Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

bs_bs_banner

Autism Spectrum Disorder, Language Disorder, and Social


(Pragmatic) Communication Disorder: Overlaps,
Distinguishing Features, and Clinical Implications
Lauren J. Taylor1 and Andrew J. O. Whitehouse2
1
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, and 2Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia

Background: The diagnostic boundaries between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and specific language impairment (SLI) are not clear-cut.
Evidence of the lack of distinct boundaries between these disorders comes from research identifying a group of children who have pragmatic
language difficulties that can be distinguished from those in children with SLI and those with ASD. These findings have led to the inclusion of
a new diagnostic category, social (pragmatic) communication disorder (SPCD), in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-
5th Edition DSM-5. While this new diagnostic category appears to capture a subgroup of children who may not have been recognised in the
DSM-IV, SPCD has been criticised due to a lack of empirical evidence, showing that the disorder is distinct from ASD in terms of aetiology,
intervention, and prognosis.
Objective: The purpose of this paper was to summarise the literature that has investigated overlaps in the phenotypes of SLI, ASD, and
SPCD. A secondary objective was to present a framework for the assessment and diagnosis of these three conditions.
Method and Results: In this paper, we review the research that has examined overlaps in the aetiologies and phenotypes of ASD, SPCD,
and SLI. While the results highlighted overlaps in the language profiles and autistic symptomatology, these three conditions could also be dis-
tinguished based on the severity of the social communication deficits and the absence of rigid and repetitive behaviour in strictly defined
cases of SPCD and SLI.
Conclusions: Strictly defined cases of SPCD and SLI can be distinguished from ASD. However, there is a lack of assessment tools that can
reliably distinguish these three conditions. We consider the clinical implications of the findings and present a model of assessment and diag-
nosis for ASD, SLI, and SPCD.

Key words: assessment; autism; diagnosis; social communication disorder; specific language impairment.

What is already known about this topic? What this topic adds

1 The boundaries between autism spectrum disorder (ASD), spe- 1 In this article, we summarise the evidence regarding overlap
cific language impairment (SLI), and social (pragmatic) commu- between ASD, SLI, and SPCD in terms of aetiology, language,
nication disorder (SPCD) are not clear-cut. autistic symptomatology, and long-term outcomes.
2 While SPCD was included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Man- 2 Strictly defined cases of SPCD can be distinguished from ASD
ual of Mental Disorders-5th Edition (DSM-5), it is unclear based on the severity of social communication deficits.
whether this disorder is distinct from ASD. 3 We present a framework for the assessment and diagnosis of
3 Assessment and diagnosis of ASD, SLI, and SPCD is challenging ASD, SLI, and SPCD.
because there are no measures that can reliably distinguish
these three conditions.

The language phenotype of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is


Correspondence: Lauren Taylor, School of Psychology, M304, University highly heterogeneous. While some individuals with ASD fail to
of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, Western Australia develop functional verbal communication, others have fluent
6009, Australia. language skills, experiencing difficulties only with social com-
Email: lauren.taylor@uwa.edu.au munication. The pragmatic difficulties in ASD contrast with the
Accepted for publication 4 April 2016 structural language impairments, that is, in phonology, mor-
doi:10.1111/ap.12222 phology, and syntax, observed in individuals with specific

