Sunteți pe pagina 1din 15

657588

research-article2016
GCQXXX10.1177/0016986216657588Gifted Child QuarterlyWinkler and Voight

Article

Gifted Child Quarterly

Giftedness and Overexcitability:


2016, Vol. 60(4) 243­–257
© 2016 National Association for
Gifted Children
Investigating the Relationship Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Using Meta-Analysis DOI: 10.1177/0016986216657588


gcq.sagepub.com

Daniel Winkler1 and Adam Voight1

Abstract
Current textbooks, websites, research articles, and popular resources have stated that gifted individuals have longer and
more pronounced responses to stimuli than the general population. This overexcitable nature of gifted persons has provided
a commonly used lens to conceptualize, identify, and understand giftedness and gifted persons’ behaviors. Yet there are
reasons to be skeptical about the evidence demonstrating that the gifted population is more overexcitable than the nongifted
population. A meta-analysis was conducted to investigate the existence and strength of the giftedness–overexcitability (OE)
relationship as presented in a number of studies comparing the OE scores of intellectually gifted and nongifted samples.
Gifted samples were found to have higher mean OE scores than nongifted samples. However, the effect size for psychomotor
OE was not statistically significant, while the effect sizes for the emotional and sensual OEs were small. Calculated effect sizes
for intellectual and imaginational OEs were medium. A number of important limitations and implications exist.

Keywords
overexcitability, giftedness, Dabrowski, meta-analysis, research synthesis, systematic review

Introduction Intellectual overexcitability is to be distinguished from


intelligence. It manifests itself as persistence in asking
Historical Background probing questions, avidity for knowledge and analysis, pre-
The construct of overexcitability (OE) encapsulates five OEs occupation with theoretical problems. Other expressions
that are part of a theory of psychological development called are: a sharp sense of observation, independence of thought
the theory of positive disintegration (TPD; Dabrowski, 1964). (often expressed in criticism), symbolic thinking . . .
The Polish psychologist and psychiatrist Kazimierz Dabrowski Imaginational overexcitability is recognized through rich
developed this theory and the OEs. For Dabrowski (1972), OE association of images and impressions, inventiveness . . .
entailed “higher than average responsiveness to stimuli, mani- intense living in the world of fantasy, predilection for
fested either by psychomotor, sensual, emotional (affective), fairy and magic tales
imaginational, or intellectual excitability, or the combination Emotional overexcitability is recognized in the way emo-
thereof” (p. 303). These responses were physiological in tional relationships are experienced, and in the great inten-
nature, resulting from especially sensitive neurology that was sity of feeling and awareness of its whole range. Characteristic
heritable. Dabrowski observed or read about individuals dem- expressions are: inhibition . . . excitation (enthusiasm), strong
onstrating the five OEs, psychomotor (POE), sensual (SOE), affective recall of past experiences or concern with death,
imaginational (MOE), intellectual (TOE), and emotional over- fears, anxieties, or depressions. . . . There is a high degree of
excitability (EOE). Piechowski and Cunningham (1985) pro- differentiation of interpersonal feeling. (pp. 154-156)
vided a useful definition of each of the five OEs:
Michael Piechowski (1979), and to a lesser extent Ogburn-
Psychomotor overexcitability is an organic excess of Colangelo (1979), introduced the five OEs to the gifted edu-
energy or heightened excitability of the neuromuscular cation community, as Dabrowski’s work was largely
system. It may manifest itself as a love of movement for
its own sake, rapid speech, violent or impulsive activity, 1
Cleveland State University, Cleveland, OH, USA
restlessness . . .
Corresponding Author:
Sensual overexcitability is expressed in the heightened Daniel Winkler, Cleveland State University, 2121 Euclid Avenue, Julka Hall
experience of sensual pleasure, the seeking of sensual 374, Cleveland, OH 44115, USA.
outlets for inner tension . . . Email: danielwinklercsuohio@gmail.com
244 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

unknown in American academia (Tillier, 2008). Piechowski Spain (Falk, Yakmaci-Guzel, Chang, Sanz, & Chavez-Eakle,
(1979) argued that OEs might be better predictors of gifted- 2008). There is anecdotal evidence of gifted individuals’
ness than other methods of identification, such as IQ tests. OEs, as parents and gifted persons themselves have found
Other researchers have since claimed that the gifted popula- the constructs as accurate descriptions (Tolan, 2009).
tion is more overexcitable than the nongifted population Other scholars have argued that these findings have
(Maxwell, 1995; Meckstroth, 1995; O’Connor, 2002; important limitations. Pyryt (2008) claimed that only high
Piechowski, 1979, 1991; Piechowski & Colangelo, 2004; scores on TOE subscales were repeatedly found in high IQ
Silverman, 1993, 2000b, 2008). In these instances, scholars students and the measured effect sizes for the OEs were often
have conceived of giftedness as intellectual or creative. trivial or small. Pyryt (2008) and Piirto (2010a, 2010b) both
Intellectual giftedness was considered membership in a observed that many of the comparative studies had small
school’s gifted program and/or a high performance on psycho- sample sizes, and Mendaglio and Tillier (2006) claimed that
metric or academic measures (Ackerman, 1997; Bouchet & many studies’ gifted samples included intellectually and cre-
Falk, 2001). Creative giftedness has commonly entailed excep- atively gifted persons.
tional performance in one of the arts (Piechowski & Colangelo, Warne (2011) critiqued the OEQ II, noting that its technical
1984) or on a psychometric instrument (Schiever, 1985). information was “slight—almost nonexistent” (p. 673). There
Scholars have argued that the these high levels of OEs can is some debate in the empirical literature regarding the appro-
serve as a potential method for identifying gifted students priate factor structure of the OEQ II, with some research calling
(Ackerman, 1997; Bouchard, 2004; Breard, 1994; into question the factorial validity of its traditionally accepted
Piechowski, 1979) and should inform mental health practi- subscales. However, there is conflicting evidence as to whether
tioners’ understanding of and approaches with gifted stu- the structure of those subscales is appropriate for different stu-
dents (Silverman, 2000a, 2000b). Teachers and parents have dent subgroups (Van den Broeck, Hofmans, Cooremans, &
also been encouraged to understand their children’s behavior Staels, 2014; Warne, 2011) and about the OEQ II’s technical
through this OE lens and make appropriate accommodations. merit in general (Botella et al., 2015; Warne, 2011).
However, the degree to which this OE lens has influenced Some scholars have critiqued the giftedness–OE relation-
parents and practitioners is unclear. ship on theoretical grounds. Wirthwein, Becker, Loehr, and
Rost (2011) observed that the OEs were similar to the Big
Five, noting that “openness to ideas” was similar to TOE and
Evidence of Giftedness–OE Relationship “openness to feelings” was similar to EOE. Reviewing the
There is some disagreement among scholars about the nature OEQ II and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, Gallagher
and strength of the giftedness–OE relationship. Some have (2013) found that the descriptions of SOE, MOE, TOE, and
argued that gifted individuals are overexcitable, noting that EOE were similar to openness to experience and several of
gifted samples have often outscored nongifted samples on its subcomponents. And Limont et al. (2014) observed that
measures of OEs (Chang & Kuo, 2013; Harrison & the OEs and Big Five are both related to personality develop-
Haneghan, 2011; Piechowski, 2006). Surveying the litera- ment and are studied using self-report instruments.
ture, Piirto, Montgomery, and May (2008) succinctly sum- In addition to this Big Five–related commentary, Mendaglio
marized this sentiment, noting that “one of the emerging (2012) has made theoretical critiques based on Dabrowski’s
ideas about academically talented students has been that they understanding of the OEs. Mendaglio observed that many
possess higher OE—that they are more sensitive and intense researchers using the OEQ II had ignored how Dabrowski
than students who do not have high scores on IQ or achieve- understood the relationship between intelligence and the OEs
ment tests” (p. 142). and how the five OEs were part of a larger theory, the TPD.
This position has been supported by comparative studies Dabrowski regarded intelligence as important for develop-
that have found that gifted samples have outscored nongifted ment, but insufficient and not necessarily correlated with the
samples on measures of OE (Ackerman, 1997; Bouchet & OEs. These OEs also interacted with other variables in a very
Falk, 2001; Gallagher, 1985; Tieso, 2007). Researchers have complex TPD framework, such as social environments and
found statistically significant differences using different dynamisms. Measuring the OEs without accounting for or
instruments, including the Overexcitability Questionnaire I measuring these other variables, as the OEQ II does, has cre-
(OEQ I; Piechowski & Colangelo, 1984), the Overexcitability ated a theoretically flawed and invalid instrument (Mendaglio,
Questionnaire II (OEQ II; Tieso, 2007), and the ElemenOE 2012; Tillier, personal communication, March 7, 2016).
(Bouchard, 2004). There is also cross-cultural validity to
these findings, with gifted samples having outscored non-
gifted samples in the United States (Harrison & Haneghan,
Statement of the Problem
2011), Canada (Ackerman, 1997), Germany (Wirthwein & To help provide clarity regarding the existence and strength of
Rost, 2011), Turkey (Yakmaci-Guzel & Akarsu, 2006), Hong the giftedness–OE relationship, a meta-analysis was conducted.
Kong (Siu, 2010), Poland (Limont, Dreszer-Drogorób, Clarity would help researchers better evaluate these varying
Bedyńska, Śliwińska, & Jastrzębska, 2014), Mexico, and positions, and could help practitioners make more informed
Winkler and Voight 245

