Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

Second Language Acquisition Theories:

Overview and Evaluation

Christina Gitsaki
This paper presents some of the most influential theories of second language
acquisition. The first part of the paper outlines some general distinctions and
categorizations concerning the different theories as well as criteria for the
evaluation of the various theories. A critical overview follows the description
of each theory and its contribution to second language acquisition research.

INTRODUCTION certain empirical facts (McLaughlin,


Over the past three decades a 1987:9).
number of different theories of second With regard to the content, theories
language acquisition have been formed in are distinguished into ‘macro’ and ‘micro’
an effort to provide explanations as to how theories. Macro theories in second
language learning takes place, to identify language acquisition have a wide scope
the variables responsible for second and cover a broad range of language
language acquisition and to offer guidance learning phenomena. Micro theories deal
to second language teachers. Each theory with specific phenomena and they have a
accounts for language acquisition from a narrow scope (McLaughlin, 1987:9). For
different perspective so some criteria are example, in the field of child second
needed in order to classify and evaluate language acquisition, a macro theory
each theory. would address a wide range of factors
involved in the language learning process,
CLASSIFICATION CRITERIA: while a micro theory would focus on a
Theories of second language specific factor such as how children
acquisition can be classified according to acquire a specific syntactic feature of the
different criteria. According to their form target language.
theories can be classified along a
continuum with ‘deductive’ on one end EVALUATING THEORIES
and ‘inductive’ on the other. Theories McLaughlin (1987) discusses two
following the deductive approach contain of the most basic criteria for evaluating a
concepts and constructs that are assumed theory: its ‘definitional adequacy’ and its
to be true without proof. These are the ‘explanatory power’.
axioms of the theory. Laws of logic are The term ‘definitional adequacy’
applied on these axioms to obtain the refers to the concepts of a theory and their
‘hypotheses’ of the theory. If these correspondence to some external reality.
hypotheses are empirically supported then That is, the concepts of a theory should be
they become the laws and facts of the defined in such a way so that ambiguity
theory (McLaughlin, 1987:8). and confusion are eliminated and different
Unlike the deductive approach, the people can interpret them in the same way
inductive approach does not begin with (McLaughlin, 1987:12). The explanatory
axioms. Instead it is empirically based. power of a theory is measured by the
Theoretical statements are formulated after correspondence of the theory to the facts
a significant amount of empirical that the theory is supposed to explain. In
relationships have been established. order to enhance the definitional adequacy
Theories that follow the inductive of theories, theoretical concepts are treated
approach formulate hypotheses based on as synonymous with the operations that are

89
necessary for their measurement resulting theories of second language acquisition. In
in ‘operational definitions’ (McLaughlin, the late 1970s Krashen developed the
1987:13). For example the operational Monitor Model, an ‘overall’ theory of
definition for the term ‘listening ability’ is second language acquisition, that had
the score that a learner achieves on a test important implications for language
designed to measure his/her listening teaching. Here are the five central
comprehension. hypotheses underlying the Monitor Model:
Furthermore, a theory should also
have explanatory power. It should not only i) The Acquisition versus Learning
describe certain phenomena but also offer Hypothesis. Acquisition is a subconscious
explanations as to ‘why’ a certain process, much like first language
phenomenon occurs. Here it is important acquisition, while learning is a conscious
that theorists do not over-estimate the process resulting into "knowing about
truth-value of their theory (McLaughlin, language" (Krashen, 1982:10). Learning
1987:14). does not "turn into" acquisition and it
Finally, a theory is validated by usually takes place in formal
what it suggests and predicts as well as by environments, while acquisition can take
what it affirms explicitly. In assessing the place without learning in informal
validity and usefulness of a theory one environments (Krashen, 1976, 1982).
should consider the theory's
correspondence to the facts and internal ii) The Monitor Hypothesis. Learning has
coherence as well as the predictions that the function of monitoring and editing the
the theory makes - researchers are always utterances produced through the
interested in and look for theories that can acquisition process (Krashen, 1982:15).
generate hypotheses (i.e. predictions) The use of the Monitor is affected by the
(McLaughlin, 1987:17). In the next amount of time that the second language
section of this paper, a number of learner has at his/her disposal to think
influential theories in second language about the utterance he/she is about to
acquisition are outlined. produce, the focus on form, and his/her
knowledge of second language rules
THEORIES OF SECOND (Krashen, 1981:3-4).
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION
Second language acquisition iii) The Natural Order Hypothesis. There
theories were developed along the lines of is a natural order of acquisition of second
first language acquisition theories. Over language rules. Some of them are early-
the past three decades, studies in acquired and some are late-acquired. This
linguistics have focused on second order does not necessarily depend on
language acquisition investigating how a simplicity of form while it could be
second language is acquired, describing influenced by classroom instruction
different stages of development and (Krashen, 1985). Evidence for the Natural
assessing whether second language Order Hypothesis was provided by a series
acquisition follows a similar route to that of research studies investigating
of first language acquisition. A number of morpheme acquisition orders.
theories of second language acquisition
were formulated, either deductively or iv) The Input Hypothesis. According to
inductively, and research in the second Krashen, receiving comprehensible input
language classroom flourished. is the only way that can lead to the
The Monitor Model acquisition of a second language. If a
Stephen Krashen's model is one of learner’s level in a second language is i,
the most influential and well-known he/she can move to an i+1 level only by

