Sunteți pe pagina 1din 5

J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:1359–1363

DOI 10.1007/s10803-009-0738-z

BRIEF REPORT

Brief Report: Sociometric Status and Behavioral Characteristics


of Peer Nominated Buddies for a Child with Autism
Jonathan M. Campbell Æ Christine A. Marino

Published online: 9 April 2009


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Abstract We examined social and behavioral correlates social functioning of CWA, such as the use of peer buddies
of children selected by their peers to serve as peer buddies (e.g., Laushey and Heflin 2000). Teachers typically select
for an unfamiliar child with autism (CWA). Participants classmates to serve as peer buddies who are compliant with
were 293 children from two public elementary schools who adult requests, academically strong, and regular school
completed social status, behavioral, and peer buddy nom- attendees. Jackson and Campbell (2009) also reported that
ination measures. Peer buddy nominations for a CWA were teachers select peer buddies who are popular, prosocial,
related to: (a) perceived unpopularity; (b) being viewed as and considered self-confident leaders in the classroom.
helpful and smart; and (c) lacking influence regarding Although peer-mediated interventions, such as the
popularity within the classroom. In contrast, peer buddy recruitment of peer buddies, typically target improving
nominations for a typical boy were related to being viewed peer interactions, peers are often not involved in the peer
as popular, helpful, and self-confident. Students may select selection process due, in part, to the time involved. Teacher
a social niche for CWA based on principles of peer hom- peer buddy nominations share only modest agreement with
ophily. Limitations and suggestions for future research are peers’ nominations (Jackson and Campbell 2009), and little
discussed. is known regarding social and behavioral characteristics
that peers use to select peer buddies for CWA. We aimed to
Keywords Autism  Inclusion  Peer buddies  extend the knowledge base in this area by examining social
Sociometry  Popularity status and behavioral correlates of peer buddy selections
for CWA. We were also interested if peers’ nominations
for a CWA differed from nominations for a typical child.
Introduction We predicted that peer buddy nominations for the CWA
would correlate with nominations of being well-liked,
Within inclusive educational settings, children with autism popular and socially skilled and that the correlates would
(CWA) experience low peer acceptance, companionship, be somewhat similar for the CWA and typical child.
and reciprocity (Chamberlain et al. 2007). Yet, when In order to capture various aspects of the social milieu,
involved in friendships with typical peers, CWA enjoy we utilized measures of social status, social influence, and
relationships characterized by positive social engagement behavioral characteristics in our search for social correlates
and experiences that resemble typical dyadic friendships, of peer buddy nominations. Our strategy was influenced by
which may be due, in part, to contact with typical peers recent re-conceptualizations of popularity which include
(Bauminger et al. 2008). To combat social isolation, peer- both sociometric popularity and perceived popularity.
mediated interventions have been used to improve the Sociometric popularity is grounded in the psychological
tradition of measuring social status through sociometric
techniques (e.g., Coie and Dodge 1983), which results in
J. M. Campbell (&)  C. A. Marino
identification of peers who are well liked, accepted, or
Department of Educational Psychology and Instructional
Technology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA preferred as a friend. Perceived popularity, however, is
e-mail: jmcmpbll@uga.edu aligned with sociology and based on a person’s attainments,