decisions about the level of OE within their gifted students/ manuscripts and theses. When searching the databases
patients. Additionally, it has been some time since such a meta- Academic Search Complete and ProQuest Dissertation &
analysis was conducted. Ackerman (1998) conducted a meta- Theses A&I, only two key terms were used: “Kazimierz
analysis almost 20 years ago, sampling studies that compared Dabrowski” and “overexcitability + giftedness.” This was
gifted and nongifted samples’ OEQ I scores (the OEQ II had because many individuals named “Dabrowski” authored stud-
yet to be developed). Since then, no researcher(s) has con- ies, creating a very high volume of irrelevant results.
ducted a meta-analysis regarding the giftedness–OE relation- Over the course of 18 months, one author completed this
ship. This is noteworthy because over the past 20 years scholars search procedure three times, though PscyINFO, APA
have produced many new studies comparing the OE levels of PyschNet, and ProQuest Dissertation & Theses A&I were
gifted and nongifted samples (Steenbergen-Hu & Olszewski- only searched once, on the most recent occasion. Titles were
Kubilius, 2016) and have used new instruments, most notably read first and screened. If the study appeared to possibly
the OEQ II (Falk & Miller, 2009). meet the inclusion criteria (listed below), the abstract and/or
Many of these new studies were collected, and gifted and full text was searched for and screened as well. On several
nongifted samples’ OE scores were compared. This compari- occasions, selected-for studies were identified while reading
son was completed in an effort to investigate the following the literature reviews mentioned above. And on multiple
research questions. occasions, Tillier’s (n.d.) bibliography provided live links to
articles, facilitating the screening and selection processes. To
Research Question 1: Is there any statistically sig- acquire unpublished studies, two different universities’ inter-
nificant difference between intellectually gifted indi- library loan services were used.
viduals and nongifted individuals on any of the five
OE subdomains?
Research Question 2: What is the heterogeneity of
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
these differences across studies? The inclusion–exclusion criteria are listed below. If a study
met these criteria, it was included in the meta-analysis.
To answer these questions, the weighted mean effect sizes
were calculated and then tested for significance. This weigh- 1. Researchers had to compare the OE scores of gifted
ing would account for the various comparative studies’ dif- samples with nongifted samples, with giftedness
fering effect sizes, sample sizes, and variance. A statistically defined as exceptional intellectual, academic ability.
significant positive effect size would indicate that the col- This kind of intellectual giftedness could have been
lected and combined studies’ gifted samples had outscored defined differently in various countries, states, and
the studies’ nongifted samples. Such a test would help aggre- school districts, with some using standardized tests
gate multiple comparative studies’ results. scores, grade point average, recommendations, iden-
tification matrices, and/or other criteria.
2. Researchers had to sample members of the general
Methodology student or adult population for their nongifted sam-
A systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted to ples. The general student population could include the
address these research questions. general K-12 student population, the general under-
graduate population at universities (American or
international), and/or a vocational school population.
Search Strategies 3. Studies that compared creatively gifted samples or
The search terms “Dabrowski, “overexcitability,” and “over- artistically gifted samples with nongifted and/or gifted
excitability + giftedness” were used to search the databases of samples were excluded from this meta-analysis.
the journals Roeper Review (1990-present), Gifted Child 4. The samples could have been drawn from any country.
Quarterly (1957-present), Journal for the Education of the 5. Any psychometric instrument could have been used
Gifted (1998-present), and High Ability Studies (1998-pres- to measure the OEs.
ent). The databases PyschINFO, APA PsycNet, Proquest 6. The necessary data had to be provided for the selected
Dissertation & Theses A&I, and Academic Search Complete meta-analytic procedures.
were also searched with no date restrictions. The first 10 pages 7. The study had to be available in English, due to the
of results from a Google Scholar search,positivedisintegration. language restrictions of the authors.
com, and the bibliographies of other literature reviews and
research syntheses including Ackerman (1998), Chang and
Coding Procedures
Kuo (2013), Falk and Miller (2009), and Pyryt (2008) were
searched. These bibliographies as well as the bibliography Relevant information was collected from the studies that met
portal on positivedisintegration.com (Tillier, n.d.) were read these criteria. The statistical information coded from each
and reviewed for potential published articles and unpublished study included the size of the gifted and nongifted subsamples
246 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Table 1.  Descriptive Statistics From Individual Studies.

POE SOE MOE EOE TOE

Study Mdif Sp ESsm Mdif Sp ESsm Mdif Sp ESsm Mdif Sp ESsm Mdif Sp ESsm
Ackerman 2.85 2.88 0.98 0.62 2.00 0.31 2.15 3.82 0.56 2.79 5.49 0.50 2.62 3.81 0.68
Bouchard −2.69 6.86 −0.39 −0.68 3.80 −0.18 0.60 2.80 0.21 0.49 5.27 0.09 6.90 9.09 0.76
Bouchet 0.18 0.70 0.26 0.35 0.75 0.46
Breard 0.28 3.73 0.07 0.16 1.95 0.08 0.59 3.32 0.18 1.77 4.24 0.41 1.12 4.20 0.26
Carman 0.11 0.74 0.15 0.23 0.76 0.30 0.35 0.73 0.48 0.10 0.67 0.15 0.39 0.70 0.55
Harrison 0.20 0.66 0.30 0.34 0.77 0.44 0.48 0.73 0.65 0.25 0.73 0.34 0.46 0.78 0.59
Limont −0.01 0.79 −0.01 0.21 0.77 0.27 0.31 0.80 0.39 −0.08 0.65 −0.12 0.32 0.64 0.50
Piirto 0.54 2.98 0.18 0.63 1.90 0.33 3.00 4.39 0.68 0.87 5.56 0.16 4.08 4.65 0.87
Siu 0.26 0.72 0.36 0.40 0.75 0.53 0.16 0.70 0.22 0.23 0.59 0.40 0.58 0.78 0.74
Tieso 0.03 0.78 0.03 0.03 0.87 0.03 0.22 0.89 0.24 0.15 0.81 0.19 0.19 0.76 0.25
Wirthwein −0.06 0.66 −0.08 0.05 0.73 0.07 0.07 0.67 0.10 −0.08 0.72 −0.11 0.18 0.60 0.30
Yakmaci-Guzel 1.46 2.44 0.59 0.87 3.21 0.27 2.50 3.50 0.71 1.12 4.55 0.24 5.84 6.06 0.96

Note. POE = psychomotor; SOE = sensual; MOE = imaginational; TOE = intellectual; EOE = emotional overexcitability; Mdif = difference in means between
gifted and nongifted subsamples; Sp = pooled standard deviation of gifted and nongifted subsamples; ESsm = unbiased standardized mean difference.

