Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Psychotherapy Practice and Research

Repairing a Strained Alliance

Marvin R. Goldfried State University of New York at Stony Brook


Barry E. Wolfe American Schools of Professional Psychology,
Virginia Campus

Although the gap between psychotherapy practice and re- efficacy of specific interventions (e.g., Task Force on Pro-
search has been present for some time, recent pressures motion and Dissemination of Psychological Procedures,
for accountability from outside the system--managed 1995). It is in this context that we believe it essential for
health care and biological psychiatry--necessitate that clinician and researcher to join forces to come up with a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

we take steps to close this gap. One such step has been better way of demonstrating how our various psycho-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for psychotherapy researchers to specify a list of empiri- therapy interventions are not only empirically grounded
cally validated therapies. However, as researchers who also but also clinically meaningful.
have a strong allegiance to clinical practice, we are con- We wrote this article as psychotherapy researchers
cerned that the conceptual and methodological constraints who also have a strong affinity for clinical practice. As
associated with outcome research may become clinical researchers, we feel more than a little bit of guilt for having
constraints for the practicing therapist. We firmly believe become involved in a research paradigm that has been
that, more than ever before, the time is ripe for us to de- too far removed from the real world of therapy practice.
velop a new outcome research paradigm that involves an This is certainly not a new phenomenon, and it has even
active collaboration between researcher and practicing been suggested in the past that "there apigears to be an
clinician. inverse relationship between the frequency with which a
treatment form is actually used by practitioners and the
frequency with which that treatment has been studied"
(Parloff, 1979, p. 304). At present, however, we are par-

S
ociologists have long documented how economic
ticularly concerned that the methodological and concep-
~and political forces can have an unexpected and
tual constraints associated with outcome research may
very powerful impact on various scientific disci-
very well turn into clinical constraints for the practicing
plines (Cole & Cole, 1973; Merton, 1938/1970). In the
therapist (cf. Frances, 1994). Yet, we continue to remain
early 1940s, World War II created numerous challenges
strong advocates of psychotherapy research. In many re-
that resulted in a huge collaborative effort in the field of
spects, our dilemma may be thought of as reflecting a
physics and other disciplines. Similarly, the Soviet Union's
conflict between a wish and a fear: Our wish is that therapy
Sputnik in the late 1950s gave rise to a host of scientific
interventions be based on psychotherapy research; our
and technological advances that might not otherwise have
fear, however, is that they might.
occurred. Although there is no doubt that the field of
psychotherapy has made important progress in the past The Worlds of the Scientist and
decade or two, it is likely that the most dramatic changes Practitioner
we are about to witness are likely to come from outside
forces. These external factors consist of pressures to justify Although therapists and researchers often begin with
empirically how we practice clinically, coming in the form similar professional training, they eventually end up living
of challenges from biological psychiatry and managed and working in very different worlds. We are keenly aware
health care. of this distinction; between the two of us, we have spent
The impetus for writing this article was our concern
that, in response to such pressures, psychotherapy out- Marvin R. Goldfried, Department of Psychology,State University of
come researchers may have overreacted and moved the New Yorkat Stony Brook; Barry E. Wolfe,American Schoolsof Profes-
field in the wrong direction, in other words, away from sional Psychology,Virginia Campus.
We would like to acknowledgewith appreciation the helpful com-
the day-to-day context of clinical practice. Although psy- ments and feedback on an earlier version of this article by Thomas D.
chotherapy research has appropriately increased in Borkovec, Louis G. Castonguay, Dianne L. Chambless, Robert M.
methodological rigor, it has become overly dependent on Liebert, Morris B. Parlofl, George Stricker, and Phoebus N. Tongas.
the "clinical trials" method to determine how to best Work on this article was supported in part by National Institute
treat "disorders." In addition to condoning the medical- of Mental Health Grant 40196.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
ization of psychotherapy, psychotherapy researchers may Marvin R. Goldfried, Department of Psychology,State University of
unwittingly be playing into the hands of third-party payers New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500. Electronic
in placing unwarranted emphasis on the putative fixed mail may be sent via Internet to mgoldfried@ccmail.sunysb.edu.

October 1996 • American Psychologist 1007


Copyright 1996 by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. 0003-066X/96/$2.00
Vol. 51, No. 10, 1007-1016
the last 50 years of our professional lives working in both problems, (b) research on the process of change, and (c)
of these worlds. Wolfe has recently completed a 22-year psychotherapy outcome research.
tenure in the extramural program of the National Institute
of Mental Health (NIMH), overseeing the research grant Basic Research on Clinical Problems
portfolio in psychotherapy research. Goldfried has de- As Arkowitz (1988) suggested, basic research on psycho-
voted his career to the development and evaluation of pathology can assist practicing therapists by providing
intervention procedures and the study of the therapeutic them with information about the "what" that needs to
cbange process. In addition to our involvement in psy- be changed. In essence, such research can provide in-
chotherapy research, we have each maintained a practice valuable findings relevant to the variables/dynamics/de-
of psychotherapy. Living in both these worlds, we have terminants that are associated with the kind of patient
been able to witness firsthand the different sets of demands difficulties one is likely to encounter clinically.
and limitations associated with research and practice. The Take the example of panic disorder, in which a shift
professional lives of clinicians are based on reliable re- was made in the centrality of panic attacks between Di-
ferrals, the ability to establish good therapeutic alliances, agnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
and a demonstration of clinical effectiveness. Researchers, Third Edition (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Associ-
on the other hand, put much of their energy in publishing, ation, 1980) and the D S M - I I I - R (revised; American
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

obtaining research grants, and achieving professional Psychiatric Association, 1987). At present, panic is seen
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