Australian Psychologist (2016) 1


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse

language impairment (SLI). A substantial body of empirical lit- criticised due to a lack of empirical evidence showing that the
erature has been dedicated to understanding the phenomenol- disorder is distinct from ASD (Ozonoff, 2012; Skuse, 2012; Tan-
ogy of SLI. However, this label has not previously been guay, 2011).
included in the diagnostic nomenclature (see Rapin, 1996 for a
practitioner review of developmental language disorders).
Research studies have used a range of criteria to identify chil- ASD, SLI, and SPCD: Genes, Behaviour,
dren with SLI, which is generally accepted to occur when chil- and Longitudinal Outcomes
dren experience a clinically significant delay in language
While extensive evidence has investigated aetiological and
development despite having adequate hearing, non-verbal
behavioural overlaps between ASD and SLI, relatively less
intelligence, and educational opportunities (Bishop, 2003c;
attention has focused on SPCD. In this section, we review the
Stark & Tallal, 1981).1
research that has investigated overlaps between ASD, SLI, and
While “textbook” cases of ASD and SLI can be readily distin-
SPCD at the genetic and behavioural levels.
guished, in clinical practice, the boundaries between these two
disorders are not clear-cut. Extensive evidence now indicates
that a subgroup of children with ASD has a language impair- Genes and the Familial Aggregation of Structural
ment that resembles SLI (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; and Pragmatic Language Deficits
Lewis, Murdoch, & Woodyatt, 2007; Lloyd, Paintin, & Botting,
2006; Rapin & Dunn, 2003; Rapin, Dunn, Allen, Stevens, & While numerous twin studies have shown that both ASD and
Fein, 2009). For example, in one important study, Kjelgaard SLI are highly heritable (Bailey et al., 1995; Bishop, North, &
and Tager-Flusberg (2001) divided children with ASD into Donlan, 1995; Constantino & Todd, 2003; Folstein & Rutter,
“normal” (ALN; standard scores greater than 85), “borderline” 1977a, 1977b), no published literature has examined the herit-
(standard scores between 70 and 84), and “impaired” (ALI; ability of SPCD, likely reflecting the recent addition of this label
standard scores less than 70) language groups based on their to the diagnostic nomenclature. Evidence does exist to suggest
scores on a series of standardised language assessments. The that structural and pragmatic language impairments aggregate
ALI subgroup demonstrated poor performance on tests of pho- within families (Bernier, Gerdts, Munson, Dawson, & Estes,
nological processing, vocabulary, and higher-order grammatical 2012; Bolton et al., 1994; Gerdts, Bernier, Dawson, & Estes,
and semantic skills, with deficits in these areas closely resem- 2013; Landa et al., 1992; Pickles, St Clair, & Conti-Ramsden,
bling the SLI phenotype. There is substantial empirical support 2013; Piven et al., 1997; Taylor et al., 2015; Whitehouse,
for these findings, with a language impairment specifier out- Barry, & Bishop, 2007). In addition, the results of early twin
lined in the most recent revision of the Diagnostic and Statisti- (Dworzynski et al., 2007, 2008) and family (Bailey, Palferman,
cal Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). This specifier will Heavey, & Le Couteur, 1998; Folstein et al., 1999; Ruser et al.,
capture children who present with ASD and comorbid lan- 2007; Tomblin, Hafeman, & O’Brien, 2003) studies have
guage impairment. revealed overlaps in the language characteristics among parents
Further evidence for the lack of clear-cut boundaries and siblings of children with ASD and those with SLI. For
between ASD and SLI comes from studies that have identified example, there is an elevated rate of language and literacy
children with communication characteristics that are interme- impairments in parents and siblings of ASD probands, and
diate between “textbook” ASD and SLI. Substantial research some of these family members also demonstrate poor perfor-
has provided an evidence base for a subgroup of children with mance on tests of non-word and sentence repetition, heritable
primary pragmatic language impairments (PLI) who can be dis- markers for SLI (Bailey et al., 1998; Folstein et al., 1999; Lindg-
tinguished from children with SLI and those with ASD ren, Folstein, Tomblin, & Tager-Flusberg, 2009; Ruser et al.,
(Bishop & Norbury, 2002; Botting & Conti-Ramsden, 1999, 2007). Results of more recent studies indicate that the heritable
2003a, 2003b). The DSM-5 contains a new category of neuro- phenotypes of ASD and SLI breed true in these families (Bolton
developmental disorders, Social (pragmatic) communication et al., 1994; Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley, 2001; Landa et al.,
disorder (SPCD), which may capture children previously con- 1992; Lindgren et al., 2009; Pickles et al., 2013; Whitehouse
sidered to have PLI.2 SPCD is identified when children demon- et al., 2007; Whitehouse, Coon, Miller, Salisbury, & Bishop,
strate deficits in using verbal and non-verbal communication 2010). The results of a prospective, longitudinal study of infant
for social purposes (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). siblings of children with ASD also indicate that at 36 months,
Key diagnostic features include difficulties using communica- high-risk siblings had worse pragmatic language skills than
tion for social purposes, understanding verbal and non-verbal low-risk siblings, with 35% of the high-risk group identified as
conversational rules, and understanding social nuances. The having PLI, relative to only 10% of the low-risk group (Miller
pragmatic difficulties that characterise SPCD are likely to et al., 2014). However, this study was limited due to the indi-
resemble the core communication deficits of ASD, which may rect measures of pragmatic language (Language Use Inventory;
suggest that ASD and SPCD are variable manifestations of the O’Neill, 2009) and the sample bias that may result in inflated
same disorder (Tanguay, 2011). The distinguishing feature, rates of pragmatic impairments in the high-risk siblings, who
though, is rigid, repetitive, and stereotyped behaviour and may be vulnerable to social communication deficits due to the
restrictive interests (RRBIs), which must be absent in SPCD family history of ASD. Nevertheless, these findings indicate that
diagnoses. While this new diagnostic category appears to cap- pragmatic language impairments are highly heritable in ASD
ture a subgroup of children who may not have been recognised and may also highlight possible relationships between ASD and
in the DSM-IV, social communication disorder has been SPCD among families.

2 Australian Psychologist (2016)


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder

The results of several studies have also revealed overlaps in this task. In contrast, the SLI, PLI Pure, and ASD groups were
genes that may confer susceptibility to ASD, SLI, and social all impaired on the past tense-marking and sentence repetition
communication deficits. A specific gene located on 7q35, Con- tasks and could not be distinguished on either of these mea-
tactin Associated Protein-Like 2 (CNTNAP2), has attracted par- sures. The PLI Plus group had significantly higher scores than
ticular interest as polymorphisms in this region have been the SLI, PLI Pure, and ASD groups on the tense-marking task
identified in multiple-incidence ASD families (Alarcón et al., and the SLI and PLI Pure groups on the sentence repetition
2008; Arking et al., 2008; Bakkaloglu et al., 2008). Vernes task. Sensitivity and specificity analysis revealed that the sen-
et al. (2008) also found significant associations between poly- tence repetition task was the most accurate marker, distin-
morphisms on CNTNAP2 and impaired non-word repetition, a guishing all but the PLI Plus group from typically developing
heritable marker for SLI. Recent evidence also indicates that peers. Therefore, while psycholinguistic markers can accurately
there may be associations between common genetic variants distinguish children with communication disorders from their
related to ASD and semantic-pragmatic skills (St Pourcain typically developing peers, these markers alone cannot reliably
et al., 2013; Steer, Golding, & Bolton, 2010). These findings discriminate ASD from SLI or from PLI.
indicate that there may be shared genetic influences on ASD
and social communication skills.