and the mean score and standard deviation for each subsample (Piechowski & Colangelo, 1984). The instrument is appropri-
on each of the five OE subdomains. Effect sizes were also col- ate for children and adults, but it has been suggested that lim-
lected and/or calculated. These data were necessary for the ited linguistic abilities can affect scores (Ackerman, 1997).
selected statistical procedures. These data are presented on Two trained coders separately spend 1 or 2 hours evaluat-
Table 1, though sample size is presented on Table 2. ing each questionnaire (Piechowski, 2006). Afterward, they
Other collected study descriptors included geographic compare scores and settle on one score from both raters. It
area from which sample was collected, instrument used to has been reported that most studies interrater reliabilities
measure the OEs, and gifted and nongifted samples’ charac- exceed 60% (Falk et al., 2008). Piechowski and Colangelo
teristics. These data were collected to provide context for (1984) noted that interrater reliability was most often
readers. Past findings have shown that country of origin can between .70 and .80. If raters greatly disagree about scores,
influence individuals’ OE profiles (Falk et al., 2008) and they discuss differences and attempt to reach an agreed score.
giftedness is operationalized in many different ways in the If the dispute is not settled, the scores are either averaged
comparative literature (Mendaglio & Tillier, 2006). It has (Falk et al., 2008) or an expert, namely Michael Piechowski
also been found that OE scores can vary across ages/year in or Frank Falk, settles the stalemate (Silverman, 2008).
school (Piechowski & Colangelo, 1984; Tieso, 2007). The OEQ II was developed by Falk, Lind, Miller,
Additionally, the instruments used to measure the OEs can be Piechowski, and Silverman (1999). The OEQ II is a self-
qualitative (OEQ I) or quantitative (OEQ II). This difference report, 5-point Likert-type with a response of 1 indicating
produces differences in scoring. In addition, the OEQ II’s not at all like me and 5 indicating very much like me. The test
validity has been contested (Warne, 2011). These data are developers used a principal components factor analysis and
presented on Table 2. found a five-factor structure aligning with the five OEs (Falk
et al., 1999). There are five calculated subscores, one for
each OE. There is no general/total test score. The subscales
Instrument were reported to have a high Cronbach’s alpha: .89 for SOE,
OEQ I and OEQ II.  Michael Piechowski (2006) developed the .89 for TOE, .86 for POE, .85 for MOE, and .84 for EOE.
OEQ I after examining 433 examples of OEs in his and Dab-
rowski’s case studies. By observing what questions and stim- ElemenOE.  The ElemenOE was used in only one study col-
uli allowed Dabrowski’s patients to demonstrate their OEs, lected for this meta-analysis, Bouchard (2004). The instru-
Piechowski was able to develop a standard set of open-ended ment was piloted in a study in which teachers rated gifted
questions. Initially, the OEQ I had 46 questions, but it was and nongifted students. Initially, the instrument had 100
reduced to 21 questions. The answers for these questions are Likert-type scale items, but five Dabrowskian scholars rated
evaluated for all five OEs, as the stimuli can produce unpre- the items, and 61 were chosen to comprise the ElemenOE
dictable responses for many overexcitable persons. Answers pilot. After piloting in over 300 classrooms, Bouchard
with a high degree of one or more OE would receive a score of reduced the instrument to its 30 strongest items. The instru-
3 (highest) or 2. Less OE presence would receive a 1 or 0 (low- ment was intended for adults to use to rate children’s
est). The highest possible score for each of the five OEs is 21 behaviors.
Table 2.  Mined Descriptive Information From Individual Studies.
Geographic area of
Study Instrument research site Operationalization of “gifted” Characteristics of “nongifted” subsample Gifted n Nongifted n

Ackerman OEQ I Canada Identified using achievement test scores, recommendation(s), Unidentified/nongifted; high school students 42 37
grades, and an IQ of at least 120 (an unknown number of
exceptions was made to IQ threshold); high school students
Bouchard ElemenOE United States Identified using evidence of high achievement capability in areas Unidentified/nongifted; elementary school children 96 75
(Houston, Texas) such as intellectual, creative, artistic, or leadership capacity,
or in specific academic fields; elementary school children
Bouchet OEQ II United States Identified using self-report as having been enrolled in a gifted Identified using self-report as either being enrolled in 142a 419
(Midwest) program while in high school; college undergraduate students Advanced Placement courses or in typical courses; college
undergraduate students
Breard OEQ I United States (South Identified using a 100-point scale system, 90 points of which is Near-gifted students identified with score between between 48 69
Carolina) based on achievement and aptitude tests and 10 points on 80 and 89.5 and nongifted scored below 80; fourth- and
individual school district discretion. The gifted group scored fifth-grade students
at or above 90; fourth- and fifth-grade students
Carman OEQ II United States Identified as gifted at some point in their life; undergraduate Students who had never been identified; undergraduate and 72 193
and graduate school students graduate
Harrison OEQ II United States Identified as creatively and/or intellectually gifted using Unidentified/nongifted students; middle and high school 73 143
achievement, IQ, and/or creativity test scores; middle and students
high school students
Limont OEQ II Poland Scored above 80 centile on the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Scored below the 80 centile on the Raven’s Advanced 132 103
Matrices; secondary school students Progressive Matrices; secondary school students
Piirto OEQ I United States Enrolled in a school’s gifted program; high school students Enrolled in vocational education classes; high school students 61b 53b
Siu OEQ II China (Hong Kong) Identified using a number of assessments including standardized Unidentified/nongifted students; students of various ages 217 229
tests on intellectual abilities and psychological profiles;
students of various ages
Tieso OEQ II United States (East Identified using a matrix that included a minimum score on Unidentified/nongifted; elementary and middle school 296 190
Coast) standardized tests of achievement, ability, or creativity; students
elementary and middle school students
Wirthwein OEQ II Germany The gifted sample was identified using IQ scores on three Unidentified/nongifted sample had a mean score of 102 at 95 309
different tests—mean 136 score in the third grade and 136 third grade and 103 at ninth grade; adults
in the ninth grade; adults who participated in a longitudinal High achievers had an IQ mean score of 117; adults
study Average achievers had a mean IQ score of 102; adults
Yakmaci- OEQ I Turkey (Istanbul) Identified using Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrix with a A low intellectual ability group was identified with a score 35 70
Guzel score of at least 27; high school students below 9; high school students
A middle or average intellectual ability group was identified
with a score between 9 and 27; high school students

Note. OEQ = Overexcitability Questionnaire; POE = psychomotor; SOE = sensual; MOE = imaginational; TOE = intellectual; EOE = emotional overexcitability.
a
Bouchet and Falk (2001) did not report results for the POE, SOE, or MOE.
b
Piirto and Fraas (2012) reported smaller sample sizes (n = 48 for each subsample) on the MOE, EOE, and TOE.

247
248 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Statistical Methods calculated using the effect size and inverse variance weight
for each study and the formula
Effect Size Calculations. For each study, an effect size was
( ∑w ES )
2
calculated to interpret the difference in subdomain scores
∑w ES 2 i i
between gifted and nongifted subsamples. The effect size Q= −
∑w
i i
statistic used was the unbiased standardized mean differ- i
ence, calculated from the information coded from each of the suggested by Hedges and Olkin (1985). The Q statistic was
reviewed studies according to treated as a chi-square statistic in a test of statistical signifi-
cance with degrees of freedom one less the study sample
 3   X gifted − X non− gifted 
size.
ES sm = 1 −  
 4 N − 9   sp 