recognition. as being primary, with the avoidance associated with ago-


One of us once participated in a roundtable discus- raphobia viewed as a means of coping with the fear of
sion at a psychotherapy research conference, in which the panic. This change in emphasis was based on findings
focus was on how to best transmit psychotherapy research that panic attacks temporally precede the development
findings to the practicing therapist. Throughout the dis- of agoraphobic avoidance behavior (Thyer & Himle,
cussion, the point was continually made that the practic- 1985) and that there appear to be comparable character-
ing clinician was "not a good consumer" of research find- istics of patients with panic disorder, whether or not there
ings. Were this a meeting of a corporate board seeking is an associated agoraphobic avoidance (Turner, Williams,
to understand the failure of its product to reach the in- Beidel, & Mezzich, 1986). The finding that patients with
tended market, the likely discussion would not have been panic were more likely than control participants to ex-
on the shortcomings of the consumer but on what could perience panic attacks in laboratory settings in which they
be done to make the product more appealing. What this were instructed to hyperventilate led to the clinical strat-
reflects is the somewhat dysfunctional relationship be- egy of actually provoking panic attacks within the ther-
tween scientist and practitioner. It is clearly not an egal- apeutic session as a way of exposing patients to and en-
itarian partnership, but rather one in which each views couraging them to cope with such attacks (Barlow, 1988).
the other with a certain a m o u n t of disdain. To a very Laboratory findings that the resting heart rates of patients
great extent, each has difficulty in understanding the needs with panic tend to be higher than those with other anxiety
and concerns of the other, and each rarely validates the disorders suggest that clinicians may need to focus on
legitimacy of the other's activities. lowering the overall chronic overarousal of patients with
Although psychotherapy researchers presumably are panic disorder. Also, the prominent role played by the
engaging in activities that can have implications for prac- catastrophic interpretations of unusual bodily sensations
tice, it is fairly well accepted that researchers typically in the development and maintenance of panic disorder
write for other researchers; the implications for clinical has led to the emergence of a strong cognitive focus in
practice, more often than not, are an afterthought. The the treatment of panic disorder (Barlow, 1988).
description of the methodological rigor that constitutes
the current state-of-the-art in psychotherapy research and Psychotherapy Process Research
the unique set of jargon associated with it (e.g., "treatment
fidelity" and "end-state functioning") receives far more In contrast to basic research, which deals with the "what"
emphasis in the literature than does the kind of infor- of therapeutic change, psychotherapy process research
mation that clinicians would find useful to their work. deals with the "how" (Arkowitz, 1988). Process research
We are not suggesting that our standards for method- dates back to the 1940s, when Rogers and his associates
ological rigor or the way research is presented to other took the bold step of making and studying wire recordings
of psychotherapy sessions (Strupp & Howard, 1992). This
researchers be changed, but that steps be taken to close
eventually led to the development of numerous proce-
the clinical-research gap. Before suggesting some possi-
bilities, however, we would like to c o m m e n t on the re- dures for analyzing therapy sessions in the hope of better
understanding the nature of the therapist-patient inter-
search bases of psychotherapy.
action (Kiesler, 1973). However, such research activities
Research Bases of Psychotherapy soon fell by the wayside (Strupp, 1973). Much of this
early psychotherapy process research involved discrete
There are three primary sources of information that can and isolated transactions between therapist and patient
inform the practicing therapist about how to proceed (e.g., silences) rather than functional units that might
clinically. These include (a) basic research on clinical conceivably reflect the process of change.

1008 October 1996 • American Psychologist


In more recent years, a new generation of process behavioral, experiential, and psychodynamic views, were
research has evolved in which the primary focus is to barely able to touch on the high points of research that
look at those aspects of psychotherapy that are likely to had been conducted in the treatment of the wide variety
contribute to change (Greenberg & Pinsof, 1986). Rather of clinical problems that had been studied over the pre-
than studying "psychotherapy process" in general, this vious four years. During the 40-year interval between
more recent approach to process research has emphasized these two reviews, outcome research in psychotherapy
studying the "process of change." The question addressed increased at a dramatic rate, moving through three gen-
by this approach to psychotherapy process research is as erations of methodological vigor.
follows: "What has the therapist done to have a particular The first generation of outcome research occurred
impact on the patient?" In many respects, we can think between the 1950s and 1960s. On the basis of the work
of such research as an attempt to construct maps that of Rogers (Strupp & Howard, 1992) and that carried out
can more clearly depict the therapeutic change process. by researchers at the Menninger Foundation, the Uni-
The implication for the practicing clinician becomes quite versity of Pennsylvania, and other facilities, the question
clear if the research question becomes slightly rephrased that was addressed was whether or not psychotherapy was
to read, "What can the therapist do to have a particular effective in producing personality change (Strupp &
impact on the client?" Howard, 1992). What characterizes this very early phase
Among the many process research findings having in the history of psychotherapy outcome research is that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

important clinical implications are those indicating that there was little specification or differentiation among the
in a psychodynamically oriented intervention, the ther- forms of therapy that were addressed and that the clinical
apist's interpretations that are based specifically on case problems and the nature of outcome were usually con-
formulations have more of an immediate emotional im- sidered in a very general way (Kiesler, 1966).
pact than do transference interpretations that are based During the 1960s and 1970s, we moved into the
on general theoretical considerations (Silberschatz, Fretter, next generation of research, in which there was a change
& Curtis 1986). In process analyses of cognitive therapy in the question that was being addressed by psychotherapy
for depression, an intriguing and somewhat unexpected outcome researchers. Largely because of the efforts of be-
finding has been that clients' level of emotional experience havior therapists, the question became not whether "psy-
and a focus on their relationship with their parents are chotherapy works" but which specific procedures are
positively associated with treatment outcome (Caston- more effective in dealing with a specific clinical problem
guay, Goldfried, Wiser, Raue, & Hayes, 1996; Hayes, (Franks, 1969). Consequently, various target problems
Castonguay, & Goldfried, 1996). were dealt with (e.g., phobias and unassertiveness), and
It is of particular interest that many of the coding specific therapy interventions that were based on written
systems used by psychotherapy process researchers can guidelines, in which there was random assignment of pa-
be used as training guidelines for practicing therapists. tients to different treatment conditions, became the state
Thus, Benjamin's (1993) Structural Analysis of Social of the art. Although a methodological advance over the
Behavior (SASB) can allow the clinician to monitor on earlier approach to answering the outcome question, this
an ongoing basis the nature of the therapeutic interaction second generation was limited because college student
along the dimensions of control and affiliation. Safran volunteers rather than actual patients were used in much
and his colleagues (Safran, Crocker, McMain, & Murray, of the research, and graduate students often served as the
1990) have provided guidelines for recognizing aspects of therapists. Still, it set the stage for the next generation of
the therapeutic interaction in which there may be a strain research.
in the alliance and what might be done to alleviate this Beginning in the 1980s, psychotherapy research
situation. The work of Greenberg, Rice, and Elliott (1993) moved onto a third generation of methodology. Continu-
on therapeutic markers can alert therapists to those points ing in the tradition of Generation II, which involved time-
in the therapy session that can call for them to proceed limited interventions that compared different treatments
therapeutically in different ways. Methods for classifying for particular clinical problems, Generation III research
a therapist's style of responding can provide clinicians has been called clinical trials--the terminology associated
with guidelines for how to word a verbal interaction (Hill, with drug studies. Further reflecting the shift toward the
1986; Stiles, 1992). Finally, process research on the ther- medical model, the methodology constituting this most
apeutic focus can have important clinical implications recent approach to psychotherapy outcome research in-
for identifying the dynamics/determinants of clinical volves the use of D S M diagnoses, particularly on Axis I.
problems, thereby assisting in both the initial and ongoing In addition, highly detailed therapy manuals are used,
case formulation (Goldfried, 1995). whereby therapists are monitored for their adherence to
the particular procedures being studied.
Psychotherapy Outcome Research Many of these methodological "upgrades" were fos-
In 1950, Snyder provided the first review of psychotherapy tered and ratified by the NIMH Treatment of Depression
research to appear in the Annual Review of Psychology, Collaborative Research Program, in which the original
in which he was able to cover the work done in the entire purpose was to test the feasibility of conducting a multisite
field within a single chapter. Four decades later, Goldfried, clinical trial in psychotherapy (Elkin, Parloff, Hadley, &
Greenberg, and Marmar (1990), representing cognitive- Autry, 1985). Although this question was intrinsically of