90
being exposed to comprehensible input are not based on well-established theories
containing i+1 (Krashen, 1985). and research (e.g. the Natural Order
hypothesis). Furthermore, the role
v) The Affective Filter Hypothesis. assigned to unconscious learning was
Comprehensible input will not be fully found to be overestimated and
utilized by the learners if there is a ‘mental exaggerated. Instead subsequent studies
block’, i.e. the ‘affective filter’, that acts as drew attention to the role of consciousness
a barrier to the acquisition process in second language learning and how
(Krashen, 1985). much learners notice and what they think
as they learn second languages.
Krashen's Monitor Theory is an Despite the various criticisms,
example of a macro theory attempting to Krashen's Monitor Theory of second
cover most of the factors involved in language acquisition had a great impact on
second language acquisition: age, the way second language learning was
personality traits, classroom instruction, viewed, and initiated research towards the
innate mechanisms of language discovery of orders of acquisition.
acquisition, environmental influences,
input, etc., but not without limitations. Interlanguage Theories
Despite its popularity, the Monitor Theory The term interlanguage was first
was criticized by theorists and researchers used by Selinker (1969) to describe the
mainly on the grounds of its definitional linguistic stage second language learners
adequacy. Gregg (1984) rejects the most go through during the process of mastering
fundamental of Krashen’s Hypotheses, the the target language. Since then,
acquisition-learning dichotomy. Following ‘interlanguage’ has become a major strand
a string of arguments, Gregg concludes of second language acquisition research
that under normal conditions the Monitor and theory. This section outlines the three
cannot be used and since it is the only way main approaches to the description of
in which learning can be utilized, there is interlanguage systems.
no need to talk about two different ways of According to Selinker (1972)
gaining competence in a second language. interlanguage is a temporary grammar
Criticism was also expressed by which is systematic and composed of
McLaughlin (1987). McLaughlin rules. These rules are the product of five
acknowledges Krashen's attempt to main cognitive processes:
develop an extensive and detailed theory
of second language acquisition but finds it i) Overgeneralisation. Some of the rules
inadequate in that some of its central of the interlanguage system may be the
assumptions and hypotheses are not result of the overgeneralisation of specific
clearly defined and thus are not readily rules and features of the target language.
testable (e.g. the acquisition-learning
dichotomy is based on “subconscious” and ii) Transfer of Training. Some of the
“conscious” processes respectively, which components of the interlanguage system
have not been clearly defined by Krashen may result from transfer of specific
although he operationalized them in his elements via which the learner is taught
studies (see Krashen, Butler, Birnbaum, & the second language.
Robertson (1978) for an investigation of iii) Strategies of Second Language
grammaticality judgments based on "feel" Learning. Some of the rules in the
and "rule" for subconscious and conscious learner's interlanguage may result from the
acquisition respectively), while other application of language learning strategies
assumptions aiming to enhance the “as a tendency on the part of the learners
explanatory power of the Monitor Theory