Effect Size Weighting Method.  Each of the 12 study’s subdo-
where X gifted is the mean OE outcome for gifted students, main effect sizes were then used to calculate a weighted
X non− gifted the mean for nongifted students, s p the pooled mean effect size for each subdomain according to
standard deviation among the two subsamples, and N the
total sample size (Lipsey & Wilson, 2000). The bracketed
ES =
∑ ( w ES )
i i
portion of the equation adjusts effect sizes downward for
small sample sizes. Note that a positive effect size indicates
∑w i

that gifted students have higher scores than nongifted, and a where ES is the effect size value and w the inverse variance
negative effect size indicates the opposite. Pooled standard weight for each reviewed study i (Lipsey & Wilson, 2000).
deviation was calculated using the formula: The inverse variance weight gives added weight to effect
sizes with smaller confidence intervals (i.e., more precise

sp =
(n gifted ) ( )
− 1 sgifted 2 + nnon− gifted − 1 snon− gifted 2 estimates and typically large sample sizes) when calculating
the cross-study mean.
(n gifted + nnon− gifted + 2)

where ngifted and sgifted are the subsample size and standard Effect Size Confidence Intervals. The sum of study inverse
deviation of the subdomain score, respectively, for gifted stu- variance weights was also used to create a 95% confidence
dents and nnon− gifted and snon− gifted are the subsample size and interval around the weighted mean effect size for each sub-
standard deviation for nongifted students. All analyses were domain (Lipsey & Wilson, 2000). This approach to calculat-
conducted using Stata 13. ing the combined study effect size is recommended when the
reviewed studies are functionally similar (Borenstein,
Identification of Outliers.  To protect against the disproportion- Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009).
ate influence on the means and variances in the meta-analy-
sis, a process for detecting outliers was employed by Random Effects Adjustments.  Operating on the assumption that
examining the distribution of study effect sizes for each OE differences in sample study characteristics may affect the vari-
subdomain. Effect sizes were determined to be outliers if ability in observed study effect sizes (in addition to variability
they were more than 2 standard deviations from the mean of due to sampling error associated with the subject samples on
all effect sizes. Only one subdomain effect size (i.e., POE) which the individual effects sizes are based), adjustments were
from one study (i.e., Ackerman, 1997) met this threshold by made to initially calculated weighted effect sizes to account
a small margin at 2.15 standard deviations. This study was for these presumed random sources of variation. The random
further inspected to detect any abnormalities in method. No effects variance component was calculated for each subdo-
such abnormalities were detected, and because it was only main to estimate the portion of the variance among effect sizes
marginally above the 2 standard deviation criterion for out- not due to sampling error using Lipsey and Wilson’s (2000)
lier detection, the study effect size was retained for subse- method of moments procedure and the formula
quent analysis. Nonetheless, a sensitivity analysis was Q − ( k − 1)
vθ =
performed to examine the effect on the overall POE effect
size estimate of omitting this study. ∑ (∑ ∑ )
wi − wi2 / wi

where vθ is the estimate of the random variance component


Heterogeneity in Effect Sizes.  Due to differences among stud- and k is the number of studies in the meta-analytic sample.
ies in samples and in measures, it was assumed that the iden- The estimate of vθ was then added to initially calculated
tified effects were heterogeneous and random effects models sampling error variance, and the inverse of the sum was sub-
were estimated to identify cross-study mean effect sizes. To stituted in the formula above for calculated weighted effects
corroborate whether there were differences in individual size and confidence intervals. These revised effect sizes and
study effect sizes for reasons other than sampling error (e.g., confidence intervals that, in effect, control for random differ-
due to variation in study characteristics), a Q statistic was ences in study characteristics are reported in the results
Winkler and Voight 249

section below. Due to the small number of retained studies, Twenty-five full-text studies were acquired. After reading
no moderation or mediation analyses were conducted. these full-text studies, 14 were found to violate one of the
inclusion criteria. Each particular rationale for exclusion is
Assessment of Publication Bias.  Funnel plots and Egger’s test listed in Table 3. Studies were excluded for a variety of viola-
for small-study effects estimated in Stata 13 were used to tions, including not comparing a gifted sample with a non-
assess publication bias (Egger, Smith, Scheider, & Minder, gifted sample (Gallagher, 1985), reporting insufficient data
1997). Two-way funnel plots graph the effect sizes from each (Bouchet & Falk, 2001), and not being published in English
study in a meta-analysis on the x-axis and the study sample (see above). It was found that 12 studies met every inclusion
size on the y-axis. It is expected that there will be more vari- criteria, and these were included in the meta-analysis (see
ability in the effect size distribution of studies with small Table 4). It should be noted that the study authored by Bouchet
sample sizes versus large sample sizes, but effect sizes from and Falk (2001) is reported in both the included and excluded
both types of study should center on the same grand mean, tables. This is because sufficient data were reported for EOE
resulting in a funnel-shape distribution. Egger’s test for and TOE, but was not reported for POE, SOE, and MOE.
small-study effects provides an associated statistical test by
regressing study effect sizes on their standard errors to deter-
mine if sample size has a statistically significant association
Meta-Analysis Results
with (i.e., biases) observed effect size. There were too few Ultimately, 12 studies were retained for the meta-analysis
unpublished studies included in the meta-analysis to assess (indicated with asterisks in the references section). The sample
publication bias by comparing the mean effect size for the sizes (combined gifted and nongifted subsamples) of the 12
published versus unpublished studies, as recommended by studies ranged from 79 to 486. One of the reviewed studies
Lipsey and Wilson (2000). (Bouchet & Falk, 2001) did not include the POE, SOE, and
MOE subdomains in their analysis, and another study (Piirto &
Fraas, 2012) administered the MOE, EOE, and TOE to a
Results smaller group of gifted and nongifted students than the POE
Search Procedure Results and SOE. All included studies reported only one effect size for
each subdomain. The Q statistic was found to be statistically
Records Identified Through Database Searching. Including significant for each of the five subdomains, which indicates
duplicates, 637 titles were identified and read during the heterogeneity in effects due to factors aside from sampling
database searches. Based on the reading of the studies’ titles, error (e.g., differences in instrumentation; see Table 5).
14 of these studies were selected for a full-text review.
Overall Effect Size Estimates.  The results suggest that for each of
Additional Records Identified Through Literature Reviews and Bib- the five subdomains of OE, gifted students have higher scores
liographies.  Nineteen studies (n = 19) were identified through than nongifted students (Table 5). Only the POE subdomain
reading literature reviews and bibliographic sources, includ- weighted mean effect size had a 95% confidence interval that
ing Tillier’s (n.d.) online bibliography, Ackerman’s meta- included zero (the lower bound was approximately zero).
analysis and bibliography (1998), Falk and Miller’s (2009) Based on the weighted mean effects sizes, the differences in
literature review and bibliography, and Pyryt’s (2008) litera- SOE (ES = 0.22), and EOE (ES = 0.19) between gifted and
ture review and bibliography. These 19 studies were selected nongifted students may be considered small. The differences in
for a full-text review. MOE (ES = 0.36) and TOE (ES = 0.55) may be considered
medium. The difference in POE between gifted and nongifted
Full-Text Articles Assessed for Eligibility and Full-Text Articles students was not statistically significant (ES = 0.17). A sensitiv-
Excluded. Thirty-three studies were identified through these ity analysis that omitted the single outlier study from the calcu-
initial searches (n = 33). Fourteen sources were identified by lation of the mean POE effect size (i.e., Ackerman, 1997)
reading titles from database searches. Nineteen sources were resulted in a smaller mean effect size (ES = 0.11) that was also
identified while reading the referenced literature reviews. Eight not statistically significant. Funnel plots and Egger’s test for
of the identified studies were not acquired. This was either small-study effects did not detect publication bias for four of
because they were not available in English (Chang, 2001; Sanz, the five OE subdomains, but suggested that small-study bias
2006; Yakmaci-Guzel, 2002) or because the full text was may be present for the MOE subdomain.
unable to be acquired (Buerschen as cited by Ackerman, 1998;
Domroese as cited by Ackerman, 1998; Kristiansen-Hagne,
1998; Piechowski & Cunningham, 1985; Sandal-Miller, 1988).
Discussion
Abstracts were acquired for three of these studies. A reading of In some ways, these findings offer valuable contributions to
these abstracts indicated that none of them met all of the inclu- the understanding of the relationship between giftedness and
sion criteria (Kristiansen-Hagne, 1998; Piechowski & Cun- OE. There appears to be a positive relationship between the
ningham, 1985; Sandal-Miller, 1988). two constructs. The calculated differences for SOE, MOE,
250 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Table 3.  Excluded Studies.