October 1996 • American Psychologist 1009


great interest to psychotherapy researchers, reflecting the psychodynamic therapy researchers about the importance
desire of the field to develop a cumulative knowledge base of including such personality variables as interpersonal
regarding the effectiveness of different forms of psycho- skills and self-concept in an outcome battery rather than
therapy, it was shaped by other more social and economic only measures of s y m p t o m reduction. This was reminis-
variables, such as (a) the growing hegemony of the psy- cent of similar discussions between psychodynamic and
chiatric model of mental illness; (b) the purported success behavior therapists in the 1970s, with the exception that
of efficacy studies of various psychotropic pharmacolog- the positions were exactly reversed!
ical agents; and (c) the need to respond to institutional On the basis of the assumption that the most recent
pressures from Congress, health care agencies, third-party generation of research is better than the earlier ones, there
payers, and the public to provide convincing data of the tends to be a neglect of findings obtained by psychother-
effectiveness of psychotherapy (Parloff & Elkin, 1992). apy researchers prior to the 1980s. Despite the fact that
The immediate result of the N I M H study has been practicing clinicians will acknowledge that interpersonal
to encourage the view that the conduct of large clinical assertiveness frequently plays a very important role as a
trials in psychotherapy is not only feasible but mandatory. determinant/dynamic in m a n y clinical problems (e.g.,
Such research now has assumed the mantle of final arbiter depression and panic attacks), "assertiveness" remains a
of efficacy in the field of psychotherapy research. These 1970s concept and appears to be out of fashion. Indeed,
studies are extensive, expensive, and difficult to conduct. because it does not represent a D S M category, it is un-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

It should be noted that these large studies focus primarily likely that N I M H funding would be available to psycho-
on the use of a particular theoretical approach in reduc- therapy outcome researchers who wanted to study this
tion of the symptomatology associated with specific D S M clinically relevant characteristic.
diagnosed disorders. Our sense is that the field has become caught up in
One of the casualties of this movement to large-scale a research paradigm that does not faithfully reflect clinical
clinical trials--at least with respect to N I M H research reality. The very nature of our current research meth-
grant funding--was psychotherapy process research. odology, constrained by a need for D S M diagnoses, limits
Process research was increasingly subordinated to the task the kinds of questions that are studied. For example, the
of establishing the efficacy of standardized psychothera- typical finding that patients with personality disorders
pies. Its deemphasis was rationalized by the contention have poorer prognoses in the treatment of various clinical
that it was not necessary or useful to conduct research symptoms has raised the question about the ways that
on the process of a specific psychotherapy until it had Axis II disorders are related to Axis I disorders. We would
been established that psychotherapy was effective. As a think that a more relevant question to ask would be about
result, N I M H funding for psychotherapy process research how certain personality characteristics contribute to the
began to p l u m m e t in the late 1980s. The grant portfolio development of anxiety and depression, such as the extent
in psychotherapy process research was reduced by over to which patients become depressed because of patterns
60%, going from 16 grants in 1986 to 6 grants in 1990 of interpersonal behavior that bring about negative re-
(Wolfe, 1993). actions from others; unfortunate life circumstances; the
The medical model of outcome research, with its failure to grieve a loss; a passive interpersonal style in
emphasis on disorders and their symptoms in current responding to problematic life issues; a tendency to cog-
clinical trials, also has the particular limitation of ne- nitively distort; or a prevailing view of oneself as a failure.
glecting the key determinants/dynamics that clinicians In the typical clinical trials paradigm, one "pure-
know well to be essential to the change process. This lim- form" theoretical approach is compared with another in
itation is illustrated in a recent N I M H conference devoted the treatment of a D S M category. Unlike clinical practice,
to developing a standardized outcome battery for panic in which we as therapists often find it more effective to
disorder research (Shear & Maser, in press). After speci- use interventions associated with different therapeutic
fying m a n y of the indications of symptomatology that orientations (Norcross & Goldfried, 1992), our current
should be included in any clinical trial focusing on panic research methodology allows little room for taking into
disorder, the conference participants concluded that it account the relevant patient determinants/dynamics that
was "not essential" to include any evaluation of person- may influence what we should do clinically. Instead, dif-
ality variables in measuring outcome. This somewhat ferent therapy interventions are administered, which typ-
surprising conclusion was based on the lack of clarity in ically focus on the preferred variables associated with a
"comorbidity" between Axes I and II as well as the dif- given theoretical orientation (e.g., cognition, behavior, af-
ficulty in measuring "personality disorders." fect, and interpersonal systems). These different therapy
Like our clients, we as psychotherapy researchers approaches are compared, often resulting in findings that
become socialized to think and behave in ways that can fail to result in a differential effectiveness between
be limiting. In our own involvement in a 1994 American orientations.
Psychological Association (APA) sponsored conference Moreover, our outcome research is characterized by
at Vanderbilt University on the development of a core a basic dilemma with respect to the type of patients that
assessment battery for outcome research, we encountered eventually participate in treatment efficacy studies. This
a similar phenomenon. Representing a cognitive-behav- quandary might be called the interpretability/generaliz-
ioral orientation, we found ourselves trying to convince ability dilemma. In order to improve the interpretability

