 3 3.36 1.20 .34 –.02 on the basis of medical, psychometric, and other additional
Lack of Information information about the individual. Many schools have “open
About the
Process 3 3.27 1.51 .72 .48* days,” on which army representatives come to the school
About the Self 5 3.06 1.40 .76 .53* and describe various military jobs to the students.
About the
Alternatives 3 3.06 1.46 .63 .47*
About Ways of Lack of Motivation
Obtaining
Dysfunctional Beliefs
Additional
Information 2 2.28 1.39 .66 .30* Indecisiveness
Inconsistant Lack of Information About the Process
Information
Lack of Information About the Self
Unreliable
Information 3 3.19 1.55 .65 .44* Lack of Information About Alternatives
Internal Conflicts 6 3.08 1.26 .73 .56* Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Information
External Conflicts 4 2.43 1.50 .84 .36* Unreliable Information
Lack of Readiness 10 3.46 0.86 .58 .29*
Internal Conflicts
Lack of Information 13 2.92 1.18 .88 .55*
Inconsistent External Conflicts
Information 13 2.90 1.23 .86 .52*
Total CDDQ 36 3.08 0.94 .91 .56* FIGURE 3
Note. See Table 1 Note. The Structure of the 10 Difficulty Scales in
*p < .001. Grade 10 (N = 787)

336 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


High School Students’ Career-Related Decision-Making Difficulties

Deliberating about one’s future military job can sometimes TABLE 3


be very stressful because this job will be one’s occupation
for 2 or 3 years and can affect one’s future civilian occupa- Means, Standard Deviations, Reliabilities of the
tion. The decision process may involve many difficulties, Scales of the CDDQ, and Their Correlation With
which can be conceptualized by the categories in the tax- Severity in Study 3 (N = 477)
onomy. First, information difficulties are often prominent in Correlation
this process because many military jobs are classified. Sec- Number With
ond, external conflicts may arise when the individual’s pref- Scale/Category of Items M SD α Severity
erences are not congruent with those of his or her family Lack of Readiness
(e.g., the individual may want to be in a combat unit, but the Lack of Motivation 3 2.89 1.26 .47 .02
family may express concerns about this preference). Another Indecisiveness 4 3.37 1.25 .60 .26*
Dysfunctional
difficulty may arise from the fact that it is the army that Beliefs 3 3.82 1.45 .64 –.15
ultimately determines the individual’s military occupation Lack of Information
(which may be incompatible with his or her preferences). About the
This may decrease the individual’s motivation to engage in Process 3 3.47 1.67 .76 .66*
About the Self 4 2.82 1.32 .66 .58*
the decision-making process in the first place. About the
Alternatives 3 3.99 1.78 .81 .51*
Method About Ways of
Obtaining
Participants. Four hundred and seventy-seven 11th-grade stu- Additional
dents (239 girls, 219 boys, 19 did not report sex) from 17 Information 1 3.33 2.03 — .45*
classes (in 3 high schools) in Israel participated in the study. Inconsistant
Information
In this sample, 89% of the students were native Israelis. Unreliable
Procedure. We adapted the CDDQ to the context of de- Information 4 2.67 1.40 .72 .34*
ciding on a military job preference using the same proce- Internal Conflicts 6 3.09 1.31 .72 .45*
External Conflicts 4 2.35 1.50 .84 .24*
dure as in the previous studies. The content of the items Lack of Readiness 10 3.36 0.83 .56 .05
was revised to match the specific decision; the revised and Lack of Information 11 3.40 1.41 .88 .65*
adapted questionnaire consisted of 35 questions. The data Inconsistent
collection procedure and analyses were similar to those of Information 14 2.70 1.19 .87 .40*
Total CDDQ 35 3.18 0.93 .90 .56*
Studies 1 and 2.
Note. See Table 1 Note.
Results *p < .001.

Table 3 presents the means, standard deviations, and


Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the 10 scale scores of the categories—Lack of Readiness, Lack of Information, and
questionnaire, its three major categories, and its overall score. Inconsistent Information—and a distinction arises between
As can be seen in Table 3, the lowest internal consistency the first major category, which includes difficulties arising
was observed for Lack of Motivation (.47). The other scales prior to the career decision-making process, and the other
had moderate to high reliabilities, ranging from .60 for the two major categories, which include difficulties arising dur-
scale of Indecisiveness to .84 for External Conflicts (the ing actual engagement in the process.
median scale reliability was .72). Among the three major As in the previous studies, we also computed the correla-
categories, Lack of Readiness had the lowest reliability (.56); tions between the overall subjective severity reported by the
the reliability of the two other major categories was again
much higher (.88 for Lack of Information, and .87 for In-
consistent Information). The reliability of the questionnaire Lack of Motivation

as a whole was .90. The mean difficulty score of students Dysfunctional Beliefs

who had already considered an alternative (3.04) was sig- Indecisiveness

nificantly lower than that of students who had not yet con- Lack of Information About the Process

sidered one (3.60), t(470) = 5.69, p < .001. Similarly, the Lack of Information About the Self

mean of severity ratings of the former group (3.14) was lower Lack of Information About Alternatives

than that of the latter group (4.96), t(468) = 9.80, p < .001. Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Information

The clustering structure derived (by ADDTREE) from Unreliable Information

the intercorrelation matrix is presented in Figure 4. The Internal Conflicts

variance linearly accounted for by the distances in the clus- External Conflicts

tering structure is .94. As can be seen in Figure 4, the em- FIGURE 4


pirical structure is very close to the theoretical model pre-
sented in Figure 1. The scales are clearly grouped into three The Structure of the 10 Difficulty Scales in
clusters corresponding to the hypothesized three major Grade 11 (N = 477)

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 337


Gati and Saka

students and each of the 10 difficulty scales. These correla- Lack of Motivation

tions are presented in the right-hand column of Table 3, and Dysfunctional Beliefs

were similar in pattern to those found in the previous studies. Indecisiveness


Lack of Information About the Process

A COMPARISON AMONG THE GRADES AND AN ACROSS-GRADE Lack of Information About the Self
Lack of Information About Alternatives
ANALYSIS Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Information
Unreliable Information
Gender Differences
Internal Conflicts
Boys reported greater difficulties than girls in all grades re- External Conflicts
garding external conflicts, t(558) = 2.53, p < .01, t(750) =
4.42, p < .0001, and t(456) = 2.13, p < .05; and in two grades FIGURE 5
regarding dysfunctional beliefs, t(561) = 2.88, p < .01, t(763) The Structure of the 10 Difficulty Scales Across All
= 1.91, p = .06, and t(456) = 3.82, p < .001, respectively for Students (N = 1,843)
Studies 1, 2 and 3. In addition, greater difficulties were re-
ported by boys in Grades 9 and 10 in the scales Lack of Figure 1. As can be seen, a distinction clearly emerges
Information About Ways of Obtaining Additional Informa- between difficulties arising prior to the career decision-making
tion, t(560) = 4.53, p < .001, t(756) = 3.92, p < .001, respec- process and those arising during the actual process. The
tively; and Internal Conflicts, t(560) = 2.48, p < .01, t(757) = latter difficulties are divided into those involving lack of
2.76, p < .01, respectively. Differences were also noted in information and those involving inconsistent information.
the major category Inconsistent Information, t(560) = 3.06, As anticipated, the scales Lack of Information About Alter-
p < .01, t(757) = 3.59, p < .001, respectively; and in the natives and Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining
overall difficulty score, t(561) = 3.60, p < .001, t(763) = 3.17, Additional Information are grouped together, as are the
p < .001, respectively. Girls reported more difficulties than scales Internal Conflicts and External Conflicts. This struc-
boys in the 11th grade in three of the scales related to lack of ture suggests that the observed deviations in the separate
information: Lack of Information About the Process, t(454) analyses of the grades may reflect grade-dependent specific
= 3.06, p < .01; Lack of Information About Alternatives, t(454) factors, whereas on the whole the structure holds.
= 5.46, p < .0001; and Lack of Information About Ways of
Obtaining Additional Information, t(454) = 2.68, p < .01.
These differences were also reflected in the major category DISCUSSION
Lack of Information, t(454) = 4.04, p < .001. The present study examined career-related decision diffi-
culties among adolescents. To assess these difficulties, we
The Relative Salience of Difficulties Involving External used revised versions of the CDDQ, adapted to suit three
Conflicts decision situations encountered by most Israeli adolescents.
We also tested the hypothesis that the relative salience of We collected and analyzed the responses of 9th-grade stu-
difficulties involving external conflicts (whose sources are dents choosing a high school, 10th-grade students choos-
“significant others,” e.g., parents) decreases with age. To test ing high school elective courses, and 11th-grade students
this hypothesis, we first computed the standard score of deciding on their military job preference.
the scale External Conflicts for each student. The mean of The structures of the adolescents’ decision-making difficul-
the standard score of the External Conflicts scale was nega- ties in the three studies were compatible with the hypoth-
tive and statistically significant in all three grades (–0.33), esized structure (presented in Figure 1), and the across-grade
t(572) = 8.86; (–0.56), t(769) = 16.34; and (–0.64), t(466) = structure replicated the findings of the Israeli sample in Gati
15.90; for Grades 9, 10, and 11, respectively. This indicates et al. (1996). The small deviations found among grades may
that these difficulties are relatively mild. As anticipated, we be accounted for by particular, grade-specific characteristics;
found that the degree of difficulty involving external however, only further research can tell whether these devia-
conflicts decreased with age: A planned contrast revealed tions are indeed consistent, and if they are, what these specific
that external conflicts were smaller in the 11th grade than in characteristics may be. Finally, in the general analysis, the three
the 9th and 10th grades, t(1808) = 4.22, p < .001, and smaller major categories emerged as expected, and a distinction was
in the 10th grade than in 9th grade, t(1341)= 4.75, p < .001. observed between the major category Lack of Readiness and
the other two, Lack of Information and Inconsistent Informa-
tion. The great similarity between the across-grade structure
The Structure of the Scales
and the theoretical structure proposed by Gati et al. (1996)
Figure 5 presents the clustering structure derived from an provides clear support for the robustness of the latter.
across-samples intercorrelation matrix (the linearly ac- The pattern of scale reliabilities obtained in this research
counted for variance is .96). This empirical structure pro- replicated previous findings (Gati et al., 1996; Gati et al.,
vides strong support for the theoretical taxonomy because 2000; Osipow & Gati, 1998). In particular, the consistently
it is highly similar to the theoretical structure presented in low reliability of the Dysfunctional Beliefs scale is a matter

338 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


High School Students’ Career-Related Decision-Making Difficulties

of concern, and further research is needed to better under- the career decision-making difficulties was based on a par-
stand this category of difficulties. The low reliability of the ticular theoretical approach and on the use of the revised
scale Lack of Motivation in the present studies also deserves CDDQ as an instrument; further research could use other
further attention. In general, as could be expected with a theoretical models and other instruments.
small number of items per scale, the reliabilities of the scales
are only moderate; further research may contribute to im- Counseling Implications
proving these reliabilities. Nevertheless, the questionnaire
was clearly able to distinguish between students who had Despite these limitations, the present research provides addi-
already considered an alternative and those who had not, tional support for the theoretical taxonomy of career decision-
in terms of their overall difficulties (with the former hav- making difficulties proposed by Gati et al. (1996). Furthermore,
ing significantly fewer difficulties), a finding which pro- the findings suggest that the CDDQ may be used to adequately
vides support for its concurrent validity. assess systematic categories of difficulties that share common
We also examined to what extent differences in the pat- features such as cause, timing, impact, or required intervention.
tern of relationships between adolescents and their parents Specifically, the revised CDDQ may be used for needs assess-
in the three grades are expressed in the relative difficulties ments of particular groups. For example, school counselors can
related to significant others’ influence. As could be expected, use the CDDQ to facilitate the identification of groups of
the severity of external conflicts decreased from 9th to 11th students who have difficulties related to one of the three major
grade. This finding indicates that difficulties stemming from categories and who may benefit from the same intervention.
the influence of significant others is positively correlated with The CDDQ also makes it possible to assess outcomes of inter-
the significant other’s influence: the lower such influence is, ventions aimed at reducing general or specific career decision-
the lower are the difficulties attributed to that source. making difficulties. However, further research aimed at
Possible gender differences were also explored in the improving the scales’ reliability is needed before the revised
present research. In external conflicts, boys reported greater CDDQ can be introduced as a potential diagnostic instrument.
difficulties than girls consistently in all three grades. A pos- This research has direct implications for counseling and
sible explanation may be that parents and society have higher general intervention programs: Students should be taught
educational and career aspirations for boys than for girls and basic decision-making skills. Such interventions may ease
that they generally expect boys to be more career-oriented students’ decision-making process and help them make
(Hoffman, 1977). Furthermore, boys are subjected to more better career decisions. Further research should aim at cross-
peer pressure regarding occupational choices (McMahon & cultural validation of the taxonomy and its relative relevance
Patton, 1997). This may lead to more intense conflicts with to adolescents in other countries. It should also investigate
significant others regarding boys’ choices. In other catego- whether the need for instruction in decision-making skills
ries, 9th- and 10th-grade students revealed a similar pattern is a common phenomenon in other countries and whether
of gender differences, wherein boys reported greater diffi- teaching such skills to students indeed helps them make
culties involving internal conflicts, unreliable information, better decisions. Further research should also examine
and lack of information about ways of obtaining additional whether the major difficulties contributing to the subjec-
information. In the 11th grade, however, girls reported more tive feeling of severity of difficulty are similar to those that
difficulties involving lack of information than boys did. The emerged in this study.
finding that 9th- and 10th-grade boys have more difficulties Finally, the present research suggests that the general struc-
related to lack of information than girls have may be attrib- ture of difficulties consisting of Lack of Readiness, Lack of
uted to the fact that boys find it more difficult to seek help Information, and Inconsistent Information underlies career-
(Gross & McMullen, 1983), whereas the reversal of the related decisions made in high school. Recent research (Shefer,
pattern in Grade 11 may be attributed to the specific char- 2000) found that the taxonomy of difficulties used in this
acteristics of the decision regarding army service. research might also underlie difficulties in other types of deci-
sions faced by young adults, such as choosing a partner. Future
research might further test the hypothesis that the same tax-
Limitations
onomy can be applied to difficulties involved in other, nonca-
This research was carried out in Israeli junior high schools reer-related decisions faced by adolescents and adults.
and high schools. In countries with different characteristics
(e.g., a different high school education system) or with dif-
ferent decision tasks, the relative salience of the various dif-
REFERENCES
ficulty categories will probably be different. In addition, Betz, N. E. (1992). Career assessment: A review of critical issues. In S.
the reliability of the scale Dysfunctional Beliefs was lower D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology
than acceptable, even given the small number of items. (pp. 453–484). New York: Wiley.
Another limitation is that we could not compare the de- Bibby, R. W., & Postersky, D. C. (1985). The emerging generation. Toronto,
Canada: Irwin.
gree of the difficulty reported for the various scales be- Brown, D. (1990). Models of career decision making. In D. Brown &
tween the three grades, due to the confounding between L. Brooks (Eds.), Career choice and development (2nd ed., pp. 295–421).
the grade and the decision task. Finally, the assessment of San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 339


Gati and Saka

Campbell, R. E., & Cellini, J. V. (1981). A diagnostic taxonomy of adult Ormond, C., Luszcz, M. A., Mann, L., & Beswick, G. (1991). A
career problems. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 19, 175–190. metacognitive analysis of decision making in adolescence. Journal of
Collins, J., & Harper, J. (1974). Problems of adolescents in Sydney. Jour- Adolescence, 14, 275–291.
nal of Genetic Psychology, 125, 189–194. Osipow, S. H. (1987). Manual for the Career Decision Scale. Odessa, FL:
Crites, J. O. (1978). Career Maturity Inventory theory and research hand- Psychological Assessment Resources.
book. Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill. Osipow, S. H. (1999). Assessing career indecision. Journal of Vocational
Eme, R., Maisiak, R., & Goodale, W. (1979). Seriousness of adolescent Behavior, 55, 147–154.
problems. Adolescence, 14, 93–194. Osipow, S. H., Carney, C. G., & Barak, A. (1976). A scale of educational-
Fouad, N. A. (1994). Annual Reviews 1991–1993: Vocational choice, vocational undecidedness: A typological approach. Journal of Voca-
decision-making, assessment, and intervention. Journal of Vocational tional Behavior, 9, 233–324.
Behavior, 45, 125–176. Osipow, S. H., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1996). Theories of career development
Friedman, A. I. (1991). Areas of concern and sources of advice for Israeli (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
adolescents. Adolescents, 26, 967–976. Osipow, S. H., & Gati, I. (1998). Construct and concurrent validity of
Friedman, A. I., & Mann, L. (1993). Coping patterns in adolescent deci- the Career Decision-Making Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of
sion making: An Israeli-Australian comparison. Journal of Adolescence, Career Assessment, 6, 347–364.
16, 187–199. Osipow, S. H., & Winer, J. (1996). Career assessment and the Career
Gati, I. (1986). Making career decisions: A sequential elimination ap- Decision Scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 117–130.
proach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 408–417. Owens, T. R. (Ed.). (1983). Helping youth become more responsible. Wash-
Gati, I., & Asher, I. (2001). The PIC model for career decision making: ington, DC: National Institute of Education (EDN00001).
Prescreening, in-depth exploration, and choice. In F. T. L. Leong & A. Phillips, S. D. (1994). Choice and change: Convergence from the decision-
Barak (Eds.), Contemporary models in vocational psychology (pp. 7–54). making perspective. In M. L. Savikas & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Convergence
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. in career development theories (pp. 155–163). Palo Alto, CA: Consult-
Gati, I., Krausz, M., & Osipow, S. H. (1996). A taxonomy of difficulties in ing Psychologists Press.
career decision making. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 43, 510–526. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York:
Gati, I., Osipow, S. H., Krausz, M., & Saka, N. (2000). Validity of the Career Basic Books.
Decision-Making Difficulty Questionnaire: Counselees’ versus career Pitz, G., & Harren, V. A. (1980). An analysis of career decision making
counselors’ perceptions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 99–113. from the point of view of information processing and decision theory.
Gross, A. E., & McMullen, P. A. (1983). The help-seeking process. In V. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 16, 320–346.
J. Derleja & J. J. Grzelak (Eds.), Cooperation and helping behavior: Theo- Rounds, J. B., & Tinsley, H. E. A. (1984). Diagnosis and treatment of
ries and research (pp. 305–326). New York: Academic Press. vocational problems. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of
Hoffman, L. W. (1977). Changes in family roles, socialization and sex counseling psychology (pp. 137–177). New York: Wiley.
differences. American Psychologist, 32, 644–657. Rutter, M. (1980). Changing youth in a changing society. Cambridge, MA:
Jepsen, D. A., & Dilley, J. S. (1974). Vocational decision-making models: Harvard University Press.
A review and comparative analysis. Review of Educational Research, Sattath, S., & Tversky, A. (1977). Additive similarity trees. Psychometrika,
44, 331–349. 42, 319–345.
Katz, M. R. (1966). A model for guidance for career decision making. Scott, E. S., Reppucci, N., & Woolard, J. (1995). Evaluating adolescent
Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 15, 2–10. decision making in legal contexts. Law and Human Behavior, 19,
Keating, D., & Bobbitt, B. (1978). Individual and developmental differ- 221–244.
ences in cognitive processing component of ability. Child Develop- Shefer, M. (2000). Difficulties of young adults in choosing a partner. Un-
ment, 52, 538–544. published master’s thesis. Department of psychology, Tel-Aviv Uni-
Lancaster, P. L., Rudolph, C., Perkins, S., & Patten, T. (1999). Difficulties versity, Israel.
in career decision making: A study of the reliability and validity of the Silverman, B., & Wells, S. M. (1987). Facing the facts regarding teenage
Career Decision Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of Career Assess- pregnancy. Social Work in Education, 9, 191–196.
ment, 4, 393–413. Slaney, R. B. (1988). The assessment of career decision making. In W. B.
Larson, M. L., & Majors, M. S. (1998). Application of the Coping with Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.), Career decision making (pp. 33–76).
Career Indecision Instrument with adolescents. Journal of Career As- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
sessment, 6, 163–179. Spokane, A. R., & Jacob, E. J. (1996). Career and vocational assessment
Lewis, C. (1981). How adolescents approach decisions: Changes over 1993–1994: A biennial review. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 1–32.
grades seven to twelve, and policy implications. Child Development, Steinberg, L. (1985). Adolescence. New York: Knopf.
52, 538–544. Sternberg, R., & Nigro, G. (1980). Developmental patterns in the solu-
Mann, L., Harmoni, R., & Power, C. (1989). Adolescent decision making: tions of verbal analogies. Child Development, 51, 27–38.
The development of competence. Journal of Adolescence, 12, 265–278. Sternberg, R., & Rifkin, B. (1979). The development of analogical
Mann, L., Harmoni, R., Power, C., Beswick, G., & Ormond, C. (1988). reasoning process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27,
Effectiveness of the GOFER course in decision making for high school 195–232.
students. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 1, 159–168. Super, D. E. (1953). A theory of vocational development. American Psy-
McMahon, M., & Patton, W. (1997). Gender differences in children and chologist, 8, 185–190.
adolescents’ perceptions of influence on their career development. Taveira, M. D. C., Silva, M. C., Rodriguez, M. L., & Maia, J. (1998).
The School Counselor, 44, 368–376. Individual characteristics and career exploration in adolescents. Brit-
Mitchell, L. K., & Krumboltz, J. D. (1984). Research on human decision ish Journal of Counselling and Guidance, 26, 89–104.
making: Implications for career decision making and counseling. In S. Taylor, K. M., & Betz, N. E. (1983). Applications of self-efficacy theory
D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (pp. to the understanding and treatment of career indecision. Journal of
238–280). New York: Wiley. Vocational Behavior, 22, 63–81.
Nevo, O. (1987). Irrational expectations in career counseling and their con- Violato, C., & Holden, W. B. (1988). A confirmatory factor analysis of a
fronting arguments. The Career Development Quarterly, 35, 239–250. four-factor model of adolescent concerns. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Nicholson, S. I., & Antill, J. K. (1981). Personal problems of adolescents cence, 17, 101–112.
and their relationship to peer acceptance and sex role identity. Jour- Walsh, W. B., & Osipow, S. H. (Eds.). (1988). Career decision making.
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 10, 309–325. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

340 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Assessing the Impact of a Prejudice Prevention Project

Michael Salzman and Michael D’Andrea

This article reports on the effectiveness of a prejudice prevention intervention that was used among a culturally diverse group of
students in Hawaii. Implications of the results of this primary prevention effort for counseling practice and interculturally adaptive
social development are discussed.

T
he increasing racial and cultural diversity in our There is substantial evidence suggesting that the type of
schools challenges counselors and educators. The intergroup contact people from diverse cultural or racial back-
potentials inherent in current demographic grounds have with one another influences the cognitive, af-
trends are multiple. For instance, diversity may fective, and behavioral outcomes of that contact (Gazda et al.,
fuel conflict or mutual enrichment, fear or de- 2001). In this regard, it has been noted that equal status and
lightful and fruitful interaction, appreciation or the dero- intimate and cooperative contact in the service of a shared
gation of different “others.” School counselors can and superordinate goal support the development of positive inter-
should be involved in maximizing the positive potentials group interactions (Allport, 1982). Counselors can work with
inherent in our diverse school communities while minimiz- teachers and administrators to promote these conditions within
ing the potentials for stereotyping, prejudice, and intergroup the school setting in order to facilitate the positive potentials
conflict. In the absence of intentional and effective educa- of intergroup interactions and thereby reduce the negative
tional programming designed to prepare young people to potentials inherent in such encounters. By cultivating these
live and work productively in a multicultural, diverse soci- intergroup characteristics and behavioral contexts, counselors
ety and global economy, the negative aforementioned po- are able to positively contribute to the construction of a more
tentials are more likely to become manifest. The promo- peaceful, respectful, and collaborative society. In doing so, coun-
tion of respect and appreciation for human diversity is an selors can help to build a more just world.
educational imperative and an ethical responsibility of
school counselors (Salzman, 1995). Ethnocentrism and FOSTERING PREJUDICE REDUCTION
prejudice can degrade the human experience and may mo-
tivate the destructive behavioral expressions of the dynam- Primary, secondary, and tertiary efforts toward prejudice
ics inherent in cross-cultural and inter-group interactions reduction and respectful appreciation of human diversity
(Gazda, Ginter, & Horne, 2001). help to contribute to the development of a healthy, peace-
Ethnocentrism, prejudice, and racism are maladaptive ful, and productive human community. Primary prevention
responses to the anxieties provoked by human differences refers to intentional programs and services that target groups
on central existential problems such as the core ontological of currently unaffected people for purposes of helping them
concerns of how one is to live and what kind of person one continue functioning in healthy ways, free from disturbance
is to be in order to achieve a sense of anxiety-buffering self- (Conyne, 1987). Secondary prevention refers to an early iden-
esteem. Diverse cultural responses to these concerns may tification of problems followed by interventions designed
produce anxieties and provoke intolerant defensive responses to minimize their further continuation, development, or
(Greenberg, Solomon, & Pyszczynski, 1997). Education, escalation. Tertiary prevention refers to efforts to remediate
counseling, and guidance can and should play a significant or diminish the intensity of serious problems that have al-
role in reducing the probability of racist and intolerant re- ready emerged (Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993).
sponses to human anxieties. There is empirical evidence that D’Andrea and Daniels (1995) tested the effectiveness of
worldviews imbued with a sense of tolerance and respect for a multicultural developmental framework that was designed
diversity are resilient to defensive manifestations of intoler- to foster prejudice reduction. This prejudice reduction
ance evoked by anxiety-provoking stimuli. People with such framework comprised 10 guidance activities that were origi-
worldviews may actually become more tolerant under such nally developed by Omizo and D’Andrea (1995). The ini-
conditions (Greenberg et al., 1997). tial research that was done by these investigators involved

Michael Salzman is an assistant professor, and Michael D’Andrea is a professor, both in the Department of Counselor Education at the University of Hawaii,
Manoa. The authors thank Salynn Ancheta, Liane Saito, and Michelle Tesoro for their invaluable contributions to this project. Correspondence regarding this
article should be sent to Michael Salzman, Department of Counselor Education, University of Hawaii, 1776 University Avenue, Honolulu, HI 96822 (e-mail:
msalzman@hawaii.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 341