123
1360 J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:1359–1363

attributes, possessions, and activities of social prestige and videotape sequence as NO-AUT/AUT and seven classrooms
influence (Lease et al. 2002). Different behavioral charac- (51.6%) with 140 students (46.7%) viewed the sequence as
teristics are associated with each group. Perceived popular AUT/NO-AUT.
children are often identified as ‘‘cool,’’ socially prominent,
prestigious, and hold a level of dominance within the peer Measures
group; whereas, sociometric popular children are found to
be prosocial and likeable, but not necessarily dominant or Sociometric status. Students nominated three classmates
powerful amongst peers (Lease et al.). We reasoned that they ‘‘like to play with the most’’ (LM) and three they ‘‘like
both aspects of social status may be important in peers’ to play with the least’’ (LM; Coie and Dodge 1983). LM
decision-making regarding whom to select to facilitate and LL nominations were standardized within classroom
social entry into a classroom of unfamiliar students. and gender and used to generate social preference (SP;
LMZ–LLZ) and social impact (SI; LMZ ? LLZ) scores. SP
and SI scores were also standardized within classroom and
gender (i.e., M = 0; SD = 1). Coie and Dodge’s classifi-
Methods
cation strategy was used to classify children into
sociometric groups: (a) popular, SP [ 1.0, LMZ [ 0, and
Participants
LLZ \ 0; (b) rejected, if SP \ -1.0, LMZ \ 0, and LLZ
score [ 0; (c) controversial, SI score [ 1.0, and LMZ and
Participants were 293 third-, fourth-, and fifth-graders [144
LLZ [ 0; (d) neglected, if SI score \ -1.0, and LMZ and
boys (49.1%), 149 girls (50.9%); M age = 9.50 years;
LLZ \ 0; (e) average, all other unclassified children.
SD = 1.05] from 15 regular education classrooms from
Perceived popularity and peer buddy nominations.
two elementary schools. Participation rates ranged from 79
Students nominated three classmates they believed were
to 100% across classrooms (M = 90.14; SD = 6.15). A
‘‘most popular at school’’ (MP) and three thought to be
detailed description of the participants appears in Jackson
‘‘least popular at school’’ (LP). After each videotape,
and Campbell (2009).
students were asked: ‘‘who would you pick as a buddy to
help this child fit in with the other kids in your class?’’
Procedure Nominations were standardized within classroom and
gender to adjust for differences in nominators across
Participants completed nominations of social status, classes and account for same-gender preferences for
behavioral characteristics, and social influence using ros- nominations.
ters of participating classmates. Students then watched two Behavioral characteristics and social influence. Partic-
videotapes and responded to questions about them. Data ipants also completed nominations of behavioral
were collected as part of a larger study examining the characteristics and social influence using a modified ver-
effect of an informational message about autism on chil- sion of the Revised Class Play (RCP; Masten et al. 1985), a
dren’s attitudes (Campbell et al. 2004). Each class watched widely used measure of peer reputation, behavioral char-
a videotape of a typical 12-year-old boy actor (‘‘Robby’’; acteristics, and social influence. Consistent with the
NO-AUT) and a second tape of the same child displaying original RCP measure, participants were asked to pretend
symptoms of autism frequently associated with lower that they were assigning roles (i.e., 11 RCP items) for an
functioning individuals, including gaze aversion, hand upcoming class play and to nominate three classmates who
flapping, immediate echolalia, and body rocking (AUT; fit each role (e.g., ‘‘This person makes good grades, is
Swaim and Morgan 2001; see ‘‘Appendix’’). Investigators smart, and usually knows the right answer;’’ ‘‘Smart’’
explained that the child on the videotape might be joining item). The RCP procedure shows good reliability, con-
their class. current validity, and predictive validity (e.g., Masten et al.
The child actor was the sibling of a child with autism and 1985; Gest et al. 2006), including analysis of single items
was asked to portray symptoms frequently displayed by his which are deemed reliable due to the use of multiple peer
brother. Four individuals familiar with symptoms of autism, raters (Lindstrom et al. 2007). As with the other peer
a parent of a CWA, a school psychologist, a child clinical nomination measures, RCP nominations were standardized
psychologist, and an advanced graduate student in child within classroom and gender (i.e., M = 0; SD = 1).
clinical psychology, reviewed the CWA videotape and Manipulation check. To ensure participants viewed the
agreed that the child actor accurately portrayed autistic conditions differently, we included a manipulation check
symptomatology (Swaim and Morgan). Videotape order was consisting of a single item: ‘‘How much like other kids in
counterbalanced randomly across classrooms; eight class- your class is Robby?’’ with four response options
rooms (53.3%) containing 153 students (52.2%) viewed the (1 = ‘‘Very different,’’ 2 = ‘‘Sort of different,’’ 3 = ‘‘Sort

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:1359–1363 1361

of the same,’’ and 4 = ‘‘Very much the same.’’). Partici- Table 2 Summary of simultaneous regression analysis for variables
pants rated the child in the NO-AUT videotape as more predicting peer buddy nominations for child without (NO-AUT) and
with autism (AUT)
similar (M = 3.16; SD = 1.06) when compared to the
child in the AUT videotape (M = 1.77; SD = 1.01; Variable NO-AUT AUT
t (282) = 18.47, p \ .001). B SE B b B SE B b