Study Rationale for exclusion Stage of review


Piechowski and Colangelo (1984) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Piechowski and Cunningham (1985) Comparison between artists and gifted adults—no Abstract
nongifted sample present
Schiever (1985) Comparison between two gifted groups of varying Full-text
creative ability—no nongifted sample present
Gallagher (1985) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Sandal-Miller (1988) Only gifted group was sampled, no comparison data Abstract
Lewis, Kitano, and Lynch (1992) No psychometric data, no comparison group Full-text
Miller, Silverman, and Falk (1994) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Ely (1995) Comparison between creatively gifted sample and an Full-text
intellectually gifted sample—no nongifted sample present
Piirto, Assone, Ackerman, and Fraas (1996) Findings were also reported in Ackerman (1997) Full-text
Ackerman (1998) Study itself is a meta-analysis Full-text
Buerschen as cited by Ackerman (1998) Unpublished manuscript was unable to be acquired Citation and some data from
study in Ackerman (1998)
Domroese as cited by Ackerman (1998) Unpublished manuscript was unable to be acquired Citation and summary of
study in Ackerman (1998)
Kristiansen-Hagne (1998) No psychometric data were collected; no comparison Abstract and table of
group contents
Bouchet and Falk (2001) Data on POE, SOE, and MOE were absent, but data for Full-text
comparisons on EOE and TOE measures were present
and included in the analysis
Chang (2001) Not available in English Citation and summary of
study in Falk et al. (2008)
Yakmaci-Guzel (2002) Not available in English Citation and summary of
study in Falk et al. (2008)
Sanz (2006) Not available in English Citation and summary of
study in Falk et al. (2008)
Falk et al. (2008) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Wirthwein et al. (2011) Findings were also reported in an included study, Full-text
Wirthwein and Rost (2011)
He and Wong (2014) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Van den Broeck et al. (2014) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures
Limont (n.d.) Insufficient data reported for the selected meta-analytic Full-text
procedures

Note. POE = psychomotor; SOE = sensual; MOE = imaginational; TOE = intellectual; EOE = emotional overexcitability.

EOE, and TOE were statistically significant. The difference effect sizes for TOE scores have been found in almost every
for POE was not statistically significant. Calculated effect sampled comparative study (see Figure 1). Smaller, but simi-
sizes for EOE and SOE were small, and effect sizes were lar differences have also been reported regarding differences
medium for TOE and MOE. This score pattern; less differ- on MOE scores (Ackerman, 1997; Carman, 2011; Harrison
ence on measures of POE, SOE, and EOE; and more differ- & Haneghan, 2011; Siu, 2010; Yakmaci-Guzel & Akarsu,
ence on measures of TOE and MOE was evident in some of 2006). Conversely, it was found in fewer of the sampled
the sampled studies. Statistically significant differences studies that gifted and nongifted samples had a significant
(Ackerman, 1997; Bouchard, 2004; Bouchet & Falk, 2001; statistical difference on EOE (Wirthwein & Rost, 2011),
Breard, 1994; Carman, 2011; Harrison & Haneghan, 2011; POE (Ackerman, 1997), and SOE scores (Siu, 2010). In
Piirto & Fraas, 2012; Siu, 2010; Tieso, 2007; Wirthwein & addition, the calculated effect sizes for these individual stud-
Rost, 2011; Yakmaci-Guzel & Akarsu, 2006) and medium ies were more often trivial or small (see Figure 1).
Winkler and Voight 251

Table 4.  Included Studies. underrepresentation. In-depth, qualitative studies might be


preferable for detecting the nature of the OEs (Peterson,
Studies
2012), or possibly even the use of neuroimaging (Chang &
Breard (1994) Kuo, 2013).
Ackerman (1997) The exact implications of the calculated effect sizes are
Bouchet and Falk (2001) unclear. While statistically significant results were found for
Bouchard (2004) SOE, MOE, EOE, and TOE, and medium effect sizes were
Yakmaci-Guzel and Akarsu (2006) found for MOE and TOE, it is not clear what this entails for
Tieso (2007) the gifted population. Effect sizes’ sizes are established
Siu (2010) somewhat arbitrarily (Cohen, 1988). These classifications
Carman (2011) are primarily suited for comparing interventions, not popula-
Harrison and Haneghan (2011)
tions. It is unclear how large an effect size would need to be
Wirthwein and Rost (2011)
to consider the gifted population as a whole as overexcitable.
Piirto and Fraas (2012)
One might conclude that the small and medium effect sizes
Limont et al. (2014)
are not that substantial and reflect little difference in pres-
ence of the OEs.
Table 5.  Random Effects Weighted Mean Effect Sizes With 95% The issue of the gifted population being overexcitable
Confidence Intervals and Variance Components (Q). relates to a larger issue in gifted education. Before Terman’s
(1925) longitudinal studies, scholars had debated whether
Subdomain n Mean ES 95% Confidence Interval Q
gifted persons were qualitatively different from nongifted
POE 11 0.17 [0.00, 0.34] 41.43a persons. Early on, many argued that gifted persons were
SOE 11 0.22 [0.09, 0.36] 26.52a qualitatively different because they were physically weaker,
MOE 11 0.36 [0.24, 0.48] 20.05a more likely to get sick, die young, and endure mental illness
EOE 12 0.19 [0.08, 0.31] 22.42a (Lombroso, 1901). Terman’s work dispelled many of these
TOE 12 0.55 [0.42, 0.67] 28.96a early beliefs (Jolly, 2004; Winkler & Jolly, 2013). In more
recent times, some scholars have argued that gifted persons
Note. ES = effect size.
a
Variance component is significantly different from zero (p < .05). were qualitatively different because they were more overex-
citable, sensitive (Piechowski, 1979, 2006), asynchronous
(Silverman, 2000a, 2000b), and even moral (Silverman,
It is important to note that a number of considerations 1994). Others have expressed agnosticism on this point, not-
complicate these results. First, the disputes regarding the ing that one should
OEQ II psychometric (Botella et al., 2015; Warne, 2011) and
theoretical merits were unaddressed (Limont et al., 2014; evaluate carefully whether most or all such characteristics are
Mendaglio, 2012; Tillier, personal communication, March 7, inherently or qualitatively different, or whether they reflect
2016). If the OEQ II were a psychometrically flawed instru- primarily the maturity of viewpoint that is a part of intellectual
ment as some have claimed (Warne, 2011), then this would and emotional giftedness. . . . Some [traits] (e.g., sensitivities
make this meta-analysis at least partially invalid. In addition, and excitabilities), however may indeed be qualitatively special
if there is a great deal of construct overlap between the OEs traits of gifted students. We need considerable research, first to
and the Big Five, there could be important implications determine whether in fact these characteristics are more common
(Limont et al., 2014). And, the two instruments most com- to gifted than nongifted youngsters, and second, to explore to
monly used to measure OEs, the OEQ I and II, were devel- what extent qualitative differences exist. (Robinson, Reis,
Neihart, & Moon, 2002, pp. 270-271)
oped by researchers who appear to have had an a priori belief
that gifted persons were more overexcitable than nongifted
persons and that the OEs could be measured in isolation of Using the presented findings and data, it is possible for
other features of TPD (Falk et al., 1999; Piechowski, 1979). scholars and practitioners to believe or disbelieve in the rela-
One should also note that while the results of this meta- tionship between giftedness and the OEs. One could think of
analysis demonstrated some differences, these could have a hypothetical spectrum in regard to the giftedness–OE rela-
been greater or lesser if more studies and unpublished data tionship. One end or pole would be a skeptical position, a
could have been included. There is some evidence that strong disbelief in the existence of the giftedness–OE rela-
researchers who find statistically significant results are more tionship. The opposite pole would be a strong belief in the
likely to have their work published (Torgerson, 2006). Some existence of the giftedness–OE relationship, the belief in a
could argue that the OEs are such broad, large constructs that kind of qualitative difference between the gifted population
any effort to measure them would be inadequate. and the nongifted population. In between, many positions of
Consequently, the many ways that gifted persons demon- varying confidence are possible, especially in regard to the
strate OEs are actually not being measured due to construct relationship between giftedness and POE, SOE, and EOE.
252 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Figure 1.  Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for studies in meta-analysis.
Winkler and Voight 253