Salzman and D’Andrea

a group of culturally diverse third-grade students who ex- were assigned to the control group. The group assignments
hibited signs of ethnic conflict and stress. More specifically, were made based on class membership in which the classes
the students who participated in this school-based prejudice were essentially equivalent in ability range, age, students’
reduction intervention demonstrated negative aggressive socioeconomic backgrounds, and ethnic diversity. Both the
behaviors (i.e., arguing and fighting in class and on the play- treatment and control groups were ethnically diverse, in-
ground during recess) and were referred to the school coun- cluding children identified as Hawaiian or part Hawaiian,
selor by their teachers. These incidents were almost always Filipino, Asian, Caucasian, African American, and Hispanic.
reported to be initiated by some student making deriding
and derogatory comments about another child’s cultural, Instruments
ethnic, or racial background (D’Andrea & Daniels, 1995). The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) used in the
The focus of the initial prejudice reduction intervention was, D’Andrea and Daniels (1995) study was used to assess stu-
therefore, on the level of secondary prevention. dents’ social skills in all conditions. The SSRS provides a
Significant differences were reported in pre- and posttest broad measure of students’ social competencies. Teacher and
measures on a number of essential social skills including student forms were used in this study. The SSRS provides a
“cooperation,” “assertiveness,” and “self-control” among the measure of youngsters’ positive behaviors or proactive
students who participated in the Multicultural Guidance social skills. It also includes an assessment of potential prob-
Project (D’Andrea & Daniels, 1995; Omizo & D’Andrea, lem behaviors students may exhibit in their interactions with
1995). Teachers also rated participating students significantly others. National norms have been compiled on a diverse
lower on a measure of “total problem behaviors” (Gresham sample (multiracial, handicapped, and nonhandicapped male
& Elliott, 1990). However, because no control group was and female students consisting of more than 4,000 chil-
used for comparison in this study, causal inference was com- dren and adolescents; Gresham & Elliott, 1990). The
promised by the researchers’ inability to control for his- Teacher Form of the SSRS provides a total Social Skills
torical and maturation threats to internal validity. score and various subscale scores. Three of the subscale
scores measure students’ Cooperation, Assertiveness, and
EXTENDING THE EXISTING RESEARCH ON SCHOOL-BASED Self-Control. The Teacher Form also includes the follow-
ing subscales:
PREJUDICE REDUCTION INTERVENTIONS
The present study extends D’Andrea and Daniels’s (1995) 1. An Externalizing subscale, which measure a student’s
initial research in the area of school-based prejudice reduc- tendency to exhibit verbal or physically aggressive
tion interventions in four fundamental ways. First, a com- behaviors toward others
parison group of students was used to enhance the validity 2. An Internalizing subscale, which measures a student’s
of causal inference. Second, the same set of 10 classroom level of anxiety, sadness, and reluctance to interact
guidance activities was applied to a group of students who with others
had not been experiencing interpersonal problems that re- 3. A Hyperactivity subscale, which measures the degree
flected intercultural and interracial conflicts with their peers. to which a student is easily distracted when interact-
This study, then, was an effort at primary prevention. Third, ing with others
the present study added a self-esteem measure (Coopersmith,
The Student Form of the SSRS also provides a total So-
1981) to assess the effects of the intervention on student
cial Skills score but includes subscale ratings. Four subscale
self-perception as well as social skill development. Fourth,
scores measure students’ Cooperation, Assertiveness, Em-
the current study tested the effectiveness of the interven-
pathy, and Self-Control. These proactive social skills are
tion on fourth-grade students at a different school site in
thought to be vital for young people’s development in or-
Hawaii, thereby extending the external validity of the treat-
der that they might experience a sense of well-being in their
ment. Thus, the overall goal of this study was to replicate
interpersonal interactions (Selman & Byrne, 1974). These
and extend D’Andrea and Daniels’s (1995) initial research
skills are also considered to be especially important in cul-
by testing the effectiveness of a primary prevention inter-
turally and racially diverse school settings (D’Andrea &
vention designed to promote social skills and attitudinal char-
Daniels, 1995).
acteristics that are necessary to reduce conflict and increase
Test–retest reliability coefficients for the teacher’s version
the potential for positive interpersonal interactions in
of the SSRS were reported to be .85 for the Social Skills
multicultural and diverse school environments.
scales and .84 for the scales measuring Problem Behaviors.
The student forms had a test–retest reliability coefficient
METHOD of .68 for their social skills (Gresham & Elliott, 1990).
Participants Stephens (1978) compared the SSRS with the Social Behav-
ior Assessment Scale and reported correlation coefficients
Fifty (fourth-grade) students attending a public school in ranging from –.15 to –.73 with the Social Skill scales and
Hawaii participated in this study. Twenty-eight of these stu- .01 to .57 with the Problem Behaviors scales. The samples
dents were assigned to the treatment group and 22 students used to assess the reliability and validity of the SSRS

342 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Assessing the Impact of a Prejudice Prevention Project

consisted of a diverse group of students and teachers. These “I am . . .” on the bottom of a blank piece of paper. The
results suggest that the SSRS is a reasonably reliable and purpose of this activity was to provide positive feedback
valid way to measure positive and negative aspects of stu- regarding any cultural issues/statements that emerged when
dents’ social development and interpersonal competency. the students completed the “I am” statement.
Intercorrelations among the subscales of the Teacher Form Class 4. “Labeling”: The boys and girls in the class were
of the SSRS ranged from .46 to .64 on the Social Skills divided into two groups. The girls were arbitrarily identi-
measures and .20 to .70 on the Problem Behaviors subscales. fied as the “good” group in the class and the boys were re-
The subscales of the Students Form of the SSRS have ferred to as the “bad” group. The facilitators announced vari-
intercorrelations ranging from .49 to .65. ous privileges the “good” group would have from that point
The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (SEI; Coopersmith, on, whereas several punitive restrictions were assigned to
1981) is a self-report measure consisting of 58 items. The items members of the “bad” group. Following these announce-
assess self-attitudes in four domains: Social, Home, School, ments, the students were asked to discuss their reactions to
and General. A total self-esteem score is derived by summing being in either group. The purpose of this session was to
the high self-esteem responses on the four subscales and dou- introduce the concepts of “prejudice” and “stereotyping” to
bling the score. The SEI is considered suitable for use within the youngsters and to explore the various ways people feel
the 8- to 15-year-old age range. The reliability of the SEI has when they are the victims of cultural, ethnic, or racial preju-
been tested and produced coefficients greater than .80 (Spatz dice and stereotyping.
& Johnson, 1973). Concurrent validity of the Coopersmith Class 5. Videotaping Activity: While one of the facilitators
SEI was assessed by correlating the SEI and the SRA Achieve- videotaped this session, the other one summarized the last
ment Series, and the SEI’s predictive validity was supported class meeting and continued to process students’ reactions.
by the finding that it predicted reading achievement (Post & The facilitator then helped the youngsters explore various
Robinson, 1998). Intercorrelations among the subscales of the ways that elementary school students demonstrate negative
SEI range from .29 to .52. prejudices and stereotyping when interacting with people
from diverse backgrounds. After exploring these issues for
Procedure about 20 minutes, the facilitators asked the students to view
the video and to comment on their own interactions with
The SSRS and the SEI were administered to two fourth-
the facilitators and other students during that session.
grade classes in an Oahu, Hawaii, public school by an advanced
Class 6. Multicultural People Bingo: The students were
graduate student doing her school counseling internship at
provided a “Multicultural Bingo Sheet” that included boxes
the school. One of these classes served as the control group
containing various cultural-specific statements (i.e., “Enjoys
and the other as the treatment group. The student then
eating sushi,” “Has tried adobo,” “Is Samoan or part Samoan,”
conducted the guidance activities with the treatment group
etc.). They were instructed to move around the room and
under the supervision of the two authors. Each guidance class
find individuals who could “sign off” in one of the boxes on
lasted 40 minutes. The classes were held once a week for 10
the sheet. To “sign off,” students had to possess the quality
consecutive weeks. The instruments were readministered at
or characteristic that was listed in one of the boxes on the
the conclusion of the treatment among students in both the
bingo sheet. After the students had all of their boxes “signed
control and the experimental groups. What follows is a brief
off” by other students, the facilitators led the class in a dis-
description of the guidance activities that were used to pre-
cussion about the positive aspects of cultural diversity.
vent the development of various cultural, racial, and ethnic
Class 7. Abstract Concept Activity: The students were pre-
prejudices by fostering positive social skills and self-esteem
sented with several abstract concepts like “love” and “fairness”
among students in the experimental group.
and encouraged to discuss the different meanings these words
The Multicultural Guidance Activities might have for people from culturally and racially diverse back-
grounds. They were then asked to draw pictures of what these
Class 1. During the initial session, the group facilitators words looked like in their class and at their school. The stu-
administered the SSRS and the SEI to the students in their dents were asked to show their pictures to the rest of the class
homeroom classes. The facilitators also discussed the rules and explain what they represented.
of the group meetings, the goals of the project, and their Class 8. Rainbow Poster Activity: The facilitators led the
expectations of the students. students in a discussion about “rainbows.” Next, they were
Class 2. Name Tag Activity: After having the students write asked to paint a large rainbow on poster board paper using
their names, favorite foods, songs and music, places they like the following colors: brown, black, white, red, and yellow.
to visit, and most enjoyable holidays on a blank piece of pa- The children were then asked to paint a picture of them-
per, the facilitators led a class discussion that focused on the selves next to the color that represented their own racial
youngsters’ responses to this exercise. The leaders empha- background. As the students were involved in this activity,
sized any cultural differences that were associated with the the facilitators walked around the room asking individual
responses the students gave to these issues. students to talk briefly about their racial background. The
Class 3. The “I am” Activity: The students were asked to facilitators provided positive feedback on various comments
draw a picture of themselves and to finish the statement the students made about their different backgrounds.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 343


Salzman and D’Andrea

Class 9. The Hands Activity: After being arranged in small TABLE 1


groups, the students were asked to place their hands in the
middle of their desks and to look at their neighbor’s hands. Adjusteda Means for Subscales of SSRS
While asking them to keep their hands in the middle of their (Teacher Form)
desks, the facilitators led a group discussion that addressed the Group
following questions: “What do you notice about your own
Measure n Treatment n Control
and your neighbor’s hands?” “Are they all the same color/shape/
size?” and “Do you think any hand is better than another?” Assertiveness 25 11.45 21 11.07
Class 10. Summary and Posttesting: The facilitators asked Cooperation 25 14.81 21 12.38
the students if they could recall all the activities that they Externalizing 25 0.92 21 0.37
had done together during the past several weeks. They were Hyperactivity 25 1.85 27 2.34
Internalizing 25 1.73 21 2.11
also asked to discuss one thing they learned that they could Self-Control 25 14.73 21 12.07
use when working or playing with other students in the Social Skills 25 40.94 21 35.53
class. Finally, the SSRS and the SEI were readministered to
Note. SSRS = Social Skills Rating System.
students in both the control and the treatment groups at a
Adjusted for pretest scores.
the end of this session.

Data Analysis
nalizing, F(1, 36) = .86, p = 360; Self-Control, F(1, 36) =
A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was 21.46, p = .000; and Social Skills, F(1, 36) = 12.88, p = .001.
used to assess if significant differences existed between The results of the MANCOVA on the Students Form of
the posttest scores on the dependent measures that were the SSRS did not yield significant results (Wilks’s lambda =
generated by the students in the control and experimen- .79. Exact F(5, 32) = 1.71, p = .16. The power at the .05-
tal groups after completing the SSRS and the SEI. This level was calculated at .51. Adjusted means for the com-
procedure was deemed necessary due to the multiple depen- parison groups on the subscales are reported in Table 2.
dent measures used and their modest-to-moderate Although the Wilks’s lambda (p = .16) did not reach the
intercorrelations (e.g., SSRS subscale intercorrelations level of significance, it is important to note the relatively
ranging from .20 to .70). The pretest scores were treated substantial differences between the adjusted means on the
as covariates thereby equalizing the pretest means and control- Cooperation subscale scores. In view of these differences and
ling for the pretest scores. Means were calculated after the relatively modest power of the test, univariate F tests
adjusting for pretest scores and then inspected. One-way were done. Although it did not reach the .05 level for signifi-
analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs; univariate F tests) were cance, the Cooperation subscale F-test score (p = .059) did
then performed, and the results were interpreted in consid- approach this level of statistical significance. This is note-
eration of intercorrelations and overlapping variances among worthy given the relatively substantial differences between
subscales. An alpha level of .05 was established to deter- the adjusted posttest means on the Cooperation subscale
mine whether significant differences existed between the scores that are noted earlier. The results of the univariate
treatment and control group scores. The F-test scores reported tests for each of the variables on the Student Form of the
in the following tables therefore represent a comparison SSRS are as follows: Assertiveness, F(1, 36) = 0.63, p = .434;
between the treatment and the control groups’ posttest Cooperation, F(1, 36) = 3.80, p = .059; Empathy, F(1, 36) =
scores on the SSRS and SEI. .06, p = .807; Self-Control, F(1, 36) = .41, p = .528; and
Social Skills, F(1, 36) = .24, p = .628.
RESULTS
The results of the MANCOVA on the Teacher Form of the
SSRS indicated a significant omnibus effect, Wilks’s lambda TABLE 2
= 4.64, Exact F(7, 30) = 4.65, p = .001. The observed power a
Adjusted Means for Subscales of SSRS
at the .05 level was .99. Adjusted posttest means for the (Student Form)
comparison groups on the subscales are reported in Table
1. Subsequent univariate F tests on the Teacher Form of Group
the SRSS indicated significant differences between the Measure n Treatment n Control
teachers’ scores on the Cooperation, Self-Control, Social
Skills, and Externalizing SSRS subscales for the students Assertiveness 26 12.00 19 13.11
Cooperation 26 14.58 19 12.90
who were assigned to the treatment and control groups. Empathy 26 14.56 19 14.79
The results of the univariate tests for each of the variables Self-Control 26 11.86 19 11.24
are as follows: Assertiveness, F(1, 36) = 0.68, p = .417; Co- Social Skills 25 53.08 19 51.93
operation, F(1, 36) = 8.50, p = .006; Externalizing, F(1, 36) Note. See Table 1 Note.
= 6.63, p = .014; Hyperactivity, F(1, 36) 1.72, p = .198; Inter- a
Adjusted for pretest scores.

344 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Assessing the Impact of a Prejudice Prevention Project

The MANCOVA for the Coopersmith SEI also did not yield from diverse cultural-racial backgrounds are able to achieve
a significant omnibus effect, Wilks’s lambda = .70, Exact F(6, when they interact with one another. Given the rapid cul-
27) = 1.93, p = .11. Adjusted means were calculated and are tural transformation of the United States and the degree to
reported in Table 3. Follow-up univariate analyses for each of which cultural prejudices and racial tensions continue to
the variables indicated one significant difference at p < .02 exist in our society, it is important from a preventive per-
between the self-esteem scores that were generated by the stu- spective that school counselors think of ways to intention-
dents in the treatment group in comparison with the students ally and effectively promote the interpersonal solidarity that
in the control group. Specifically, this involved a significantly Allport described in his work among students from diverse
lower Social Self-Esteem subscale score among the students in groups and backgrounds. In part, this can be accomplished
the treatment group. The results are as follows: General, by implementing school-based interventions that effectively
F(1, 33) = .77, p = .388; Home, F(1, 33) .77, p = .399; School, foster the development of cooperative social skills, much
F(1, 33) = .04, p = .843; Lie (F = .94, p = .339); and Social like those of the prejudice prevention guidance classes de-
(F = 7.27, p = .011). scribed in this article.
It should be further noted that significant negative find-
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL COUNSELORS ings were observed on the teachers’ ratings of the students
in the control and treatment groups on the Externalizing
The posttest scores on the SSRS indicated that teachers subscale. Initially, these findings seemed to represent a con-
observed significant improvement in the students’ coop- tradiction in the other positive findings that were reflected
erative social skills as a result of participating in the in the treatment group’s scores. However, a close scrutiny of
multicultural guidance activities that were designed by Table 1 shows that the adjusted mean posttest scores for
Omizo and D’Andrea (1995). The students’ ratings on the both the treatment and the control groups on the External-
Cooperation subscale, although not quite achieving signifi- izing subscale are less than 1 (i.e., control group, M = .37;
cance at the .05 level did yield a p = .059. In fact the F treatment group, M = .92). This means that the teachers
value on this subscale seems dramatically higher than the reported very few times in which the students in both the
other F values reported on the Students Form of the SSRS. treatment and the control groups manifested negative
The fact that both the teachers and the students ratings seemed “externalizing” behaviors at the time posttesting was done.
to converge on this essential dimension is worth noting. Gains However, this finding was difficult to interpret and war-
in this social skill enhances the validity of the inference that rants further study.
this essential intercultural skill was fostered by the multicultural One of the common problems that school counselors en-
guidance intervention. These results also provided the research- counter when working with students of all ages involves
ers with converging evidence that led to the conclusion that difficulties that occur when youngsters fail to demonstrate
the students in the treatment group benefited from partici- self-control and instead act in an impulsive manner in in-
pating in the prejudice reduction intervention as demonstrated terpersonal situations in which conflicts or differences are
by their improved cooperation scores. apparent. Given the frequency with which interpersonal
The importance of the converging findings on the Coop- conflicts occur within schools that comprised students from
eration subscale scores is particularly impressive because diverse cultural-racial backgrounds (D’Andrea & Daniels,
of the centrality that this interpersonal factor has been given 1996), the significant difference reported between the teach-
in prejudice reduction theory. In his seminal work on preju- ers’ posttest Self-Control subscale scores may be particularly
dice, Allport (1982) explained that the development of interesting and relevant for the challenges school counselors
cooperative skills and attitudes not only helps prevent the are likely to experience when working within the context of a
promotion of inaccurate and prejudicial views toward oth- culturally diverse school setting. Recognizing that (a) this
ers, but it also underlies the sense of solidarity that people subscale provides a measure of students’ ability to “control
one’s temper in conflict situations with peers” (Gresham &
Elliott, 1990, p. 3) and (b) noting that the students in the
TABLE 3
treatment group received higher ratings in this area by their
Adjusteda Means for Coopersmith Self-Esteem teachers than the control group youngsters, counselors are en-
Inventory couraged to consider using Omizo and D’Andrea’s (1995)
guidance lessons as a way both to prevent prejudice and to
Group promote greater self-control among students.
Measure n Treatment n Control Another interesting finding involved the significant differ-
ence that was observed on the students’ Social Self-Esteem
General 25 17.04 19 16.38 subscale scores. As reported, the students in the treatment
Home 25 5.32 19 4.77
School 25 5.00 19 5.21 group scored significantly lower (result of F test) on social
Social 25 5.00 19 6.28 self-esteem than their control group peers did. Rather than
Lie 25 2.59 19 3.11 interpreting this finding in negative terms (i.e., that young-
Total Self-Esteem 25 64.86 19 64.81
sters have a reduced sense of social self-esteem as a result
a
Adjusted for pretest scores. of participating in this intervention), this finding may be

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 345


Salzman and D’Andrea

interpreted as potentially positive. That is, the lower Social CONCLUSION


Self-Esteem subscale scores that were reported by the stu-
dents in the treatment group may correlate with an emerg- In conclusion, counselors can make a difference in the con-
ing sense of cultural relativism and dissonance that the stu- struction of a more respectful, harmonious, and enriching
dents might have experienced as a result of participating in multicultural environment if they intentionally choose to
the prejudice prevention intervention. do so. By not doing so they run the risk of having students
Like many adults, many students possess ethnocentric ways maintain various types of ethnocentric biases that contrib-
of looking at themselves, others, and the world in which they ute to the perpetuation of divisive cultural and racial preju-
live. This sort of ethnocentric thinking is likely to help fuel dices. All counselors should work to infuse a sense of re-
positive feelings individuals have about themselves and other spect and appreciation for human diversity into the
culturally and racially similar people as well as to contribute worldviews of young people, thereby making tolerance more
to the negative attitudes individuals hold toward people who likely in times of stress and heightened anxiety.
come from different cultural-ethnic groups. Such an ethno- The potentials inherent in the increasing diversity of com-
centric perspective, if left unchallenged, represents fertile munities and schools across the United States range from
terrain for the development of various types of cultural and horrible conflict to a genuine appreciation of the richness
racial prejudices during one’s childhood and adolescence. and vitality of a multicultural society. Counselors can choose
When thinking about the complex psychological factors to nourish the latter awareness and make its behavioral
that contribute to prejudice prevention, it may be impor- manifestations more probable if we act intentionally to
tant for school counselors to implement strategies that are develop the positive potentials that exist in our increas-
intentionally designed to disrupt students’ ethnocentric con- ingly diverse society and interdependent world. The
struction of self-esteem. This can be accomplished, in part, Multicultural Guidance Project is one such intentional act.
by providing youngsters with alternative cultural construc-
tions of reality and human “being-ness.” In doing so, a self- REFERENCES
esteem structure that may have been built on ethnocentric
notions of superiority and inferiority may well give way to a Allport, G. (1982). The nature of prejudice (4th ed.). Reading, MA: Adios-
Wesley.
more enduring and constructive sense of personal value. We Conyne, R. K. (1987). Primary preventative counseling: Empowering people
also suggest that this would help promote a sense of per- and systems. Muncie, IN: Accelerated Development.
sonal value that is derived from a profound sense of equality Coopersmith, S. (1981). Self-esteem inventories. Palo Alto, CA: Consult-
and belonging to the diverse human community. This con- ing Psychology Press.
struction of self-esteem offers a more enduring and less de- D’Andrea, M., & Daniels, J. (1995). Helping students learn to get along:
Assessing the effectiveness of a multicultural guidance project. El-
structive foundation for the achievement of this necessary ementary School Guidance & Counseling, 30,143–154.
psychological resource. Such a sense of belonging and equal- D’Andrea, M., & Daniels, J. (1996). Promoting peace in our schools:
ity was concretely manifested by the students who took part Developmental, preventive, and multicultural considerations. The
in the prejudice prevention project that is described in this School Counselor, 44, 55–64.
article. This was particularly evident when the youngsters Gazda, G., Ginter, E., & Horne, A. (2001). Group counseling and group
psychotherapy. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
worked together on the Rainbow Poster Activity (Class 8) Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror manage-
and when discussing the Hands Activity (Class 9). ment theory of self-esteem and cultural world views: Empirical as-
sessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Ad-
vances in experimental social psychology (pp. 61–139). San Diego, CA:
LIMITATIONS Academic Press.
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). The Social Skills Rating System
The limitations of this research include the relatively small (SSRS). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
sample size that inhibits the external validity of the study. Omizo, M. M., & D’Andrea, M. (1995). Multicultural classroom guidance.
In addition, although covariance analysis may, in a limited In C. C. Lee (Ed.), Counseling for diversity: A guide for school counselors
sense, adjust for a failure of random assignment to groups and related professionals (pp. 143–158). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Ponterotto, J. G., & Pedersen, P. (1993). Preventing prejudice: A guide for
within the design, it does not adjust for a failure to sample
counselors and educators. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
from portions of the population to which one wishes to Post, P., & Robinson, B. E. (1998). School-age children of alcoholics and
generalize. The negative finding on the Externalizing non-alcoholics: Their anxiety, self-esteem, and locus of control. Profes-
subscale of the Teacher Form of the SSRS is somewhat sional School Counseling, 5, 36–45.
puzzling and warrants further study. Salzman, M. (1995). Attributional discrepancies and biases in cross-
cultural interactions. The Journal of Multicultural Counseling and
Another limitation in this study relates to the possible Development, 23, 181–193.
confounding effect that attention may have played in this Selman, R. L., & Byrne, D. A. (1974). A structural-developmental
study. In this regard, the researchers acknowledge that the analysis of role-taking in middle childhood. Child Development, 45,
attention that was given to the students in the treatment 803–806.
group as a result of participating in the multicultural guid- Spatz, K., & Johnson, J. (1973). Internal consistency of the Coopersmith
Self-Esteem Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 33,
ance activities in comparison with the absence of similar 875–876.
attention directed to the control group students may have Stephens, T. (1978). The Social Behavior Assessment Scale (SBA). Circle
affected their test scores. Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

346 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Training AIDS and Anger Prevention Social Skills in
At-Risk Adolescents

Melbourne F. Hovell, Elaine J. Blumberg, Sandy Liles, Linda Powell, Theodore C. Morrison,
Gabriela Duran, Carol L. Sipan, Susan Burkham, and Norma Kelley

Youth in alternative schools often engage in high rates of risk behaviors for AIDS and violence. This study included 42 youth, ages
13 to 18, from alternative schools in San Diego, CA, and tested the effectiveness of behavioral skills training based on the
Behavioral-Ecological Model. Two interventions were evaluated: one teaching condom use skills and the other teaching anger
management skills. Changes in most skills were significant at postintervention but were not maintained at 6 months. Few risk-
related attitudes or behaviors improved at 6 months for either group.

A
mong adolescents, violence (Centers for Dis- Certain higher risk groups of adolescents are more likely
ease Control [CDC], 1992a, 1993, 1997; Na- to demonstrate risk behaviors for HIV infection and vio-
tional Institutes of Health [NIH], 1997; Se- lence than the general population of youth. The propor-
lect Committee on Children, Youth, and tions of youth engaging in AIDS risk or violent behaviors
Families, 1992) and sexually transmitted are higher among youth in detention (Shafer et al., 1993),
diseases (STDs), including AIDS (Bureau of the Census, those involved with the criminal justice system (Melchert
1993; CDC, 1996; Children’s Defense Fund, 1994; & Burnett, 1990), out-of-school youth (CDC, 1994a), and
Fingerhut & Kleinman, 1990; National Center for Health youth in alternative school programs (Bjerregaard & Smith,
Statistics, 1993; Whitaker & Bastian, 1991), are major causes 1993; CDC, 1994b; Grunbaum & Basen-Engquist, 1993).
of morbidity and mortality having behavioral determinants. In 1994, 5.3% of high school students enrolled the previ-
A significant proportion of adolescents engage in risk ous year dropped out (National Center for Educational
behaviors that can lead to STD infection (CDC, 1992b, Statistics, 1997); over 2 million youth under the age of 18
1992c) or violent injury (Valois, McKeown, Garrison, & were arrested in 1995 (Federal Bureau of Investigation,
Murray, 1995). These behaviors increase the opportunities 1996); and over 100,000 youth resided in juvenile institu-
for transmission of disease or for physical injury and in- tions in 1990 (Bureau of the Census, 1994). Youth in alter-
clude multiple sexual partners, non-use of condoms, alco- native school settings represent a sizable portion of the
hol/substance use or abuse, fights, weapon carrying, and adolescent population. Nonetheless, few studies of risk re-
gang membership. According to the 1995 Youth Risk Be- duction have been conducted among adolescents attending
havior Survey, more than half (53.1%) of students in Grades alternative schools (Grunbaum & Basen-Engquist, 1993)
9–12 have had sexual intercourse, and 17.8% reported or those outside the mainstream school system (O’Hara,
sexual intercourse with four or more partners during their Messick, Fichtner, & Parris, 1996). Limited data exist on
lifetime (CDC, 1996). Of sexually active students, only adolescents in alternative school settings in terms of their
54.4% reported condom use at last intercourse (CDC, receptivity to or lack of training regarding AIDS and anger
1996). In the 30 days prior to the survey, 20.0% of students or violence prevention. Given statistics on risk behaviors in
had carried a weapon (such as a gun, knife, or club); 7.6% these respective areas for these high-risk youth, it is par-
had carried a gun specifically. In the past year, 38.7% had ticularly important to target such groups for risk-behavior
been in a physical fight (CDC, 1996). reduction training.