Social status
Results Like most .04 .06 .04 .08 .07 .08
Like least -.10 .07 -.10 .11 .07 .11
Sociometric classifications were as follows: (a) popular, Most popular .23 .09 .25** .16 .10 .16
n = 41 (14.0%); (b) rejected, n = 46 (15.7%); (c) average, Least popular .13 .07 .13 .26 .07 .27**
n = 150 (51.2%); (d) controversial, n = 20 (6.8%); and Behavioral characteristics
(e) neglected, n = 36 (12.3%). A 2 (Child Type) 9 5 Cool .10 .08 .11 .03 .09 .04
(Sociometric) mixed-model ANOVA, revealed a main Socially skilled .08 .07 .09 -.01 .09 -.01
effect for Sociometric, F(4, 288) = 4.64, p \ .05; partial Helpful .14 .06 .14* .16 .06 .16*
g2 = .06, and significant Child Type 9 Sociometric Athletic .01 .06 .01 -.03 .07 -.03
interaction, F(4, 288) = 3.43, p \ .01; partial g2 = .05, for Smart .08 .08 .08 .22 .09 .22**
peer buddy nominations (see Table 1 for descriptive data). Values school -.03 .05 -.03 .04 .06 .04
For NO-AUT, Popular children received more peer buddy Social influence items
nominations than Rejected or Average children. For AUT, Leader .12 .08 .12 .00 .09 .00
Popular children received more peer buddy nominations Admire -.10 .08 -.11 .10 .09 .10
than Neglected children. As illustrated in Table 1, Rejected Influence .02 .08 .02 .03 .09 .03
students received more tutor nominations for the child with Control -.02 .07 -.02 -.29 .07 -.29***
AUT (M = .17) than the child with NO-AUT (M = -.33). Self-confident .16 .06 .16** .10 .06 .09
Peer buddy nominations did not differ between the NO-
* p \ .05
AUT and AUT conditions for the remaining sociometric
** p \ .01
categories.
*** p \ .001
Male gender correlated with NO-AUT, r(291) = .44,
p \ .001, and AUT, r(291) = .30, p \ .001, peer buddy
nominations. We conducted two separate simultaneous
regression analyses, one for each condition, to examine F(15, 291) = 12.35, p \ .001, and 28.9% of the variability
relationships between social and behavioral nomination in AUT buddy selections, F(15, 291) = 7.50, p \ .001.
items and peer buddy nominations. We entered all social ‘‘Helpful,’’ ‘‘smart,’’ and ‘‘self-confident’’ were behavioral
status, behavioral characteristics, and social influence characteristics that emerged as significant predictors for
nominations for each model and identified significant pre- both NO-AUT and AUT; however, AUT nominations were
dictors via t test values. As a set, social status, behavioral also predicted by ‘‘like least’’ and ‘‘least popular’’ nomi-
characteristics, and social influence nominations accounted nations while NO-AUT selections were predicted by ‘‘like
for 40.2% of the variability in NO-AUT buddy selections, most,’’ ‘‘cool,’’ and ‘‘influential’’ nominations (Table 2).
NO-AUT and AUT selections were moderately correlated,
r(291) = .31, p \ .001.
Table 1 Means and standard deviations for peer buddy nominations
by sociometric status and presence or absence of autism
Sociometric group No autism Autism Discussion
(NO-AUT) (AUT)

Popular (n = 41) .58 (1.01)a,b .42 (1.03)a Similar to teachers’ peer buddy selections (Jackson and
Rejected (n = 46) -.33 (.92) a
.17 (1.06) Campbell 2009), peers’ nominations were more likely male
Average (n = 150) .07 (.87)b .00 (.91) as is to be expected given the strong same-gender prefer-
Controversial (n = 20) .13 (.74) .08 (.78) ence that students show in middle childhood. Popular
Neglected (n = 36) .03 (.95) -.25 (.77)a students received more peer buddy nominations for an
unfamiliar CWA when compared to Neglected children.
Standard deviations appear in parentheses. Scores are standardized
within classrooms and gender (M = 0; SD = 1). Means with like
Rejected students were nominated more frequently for the
superscripts within columns differed on Tukey post hoc tests CWA versus the typical child. Similar to peer buddy
(p \ .05) nominations for the typical child, nominations for the