1010 October 1996 • American Psychologist


of findings, rigorous inclusion and exclusion criteria are findings will be used to limit us clinically, but also how
used for the selection of research patients. As the n u m b e r we may inadvertently be encouraging this.
and rigor of these criteria increase, the generalizability of To begin with, our research base of outcome research
the findings from treatment efficacy research decreases. is insufficient because of what it does not tell the practicing
However, any effort to reduce the gap between research clinician. The most general problem, as Havens (1994)
patients and "real-world" patients leads to the decreased noted, is that there is a gap between the global nature of
interpretability of research findings. For example, the research findings and the usually specific nature of clinical
typical panic disorder patient seen in a private practice dilemmas. More specifically, we would suggest that clin-
setting would often meet criteria for other Axis I and II ical trial data do not tell us the following:
disorders. Yet the available treatment-efficacy research
• How to treat patients who suffer from more than
data are typically based on patients whose clinical pictures
one Axis I disorder, which is more typical of what
are not nearly as complicated. This leads to a translation
the clinician faces on a daily basis.
problem when practitioners attempt to apply empirically
• Why treatments work. It may well be that there
validated treatments to real-world patients.
are mechanisms and processes other than those
This fact was humorously yet sadly brought home
outlined by the specific brand-name treatment
to one of us (Wolfe) when I had organized an N I M H
under study that are responsible for whatever
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

research workshop a number of years ago on the treatment


changes occur in the patient's symptoms.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of specific and social phobias. After listening to several


• How to treat underlying "personality" issues and
of the world's experts declaim on the ease with which
other determinants/dynamics that may be directly
simple phobias are treated, often within one to five ses-
related to symptoms of a particular disorder. It is
sions, he said with great exasperation: "I don't know what no longer tenable, if it ever was, to argue that these
kinds of patients are being treated in such studies, because issues either do not exist or are not relevant for
the simple phobic patients that I see in my private practice
effective treatment.
are often quite difficult to treat and require protracted • What to do with variations within a given disorder.
periods of treatments." One of the participants responded, For example, some years ago, Chambless and
"Barry, you are overlooking the first law of research: Don't Goldstein (1988) had identified simple and com-
use real patients." This was said in jest, but the reality of plex forms of agoraphobia, which required some-
the clinician-researcher gap was very much apparent in what different treatment plans. Similarly, different
the uneasy laughter that exploded in the room. forms of depression have been identified, includ-
Addressing the gap between psychotherapy outcome ing achievement-based and abandonment-based
research and the way psychotherapy is conducted in the forms of depression (Blatt, 1974). It is not clear
real world, Seligman (1995) similarly concluded the fol- from the available efficacy data what the practicing
lowing: " T h e efficacy study is the wrong method for em- clinician should do with each subtype of disorder.
pirically validating psychotherapy as it is actually done,
because it omits too m a n y crucial elements of what is Clinical trial efficacy data are also mute on how to
done in the field" (p. 966). Although lacking the tight resolve clinical impasses and dilemmas that typically
controls and internal validity characteristic of Generation characterize psychotherapy, independent of the specific
III research, the more externally valid Consumer Reports nature of the disorder under treatment. That our outcome
(1995) survey of psychotherapy patients is offered as a research often overlooks these clinical realities is illus-
means of documenting therapeutic effectiveness. trated in an observation made by Raw (1993), a practicing
therapist who lamented the difficulties he had in applying
The Use of Outcome Research by outcome findings to his clinical work. He reported at-
Policymakers tending a conference in which a well-known therapy re-
searcher described his method of dealing with the problem
The gap that exists between research and practice is cer- of noncompliance, which was to tell patients that if they
tainly not a new issue. Indeed, some may argue that it is did not follow through on what they were supposed to
a lost cause, and the entire topic should be laid to rest. do, they could not be included in the study. Not only
However, the current crisis associated with the influence does this procedure have limited use to practicing ther-
of insurance companies on how psychotherapy is prac- apists, it also throws into sharp relief how our research
ticed makes it imperative that we examine how the results methodology severely constrains our ability to generalize
of our outcome findings may be used by policymakers. to the clinical situation.
This concern was foreshadowed in an insightful article We realize that these observations fly in the face of
published several years ago by Parloff (1982), when it some recent attempts that are being taken to translate
seemed that some form of national health insurance might research findings into recommendations for clinical
be adopted. Parloff warned us about how the efforts of practice. Toward the goal of basing our therapeutic in-
psychotherapy researchers could be misused by policy- terventions on a stronger empirical foundation, the APA
makers, subtitling his article "Bambi Meets Godzilla." Divison of Clinical Psychology's Task Force on Promotion
This issue has never been dealt with adequately, and we and Dissemination of Psychological Procedures (1995)
are currently concerned not only about how our research was formed "to consider methods for educating clinical

October 1996 • American Psychologist 1011


psychologists, third party payers, and the public about into an opportunity remains to be seen. However, one
effective psychotherapies" (p. 3). After reviewing the out- thing is clear: The time is ripe for us to examine very
come research literature--primarily Generation I I I - - t h e seriously the gap between research and practice and take
task force came up with a list of "empirically validated bold steps to close this gap. We cannot afford to ignore
treatments" (EVTs) and recommended that EVTs be used the limitations of our current outcome research paradigm,
as criteria by site visitors for APA accreditation of grad- as it very well may dictate the future direction of psy-
uate programs, the accreditation of internship facilities, chotherapy in general.
approval of continuing education credit, and third-party We suggest three tiers of recommendations for clos-
payers. ing the gap. These include (a) extending our current psy-
As psychotherapy researchers, we certainly under- chotherapy outcome research paradigm; (b) increasingly
stand and support the intent of the task force. There are focusing on a new paradigm of psychotherapy research
far too many therapists who justify what they do clinically that emphasizes the elucidation of the various processes
on the basis of what has been done in the past, and they of change and individualizes the interventions being
desperately need to change their practice in light of cur- studied; and (c) increasing collaboration between clini-
rent findings. However, we have grave concerns about the cians and researchers.
possible impact of the task force's report. A question that
we often ask ourselves is whether we would want to refer Extending Our Current Psychotherapy
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a friend to a therapist whose training was based on the Paradigm