Melbourne F. Hovell is a professor and director, Elaine J. Blumberg is a senior research associate, Sandy Liles is a research associate, Gabriela Duran is
a research assistant, Carol L. Sipan is a senior research associate, and Norma Kelley is a data coordinator, all in the Center for Behavioral Epidemiology and
Community Health, Graduate School of Public Health, San Diego State University, San Diego, California. Linda Powell is a graduate student in the
Department of Human Development and Family Life, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Theodore C. Morrison is a graduate student at the University of
Alabama at Birmingham, School of Public Health, Department of Health Behavior. Susan Burkham is a special projects director/epidemiologist at the Bureau
of Information Resources, Health Care Financing, Texas Department of Health in Austin. This research was supported by grants from the California State
Office of AIDS (93-18444) and the University -Wide Aids Research Program, University of California (R98-SDSU-092 and IS99-SDSUF-206). The authors
thank the students, teachers, and other participating personnel from the San Diego County Juvenile Court and Community Schools for their contributions to
this work. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Melbourne F. Hovell, Center for Behavioral Epidemiology and Community Health, Graduate
School of Public Health, San Diego State University, 9245 Sky Park Court, Suite 230, San Diego, CA 92123 (e-mail: hovell@mail.sdsu.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 347


Hovell, Blumberg, Liles, Powell, Morrison, Duran, Sipan, Burkham, and Kelley

The Behavioral-Ecological Model (Hovell et al., 1994) and Adolescents were recruited in the classroom after an oral
related community applications of social learning theory presentation detailing study requirements. All participants
(Biglan & Hayes, 1996; Biglan et al., 1990) support the use as well as a parent of each participant under the age of 18
of behavioral skills training techniques for interventions tar- reviewed and signed informed consent forms. More than 40
geting violence-risk and AIDS-risk behaviors. These models adolescents (N = 42) from two sequential cohorts volun-
integrate learning theories with an ecological approach to teered and received baseline interviews for the present study.
explain risk behaviors. The models assume a developmental All descriptive statistics and analyses were based on the
process of learning in which social competencies and ongo- consistent cohort of participants retained across the three
ing social influences, including reinforcement contingencies measurement periods (N = 29). The mean age of the sample
and other motivational variables, are responsible for routinely at pretest was 16.3 years; 28% were female. The majority
practiced behaviors. Although skills training studies have dem- of study participants identified themselves as ethnic mi-
onstrated significant increases in condom use (Main et al., 1994; norities: 31% were African American, 38% were Latino, and
O’Hara et al., 1996; Rotherum-Borus, Koopman, Haignere, & 21% were White. There was one (3%) American Indian,
Davies, 1991; Stanton et al., 1996; St. Lawrence et al., 1995) and 2 (7%) participants who did not classify themselves.
and significant decreases in aggressive behaviors among See Table 1 for additional demographic information and
children and adolescents (Grossman et al., 1997; Larson, 1992), baseline risk profiles.
the degree to which social skills are sufficient to alter risk prac-
tices remains unclear. Theoretically, one would assume that Procedure
social contingencies, including peer models (norms), influence
Design. This investigation implemented two interventions that
risk practices directly as well as through skills acquisition
(Catania, 1998; Hovell et al., 1994). served as alternate controls for one another. Kellam and Rebox
Comparative studies of approaches to aggressive behavior (1992) have noted the advantage of testing two interventions
in a parallel design: “Such a dual intervention strategy allows
have found that interventions targeting internal processes, such
direction of effects among the variables to be tested along with
as cognitions, have been less effective than those targeting
the specificity of effects of the interventions” (p. 172). How-
observable behaviors (e.g., Gudjonsson & Drinkwater, 1986;
ever, because it was not possible to randomly assign students to
Schneider, 1991). A recent meta-analysis also demonstrated
the relative success of social learning theory-based behavioral condition, this analysis remains quasi-experimental. Following
interventions among diverse populations for HIV risk reduc- advice reported by Slymen and Hovell (1997) for cluster de-
signs and small samples, the unit of analysis was the individual.
tion (Kalichman, Carey, & Johnson, 1996).
Intervention. Trainings were simultaneously conducted for
The present study tests the effectiveness of behavioral
the two conditions in sequential cohorts. Each condition
skills training methods in two separate, parallel interven-
tions—one teaching skills in condom use and negotiation TABLE 1
related to AIDS risk reduction, the other teaching anger
management skills related to violence risk reduction—in Characteristics and Risks of Sample (N = 29)
youth placed in an alternative school by either the courts
or the mainstream school system. The purpose of this in- Measure %
vestigation was to determine whether skills training, with- Participant’s family receiving government assistance 37
out formal contingencies directed to risk practices, increased Participant’s family renting home 72
Anger/Aggression and HIV prevention skills, whether ac- Sexually active (vaginal or anal sex), past 6 months 83
Lifetime history of 4 or more vaginal sexual partners 68
quired skills were sustained at the 6-month follow-up, and Never used a condom during vaginal sex, past 6 monthsa 25
whether changes in AIDS or violence-related risk behav- Lifetime history of having had an STD 14
iors occurred. This feasibility study sets the stage for larger Knows someone with AIDS or who died from AIDS 21
scale studies of skills training as well as informing the need Alcohol use, past 6 months 79
Marijuana use, past 6 months 66
for skills plus contingency interventions. Cocaine use, past 6 months 14
Intravenous drug use, past 6 months 0
Sex under the influence of alcohol, past 6 monthsa 42
METHOD Currently a gang member 31
Participants Hurt someone, past 6 months
Using a gun 7
Adolescents ages 13 to 18 were recruited from four Juve- Using a knife 17
nile Court and Community School (JCCS) sites in San Using a stick or bottle 38
Using one’s body 52
Diego County. Students who attend these sites live with Lifetime history of one or more arrests 76
their parents, in foster care, or in group homes and attend Lifetime history of one or more convictions 59
school during the day. Educational programs are designed Currently on probation 50
to meet the needs of students who are detained due to court Note. Descriptive statistics are given for the consistent cohort of youth
action, either as a delinquent or neglected ward of the court, who were retained for all measures.
or placed by local school districts through the probation a
Percentage based on the sexually active subsample (participants who
department (Juvenile Court & Community Schools, 1995). reported having vaginal or anal sex in the past 6 months, n = 24).

348 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Tr a i n i n g A I D S a n d A n g e r P r e v e n t i o n S o c i a l S k i l l s

received the intervention program at widely separated JCCS and one anger management skill (Resisting Taunts). Role
school sites, minimizing the chance of cross-contamination. play testing occurred at pretest, posttraining, and at 6-month
The goal of the HIV prevention intervention was to follow-up. The Condom Negotiation (CN) vignette assessed
increase competencies in four skills: condom negotiation, the ability to request condom use and the ability to refuse
discussing a friend’s risk of AIDS, condom application sexual intercourse if a partner refused to use condoms.
technique, and teaching AIDS risk reduction skills to a friend.
The goal of the Anger Management intervention was to The Condom Negotiation Vignette: You and__________are at a
increase competencies in four skills: negotiating an agree- party. You both like each other and have been hanging out for a few
weeks. While at the party, you and________start making out. You
ment, resisting taunts, dealing with authority figures, and both want to have sex. You go off into one of the bedrooms. You
teaching anger management skills to a friend. Skills were want to use a condom, but_________does not. Show what you
chosen based on interventions previously used with nonde- would do.
linquent Latino and Anglo adolescents (Blumberg et al., 1997;
Hovell et al., 1998). For anger management, the Resisting Taunts (RT) vignette
Both groups were trained using the same 10-step behav- assessed the ability to resist taunts from an “authority fig-
ioral rehearsal training approach: ure” (confederate) while being incited to anger by a “friend”
(confederate), as well as the ability to exit the situation
1. Presentation of skill and rationale without escalation.
2. Explicit modeling by teacher and peer trainers
3. Directed role play practice in small groups The Resisting Taunts Vignette: You and your friend_________have
4. Instructor feedback just walked into a store and are looking around to buy something.
5. New modeling with corrections The storekeeper, ____________, the brother or sister of a rival gang
6. Repeated role play with corrections member, keeps on looking at you and then comes up to you and
asks what you are doing in the store. Show what you would do.
7. Pairs demonstration of skills in front of class
8. Instructor, peer trainer, and class feedback
Measurement role plays were videotaped at the JCCS
9. Repeated role play with corrections
sites in rooms separate from the classroom. Each measure
10. Group discussion was approximately 15 minutes in length. Role plays were
Graduate students and peer trainers who had received 8 hours performed with an opposite-sex confederate for CN. RT
of training conducted twelve 1½- to 2-hour training sessions role plays were performed with two confederates, a same-
over a 4-week period for each condition. Training for the sex confederate as the store manager and an opposite-sex
two conditions differed in content only (see Table 2). confederate as a “friend.”
During Sessions 2 through 6, a given skill was introduced, The setting for CN was two chairs side by side. For RT a
discussed, and divided into specific objectives. The trainer simulated store counter was placed between the store man-
or peer trainer, or both, modeled incorrect (passive or ag- ager confederate and the participant with his or her “friend.”
gressive) and correct (assertive) forms of a given skill, and A research assistant gave instructions before each role play.
guided students to practice skills during structured role play Each measurement session began with a practice role play
exercises. Each participant role-played each skill repeatedly intended to put the participant at ease. CN was measured
with another student or the peer trainer serving as the per- first, followed by RT, and concluding with another nonsen-
son who pressured the participant. During role plays, each sitive role play to diffuse aggressive feelings potentially
incorrect response by the participant was given immediate raised by the RT role play.
verbal feedback and the correct response was modeled by Confederates were undergraduate and graduate students
the trainer. Role-playing continued with each participant selected for their youthful appearance so that the measure-
until the trainer judged the performance satisfactory. In the ment scenario more closely resembled a real-life encounter
remaining sessions, time was set aside for continued role with participants’ peers. Confederates were provided 20
play practice. Participants were encouraged to create sce- hours of training. In CN, they were trained to resist con-
narios with increasing degrees of difficulty. In Session 10, dom use for five interchanges and to “give in” on the sixth
they were encouraged to design their own risk reduction interchange. In RT, the storekeeper confederate was trained
skill and demonstrate teaching the skill to peers. In addi- to taunt the teen to anger within seven interchanges. The
tion to verbal feedback and praise, raffle tickets for a weekly confederate acting as the friend was trained to further pro-
prize of a $25 gift certificate were awarded for each ob- voke the teen when the storekeeper’s taunts proved unsuc-
served correct response or behavior. At the end of the in- cessful. For example, the confederate might say, “C’mon
tervention, tickets from each weekly raffle were combined Mary, are you gonna let him talk to you like that?”
for a grand prize drawing of a $100 gift certificate. Coding. The videotape of each role play was consecu-
tively evaluated for Verbal Content, Nonverbal Behavior,
Variables and overall levels of Aggression (RT skill only), Assertiveness,
Skills measures. Videotaped role plays were used to assess and Anxiety. Coders reviewed videotapes repeatedly to score
skills. Due to cost constraints, participants were measured the presence of each of these components of HIV risk re-
for only one HIV prevention skill (Condom Negotiation) duction and anger resistance skills, respectively. Coders were

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 349


Hovell, Blumberg, Liles, Powell, Morrison, Duran, Sipan, Burkham, and Kelley

TABLE 2
Summary of Training Sessions

Session AIDS Risk Reduction Anger Reduction


1 Introduction; ground rules; focus group; role play process Introduction; ground rules; focus group; role play process;
Video: Kids Killing Kids
2 Opening discussion; passive, aggressive, and assertive Opening discussion; passive, aggressive, and assertive
behaviors; condom negotiation skill behaviors; negotiating an agreement skill
3 Opening discussion; continued condom negotiation skill; correct Opening discussion; relaxation techniques; resisting taunts
condom use technique skill
4 Opening discussion; introduce discussing a friend’s risk of AIDS skill Opening discussion; continued resisting taunts skill
5 Opening discussion (focus on peer norms and skills Opening discussion; Introduce dealing with authority
dissemination); introduce teaching condom application figures
technique
6 Opening discussion; introduce teaching AIDS risk reduction skills Opening discussion; introduce teaching anger management
to a friend skills to a friend; discussion of importance of skill
dissemination
7 Opening discussion; how to get friends to think in terms of long-term Opening discussion; understand the effects of violence on
consequences; continued role play practice; film: Teen AIDS in others; continued role play practice
Focus; discussion
8 Opening discussion; peer training skills; discussion of long-term Opening discussion; continued role play practice; discussion
consequences of sexual behavior (teen pregnancy); guest speakers: of long-term consequences of behavior on others; interview
teen mom & dad with a panel of “victims” (Bureau of Victims)
9 Opening discussion; continued role play practice; Long-term Opening discussion; continued role play practice; discussion
consequences of sexual behavior: AIDS; guest speaker: person of long-term consequences of violent behavior (jails,
living with AIDS institutions, and death or dismemberment) and “How to
Change”; interview with former gang member
10 Opening discussion; continued role play practice; view and critique Opening discussion; continued role play practice; discussion
government ad campaign promoting safer sex; discussion: “How to of long-term consequences of violent behavior; interview
Get the Message Across”; participants design their own risk with ex-offenders; participants design their own risk
reduction skill and demonstrate teaching reduction skill and demonstrate teaching
11 Opening discussion; continued role plays; STD film: A Million Teens Opening discussion; continued role play practice; discuss
behavioral expectations of employers; guest speaker: Job
Corp.
12 Values clarification exercise; presentation of teen-developed skills; Continued role play practice; closing focus group and
evaluation of training evaluation

Note. Bold text indicates skills that were taught in various sessions.

blind to condition. Components of verbal behavior devel- Aggression, Assertiveness, and Anxiety, coders judged the
oped for the CN skill were distinct from those developed degree to which each characteristic represented low to high
for the RT skill. In CN, coding for verbal behavior was Aggression, Assertiveness, and Anxiety, using a 1-to-5 scale
broken down into measures for Content and Style. The in increments of 0.5.
Content score assessed levels of negotiating skill using a Reliability. The videotapes for each participant were coded
10-point scale ranging from 0 (giving in to sex without a independently by two coders. A criterion-referenced stan-
condom) to 9 (both requesting condom use and strongly say- dard of 80% agreement was used. Reliability between cod-
ing no to sex without a condom). Style score assessed level ers for Verbal and Nonverbal Behaviors was determined for
of social communication skill using a 10-point scale rang- each measure by calculating a point-to-point percent agree-
ing from 0 (not admitting to responsibility) to 9 (both taking ment. Reliability of the three overall measures for the RT
responsibility and acknowledging a partner’s feelings). In RT, skill—Aggression, Assertiveness, and Anxiety—were deter-
codings for verbal behavior were broken down into measures mined by calculating percentage agreement for the three
for appropriate and inappropriate responses. The sum of scales combined; reliability for the two overall measures
positive attributes in the vignette tallied by the coder comprised for the CN skill—Assertiveness and Anxiety—were deter-
the score for positive responses, such as attempting to diffuse mined by calculating percentage agreement for the two
the situation, remaining assertive in the situation, or asser- scales combined. For overall measures, agreements were
tively stating one’s rights as a customer. The score for nega- defined as ratings within + .5 between coders. All coding
tive responses was the sum of negative attributes tallied by that failed to reach 80% agreement was recoded by one of
the coder, such as interrupting, insulting, cussing, or intimi- the original coders and a third coder independently but
dating the storekeeper confederate. For Nonverbal Behavior, simultaneously to avoid any variation in timing of video-
coders alternated 15-second intervals of observing the tape viewing. Despite the 80% criterion, final reliability
participant’s behavior with 15 seconds of coding occurrence coefficients always exceeded this standard. In all cases,
or nonoccurrence of 15-component Nonverbal Behaviors, mean reliability percentages were greater than 90, and in
such as eye contact and body posture. For overall levels of several instances reached 100%.

350 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Tr a i n i n g A I D S a n d A n g e r P r e v e n t i o n S o c i a l S k i l l s

Interview measures. Participants were administered a ques- tion out of San Diego County, running away from home,
tionnaire at pretest and at 6-month follow-up, conducted and evasion of authorities (i.e., hiding to elude warrant for
by trained interviewers in private settings at JCCS sites. arrest). In addition, some youth elected not to complete
For those students no longer attending JCCS at the time of selected skill measures. As a result, skill and interview data
the 6-month follow-up, the interview was conducted at the were obtained for a consistent cohort of 29 participants
research offices. Each interview took approximately 45 (69% of original sample) across the three measurement
minutes and assessed demographics, drug use history, sexual periods. All descriptive and analytical statistics were based
history (including history of condom use and history of sex on this sample (N = 29).
under the influence of alcohol/drugs), attitudes and knowl-
edge pertaining to AIDS, and other variables related to be- Preliminary Analyses
havioral risk for HIV infection. The instrument also assessed
types of situations and people that elicit anger responses; A series of t tests on demographic and baseline risk factors
type and frequency of participants’ responses to those situ- were used to compare the 13 excluded teens who failed to
ations; type of responses to various authority figures (e.g., complete all parts of the study and the 29 teens retained
police officers); peer influences on prosocial and antisocial for all measures. Significant differences were found for four
behaviors; frequency of verbally or physically aggressive behaviors during the past 6 months: times used marijuana,
behaviors; frequency of weapon use; and history of arrest, times friends encouraged you to fight, times damaged prop-
conviction, and incarceration. In the anger domain, several erty, and “Harm” (a composite measure of the frequency of
scaled variables were created. Questions about arrest, convic- physically injuring another person). The excluded teens
tion, and detention were combined into a Crime scale (α = were more likely to exhibit each of these behaviors. Thus
.68). Harm was a composite of seven questions of how often a the 29 participants on whom the descriptive and analytical
participant had used various sorts of weapons, ranging from statistics were based may represent a less extreme risk pro-
hands to guns, to hurt another person (α = .64). An Argue scale file than is typical of the JCCS population.
summed four items assessing instances of various sorts of pro- Risk conditions. Nonetheless, large proportions of teens
vocative speech, such as cussing and yelling (α = .76). Actout in the sample reported at-risk behavior on a number of mea-
comprised eight measures of verbally or physically aggressive sures. Sixty-eight percent had had at least four vaginal sex
behaviors, such as shoves, slaps, or threats of injury (α = .66). partners. Seventy-nine percent had used alcohol in the past
6 months. Seventy-six percent had been arrested. Other
indicators of risk are given in Table 1.
RESULTS Group comparability. The AIDS (n = 14) and anger (n =
All data were analyzed on a Pentium-90 PC using SPSS for 15) groups were compared at pretest to assess potential
Windows 95, Version 6.1.3 (SPSS, 1995). One-way differences on demographics, skill levels, AIDS-related risk
ANOVAs or t tests (for interval level variables) and chi- behaviors and attitudes, and violence-related risk behaviors
square tests or Fisher’s exact tests (for dichotomous vari- and attitudes. There was only one significant difference at
ables) were used to test for differences on pretest variables pretest: 21% of the AIDS group and 57% of the anger group
by retention status, experimental condition, gender, and reported a history of HIV antibody testing (Fisher’s exact
ethnicity. All tests were two-tailed. test, two-tailed, p = .02).
For the skills measures, separate 2 × 2 (group-by-time)
Main Analyses
repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were
conducted for the pretest to posttest and the posttest to Role play skill change—repeated measures MANOVAs. For
follow-up periods. This was done to more clearly distin- both the AIDS prevention and the anger management in-
guish the effects due to training in the 4-week intervention terventions, separate Group × Time repeated measures
period (pretest to posttest) from the maintenance of ef- ANOVAs were used to analyze changes in skill over time
fects during the 6-month follow-up period (posttest to fol- within each of two time periods—pretest to posttest, and
low-up). As a check on possible influences of pretest group posttest to 6-month follow-up. Analyses were conducted
differences on the results in the pre-to-post period, analy- on five measures (Style, Content, Assertiveness, Anxiety,
ses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were run on the posttest and Nonverbal) of the CN skill and on six measures (Nega-
measures using the pretest scores as covariates. Finally, one- tive Responses, Positive Responses, Aggression,
way ANOVAs were conducted on the follow-up skills mea- Assertiveness, Anxiety, and Nonverbal) of the RT skill. To
sures to ascertain posttraining effects. rule out possible confounding due to differences at baseline,
Repeated measures analyses of variance, using 2 × 2 (group- ANCOVAs, using baseline scores as covariates, were used
by-time) ANOVAs, were conducted on interview data (pre- as secondary analyses. In general, ANCOVAs confirmed all
test to follow-up), supplemented by ANCOVAs run on the ANOVA findings for both the AIDS-related training and
follow-up measures, using pretest scores as covariates. the anger-related training outcomes; the single exception is
Cohort retention. Pretest measures (both interview and noted in the Condom Negotiation results description in the
role play) were completed by 42 youth. Attrition occurred following section. With a sample size of 29 youth, results
for several reasons: incarceration in Juvenile Hall, reloca- should be interpreted based on both the consistency across

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 351


Hovell, Blumberg, Liles, Powell, Morrison, Duran, Sipan, Burkham, and Kelley

measures and the experimental conditions as illustrated in Mean Content scores increased from pretest to posttest
Table 3, as well as by formal significance testing. for the AIDS group and decreased for the anger group. This
Condom Negotiation: AIDS prevention training versus an- differential change did not reach significance. No signifi-
ger management controls. A significant group-by-time (pre/ cant effects were found for Content scores for the posttest
posttest) interaction was found for the Condom Negotia- to 6-month follow-up period.
tion Style score, F(1, 27) = 4.50, p = .04. Mean Style scores Assertiveness for the AIDS group increased from pretest
increased dramatically for the AIDS group and only slightly to posttest, while remaining essentially unchanged for the
for the anger group. However, ANCOVA results were non- anger group, and this differential reached significance, F(1,
significant, suggesting that differential change was not sus- 27) = 5.82, p = .02. There was also a significant group-by-
tained after adjustment for baseline differences in Style. time interaction from posttest to 6-month follow-up, F(1,
During the posttest to 6-month follow-up period, Style 27) = 4.78, p = .04. Assertiveness decreased for the AIDS
scores for the AIDS group declined, while those for the group and increased for the anger group.
anger group remained essentially unchanged. The posttest A significant, F(1, 27) = 8.19, p = .01, group-by-time (pre/
to 6-month follow-up changes did not reach significance. posttest) interaction was found for Anxiety. Anxiety for the
AIDS group decreased from pretest to posttest, while mean
Anxiety scores for the anger group increased. The posttest
TABLE 3 to 6-month follow-up changes did not reach significance.
There were no significant effects for the Condom Nego-
Change in Condom Negotiation Skills and tiation Nonverbal score for pretest to posttest or for posttest
Resisting Taunts Skills by Groupa and Time to follow-up measures.
Resisting Taunts: Anger management training versus AIDS
Time of Measure prevention controls. A significant, F(1, 27) = 11.91, p = .01,
Pretest Posttest 6-mo. Follow-up group-by-time (pre/posttest) interaction was found for the
Skill Component
and Group M SD M SD M SD Negative Responses score. Mean Negative Responses scores
decreased from pretest to posttest for the anger group and
Condom Negotiation
(CN) Skills
increased for the AIDS group. Negative Responses scores
Style for the posttest to 6-month follow-up period increased for
AIDS 0.43 1.34 2.62 2.02 1.86 2.14 the anger group, while they decreased for the AIDS group.
Anger (control) 1.33 1.98 1.67 1.99 1.60 2.69 This differential change did not reach significance, F(1, 27)
Content
AIDS 4.93 3.22 6.15 2.94 6.07 2.27 = 3.96, p = .06.
Anger (control) 4.60 3.22 3.87 3.14 5.33 2.64 A significant, F(1, 27) = 6.03, p = .02, group-by-time (pre/
Assertiveness posttest) interaction was found for the Positive Responses
AIDS 2.89 0.72 3.65 0.80 3.46 0.60 score. Positive Responses increased for the anger group from
Anger (control) 2.77 0.94 2.73 0.75 3.27 0.56
Anxiety pretest to posttest, while Positive Responses for the AIDS
AIDS 2.21 0.72 1.85 0.75 1.75 0.58 group decreased. A significant, F(1, 27) = 4.72, p = .04, group-
Anger (control) 1.83 0.41 2.00 0.33 1.77 0.32 by-time interaction for this measure also was found for the
Nonverbal
AIDS n/a b n/ab 2.85 0.69 2.79 0.43
posttest to 6-month follow-up period. Positive Responses
Anger (control) 2.73 0.59 2.80 0.41 2.67 0.72 decreased for the anger group, while Positive Responses for
Resisting Taunts the AIDS group increased.
(RT) Skills A significant, F(1, 27) = 10.54, p = .01, group-by-time (pre/
Negative Responses
AIDS (control) 1.43 1.99 3.77 3.09 3.00 2.80 posttest) interaction was found for the Aggression score. The
Anger 2.73 2.25 1.93 2.15 3.87 3.23 mean score decreased slightly from pretest to posttest for
Positive Responses the anger group, while the mean score increased for the AIDS
AIDS (control) 4.14 3.46 3.15 3.65 3.64 2.41
Anger 3.13 1.77 5.07 3.43 3.07 2.49
group. No significant effects were found for Aggression scores
Aggression for the posttest to 6-month follow-up period.
AIDS (control) 1.89 1.15 2.85 1.36 2.93 1.60 The group-by-time interaction for Assertiveness for the
Anger 2.60 1.30 2.33 1.29 2.90 1.71 pretest to posttest period was significant, F(1, 27) = 4.75,
Assertiveness
AIDS (control) 2.64 0.82 2.46 0.92 3.00 1.02 p = .04. The mean Assertiveness score for the anger group
Anger 2.73 0.80 3.17 1.03 2.80 1.18 increased from pretest to posttest, while the mean score
Anxiety for the AIDS group decreased. The group-by-time interac-
AIDS (control) 2.00 0.55 1.85 0.72 1.68 0.37 tion for Assertiveness for the posttest to 6-month follow-up
Anger 1.80 0.37 1.70 0.32 1.83 0.36
Nonverbal period was also significant, F(1, 27) = 4.64, p = .04. The mean
AIDS (control) 3.21 0.80 2.85 0.80 3.36 1.74 Assertiveness score for the anger group declined while that
Anger 3.13 0.64 3.27 0.70 3.13 0.35 for the AIDS group increased.
a
AIDS group, n = 14; anger group, n = 15. bUnable to compute M The group-by-time effect for the Resisting Taunts Anxiety
and SD because the CN Nonverbal measure had no variance at score in the pretest to posttest period was not significant.
pretest for the AIDS condition. The group-by-time interaction for this measure in the

352 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Tr a i n i n g A I D S a n d A n g e r P r e v e n t i o n S o c i a l S k i l l s

posttest to 6-month follow-up period was also not signifi- For both skills targeted and the dimensions assessed, re-
cant, F(1, 27) = 3.15, p = .09. Mean Anxiety scores for the sults tended to be consistent across conditions. Measures of
anger group increased, returning to pretest level, while Condom Negotiation Style and Assertiveness increased and
scores for the AIDS group continued to decline. Anxiety decreased after HIV-prevention social skills train-
There were no significant effects for pretest to posttest ing, relative to controls. Similarly, an increase in Positive
or for the posttest to 6-month follow-up time period for Reactions, a decrease in Negative Reactions to Taunts, a de-
Resisting Taunts Nonverbal scores. crease in Aggression and an increase in overall Assertiveness
Behavior/Attitude Change. Self-report items from the followed anger management social skills training, relative to
behavior/attitudes interview were used to assess changes controls. Most of these tests proved significant, even in the
from pretest to 6-month follow-up. Using repeated mea- context of a relatively small sample size and limited statis-
sures ANOVAs and confirmed by ANCOVA, significant tical power. The consistency of effects across dimensions
group-by-time effects were found for four variables. Agree- within condition and the replication of effects across groups
ing with the statement, “Using a condom is more trouble suggest that role playing procedures can be effective for
than it’s worth,” F(1, 26) = 6.08, p = .02, decreased for the increasing both HIV and anger prevention social skills for
AIDS group and increased for the anger group (5-point delinquent adolescents.
ordinal scale ranging from strongly disagree = 0 to strongly The results showed essentially no generalization across
agree = 4). Having used alcohol in the past 6 months, F(1, conditions for the reciprocal skills and dimensions. With
27) = 6.67, p = .02, and having used marijuana in the past 6 the possible exception of anxiety, which tended to decrease
months, F(1, 27) = 4.44 p = .04, decreased for the AIDS in both groups (although not significantly with the anger
group and increased for the anger group (7-point ordinal group), skill taught in one group did not improve in the
scale ranging from not at all = 0 to daily = 6). Complying alternate group at posttest. Similarly, change was minimal
with requests by authority figures without arguing or fight- or nonexistent for specific dimensions, such as Anxiety and
ing back in the past week, F(1, 23) = 4.29, p = .05, decreased Nonverbal Responses, respectively, which were the subset
for the anger group and increased for the AIDS group, con- dimensions of the target skills given the least amount of
trary to expectation (5-point ordinal scale ranging from never explicit training. These findings add discriminant validity
= 1 to always = 5). Changes by group were also examined for the attribution of response-specific change due to train-
for all other behavioral or attitudinal risks related to AIDS ing. These results in total suggest that training increased
and violence, including frequency of vaginal or anal sex; target skills; they also suggest that increasing assertiveness
number of sex partners; pregnancy; condom use; sex under for one or a few specific skills will not reliably produce a
the influence of alcohol and other drugs; sex with high-risk general increase in assertiveness for all social skills. Whether
partners; discussion with peers about their AIDS-related such a generalized assertiveness repertoire can be established
risk behavior; arrest, incarceration, or probation history; gang without training component skills individually remains to
membership; weapon use; and verbally and physically aggres- be determined.
sive acts. There were no significant group-by-time effects These results also show that almost all skills trained were
for these measures. not sustained during the 6-month follow-up period. This
suggests that training needs to be continued, skills need to
DISCUSSION be increased to still higher levels, or some other system of
support must be in place to sustain training effects. This is
Despite obstacles to recruitment, training, measurement, and consistent with the Behavioral-Ecological Model; without
follow-up typical for high-risk adolescents, the encouraging ongoing practice and feedback and changes in culture such as
participation rate in this study demonstrates the feasibility peer norms, obtained skill level can be expected to decrease.
of conducting this type of intervention in an alternative school Educational interventions may need to be sustained to main-
setting. Given the pronounced difficulty of carrying out in- tain achievements and contribute to HIV or anger prevention.
terventions within this population, the outcome is notably Interview measures taken at 6-month follow-up were
encouraging and suggests that similar skills-training interven- examined with respect to changes in risk practices or atti-
tions could be successfully implemented among other groups tudes. Although three of these variables changed in the
of high-risk youth, such as runaways (Rotherum-Borus et expected direction, one changed in a contrary direction,
al., 1991) or the economically disadvantaged (DiClemente and the majority of variables in both interventions did not
& Wingood, 1995). change significantly. A reasonable and conservative inter-
However, given the volunteer nature and subsequent at- pretation of these results is that neither type of training
trition of the sample, and the small sample size, the results was associated with reliable change in behavior. This is con-
should be cautiously interpreted as indicative of the gen- sistent with our inability to sustain skills for the 6-month
eral population of alternative school students, particularly follow-up. It seems unlikely that temporary skill improve-
those who are most severely at risk for AIDS or violence. ments would lead to changes in risk practices or attitudes.
The attrition rate also suggests that more attention to in- Training a few skills in a short time may be insufficient
centives aimed at retaining high-risk students in training to increase adolescents’ frequent use of these skills and
would be important for future studies or services. ultimately reduce their risk practices. Future studies will