ASD Symptomatology
Psycholinguistic Markers of SLI
Investigations of ASD symptomatology in children with PLI
Putative behavioural markers for SLI include impaired non- and those with SLI provide further insight into overlaps in the
word repetition, poor oromotor skills (Barry, Yasin, & Bishop, behavioural presentations of these conditions. Leyfer, Tager-
2007), and deficits in the acquisition and use of tense-marking Flusberg, Dowd, Tomblin, and Folstein (2008) reported that
morphemes (Rice & Wexler, 1996; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 41% of a group of children with SLI met diagnostic thresholds
2003). Considerable research in this area has focused on non- for ASD in social and communication domains on “gold stand-
word repetition tasks, which are purported to test phonological ard” measures of ASD in addition to showing a structural lan-
short-term memory. In this task, children hear nonsense words guage deficit. Botting and Conti-Ramsden (1999) reported
of increasing length and complexity and repeat them verbatim. similar results for a sample of children with PLI, the finding
Extensive evidence indicates that children with SLI demon- that 40% of a sample of children identified as having primary
strate poor performance on non-word repetition tasks, thought pragmatic language difficulties was considered to have difficul-
to index phonological short-term memory, a core cognitive def- ties in communication, reciprocal social interaction, and RRBI,
icit in this population (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990). Extensive that is, the “triad” of autistic impairments.
evidence indicates that a subgroup of children with ASD who Two studies have used standardised diagnostic measures of
have comorbid language impairments demonstrate non-word ASD to determine whether PLI can be distinguished from ASD
repetition performance that resembles children with SLI and from SLI. Bishop and Norbury (2002) divided children
(Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Lindgren et al., 2009; Lou- with communication difficulties into PLI (n = 13) and SLI
cas et al., 2010; Riches, Loucas, Baird, Charman, & Simonoff, (n = 8) subgroups based on their scores on the Children’s Com-
2011; Tager-Flusberg, 1996, 2006; Whitehouse, Barry, & munication Checklist (CCC; Bishop, 1998). These authors then
Bishop, 2008), highlighting overlaps in the language pheno- investigated whether the SLI and PLI groups could be better
types of these two groups. However, the groups can be distin- characterised as ASD. Bishop and Norbury (2002) found that
guished based on the error patterns as children with SLI make eight children (six PLI and two SLI) met diagnostic thresholds
more errors than children with ASD on long (four-syllable) rel- for ASD on the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R;
ative to short (two-syllable) non-words (Riches et al., 2011; Le Couter, Rutter, & Lord, 2003). While 10 of these children
Whitehouse et al., 2008). These findings indicate that the cog- (eight PLI and two SLI) also scored above the cut-off for PDD-
nitive origin of the non-word repetition deficits may differ NOS on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-G
between the two conditions. (ADOS-G; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999), there was
Findings regarding children with PLI have been less conclu- poor agreement between the ADOS-G and ADI-R scores, with
sive. Botting and Conti-Ramsden (2003a, 2003b) explored only four children (all from the PLI group) meeting diagnostic
whether performance on non-word repetition, sentence repeti- thresholds for PDD-NOS on both of these measures. Reisinger,
tion, and past tense-marking tasks could be used to distinguish Cornish, and Fombonne (2011) reported similar findings.
ASD, PLI, and SLI. While the scores for the ASD and SLI groups While all of the children in the ASD group (n = 22) met
were uniform, scores for the PLI group were highly variable. ADOS-G criteria for ASD, only seven of the 19 PLI participants
Therefore, the author divided this group into two subgroups: (36.8%) scored above this cut-off. Identical results were
“PLI pure,” that is, children with pragmatic impairments only, reported for the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ;
and “PLI plus,” those with pragmatic impairments as well as Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003), a parent-report measure of the
restricted social behaviour and/or interests. This division social and communication characteristics of ASD. The ASD
resulted in significant differences across the four groups. While group also had significantly higher Communication and Recip-
the SLI groups could be distinguished from the ASD, PLI Pure, rocal Social Interaction scores on the ADOS-G, indicating more
and PLI Plus groups based on poor non-word repetition perfor- severe symptoms than the PLI group. These findings indicate
mance, the latter three groups could not be differentiated on that while a small proportion of children with PLI meet