91
to reduce the TL [target language] to a systematic variability of interlanguage
simpler system” (Selinker, 1972:219). systems is reflected to the variable effect
which the different tasks and different
iv) Strategies of Second Language linguistic contexts have on the learners’
Communication. Interlanguage system use of syntactic, phonological and
rules may also be the result of strategies morphological structures (Tarone, 1982).
employed by the learners in their attempt Even though Tarone does not deny that
to communicate with native speakers of other theories can provide explanations of
the target language. second language acquisition, she argues
that “any adequate model of SLA [second
v) Language Transfer. Some of the rules language acquisition] must take IL
in the interlanguage system may be the [interlanguage] variation into account”
result of transfer from the learner’s first (Tarone, 1990:398).
language. Different approaches were
employed for explaining the acquisition of
Selinker's description of the interlanguage and how learners discover
interlanguage system has a cognitive and organize form-function relationships
emphasis and a focus on the strategies that in a second language. Ellis (1985) argues
learners employ when learning a second that learners begin with forms which are
language. A different approach to the used in free variation during the early
theory of interlanguage was adopted by stages of second language acquisition
Adjemian (1976) in his attempt to describe (non-systematic variability) until more
the nature of the interlanguage systems. organizing and restructuring has taken
Adjemian argues that interlanguages are place (systematic variability). In contrast
natural languages but they are unique in to Ellis’s claims, the functional approach
that their grammar is permeable to the analysis of interlanguage argues that
(Adjemian, 1976). He also differentiates discourse functions develop before
between the learning strategies that grammatical functions and evidence is
learners employ and the linguistic rules provided of the acquisition of function
that are “crucially concerned in the actual occurring without the acquisition of form
form of the language system” (Adjemian, (Pfaff, 1987).
1976:302). Adjemian (1976) concludes The role of the mother tongue (L1)
that the description of these linguistic rules in the acquisition of the target language
that will reveal the properties of the (L2) was re-examined under the scope of
learner’s grammar should be the primary the interlanguage theory and predictions
goal of linguistic research. were made about when the influence of L1
The third approach to the is greatest. Zobl (1980a, 1980b)
description of interlanguage was initiated investigated the L1 influence on L2
by Tarone (1979, 1982). She describes acquisition and argued that it is “the
interlanguage as a continuum of speech formal features of L2 that control the
styles. Learners shift between styles formal aspects of its acquisition, including
according to the amount of attention they the activation of L1 transfer” (Zobl,
pay to language form- from the 1980a:54, 1980b).
superordinate style in which attention is The approaches to the study of
mainly focused on language form to the interlanguage, as described above, agree
vernacular style in which the least on two basic characteristics of
attention is paid to language form. The interlanguage systems: interlanguages are
new target language forms first appear in systematic (systematicity either in the form
the more careful style and progressively of learning strategies the learners employ
move towards the vernacular style. The or linguistic rules that govern the learners'