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 353


Hovell, Blumberg, Liles, Powell, Morrison, Duran, Sipan, Burkham, and Kelley

need to assess conditions required to sustain skills and needed to sustain skills and whether skills training alone is
their application in order to test their effects on risk be- sufficient to change risk practices.
havior. On the basis of the Behavioral-Ecological Model,
we presume that skills may be prerequisite but not suffi- REFERENCES
cient to change risk practices. In other words, condom use
may be dependent not only on having the requisite skills Biglan, A., & Hayes, S. C. (1996). Should the behavioral sciences become
for correctly using a condom and convincing a partner to more pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on
human behavior. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 5(1), 47–57.
do so but on other factors including social support to apply Biglan, A., Metzler, C. W., Wirt, R., Ary, D., Noell, J., Ochs, L., French,
those skills. The same can be assumed for practicing anger C., & Hood, D. (1990). Social and behavioral factors associated with
management skills. Targeting peer norms, as suggested by high-risk sexual behavior among adolescents. Journal of Behavioral
Winslow, Franzini, and Hwang (1992), may be necessary to Medicine, 13, 245–262.
Bjerregaard, B., & Smith, C. (1993). Gender differences in gang partici-
affect motivation sufficiently to change behavior. The use
pation, delinquency, and substance use. Journal of Quantitative Crimi-
of a “booster” intervention, such as that used by Levy et al. nology, 9, 329–355.
(1995), may be required to sustain those changes. Blumberg, E. J., Hovell, M. F., Werner, C. A., Kelley, N. J., Sipan, C. L.,
The results from this study need be interpreted with caution Burkham-Kreitner, S. M., & Hofstetter, C. R. (1997). Evaluating AIDS-
for a number of reasons: inability to use random assignment, related social skills in Anglo and Latino adolescents: Focus on assess-
ment. Behavior Modification, 21, 281–307.
small sample size, limited training time, and the potential for Bureau of the Census. (1993). Statistical abstract of the United States
unreliable self-reported information concerning risk practices. (113th ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce, Eco-
Technically, this study is quasi-experimental, in absence of nomics and Statistics Administration.
random assignment. However, examination of group similari- Bureau of the Census. (1994). Statistical abstract of the United States
ties and the use of alternate interventions for different skills, (114th ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce, Eco-
nomics and Statistics Administration.
with replication of training effects, provide strong evidence that Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
effects were real. The risk of confounding due to uncontrolled Centers for Disease Control. (1992a). Preventing risk behaviors among
alternate independent variables seems limited. students, HIV/AIDS Prevention, Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Re-
Perhaps the most damaging limitations for this study are port, 3, 1–2.
limited statistical power due to small sample size and lim- Centers for Disease Control. (1992b). Selected behaviors that increase
risk for HIV infection among high school students—United States, 1990.
ited effects due to limited training time. These limitations Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 41, 231–240.
tend to bias the study in the direction of null outcomes. For Centers for Disease Control. (1992c). Sexual behavior among high school
the few skills or dimensions that did not reach significance, students—United States, 1990. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report,
these null outcomes could have been true null effects of 40, 885–888.
Centers for Disease Control. (1993). 1993 Sexually transmitted disease
training or artifacts of limited power or effect size. In fact,
guidelines. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 42(RR-14), 9.
having attained significant results posttraining for most vari- Centers for Disease Control. (1994a). Health risk behaviors among ado-
ables evaluated in the context of limited power strongly lescents who do and do not attend school—United States, 1992. Mor-
suggests that more complete training for larger samples bidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 43, 129–132.
could produce even more substantial changes in skills. Re- Centers for Disease Control. (1994b). Sexual behaviors and drug use among
youth in dropout-prevention programs—Miami, 1994. Morbidity and
sults support the replication of these procedures under con- Mortality Weekly Report, 43, 873–876.
ditions of greater support and more complete control. Centers for Disease Control. (1996). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United
For assessing the risk practices of adolescents, especially States, 1995. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 45, No. SS-4.
youth with a history of less than complete honesty, self- Centers for Disease Control. (1997). HIV/AIDS surveillance report, Morbid-
reports of criminal activity, or sexually sanctioned practices, ity and Mortality Weekly Report, 9(1), 13.
Children’s Defense Fund. (1994). The state of America’s children: Yearbook
or both should be considered very cautiously. Future studies 1994. Washington, DC: Author.
of high-risk youth should include objective outcomes in DiClemente, R. J., & Wingood, G. M. (1995). A randomized controlled trial
order to validate possible prevention effects. These might of an HIV sexual risk-reduction intervention for young African-American
include assessment of arrests, physical examinations of injury women. Journal of the American Medical Association, 274, 1271–1276.
due to violence, pregnancy, sexually transmitted disease Federal Bureau of Investigation, U.S. Department of Justice. (1996). Uni-
form Crime Reports for the United States, 1995. Washington, DC: Author.
infections, and so on, as outcome indicators of skills train- Fingerhut, L. A., & Kleinman, J. C. (1990). International and interstate
ing or other behavioral interventions. comparison of homicide among young males. JAMA, 263, 3292–3295.
Given the continued rise in AIDS cases among youth and Grossman, D. C., Neckerman, H. J., Koepsell, T. D., Liu, P. Y., Asher,
young adults and the continued epidemic of violence and K. N., Beland, K., Frey, K., & Rivara, F. P. (1997). Effectiveness of a
violence prevention curriculum among children in elementary school.
criminal activity among low income youth, additional study
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 277, 1605–1611.
and the identification of effective preventive interventions Grunbaum, J. A., & Basen-Engquist, K. (1993). Comparison of health
is urgently needed. Although only representative of a small risk behaviors between students of a regular school and students in an
part of the needed research and development process, the alternative high school. Journal of School Health, 63, 421–425.
present study suggests that role play technology may con- Gudjonsson, G. H., & Drinkwater, J. (1986). Intervention techniques for
violent behavior. Issues in Criminological and Legal Psychology, 9, 37–47.
tribute to skill acquisition for preventing HIV infection and Hovell, M., Blumberg, E., Sipan, C., Hofstetter, C. R., Kreitner, S., Atkins, C.,
violence among delinquent and high-risk youth. The present & Felice, M. (1998). Skills training for pregnancy and AIDS prevention in
study also has raised serious concerns about the factors Anglo and Latino youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 23, 139–149.

354 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Tr a i n i n g A I D S a n d A n g e r P r e v e n t i o n S o c i a l S k i l l s

Hovell, M. F., Hillman, E. R., Blumberg, E., Sipan, C. L., Atkins, C. J., AIDS prevention program for students in an alternative school. Jour-
Hofstetter, C. R., & Myers, A. (1994). A behavioral-ecological model nal of School Health, 66, 176–182.
of adolescent sexual development: A template for AIDS prevention. Rotherum-Borus, M. J., Koopman, C., Haignere, C., & Davies, M. (1991).
Journal of Sex Research, 31(4), 1–15. Reducing HIV sexual risk behaviors among runaway adolescents. Jour-
Juvenile Court & Community Schools. (1995). School accountability re- nal of the American Medical Association, 226, 1237–1241.
port card. San Diego, CA: San Diego County Office of Education. Schneider, B. H. (1991). A comparison of skill-building and desensitiza-
Kalichman, S. C., Carey, M. P., & Johnson, B. T. (1996). Prevention of sexu- tion strategies for intervention with aggressive children. Aggressive
ally transmitted HIV infection: a meta-analytic review of the behavioral Behavior, 17, 301–311.
outcome literature. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 18(1), 6–15. Select Committee on Children, Youth, and Families. (1992). A decade of
Kellam, S. G., & Rebox, G. W. (1992). Building developmental and etio- denial: Teens and AIDS in America. Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-
logical theory through epidemiologically based preventive interven- ment Printing Office. (House of Representatives, 102nd Congress,
tion trials. In J. McCord & R. E. Tremblay (Eds.), Preventing antisocial ISBN 0-16-039006-0)
behavior: Interventions from birth through adolescence (pp. 162–195). Shafer, M. A., Hilton, J. F., Ekstrand, M., Keogh, J., Gee, L., DiGiorgio-
New York: Guilford Press. Hag, L., Shalwitz, J., & Schachter, J. (1993). Relationship between
Larson, J. D. (1992). Anger and aggression management techniques drug use and sexual behaviors and the occurrence of sexually trans-
through the Think First curriculum. Journal of Offender Rehabilita- mitted diseases among high-risk male youth. Sexually Transmitted
tion, 18(1–2), 101–117. Diseases, 20, 307–313.
Levy, S. R., Perhats, C., Weeks, K., Handler, A. S., Zhu, C., & Flay, B. R. Slymen, D. J., & Hovell, M. F. (1997). Cluster versus individual random-
(1995). Impact of a school-based AIDS prevention program on risk ization in adolescent tobacco and alcohol studies: A basis for design
and protective behavior for newly sexually active students. Journal of decisions. International Journal of Epidemiology, 26, 766–771.
School Health, 65, 145–151. SPSS for Windows (Version 6.1.3) [Computer software]. (1995). Chi-
Main, D. S., Iverson, D. C., McGloin, J., Banspach, S. W., Collins, J. L., cago: SPSS Inc.
Rugg, D. L., & Kolbe, L. J. (1994). Preventing HIV infection among Stanton, B. F., Li, X., Ricardo, I., Galbraith, J., Feigelman, S., & Kaljee, L.
adolescents: Evaluation of a school-based education program. Preven- (1996). A randomized, controlled effectiveness trial of an AIDS pre-
tive Medicine, 23, 409–417. vention program for low-income African-American youths. Archives of
Melchert, T., & Burnett, K. F. (1990). Attitudes, knowledge, and Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 150, 363–372.
sexual behavior of high-risk adolescents: Implications for coun- St. Lawrence, J. S., Brasfield, T. L., Jefferson, K. W., Alleyne, E., O’Bannon,
seling and sexuality education. Journal of Counseling & Develop- R. E., III, & Shirley, A. (1995). Cognitive-behavioral intervention to
ment, 68, 293–298. reduce African American adolescents’ risk for HIV infection. Journal
National Center for Educational Statistics. (1997). The condition of edu- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 221–237.
cation 1996 (NCES 96-304). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Valois, R. F., McKeown, R. E., Garrison, C. Z., & Murray, M. L. (1995).
Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement/U.S. Correlates of aggressive and violent behaviors among public high school
Government Printing Office. adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 16, 26–34.
National Center for Health Statistics. (1993). Advance report of final Whitaker, C., & Bastian, L. (1991). Teenage victims: A national crime sur-
mortality statistics, 1991. (Monthly Vital Statistics Report, 42[2, vey report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of
Suppl.]). Hyattsville, MD: Public Health Service. Justice Statistics (Publication NCJ-128129).
National Institutes of Health. (1997). NIH consensus statement: February Winslow, R. W., Franzini, L. R., & Hwang, J. (1992). Perceived peer norms,
11–13. Bethesda, MD: Department of Health and Human Services. casual sex, and AIDS risk prevention. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
O’Hara, P., Messick, B. J., Fichtner, R. R., & Parris, D. (1996). A peer-led chology, 22, 1809–1827.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 355


Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity

Hardin L. K. Coleman, Sherry B. Casali, and Bruce E. Wampold

This investigation tested H. L. K. Coleman’s (1995b) hypotheses that the strategies adolescents use to cope with cultural diversity
will be organized in a sequential manner and that adolescents will use different strategies depending on the situation. To test
these hypotheses, the authors had 398 adolescents rate the likelihood of using 6 strategies for coping with cultural diversity:
Separation, Assimilation, Acculturation, Alternation, Integration, and Fusion. The authors make suggestions for future research ,
and implications for counseling are suggested.

A
group of adolescent African American girls, the process of second culture contact has historically been
while sharing their feelings about “wannabees” described and the conceptualizations used to explain this
(G. Lindsay, personal communication, March process and then presents a model that integrates previous
31, 1995), were clear in their absolute disdain theories concerning second culture contact. We then present
for those who tried to be something they were an attempt to empirically validate that model.
not. They did not like African Americans who acted “White” Berry, Poortinga, Segall, and Dasen (1992), Coleman
with European Americans and “Black” with African Ameri- (1995b), and LaFromboise et al. (1993) have observed that
cans. They did not like European Americans who tried to the process of coping with second culture contact has gen-
talk or act “Black” when they were with African Americans. erally been called “acculturation.” LaFromboise et al. pointed
After describing several situations in which adolescents out that the term acculturation has been used to describe
could be considered “wannabees,” these girls concluded that both a particular strategy for coping with second culture
they respected people who acted like themselves no mat- contact and the process for coping with second culture con-
ter where they were. The European Americans they liked tact. LaFromboise et al. suggested that the phrase second
were ones who used their own vernacular when spending culture acquisition be used to describe the process of coping
time with others from different cultures. What these girls with second culture contact and reserve the term accul-
were describing are some strategies individuals use when turation for a way to explain or describe the process. This
they have contact with members of a different culture. convention is used throughout the article.
What an individual does when he or she comes into con- In a review of the psychological impact of biculturalism,
tact with a second culture is assumed to have an effect on his LaFromboise et al. (1993) found six ways that have been
or her sense of emotional well-being (LaFromboise, Coleman, used to describe the process of second culture acquisition.
& Gerton, 1993; Padilla, 1980). It has been hypothesized These six descriptions represent different conceptualizations
that how one copes with that contact will affect such of what happens when individuals come into contact with
socioemotional factors as self-esteem (e.g., Phinney, 1991), people from a different culture. Three of the descriptions—
social adjustment and academic performance (e.g., Gomez assimilation, acculturation, and separation—reflect a tradi-
& Fassinger, 1994; Oliver, Rodriguez, & Mickelson, 1985), tional assumption (Stonequist, 1935, 1937) that the process
counseling process and outcome (e.g., Atkinson, Casas, & of second culture acquisition is essentially linear: individuals
Abreu, 1992; Coleman, Wampold, & Casali, 1995), and mental either let go of their culture of origin and join the second
health (e.g., Rogler, Cortes, & Malgady, 1991). Furthermore, culture or they remove themselves from contact with the
Coleman (1995a, 1995b) has hypothesized that the strategies second culture. A person who assimilates or acculturates is
an individual uses to cope with second culture contact will one who attempts to join the second culture. A person who
have an effect on that individual’s academic or job per- separates is one who withdraws from, or avoids contact with,
formance, sense of social competence, and psychological persons who are not members of his or her culture of origin.
well-being. This article briefly reviews the ways in which As Rogler et al. (1991) have indicated, most instruments

Hardin L. K. Coleman is an associate professor of counseling psychology, and Bruce E. Wampold is a professor of counseling psychology, both at the
University of Wisconsin–Madison. Sherry B. Casali is a doctoral candidate in counseling psychology at the University of Wisconsin–Madison and a psychi-
atric nurse at Meritter Hospital in Madison, Wisconsin. This investigation was made possible, in part, by a grant from the Institute for Race and Ethnicity,
University of Wisconsin-System. The authors thank the staff, parents, and students of the Cherokee Heights Middle School for participating in this project, and
Miguel Ybarra, Alfiee Breland, and Marcia Moody for their help in collecting data. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Hardin L. K.
Coleman, Department of Counseling Psychology, 321 Education Building, 1000 Bascom Mall, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI 53706 (e-
mail: hcoleman@Facstaff.wisc.edu).

356 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity

designed to measure second culture acquisition have used issues related to second culture acquisition and policy or
this linear model. Most of these measurements assess the program development (e.g., curriculum development for cul-
degree to which a person has become a member of a new turally diverse schools). These traditional conceptualizations
culture (has assimilated or acculturated) or has stayed in- of second culture acquisition have also focused on the out-
volved with his or her culture of origin (usually being la- comes of the process. These approaches to conceptualiz-
beled as “low-acculturated” or “traditional”), even if he or ing second culture acquisition suggest that the individual
she has migrated to a new culture. Rogler et al. observed becomes “something” as the result of coming into contact
that most conceptualizations of the process of second cul- with a second culture. For example, a person who lets go of
ture acquisition included the “assumption that increments his or her culture of origin to gain competence in a second
of involvement in the . . . host society necessarily entail culture becomes “assimilated.” A person who creates a
corresponding decrements of disengagement from the living and working situation in which he or she maintains
immigrant’s traditional culture” (p. 587). A variation on this his or her culture of origin while simultaneously interacting
theme is represented by efforts to include a bicultural di- with individuals from other cultures becomes “integrated.”
mension between the poles of high and low acculturation The assessment tools used to measure this process of second
(e.g., Landrine & Klonoff, 1994; Szapocznik, Kurtines, & culture acquisition reflect this perspective. These measures
Fernandez, 1980). are often used to determine whether an individual is accul-
The other three descriptions of second culture acquisition— turated, assimilated, biculturated, separated, fused, integrated,
alternation, integration, and fusion—represent a different or separated. An individual can either score high, medium,
conceptualization of the process. According to these or low in these categories (e.g., acculturated, bicultural, low-
conceptualizations, it is possible to both maintain involve- acculturated).
ment with one’s culture of origin and develop competence Coleman (1995b) proposed an alternate paradigm for
in a second culture. Each conceptualization includes understanding how individuals manage the process of sec-
orthogonal dimensions (LaFromboise et al., 1993; Oetting ond culture acquisition. Coleman argued that traditional
& Beauvais, 1991; Rogler et al., 1991) that represent a dis- conceptualizations focused on the outcomes of the second
tinct behavioral pattern operating independently of the culture acquisition. He hypothesized that the process or
other behavior patterns. Each of these three descriptions strategies individuals use to achieve particular ends within
suggests there are different ways in which an individual particular contexts is an equally important focus for in-
could manage this process. Cuellar, Arnold, and Maldonado vestigation. Coleman suggested that the context in which
(1985), in a revision of the Acculturation Rating Scale for an individual is coping with cultural difference and the
Mexican Americans (ARSMA-II), used this approach to goals that individual wants to achieve in that context will
capture the degree to which Mexican Americans integrate influence the individual’s choice of strategy. This
Anglo and Mexican cultures and assimilate, remain sepa- conceptualization suggests that individuals manage their
rate from, or become marginalized in either the Anglo or behavior in cross-cultural situations by developing what
Mexican cultures. The alternation conceptualization (Ogbu Ford (1992) called “behavioral episode schemata.” This
& Matute-Bianchi, 1986) assumes that it is possible to al- means an individual determines his or her goals in a particu-
ternate between two cultures in the same manner as one lar situation and will use a particular behavioral strategy
alternates the use of language in different contexts. The inte- that he or she believes will achieve those goals. The strat-
gration conceptualization (Berry, Kim, Power, Young, & Bujaki, egy an individual uses, therefore, will be related to his or
1989) assumes that it is possible to have individuals from her goals, the context in which the behavior is to be displayed,
different cultures coexist without compromising their cul- and the skills the individual possesses to use a particular
tural identities. In other words, each person maintains their strategy. Coleman hypothesized that the negative and
culture of origin while interacting with others from differ- positive outcomes associated with the process of second
ent cultures. The fusion conceptualization (LaFromboise et culture acquisition (LaFromboise et al., 1993) reflect how
al., 1993; Coleman, 1995b) assumes that individuals from effective the individual’s strategy for coping with cultural
different cultures who are in consistent contact with each diversity is at helping that individual achieve his or her
other will eventually fuse to create a new culture that sub- goals within a particular cultural context. Coleman sug-
sumes individuals’ cultures of origin. gested that the strategies a person uses to cope with sec-
Research on second culture acquisition has attempted ond culture contact will affect his or her performance in
either to determine which description best describes the particular contexts and how the individual feels about him
behavior of a particular individual or group of individuals or herself.
when coming into contact with a second culture or to deter- Coleman (1995b) also hypothesized that these strategies
mine which reflects the most effective way to act when may be organized in a sequential rather than a linear or
experiencing second culture contact. The focus on individual orthogonal manner. As Rogler et al. (1991) have indicated,
behavior is common to the literature that has investigated theories of second culture acquisition have described the
the relationship between psychological variables (e.g., mental process in either linear or orthogonal terms. These theories
health) and second culture acquisition. The focus on group suggest that an individual is either at one end of a mutually
behavior is common to the literature that has addressed exclusive bipolar relationship (e.g., American or Hispanic)

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 357


Coleman, Casali, and Wampold

or the individual has, or does not have, orthogonal compe- time, then he or she will use an alternation strategy. If the
tence in both cultures (e.g., American and Hispanic). Fig- individual does intend to combine his or her culture of origin
ure 1, building on a suggestion by I. Pagan (personal com- with the second culture, there are two more strategies he or
munication, January 12, 1995), represents a hypothesized she can use. If the person intends to blend the two cultures, he
sequential relationship among these strategies. or she will be using the fusion strategy. If the person intends to
The model, depicted in Figure 1, suggests that an indi- have his or her culture of origin coexist with the second cul-
vidual who comes into contact with a second culture will ture, then he or she is using the integration strategy.
have to make a series of choices, consciously or uncon- Rather than conceptualizing the process of second cul-
sciously, about how he or she wants to associate with the ture acquisition as linear or orthogonal, this model suggests
new and old cultures, and those goals will be reflected in that the strategies individuals use to cope with cultural di-
his or her behavioral response. As Ford (1992) suggested, versity will be determined by the goals individuals have
the goals an individual has will affect his or her motivation within particular contexts. This suggests that an individual
to perform certain behaviors. This sequential model sug- will possess a behavioral episode schemata for each strat-
gests that the first choice an individual makes is whether or egy but may be more likely to use one than another as a
not he or she will associate with more than one cultural result of his or her socialization, the context in which he or
group. If the individual chooses to associate with only one she is coming into contact with a second culture, and the
group, then he or she needs to decide whether to associate goal he or she is attempting to achieve within that context.
only with his or her culture of origin. If the individual makes The purpose of this investigation is to test Coleman’s
that choice, the model proposes that he or she will use a (1995b) hypothesis that the strategies that individuals use
separation strategy when coming into contact with indi- to cope with cultural diversity reflect a sequential process
viduals from another culture. In contrast, if the individual and that those choices are context dependent. To achieve
does not associate with his or her culture of origin, then a this purpose, we present a method for identifying these strat-
third choice may be made. If the individual wants to be- egies within an adolescent population. Adolescents were
come a full member of the second culture, then he or she selected because they are a group that is most likely to have
will use the assimilation strategy. If the individual does not contact with individuals from different cultures within their
intend to become a full member of the second culture to schools. This investigation determines the reliability of the
achieve a particular goal (e.g., economic stability) but only method developed to identify the strategies, examines the
associates with that culture, then he or she is using the ac- sequential relationship among these strategies, and tests
culturation strategy. (See Coleman, 1995b, for a more ex- whether the choice of strategy is dependent on context. It
tensive discussion of these strategies.) also reports on the degree to which use of these strategies
If, at the beginning of this process, the individual chooses may be affected by the ethnicity, gender, or socioeconomic
to associate with more than one culture at a time, then status of the participant.
there is another range of strategies for coping with cultural
diversity available to him or her. If the individual intends to METHOD
associate with more than one culture, but not at the same
Participants
Do you seek to Fusion Three hundred and ninety-eight students in a midwestern
Do you attempt to associate combine two or middle school (Grades 6–8) completed at least part of the
with your culture of origin more cultures?
and a second culture at Yes questionnaire. Of the students, 237 were European Ameri-
the same time? No can, 41 were African American, 33 were Asian American, 8
Yes
Integration were Hispanic American, 3 were Native American, 15 were
Do you associate No biracial, and 2 did not report their ethnicity. All the partici-
with more than one
Alternation pants were volunteers and were treated in accordance with
cultural group?
Yes the American Counseling Association’s (1995) Code of Ethics
No
and Standards of Practice.
Do you associate
with only your
Separation
Materials
cultural group?
Do you try to
Yes become a full Assimilation Stimulus. A questionnaire was developed to help us identify
No member of the the strategies adolescents use to cope with cultural diversity.
second culture? Yes This questionnaire, referred to as the Coping With Cul-
No
tural Diversity Scale (CCDS), consists of nine descriptions
Acculuration of situations that involve coping with cultural diversity. After
each situation, there are six responses an individual can
FIGURE 1
make. Each response represents one of the strategies
Decision Tree of Strategies for Coping With Cultural described earlier. On the CCDS, the participant is ask to
Diversity state, on a scale of 0 to 6, how likely he or she is to choose

358 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity

each of the responses to the situation. A 0 indicates that the = .57, Separation = .69, Alternation = .66, Integration =
participant is not at all likely to use that response in that situa- .75, and Fusion = .81. These coefficients suggest that the
tion; a 6 means he or she would definitely use that response in total scale has strong internal reliability with adequate-to-
that situation. This questionnaire yields 7 scores. There is a strong internal reliability for most of the subscales. The
score for each strategy (Assimilation, Acculturation, Alterna- reliability of the strategy-specific questions ranged from
tion, Integration, Separation, and Fusion). The higher the score, adequate to strong.
the more likely it is that the participant may use that strategy. Relationship among and between strategies. The zero-order
There is also a total score. This score represents the range of correlation matrix among these strategies is presented in
strategies an individual has at his or her disposal. The higher Table 1. These correlations suggest a very strong relationship
the total score, the more strategies the individual believes he among the Fusion, Integration, and Alternation strategies,
or she is likely to use. The Appendix presents an example of and a strong relationship among the Acculturation, Assimi-
one of the situations and the possible response. (Details on lation, and Alternation strategies. The Separation strategy
the development of the questionnaire are available on request seems to have a low correlation with the other five strate-
from the first author.) gies. This matrix supports the hypothesis that strategies that
Demographic information. At the end of the CCDS was a involve affiliating with a single culture are distinct from
section that solicited demographic information concerning the strategies that involve affiliating with one or more cultures.
participant. Participants were asked to identify their ethnicity The fact that the Alternation strategy is highly correlated
from a list and to state their mother’s and father’s employ- with both groups is further support for this hypothesis
ment. These latter two pieces of information were used to because using that strategy demands affiliating with one
determine the participant’s socioeconomic status (SES) using culture at a time.
Duncan’s Revised Socioeconomic Index (Featherman & To further understand the relationship between and among
Stevens, 1982; Stevens & Cho, 1985). these strategies, the data were submitted to an average link-
age agglomerative method of cluster analysis. As represented
Procedure in Figure 2, the six strategies seemed to reflect the hypoth-
esized sequential relationship. Fusion and Integration seem
A notice was sent to parents in the school newsletter in- to share the most similar properties. They then joined with
forming them about the purpose and timing of this inves- Alternation as a group. At the same time, Acculturation and
tigation. Over a 2-day period, students completed the scale Assimilation seem to cluster into a second set. These two
in their social studies class. They watched a video explain- sets eventually cluster together and then, at the end of the
ing the purpose of the investigation, and then they com- solution, join with Separation. This solution closely matches
pleted the questionnaire. the proposed model in that it indicates a distinct relation-
ship between those strategies that involve affiliation with
RESULTS more that one culture (i.e., Fusion and Integration) and those
that involve affiliating with only one culture (i.e., Assimila-
To test Coleman’s (1995b) hypothesis that these strategies
tion and Acculturation). The solution also suggests that the
are organized in a sequential manner and that an individual’s
behaviors associated with the Alternation strategy share simi-
responses are context dependent required a series of analy-
larities with both groups, whereas the Separation strategy is
ses. The internal consistency of the CCDS was established
distinctly different from the other five.
using Cronbach’s alpha. The data were then submitted to a Tests of context dependence and adequacy of construct mea-
cluster analysis to test the sequential organization of the surement. One of the important aspects of Coleman’s
strategies. To test the context-dependent nature of these
(1995b) conceptualization of cultural coping strategies is
strategies, structural equation modeling was used. Finally,
that the choice of strategy depends on the context. That is,
group differences in the use of the strategies were initially
determined by submitting the data to a series of 1 × 6 fac-
torial analysis of variance to determine the effects of group
membership (i.e., ethnicity, gender, socioeconomic status TABLE 1
as independent variables) on the use of the six strategies as Correlations Among Strategies for Coping With
the dependent variables. Univariate analysis of variance was Cultural Diversity
completed on the total scale score and those strategies that
indicated significant differences in the multivariate analy- Strategy 1 2 3 4 5 6
sis of variance. A general linear model was used to correct
for uneven cell numbers. 1. Acculturation —
2. Alternation .62 —
Reliability Estimates 3. Assimilation .56 .55 —
4. Fusion .46 .78 .48 —
The internal reliability of the whole questionnaire and strat- 5. Integration .45 .75 .39 .84 —
6. Separation .35 .21 .24 .14 .11 —
egy-specific questions using Cronbach’s alpha is as follows:
total questionnaire = .90, Assimilation = .62, Acculturation Note. All correlations were significant at p < .05.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 359