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

manual-driven intervention that has been used in our Standardized therapies that have been tested and found
clinical trials. We are very concerned that the available efficacious need to be applied in more everyday settings,
outcome research findings will be taken too literally (a) such as primary care settings, outpatient clinics, and
by therapists, who learn only the pure-form interventions community mental health centers. At this writing, the
used in clinical trials; (b) by clients, who expect that their N I M H has been attempting to support increasing re-
progress will parallel those carefully selected participants search out of its Services Research Branch to test the
in the research and be s y m p t o m free by a certain number clinical effectiveness of efficacious, standardized psycho-
of sessions; and (c) by managed health care policymakers, social treatments. Such research can give us clearer in-
who will limit clinical practice so that it conforms to the dications of the effects of treatments once they are applied
methodological constraints associated with psychotherapy in more real-world settings than those in which their ef-
outcome research. ficacy in controlled settings is tested.
There is yet another dilemma involved in this use
If we are to continue conducting clinical trials of
of clinical trial efficacy data, and that is the impossibly
standardized treatments, we must also focus on more than
large volume of research that will be required to establish
just the symptoms of a particular Axis I disorder. The
an EVT for a particular disorder. In 1980, Parloff and
high relapse rates and mediocre recovery rates that are
Wolfe (as cited in Herink, 1980) had calculated that the
associated with tested therapies in which efficacy has been
250 identified brand-names of therapy could be reduced
established for specific disorders indicate that symptom-
to 17 generic forms, and the number of groupings of pa-
tients from the 150 disorders listed in the D S M - I I I c o u l d focused treatments are necessary but not sufficient (Wolfe,
be collapsed to about 50. Parloff (1982) later reported, 1994). Clinical trials should now routinely study life
"Even this gross oversimplification would require 6,800 events, personality characteristics, as well as other theo-
clinical trial studies" (p. 724). retically derived variables that are presumed to be in-
In the current research scene, "Parloff's parody" volved in the generation or maintenance of symptoms of
still applies. Currently, the D S M - I V (American Psychi- the disorders under study. A related issue is the need to
atric Association, 1994) includes over 300 separate dis- study patients who are "comorbid" for more than one
orders. Even if we were to collapse them into 100 different D S M disorder, particularly those who would meet criteria
patient groups and reduce the therapeutic approaches to for an Axis II as well as an Axis I disorder.
10, we are talking about 1,000 clinical trials just to have In commenting on our current outcome research
1 study of each treatment for each separate patient group. paradigm, other psychotherapy researchers (e.g., Borko-
And if we adopt the APA Division of Clinical Psychology's vec, 1994; Davison, 1994; Davison & Lazarus, 1994) have
task force's recommendation of requiring at least 2 studies noted that the typical practice of comparing one type of
to verify the efficacy of a potential EVT, then we are up therapy with another is severely limited in that it provides
to 2,000 studies. Our question is, who will fund this no conceptual understanding about the clinical problem
quantity of research? As m a n y of us have so painfully or how the treatment might work. Answers to these ques-
learned in recent years, it will not be the N I M H . Unless tions can be built into the research design, argued Bor-
the field of psychotherapy research acquires a patron with kovec, if we were to include conditions that were com-
extremely "deep pockets," the task of establishing EVTs posed of different aspects of a treatment package, either
for the full range of D S M disorders is clearly not feasible. individually or in varying combinations. This is the re-
search strategy used in the second generation of outcome
W h e r e Do W e Go From Here? research on behavior therapy techniques, as when treat-
There is no question that the field of psychotherapy is ment conditions involving relaxation and imaginal ex-
approaching a crisis. Whether this crisis can also be turned posure to fearful situations were compared with system-

1012 October 1996 • American Psychologist


atic desensitization, involving both together. A model such other patient characteristics, in addition to the target
as this can be used to study other therapy orientations, symptom, that are critical to successful therapy. In their
such as a comparative investigation of the effects of trans- discussion of matching therapy intervention with relevant
ference interpretations that are linked to the past (i.e., patient characteristics, Beutler and Clarkin (1990) have
parental reactions) or involve reference to the therapist identified such dimensions as problem severity and level
without links to parental figures. of impairment, the extent to which patients' problems
are delimited or part of a more complex theme, the degree
An Alternative Paradigm for Psychotherapy to which patients are amenable to the influence of others,
Outcome Research
and whether or not patients have internal or external cop-
Despite its merits, the above-mentioned extensions of our ing styles. This list is not necessarily exhaustive, but il-
current research paradigm will neither provide us with lustrates some of the clinical realities that can influence
all of the information that a clinician will need, nor will the selection of the most effective intervention.
it substantially close the gap between research and prac- An a t t e m p t to take into account the matching of
tice. What we need is an alternate research paradigm for therapy intervention with relevant patient character-
building and testing an effective approach to psychother- istics was made by Nelson-Gray ( 1991 ) and her research
apy, one that both emerges from therapist-patient inter- group in an outcome study on the t r e a t m e n t of depres-
actions and individualizes the intervention for the par- sion. Their preliminary findings point to the superiority
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ticular case at hand. The implication of such research is of a cognitive-behavioral intervention that focuses on
that what needs to be specified and replicated is not brand- the particular variables that appear to be associated
name therapies but identifiable processes of patient with the patient's depression, such as irrational beliefs,
change and the therapist behaviors that bring these about. deficiencies in interpersonal behavior, or infrequent
Such research would be focused on at least two different gratifying activities. Similar encouraging but similarly
types of change processes: (a) intrapersonal and inter- preliminary findings were obtained by Jacobson and
personal indications of patient change and (b) therapist colleagues (Jacobson et al., 1989), who found a trend
actions that reliably bring about such patient change. For for cognitive-behavioral marital therapy to be m o r e
example, what therapist behaviors and patient indicators effective when it was individually designed to the needs
of change are present when intrusive traumatic memories of the particular couple than when it was based on a
have been emotionally processed (allowing the patient to predetermined intervention.
rebuild his or her sense of trust in people)? Another a p p r o a c h to individualizing our inter-
The need to develop a research paradigm that also ventions is to bring back research on the intensive study
individualizes the intervention on the basis of an initial of the single case. In c o m m e n t i n g on the need for a
assessment and case formulation is essential for closing m o r e idiographic research methodology for studying
the clinical-research gap, a point that has been discussed applied problems, Skinner was once said to have ob-
by Fishman (1981) and Persons (199 I), both practicing served that no one goes to the circus to see the average
clinicians. Referring to the second generation of outcome dog j u m p through the average hoop. Single-subject
research, Fishman (1981) commented on the gap between methodology in the study of psychotherapy outcome
research and practice by noting that "in their quest for has been c h a m p i o n e d in the past (e.g., Barlow, 1981;
'topographical equivalence,' researchers tend to lump to- Kazdin, 1981) but has fallen by the wayside with the
gether all subjects with the same manifest problem, re- ascendence of clinical trials. In a m o r e recent consid-
gardless of the etiological, mediational, contextual, and eration of single-case research in psychotherapy, Jones
maintenance facts that underlie and act to perpetuate the (1993) noted, " T h e p r i m a r y m e a n s of clinical inquiry,
maladaptive pattern" (p. 244). This issue was expanded teaching, and learning in psychotherapy has been and
on a decade later by Persons (1991) with the third gen- still remains the case-study method, grounded in the
eration of outcome research. Without an individualized tradition of naturalistic observation" (p. 371). The
assessment and case formulation as a key element in our promise of single-case research is that it may elucidate
outcome research, argued Persons, there is no way that with precision the link between what transpires in ther-
these research findings can intelligently inform practicing apy and patient change.
therapists; clinicians do not randomly assign patients to One of the main objections to single-case research
therapeutic interventions. Although we very much agree has been the difficulties that it presents for generalizing
with Person's thesis, we would go beyond her suggestion its findings to other patients. What has been either over-
that case formulation-intervention matching be done looked or just rejected is the idea that such studies rep-
within a particular theoretical orientation. Given the fact resent a different method of aggregating information
that practicing therapists do not typically adhere to a given across cases. Single-case data, as Hilliard (1993) proposed,
orientation (Norcross & Goldfried, 1992), the most clin- are basically intrasubject designs that explore the tem-
ically valid research paradigm is one that would not nec- poral unfolding of variables in individual participants.
essarily involve pure-form therapies (Wolfe & Goldfried, Generalization of findings is achieved by replication on
1988). a case-by-case basis. Too often in the past, single-case
To make our research findings more relevant to the research has not been conducted with clearly formulated
clinical setting, our paradigm needs to take into account questions in mind. However, Hilliard m a i n t a i n e d - - a n d