92
grammars), and dynamic (interlanguages foreign language competence (Bley-
keep changing until the target language Vroman, Felix, & Ioup, 1988).
system is fully acquired). The scope of A model very similar to Chomsky's
these approaches is also common: Universal Grammar was proposed by Felix
interlanguage is seen as a kind of interim (1985). The ‘Competition Model’ consists
grammar gradually progressing towards of two subsystems: the Language-Specific
the target language grammar. Morpheme Cognitive System (LSC-system) and the
studies were employed to describe the Problem-Solving system (PSC-system)
systematicity of interlanguage systems and and it is responsible for the differences in
also the various stages of interlanguage the learning processes employed by
development until the target form is children and adults. It is argued that the
acquired. The interlanguage theories were children’s learning process is guided by
inductively derived from studies following the LSC-system, while adults employ the
Error Analysis, the view that by analyzing problem solving module which then enters
learners’ errors we can predict the into competition with the language-
linguistic stage that a learner is at. specific system. Even though the LSC-
However, Error Analysis as a mode of system is governed by principles similar to
inquiry was limited in its scope and the principles of the Universal Grammar,
concentrated on what learners did wrong the principles of the PSC-system are
rather than on what made them successful largely unknown (Felix, 1985:70).
(Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1992:61). In Another UG based theory, the
that respect, interlanguage theories are Creative Construction theory, was
limited in their explanatory power. suggested by Dulay and Burt (1974).
According to this theory children engaged
Universal Grammar Theories in second language learning progressively
Universal Grammar (UG) theories reconstruct rules for the target language
are based on Chomsky’s claim that there speech they hear guided by ‘universal
are certain principles that form the basis innate mechanisms’ which lead them to
on which knowledge of language construct certain types of hypotheses about
develops. These principles are biologically the system of the language they are
determined and specialized for language acquiring until the mismatch between
learning (Chomsky, 1969, 1980, 1986). what they are exposed to and what they
Originally, UG theory did not concern actually produce is resolved (Dulay &
itself with second language learning. It Burt, 1974:37). Empirical evidence from
referred to the first language learner. Its comparing the errors produced by Spanish
principles though were adopted by second children learning English with those
language researchers and were applied in produced by children learning English as
the field of second language acquisition. their mother-tongue, showed that most of
UG was used in order to provide the syntax errors in English produced by
explanations for the existence of the Spanish children were of the same type
developmental sequences in interlanguage of errors made by children learning
and to support the view of interlanguage as English natively (Dulay & Burt, 1973).
a natural language which is subject to the Also, finding Spanish and Chinese
constraints of the Universal Grammar children acquiring English morphemes in
(Hilles, 1986:45). The use of UG for similar orders, Dulay and Burt conclude
language transfer, fossilization and L2 that it is the L2 system rather than the L1
pedagogy was also suggested. Evidence system that guides the acquisition process
was provided that adults have some sort of (Dulay & Burt, 1974:52).
access to knowledge of UG, and this The effect of the mother-tongue in
knowledge is used in the development of determining the magnitude of the second

93
language learning task is reflected in the 1987:134). These sub-skills become
model of the learning process that Corder automatic with practice (Posner & Snyder,
(1978) suggested. According to this model 1975). During this process of
the learner begins his/her learning task automatisation, the learner organizes and
from a basic Universal Grammar (or built- restructures new information that is
in syllabus) which gradually becomes acquired. Through this process of
more complex in response to the learner’s restructuring the learner links new
exposure to target language data and the information to old information and
communicative needs he/she is faced with. achieves increasing degrees of mastery in
This elaboration or complexification the second language (McLaughlin, 1987,
process follows a constant sequence for all 1990a). This gradual mastering may
learners of a particular second language, follow a U-shaped curve sometimes
but the progress of any particular learner is (Lightbown, Spada, & Wallace, 1980)
affected by the degree to which his/her indicating a decline in performance as
knowledge of the target language in the “more complex internal representations
form of mother-tongue-like features replace less complex ones” followed by an
facilitates his/her learning process. increase again as skill becomes expertise
In summary, Universal Grammar (McLaughlin, 1990b).
theories of second language acquisition From the cognitivist’s point of
were generated in order to provide view language acquisition is dependent “in
explanations for empirical evidence and both content and developmental
they were primarily concerned with the sequencing on prior cognitive abilities”
internal mechanisms that lead to the and language is viewed as a function of
acquisition of the formal aspects of the “more general nonlinguistic abilities”
target language and the similarities and (Berman, 1987:4).
differences between acquiring a particular Evidence against the cognitivist
language as a first or a second language. theory is provided by Felix (1981) who
Although researchers have used UG to describes the general cognitive skills as
generate a number of interesting “useless” for language development
hypotheses about second language (Felix, 1981). The only areas that
acquisition, and generative theorists regard cognitive development is related to
UG as the best theory of grammar because language development is vocabulary and
of its descriptive and explanatory meaning, since lexical items and meaning
adequacy (Ellis, 1994:429), empirical relations are most readily related to a
evidence has been restricted to the conceptual base (Felix, 1981).
acquisition of a small set of syntactic A base in cognitive theory is also
phenomena. A general theory of second claimed by the interactivist approach to
language acquisition needs to cover a second language learning (Clahsen, 1987).
wider range of phenomena (McLaughlin, The language processing model proposed
1987:108). by the interactivist approach “assumes an
Cognitive Theories autonomous linguistic level of processing”
Psychologists and psycholinguists and contains a general problem solver
viewed second language learning as the mechanism (GPS) that allows “direct
acquisition of a complex cognitive skill. mappings between underlying structure
Some of the sub-skills involved in the and surface forms, thus short-circuiting the
language learning process are applying grammatical processor” (Clahsen,
grammatical rules, choosing the 1987:105).
appropriate vocabulary, following the The language acquisition theories
pragmatic conventions governing the use based on a cognitive view of language
of a specific language (McLaughlin, development regard language acquisition