Implications for Practice Another area of future research would be to provide some
theoretical clarity to the relationship between TPD and gifted
It is unclear if OE scores could be used to identify gifted stu- individuals. This has been attempted and arguably done in
dents. The TOE and MOE effect sizes were medium sized several venues (Ackerman, 2009; Daniels & Piechowski,
according to Cohen (1988). However, as noted above, effect 2009; Mendaglio, 2008). Yet different perspectives on the
classifications are designed for intervention purposes, not relationship were given in these different venues, and a litera-
necessarily for establishing thresholds to identify subpopula- ture review or conceptual synthesis demonstrating how and
tions. It is unclear how large an OE effect size would have to why the different theoretical attitudes exist might be helpful.
be to be for valid and reliable identification of gifted students. This is important because on theoretical grounds, it is unclear
Additionally, the inconsistent findings among comparative if one can measure OEs in isolation without somehow
studies (Winkler, 2014), the aforementioned psychometric accounting for other variables within Dabrowski’s theory.
dispute about the OEQ II (Warne, 2011), and the theoretical Some scholars have argued that it is impossible to do this
dispute about the OEQ II create much uncertainty about the (Mendaglio, 2012), and that Dabrowski would have consid-
instrument (Mendaglio, 2012). Cautious practitioners may ered such a procedure as invalid and incapable of measuring
want to reserve commitment on the relationship between gift- the OEs (Tillier, personal communication, March 7, 2016).
edness and OE and on the OEQ II’s viability. One could cer- Essentially, are OEs immutably intertwined with other vari-
tainly consider individual students and patients as ables in the TPD framework, such as dynamisms? If OEs are
overexcitable, but the OEQ II might not be the most valid intrinsically embedded constructs, efforts to reduce them to
method for confirming or disconfirming such opinions. singular, discrete variables would be inherently flawed. Those
who have designed and used the OEQ II instrument must nec-
Areas for Future Research essarily disagree with, disregard, or be unaware of this com-
plication, as the instruments do not account for any other
There are several issues that researchers could address in the feature of TPD. Researchers who choose to use the OEQ II
future. For instance, many educators, parents (Tolan, 2009), should develop a theoretical response to this critique.
and other professionals (Roeper, 2009; Silverman, 1994) Finally, the OEQ II should be studied. Several scholars
have reported that the concept of OEs has helped them better have offered psychometric analyses of the instrument
understand gifted persons in their lives. This kind of anec- (Botella et al., 2015; S. Peters, personal communication,
dotal evidence may not be in the empirical literature, but it November 15, 2014; Van den Broeck et al., 2014; Warne,
should not be discarded. Researchers could investigate this 2011), and their work can serve as a starting point for future
anecdotal evidence through surveys. A random sampling of investigation.
parents or teachers could help determine what popular belief
is about gifted individuals’ OE levels.
The anecdotal evidence might also indicate that there is a Limitations
subpopulation within the gifted population that scores very Selection Bias
highly on OEs and demonstrates the OEs in a memorable
fashion. Researchers have already used cluster analysis tech- One selection bias inherent in this research synthesis is that
niques to demonstrate that OE scores can be grouped into only comparative studies were selected. A large number of
subpopulations (Rinn, Mendaglio, Rudasill, & McQueen, noncomparative studies such as case studies (Edmunds &
2010). Alternatively, perhaps this anecdotal evidence dem- Edmunds, 2005; Peterson, 2012), clinical/anecdotal evi-
onstrates that the OEQ I, OEQ II, and other instruments are dence from parents/professionals (Gatto-Walden, 2009;
inhibited in measuring OEs by a ceiling effect or construct Roeper, 2009; Silverman, 1994; Tolan, 2009) and general
underrepresentation. Or, perhaps it demonstrates something interviews/surveys (Lewis et al., 1992; Piechowski, 2006)
else or nothing at all. were excluded. While this is a serious limitation to consider,
Additionally, future research should continue to explore it is important to note that it is difficult to make useful infer-
the mean OE differences between gifted and nongifted sam- ences regarding the difference between nongifted and gifted
ples. Researchers could reconduct this meta-analysis with populations’ OE levels from noncomparative studies and
more data from unpublished or unidentified sources. Or, data. This is because the OEQ I, OEQ II, and ElemenOE lack
researchers could aggregate all of the available OEQ I or II norms. Consequently, it is difficult to interpret gifted stu-
scores for one large significance test and effect size calcula- dents’ scores or behaviors and regard them as significantly
tion, similar to Ackerman’s (1998) work. However, research- overexcitable. While it is true that a number of researchers
ers might find that slight mean differences were statistically have done this (Piechowski, 2006), this kind of study is simi-
significant due to a large sample size, so calculating effect lar to expert opinion. Claiming that certain OEQ I or II
sizes as done in this article would be prudent. More individ- responses or behaviors demonstrated significant excitability
ual comparative studies could also be conducted, as the sam- levels is difficult when other respected experts have disputed
ple of this meta-analysis (n = 12) was somewhat small. such interpretations (Pyryt, 2008).
254 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Publication Bias Acknowledgments


Unpublished studies such as dissertation abstracts and theses The authors would like to thank the editors and reviewers’ patience
and thoughtful commentary, without which this project would have
were sought through a number of outlets. Yet some unpub-
never come to fruition. The first author would also like to thank Rita
lished studies were still unable to be acquired, such as Culross and Jennifer Jolly for their guidance and commentary on
Domroese (1993) and Buerschen (as cited by Ackerman, earlier versions of this work, and Bill Tillier for his commentary on
1998). There may have been more unpublished studies that later drafts.
were not found or were overlooked in the search process as
well, as the reading of only titles may have been misleading. Declaration of Conflicting Interests
Additionally, only one researcher screened the titles, making
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
omission mistakes more probable than if multiple screeners to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
had been used.
Funding
Studies Omitting Necessary Data The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
Some studies did not provide sufficient data for this meta-
analysis (Gallagher, 1985; Piechowski & Colangelo, References
1984). These studies often found statistically significant
differences, with the gifted sample scoring higher than References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
their nongifted counterparts. Some of these data were
*Ackerman, C. M. (1997). Identifying gifted adolescents using per-
identified, and authors have been contacted. However, sonality characteristics: Dabrowski’s overexcitabilities. Roeper
there was not sufficient time to collect all of the unpub- Review, 19, 229-236. doi:10.1080/02783199709553835
lished data and include it in this analysis, so these results Ackerman, C. M. (1998). A secondary analysis of research using
were not included in this meta-analysis. Consequently, the the Overexcitability Questionnaire (Unpublished doctoral dis-
meta-analysis’ results may be more conservative than the sertation). College Station: Texas A& M University.
body of literature as a whole. Ackerman, C. M. (2009). The essential elements of Dabrowski’s
theory of positive disintegration and how they are connected.
Roeper Review, 31, 81-95. doi:10.1080/02783190902737657
Studies Not Published in English Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R.
(2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. West Sussex, England:
Some studies found that gifted samples outscored nongifted Wiley.
samples, but were not published in English (Chang, 2001; Botella, M., Fürst, G., Myszkowski, N., Storme, M., Da Costa, M.,
Sanz, 2006; Yakmaci-Guzel, 2002). These studies were not & Luminet, O. (2015). French validation of the Overexcitability
included in the meta-analysis. These studies results were also Questionnaire 2: Psychometric properties and factorial struc-
published in a book chapter, Falk et al. (2008). However, the ture. Journal of Personality Assessment, 97, 209-220. doi:10.1
sufficient data needed for the meta-analysis were not reported 080/00223891.2014.938750
in that chapter. *Bouchard, L. (2004). An instrument for the measure of
Dabrowskian overexcitabilities to identify gifted ele-
mentary students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 339-350.
Conclusion doi:10.1177/001698620404800407
*Bouchet, N., & Falk, R. (2001). The relationship among gifted-
In short, there appears to be a great deal about the OEs that is ness, gender, and overexcitability. Gifted Child Quarterly, 45,
unknown. The instruments (Botella et al., 2015; Warne, 260-267. doi:10.1177/001698620104500404
2011), the OE–giftedness relationship (Piirto, 2010a; *Breard, N. S. (1994). Exploring a different way to identify gifted
Silverman, 2000b), and the five OEs’ prevalence within the African-American students (Unpublished doctoral disserta-
gifted population are contested matters. It is possible that tion). University of Georgia, Athens.
more data and research could help ameliorate this situation *Carman, C. A. (2011). Adding personality to gifted identification:
by providing indisputable clarity. However, it is also possible Relationships among traditional and personality-based con-
that skeptical scholars and those who believe in the gifted- structs. Journal of Advanced Academics, 22, 412-446. doi:10
.1177/1932202X1102200303
ness–OE relationship will continue to adhere to their posi-
Chang, H. J. (2001). A research on the overexcitability traits
tions, regardless of new empirical evidence. Indeed, there of gifted and talented students in Taiwan (Unpublished
will probably be evidence produced that can reasonably sup- master’s thesis). Taipei, Taiwan: National Taiwan Normal
port each of these positions—as this meta-analysis does, University.
arguably. Perhaps eventually this process would culminate, Chang, H. J., & Kuo, C. (2013). Overexcitabilities: Empirical stud-
and a level of acceptable certainty would emerge that all, or ies and application. Learning and Individual Differences, 23,
at least most, would find acceptable. 53-63. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.010
Winkler and Voight 255