Coleman, Casali, and Wampold

Rescaled Distance Cluster Combine

CASE 0 5 10 15 20 25
Label Number + -------------- + --------------- + --------------- + --------------- + ------------ +

FUSION 4

INTEGRATION 5

ALTERNATION 2

ACCULTURATION 1

ASSIMILATION 3

SEPARATION 6

FIGURE 2
Dendrogram Using Average Linkage (Between Groups)

an individual’s choice of strategy will depend, in part, on the ing paths, this method capitalizes on chance (MacCallum,
situation; a person’s exhibited strategy will be a function of Roznowski, & Necowitz, 1992). As explained by MacCallum
the environment as well as the person. The present data pro- et al., the preferable way to use structural equation model-
vided the opportunity to test whether or not the context ac- ing is to compare and test two competing models, where
counts for variance in reported use of strategies. the superiority of one model has theoretical implications.
To test the context dependence hypothesis, we used struc- In the present study, if the context-dependent model fits
tural equation modeling. To obtain multiple measures of each the data significantly better than the context-independent
strategy, the nine situations for each strategy were distributed model, then dependence on context is confirmed. The con-
into two groups of situations. Group 1 included Situations 3, text independence model is the one in which each of the
2, 5, 1, and 9; and Group 2 included Situations 8, 4, 7, and 6. strategies loads on the two corresponding subscales (i.e.,
This strategy provided two subscales for each strategy. The the subscales are the observed variables and the strategies
variance/covariance matrix for the 12 subscales (two for each are the latent variables), the strategies are allowed to corre-
strategy) was computed and served as input for the modeling. late, and the error variances of the subscales are indepen-
The test of context dependence was accomplished by dent. The context-dependent model is identical except that
comparing two structural equation models, one of which the error covariances for the subscales with the same situ-
modeled context dependence and one of which did not. ations are allowed to correlate. The reasoning is that if choice
Although the typical method in structural equation mod- of strategy is dependent on the context, then some of the
eling is to specify a model and modify it by adding or delet- variance of each of the subscales is unique to the situations

360 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity

that compose those subscales, and consequently the error Group Differences
variances would be correlated (see Cole, 1987; Jöreskog &
Sörbom, 1993). To determine the effect of ethnicity, gender, and SES on
The fit statistics for the two models are presented in Table the degree to which participants were likely to use a par-
ticular strategy, we performed a series of multivariate and
2. The difference between the models was tested by calcu-
univariate analyses. Post hoc analysis of mean differences
lating the difference in chi-square values for the fit of each
were computed using a Bonferroni test with a significance
model and comparing the difference to a chi-square distri-
level of .05
bution with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in
degrees of freedom. Because the chi-square for the differ- Effect of ethnicity of total scale score and strategy scores. A
ence, χ2(26) = 109.60, p < .001, is sufficiently large to reject multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) found a sig-
nificant main effect for ethnicity, F(5, 299) = 2.52, p < .001.
the null hypothesis that the two models fit the data equally
Univariate analysis of the effect of ethnicity on the strategies
well, we conclude that the context-dependent model is
for coping with cultural diversity found the following
more explanatory of the phenomenon under study. That is,
effects: on the Separation strategy, F(5, 299) = 2.91, p = .01,
choice of strategy was dependent on the situation.
It should be noted that the context-dependent model pro- with African Americans using this strategy significantly more
vides evidence that the CCDS adequately measures the than European Americans; on Acculturation F(5, 299) = 2.64,
p = .02, with Asian Americans more likely to use this strategy
strategies it was intended to measure. Although the chi-
than European Americans; on Alternation, F(5, 299) = 3.58,
square for this model, χ2(13) = 55.25, is large enough to
p = .004, with no significant group contrasts; and on Inte-
reject the model, this is due to the large number of partici-
gration, F(5, 299) = 4.99, p < .001, with Asian Americans
pants (330); rejection of the model is not uncommon for
studies with large samples. However, both the root mean more likely to use this strategy than European Americans.
square residual (1.35) and the goodness-of-fit index (.97) There were no significant effects for ethnicity on the
Assimilation or Fusion strategies. A test of the effect of
are within the range of cut-off statistics suggested for ad-
ethnicity on the Total scale score revealed a significant
equate models (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993).
effect, F(5, 299) = 3.95, p = .002, with Asian Americans
Correlations among the latent variables (i.e., the strate-
having a significantly higher total score than European
gies) of the context-dependent model were also examined.
These correlations are the disattenuated correlations in that Americans. The means and standard deviations for the
they are adjusted for the reliability of the latent variables effects of group membership on the total score and strate-
gies are available from the first author on request.
(and consequently differ from those in Table 1, which are
Effect of gender on total scale and strategy scores. A
attenuated by the unreliability of the measurement). These
MANOVA showed a significant main effect for gender,
correlations, as well as the standard errors, are presented in
F(1, 321) = 5.47, p < .001. Univariate analysis of the effect
Table 3. A correlation between two latent variables that is
more than two standard errors less than 1.00 implies that of gender on the strategies for coping with cultural diversity
the latent variables represent distinct, although related, were significant for every strategy—Separation, F(1, 321)
= 3.58, p = .05; Acculturation, F(1, 321) = 10.25, p = .002;
constructs (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993); on the other hand,
Alternation F(1, 321) = 10.11, p = .002; Integration, F(1, 321)
correlations within two standard errors of 1.00 indicated
= 9.71, p = .002; and Fusion, F(1, 321) = 10.64, p = .001—
that the two constructs, as measured, are identical. Accord-
except Assimilation, in which there were no significant
ingly, this analysis suggests that Fusion, Alternation, and
Integration are essentially identical, as measured. differences. Gender had a significant effect on the Total
scale score, F(1, 321) = 6.65, p = .01, with women, as a
group, having significantly higher scores than men.
Effect of SES on total scale and strategy scores. A MANOVA
TABLE 2 found a significant main effect for SES, F(2, 285) = 3.17, p
< .001. Univariate analysis of the effect of SES on the strat-
Comparison of Fit Statistics for Context-Dependent egies for coping with cultural diversity being significant on
and Context-Independent Models every strategy: Assimilation, F(2, 285) = 4.41, p = .01, with
the middle group more likely to use this strategy than the
Fit Statistics
upper SES group; Acculturation, F(2, 285) = 3.59, p = .03,
Model χ 2
df RMSR GFI with the lower SES group more likely than the either the
middle or the upper groups to use this strategy; Alterna-
Context Independent 164.85 39 1.47 .93
Context Dependent 55.25 13 1.35 .97
tion, F(2, 285) = 5.42, p = .005, with the lower group more
Difference in Modelsa 109.60 26 likely than the upper group to use this strategy; Integra-
tion, F(2, 285) = 5.17, p = .006, with the lower SES group
Note. n = 330. RMSR = root mean square residual; GFI = goodness-
of-fit index.
more likely than either the middle or the upper group to
a
Chi-square (.26) = 109.60, p < .001, indicating that the use this strategy; and Fusion, F(2, 285) = 3.64, p = .03, with
context-dependent model accounts for significantly more variance in no significant post hoc group contrasts) except separa-
the data than does the context-independent model. tion. SES had a significant effect on the Total scale score,

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 361


Coleman, Casali, and Wampold

TABLE 3
Correlations Among Latent Variables (i.e., Strategies) in the Context-Dependent Model

Latent Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Acculturation — —
2. Alternation 0.89 (.06) — —
3. Assimilation 0.80 (.06) 0.81 (.05) — —
4. Fusion 0.63 (.06) 1.03 (.03) 0.64 (.05) — —
5. Integration 0.59 (.06) 0.98 (.03) 0.52 (.06) 1.01 (.02) — —
6. Separation 0.68 (.08) 0.49 (.08) 0.41 (.08) 0.26 (.07) 0.28 (.07) — —
Note. Standard errors in parentheses.

F(2, 285) = 5.19, p = .006, with the lower SES group having are distinct behavior patterns, but they are not mutually
significantly higher total scores than the middle and higher exclusive. In other words, one individual may be able to use
SES groups. all the strategies at different times. This is the core assump-
These results indicate a main effect for ethnicity, gender, tion in Coleman’s (1995b) hypothesis: that the use of the
and class in the likelihood that an individual will choose a strategies is context dependent. The structural equation
particular strategy when coping with cultural diversity. What modeling analysis of this data supports this hypothesis.
this pattern of usage means is discussed as follows. That analysis confirmed, as well, that the situations in the
CCDS had a significant effect on the participants’ choice of
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS FOR COUNSELING strategy. In other words, the strategy an individual will use is
dependent on the social context in which it will be used. For
The purpose of this investigation was to test Coleman’s example, an adolescent client may use an assimilation or ac-
(1995b) hypotheses that the strategies individuals use to cope culturation strategy in a classroom situation and then use a
with cultural diversity are organized in a sequential manner separation or integration strategy in a social situation. This
and that the use of these strategies is context dependent. To further supports the hypothesis that the strategies an indi-
test these hypotheses, we successfully developed a question- vidual client uses to cope with cultural diversity are not
naire that reliably measures the strategies adolescents use to mutually exclusive, as is suggested by linear and orthogonal
cope with cultural diversity. The CCDS allowed us to test models of second culture acquisition, but are simultaneously
the sequential relationship among the strategies and the ef- available to an individual, as is hypothesized in a sequential
fect of context on the use of these strategies. model. Which strategy an individual client will use is depen-
Both the correlation matrix of the scores for the strate- dent on the setting, his or her goals in the setting, and his or
gies and the subsequent cluster analysis supported the hy- her ability to achieve those goals in that setting.
pothesis that the strategies individuals use to cope with Coleman (1997) has suggested that understanding a
cultural diversity have a sequential rather than a linear or client’s strategy for coping with cultural diversity will
orthogonal relationship to each other. Coleman’s (1995b) have an important influence on the counseling process.
model hypothesized that the strategies would cluster to- How an adolescent copes will influence how well they
gether in a particular manner. He suggested that those strat- work within a school environment. A student of color
egies that involve association with only one culture would who attempts to assimilate into a predominately White
be strongly related to each other as would those that in- school environment and is rejected will have one set of
volved associating with more that one culture. The cluster concerns and a student of color who uses a separation strat-
analysis supports this hypothesis. It shows that the monoc- egy in that context will present with a very different set of
ultural strategies, Assimilation and Acculturation, are closely issues. Furthermore, the strategy a counselor uses to cope
aligned, as are the multicultural strategies, Fusion and Inte- with cultural diversity can either converge or conflict with
gration. These groups are joined through the Alternation a client’s, which will affect the development of a working
strategy, which shares characteristics of both groups. The alliance. The results of this investigation reinforce the idea
Separation strategy seems to form a distinct group. This that counselors need to look beyond their own or their
makes conceptual sense because it is the one strategy that clients’ group membership and focus on how within-group
allows individuals to remove themselves from contact with variations lead to problems for clients and solutions within
persons from other cultures. The cluster analysis suggests counseling.
that it remains within the monocultural grouping but is The sequential and context-dependent nature of these
still distinct. coping strategies are further supported by analysis of group
If these strategies represented typologies of ways to en- differences in the use of these strategies. It would be a
gage in the second culture acquisition process, that is, if mistake to overinterpret these data, but there are some
they were distinct and nonoverlapping behavior patterns, important trends that need to be noted. One significant
there would have been no interpretable solution to the clus- trend is the fact that those individuals who most closely
ter analysis. This solution reveals that the coping strategies represented the mainstream values of the school in which

362 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Adolescent Strategies for Coping With Cultural Diversity

this investigation was completed, middle and upper SES need to know more about the role of context in the
European American boys, had the lowest Total scale score development and use of these strategies and how they are
and, often, lower subscale scores, than their lower SES, related to similar constructs, such as ethnic identity. We
ethnic minority, and female peers. This suggests that in a con- also need to identify the degree to which the likelihood
text in which one is the dominant group, one needs less of a to use particular strategies within particular settings is pre-
repertoire for coping with cultural diversity, and vice versa. What dictive of such significant outcomes as academic achieve-
the results of this investigation suggest is that factors such as ment, social competence, and mental status. The results
ethnicity, gender, social class, and social skills will affect how a of this investigation lay the groundwork for these future
client behaves in different situations. investigations.
The fact that these strategies seem to be context depen-
dent highlights the distinction between Coleman’s model
(1995b) and models of ethnic identity (Cross, 1991; Helms, REFERENCES
1993; Phinney, 1991). These results suggest that the strate- American Counseling Association. (1995). Code of ethics and standards
gies a client uses to cope with cultural diversity will vary of practice. Alexandria, VA: Author.
depending on the social context. These strategies, therefore, Atkinson, D. R., Casas, A., & Abreu, J. (1992). Mexican-American accul-
seem to be exogenous variables in contrast to ethnic iden- turation, counselor ethnicity and cultural sensitivity, and perceived
counselor competence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 515–520.
tity, which is an endogenous variable. Future research is Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M., & Bujaki, M. (1989). Accul-
needed to better establish the relationship between ethnic turation attitudes in plural societies. Applied Psychology: An Interna-
identity, these strategies for coping with cultural diversity, tional Review, 38, 185–206.
and counseling outcomes. Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. P., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (1992). Cross-
cultural psychology: Research and applications. New York: Cambridge
There are certain limitations to this investigation that need to
University Press.
be addressed in future research in this area. Given the limited Cole, D. A. (1987). Utility of confirmatory factor analysis in test validation
number of culturally diverse participants, we were unable to research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 584–594.
examine the multivariate effects of group membership on the Coleman, H. L. K. (1995a). Cultural factors and the counseling process:
use of these strategies. Given the support of the context-de- Implications for school counselors. The School Counselor, 42, 180–185.
Coleman, H. L. K. (1995b). Strategies for coping with cultural diversity.
pendent nature of these strategies, it will also be necessary to The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 722–740.
examine this process in settings where ethnic minorities are the Coleman, H. L. K. (1997). Conflict in multicultural counseling relation-
majority to see how context affects these patterns. ships: Source and resolution. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and
Although the psychometric properties of the Coping With Development, 25, 195–200.
Cultural Diversity Scale were adequate for the needs of Coleman, H. L. K., Wampold, B. E., & Casali, S. B. (1995). Ethnic mi-
norities’ ratings of ethnically similar and European American counse-
this investigation, the results revealed several issues that lors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 55–64
need to be examined further. The first is that the reliability Cross, W. E. (1991). Shades of black. Philadelphia: Temple University
for the Acculturation subscale is marginally adequate. Ef- Press.
forts should be made to improve those items if subsequent Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1985). Acculturation Rating
investigations using the questionnaire do not produce Scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA
Scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275–304.
better results. The structural equation modeling analysis Featherman, D. L., & Stevens, G. (1982). A revised socioeconomic
also suggests that there may be fewer than six distinct strat- index of occupational status: Application in analysis of sex differ-
egies. Specifically, that analysis suggests that Alternation, ences in attainment. In M. G. Powers (Ed.), Measures of socioeco-
Integration, and Fusion may actually be a single strategy. If nomic status: Current issues (pp. 83–127). Boulder, CO: Westview
Press.
that is the case, then it would suggest that Acculturation
Ford, M. E. (1992). Motivating humans. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
and Assimilation make a second group, and Separation Helms, J. E. (1993). Black and White racial identity: Theory, research, and
remains as a distinct strategy or third behavior pattern. practice. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Further examination of the psychometric properties of Gomez, M. J., & Fassinger, R. E. (1994). An initial model of Latina achieve-
the CCDS, particularly its construct validity, is needed to ment: Acculturation, biculturalism, and achieving styles. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 41, 205–215.
clarify these relationships. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation
In this investigation, we have observed that the strategies modeling with SIMPLIS command language. SSI Scientific Software In-
adolescents use to cope with cultural diversity possess a se- ternational. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
quential relationship to each other and that their use is con- LaFromboise, T. D., Coleman, H. L. K., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psychological
text dependent. This suggests that the wider the range of impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114,
395–412.
strategies an individual has to cope with cultural diversity, Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1994). The African-American Accultura-
the better he or she will be able to effectively manage a cul- tion Scale: Development, reliability, validity. Journal of Black Psychol-
turally diverse or monocultural environment. If that is the ogy, 20, 104–127.
case, helping individuals to effectively use a wide range of MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., & Necowitz, L. B. (1992). Model
strategies may have a beneficial effect on their school or job modifications in covariance structure analysis: The problem of capi-
talization on chance. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 490–504.
performance and their sense of psychological well-being. Oetting, E. R., & Beauvais, F. (1991). Orthogonal cultural identification
Future research in this area needs to examine that possi- theory: The cultural identification of minority adolescents. The Inter-
bility, especially in the context of working with clients. We national Journal of the Addictions, 25, 655–685.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 363


Coleman, Casali, and Wampold

Ogbu, J. U., & Matute-Bianchi, M. A., (1986). Understanding sociocul- APPENDIX


tural factors: Knowledge, identity, and social adjustment. In Language
Minority Children/Bilingual Education Office, Beyond language: Social Example of Items From the Coping With Cultural
and cultural factors in schooling (pp. 73–142). Los Angeles: California Diversity Scale: Adolescent Version
State Department of Education Evaluation, Dissemination and Assess-
ment Center, California State University. Rating Scale:
Oliver, M. L., Rodriguez, C. J., & Mickelson, R. A. (1985). Brown and Not at all likely to do Likely to do Definitely would do
Black in White: The social adjustment and academic performance of 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
Chicano and Black Students in a predominately White university. The
Urban Review, 17, 3–23. Situation 4
Padilla, A. M. (1980). Acculturation: Theory, models, and some new find- If you were with someone from a different race or culture than you,
ings. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. and someone from your race or culture started to bother both of
Phinney, J. S. (1991). Ethnic identity and self-esteem: A review and inte- you for being together, how likely are you to:
gration. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 13, 193–208.
1. Stop hanging out with the person from the other race or culture?
Rogler, L., Cortes, D. E., & Malgady, R. G. (1991). Acculturation and
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
mental health among Hispanics. American Psychologist, 46, 585–597.
2. Just laugh if off?
Stevens, G., & Cho, J. H., (1985). Socioeconomic indexes and the new
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
1980 census occupational classification scheme. Social Science Research,
14, 142–168. 3. Pretend not to hear them?
Stonequist, E. V., (1935). The problem of marginal man. American Jour- 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
nal of Sociology, 7, 1–12. 4. Ask the person who is bothering you what their problem is?
Stonequist, E. V., (1937). The marginal man: A study in personality and 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
culture conflict. New York: Scribner. 5. Ask him or her to leave the two of you alone?
Szapocznik, J., Kurtines, W. M., & Fernandez, T. (1980). Bicultural in- 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
volvement in Hispanic American youths. International Journal of In- 6. Tell the person bothering you that she or he is wrong?
tercultural Relations, 4, 353–365. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

364 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Suicide: An Empirical Investigation of
Counselor Attitudes

James R. Rogers, Christine M. Gueulette, Jodi Abbey-Hines, Jolynn V. Carney, and James L. Werth, Jr.

This study investigated attitudes of mental health counselors toward rational suicide. Over 80% of respondents were moderately
supportive of the idea that people can make well-reasoned decisions that death is their best option, and further, they identified a
consistent set of criteria to evaluate such decisions. Additionally, analyses revealed that as the scenario moved from client, to
friend, to self, rational suicide seemed to gain in acceptability for these professionals. Implications for training and practice in light
of the apparent responsibilities with suicidal clients defined in the 1995 American Counseling Association’s Code of Ethics and
Standards of Practice are discussed.

I
n recent years, the highly publicized issue of rational lenge, and the 1996 ruling by the Ninth Circuit Court of
suicide has generated considerable debate in both the Appeals that state laws prohibiting physician-assisted sui-
popular (e.g., Quill, 1993) and professional literature cide were unconstitutional (Compassion in Dying v. Wash-
(e.g., Werth, 1996, 1999a). Although the concept of ington). Similarly, in that same year on the East coast, the
rational suicide has historical roots reaching back cen- Second Circuit Court of Appeals drew the same conclu-
turies (e.g., Previn, 1996) and is clearly embedded in the sion, albeit by a different constitutional route, in its ruling
existential literature (e.g., Camus, 1955), contemporary at- in the case of Quill v. Vacco.
tention to this highly charged issue has been fueled by In response to legislative challenges to the rulings of the
multiple sources. For example, concern over the issue of Second and Ninth Circuit Courts of Appeals, the United
rational suicide has resulted from such realities as the on- States Supreme Court agreed to hear the two cases and in
going advancement in life-saving medical technology and 1997 ruled that terminally ill people do not have a consti-
the related aging of society in the United States (Rogers, tutional right to physician-assisted suicide (Brody, 1998).
1996), the AIDS pandemic (Rogers & Britton, 1994; Werth, In this ruling, the Supreme Court declared that the deci-
1992), and general changes in the way human beings die sion whether to allow physician-assisted suicide was under
(Battin, 1994). According to Battin, the way we die has the purview of the individual states.
changed drastically over the past century from acute death Of particular relevance to counselors in this legislative
at an early age resulting from opportunistic infections to process was the fact that the American Counseling Asso-
death from chronic and degenerative diseases later in life. ciation (ACA) signed an amicus curiae (“friend of the
Thus, in our contemporary society, rational suicide is an court”) brief focused on mental health issues associated
attempt to control the timing and process of death and, with hastened death decisions and in support of the de-
hereby, secure what has been termed by Battin as “the least cisions of the circuit courts (“Brief of the Washington
worst death” (p. 36). State Psychological Association,” [“Brief–WSPA”] 1996).
The issue of rational suicide is especially relevant in the ACA’s decision to sign onto this brief was made unilater-
United States because it underlies the passionate debate ally based on the results of discussions among the
regarding physician-assisted suicide (Werth, 1999a). For organization’s leadership and management (Morrissey,
example, in the last decade this debate was stimulated on 1997b) and with limited input from the organization’s
the West Coast by voter approval of the Oregon Death membership. After the announcement of ACA’s involve-
With Dignity Act (1995), its subsequent legislative chal- ment in the amicus curiae brief in that organization’s

James R. Rogers is an assistant professor in the Collaborative Program in Counseling Psychology and the Counselor Education and Supervision programs,
and Christine M. Gueulette is a Marriage and Family doctoral student, both in the Department of Counseling and Special Education at The University of
Akron, Akron, Ohio. Jodi Abbey-Hines is a licensed professional counselor working in community mental health in Golden, Colorado. Jolynn V. Carney is
an assistant professor in the Department of Counseling at Youngstown State University, Youngstown, Ohio. James L. Werth, Jr., is an assistant professor in the
Collaborative Program in Counseling Psychology, Department of Psychology, The University of Akron. This research was supported by a grant from the
University Research Council of the School of Graduate Studies of Youngstown State University. The authors thank Angel Olsen and Jeanette Higgins for their
assistance in the data collection and coding activities related to this research. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to James R. Rogers,
Department of Counseling and Special Education, Carroll Hall 127, The University of Akron, Akron, OH 44325-5007 (e-mail: jrrogers@uakron.edu).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 365


R o g e r s , G u e u l e t t e , A b b e y - H i n e s , C a r n e y, a n d W e r t h , J r.

professional newsletter (Morrissey, 1997a), members of concept of rational suicide that, collectively, included the
ACA registered protests through letters to the editor (e.g., following components:
Battersby, 1997; Duggan, 1997). Although some litera-
ture does exist related to the attitudes of psychotherapists (1) The person considering suicide has an unremitting hopeless
condition. Hopeless conditions include, but are not necessarily
toward the issue of rational suicide (e.g., Werth, 1996; Werth limited to, terminal illnesses, severe physical pain, and/or psy-
& Cobia, 1995: Werth & Liddle, 1994), there has been no chological pain, physically or mentally debilitating and/or de-
research investigating these attitudes specifically for mem- teriorating conditions, or quality of life no longer acceptable to
bers of a counseling association, and thus no empirically the individual.
(2) The person makes the decision as a free choice (i.e., is not
based support for the position (i.e., “Brief–WSPA,” 1996)
pressured by others to choose suicide).
supported by ACA. (3) The person has engaged in a sound decision-making process.
Given that the Supreme Court has left the question of This process should include the following:
the legalization of physician-assisted suicide up to the indi- (a) Consultation with a mental health professional who can
vidual state legislatures and the prediction of legislative make an assessment of psychological competence.
(b) Nonimpulsive consideration of all of the alternatives.
experts that the question of legalization will continue to be (c) Consideration of the congruence of the act with one’s
argued at the state level in the foreseeable future, it seems personal values.
important to have an empirical sense of where counselors (d) Consideration of the impact of the act on significant others.
stand on the underlying issue of rational suicide. The pur- (e) Consultation with objective others (e.g., medical and
pose of this study was to begin to develop a knowledge religious professionals) and with significant others. (p. 238)
base regarding counselor attitudes toward rational suicide
by modifying the methodology used by Werth and Liddle In tandem, these reports have provided some sense of the
(1994) and Werth and Cobia (1995) in their investigations attitudes of APA member psychotherapists toward the con-
of the attitudes of psychotherapists toward rational suicide cept of rational suicide and have resulted in an initial set of
(see also Werth, 1996). empirically based criteria for differentiating rational from
nonrational suicide. It remains an empirical question whether
similar attitudes exist among members of a counseling orga-
RATIONAL SUICIDE AND ATTITUDES OF MENTAL HEALTH nization. To address this question, we developed a modified
PROFESSIONALS version of the survey instrument used in Werth and Liddle
(1994), Werth and Cobia (1995), and Werth (1996) to in-
Although there has been some work investigating attitudes vestigate counselor attitudes toward rational suicide.
toward rational suicide in the sociological literature (Stack,
1999), with the exception of Werth and Liddle (1994), METHOD
Werth and Cobia (1995), and Werth (1996), this specific
Participants
issue as it relates to attitudes of mental health practitio-
ners has received little empirical attention in the published To investigate counselor attitudes toward the issue of ratio-
counseling literature. nal suicide, a random sample of 1,000 professional members
Werth and Liddle (1994; see also Werth, 1996) reported of the American Mental Health Counselors Association
the quantitative results of a survey of 400 members of (AMHCA) were mailed the survey instruments and invited
Division 29 (Psychotherapy) of the American Psychological to participate in the study. AMHCA members were chosen
Association. On the basis of their 50% response rate, these with the assumption that members of this organization (i.e.,
authors concluded that psychotherapists were “differen- specialty area) are more likely than some types of counselors
tially accepting of suicidal ideation” (Werth & Liddle, 1994, to work in settings where they would encounter suicidal in-
p. 440) and reported differing amounts of preventative dividuals and would have professional experiences to inform
action as a function of contextual circumstances. Specifically, their reported attitudes (see Rogers, 1996, for a discussion of
their results suggested that psychotherapists were most the importance of participant experience in assessing right-
accepting of suicidal ideation and least prone to inter- to-die attitudes). Usable responses were obtained from 241
vene in cases of terminal illness and were progressively (24%) of the surveyed professionals representing 40 states.
less accepting and more prone to intervene in situations Of the 241 respondents, 73% (176) were women, 93.4%
reflecting physical pain, psychological pain (e.g., depression), (225) reported their race as White, 17% had a doctoral-
and bankruptcy, respectively. These authors also reported level education, and 44% were trained at the master’s level.
that 81% of their sample indicated a belief in the concept In terms of discipline, 40.7% were professional counselors,
of rational suicide. 34.4% counseling psychologists, 16.2% social workers, and
In 1995, Werth and Cobia (see also Werth, 1996) pre- 8.71% clinical psychologists. Reported work settings included
sented the results of their analysis of qualitative data col- private practice (53.1%), community agencies (22.8%),
lected in the same survey of psychotherapists focused on hospitals (7.1%), academic settings (6.2%), and others
identifying “empirically based criteria for rational suicide” (10.8%). Sixty-six percent of the respondents were inde-
(p. 231). According to Werth and Cobia, 88% of the re- pendently licensed and the mean years of experience was
spondents in the qualitative data indicated a belief in the 12.24 (SD = 8.74), ranging from 0 to 44 years. Religious