Australian Psychologist (2016) 3


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse

diagnostic thresholds for ASD, there is a group of children with Outcomes for Adults with Developmental
core deficits in pragmatic language distinct from ASD. Disorders of Language in Childhood
The results of a comprehensive investigation of the beha-
vioural and linguistic profiles of children with ASD, children Longitudinal data have shown that many individuals diagnosed
with SLI, and those with PLI revealed clear distinctions with SLI as children demonstrate behaviours reminiscent of
between these three groups (Gibson, Adams, Lockton, & autism when reassessed in adolescence and adulthood (Howlin,
Green, 2013). Gibson et al. (2013) recruited children with clini- Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000; Mawhood, Howlin, & Rutter,
cal diagnoses of PLI (n = 22), ASD (n = 21), and SLI (n = 19) 2000). In addition, Conti-Ramsden, Simkin, and Botting (2006)
from specialist mental health services, speech and language reported that 3.9% of a sample of adolescents with a history of
therapists, and specialist education providers in England and SLI exhibited sufficient behavioural characteristics of autism to
Scotland. Inclusion in these groups was confirmed by scores on warrant an autism diagnosis, a figure that is approximately four
the ADOS-G, SCQ, Children’s Communication Checklist-2 times the current autism prevalence estimates (Rice, 2009).
(CCC-2; Bishop, 2003a), and Test for Reception of Grammar-2 Diagnostic classifications for children with communication dis-
(TROG-2; Bishop, 2003b). The three groups were compared on orders are also unstable over time. Conti-Ramsden, Crutchley,
measures of social interaction, RRBI, and language ability, and and Botting (1997) classified children attending specialist lan-
odds ratios from a multinomial logistic regression were used to guage schools into six subgroups of language impairment (see
determine the relative importance of each measure in differen- also Rapin & Allen, 1987). In a subsequent analysis, Botting
tiating ASD, PLI, and SLI. The PLI group could be distinguished and Conti-Ramsden (1999) found that 45% of their sample of
from the SLI group based on more atypical social interactions 201 children with SLI moved clusters within a 1-yr time period.
and better expressive language skills. These two groups could These changes represented real, clinical shifts in language pro-
not be distinguished in terms of repetitive behaviour. However, files and highlighted the dynamic nature of language
repetitive behaviour, impaired social interaction, and high impairments.
expressive language scores were significantly associated with Several studies have also investigated the outcomes for adults
ASD but not with PLI. These results suggest that there is a with childhood diagnoses of ASD, SLI, and PLI (Eales, 1993;
graded pattern of social difficulties, with the SLI group demon- Whitehouse et al., 2007, 2009). In two studies, Whitehouse
strating the least impairment in this area, followed by the PLI and colleagues investigated the language, literacy, and psycho-
and then the ASD groups. The findings also indicate that under social outcomes of adults who were classified as having SLI,
rigorous conditions, PLI can be distinguished from ASD based PLI, or ASD in childhood (Whitehouse, Line, Watt, & Bishop,
on the severity of social interaction impairments and repetitive 2009; Whitehouse, Watt, Line, & Bishop, 2009). The results
behaviour in the latter group. revealed different language and psychosocial profiles of these
While ASD is distinguished diagnostically from SPCD based three groups in adulthood. In contrast to the individuals with
on the absence of rigid and repetitive behaviour in this latter SLI, who had enduring structural language and literacy difficul-
group, few previous studies have established whether social ties, those with PLI had persistent difficulties with language
communication impairments occur without rigid and repeti- use. Whitehouse et al. (2009) also reported a linear relationship
tive behaviours. In an investigation of the convergence of between the pragmatic and structural language abilities (e.g.,
DSM-5 criteria for ASD with DSM-IV diagnoses of pervasive pragmatic skills increased alongside structural language abil-
developmental disorders (PDDs), Huerta, Bishop, Duncan, ities) for those with SLI but not for those with PLI. In adult-
Hus, and Lord (2014) found that only 1.5% of a sample of hood, the individuals with ASD continued to present with
4,453 children with PDD and non-PDD diagnoses met social substantial pragmatic difficulties in addition to social impair-
communication criteria for ASD without displaying rigid and ments and repetitive behaviours. While the ongoing pragmatic
repetitive behaviours. In contrast, Mandy, Charman, Gilmour, impairment in PLI was qualitatively similar to that in ASD,
and Skuse (2011) reported that 64/66 (97%) of a sample of those with a childhood diagnosis of PLI could be distinguished
children with PDD-NOS displayed one distinct symptom pat- from those with ASD by the severity of the pragmatic and
tern, usually impaired social communication, without display- social impairments as well as the relative absence of repetitive
ing stereotyped and repetitive behaviour. The social behaviours in adulthood. These results provide empirical evi-
communication deficits in these children were of comparable dence indicating that there is a group of children with ongoing
severity to the pragmatic language deficits observed in ASD. communication difficulties, who have a language and beha-
Direct comparisons of children with PLI and those with ASD vioural profile that differs from SLI and from ASD in childhood
have also yielded discrepant findings. While Reisinger (Bishop & Norbury, 2002; Botting & Conti-Ramsden, 1999,
et al. (2011) found no significant group differences in ADOS- 2003a, 2003b) and also have social and language outcomes that
G or SCQ scores for rigid, repetitive or stereotyped behaviour, are distinct from SLI and ASD in adulthood (Whitehouse, Line,
or restricted interests, a more recent investigation revealed et al., 2009, Whitehouse, Watt, et al., 2009).
that the PLI group could be distinguished from the ASD group
based on less severe RRBI (Gibson et al., 2013). These results
indicate that some children with pragmatic language impair- Implications for Clinical Practice
ments also demonstrate RRBI to a lesser degree than children Assessment and Diagnosis
with ASD. However, there may also be a group of children
who have “pure” pragmatic impairments and meet the strin- The current evidence base overwhelmingly suggests that there
gent diagnostic criteria for SPCD. are no clear boundaries between ASD, SLI, and SPCD, with