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-
ences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Dabrowski, K. (1964). Positive disintegration. Boston, MA: Little, Jolly, J. L. (2004). A conceptual history of gifted education: 1910-
Brown. 1940. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Baylor University,
Dabrowski, K. (1972). Psychoneurosis is not an illness. London, Waco, TX.
England: Gryf. Kristiansen-Hagne, S. (1998). Using Dabrowski’s overexcitabili-
Daniels, S., & Piechowski, M. M. (Eds.). (2009). Living with ties to identify gifted students and augment their gifted poten-
intensity: Understanding the sensitivity, excitability and emo- tial. Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses A&I.
tional development of gifted children, adolescents and adults. (Order No. 1393131)
Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Press. Lewis, R. B., Kitano, M. K., & Lynch, E. W. (1992). Psychological
Domroese, C. (1993). Investigating an alternate method for identi- intensities in gifted adults. Roeper Review, 15, 25-31.
fying gifted students (Unpublished master’s thesis). University doi:10.1080/02783199209553452
of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. *Limont, W., Dreszer-Drogorób, J., Bedyńska, S., Śliwińska,
Edmunds, A. L., & Edmunds, G. A. (2005). Social emotional devel- K., & Jastrzębska, D. (2014). “Old wine in new bottles?”
opment: Sensitivity: A double-edged sword for the pre-adoles- Relationships between overexcitabilities, the Big Five per-
cent and adolescent gifted child. Roeper Review, 27, 69-77. sonality traits and giftedness in adolescents. Personality
doi:10.1080/02783190509554293 and Individual Differences, 69, 199-204. doi:10.1016/j.
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias paid.2014.06.003
in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2000). Practical meta-analysis.
Medical Journal, 315, 629-634. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ely, E. I. (1995). The overexcitability questionnaire: An alterna- Lombroso, C. (1901). The man of genius. New York, NY: Scribner.
tive method for identifying creative giftedness in seventh grade Maxwell, E. (1995). Changing conceptions of intelligence and their
junior high school students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, impact upon the concept of giftedness: The triarchic theory of
Kent State University, OH. intelligence. In J. L. Genshaft, M. Bireley, & C. L. Hollinger
Falk, R. F., Lind, S., Miller, N. B., Piechowski, M. M., & Silverman, (Eds.), Serving gifted and talented students: A resource for
L. K. (1999). The Overexcitability Questionnaire-two (OEQII): school personnel (pp. 17-30). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Manual, scoring system, and questionnaire. Denver, CO: Meckstroth, E. A. (1995). Complexities of giftedness: Dabrowski’s
Institute for the Study of Advanced Development. theory. In J. F. Smutny (Ed.), The young gifted child: Potential
Falk, R. F., & Miller, N. B. (2009). Building firm foundations: and promise, and anthology (pp. 295-307). Cresskill, NJ:
Research and assessments. In S. Daniels & M. M. Piechowski Hampton Press.
(Eds.), Living with intensity: Understanding the sensitivity, Mendaglio, S. (2012). Overexcitabilities and giftedness research:
excitability and emotional development of gifted children, A call for a paradigm shift. Journal for the Education of the
adolescents and adults (pp. 239-260). Scottsdale, AZ: Great Gifted, 35, 207-219. doi:10.1177/0162353212451704
Potential Press. Mendaglio, S. (Ed.). (2008). Dabrowski’s theory of positive disinte-
Falk, R. F., Yakmaci-Guzel, B., Chang, A., Sanz, R. P., & Chavez- gration. Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Press.
Eakle, R. A. (2008). Measuring overexcitability: Replication Mendaglio, S., & Tillier, W. (2006). Dabrowski’s theory of posi-
across five countries. In S. Mendaglio (Ed.), Dabrowski’s the- tive disintegration and giftedness: Overexcitability research
ory of positive disintegration (pp. 184-200). Scottsdale, AZ: findings. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30, 68-87.
Great Potential Press. Miller, N. B., Silverman, L. K., & Falk, R. F. (1994). Emotional
Gallagher, S. (1985). A comparison of the concept of overexcit- development, intellectual ability, and gender. Journal for the
abilities with measures of creativity and school achieve- Education of the Gifted, 18(1), 20-38.
ment in sixth-grade students. Roeper Review, 8, 115-119. O’Connor, K. J. (2002). The application of Dabrowski’s theory to the
doi:10.1080/02783198509552950 gifted. In M. Neihart, S. M. Reis, N. M. Robinson, & S. M. Moon
Gallagher, S. (2013). Building bridges: Research on gifted chil- (Eds.), The social and emotional development of gifted children:
dren’s personalities from three perspectives. In C. S. Neville, What do we know? (pp. 51-60). Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
M. M. Piechowski, & S. S. Tolan (Eds.), Off the charts: Ogburn-Colangelo, M. K. (1979). Giftedness as multilevel poten-
Asynchrony and the gifted child (pp. 48-98). Unionville, NY: tial: A clinical example. In N. Colangelo & R. T. Zaffrann
Royal Fireworks Press. (Eds.), New voices in counseling the gifted (pp. 165-187).
Gatto-Walden, P. (2009). Living one’s spirit song: Transcendent Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
experiences in counseling gifted adults. In S. Daniels & M. M. Peterson, J. S. (2012). The asset-burden paradox of giftedness: A
Piechowski (Eds.), Living with intensity: Understanding the 15-year phenomenological, longitudinal case study. Roeper
sensitivity, excitability and emotional development of gifted Review, 34, 244-260. doi:10.1080/02783193.2012.715336
children, adolescents and adults (pp. 203-224). Scottsdale, Piechowski, M. M. (1979). Developmental potential. In N.
AZ: Great Potential Press. Colangelo & R. Zaffrann (Eds.), New voices in counseling the
*Harrison, G. E., & Haneghan, J. P. (2011). The gifted and the gifted (pp. 25-57). Dubuque, IA: Kendall Hunt.
shadow of the night: Dabrowski’s overexcitabilities and Piechowski, M. M. (1991). Emotional development and emotional
their correlation to insomnia, death anxiety, and fear of the giftedness. In N. Colangelo & G. Davis (Eds.), Handbook for
unknown. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 34, 669-697. gifted education (1st ed., pp. 285-306). Boston, MA: Allyn &
doi:10.1177/016235321103400407 Bacon.
256 Gifted Child Quarterly 60(4)