October 1996 • American Psychologist 1013


we a g r e e - - t h a t "theory-based, question-driven, single- however, they all moved o n - - i n somewhat different
case research, in which disconfirmation remains a real w a y s - - t o deal with issues in the client's life that appeared
possibility, is necessary within psychotherapy research" to have been related to the symptomatology.
(p. 379). Earlier in this article, we expressed some concern
about clinicians learning to conduct therapy by means of
Increasing Clinician-Researcher Collaboration treatment manuals. As we see it, the danger is that these
As we have noted earlier, there are findings that have manuals may function as more of a straitjacket than a
emerged from basic, process, and outcome research that set of guidelines. Indeed, there is some research evidence
can have important implications for clinical practice. In- that therapists who adhere too closely to the dictates of
deed, therapists continuing to work clinically on the basis the manual may face the risk of being less effective clin-
of how they may have originally been trained, without ically. This has been found in process studies of both
any regard for empirical advances in the field, raises se- psychodynamic therapy (Henry, Strupp, Butler, Schacht,
rious ethical issues. As suggested by Stricker (1992), & Binder, 1993) and cognitive therapy (Castonguay et al.,
1996). In each of these investigations, the problem oc-
although it may not be unethical to practice in the absence of curred when therapists followed the manual at the expense
knowledge, it is unethical to practice in the face of knowledge.
of clinical judgment.
We must all labor with the absence of affirmative data, but there
There clearly is much that goes on in the clinical
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

is no excuse for ignoring contradictory data. An insistence on


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

relying on overlearned, favored, but invalid approaches is not situation that cannot be easily specified in therapy man-
justified. (p. 546) uals. Still, it is possible that the interventions that are
studied in our outcome research, and even the nature of
For practicing therapists to keep abreast of clinically the designs themselves, can be better clinically informed
relevant research findings, they must be presented in a than they are at present. In her presidential address before
form they can readily use. As we suggested earlier, re- the 1994 Association for the Advancement of Behavior
search reports and reviews of research findings are written Therapy, Sobell (in press) addressed this issue of the gap
mainly for other researchers, not clinicians. In doing so, between research and practice. Her premise was one that
these reports have understandably emphasized research we touched on earlier in this article, namely that it is
design and the methodological constraints associated with important for the producers of research findings to meet
various studies, pointed to important research issues, and the needs of the clinical consumers. The impetus for her
focused on possible future research directions. Although concern came from the Ministry of Health in Ontario,
this information is essential to researchers, it says rela- Canada, which was interested in having various agencies
tively little to the practicing clinician. show better accountability for meeting community needs,
As a way of addressing this shortcoming, a new jour- including outpatient treatment.
nal for the practicing clinician has a p p e a r e d - - I n Session: Influenced by the relevance of a needs assessment
Psychotherapy in Practice. Each issue deals with a the- as carried out in the corporate world as well as its practice
matic t o p i c - - f o r example, the therapeutic alliance, panic of having customers participate in the development of the
disorder, and resistance--and contains a review of the product, Sobell (in press) approached the clinicians in
relevant research in a format that is useful to the prac- her community who dealt with addictive behaviors, the
ticing therapist. Thus, instead of presenting study-by- area of research in which she had been involved. Toward
study accounts of research, which emphasize their meth- the goal of making her research findings more applicable
odological strengths and limitations, reviews of both basic to the practicing clinician, she involved them in the actual
and applied research offer jargon-free summaries of what design of the intervention protocol, which ended up as
we currently know about the clinical problem and its having individualized flexibility, depending on the pro-
treatment.
gress made by clients during treatment. In meeting the
For example, the purpose of the third issue of In realistic clinical needs in the community, Sobell hoped
Session was to highlight, against a backdrop of current
that the findings would ultimately be of use to practicing
research findings, the conceptualizations and intervention therapists.
strategies used by the practicing clinician in treating in-
Although the success of this innovative approach to
dividuals suffering from panic disorder with agoraphobia
outcome research is still being evaluated, preliminary as-
(Wolfe, 1995). A second purpose was to explicate what
sessments have found that therapists and clients alike re-
clinicians deem necessary for the treatment of this dis-
sponded quite favorably to the project. Despite the ad-
order in addition to the symptom-focused treatments that
ditional efforts needed to develop a working alliance be-
have been empirically tested. Thus, the issue included
tween researchers and clinician, Sobell (in press) was very
two research reviews, one of the basic research literature
much convinced it was more than worthwhile. As she
and the other of the treatment research literature. In ad-
indicated,
dition, four different approaches to treatment were pre-
sented by clinicians who all began with the mainstream, My only regret is that I did not get here sooner. By adopting a
empirically tested, cognitive-behavioral treatment for the new approach to the dissemination of my research, similar to
management of panic symptoms and agoraphobic avoid- that used in the business community, I have reached more agen-
ance. Once s y m p t o m reduction had been accomplished, cies, more practitioners, and ultimately, more clients than in