4 Australian Psychologist (2016)


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder

pragmatic and structural language deficits falling along a con- presenting with language or communication difficulties.
tinuum rather than in discrete categories. The validity of SPCD Figure 1 presents a pathway for the assessment and diagnosis
as a distinct diagnostic category has been challenged due to the of children presenting with significant language and communi-
lack of empirical evidence supporting the presence of pragmatic cation difficulties or ASD symptoms. The assessment process
deficits independent of other neurodevelopmental and psychi- will differ depending on the results obtained from screening
atric disorders. For instance, pragmatic language impairments measures that detect the communication and social deficits that
co-occur with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Bishop & are indicative of ASD, SPCD, or SLI.
Baird, 2001; Geurts & Embrechts, 2008; Geurts et al., 2004), Measures such as the CCC-2 (Bishop, 2003a), which identi-
Williams Syndrome (Laws & Bishop, 2003), conduct disorder fies children with pragmatic and social deficits disproportionate
(Gilmour, Hill, Place, & Skuse, 2004), and learning disabilities to their structural language skills, or the Social Responsiveness
(Lapadat, 1991). The overlapping symptomatology and Scale (SRS; Constantino, 2005; Constantino et al., 2003), a
unknown aetiology of language and communication disorders quantitative measure of the characteristics of ASD, may be use-
presents a diagnostic challenge. ful to include at this screening stage. When the results of the
While several parent-report assessments of structural and screening reveal structural language difficulties, these should
pragmatic language have been developed, it is unclear whether be assessed using standardised measures of language ability. An
performance on these measures distinguishes the communica- ASD assessment should be pursued if the screening identifies
tion impairment in SLI from the pragmatic language impair- pragmatic difficulties or ASD symptoms. SLI may be diagnosed
ments and ASD symptomatology that characterise SPCD and when results of the comprehensive language assessment reveal
ASD, respectively. Measures such as the CCC-2 can reliably dis- core difficulties in structural language. While children present-
criminate children with communication difficulties from their ing with structural and pragmatic difficulties may have co-
typically developing peers and can identify children with prag- occurring diagnoses of SLI and SPCD, if repetitive behaviour is
matic and social deficits disproportionate to their structural lan- also present, ASD with a language impairment specifier should
guage skills. However, its ability to distinguish groups of be considered (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Chil-
children with ASD, SLI, and PLI is limited (Norbury, Nash, dren with pragmatic difficulties and RRBI may meet diagnostic
Baird, & Bishop, 2004). Nevertheless, the CCC-2 is a useful thresholds for ASD on “gold-standard” measures of ASD symp-
screening tool, which may identify children with significant tomatology, and pragmatic difficulties that occur in the absence
pragmatic difficulties or ASD symptoms that require further of repetitive behaviour would characterise SPCD diagnoses.
assessment.
Performance on standardised language assessments also does Intervention
not reliably distinguish ASD, SLI, and SPCD. Like the CCC-2,
scores on psycholinguistic markers of SLI, such as non-word The results of longitudinal investigations of ASD, SLI, and PLI
and sentence repetition tasks, can reliably discriminate children indicate that the communication impairments that define these
with ASD, SLI, and PLI from their typically developing peers groups can be severe and persistent, impacting quality of life in
but not from each other (Botting & Conti-Ramsden, 2003a, adulthood. While individual outcomes are variable, the effec-
2003b). Sentence repetition deficits appear to be sensitive and tiveness of the early behavioural intervention for ASD is well
specific markers of communication difficulties, accurately dis- established. Young children who access early intervention
criminating children with SLI, ASD, and PLI from their typically make gains in IQ, language, adaptive behaviour, and social
developing peers. While these three groups also cannot be dis- skills (Dawson et al., 2010; Remington et al., 2007, see also
tinguished on tests of narrative skills or story comprehension, Howlin, Magiati, & Charman, 2009 and Warren et al., 2011,
children with ASD are more likely to have inferencing deficits for systematic reviews of the literature). While there is some
than children with SLI or those with PLI. evidence supporting the effectiveness of language intervention
Overlapping presentations are also observed on “gold stand- for children with SLI, the literature is limited by a lack of
ard” measures of autistic symptoms. Some children with research that has investigated the efficacy of specific types of
PLI meet diagnostic cut-offs for ASD on the ADI-R and the interventions tailored for specific profiles of language impair-
ADOS-G, with a small proportion of this group exceeding ment (Ebbels, 2014).
diagnostic thresholds on both of these measures. In addition, a Children with pragmatic language impairments also benefit
proportion of children with SLI show autistic symptomatology from individualised intervention tailored to their specific com-
when assessed in adolescence or adulthood. However, ASD munication needs (Adams et al., 2012). In a randomised con-
may be distinguished from SLI and from PLI based on more trolled trial, Adams et al. (2012) assigned 88 children with
severe impairments in social interaction, indexed by higher pragmatic language impairments to receive 16–20 sessions of
ADOS-G and SCQ scores (Bishop & Norbury, 2002; Reisinger an intensive, manualised social communication intervention or
et al., 2011). While findings regarding the presence of RRBI in treatment as usual (TAU). The social communication interven-
children with SPCD have been mixed, the results of recent tion was aimed at improving semantic and high-level language
studies indicate that PLI can be distinguished from ASD based skills, pragmatic abilities, and social interaction. Children
on less severe rigid and repetitive behaviour and restricted assigned to the TAU condition continued with their usual
interests (Gibson et al., 2013). speech and language therapy. While there was no effect of the
Diagnosis is a challenge without measures that accurately treatment on Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4
distinguish subgroups of children with language disorders. (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2006) scores, significant treat-
Therefore, comprehensive assessment is required for any child ment effects were observed for conversational competence and

Australian Psychologist (2016) 5


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse

Developmental History Core difficulties with


Standardised assessments of structural language
phonology, morphology,
Comprehensive Language
semantics, syntax, pragmatics SLI
Assessment
Language Sample
Identify structural
Parent/Teacher report
language difficulties
Direct observation

Screening Difficulties with structural


Hearing test and pragmatic language
Developmental history
Parent/teacher report, e.g. CCC-2,
SRS-2 Standardised assessments of 'SLI plus'
language and ASD
Child presents with significant Assess Language and symptomatology
language or communication ASD symptoms Naturalistic observation
difficulties or ASD symptoms Identify structural and Language Sample
ASD + LI
pragmatic language Parent/Teacher report
difficulties
Difficulties with pragmatic
and structural language
plus RRBI

Developmenal History
Standardised assessment Pragmatic difficulties and RRBI
of ASD symptoms
Identify pragmatic
Parent interview
language difficulties Comprehensive ASD ASD
assessment Naturalistic observation
Rule out other developmental
disorders associated with
pragmatic difficulties, e.g. ADHD
Assess repetitive behaviour SPCD

Core difficulties with pragmatic


language

Figure 1 Proposed Pathway for the Assessment and Diagnosis of SLI, PLI, and ASD.