366 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Suicide: An Empirical Investigation

affiliations were reported as Protestant (22.0%), Catholic of the respondents. There were no follow-up mailings or
(19.5%), Jewish (10.8%), Methodist (6.2%), Agnostic/Atheist other attempts to increase the response rate for the survey.
(5.8%), Lutheran (5.0%), and Other (30.7%) (see Table 1
for a precise breakdown of demographics; due to rounding, Materials
percentages may not total 100%).
Demographics. A one-page demographic questionnaire was
Procedure constructed to include a number of respondent factors that
could potentially influence mental health professionals’ atti-
The random sample of 1,000 professional members (i.e., tudes toward suicide. In addition to sex and race, the demo-
excluding student members) of AMHCA was obtained from graphic form collected information on participant religious
the organization’s national headquarters. Each member iden- affiliation, discipline, work setting, experience, and licensure.
tified in this process received a cover letter explaining the Finally, participants were asked to respond to the following
purpose of the study, the questionnaire materials, and a self- three questions: (1) Have you ever had a client attempt sui-
addressed, stamped envelope for return of the completed cide? (2) Have you ever had a client commit suicide? and
survey. Surveys were constructed to ensure the anonymity (3) Have you ever seriously considered suicide?
Survey. The survey instrument was adapted from the
TABLE 1 questionnaire used in Werth and Liddle (1994), Werth and
Cobia (1995), and Werth (1996). First, because our inter-
Sample Demographic and Informational Results est was not in counselors’ judgments of the personal char-
acteristics of the individual depicted in the vignette, we
Variable na % dropped a 10-item semantic differential scale from the
Sex original survey that focused on those characteristics. Sec-
Women 176 73.0 ond, on the basis of literature suggesting that attitudes
Men 65 27.0 vary as a function of the personal relevance of the issues
Race
White 225 93.4 being investigated (e.g., Ableson, 1988; Rogers, 1996), we
Other 16 6.6 added two items that progressively increased the personal
Religious Affiliation relevance of the issue of rational suicide to the respondents.
Catholic 47 19.5
Methodist 15 6.2
The first of these items changed the individual in the vignette
Protestant 53 22.0 from a client to a friend, and the second item asked the
Jewish 26 10.8 respondents to place themselves in the context of the scenario.
Agnostic/Atheist 14 5.8 For each of these new items as well as for the original client-
Lutheran 12 5.0
Other 74 30.7 focused item, respondents were asked to provide a narrative
Discipline rationale for their responses. Also, to investigate possible gen-
Clinical psychology 21 8.7 der effects, we varied the sex of the individual depicted in
Professional counseling 98 40.7
Counseling psychology 83 34.4
the case vignette. Finally, on the basis of Maltsberger (1994),
Social work 39 16.2 we added an item to investigate the effects of changing the
Primary Work Setting word rational to reasonable on the respondents’ attitudes
Community agency 55 22.8 toward the suicidal intent expressed in the vignette. These
Academic setting 15 6.2
Hospital 17 7.1 adaptations resulted in a survey instrument that was more
Private practice 128 53.1 clearly focused on the attitudes and perceptions that we
Other 26 10.8 were interested in investigating.
Licensure Our survey presented the following vignette describing
Independent 159 66.0
Licensed 46 19.1 the context and condition of an individual who has made a
Not licensed 36 14.9 decision to commit suicide. The sex of the individual de-
picted in the vignette was randomly varied.
Experienceb
Have you ever had a client attempt suicide?
Yes 170 72.0 John/Joan, a 45-year-old factory worker who is currently a client of
No 66 28.0 yours, has been suffering from severe physical pain related to ma-
Have you ever had a client commit suicide? lignant bone cancer. Despite having received a wide variety of thera-
Yes 68 28.7 pies, the cancer is spreading. John/Joan feels that both you and his/
No 169 71.3 her physicians have already done all you can and he/she has no
Have you ever seriously considered suicide? hope that his/her symptoms will be reduced. John/Joan is currently
Yes 48 20.0 experiencing a great deal of pain and is very upset over the fact
No 192 80.0 that his/her condition is draining the emotional and financial re-
sources of his/her family and friends. John/Joan feels that the qual-
Note. N = 241. ity of his/her life now is very poor and will only get worse. He/she
a
Number of participants responding to the open-ended questions also fears that he/she will be an increasingly large burden to his/
differed from one question to another. b Mean = 12.24 years, N = her loved ones. With the support of his/her family and friends, John/
237, SD = 8.74 years, Range = 0–44 years. Joan has decided to kill him-/herself.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 367


R o g e r s , G u e u l e t t e , A b b e y - H i n e s , C a r n e y, a n d W e r t h , J r.

Participants were asked to respond to the following three were clearly not in support of a decision to commit suicide,
quantitative questions based on their reading of the case citing moral and ethical positions as the basis for their
vignette using a 7-point rating scale: decision. An additional 40% (n = 94) of the responses were
supportive of a suicide decision citing rationality and client
1. As a professional, how much action would you autonomy as the primary considerations. The remaining 36%
take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/her- (n = 86) of the responses were either ambivalent in terms of
self? Anchors—“Nothing” (1) and “Everything/Any- the basis for their quantitative responses (n = 22) or wanted
thing including involuntary hospitalization” (7). more specification regarding issues of pain control and pal-
2. As a friend (in this case John/Joan is not a client), liative care (n = 40) or more information as to whether or
how much action would you take to prevent John/ not all other options had been exhausted (n = 24).
Joan from killing him-/herself? Anchors—“Nothing” For Item 2, “As a friend, how much action would you take
(1) and “Everything/Anything including steps that to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?” the total
may lead to involuntary hospitalization” (7). mean score was 3.38 (SD = 2.1). There were 239 responses
3. If you were in John/Joan’s circumstance, how viable to the question “What is the basis for your decision?” Six-
would you view suicide as an option? Anchors—“Not teen of those responses were ambiguous and dropped from
viable at all” (rated as 1) and “Extremely viable” (7). the analysis for a final total of 223 responses. Of the remain-
ing 223 responses, 16% (n = 36) were not supportive of a
After the first two questions, respondents were asked to
decision to commit suicide with issues of morality, hope for
present a rationale for their ratings.
condition improvement, and the impact of suicide on sig-
Finally, participants were asked to respond to three open-
nificant others as the most cited bases for their decision. Fifty-
ended items: nine percent (n = 132) were supportive of a decision to com-
1. Do you believe in the idea of “rational suicide?” Why mit suicide citing autonomy, hopeless conditions, pain, and
or why not? rationality as the primary considerations. The remaining 25%
2. Under what circumstances would you consider a (n = 55) of the responses were ambivalent or in some way
decision to commit suicide to be rational? wanting to have more information than provided in the vi-
3. If the word “rational” in the above questions was re- gnette to support the response to the quantitative item.
placed with the term “reasonable,” would that affect Finally, for Item 3, “If you were in John/Joan’s circum-
your responses? If so, in what ways? stance, how viable would you view suicide as an option?”
the total sample mean score was 4.79 (SD = 2.3). Narrative
responses were not solicited for this item.
RESULTS To explore possible differences in responses to the three items
Seventy-one percent (n = 170) of the survey respondents as a function of respondent characteristics, a multivariate
reported having at least one client who had made a suicide analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed with each
attempt. In addition, 28% (n = 68) reported having at least characteristic as the independent variable and the three items
one client who committed suicide, and 20% (n = 48) indi- as the dependent variables. MANOVA results were not statisti-
cated that they themselves had seriously considered suicide. cally significant as a function of the respondent characteristics,
Responses to the three quantitative questions were analyzed with the exception of religious affiliation (Pillai’s Trace = .218,
first for the total sample and then as a function of respon- F = 2.99, df = 18, 687, p < .0001, η2 = .074) and vignette sex
dent sex, race, religious affiliation, discipline, primary work (Pillai’s Trace = .047, F = 3.85, df = 3, 237, p < .01, η2 = .047).
setting, vignette sex, having clients who have attempted sui- Religious affiliation. Follow-up analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
cide, having clients who have committed suicide, and having with religious affiliation as the independent variable revealed
personally considered suicide. Qualitative responses were statistically significant differences on all three quantitative items.
coded by two researchers who worked to achieve consensus Table 2 lists the means and standard deviations for the three
on the coding categories. In cases in which consensus could quantitative items as a function of religious affiliation. For Item
not be reached in categorizing the responses as either against 1, “As a professional, how much action would you take to pre-
rational suicide, in support of rational suicide, or ambivalent, vent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?” differences as a func-
responses were dropped from the analyses. tion of religious affiliation were found at the p = .0002 level
(F = 4.62, df = 6, 234). Tukey’s post hoc standardized range
tests were used in the follow-up analyses to control the experi-
Quantitative Data
ment-wise error rate. Tukey’s procedure is a conservative post
For Item 1, “As a professional, how much action would you hoc test analogous to a Bonferroni correction with alpha di-
take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?” the vided by the number of all possible paired comparisons
total sample mean was 3.89 (SD = 2.1). There were 245 (Marascuilo & Serlin, 1988). Tukey’s post hoc standardized
individual responses to the question “What is the basis for range tests indicated statistically significant (p < .05) differ-
your decision?” Eight of those responses were ambiguous ences between Catholic and Jewish respondents, between
and were dropped from the analysis for a final total of 237 Catholic and Agnostic/Atheist respondents, and between Meth-
responses. Of the remaining 237 responses, 24% (n = 57) odist and Jewish respondents with the Catholic and Methodist

368 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Suicide: An Empirical Investigation

TABLE 2

Means and Standard Deviations for the Three Quantitative Items as a Function of Religious Affiliation
Religious Item 1 Item 2 Item 3
Affiliation M SD M SD M SD n

Catholic 4.91 a 2.1 4.77a 2.1 3.45 a 2.4 47


Methodist 4.93 1 2.1 4.12 2.4 4.27 2.7 15
Protestant 3.76 2.1 2.91b 2.0 4.83 b 2.2 53
Jewish 2.85 b2 1.9 2.50b 1.6 5.96 b 1.2 26
Agnostic/Atheist 2.79 b 1.6 2.57b 1.6 5.57 b 2.2 14
Lutheran 4.42 1.8 3.17 1.6 4.33 2.2 12
Other 3.88 2.1 3.17b 2.0 5.16 b 2.3 69

Note. Within columns, means with nonsimilar subscripts and superscripts are significantly different at the p ≤ .05 level.

respondents scoring higher (i.e., adopting a more preventa- in the idea of “rational suicide? Why or why not?” Of the
tive stance) than the Jewish and Agnostic/Atheist respondents. participants, 81% (n = 184) indicated some level of belief
For Item 2 (“As a friend, how much action would you in the concept of rational suicide. Of the remaining 42 re-
take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?”), dif- spondents (one participant’s response could not be inter-
ferences were statistically significant at the p < .0001 level preted) who indicated that they did not believe in the con-
(F = 6.12, df = 6, 234). Again, Tukey’s post hoc standard- cept of rational suicide, 22 responses indicated that the belief
ized range tests indicated statistically significant (p < .05) was grounded in their moral values. The remaining 20 indi-
differences between the means of the Catholic respondents viduals either gave no rationale for their position (n = 14)
and those of the Protestant, Jewish, Agnostic/Atheist, and or in some form restated their belief that suicide ipso facto
Other respondents with the Catholic respondents scoring could not be rational.
higher (i.e., adopting a more preventative stance). The 184 participants indicating some level of belief in
For Item 3 (“If you were in John/Joan’s circumstance, how rational suicide provided 195 responses. Of these, 33 re-
viable would you view suicide as an option?”), differences were sponses indicated a degree of qualified ambivalence; that
statistically significant at p < .0001 (F = 4.92, df = 6, 234). is, these respondents reported a belief in rational suicide
Similarly, Tukey’s post hoc standardized range tests indi- but qualified their reported belief in ways that could not
cated statistically significant (p < .05) mean differences between be unambiguously coded as either supportive or
the Catholic respondents and the Protestant, Jewish, Agnostic/ unsupportive. An additional 54 responses repeated their
Atheist, and Other respondents, with Catholic respondents scor- affirmative position but did not provide any further ex-
ing lower (i.e., seeing suicide as a less viable personal option). planation for their response. The remaining responses were
Vignette sex. Follow-up ANOVAs with vignette sex as the categorized based on the criteria identified by Werth and
independent variable revealed statistically significant dif- Cobia (1995; see also Werth, 1996). Within this framework,
ferences for Item 3 only (F = 9.61, df = 1, 239, p < .002). In 66 responses clearly mirrored Criterion 1: “The person con-
this analysis, the mean score for respondents exposed to sidering suicide has an unremitting hopeless condition.” Of
the vignette depicting a woman was higher (M = 5.22, SD these responses, 26 cited an unacceptable quality of life, 20
= 2.2, n = 126) than the mean for the respondents pre- cited severe physical pain, 19 cited terminal illness, and 1 cited
sented with the vignette depicting a man (M = 4.31, SD = physical disability as providing an acceptable rationale for
2.4, n = 115). In an attempt to explore this finding further, suicide. An additional 27 responses fit within Criterion 2:
we conducted an ANOVA with respondent sex and vignette “The person makes the decision as a free choice.” Fifteen
sex as main effects and tested the interaction term to inves- responses fit within Criterion 3: “The person has engaged in
tigate a possible gender effect. The interaction term in the a sound decision-making process.” Of the 15 responses
model was not statistically significant (F = .61, df = 1, 239, matching Criterion 3, 13 cited a consideration of all pos-
p = .435). Thus, the meaning of this result is unclear given sible alternatives, 1 cited congruence with personal values,
that Item 3 asked the respondents to rate the viability of a and 1 cited a consideration of the impact on others as
rational suicide option for themselves. Any guess as to why defining standards.
those responses would vary systematically as a function of For the second open-ended question (“Under what circum-
the sex of the person depicted in the vignette would be stances would you consider a decision to suicide to be ratio-
pure speculation given the data. nal?”), there were 349 responses relevant to the question. Of
those responses, 38 indicated that there were no circum-
Open-Ended Questions stances that would lead to a rational decision to suicide. The
remaining responses are listed in Table 3 by category.
Two-hundred twenty-seven individuals provided 234 re- Finally, 41 individuals responded to one or both of the
sponses to the first open-ended question, “Do you believe questions “If the word ‘rational’ in the above question was

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 369


R o g e r s , G u e u l e t t e , A b b e y - H i n e s , C a r n e y, a n d W e r t h , J r.

TABLE 3 research that has consistently shown that about one fourth
to one third of nonpsychiatric mental health professionals
Responses to “Under What Circumstances Would can expect to have at least one client die by suicide during
You Consider a Decision to Commit Suicide to their counseling career (Chemtob, Bauer, Hamada, Pelowski,
Be Rational?” & Muraoka, 1989; Werth, 1996).
One surprising finding was that one fifth of our sample
Number of
Category Responses of counselors reported that they had personally considered
suicide as an option at some point in their lives. Although
Terminal illness 85 there may be additional implications of this last finding, for
Severe physical pain 53 our purposes, these data provide further validation for our
Nonimpulsive consideration of alternatives 37
Unacceptable quality of life 36
decision to survey members of AMHCA and strengthen
Consultation with significant others 23 our confidence in the responses to our survey as reflecting
Consultation with other professionals 20 the respondents’ attitudes. Additional support for the va-
Physical debilitation 12 lidity of the responses to the survey can be interpreted from
Clear state of mind 11
Free choice 10 our results related to religious affiliation. As indicated in
Consultation with mental health professional 9 Table 2, attitudes toward rational suicide reported on the
Severe psychological pain 6 three quantitative items evidenced a somewhat predictable
Congruence with values 6
Mental disability 3
pattern regarding religious doctrine (Anderson & Caddell,
1993; Rogers, 1996; Ross & Kaplan, 1993–94). In general,
Total Responses 311 Catholic respondents were least accepting of the concept
of rational suicide across the three items, and religious af-
filiation responses varied as a function of a belief that one’s
replaced with the term ‘reasonable,’ would that affect your life belongs to God.
responses? If so, in what ways?” Only 51% (n = 21) indi- In terms of the quantitative results, the average response
cated that the change in terminology would have affected to Item 1 (“As a professional, how much action would you
their responses to the previous question. Of those respon- take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?”) sug-
dents, 17 indicated that they would be less accepting of the gests that these professionals would not apply a great deal
decision to commit suicide and 1 indicated that he or she of action. The mean in this study of 3.89 is between 3.41
would be more favorable if the qualifier “rational” were re- (Werth & Liddle, 1994) and 4.65 (Werth, 1996) obtained
placed by “reasonable.” The other 3 respondents indicated in two studies of psychologists. Given that the present mean
that the change in terminology would affect their responses, is near the middle of the scale, this finding clearly indicates
but the direction of the effect could not be determined a moderate acceptance of rational suicide. In fact, the re-
from the written narratives. The remaining 20 respondents lated qualitative data that were generated indicated that
did not indicate whether the change in terminology would only 24% of the respondents were unambiguously opposed
affect their responses but provided narratives discussing to the concept of rational suicide and 40% were clearly sup-
their perceptions of the differences between the two terms portive. With 76% of the respondents indicating some level
as they would relate to a decision to commit suicide. In of acceptance of the concept of rational suicide in working
most of those narratives, “rational” was seen as the more with an individual in a professional context, it seems that
objective and definable term, whereas “reasonable” was seen there may be support for the position taken by ACA re-
as more subjective and emotionally laden. garding the 1997 Supreme Court decisions on the issue of
physician-assisted suicide. It would be interesting to explore
DISCUSSION how these moderately accepting attitudes for rational sui-
cide in a professional relationship fit for these mental health
The purpose of the present study was to provide empiri- professionals in relationship to the relatively unambiguous
cally based data related to attitudes of counselors toward guidelines embedded in the ACA Code of Ethics (ACA, 1995;
rational suicide. Our decision to survey members of see Werth, 1999b, for an analysis of how the ACA ethics
AMHCA with the assumption that members of this coun- code can be reconciled with ACA’s decision to sign onto
seling organization would have professional experiences to the amicus brief). Of course, one possibility is that the ex-
inform their reported attitudes was supported in the data, pressed attitudes reflected in these data do not translate
which indicated that 71% of the respondents reported into actual professional behavior (see Rogers, 1996 for a
working with clients who had attempted suicide and 28% discussion of the attitude/behavior issue as it relates to right
reported having at least one client who committed suicide. to die concerns).
These high percentages of experience with suicide ideation Similarly, for Item 2 (“As a friend, how much action would
and completion indicate that training in the detection of you take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/herself?”),
suicidality and interventions to prevent suicidal deaths the average response of this sample of counselors indicated
should be a part of counselor training programs (see a moderate acceptance of rational suicide. Although the av-
Westefeld et al., 2000). The 28% figure adds to the body of erage score on Item 2 was indicative of slightly less action

370 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Rational Suicide: An Empirical Investigation

in preventing suicide when the individual was a friend than criteria reported by Werth and Cobia (1995) and Werth
when the individual was a client, the qualitative data sug- (1996) and, thus, provide some measure of support for these
gested that only 16% of the respondents were unambigu- classification schemes. In addition, the most typical circum-
ously against the idea of rational suicide in this situation stances under which these counselors would consider a
compared with 24% when the individual was a client. Thus, decision to commit suicide to be rational were reported as
84% of the respondents reported at least some level of sup- (a) terminal illness, (b) severe physical pain, (c) a nonimpulsive
port for rational suicide, with 59% unambiguously support- consideration of alternatives, and (d) an unacceptable quality
ive when the individual involved was a friend. of life.
Finally for the quantitative data, the average response Although the findings are consistent with previous
to Item 3 (“If you were in John/Joan’s circumstances, how research, they must still be considered preliminary because
viable would you view suicide as an option?”) indicated of the limitations associated with this study. The relatively
that, in circumstances similar to those depicted in the sce- low response rate is a concern; however, given the combi-
nario, rational suicide would be viewed as a moderately viable nation of funding restraints and the desire to avoid raising
personal option. It is intriguing that as the scenario moved fears about the loss of anonymity by using tracking num-
from depicting client, to friend, to self, rational suicide seemed bers to allow the targeting of nonrespondents, the rate may
to gain in acceptability for these mental health professionals. be as good as could be expected. Another limitation is the
It is interesting that the responses to the quantitative fact that only AMHCA members were surveyed. Although
items in the survey instrument did not vary significantly the demographic results indicate that the participants were
as a function of respondent sex, race, discipline, work set- experienced counselors, nonmembers of this counseling
ting, experience, licensure, on the basis of whether the association may differ systematically from those who choose
respondents had clients who attempted or committed suicide, to join AMHCA. Thus, it is possible that the current find-
or as a function of the clinicians’ history of personal ings, even though empirical in nature, are limited in terms
suicidality. The two areas for which the responses did vary of generalizability.
significantly were religious affiliation and vignette sex. However, notwithstanding these limitations, the overall
As with the total sample results for the three attitude findings of this study are important for the profession in
items, the religious affiliation data generally suggest that as that the responses of the sample population indicated a
the issue of rational suicide moves across the contexts of moderate (based on ratings) acceptance of rational suicide
client, friend, and self, attitudes become more accepting. for clients and for friends, and an even greater acceptance
These data seem to make intuitive sense especially because of rational suicide for the therapists themselves. The re-
they may reflect a progressive move from the role of the sults indicating support for rational suicide are particularly
professional counselor (where the profession emphasizes interesting in light of the ACA (1995) ethical guidelines
protecting clients from inflicting harm on others or self) to that promote clinical involvement, at all costs, in instances
a personal value position. From the professional role as re- in which suicide is a possible outcome (see also Werth,
flected in the item “As a professional, how much action 1999b). Again, these findings provide valuable insight into
would you take to prevent John/Joan from killing him-/ the beliefs and attitudes of therapists regarding rational sui-
herself?” however, these results may indicate the influence cide and may be useful for ACA to consider as individual
of religious doctrine on therapists’ judgment processes re- states continue to grapple with the issue in the future.
lated to rational suicide communications. This suggests that
counselors choosing to work with individuals about has-
tened death decisions such as rational suicide may need to REFERENCES
engage in personal value clarification exercises in this area Ableson, R. P. (1988). Conviction. American Psychologist, 43, 267–275.
to ensure that they provide appropriate unbiased services American Counseling Association. (1995). Code of ethics and standards
to their clientele (Fenn & Ganzini, 1999; Werth, 1996). of practice. Alexandria, VA: Author.
The 81% affirmative response to the question “Do you Anderson, J. G., & Caddell, D. P. (1993). Attitudes of medical profes-
sionals toward euthanasia. Social Science Medicine, 37, 105–114.
believe in the idea of “rational suicide?” provides some Aulbach, P. (1997). Attitudes of psychologists in regard to prevailing argu-
empirical support for the ACA’s decision to support the ments for and against rational suicide as an option for the terminally ill
WSPA amicus curie brief to the Supreme Court. It is also (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Spalding University, Louisville, KY.
consistent with the levels of support for rational suicide Battersby, A. (1997, April). Physician-assisted suicide [Letter to the edi-
found in research with psychologists (77%, Aulbach, 1997; tor]. Counseling Today, p. 4.
Battin, M. P. (1994). The least worst death: Essays in bioethics on end-of-
81%, Werth & Liddle, 1994; 86%, Werth, 1996) as well as life. New York: Oxford University Press.
support for physician-assisted suicide found among psy- Brief of the Washington State Psychological Association (WSPA); The Ameri-
chologists and psychiatrists in Oregon (78% of psycholo- can Counseling Association; The Association for Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual
gists, Fenn & Ganzini, 1999; 66% of psychiatrists, Ganzini, Issues in Counseling; and the Coalition of Mental Health Professionals Sup-
Fenn, Lee, Heintz, & Bloom, 1996) and social workers in porting Individual Self-Determination in Decisions to Hasten Death. (1996,
December). Filed with the Supreme Court of the United States, Octo-
British Columbia (78%, Ogden & Young, 1998). ber, 1996 term for Washington v. Glucksberg and Vacco v. Quill.
The qualitative data generated by participants to support Brody, H. (1998). Physician-assisted suicide in the courts: Moral equivalence,
their affirmative responses were easily classified within the double effect, and clinical practice. Minnesota Law Review, 82, 939–963.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 371


R o g e r s , G u e u l e t t e , A b b e y - H i n e s , C a r n e y, a n d W e r t h , J r.

Camus, A. (1955). The myth of Sisyphus and other essays. New York: Quill v. Vacco, 870 F. Supp. 78 (S.D.N.Y. 1994), rev’d, 80 F.3d 716 (2nd
Knopf. Circuit, 1996).
Chemtob, C. M., Bauer, G. B., Hamada, R. S., Pelowski, S. R., & Muraoka, M. Rogers, J. R. (1996). Assessing right to die attitudes: A conceptually guided
Y. (1989). Patient suicide: Occupational hazard for psychologists and measurement model. Journal of Social Issues, 52, 63–84.
psychiatrists. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 20, 294–300. Rogers, J. R., & Britton, P. J. (1994). AIDS and rational suicide: A coun-
Compassion in Dying v. Washington, 79 F.3d 790 (9th Circuit 1996) (en banc). seling psychology perspective or a ride on the slippery slope. The Coun-
Duggan, D. D. (1997, March). Opposed to physician-assisted suicide seling Psychologist, 22, 171–178.
[Letter to the editor]. Counseling Today, p. 4. Ross, L. T., Kaplan, K. J. (1993-94). Life ownership orientation and atti-
Fenn, D. S., & Ganzini, L. (1999). Attitudes of Oregon psychologists tudes toward abortion, suicide, doctor-assisted suicide and capital
toward physician-assisted suicide. Professional Psychology: Research and punishment. Omega, 28, 17–30.
Practice, 30, 235–244. Stack, S. (1999). The influence of rational suicide on nonrational sui-
Ganzini, L., Fenn, D. S., Lee, M. A., Heintz, R. T., & Bloom, J. D. (1996). cide: A sociological analysis of attitudes. In J. L. Werth, Jr. (Ed.), Con-
Attitudes of Oregon psychiatrists toward physician-assisted suicide. temporary perspectives on rational suicide (pp. 41–47). Philadelphia:
American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1469–1475. Taylor & Francis.
Maltsberger, J. T. (1994). Calculated risk-taking in the treatment of sui- Werth, J. L., Jr. (1992). Rational suicide and AIDS: Consider-
cidal patients: Ethical and legal problems. In A. A. Leenaars, J. T. ations for the psychotherapist. The Counseling Psychologist, 20,
Maltsberger, & R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Treatment of suicidal people (pp. 645–659.
195–205). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. Werth, J. L., Jr. (1996). Rational suicide? Implications for mental health
Marascuilo, L. A., & Serlin, R. C. (1988). Statistical methods for the social professionals. Washington, DC: Taylor and Francis.
and behavioral sciences. New York: Freeman. Werth, J. L., Jr. (Ed.). (1999a). Contemporary perspectives on rational
Morrissey, M. (1997a). ACA, AGLBIC file brief supporting physician- suicide. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.
assisted suicide. Counseling Today (On-line), 1–4. Available: http:// Werth, J. L., Jr. (1999b). Mental health professionals and assisted death:
www.counseling.org/ctonline/archives/suicide297.htm Perceived ethical obligations and proposed guidelines for practice.
Morrissey, M. (1997b, March). Editor’s note. Counseling Today, p. 4. Ethics and Behaviors, 9, 159–183.
Ogden, R. D., & Young, M. G. (1998). Euthanasia and assisted suicide: A Werth, J. L., Jr., & Cobia, D. C. (1995). Empirically based criteria for
survey of registered social workers in British Columbia. British Jour- rational suicide: A survey of psychotherapists. Suicide and Life-
nal of Social Work, 28, 161–175. Threatening Behavior, 25, 231–240.
Oregon Death With Dignity Act. (1995). Or. Rev. Stat. 127.800–127.995. Werth, J. L., Jr., & Liddle, B. J. (1994). Psychotherapists’ attitudes to-
Previn, M. P. (1996). Assisted suicide and religion: Conceptions of the ward suicide. Psychotherapy, 31, 440–448.
sanctity of human life. The Georgetown Law Journal, 84, 589–616. Westefeld, J. S., Range, L. M., Rogers, J. R., Maples, M. R., Bromley, J. L,
Quill, T. E. (1993). Death and dignity: Making choices and taking charge. & Alcorn, J. (2000). Suicide: An overview. The Counseling Psychologist,
New York: Norton. 28, 445–510.