1014 October 1996 • American Psychologist


my 25 years in the field. The rewards of effective dissemination Research evidence, however, is but one of the sets of pertinent
are immense for everyone. information that should be considered by the policymaker in
making reimbursement decisions . . . . In addition to rigorous,
Sobell (in press) has not been alone in recognizing scientifically credible evidence, decision makers will wish to
the urgent need for collaboration between researcher and consider clinical evidence, evidence regarding the acceptability
clinician. Wiens, Brazil, Fuller, and Solomon (1995), ac- to clinicians and patients of particular therapeutic approaches,
and evidence regarding social, economic, and ethical implica-
knowledging that the "era of non-research documented
tions of the various°forms of psychotherapy. (p. 724)
practice has ended with the advent of health-care reform"
(p. 47), have urged practicing clinicians to develop net- Parloff (1982) also suggested that a standing com-
works with researchers so that outcome studies can be mittee be empowered to make recommendations regard-
conducted in the naturalistic clinical environment. ing reimbursable therapies on the basis of these often
T h o m a s D. Borkovec (personal communication, August contradictory sets of data. Although he did not spell out
5, 1995) of Pennsylvania State University has recently the composition of this committee, the implications of
spearheaded such a network in the Pennsylvania Psycho- his remarks suggest that the committee should be com-
logical Association. With support from the APA, this net- posed of practitioners, researchers, policymakers, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

work is dedicated to the evaluation of psychotherapy as representatives of the public.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

it is practiced in actual clinical settings. We would also maintain that psychotherapy outcome
research is ready to move onto a fourth generation, one
Conclusion that allows us to generalize our findings more faithfully
to what is needed clinically. Until that happens, however,
Ever since the Boulder Conference on clinical training we need to deal with the question of whether our existing
urged the implementation of the scientist-practitioner psychotherapy outcome findings should be used to de-
model (Raimy, 1950), the field has struggled to foster the termine clinical practice. Our answer to this question is
synergy between therapy research and practice. Given the a qualified "yes, but." Yes, sometimes the emperor does
external pressures facing the field to provide an empirical indeed have new clothes, and it is important for us to
accounting for what we do clinically, the long-standing acknowledge it. Other times, however, he is naked, and
gap between practice and research is in need of serious that should also be acknowledged. Researchers, working
reexamination. with practicing clinicians, need to collaborate in making
Despite the fact that outcome research has advanced this distinction.
in its methodological rigor over the past few decades, the
discrepancy between clinical practice and research con- REFERENCES
tinues to be large. We have discussed some of the reasons
behind this and voiced our concerns that with recent ef- American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
forts to certify EVTs, the methodological constraints as- manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
sociated with "clinical trials" for the treatment of D S M American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
"disorders" will seriously constrain how we conduct psy- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
chotherapy. This indeed is a concern shared by other re- manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
searchers and practitioners in the field (e.g., Benjamin, Arkowitz, H. (1988). The role of theory in psychotherapyintegration.
in press; Davison, 1994; Fensterheim & Raw, in press; Journal of lntegrative and Eclectic Psychotheral~, 8, 8-16.
Barlow, D. H. (1981). On the relation of clinical research to clinical
Garfield, in press; Messer, 1994). Although we strongly practice: Current issues, new directions. Journal of Consulting and
believe that clinical practice should be informed by out- Clinical Psychology, 49, 147-155.
come research, it is quite another thing for it to be dictated Barlow, D. H. (1988). Anxiety and its disorders. New York: Guilford
by such findings. G r o u p designs, in which patients are Press.
randomly assigned to treatment conditions, simply do Benjamin, L. S. (1993). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of per-
sonality disorders. New York:Guilford Press.
not generalize to how we practice clinically. Moreover, Benjamin, L. S. (in press). Personalitydisorders: Models for treatment
we question the current process of selecting EVTs, in that and strategies for treatment development.Journal of Consulting and
the outcome research from which these treatments have Clinical Psychology.
been selected has involved theoretically pure interven- Beutler, L. E., & Clarkin, J. (1990). Differential treatment assignment.
Toward prescriptive psychological treatments. New York: Brunner/
tions. Such findings deafly lack ecological validity, as Mazel.
practicing therapists find their clinical impact more ef- Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levelsof object representation in anaclitic and in-
fective when they do not limit themselves to pure-form trojectivedepression. PsychoanalyticStudy of the Child, 24, 107-157.
treatments. Borkovec,T. D. (1994). Between-grouptherapyoutcomeresearch: Design
and methodology.In L. S. Onken & J. D. Baline(Eds.), N1DA research
Although it is essential that what we do clinically be monograph (No. 137,pp. 249-289). Rockville,MD: NationalInstitute
tied to research findings, it is equally important that our of Drug Abuse.
conclusions about therapeutic effectiveness not be too far Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S. L., Raue, P. J., & Hayes,
removed from clinical reality. Parloff(1982), who dedi- A. M. (1996). Predictingthe effectof cognitivetherapy for depression:
A study of unique and common factors. Journal of Consulting and
cated his entire career to fostering a research database Clinical Psychology, 64, 497-504.
for psychotherapy, made several suggestions that make Chambless, D. L., & Goldstein, A. J. (Eds.). (1988). Agoraphobia. New
sense to us: York: Wiley-lnterscience.