parent-reported social communication, social behaviour, and Notes


language skills. Children in the social communication interven-
tion had significantly higher scores on these measures than the 1 While the Language Disorder category contained in the
TAU group, and these gains were retained at 6-month follow- DSM-5 closely resembles SLI, much of the literature in this area
up. Therefore, the results of this study indicate that children refers to SLI. Therefore, we have used this latter term through-
with social communication deficits derive benefit from indivi- out the review.
dualised, tailored interventions. 2 Researchers and clinicians have historically used the terms
social communication and pragmatic language interchangeably,
with descriptions of social communication and pragmatic lan-
Concluding Remarks guage encompassing the same behaviours (Norbury, 2014).
Therefore, in this paper, we use the terms Pragmatic Language
Undoubtedly, some children with marked language difficulties
Impairment (PLI) and Social (Pragmatic) Communication Dis-
will fall between the boundaries of conventional diagnostic cri-
order (SPCD) synonymously.
teria for ASD and SLI and may meet criteria for SPCD. It is
likely that SLI, SPCD, and ASD are related disorders that vary
according to the degree of difficulty with structural language, References
pragmatic language, and circumscribed interests (Whitehouse, Adams, C., Lockton, E., Freed, J., Gaile, J., Earl, G., McBean, K., … Law, J.
Line, et al., 2009). The marked heterogeneity within the lan- (2012). The: A randomized controlled trial of the effectiveness of
guage profiles of children with communication disorders high- speech and language therapy for school-age children who have
lights the need for comprehensive, individual evaluation of the pragmatic and social communication problems with or without
communication strengths and weakness, socio-cognitive skills, autism. International Journal of Language and Communication
and cognitive profile for each child that presents for a diagnos- Disorders, 47, 233–244. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00146.x
tic evaluation. These assessments may help in discerning Alarcón, M., Abrahams, B. S., Stone, J. L., Duvall, J. A., Perederiy, J. V.,
Bomar, J., … Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Linkage, association, and gene-
whether the child’s difficulties relate primarily to structural lan-
expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism susceptibility
guage or pragmatic language impairments or ASD. Regardless
gene. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 150–159.
of the origin of the language impairment, or the diagnostic doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005
label, all children who have language and communication diffi- American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
culties that interfere with academic and social functioning manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
require individualised intervention that is tailored to their Arking, D. E., Cutler, D. J., Brune, C. W., Teslovich, T. M., West, K.,
unique cognitive and linguistic profile. Ikeda, M., … Chakravarti, A. (2008). A common genetic variant in the

6 Australian Psychologist (2016)


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder

neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the Social
autism. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 160–164. Responsiveness Scale with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised.
doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.015 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 427–433.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Siminoff, E., doi:10.1023/A:1025014929212
Yuzda, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general
Evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77. population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 524–530.
doi:10.1017/S0033291700028099 doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.524
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Couteur, A. (1998). Autism: The Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A., & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to
phenotype in relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with SLI.
Disorders, 28(5), 369–389. doi:10.1023/A:1026048320785 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 765–777.
Bakkaloglu, B., O’Roak, B. J., Louvi, A., Gupta, A. R., Abelson, J. F., doi:10.1044/jslhr.4004.765
Morgan, T. M., … State, M. W. (2008). Molecular cytogenetic analysis and Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of
resequencing of Contactin associated protein-like 2 in autism spectrum autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific
disorders. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 165–173. language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47,
doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.017 621–628. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01584.x
Barry, J. G., Yasin, I., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Heritable risk factors Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., …
associated with language impairments. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 6, Varley, J. (2010). Randomised, controlled trial of an intervention for
66–76. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00232.x toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125,
Bernier, R., Gerdts, J., Munson, J., Dawson, G., & Estes, A. (2012). e17–e23. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0958
Evidence for broader autism phenotype characteristics on parents Dworzynski, K., Ronald, A., Hayiou-Thomas, M., McEwan, F., Happé, F.,
from multiple-incidence autism families. Autism Research, 5, 13–20. Bolton, P. F., & Plomin, R. (2008). Developmental path between
doi:10.1002/aur.226 language and autistic-like impairments: A twin study. Infant and Child
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communication Development, 17, 121–136. doi:10.1002/icd.536
Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of Dworzynski, K., Ronald, A., Hayiou-Thomas, M., Rijsdijk, F., Happé, F.,
communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology Bolton, P. F., & Plomin, R. (2007). Aetiological relationship between
and Psychiatry, 39, 879–893. language performance and autistic-like traits in childhood: A twin
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003a). The Children’s Communication Checklist-2. study. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
London, England: Psychological Corporation. 42(3), 273–292. doi:10.1080/13682820600939002
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003b). Test for Reception of Grammar-2. London, Eales, M. J. (1993). Pragmatic impairments in adults with childhood
England: The Psychological Corporation. diagnoses of autism or developmental receptive language disorder.
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003c). Autism and specific language impairment: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 593–617.
Categorical distinction or continuum? Autism: Neural basis and doi:10.1007/BF01046104
treatment possibilities (pp. 213–234). Chichester: Wiley. Ebbels, S. (2014). Effectiveness of intervention for grammar in school-
Bishop, D. V. M., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of aged children with primary language impairments: A review of the
pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children’s evidence. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 30, 7–40.
Communication Checklist in a clinical setting. Developmental Medicine doi:10.1177/0265659013512321
and Child Neurology, 43, 809–818. doi:10.1017/S0012162201001475 Folstein, S., Gilman, S. E., Landa, R., Hein, J., Santangelo, S. L., Piven, J.,
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of … Wzorek, M. (1999). Predictors of cognitive test patterns in autism
autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(7), 1117–1128.
standardized diagnostic instruments. The Journal of Child Psychology Folstein, S. E., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Genetics of autism: Complex
and Psychiatry, 43, 917–929. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00114 aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nature Reviews Genetics, 2,
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1995). Nonword repetition as a 943–955. doi:10.1038/35103559
behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977a). Genetic influences and infantile autism.
twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(4), 391–403. Nature, 265, 726–728.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.x Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977b). Infantile autism: A genetic study of
Bolton, P. F., MacDonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Goode, S., Crowson, M., 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18,
… Rutter, M. (1994). A case–control family history study of autism. 297–321. doi:10.1038/265726a0
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(5), 877–900. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb02300.x language disordered children: Is there are causal connection? Journal
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1999). Classification of children with of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)
specific language impairment: Longitudinal considerations. Journal of 90004-J
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1195–1204. Gerdts, J. A., Bernier, R., Dawson, G., & Estes, A. (2013). The broader
doi:10.1044/jslhr.4205.1195 autism phenotype in simplex and multiplex families. Journal of Autism
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003a). Autism, primary pragmatic and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1597–1605. doi:10.1007/s10803-
difficulties, and specific language impairment: Can we distinguish them 012-1706-6
using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Hartman, C. A.,
Neurology, 45, 515–524. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00951.x Mulder, E. J., … Sergeant, J. A. (2004). Can the Children’s
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003b). Pragmatic language Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism,
impairment without autism: The children in question. Autism, 3, children with ADHD, and normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology
371–396. doi:10.1177/1362361399003004005 and Psychiatry, 45, 1437–1453. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00326.x
Constantino, J. M. (2005). Social Responsiveness Scale. Torrance, CA: Guerts, H. M., & Embrechts, M. (2008). Language profiles in ASD, SLI and
Western Psychological Services. ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1931–1943.
Constantino, J. M., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gibson, J., Adams, C., Lockton, E., & Green, J. (2013). Social
Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., … Reich, W. (2003). Validation of a brief communication disorder outside autism? A diagnostic classification