123
1362 J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:1359–1363

CWA were associated with prosocial characteristics, i.e., Appendix


showing concern and being helpful toward others. In con-
trast, however, peer buddies for the CWA were perceived Video Script for Typical Child (NO-AUT Condition)
as unpopular and lacking control within the social milieu,
but were seen as smart students who made good grades. ‘‘This is Robby. He is about the same age as you, and he
Elementary students seemed to have selected peer goes to school like you do. Robby’s family lives in a town
buddies for the CWA with a mixed social and behavioral called Woodmont. Woodmont is a medium sized town that
profile. On one hand, peer buddy selections for the CWA has schools, churches, shopping malls, movie theaters,
demonstrated characteristics that would seem to facilitate libraries, and most of the other things towns have. Robby
social acceptance within the classroom, such as being and his family go to the zoo, the shopping mall, and
helpful, showing concern, and making good grades. On the church. During the summer, they sometimes go to a park or
negative side, however, the selected peers’ social standing a lake to enjoy being outdoors. Robby also likes to eat at
does not appear to be conducive to fostering widespread McDonald’s.’’
peer acceptance. We identify two possible reasons for the [Voice over ends. Adult female walks into view, toward
low social influence of peer buddies for CWA. First, peers Robby, and says, ‘‘Hi Robby, what are you doing, working
report negative initial attitudes towards unfamiliar CWA on a puzzle?’’ Robby responds, ‘‘Yeah. It’s one I got at the
(e.g., Campbell et al. 2004), which may be associated in mall. Its got sharks and fish and stuff on it.’’ Adult female
some way with unpopular peers within their own class- replies, ‘‘Do you want to go play with one of your friends
rooms. A second possibility is related to the idea of when you’re done with the puzzle?’’ Robby responds,
homophily, i.e., the tendency for children to be attracted to ‘‘Sure! That sounds like fun!’’ Adult female state, ‘‘Well it
and form relationships with others who share similar looks like you’re doing a real good job, Robby.’’ Robby
characteristics (Farmer and Farmer 1996). Recall that the replies, ‘‘Thanks.’’ Adult female walks out of view, and
task was to identify peers who will help a CWA ‘‘fit in’’ voice-over returns].
with other students in the class. As such, students may ‘‘Robby likes to do many of the things you do. He likes
have matched the CWA with peers of a similar ilk, those to watch television, play with puzzles and put model cars
who may experience social difficulties and perhaps mar- together. Robby lives in Woodmont now, but his family
ginalization within the classroom. Peers may have might be moving to your neighborhood soon. If they do,
identified a social niche for the CWA’s entry into the he’ll be starting at your school and may be in your class.’’
classroom, a subgroup of peers who are neither popular nor
dominant, but nevertheless, helpful, empathic, and smart. Video Script for Child with Autism (AUT Condition)
Affiliation with a small network of peers is clearly a
favorable social outcome for CWA, and peers may have ‘‘This is Robby. He is about the same age as you, and he
suggested an appropriate network in their peer buddy goes to school like you do. Robby’s family lives in a town
selections. called Woodmont. Woodmont is a medium sized town that
Our conclusions must be tempered by the analogue has schools, churches, shopping malls, movie theaters,
nature of our investigation, which limits the social validity libraries, and most of the other things towns have. Robby
of our findings. Students informed their peer buddy selec- and his family go to the zoo, the shopping mall, and
tions based on information viewed from videotape as church. During the summer, they sometimes go to a park or
opposed to making decisions with respect to a ‘‘real’’ CWA a lake to enjoy being outdoors. Robby also likes to eat at
who may be entering their class. We also restricted our McDonald’s.’’
pool of RCP nomination items by excluding negative [Voice-over ends. Adult female walks into view, toward
behavioral characteristics that may describe peers within Robby, and says, ‘‘Hi Robby, what are you doing working
the classroom (e.g., withdrawn; aggressive; odd). In ret- on a puzzle?’’ Robby replies, ‘‘Hi Robby working on
rospect, including items of this sort would assist in fuller puzzle’’ while showing gaze aversion and stereotypic hand
understanding the behavioral profile of the CWA peer movements and rocking. Adult female replies, ‘‘Do you
buddy group. A final limitation involves our lack of want to go play with one of your friends when you’re done
manipulation check regarding whether children understood with the puzzle?’’ Robby replies, ‘‘Friend done with puz-
that the child viewed in the AUT condition was portraying zle.’’ Adult female states, ‘‘Well, it looks like you’re doing
symptoms of autism. Although we included a general a real good job, Robby.’’ Robby replies ‘‘Good job
manipulation check regarding students’ perceptions of Robby.’’ Adult female walks out of view, and voice-over
similarity to classmates, we did not specifically ask about returns].
their understanding of the AUT message included in the ‘‘Robby likes to do many of the things you do. He likes
videotape. to watch television, play with puzzles and put model cars