October 1996 • American Psychologist 1015


Cole, J. R., & Cole, S. (1973). Social stratification in science. Chicago: Messer, S. B. (1994). Adapting psychotherapy outcome research to clinical
University of Chicago Press. reality. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 4, 280-282.
Consumer Reports. (1995, November). Mental health: Does therapy help? Nelson-Gray, R. O. ( 1991, November). Treatment matching for unipolar
734-739. depression. In M. E. Addis (Chair), Matching clients to treatments.
Davison, G. C. (1994). On the limits of comparative therapy outcome Clinical and research consideration. Symposium conducted at the
research: A response to Wolfe. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
4, 275-278. New York.
Davison, G. C., & Lazarus, A. A. (1994). Clinical innovation and eval- Noreross, J. C., & Goldfried, M. R. (Eds.). (1992). Handbook of psy-
uation: Integrating practice with inquire. Clinical Psychology: Science chotherapy integration. New York: Basic Books.
and Practice, I, 157-168. Parloff, M. B. (1979). Can psychotherapy research guide the policymaker?
Elkin, I., Parloff, M. B., Hadley, S. W., & Autry, J. H. (1985). NIMH A little knowledge may be a dangerous thing. American Psychologist,
treatment of depression collaborative research program: Background 34, 296-306.
and research plan. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 305-316. Parloff, M. B. (1982). Psychotherapy research evidence and reimburse-
ment decisions: Bambi meets GodziUa. American Journal of Psy-
Fensterheim, H., & Raw, S. D. (in press). Psychotherapy research is not
chiatry, 139, 718-727.
psychotherapy practice. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice.
Parloff, M. B., & Elkin, I. (1992). The NIMH Treatment of Depression
Fishman, S. T. (1981). Narrowing the generalization gap in clinical re- Collaborative Research Program. In D. K. Freedheim (Ed.), History
search. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 243-248. of psychotherapy." A century of change (pp. 442-450). Washington,
Frances, A. (1994). Psychotherapy research and practice: A reply to DC: American Psychological Association.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Wolfe. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 4, 278-279. Persons. J. B. (I 991). Psychotherapy outcome studies do not accurately
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Franks, C. M. (Ed.). (1969). Behavior therapy. Appraisal and status. represent current models of psychotherapy: A proposed remedy.
New York: McGraw-Hill. American Psychologist, 46, 99-106.
Garfield, S. L. (in press). Same problems associated with "validated" Raimy, V. (Ed.). (1950). Training in clinical psychology New York:
forms of psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. Prentice Hall.
Go|dfried, M. R. (1995). Toward a common language for case formu- Raw, S. D. (1993). Does psychotherapy research teach us anything about
lation. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 5, 221-244. psychotherapy? The Behavior Therapist, 16, 75-76.
Goldfried, M. R., Greenberg, L. S., & Marmar, C. (1990). Individual Safran, J. D., Crocker, P., McMain, S., & Murray, P. (1990). The ther-
psychotherapy: Process and outcome. Annual Review of Psychology, apeutic alliance rupture as an event for empirical investigation. Psy-
41, 659-688. chotherapy, 27, 154-165.
Greenberg, L. S., & Pinsof, W. M. (Eds.). (1986). The psychotherapeutic Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The effectiveness of psychotherapy: The Con-
process: A research handbook. New York: Guilford Press. sumer Reports study. American Psychologist, 50, 965-974.
Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating emotional Shear, M. K., & Maser, J. D. (in press). Standardized assessment for
change. New York: Guilford Press. panic disorder research. Archives of General Psychiatry.
Havens, L. (1994). Some suggestions for making research more applicable Silberschatz, G., Fretter, P. B., & Curtis, J. T. (1986). How do interpre-
to clinical practice. In P. E Tally, n . H. Strupp, & S. E Butler (Eds.), tations influence the process of psychotherapy? Journal of Consulting
Psychotherapy research and practice. Bridging the gap (pp. 88-98). and Clinical Psychology, 54, 646-652.
Snyder, W. U. (1950). Clinical methods: Psychotherapy. Annual Review
New York: Basic Books. of Psychology, 1, 221-234.
Hayes, A. M., Castonguay, L. G., & Goldfried, M. R. (1996). The ef- SobeU, L. (in press). Bridging the gap between science and practice: The
fectiveness of targeting the vulnerability factors of depression in cog- challenge before us. Behavior Therapy
nitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 623- Stiles, W. B. (1992). Describing talk: A taxonomy of verbal response
627. modes. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Henry, W. P., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. E, Schacht, T. E., & Binder, J. Stricker, G. (1992). The relationship of research to clinical practice.
L. (1993). Effects of training in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy: American Psychologist, 47, 543-549.
Changes in therapist behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Strupp, H. H. (1973). Foreword. In D. J. Kiesler (Ed.), Theprocess of
Psychology, 61, 434-440. psychotherapy, Chicago: Aldine.
Herink, R. (1980). The psychotherapy handbook: The A to Z guide to Strupp, H. H., & Howard, K. I. (1992). A brief history of psychotherapy
more than 250 different therapies in use today, New York: New Amer- research. In D. K. Freedheim (Ed.), History ofpsychotherapy:A century
ican Library. of change (pp. 309-334). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Hill, C. E. (1986). An overview of the Hill Counselor and Client Verbal Association.
Response Modes Category Systems. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of Psychological Procedures.
Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (1995). Training in and dissemination of empirically-validated psy-
(pp. 131-160). New York: Guilford Press. chological treatment: Report and recommendations. The Clinical
Hilliard, R. B. (1993). Singie-case methodology in psychotherapy process Psychologist, 48, 3-23.
and outcome research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Thyer, B. A., & Himle, J. (1985). Temporal relationships between panic
61, 373-380. attack onset and phobic avoidance in agoraphobia. Behaviour Research
Jacobson, N. S., Schmaling, K. B., Holtzworth-Monroe, A., Klatt, J. L., and Therapy, 23, 607-608.
Wood, L. E, & Follette, V. M. (1989). Research-structured vs. clinically Turner, S. M., Williams, S. L., Beidel, D. C., & Mezzich, J. E. (1986).
Panic disorder and agoraphobia with panic attacks: Covariation along
flexible versions of social learning-based marital therapy. Behaviour
the dimensions of panic and agoraphobic fear. Journal of Abnormal
Research and Therapy, 2 7, 173-180. Psychology, 95, 384-388.
Jones, E. E. (1993). Introduction to special section: Single-case research Wiens, A. N., Brazil, P. J., Fuller, K. H., & Solomon, S. E. (1995). The
in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, practitioner's new weapon: Data. Psychotherapy, 30, 46-53.
371-372. Wolfe, B. E. (1993). Psychotherapy research funding for fiscal years 1986-
Kazdin, A. E. ( 1981). Drawing valid inferences from case studies. Journal 1990. Psychotherapy & Rehabilitation Research Bulletin, 1, 7-9.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 183-192. Wolfe, B. E. (1994). Adapting psychotherapy outcome research to clinical
Kiesler, D. J. (1966). Some myths of psychotherapy research and the reality. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 4, 160-166.
search for a paradigm. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 110-136. Wolfe, B. E. (1995). Introduction: The treatment of panic disorder. In
Kiesler, D. J. (1973). The process ofpsychotherapy: Empirical foundations Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, 1, 1-6.
and systems of analysis. Chicago: Aldine. Wolfe, B. E., & Goldfried, M. R. (1988). Research on psychotherapy
Merton, R. K. (1970). Science, technology, and society in seventeenth- integration: Recommendations and conclusions from an NIMH
century England. New York: Harper & Row. (Original work published workshop. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 448-
1938) 451.

1016 O c t o b e r 1996 • A m e r i c a n P s y c h o l o g i s t

S-ar putea să vă placă și