Australian Psychologist (2016) 7


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse

approach to delineating pragmatic language impairment, high Mandy, W., Charman, T., Gilmour, J., & Skuse, D. (2011). Toward specifying
functioning autism and specific language impairment. Journal of pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified. Autism
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 1186–1197. doi:10.1111/ Research, 4, 121–131. doi:10.1002/aur.178
jcpp.12079 Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental
Gilmour, J., Hill, B., Place, M., & Skuse, D. H. (2004). Social communication receptive language disorder - a comparative follow-up in early adult
deficits in conduct disorder: A clinical and community survey. Journal life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 967–978. doi:10.1111/j.1469- and Psychiatry, 41(5), 547–559. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00642
7610.2004.t01-1-00289.x Miller, M., Young, G. S., Hutman, T., Johnson, S., Schwichtenberg, A. J., &
Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic review of early Ozonoff, S. (2014). Early pragmatic language difficulties in siblings of
intensive behavioural interventions for children with Autism. American children with autism: Implications for DSM-5 Social Communication
Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114, 23–41. Disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 774–781.
doi:10.1352/2009.114:23;nd41 doi:10.1111/jcpp.12342
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental Norbury, C. F. (2014). Practitioner review: Social (pragmatic)
receptive language disorder—A follow-up comparison in early adult communication disorder conceptualisation, evidence and clinical
life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 204–216.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 561–578. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12154
Huerta, M., Bishop, S. L., Duncan, A., Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Using a
Application of DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder to three parental checklist to identify diagnostic groups in children with
samples of children of DSM-IV diagnoses of pervasive developmental communication impairment: A validation of the Children’s
disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 1056–1064. Communication Checklist-2. International Journal of Language and
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020276 Communication Disorders, 39, 345–364. doi:10.1080/
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of 13682820410001654883
language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. O’Neill, D. (2009). Language Use Inventory: An assessment of young
Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308. doi:10.1080/ children’s pragmatic language development for 18- to 47-month-old
01690960042000058 children. Waterloo, Ontario, Canada: Knowledge in Development.
Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O., Chase, G. A., & Ozonoff, S. (2012). Editorial perspective: Autism Spectrum Disorders in
Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social language use in parents of autistic DSM-5 – An historical perspective and need for change. Journal of
individuals. Psychological Medicine, 22, 245–254. doi:10.1017/ Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 1092–1094. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
S0033291700032918 7610.2012.02614.x
Lapadat, J. C. (1991). Pragmatic language skills of students with language Pickles, A., St Clair, M. C., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Communication
and/or learning disabilities: A quantitative synthesis. Journal of and social deficits in relatives of individuals with SLI and relatives of
Learning Disabilities, 24, 147–158. doi:10.1177/002221949102400303 individuals with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). A comparison of language abilities in 43, 156–167. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1556-2
adolescents with Down Syndrome and children with specific language Piven, J., Palmer, P., Landa, R., Santangelo, S. L., Jacobi, D., & Childress, D.
impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, (1997). Personality and language characteristics in parents from
1324–1339. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2003/103) multiple-incidence autism families. American Journal of Medical
Le Couter, A., Rutter, M., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview- Genetics, 74, 398–411. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19970725)74:4
Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. <398::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-D
Lewis, F. M., Murdoch, B. E., & Woodyatt, G. C. (2007). Linguistic abilities Rapin, I. (1996). Practitioner review: Developmental language disorders: A
in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism clinical update. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 643–
Spectrum Disorders, 1, 85–100. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2006.08.001 655. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01456.x
Leyfer, O. T., Tager-Flusberg, H., Dowd, M., Tomblin, J. B., & Folstein, S. E. Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1987). Developmental dysphasia and autism in
(2008). Overlap between autism and specific language impairment: preschool children: Characteristics and subtypes. Proceedings of the
Comparison of Autism Diagnostic Interview and Autism Diagnostic First International Symposium on Specific Speech and Language
Observation Schedule Scores. Autism Research, 1, 284–296. Disorders in Children (pp. 20–35). London, England: Afasic.
doi:10.1002/aur.43 Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of
Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development, 25,
Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum 166–172. doi:10.1016/S0387-7604(02)00191-2
disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree Rapin, I., Dunn, M., Allen, D. A., Stevens, M. C., & Fein, D. (2009).
relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38. doi:10.1002/aur.63 Subtypes of language disorders in school-age children with autism.
Lloyd, H., Paintin, K., & Botting, N. (2006). Performance of children with Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 66–84. doi:10.1080/
different types of communication impairment on the Clinical 87565640802564648
Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. Child Language Teaching and Reisinger, L. M., Cornish, K. M., & Fombonne, E. (2011). Diagnostic
Therapy, 22, 47–67. doi:10.1191/0265659006ct297oa differentiation of autism spectrum disorders and pragmatic language
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41,
Observation Schedule-Generic. Los Angeles, CA: Western 1694–1704. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1196-y
Psychological Services. Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovschoff, H., degli Espinosa, F., Jahr, E.,
Loucas, T., Riches, N. G., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Siminoff, E., Brown, T., … Ward, N. (2007). Early intensive behavioural intervention:
Chandler, S., & Baird, G. (2010). Speech perception and phonological Outcomes for children with Autism and their parents after two years.
short-term memory capacity in language impairment: Preliminary American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
evidence from adolescents with specific language impairment (SLI) 112, 418–438. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112 [418:EIBIOF]2.0.CO;2
and autism spectrum disorders (ASD). International Journal of Rice, C. (2009). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Autism and
Language & Communication Disorders, 45(3), 275–286. doi:10.3109/ developmental disorders monitoring network, United States, 2006.
13682820902936433 Morbidity and Mortality Monthly Report, 58, SS-10.