94
as the gradual automitization of skills learning the target language well (see also
through stages of restructuring and linking Clahsen, Meisel & Pienemann, 1983).
new information to old knowledge. The Multidimensional Model has
However, the differences between the both explanatory and predictive power in
various cognitive models makes it that it not only identifies stages of
impossible to construct a comprehensive linguistic development but it also explains
cognitive theory of second language why learners go through these
acquisition and furthermore, as Schimdt developmental stages and it predicts when
(1992) observes: other grammatical structures will be
acquired (Ellis, 1994:384). Although the
“there is little theoretical Multidimensional Model has made
support from psychology important contributions to second
on the common belief that language acquisition research, there are
the development of fluency some problems with the “falsifiability” of
in a second language is its predictive framework, such as
almost exclusively a matter explaining how it is that learners learn
of the increasingly skillful whatever they manage to produce despite
application of rules” the processing constraints (see also
(Schmidt, 1992:377). Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991:285;
McLaughlin, 1987:114-115). Furthermore,
The last two theories dealt with in the Multidimensional Model does not
this paper, the Multidimensional Model explain the process through which learners
and the Acculturation/Pidginization obtain intake from imput and how they use
Theory, refer mainly to the acquisition of a this intake to reconstruct internal
second language by adults in naturalistic grammars (Ellis, 1994:388). In this respect
environments. the Multidimensional Model is limited.

Multidimensional Model Acculturation/Pidginization Theory


In the Multidimensional Model, the According to Schumann (1978):
learner's stage of acquisition of the target
language is determined by two “second language
dimensions: the learner’s developmental acquisition is just one
stage and the learner’s social- aspect of acculturation and
psychological orientation. The the degree to which a
developmental stage is defined by learner acculturates to the
accuracy orders and developmental target-language group will
sequences, but within a stage learners may control the degree to which
differ because of their social-psychological he acquires the second
orientation, which is independent of language.” Schumann
developmental stage. Thus a (1978).
‘segregatively’ oriented learner uses more
restrictive simplification strategies than an From this perspective, second
‘integratively’ oriented learner who uses language acquisition is greatly affected by
elaborate simplification strategies. The the degree of social and psychological
segregative learner is more likely to distance between the learner and the
fossilize at that stage than is the target-language culture. Social distance
integrative learner who has a more refers to the learner as a member of a
positive attitude towards learning the social group that is in contact with another
target language and a better chance of social group whose members speak a
different language. Psychological distance