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:1359–1363 1363

together. Robby lives in Woodmont now, but his family Chamberlain, B., Kasari, C., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2007). Involve-
might be moving to your neighborhood soon. If they do, ment or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in
regular classrooms. Journal of Autism and Developmental
he’ll be starting at your school and may be in your class. Disorders, 37, 230–242. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0164-4.
Robby has autism, which means that there’s something Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Continuities and changes in
wrong with his brain that makes it hard for him to look at children’s social status: A five-year longitudinal study. Merrill-
other people and talk to them. When he talks, he sometimes Palmer Quarterly, 29, 261–282.
Farmer, T. W., & Farmer, E. M. Z. (1996). Social relationships of
repeats what was said to him instead of answering the students with exceptionalities in mainstream classrooms: Social
question. And sometimes it seems like he can’t hear, even networks and homophily. Exceptional Children, 62, 431–450.
though he can. Sometimes Robby waves his hands around, Gest, S. D., Sesma, A., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2006).
or spins around or rocks back and forth, or bounces up and Childhood peer reputation as a predictor of competence and
symptoms 10 years later. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
down in his chair. He may even hit or bite himself or other ogy, 34, 509–526. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9029-8.
people and things And Robby has a hard time changing Jackson, J. N., & Campbell, J. M. (2009). Teachers’ peer buddy
activities, going from one thing to doing something else. selections for children with autism: Social characteristics and
Robby doesn’t mean to be different or hard to get along relationship with peer nominations. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 39, 269–277. doi:10.1007/s10803-
with. He’s not trying to make trouble; he does these things 008-0623-1.
because he has autism. But people with autism can learn to Laushey, K. M., & Heflin, J. (2000). Enhancing social skills of
do many of the things that people without autism can do, kindergarten children with autism through the training of
like talk to and play with other people, go to school, read, multiple peers as tutors. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 30, 183–193. doi:10.1023/A:1005558101038.
and write, and even get jobs or go to college when they Lease, A. M., Kennedy, C. A., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002). Children’s
grow up. They just need some help to learn how to do these social constructions of popularity. Social Development, 11, 87–
things.’’ 109. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00188.
Lindstrom, W. A., Lease, A. M., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2007). Peer-
and self-rated correlates of a teacher-rated typology of child
adjustment. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 579–599. doi:
10.1002/pits.20249.
References Masten, A. S., Morison, P., & Pellegrini, D. S. (1985). A revised class
play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology, 21,
Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., Aviezer, A., Heung, K., Brown, J., & 523–533. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.523.
Rogers, S. J. (2008). Friendship in high-functioning children Swaim, K. F., & Morgan, S. B. (2001). Children’s attitudes and
with autism spectrum disorder: Mixed and non-mixed dyads. behavioral intentions toward a peer with autistic behaviors: Does
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1211– a brief educational intervention have an effect? Journal of
1229. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0501-2. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 195–205. doi:10.1023/
Campbell, J. M., Ferguson, J. E., Herzinger, C. V., Jackson, J. N., & A:1010703316365.
Marino, C. A. (2004). Combined descriptive and explanatory
information improves peers’ perceptions of autism. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 25, 321–339. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.
2004.01.005.

123

S-ar putea să vă placă și