372 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents

Linda L. Viney, Rachael M. Henry, and Joanne Campbell

The impact of personal construct and psychodynamic group work on the psychosocial functioning of offender adolescents was
evaluated. The repeated measures design involved 3 data collections. Data from 102 participants included measures of 5 pairs of
maturational processes and 5 psychological states. Group work was effective immediately after terminating treatment in increas-
ing helpful maturational processes and in reducing the less helpful maturational processes. The group work was also effective to
some extent in reducing less helpful psychological states. The implications of this research for future interventions and its evalu-
ation are discussed.

T
he aim of this research was to evaluate the im- Erikson asserted that human development is a life span
pact of psychological interventions on offender process passing through eight stages in a fixed order, each
adolescents. The significance of this research lies stage presenting conflicts that become crises. The resolu-
in the need for effective interventions for ado- tion of each conflict is also seen as the basis for “tackling”
lescents who have repeatedly committed crimes the next developmental stage. Later theorists dealing with
against property and people (Darden, Gazda, & Ginter, 1996; adolescence have focused on identity foreclosure and dif-
Fine, Forth, Gilbert, & Haley, 1991; Quay, 1987). These fusion (Marcia, 1980), symbiosis and differentiation (Blos,
interventions should enhance the maturational functioning 1979), and whether the balance that is maintained at this
and improve the psychological states of these adolescents. stage is imperial or interpersonal (Kegan, 1983). Only five
Offender adolescents, being susceptible to peer pressure, are of the Eriksonian stages are seen as relevant to the psy-
particularly responsive to interventions that involve peers chosocially immature adolescents of this study. The levels
(Zimpfer, 1992). Group work may also be the preferred of maturity of the participants are to be assessed in terms
intervention of offender adolescents because they frequently of whether they are interacting according to Erikson’s
operate in gangs (i.e., to make plans, finalize decisions; meanings of trust, autonomy, initiative, industry, and af-
Macedo, 1955). The important social processes of group work filiation, and whether they have been able to give up, to
for these adolescents include building trust, testing the sta- some extent, the meanings of mistrust, constraint, hesi-
bility of their environments, settling struggles for leadership, tancy, inferiority, and isolation.
exploring interpersonal issues, extending self-understanding, Evaluation studies of group work (cognitive behavioral,
and accepting responsibility for self (Leader, 1991; MacLennan behavioral, and multicomponent) with such adolescents
& Dies, 1992). Group work was therefore judged appropriate yielded encouraging results. Offender adolescents who had
for the interventions of this research. group work have showed improvement in self-esteem
Previous research has shown that the psychosocial func- (Fashinger & Harris, 1987), problem solving (Hains,
tioning of offender adolescents is poor, placing them at risk of Herrman, & Balker, 1986), school achievement (Fashinger
major problems. These adolescents have been shown to have & Harris, 1987), empathy with and support of others (Car-
relatively high levels of uncertainty, anxiety, depression, and penter, 1984; Darden et al., 1996; Fashinger & Harris, 1987;
anger (Burton, Foy, Bwanausi, & Johnson, 1994; Coolidge, Madonna & Caswell, 1991) and reoffending on release
Reilman, Lee, & Cass, 1992). High levels of traumatic stress (Fashinger & Harris, 1987; Larson, 1990; Leeman, Gibbs, &
have also been reported in this population (Burton et al., 1994). Fuller, 1993) in contrast with offender adolescents who did
The positive affect of the participants is rarely investigated in not participate in group work. Also, a meta-analysis of nine
outcome research but is included here to tap into their evaluation studies that have compared the effects of indi-
strengths and resources, as well as their distress. vidual and group interventions has indicated consistently
There is also evidence that offender adolescents’ ability better effects for group than for individual interventions
to cope with negative emotional states is influenced by the with adolescents, including offender adolescents, but no such
levels of maturity of those adolescents (Erikson, 1959). differential effects occur with children (Tillitski, 1990).

Linda L. Viney is a personal construct therapist and researcher and was director of Clinical Psychology Training Programs at the University of Wollongong,
Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia, when this article was written. Rachael M. Henry is a psychodynamic therapist and researcher and was director of
the Graduate Diploma of Psychodynamic Psychotherapy at the University of Wollongong. Joanne Campbell is a psychologist in New South Wales, Australia.
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Linda L. Viney, Department of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Northfields Avenue, Wollongong,
NSW 2500, Australia (e-mail: linda_viney@uow.edu.au).

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 373


V i n e y, H e n r y, a n d C a m p b e l l

Hoag and Burlingame (1997), using a set of meta-analyses, the ability of acting out adolescents to understand one an-
concluded that group work was effective for adolescents. other (Jackson, 1990).
The forms of group work to be tested, personal construct
and psychodynamic group work, were those that were se- PSYCHODYNAMIC GROUP WORK WITH OFFENDER ADOLESCENTS
lected as alternatives to the prevailing cognitive behavioral
and behavioral group work (e.g., Carpenter, 1984; Fine et According to theory, with the onset of puberty, there is an
al., 1991; Larson, 1990) for many reasons. They were seen intensification of sexual impulses, producing a regression
to be sufficiently complex to do justice to the psychosocial to more immature stages of development and a return of
functioning of offender adolescents (Klein, 1957; Neimeyer, early infantile impulses (Klein, 1932). Earlier anxieties and
1993). These approaches have a wide range of strategies other negative emotions are aroused as earlier unresolved
with which to assess clients, the assessment presented here conflicts threaten to reemerge. The defensive processes of
focusing on both the psychological and the social processes projection and splitting are readily apparent. Both of these
(Klein, 1948; Neimeyer, 1993). They provide the benefits developments contribute to adolescents’ struggles to achieve
of acknowledging the role of unconscious processes in the their sense of identity (the central task of adolescence), as
functioning of adolescents and of the history of adolescents does the young person’s attempt to reestablish the disturbed
as important to their present functioning (Klein, 1948; emotional equilibrium (Klein, 1957). It seems reasonable to
Neimeyer, 1993). Finally, both approaches also take the assume that these struggles will be particularly severe for
history of the therapists into account so that the concepts offender adolescents because it is more likely that they
of transference and countertransference are used by both will have more unresolved losses and conflicts and less
of them (Klein, 1957; Viney, 1996). These concepts help to well developed ego mechanisms of coping and defense. Psychody-
greatly strengthen the understanding of group work pro- namic group work provides opportunities for understanding
cesses with offender adolescents. the complex emotional episodes making up group life (Bion,
1961). How the group is used, whether as “waste bin” or
PERSONAL CONSTRUCT GROUP WORK WITH OFFENDER “creative container,” will determine whether a developmental
transformation to a higher level of organization is achieved
ADOLESCENTS (Bion, 1970). Through interpretation, it is possible for the
Adolescents, as do others, continually try to predict and leaders to demonstrate the following in the here and now:
control their worlds through interpretation and anticipa- the emotional attitudes the group has to individuals; the
tion (Kelly, 1955). For this purpose, they create systems of members’ perceptions of the attitude of the group to each
constructs, or personal meanings. These meanings develop member; the attitude of the individual to the group; and the
through individual experiences and yet are subject to change. attitude of members to the leader or authority figure (Shapiro,
Because of the limited range of the meaning systems of Zimmer, Berkowitz, & Shapiro, 1963). These processes of
adolescents, their meanings about self can be invalidated by demonstration and interpretation facilitate integration and
others. Anxiety, anger, and threat may then result (Viney, reduce splitting and ego fragmentation.
1996). Such emotions may be especially strong for offender
adolescents whose illegal behavior often seems designed to AIM AND HYPOTHESES
elicit invalidation of meanings about self from others. More
mature adolescents who are functioning normally will be The intent of the present research was to build on the prom-
able to discriminate in terms of others and be responsive in ising research with offender adolescents by evaluating two
interacting with them. They will also be able to be commit- forms of group work using the rarely assessed but key matu-
ted to and forgiving with others and to respect their own rational processes and psychological states of the participants.
intimate relationships (Leitner & Pfenninger, 1994). Poorly The purpose of this research was to evaluate the effects
functioning and less mature adolescents will develop fewer of group work on offender adolescents. We investigated the
of these capacities and develop them to a lesser extent. Their following hypotheses.
problematic meanings can be changed through structured
group work, which provides a laboratory in which these prob- 1. Offender adolescents who experience group work com-
lematic meanings about people can be tested, with different pared with those who have no group work will show greater
types of structure providing different experiments for them immediate improvement, in terms of the following:
(Kelly, 1955). The validating or confirming climate of the a. Five pairs of helpful and less helpful maturational
group minimizes, most importantly, the threat to the most processes——trust and mistrust, autonomy and constraint,
influential meanings about self of the members during this initiative and hesitancy, industry and inferiority, and affilia-
experimentation (Viney, 1996). It should also reduce their tion and isolation
anxiety and anger because more of their meanings are b. Five psychological states——uncertainty, anxiety, depres-
effective in predicting and controlling their personal and sion, anger and good feelings (positive affects).
interpersonal worlds. Personal construct group work has 2. Offender adolescents who experience group work will
proved effective with a range of goals and with clients of show more gains 6 months after group work than those who
different ages (Winter, 1992). For example, it increased have no group work.

374 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents

Tests of some secondary hypotheses about the impact of 1999) because higher structure of such groups has been
participant sex, race, and language use and group leader sex linked with more therapeutic movement (G. J. Neimeyer
and age on the effects of the group work were also to be & Merluzzi, 1981). The tasks used aimed to extend the range
carried out. of interpretations of the offenders. Tasks were selected ini-
tially by the leaders, but, as the group developed, they were
METHOD often selected jointly by leaders and group members. Such
tasks included the interpersonal transaction group exercise
Participants and Design (Landfield & Rivers, 1975; R. A. Neimeyer, 1988), involv-
ing responses to some carefully chosen questions: “Who am
One hundred and two 14- to 18-year-olds volunteered to
I?” “How are my expectations of the opposite sex like their
participate in the research in three juvenile justice centers.
expectations of ours, and how are they different?” “How
The offences with which adolescents in the juvenile justice
can I give help to others and seek help from them?” “When
center had been successfully charged were all repeated of-
do I feel safe, and when do I not?” and “What would I most
fences. The offences were breaking and entering (39%),
like to say to each group member?” Yet another structuring
unlawful possession and driving of a vehicle (34%), break-
task involved role play of common but problematic interac-
ing and entering with a weapon (19%), and attacks against
tions with other adolescents, such as negotiating a sexual
persons (8%). Of the sample, 63% were male and 37% were
interaction, and then one that is safe from sexually transmit-
female. Ages were 14 years (21%), 15 years (27%), 16 years
ted diseases. Such an enactment, followed by role reversal,
(38%), 17 years (10%), and 18 years (4%). Sixteen percent
aided in helping group members to better understand the
were aboriginal. Nineteen percent reported a language other
meanings used by their potential sexual partners in this area,
than English being spoken at home. The recruitment rate
as well as their own. In the second to last group session,
was 91%. The participants were assigned randomly in this
members created, alone or jointly, a certificate to represent
design to one of two conditions: group work (personal con-
what they had learned from the group.
struct/psychodynamic) and control with no group work.
The psychodynamic group work. The psychodynamic group
work used an unstructured discussion or activity group (Henry,
Groups 1996; Henry, Wesley, Jones, Cohen, & Fairhall, 1997). This
Both types of group work occurred 1½ hours weekly for group work involved (a) rapid establishment of a working
10 weeks. Twelve 10-session groups were run, 6 of personal collaboration between members and leaders by providing struc-
construct group work for a third of the sample and 6 of ture and fostering group cohesiveness; (b) intense, focused
psychodynamic group work for another third of the sample. work on a carefully delineated problem area; (c) careful,
The group work was conducted to follow the manuals that interpretive attention to group process about the problem
have been published for the personal construct group work theme and to processes in the self; and (d) a productive
(Viney, Truneckova, Weekes, & Oades, 1999) and the psy- and manageable termination. Although technically unstruc-
chodynamic group work (Henry, 1996). Two group leaders tured, subtle but essential forms of structure were supplied
conducted each group, which had from 5 to 7 members. within the group. For example, both physical and psycho-
All prospective members were screened by appropriate ju- logical boundary control required careful management of
venile justice center staff to exclude any who were likely to what belonged psychologically inside or outside the group.
be physically violent in the group setting. The 16 leaders Drawing boundaries also included protecting time and com-
volunteered for training in the group work of their choice. municating its importance, as well as making the setting
They were all trained mental health professionals, including physically secure. Psychological continuity was provided
registered psychologists and drug and alcohol counselors, through elements of ritual (e.g., beginning and ending a
with appropriate degrees for their positions. All of them session and other procedural matters) to reduce the anxi-
were experienced in leading therapeutic groups, with years ety of members to a tolerable level where intense feelings,
of experience ranging from to 2 to 16 years. Ten were women impulses, and thoughts could be expressed in ways that
and 6 were men. Two of the leaders were aboriginal. Their were not overwhelming or inhibiting. Firm boundaries
ages ranged from 23 to 45 years. The leaders attended a fostered a sense of group cohesiveness in which mem-
1-day training workshop and read and discussed the train- bers could gain or regain a sense of kinship, which then
ing manuals, together with selected background material. allowed optimism and facilitated group and individual
Under the guidance of the manuals’ authors, they also had development. Working with the dynamics, process, and
2 hours of peer and expert supervision weekly for 14 weeks emotional realm of groups, rather than with content
in which conceptualization of the group processes in each alone, required the leaders to be clear about the instru-
session took place, followed by planning for the next ses- mental purposes of group work, to develop and refine an
sion. (Linda L. Viney supervised the personal construct understanding of each person in the group, to keep in touch
group work and Rachael M. Henry the psychodynamic with the dynamics of the group as a whole, to keep in
group work.) touch with their own feelings, to note their own behavior
The personal construct group work. The personal construct and its consequences, and to see connections between
group work used a structured group format (Viney et al., group and individual dynamics.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 375


V i n e y, H e n r y, a n d C a m p b e l l

Treatment validation. Given the more active overtly struc- with Erikson’s (1959) epigenetic tasks: Trust and Mistrust,
turing role of the personal construct leader compared with Autonomy and Constraint, Initiative and Hesitancy, In-
that of the psychodynamic leader that is apparent in our dustry and Inferiority, and Affiliation and Isolation. Follow-
accounts of these groups, we hypothesized that if these group ing are examples of statements from research participants
processes were representative of the two types of group work, that would be scored on the first pair of these scales: for
then members of the personal construct group would report the Trust scale—“I get on good with my mum now”; for the
a higher level of activity for leaders than would members Mistrust scale—“I try to stay out of the house when my
of the psychodynamic group but that there would be no stepfather is home.” A manual to facilitate the scoring of
differences for other contributions of the leaders in both these scales is available (Viney, Rudd, Grenyer, & Tyche,
groups. Samples of 5 participants in personal construct 1995). These CASPM scales have appropriate levels of
group work and another 5 in psychodynamic group work interjudge reliability, with a range of Pearson’s product–
were asked to report their impressions of the contributions moment correlation coefficients from .80 to .95 and no sig-
of leaders and other members to the group process. They nificant differences in the mean score sizes of independent
used a 3-point ranking scale to report on the activity level judges. They have good test–retest reliability ranging from
(choice of topic and talk) of leaders and members and of .65 to .71 (Viney & Tyche, 1985). There has been an exten-
their other contributions (their levels of thinking and feel- sive network of evidence for their construct validity. Most
ing). Agreement of rankings between each of the two sets important, they have shown the expected discriminations
of group members was considerable, averaging 88% for both for 813 participants of the age group 7 to 96, both in Aus-
types of group work. These hypotheses about the differen- tralia and in the United States, and with both White and
tial rankings of the two types of participants were supported. African American youth (Viney, 1987). The scales have been
shown to discriminate as predicted between children and
Outcome Measures adolescents of different ages (Wang & Viney, 1997) and cul-
Content analysis scales. The maturational processes and psy- tures in the People’s Republic of China and in Australia
chological states of these research participants were assessed (Wang & Viney, 1996). The scales have discriminated between
using content analysis scales applied to the following open- people with different health, religious, and employment
ended request (the responses were recorded with permis- status, as well as relating differentially as predicted to the
sion): “Now I’d like you to talk for a few minutes about measures of psychological states subsequently described
your life at the moment, the good things and the bad, what (Viney & Tyche, 1985). As measures of psychotherapy pro-
it’s like for you.” Content analysis scales, based on thematic cess, the scales have successfully predicted outcome
analysis, result in continuous measurement with normal- (Grenyer, Viney, & Luborsky, 1996).
ized distributions of scores (Gottschalk, Lolas, & Viney, Psychological states. The psychological states of the of-
1986; Viney, 1983a). Such scales were preferred to ques- fender adolescents were also assessed using content analy-
tionnaire and inventory measures of the variable assessed sis scales applied to answers to the same open-ended inter-
for these reasons. The content analysis of free responses in view question. Five content analysis scales were selected to
these scales overcomes many of their problems, such as the measure the psychological states of uncertainty, anxiety,
ambivalence of the emotions of offender adolescents and depression, and anger, as well as positive affect. The Cogni-
the effects of social desirability on their responses. It also tive Anxiety Scale (Viney & Westbrook, 1976) is a measure
avoids the use of selected words to identify for the partici- of the uncertainty that occurs when a person has difficulty
pant the area of interest to the researcher. Scales based on in making sense of their experiences, as for example in a
that content analysis also make possible an ethical, honest new, unfamiliar situation (e.g., “I was not sure what was
approach to offender adolescents while giving them the happening”). The Total Anxiety Scale (Gottschalk, 1982)
opportunity to deal with what is important to them. The consists of six subscales, measuring different types of anxi-
results from content analysis scales are less influenced by ety, examples of which follow for the first two of these
interviewer characteristics than are those from other types subscales: Death (“She is scared that I am going to get
of measures (Gottschalk, 1982). Content analysis scales are killed”), Bodily Mutilation (“I broke my knuckles trying to
also conceptually and methodologically compatible with both belt the crap out of somebody”). The Hostility In Scale
the personal construct and the psychodynamic approaches (Gottschalk, 1982) measures depression, focusing on self-
and have proved useful in other studies of adolescents (Viney, critical responses in which anger is directed to the self (“I
1983b, 1987; Wang & Viney, 1996, 1997). am not good at anything”); and the Hostility Out Scale as-
Maturational processes. Maturational processes were sesses anger directly expressed to others or to the external
measured by applying the Content Analysis Scales of Psy- world (“Most of the guys in charge are nerds”). The Posi-
chosocial Maturity (CASPM; Viney & Tyche, 1985) to answers tive Affect Scale (Westbrook, 1976) taps good, positive feel-
to the open-ended question provided. Five of the eight ings associated with happy, enjoyable experiences, which
pairs of positive and negative scales making up CASPM are contrasted with disruptive and disconcerting experiences
were used because of their appropriateness for this type of (“I enjoy hanging out with my friends”). The Cognitive
participant (i.e., offender adolescents). These scales assess Anxiety, Total Anxiety, Hostility In and Hostility Out, and
the helpful and less helpful meanings people use to deal Positive Affect Scales have appropriate levels of interjudge

376 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents

reliability, with a range of Pearson’s product–moment corre- The sample of offender adolescents had been assigned
lation coefficients from .71 to .99, and with no significant randomly to the two conditions of the treatment factor of
differences in mean score size for the two independent the design: group work and no-group-work. The similarity
judges. Good test–retest reliability would not be appro- of the distributions for age, for aboriginal status, and for
priate for these measures of transient psychological states. use of language other than English in the home across the
Evidence of their criterion and construct validity has been group work section of the design was apparent, although
provided by many studies, showing the correlation of the the no-group-work condition proved to have a few more
scale scores with self reports and psychiatric ratings of female adolescents than did the group work condition. Sex
these states and with other measures of the same psy- of the leader was balanced over the intervention cells of
chological states, including psychophysiological indices, the design. For the measures of maturational processes
discrimination of appropriate samples of research partici- (trust, autonomy, initiative, industry and affiliation on the
pants and situations, and responsivity to psychoactive drugs one hand, and mistrust, constraint, hesitancy, inferiority, and
(Gottschalk, 1982; Gottschalk et al., 1986; Viney, 1983a; isolation on the other) and psychological states (uncertainty,
Viney, Henry, Walker, & Crooks, 1989; Viney et al., 1994). anxiety, depression, anger, and good feelings), two-factor
repeated measures multivariate analyses of variance
Procedure (MANOVAs) were used to test for differences according
The data were collected during standardized interviews in to treatment versus no treatment (group). Because of the
the juvenile justice centers before and after the group work large number of follow-up comparisons, alpha levels were
and at 9-months follow-up. The interviews took ½ hour to 1 set at p = .01.
hour. One female interviewer, experienced in working with
adolescents, conducted all of the interviews. This interviewer Preliminary Analyses
was not one of the group leaders and was blind to the treat- The retention rate in the group from Time 1 to Time 2
ments to which the research participants had been assigned. three months later was high for such mobile populations at
The responses of the participants to the previously pro- 78%, leaving a subsample at Time 2 of 78. From Time 1 to
vided open-ended question in the interview were tran- Time 3 nine months later the retention rate was consider-
scribed and divided into clauses, each one containing an ably lower at 55%, leaving subsample of 55. This retention
active verb. The responses were content analyzed follow- rate was low, but not unexpected, for a sample of offend-
ing the standard scoring instructions for each of the con- ers. Only 17% of those who had dropped out of the group
tent analysis scales (Viney, 1983a; Viney et al., 1995). Each at Time 3 refused to participate. The other 83% had relo-
clause was compared with the sets of content analysis cat- cated to distant towns in New South Wales, Queensland,
egories, which provided verbal cues for each statement that or Victoria, distances that were not covered by the research
could be scored. Clauses matching these verbal cues were funding. When MANOVAs were conducted to test differ-
then summed, and the total score thus calculated was mul- ences between Time 1 scores for the two groups using each
tiplied by a weight representing the verbal productivity of of the four sets of their variables, no significant differences
each participant. The final total score consisted of the square were found. MANOVAs also tested for bias in these data
root of this multiplied score, the square root procedure by contrasting those who had dropped out of the group
designed to normalize each distribution of scores. The con- with those who had remained at Time 2 for their sets of
tent analysis was conducted by a trained and experienced demographic variables, together with their two sets of five
coder, a female psychologist, who remained blind to the CASPM scales and their set of five other content analysis
treatment group to which each research participant had scales measuring psychological states at Time 1. They all
been assigned. For a subsample of 30 transcripts, interjudge showed no significant differences. The same comparisons
reliability (Pearson’s product–moment correlation coeffi- were also made for those who had dropped out of the group
cients) with an independent blind scorer (also a female psy- with those who had remained at Time 3, on their Time 1
chologist) varied from .82 to .94 for the 15 content analy- data, with the same consistent lack of any significant results.
sis scales. There were no significant differences between The retained samples at both Times 2 and 3 were demo-
the mean scores of the two judges. graphically comparable then, as well as psychologically so.
Also, tests of several effects showed no significance for any
RESULTS of the sets of content analysis scale scores: participant sex,
race, language usage, and leader sex and leader age.
The design and analyses of the study are described, then
the preliminary data analyses dealing with retention rates
Time 1 Versus Time 2 Analyses
and analyses of bias, together with analyses of the possible
secondary effects of both participant and leader variables, Maturational processes. These results are described in terms
are provided. Comparisons of data at Times 1 and 2 are of the positive and then the negative CASPM scores. The
then presented for the maturational processes and psycho- means and standard deviations for five positive CASPM
logical states. The results of comparisons of the same vari- scale scores dealt with first are available in Table 1.
ables at Times 1 and 3 are presented later. MANOVA showed significant effects for group (multivari-

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 377


V i n e y, H e n r y, a n d C a m p b e l l

TABLE 1

Means and Standard Deviations for Five Positive Content Analysis Scales of Psychosocial Maturity
(CASPM) for Offender Adolescents With Group Work and No-Group-Work at Times 1, 2, and 3
Positive CASPM
Trust Autonomy Initiative Industry Affiliation
Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Group Work
Time 1 1.82 0.73 1.05 0.59 1.86 1.22 1.86 1.22 1.41 0.95
Time 2 1.82 0.60 0.85 0.47 1.41 0.72 2.66 1.12 1.64 0.96
Time 3 1.30 0.72 0.95 0.61 1.39 0.81 3.01 1.10 1.32 0.89
No-Group-Work
Time 1 1.79 0.62 1.18 0.65 1.93 0.97 1.93 0.97 1.99 1.17
Time 2 1.87 0.64 1.01 0.61 1.34 0.78 1.87 1.04 2.00 1.10
Time 3 1.55 0.57 0.81 0.36 1.28 0.61 2.61 1.28 1.66 1.15

ate F = 10.76, df = 5, 96, p < .001) in comparisons of group parisons of group work with no-group-work for the five
work and no-group-work for Time 1 and Time 2 scores. psychological states. Although it did not achieve significance
The subsequent univariate analyses for these scores showed at .01, we believe this finding has important clinical impli-
Industry (univariate F = 7.10, df = 1, 98 p < .01) and Affili- cations. The means and standard deviations for five scales
ation (univariate F = 7.86, df = 1, 98, p < .01) to make measuring the psychological states are available in Table 3.
significant contributions to the group effect. Inspection of The subsequent univariate analyses for the group effect in
the means of Table 1 indicated that those offender adoles- these scale scores revealed Uncertainty (univariate F = 8.33,
cents who experienced interventions in the treatment con- df = 1, 98, p < .01) to make the main significant contribu-
dition showed more gains in Industry and Affiliation at Time tion to this effect.
2 than did those who had the no-group-work condition.
The group (multivariate F = 10.13, df = 5, 96, p < .01) Time 1 Versus Time 3 Analyses
effect proved significant for comparisons of group work
with no-group-work for the five negative CASPM scale MANOVA analyses identified no significant differences in
scores for Times 1 and 2. The means and standard devia- the sets of maturational processes and psychological states
tions for five negative CASPM scales are available in Table data from Time 3 (9 months after the end of the group
2. The subsequent univariate analyses for the group effect work) and Time 1.
on these scores showed Hesitancy (univariate F = 8.04, df =
1, 98, p < .01) to make the main significant contribution to DISCUSSION
this effect. The participants in the treatment condition
showed more reduced Hesitancy at Time 2 than those in The confirmation and lack of confirmation for the hypoth-
the control condition. eses about the impact of the group work for the four sets
Psychological states. The group (multivariate F = 3.98, df of data immediately after its conclusion are first consid-
= 5, 96) showed significance (assuming p = .10) for com- ered, together with the results for these data sets 9 months

TABLE 2

Means and Standard Deviations for Five Negative Content Analysis Scales of Psychosocial Maturity
(CASPM) for Offender Adolescents With Group Work and No-Group-Work at Times 1, 2, and 3
Negative CASPM
Mistrust Constraint Hesitancy Inferiority Isolation
Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Group Work
Time 1 1.39 0.55 2.12 0.81 1.42 0.92 0.87 0.46 1.12 0.75
Time 2 1.00 0.37 1.86 0.86 1.13 0.88 0.78 0.46 0.89 0.52
Time 3 1.12 0.62 1.33 0.42 1.30 0.82 0.79 0.40 0.92 0.62
No-Group-Work
Time 1 1.31 0.61 2.07 1.14 1.42 1.03 0.89 0.61 1.05 0.64
Time 2 1.01 0.39 1.67 0.63 1.57 1.14 1.02 0.63 1.05 0.63
Time 3 1.07 0.65 1.23 0.65 1.13 0.81 0.69 0.34 1.16 0.77

378 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents

TABLE 3

Means and Standard Deviations for Five Scales Measuring Psychological States for Offender Adolescents
With Group Work and No-Group-Work at Times 1, 2, and 3
Psychological State Scales