8 Australian Psychologist (2016)


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society
L.J. Taylor and A.J.O. Whitehouse Autism, SLI, and social communication disorder

Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of Taylor, L. J., Maybery, M. T., Wray, J., Ravine, D., Hunt, A., &
specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2015). Are there differences in the behavioural
Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257. doi:10.1044/ phenotypes of autism spectrum disorder probands from simplex and
jshr.3906.1239 multiplex families. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11, 56–62.
Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non- doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.003
word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and Tomblin, J. B., Hafeman, L. L., & O’Brien, M. (2003). Autism and autism
autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of risk on siblings of children with specific language impairment.
Communication Disorders, 44, 23–36. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.06.003 International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3),
Ruser, T. F., Arin, D., Dowd, M., Putnam, S., Winklosky, B., Rosen- 235–250. doi:10.1080/1368282031000086363
Sheidley, B., … Folstein, S. (2007). Communicative competence in Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J.,
parents of children with autism and parents of children with specific Groszer, M., … Fisher, S. E. (2008). A functional genetic link between
language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental distinct developmental language disorders. The New England Journal
Disorders, 37, 1323–1336. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0274-z of Medicine, 359, 2337–2345. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa0802828
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-Feig, J., Glasser, A., &
Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2011). A systematic review of early
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical Evaluation of intensive behavioural intervention for autism spectrum disorders.
Language Fundamentals-Fourth Edition. Hove: Harcourt Assessment. Pediatrics, 2011, e1303–e1311. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-0426
Skuse, D. H. (2012). DSM-5’s conceptualisation of autistic disorders. Journal Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). The broader
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 344. language phenotype of autism: A comparison with specific language
doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.02.009 impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(8),
St Pourcain, B., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Ang, W. Q., Warrington, N. M., 822–830. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01765.x
Glessner, J. T., Wang, K., … Davey Smith, G. (2013). Common variation Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further
contributes to the genetic architecture of social communication traits. defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific
Molecular Autism, 4. doi:10.1186/2040-2392-4-34 language impairment subtype. Journal of Communication Disorders,
Stark, R. E., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific language 41, 319–336. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.01.002
deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 114–122. Whitehouse, A. J. O., Line, E. A., Watt, H. J., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009).
doi:10.1044/jshd.4602.114 Qualitative aspects of developmental language impairment relate to
Steer, C. D., Golding, J., & Bolton, P. F. (2010). Traits contributing to the language and literacy outcome in adulthood. International Journal of
autistic spectrum. PLoS One, 5. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012633 Language and Communication Disorders, 44, 489–510. doi:10.1080/
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1996). Brief report: Current theory and research on 13682820802708080
language and communication in autism. Journal of Autism and Whitehouse, A. J. O., Watt, H. J., Line, E. A., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009).
Developmental Disorders, 26(2), 169–172. doi:10.1007/BF02172006 Adult psychosocial outcomes of children with specific language
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical impairment, pragmatic language impairment and autism. International
Neuroscience Research, 6, 219–224. doi:10.1016/j.cnr.2006.06.007 Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 44, 511–528.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive doi:10.1080/13682820802708098
phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Whitehouse, A. J. O., Coon, H., Miller, J., Salisbury, B., & Bishop, D. V. M.
of London B, 358, 303–314. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1198 (2010). Narrowing the broader autism phenotype: A study using the
Tanguay, P. E. (2011). Autism in DSM-5. The American Journal of Communication Checklist-Adult. Autism, 14, 559–574. doi:10.1177/
Psychiatry, 168, 1142–1144. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11071024 1362361310382107

Australian Psychologist (2016) 9


© 2016 The Australian Psychological Society

S-ar putea să vă placă și