Piechowski, M. M. (2006). “Mellow out,” they say. If I only could: handbook of research and development of giftedness and talent
Intensities and sensitivities of the young and bright. Madison, (pp. 631-647). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
WI: Yunasa Books. Silverman, L. K. (1994). The moral sensitivity of gifted children
Piechowski, M. M., & Colangelo, N. (1984). Developmental and the evolution of society. Roeper Review, 17, 110-116.
potential of the gifted. Gifted Child Quarterly, 28, 80-88. doi:10.1080/02783199409553636
doi:10.1177/001698628402800207 Silverman, L. K. (2000a). Counseling the gifted and talented.
Piechowski, M. M., & Colangelo, N. (2004). Developmental potential Denver, CO: Love.
of the gifted. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Definitions and conceptions Silverman, L. K. (2000b). The gifted individual. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.),
of giftedness (pp. 117-132). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. Counseling the gifted & talented (pp. 3-28). Denver, CO: Love.
Piechowski, M. M., & Cunningham, K. (1985). Patterns of over- Silverman, L. K. (2008). The theory of positive disintegration in the
excitability in a group of artists. Journal of Creative Behavior, field of gifted education. In S. Mendaglio (Ed.), Dabrowski’s
19, 153-174. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.1985.tb00655.x theory of positive disintegration (pp. 157-174). Scottsdale, AZ:
Piirto, J. (2010a). 21 Years with the Dabrowski theory: An auto- Great Potential Press.
ethnography. Advanced Development Journal, 12, 68-90. *Siu, F. Y. (2010). Comparing overexcitabilities of gifted and
Retrieved from http://personal.ashland.edu/jpiirto/21%20 non-gifted school children in Hong Kong: Does culture make
years%20with%20Dabrowski.pdf a difference? Asia Pacific Journal of Education, 30, 71-83.
Piirto, J. (2010b). Rejoinder. Advanced Development, 12, 95-96. doi:10.1080/02188790903503601
*Piirto, J., & Fraas, J. (2012). A mixed-methods comparison of Steenbergen-Hu, S., & Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2016). How to con-
vocational and identified-gifted high school students on the duct a good meta-analysis in gifted education. Gifted Child
Overexcitability Questionnaire. Journal for the Education of Quarterly, 60, 134-154. doi:10.1177/0016986216629545
the Gifted, 35, 3-34. doi:10.1177/0162353211433792 Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius (Vol. 1). Paolo
Piirto, J., Montgomery, D., & May, J. (2008). A comparison of Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
Dabrowski’s overexcitabilities by gender for American and *Tieso, C. (2007). Overexcitabilities: A new way to think about talent?
Korean high school gifted students. High Ability Studies, 19, Roeper Review, 29, 232-239. doi:10.1080/02783190709554417
141-153. doi:10.1080/13598130802504080 Tillier, W. (2008). Kazimierz Dabrowski: The man. In S. Mendaglio
Pyryt, M. C. (2008). The Dabrowskian lens: Implications for under- (Ed.), Dabrowski’s theory of positive disintegration (pp. 3-11).
standing gifted individuals. In S. Mendaglio (Ed.), Dabrowski’s Scottsdale AZ: Great Potential Press.
theory of positive disintegration (pp. 175-182). Scottsdale, AZ: Tillier, W. (n.d.). A bibliography of Dabrowski materials. Retrieved
Great Potential Press. from http://www.positivedisintegration.com/DRIBiblio.htm#a-c
Rinn, A. N., Mendaglio, S., Rudasill, K. M., & McQueen, K. S. Tolan, S. S. (2009). What we may be: What Dabrowski’s work can
(2010). Examining the relationship between the overexcit- do for gifted adults. In S. Daniels & M. M. Piechowski (Eds.),
abilities and self-concepts of gifted adolescents via multi- Living with intensity: Understanding the sensitivity, excitability
variate cluster analysis. Gifted Child Quarterly, 54, 3-17. and emotional development of gifted children, adolescents and
doi:10.1177/0016986209352682 adults (pp. 225-236). Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Press.
Robinson, N. M., Reis, S. M., Neihart, M., & Moon, S. M. (2002). Torgerson, C. J. (2006). Publication bias: The Achielles’ heel of
Social and emotional issues: What have we learned and what systematic reviews? British Journal of Educational Studies,
should we do now? In M. Neihart, S. M. Reis, N. M. Robinson, 54, 89-102. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8527.2006.00332.x
& S. M. Moon (Eds.), The social and emotional development of Van den Broeck, W., Hofmans, J., Cooremans, S., & Staels,
gifted children: What do we know? (pp. 267-288). Waco, TX: E. (2014). Factorial validity and measurement invariance
Prufrock Press. across intelligence levels and gender of the Overexcitabilities
Roeper, A. (2009). The emperor has no clothes: Exquisite perception, Questionnaire-II (OEQ-II). Psychological Assessment, 26, 55-
stress, and the gifted child. In S. Daniels & M. M. Piechowski 68. doi:10.1037/a0034475
(Eds.), Living with intensity: Understanding the sensitivity, Warne, R. T. (2011). A reliability generalization of the
excitability and emotional development of gifted children, ado- Overexcitability Questionnaire–Two. Journal of Advanced
lescents and adults (pp. 73-82). Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential Academics, 22, 671-692. doi:10.1177/1932202X11424881
Press. Winkler, D. L. (2014). Overexcitabilities and giftedness:
Sandal-Miller, K. (1988). A comparison of Dabrowski’s concept of Investigating the evidence (Unpublished dissertation).
overexcitabilities with measures of verbal and mathematical Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge.
aptitude among academically precocious adolescents. Available Winkler, D. L., & Jolly, J. L. (2013). Lewis Terman: A twisted
from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses A&I. (Order No. 8905918) legacy. In A. Robinson & J. Jolly (Eds.), A century of contribu-
Sanz, R. P. (2006). El alumno superdotado y sus problemas de tions to gifted education: Illuminating lives (pp. 64-78). New
aprendizaje: Validación del OEQ-II como prueba de diag- York, NY: Routledge.
nóstico [The gifted student and learning disabilities: Validation Wirthwein, L., Becker, C., Loehr, E., & Rost, D. H. (2011).
of the OEQ-II as a diagnosis test]. Universidad Complutense de Overexcitabilities in gifted and non-gifted adults: Does sex
Madrid, Madrid, Spain. matter? High Ability Studies, 22, 145-153. doi:10.1080/1359
Schiever, S. W. (1985). Creative personality characteristics and 8139.2011.622944
dimensions of mental functioning in gifted adolescents. Roeper *Wirthwein, L., & Rost, D. H. (2011). Focusing on overexcit-
Review, 7, 223-226. doi:10.1080/02783198509552901 abilities: Studies with intellectually gifted and academically
Silverman, L. K. (1993). Counseling needs and programs for the gifted. talented adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 51,
In K. Heller, F. Monks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), International 337-342. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.041
Winkler and Voight 257

Yakmaci-Guzel, B. (2002). Ustun yeteneklilerin belirlenmesinde gifted education at Cleveland State University. He researches over-
yardimci yeni bir yaklasim: Dabrowskinin asiri dduyarlilik excitabilities, acceleration, and affective characteristics of gifted
alanlari [A supplementary method in the identification of gifted persons.
individuals: Dabrowski’s overexcitabilities] (Unpublished
Adam Voight is assistant professor of curriculum and foundations
doctoral dissertation). University of Istanbul, Istanbul, Turkey.
in the College of Education and Human Services and serves as the
*Yakmaci-Guzel, B., & Akarsu, F. (2006). Comparing overexcitabil-
Director of the Center for Urban Education. He researches the inter-
ities of gifted and non-gifted 10th grade students in Turkey. High
play between young people’s social contexts—such as schools,
Ability Studies, 17, 43-56. doi:10.1080/13598130600947002
neighborhoods, and families—and their educational and social–
emotional outcomes. In addressing these research topics, he works
Author Biographies in partnership with school communities and nonprofit community-
Daniel Winkler is a recent graduate of Louisiana State University, based, and youth organizations. His work is grounded in commu-
and is the current assistant professor of and program coordinator for nity and developmental psychological theory.

S-ar putea să vă placă și