Uncertainty Anxiety Depression Anger Positive Affect


Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Group Work
Time 1 1.46 0.86 2.11 0.93 2.16 0.97 1.91 0.72 1.21 0.60
Time 2 1.26 0.77 1.52 0.81 1.26 0.67 1.33 0.54 1.38 0.66
Time 3 1.25 0.88 1.34 0.76 1.65 0.60 1.36 0.75 1.28 0.59
No-Group-Work
Time 1 1.39 0.81 2.26 0.93 2.33 1.17 1.67 0.87 1.24 0.53
Time 2 1.59 0.93 1.55 0.77 1.53 0.65 1.25 0.66 1.34 0.68
Time 3 1.18 0.81 1.39 0.91 1.48 0.84 1.26 0.63 1.41 0.51

later. Finally, some criticisms of and recommendations from in Australia. The results of these short-term group work
the research are considered. evaluations are encouraging. Counselors planning to use
The first set of hypotheses was concerned with the im- group work with offender adolescents first need to assess
mediate impact of the group work on offender adolescents. their maturational processes and their psychological states.
The hypothesis that those adolescents who had experienced Given that at least short-term goals were accomplished
group work compared with those who had no group work by this group work, it is our intent to briefly focus now on
would experience gains immediately after was confirmed the findings from another examination of these data that
overall for the five positive, helpful maturational processes. looks at differences in the effects of personal construct and
In particular, the offender adolescents showed greater gains psychodynamic group work described in detail elsewhere
in industry and affiliation with others after group work com- (Henry, Viney, & Campbell, 1996; Viney, Henry, &
pared with those who had no group work. The linked hy- Campbell, in press). The personal construct group work
pothesis about reductions in the five negative, less helpful proved to be particularly effective in reducing less helpful
processes was also confirmed overall and, in particular, offender maturational processes such as mistrust, as well as distressing
adolescents who had group work showed less hesitation than psychological states, especially anger, for offender adolescents.
those who had no group work. This first hypothesis about the The psychodynamic group work proved to be effective in
immediate gains of group work was also confirmed to some increasing more helpful maturational processes such as ini-
extent for the five psychological states; in particular, offender tiative, as well as distressing psychological states such as
adolescents undergoing group work reported less uncertainty uncertainty and depression. One of the main implications of
than did those who had no group work. Effect sizes (Cohen, these findings for practice with offender adolescents is that
1988) were calculated for the samples with interventions they highlight the need to select the goals for group work
from Time 1 to Time 2 (.55 for Industry, .61 for Affinity, and to match them with forms of group work that are likely to
.25 for Hesitancy), showing only small to moderate effects. achieve these goals.
These effects of the group work did not vary according to Although the immediate gains for offender adolescents
the sex, aboriginal background, or language usage of the group from the group work were encouraging, the gains in matu-
members or to the sex or age of the group leaders. These rational processes and psychological states at the conclu-
overall gains for offender adolescents support the use of the sion of the group work of the second set of hypotheses
group work with them. were not maintained at 9 months after its conclusion. Two
These offender adolescents showed higher levels of mis- possible causes for this finding are considered. First, one
trust, constraint, and hesitancy, together with anxiety and possible reason for the failure to maintain gains over the
depression than did those in other samples of adolescents longer term may be that the brief interventions that we
(Viney, 1983b, 1987). The types of offenses for which the were able to offer simply did not match the level of depri-
offender adolescents were sentenced are meaningfully re- vation our participants encountered. The poor psychoso-
lated to these findings. It was often violence, mainly against cial functioning apparent in the scale scores of offender
property and sometimes against people. The fact that of- adolescents in these samples, when they were compared
fender adolescents derived benefit from group work is con- with those of adolescents in other samples, supports this
sistent with findings in the United States that offender ado- explanation. More sustained and longer term group work,
lescents are responsive to interventions (Fashinger & Har- or perhaps even more costly individual or multicomponent
ris, 1987; Leeman et al., 1993; Madonna & Caswell, 1991). interventions, may be needed. That interpretation of the
No research into the potential for interventions for this vul- pattern of findings here received informal support from
nerable group of adolescents has been conducted previously reflections of group leaders and professionals responsible

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 379


V i n e y, H e n r y, a n d C a m p b e l l

for the welfare of the offender adolescents. Second, a com- South Wales Department of Juvenile Justice, Australia. The
mon criticism of outcome measures that demonstrate short- support of administrative and professional staff in this or-
term gains is that impact measures, selected to tap directly ganization has been very much appreciated. We would also
into the content of the interventions, falsely exaggerate the like to thank the senior staff of the Keelong, Mount Penang
impact of the interventions. This possibility can be ruled and Reiby Juvenile Justice Centres for making this work
out in the present study, which used instruments with in- possible. Furthermore, the gifts made to us of time and at-
dependent content, with demonstrated reliability and va- tention from the busy lives of our adolescent research par-
lidity, and that were sensitive to underlying processes rather ticipants was invaluable.
than to surface content. The instruments used, although Among the leaders of the preventive group work, the
experienced-based, were more subtle and so more demand- seminal contributions of two should be specially acknowl-
ing than self-reports of gains by participants. edged: Patricia Weekes and Lindsay Oades. The important
The aim of this research, to evaluate the impact of two contributions of the other group leaders who trained in and
forms of group work with offender adolescents, was conducted the group work is also very gratefully acknowl-
achieved. The findings from the interviews collected im- edged: Craig Perkins, Tony Weaver, Ann Harwood, Ann
mediately after the intervention were encouraging. The Rodum, Hany Gayed, Jenny Barton, Annalisa Dezarnaulds,
group work was effective in increasing helpful maturational Nasim Wesley, Harry Smith, Monique Cohen, Greg Konza,
processes, especially ability to work in a sustained way and Chris Fairhall, Renata Kautz, Paul Whetham, Cassandra Mc
to achieve a sense of relationship with others, and in reduc- Naught and Alexandra Jones. Peter Caputi, Ross Colquhoun,
ing the less helpful maturational processes, especially lack Marilyn Rudd and Lindsay Oades also aided us in our analy-
of confidence. The interventions were also effective in re- ses of the data.
ducing less helpful psychological states, especially uncer-
tainty. However, the gains were not found months later. It
is possible that group work with more than 10 sessions, say
REFERENCES
15 or 20, would show gains over the follow-up period. Be- Bion, W. R. (1961). Experiences in groups. London: Tavistock Publications.
cause that solution could, in fact, double the cost of the Bion, W. R. (1970). Attention and interpretation. London: Tavistock
intervention, the use of a few booster group sessions, or Publications.
Blos, P. (1979). The adolescent passage: Developmental issues. New York:
even a telephone session with a group leader, would be International Universities Press.
worth evaluating. The encouraging findings can probably Burton, A. R., Foy, D. W., Bwanausi, C., & Johnson, P. (1994). The rela-
be generalized to offender adolescent populations because tionship between traumatic exposure, family dysfunction and post-
those who had dropped out of the group were not distin- traumatic stress symptoms in male juvenile offenders. Journal of Trau-
matic Stress, 7, 83–93.
guishable demographically or psychologically from those
Carpenter, P. (1984). “Green Therapy” revisited: The evolution of 12
who stayed. Yet, some criticism of the study must be made. years of behaviour modification and psychoeducationist techniques
Its sampling has been based on volunteers, limiting the with young delinquent boys. Psychological Report, 54, 99–111.
generalizability of the findings only to those who would Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
choose to be involved in such work. The retention rate im- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coolidge, F. L., Reilman, B. J., Lee, L. A., & Cass, V. J. (1992). Emotional
mediately after the group work was just 78%, and at follow problems and neuropsychological symptoms in juvenile non-offenders.
up, only 55%. Also, the design of the research could have Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 8, 7–13.
been improved by the inclusion of more extensive treat- Darden, C. A., Gazda, G. M., & Ginter, E. J. (1996). Life- skills and mental
ment validation data and a better nontherapeutic group health counseling. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 18, 134–141.
work control. Erikson, E. (1959). Growth and crises in the healthy personality. Psycho-
logical Issues, 1, 50–100.
Further research examining the impact of group work on Fashinger, E., & Harris, L. T. (1987). Social work at 30MPH: Mini-bike
offender adolescents with larger samples, better retention rehabilitator groups for juvenile delinquents. Social Work with Groups,
rates, better validated interventions, a better nontherapeutic 10, 33–45.
group work contrast condition, a wider range of appropri- Fine, S., Forth, A., Gilbert, M., & Haley, G. (1991). Group therapy for
ate outcome measures, closer examination of group pro- adolescent depressive disorder: A comparison of social skills and thera-
peutic support. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
cess with these and other models of adolescent functioning chiatry, 30, 79–85.
is recommended. We have already begun the examination Gottschalk, L. A. (1982). Manual of uses and applications of the
of group process in this area (Henry, 1996; Henry et al., Gottschalk-Gleser verbal behaviour scales. Research Communications
1997; Viney et al., 1999). Promoting psychological matu- in Psychiatry, 7, 273–327.
Gottschalk, L. A., Lolas, F., & Viney, L. L. (Eds.). (1986). Content analysis
ration and more helpful psychological states in offender
of verbal behaviour in clinical medicine. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
adolescents still remains a goal with high priority, and the Grenyer, B. F. S., Viney, L. L., & Luborsky, L. (1996, August). Changes in
findings of this research make some contribution to achiev- Eriksonian psychosocial maturity levels over the course of psycho-
ing that goal using two promising forms of group work. therapy and their relationship to interpersonal mastery and psycho-
Author note. This research was funded by a grant from logical health-sickness. Twenty-ninth Annual Meeting of the Society
for Psychotherapy Research, Amelia Island, FL.
the Australian Commonwealth Committee on AIDS Re- Hains, A. A., Herrman, L. P., & Balker, K. L. (1986). The development of
search Grants (CARG) to the two senior authors. Permis- a psycho-education group program for adolescent sex offenders. Jour-
sion was kindly given to conduct the research by the New nal of Offender Counselling Services and Rehabilitation, 11, 63–76.

380 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


The Impact of Group Work on Offender Adolescents

Henry, R. M. (1996). Psychodynamic group psychotherapy with adoles- Neimeyer, R. A. (1993). An appraisal of constructivist psychotherapies.
cents: Exploration of HIV-related risk taking. International Journal of Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 221–234.
Group Psychotherapy, 46, 229–253. Quay, H. C. (1987). Handbook of juvenile delinquency. New York: Wiley.
Henry, R. M., Viney, L. L., & Campbell, J. (1996). Mechanisms for change Shapiro, R. L., Zimmer, J., Berkowitz, D. A., & Shapiro, E. R. (1963). The
in adolescent group interventions for HIV risk-taking: A contrast of impact of group experiences on adolescent development. In M. Sugar
psychodynamic and personal construct models. Unpublished manu- (Ed.), The adolescent in group and family therapy (pp. 145–161). New
script, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, New South Wales, York: Brunner Mazel.
Australia. Tillitski, C. J. (1990). A meta-analysis of estimate sizes for group versus
Henry, R. M., Wesley, N., Jones, A., Cohen, M., & Fairhall, C. (1997) AIDS individual versus control treatments. International Journal of Group
and the unconscious. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 23, 373–398. Therapy, 40, 215–224.
Hoag, M. J., & Burlingame, G. M. (1997). Child and adolescent group Viney, L. L. (1983a). The assessment of psychological states through content
psychotherapy: A narrative review of effectiveness and the case for meta- analysis of verbal communications. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 542–563.
analysis. Journal of Child and Adolescent Group Therapy, 7, 51–68. Viney, L. L. (1983b). Psychological reactions of young people to unem-
Jackson, S. (1990). A personal construct therapy group for adolescents. ployment. Youth and Society, 14, 457–474.
In P. Maitland (Ed.), Personal construct theory, deviancy and social work Viney, L. L. (1987). A sociophenomenological approach to life span de-
(pp. 1–72). London: Centre for Personal Construct Psychology. velopment complementing Erikson’s psychodynamic approach. Hu-
Kegan, R. (1983). A neo-Piagetian approach to object relations. In B. Lee man Development, 30, 125–136.
& G. G. Noam (Eds.), Developmental approaches to the self (pp. 34– Viney, L. L. (1996). Personal construct therapy: A handbook. Norton, NJ:
48). New York: Plenum. Ablex.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Viney, L. L., Henry, R. M., & Campbell, J. (in press). Evaluating personal
Norton. construct and psychodynamic group work with offenders and non-
Klein, M. (1932). The psychoanalysis of children. London: Hogarth. offenders. In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in
Klein, M. (1948). Contributions to psychoanalysis 1921–1945. London: personal construct psychology, Vol. 4. New York: JAI Press.
Hogarth. Viney, L. L., Henry, R. M., Walker, B. M., & Crooks, L. (1989). The emotional
Klein, M. (1957). Envy and gratitude. London: Tavistock. reactions of HIV positive men. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62,
Landfield, A. W., & Rivers, P. C. (1975). An introduction to interper- 153–161.
sonal transaction and rotating dyads. Psychotherapy, 12, 366–374. Viney, L. L., Rudd, M., Grenyer, B., & Tyche, A. M. (1995). Content
Larson, J. D. (1990). Cognitive-behavioural group therapy with delin- Analysis Scales to Measure Psychosocial Maturity (CASPM): A manual.
quent adolescents: A cooperative approach with the juvenile courts. Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia: University of Wollongong.
Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 16, 47–64. Viney, L. L., Truneckova, D., Weekes, P., & Oades, L. (1999). Personal
Leader, F. (1991). Why adolescent group therapy? Journal of Child and construct group work for adolescent offenders: Dealing with their
Adolescent Group Therapy, 1, 81–93. problematic and more helpful meanings. Journal of Child and Adoles-
Leeman, L. W., Gibbs, J. C., & Fuller, D. (1993). Evaluation of a multi- cent Group Work, 9, 169–185.
component group treatment program for juvenile delinquents. Ag- Viney, L. L., & Tyche, A. M. (1985). Content analysis scales measuring
gressive Behaviour, 19, 281–292. psychological maturity in the elderly. Journal of Personality Assess-
Leitner, L. M., & Pfenninger, D. T. (1994). Sociality and optimal func- ment, 49, 311–317.
tioning. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 7, 119–136. Viney, L. L., Walker, B. M., Robinson, T., Pincombe, J., Lilley, B., & Ewan, C.
Macedo, G. (1955) Group psychotherapy in juvenile criminology. Inter- (1994). Dying in palliative care units and hospital: Quality of life of terminal
national Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 5, 54–60. cancer patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 157–165.
Madonna, J. M., & Caswell, P. (1991). The utilization of flexible tech- Viney, L. L., & Westbrook, M. T. (1976). Cognitive anxiety: A method of
niques in group therapy with delinquent adolescent boys. Journal of content analysis for verbal samples. Journal of Personality Assessment, 40,
Child and Adolescent Group Therapy, 1, 147–157. 140–150.
Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Hand- Wang, W., & Viney, L. L. (1996). A cross-cultural comparison of
book of adolescent psychology (pp. 97–108). New York: Wiley. Eriksonian psychosocial development: Chinese and Australian chil-
MacLennane, B. W., & Dies, K. R. (1992). Group counseling and psycho- dren. School Psychology International, 17, 33–48.
therapy with adolescents. New York: Columbia University. Wang, W., & Viney, L. L. (1997). The psychosocial development of chil-
Neimeyer, G. J. (Ed.). (1993). Constructivist assessment: A casebook. New dren and adolescents in the People’s Republic of China. International
York: Sage. Journal of Psychology, 32, 139–153.
Neimeyer, G. J., & Merluzzi, T. V. (1981). Group structure and group Westbrook, M. T. (1976). The measurement of positive affect using content
process: Explorations in therapeutic sociality. Small Group Behavior, analysis scales. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 12, 85–86.
13, 150–164. Winter, D. (1992). Personal construct psychology in clinical practice. Lon-
Neimeyer, R. A. (1988). Clinical guide-lines for conducting Interper- don: Routledge.
sonal Transaction Groups. International Journal of Personal Construct Zimpfer, D. F. (1992). Group work with delinquents. Journal for Special-
Psychology, 1, 181–190. ists in Group Work, 17, 116–126.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 381


TRENDS
Finding Happiness for Ourselves and Our Clients
Geri Miller

D. G. Myers (2000) examined the contributing factors of happiness: money, relationships, and religion. The implications of these factors for
counseling are discussed with specific recommendations made for counselors regarding their own self-care and their work with their clients.

F
inding Happiness for Ourselves and Our Clients” well-being: some wealthy countries have more satisfied people.
is a review of Myers (2000) article “The Funds, The very poor tend not to be happy, but as people become
Friends, and Faith of Happy People.” Myers pro- more comfortable, money does not seem to make a substan-
vided an analysis of the current research on hap- tial difference. Once individuals have the necessities of life
piness and made a plausible argument for the and are not extremely poor, then income does not seem to
necessary follow-up research regarding happiness and its make a difference in happiness (people can adapt to
causes. The impact of culture, personality, and activities on changes so that an earlier extravagance becomes a neces-
happiness were examined. In addition, Meyers emphasized sity). Myers (2000, p. 61) cited the example of happiness
the relationship of three factors (money, relationships, not being related to the economy in what he calls “the
religion) with regard to happiness. The main points of the American paradox”: Americans are prosperous materially,
article are reviewed here, followed by implications for the but have a recession socially.
professional counselor. When discussing relationships, Myers highlighted the impor-
tance of belonging and how the need to belong can result in
ARTICLE REVIEW healing or pain. The research indicates that close relationships
seem related to good health, whereas a lack of meaningful rela-
Myers (2000) began the article with an overall view of happi- tionships can be harmful. For example, social support of close
ness: “our working definition is simply whatever people mean friends is related to happiness; attached people (married) are
when describing their lives as happy” (p. 57). He noted that happier overall than unattached people. Yet there are some
according to research, age and gender do not seem to be con- exceptions to these findings. Although people in happy mar-
tributing factors to happiness. He also discussed factors that riages tend to be some of the happiest people, people in
do seem to contribute to happiness. One of these is culture, unhappy marriages are even less satisfied than unmarried
especially those cultures that offer individuals opportunities or divorced people. The happiness in marriage may be a result
for political freedom and affluence. Others are personality traits of happy people tending to marry (they are more enjoyable to
and temperament such as extroversion. Finally, he cited the be around than unhappy people) or possibly due to the
work of Csikszentmihalyi (1999), who has studied what he benefits of intimacy.
calls “flow.” This experience of flow, described as an activity The third area, religious faith, also seems to have an impact
that involves “skills, concentration, and perseverance” on happiness. Individuals with faith seem better able to handle
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1999, p. 825), is enjoyed for its own sake crises in their lives. The well-being related to faith may be a
even though there may not be a specific result from the result of social support because religious practices usually oc-
activity; a person “loses oneself” in a mindful challenge. Happi- cur within the context of a community. The well-being may
ness, then, is relevant to an ability to experience flow during also be related to meaning and purpose because faith provides
activities, with individuals experiencing happiness because of individuals with a framework from which they can view their
“how” they do an activity, not “what” they do. lives as making a difference. Finally, faith can provide people
In the article, Myers examined in depth three factors that with hope as they face the existential questions connected to
potentially contribute to happiness: money, relationships, and death and suffering in their lives. Therefore, faith seems to
religion. Regarding money, there seems to be a connection to provide people a cushion when coping with life’s struggles.

Geri Miller is an associate professor in the Department of Human Development and Psychological Counseling at Appalachian State University in Boone,
North Carolina, and a licensed psychologist and certified substance abuse counselor in North Carolina. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent
to Geri Miller, Department of Human Development and Psychological Counseling, Edwin Duncan Hall, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC 28608 (e-
mail: millerga@appstate.edu).

382 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79


Finding Happiness for Ourselves and Our Clients

In summary, the author noted that to understand happiness professional concessions regarding techniques, for example,
better, researchers need to examine in more depth personality using more behavioral focused techniques and having the
traits, work and leisure activities, relationships, and religious general feeling of “needing to do more with less.” These added
faith. A deeper understanding of these general factors can fa- stresses cause some counselors to struggle financially, and these
cilitate how our culture looks at its priorities and encourages stresses place heavier administrative burdens on them (reim-
the well-being of humans. So how can counselors make use of bursement of services, accountability, additional paperwork,
such conclusions? What are the implications of Myers’s analy- ethical dilemmas related to diagnosis and delivery of services).
sis of happiness research? These additional stresses can result in negativity and burn-
out for counselors. In addition, given the important work of
IMPLICATIONS FOR COUNSELORS counselors, they may legitimately view themselves as sub-
stantially underpaid, resenting their financial difficulties.
Initially counselors may simply say, “Of course” to reviews of Yet the research regarding happiness does not indicate that
research on happiness: “I already knew that.” “I already en- a change in income is necessarily related to happiness. Perhaps
courage that with my clients.” However, a challenge to the the struggle here is related more to the loss of the flow, in that
counseling profession is to suspend such typical responses and the activity of counseling has changed to the point that one
wonder how much of this information is truly taken in and cannot become “lost” in the skillful process. One must always
known by counselors. How much do counselors practice these be mindful that “others” are controlling when and how
factors related to happiness in their own lives, and how much services are delivered or that one’s work is not valued in terms
do counselors encourage the presence of these factors in the of financial reimbursement within the culture.
lives of their clients? Our clients may struggle with the same types of thoughts
A similar challenging process may occur in counselor edu- about money. If only they had more money they would
cation training in which students learning about the impor- have less stress, fewer problems. This misconception may
tance of listening (i.e., of hearing someone’s story in counsel- be quite strong for our clients, causing them to stay in rela-
ing) may say, “I know, I know” to their professor “Now, teach tionships or jobs that are very difficult and draining. The
me some counseling techniques, something I can do. Listen- exception may be for those counselors who work directly
ing is easy. I already know how to do that.” And the professor with the extremely poor and witness the trauma poverty
responds, “Do you already know how to listen? Do you really causes. Our work with low-income individuals entails help-
listen to others in your life? To your clients? Do you stop ing them locate resources that ease the burden of their pov-
everything, all thoughts of the past and future and focus to- erty. However, those clients whose basic needs are met may
tally on the present and the person with you? Listening is the be caught in the illusions of money equalling happiness. In
hardest, most important activity done with a client. Listening this instance, the counselor can approach the illusion as
is hard work. It begins with slowing oneself down. By slowing any other addressed in the session: with a sensitive, focused
down, the counselor can hear and care for self better, thereby inquiry that encourages the client to examine his or her
allowing the counselor to hear and care for others better.” own life regarding that aspect.
The happiness research findings involve similar dynamics:
what sounds simple and easy may be simple but not easy Relationships
to achieve. Just as clients can sense if counselors are really Although all human beings vary in terms of the number
listening to them, they may also sense the counselor’s level of and type of intimate relationships they require, the under-
happiness. It is difficult for counselors to encourage happiness lying need to belong is a core, common one in relationships
in clients if they are not happy themselves. So the questioning that counselors must examine in their lives. What are our
begins with counselors’ happiness: Are counselors happy? Do own needs for belonging? I remember a respected clinician
they experience the flow as described by Csikszentmihalyi telling me one time, “I need fewer intimacies outside of
(1999)? Just as counselors can pass on happiness to their clients, being a counselor because I have many of my intimacy needs
so can they pass on unhappiness. “Happiness choice points” are met with my clients.” Although I am unaware of any time
critical for counselors. For example, do counselors emphasize that this counselor acted inappropriately regarding client
aspects of counseling such as the restrictions of managed care involvement, I was concerned for the well-being of both
and the possibility of liability or do counselors emphasize their counselor and client if the need for closeness was being
participation in the healing of others where they witness and almost entirely met through the counseling relationship.
honor the sacredness of hearing another’s story? Do counselors Yet there is a genuine danger for all counselors, personally
nurture their own personal happiness and that of their clients? and professionally, of relying on closeness with our clients
Implications of these happiness findings for counseling, then, begin in lieu of a life outside the counseling session. It is critical
with counselors’ lives and extend to the lives of their clients. that we have a life away from clients’ problems—that we
have relationships in which we can simply be, without trans-
Money
ference issues influencing them.
In the last few years, managed care issues have placed tre- The social support of friends is one avenue for our self-care.
mendous constraints on the counseling profession. These Individuals who know us and are willing to be honest with us
constraints unfortunately result in the counselor’s need to make provide the needed support to survive our demanding work.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79 383


Miller

This does not mean that these relationships should be en- what force(s) outside of the counselor helps him or her feel
tirely focused on a serious processing of thoughts and feelings that living has meaning and purpose, experience a sense of
but that they should also allow us to play and to live fully— hopefulness, and simply enjoy being alive. For some counse-
to be real, genuine, and spontaneous in our interactions. Such lors, this evolves from a religious perspective, whereas for
a network provides a life away from our work that sustains us. others, it may be nature, for example. It is related once again
Social support is also critical for our clients. Who in his or to Csikszentmihalyi’s (1999) sense of flow: an activity that
her life is a natural healer? With whom, after sitting in his or revitalizes us, that provides us with a sense of being alive.
her presence, does the client simply feel better about life? Is it These same resources need to be explored with our cli-
a religious leader, a hair stylist, a neighbor? Assisting clients in ents. Depending on the client and their spiritual views, this
identifying their natural healers allows them to structure ac- avenue toward happiness may take different shapes. Our
tivities to involve those individuals. Counselors need to en- responsibility is to carefully and respectfully access with our
courage clients to seek people who are naturally good matches clients those views that provide him or her a sense of mean-
for them, people that make them feel as though the world is a ing, purpose, hope, and life enjoyment. Then, for our clients
good, safe, nurturing place in which to live. Once this is ac- as well as for ourselves, we should encourage participation
complished (and in the case of some of our clients, they may in activities related to these resources. Some of our clients
truly be starting from “scratch”), they need to work at main- have such narrow life views that they are not aware of avail-
taining those contacts. This may be especially important in a able resources. For example, clients caught in an addiction
culture where the client receives conflicting messages as to cycle may not know of other avenues that could be fulfilling
the importance of being independent and “pulling oneself up for them because they are so lost in their addiction. Here the
by the bootstraps.” Because the reality of needing and relying counselor may need to educate the client about options and
on another individual may be difficult for the client, the coun- encourage a process of self-exploration. The importance of
selor should continue to encourage the client to make and main- this area of faith, no matter how uncomfortable, cannot be
tain these connections. neglected by the counselor due to its potentially significant
Regarding marriage or other forms of intimate commitment, impact on assisting the client in coping with life.
the counselor and client may again face the same struggles. If the
counselor has a difficult partnership, how much happiness does
this drain from the counselor? Where does the counselor draw
SUMMARY
support? Counselors who are involved in a happy intimate Myers’s examination of three factors related to happiness
relationship can use this relationship as a resource of sustenance (money, relationships, and religion) is important for counselors
both personally and professionally, but the counselor in an and their clients. To be qualitatively helpful to their clients,
unhappy relationship may need a better honed support network counselors need to examine their own lives in relation to
to compensate for the unmet needs of the committed relation- these factors and remedy those areas where possible. With-
ship. As in other difficult situations, counselors themselves out examining our own wounds and strengths we reduce
might need counseling to determine the best long-range plan. our clients’ possibilities for happiness through our intentions
Finally, counseling may be beneficial for those counselors who or our ignorance. Through self-examination and a commit-
anguish over the lack of an intimate relationship. ment to self-care, we can diminish our suffering and increase
Clients go through a process similar to that of counse- our happiness. This process can serve as both a role model
lors. Those who are in unhappy relationships may use for our clients and a wellspring of happiness and joy that can
counseling as a safe place to process their options; a place have a contagious effect on our clients. We can counsel cli-
not affected by family, friends, or others invested in one ents from a place of balance within ourselves that is main-
option over another. The nonjudgmental counselor offers tained by both internal and external resources.
a safe, healing place for the client with the time and space By improving our chances of happiness, we increase the likeli-
to sort. Those clients who are single may need to review hood of our clients “catching” happiness from us. Such a focus on
their conceptions of marriage or a committed relation- happiness in life can also influence how we punctuate our clients’
ship and process their suffering over the lack of such a stories and assist them in enhancing the happiness of their lives, the
support. They may also need assistance in examining where functional aspects, and reducing the unhappiness, the dysfunctional
and how they are looking for a partner. Finally, those cli- aspects. A commitment to the happiness, the well-being of our-
ents who have a sustaining intimate relationship may use selves and our clients results in a tremendous amount of healing
that resource as one that helps them cope with the other merely as a natural part of the counseling process. This healing is
areas in which they struggle. deep, lasting, and meaningful in the context of our clients’ lives.

Religion REFERENCES
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy?
Of the factors related to happiness as reviewed by Myers, American Psychologist, 54, 821–827.
this one may be the most uncomfortable for some counse- Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. Ameri-
lors. If so, reframe it by discussing religion in the sense of can Psychologist, 55, 56–67.

384 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SUMMER 2001 • VOLUME 79

S-ar putea să vă placă și