95
results from a number of different second language acquisition. Each theory
affective factors that concern the learner as offers a different insight in the complex
an individual, such as language shock, process of second language acquisition.
culture shock, culture stress, etc. If the For example, during the era of
social and/or psychological distance is developmental studies, Larsen-Freeman
great then acculturation is impeded and the (1978), in an effort to provide an
learner does not progress beyond the early explanation for the morpheme acquisition
stages of language acquisition. As a result order in second language learning,
his/her target language will stay concludes that the morpheme frequency of
pidginized. Pidginization is characterized occurrence in native speaker speech is the
by simplifications and reductions principle determinant for the morpheme
occurring in the learner’s interlanguage order in the speech production of second
which lead to fossilization when the language learners. However this
learner’s interlanguage system does not conclusion seen under the light of different
progress in the direction of the target theories of second language acquisition
language (for a review see McLaughlin, can provide a number of different
1987:110-112). explanations. From the cognitivist's point
Schumann’s theory received of view this finding is evidence that the
limited empirical support. Among some of learner, in the process of testing his/her
the criticisms that the acculturation theory hypotheses about the target language
received was that social factors are system, has managed due to the frequency
assumed to have a direct impact on second of occurrence of a particular L2
language acquisition while they are more construction to refine his/her hypothesis
likely to have an indirect one (Ellis, about a specific L2 rule. Another
1994:233). Also, pidginization is a group explanation based on the affective factors
phenomenon, while language acquisition influencing second language acquisition
is an individual phenomenon. Finally, the could suggest that the learners in their
acculturation model fails to explain how effort to match the gestalt of the native
the social factors influence the quality of speaker input to which they are exposed,
contact the learners experience (Ellis, acquire and produce the appropriate
1994:234). morphemes in their speech (Larsen-
Freeman, 1978). Larsen-Freeman (1978)
SUMMARY concludes that there is not a single
The second language acquisition explanation that could work for all
theories reviewed in this paper have paid learners, and that different learners may
attention to different aspects of the second rely on different strategies when learning a
language acquisition process and have second language, depending on a number
provided valuable background and of different variables such as the target
hypotheses for numerous research studies. language input they are exposed to, their
All of the theories regard second language cognitive style, their motivation, their
acquisition as a gradual process. Whether proficiency in the target language, etc.
language learners use strategies, cognitive The large number of second
or innate mechanisms, they still have to language acquisition theories shows the
progress towards the target language going great interest that the study of second
through various stages of development. language acquisition has produced over
Although theories are primarily the past three decades. Despite their
concerned with providing explanations controversies, the theories of second
about how languages are acquired, no language acquisition managed to initiate
single theory can offer a comprehensive various research questions and to shed
explanation about the whole process of light on a number of linguistic and

96
cognitive processes that are part of this learning task. Studies in Second
large jigsaw puzzle called ‘second Language Acquisition, 2, 27-36.
language acquisition’. Dulay, H. & Burt, M. (1973). Should we
teach children syntax? Language
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Learning, 23, 245-258.
I wish to thank Dr. W. Acton and Dulay, H. & Burt, M. (1974). Natural
Mr. R. Taylor for reading this article in sequences in child second language
draft and for suggesting improvements. acquisition. Language Learning,
However, I remain solely responsible for 24, 37-53.
the contents of this paper. Ellis, R. (1985). Sources of variability in
interlanguage. Applied Linguistics,
REFERENCES 6, 118-131.
Adjemian, C. (1976). On the nature of Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second
interlanguage systems. Language language acquisition. Oxford:
Learning, 26, 297-320. Oxford University Press.
Berman, R. (1987). Cognitive principles Felix, S. (1981). On the (in)applicability of
and language acquisition. In C. Piagetian thought to language
Pfaff (Ed.), First and second learning. Studies in Second
language acquisition processes, Language Acquisition, 3(2), 201-
pp. 3-27. Cambridge, Mass.: 220
Newbury House. Felix, S. (1985). More evidence on
Bley-Vroman, R., Felix, S. & Ioup, G. competing cognitive systems.
(1988). The accessibility of Second Language Research, 1(1),
universal grammar in adult 47-72.
language learning. Second Gregg, K.R. (1984). Krashen’s Monitor
Language Research, 4(1), 1-32. and Occam’s razor. Applied
Chomsky, N. (1969). Linguistics and Linguistics, 5, 79-100.
philosophy. In S. Hook, (Ed.), Hilles, S. 91986). Interlanguage and the
Language and philosophy. New pro-drop parameter. Second
York: New York University Press. Language Research, 2(1), 33-52.
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and Krashen, S. (1976). Formal and informal
representations. New York: linguistic environments in
Columbia University Press. language acquisition and language
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of learning. TESOL Quarterly, 10,
language: Its nature, origin and 157-168.
use. New York: Praeger. Krashen, S. (1981). Second language
Clahsen, H. (1987). Connecting theories of acquisition and second language
language processing and (second) learning. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
language acquisition. In C. Pfaff Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and
(Ed.), First and second language practices of second language
acquisition processes, pp. 103-116. acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon
Cambridge, Mass.: Newbury Press.
House. Krashen, S. (1985). The input hypothesis:
Clahsen, H., Meisel, J. & Pienemann, M. Issues and implications. London:
(1983). Deutsch als Zweitsprache: Longman.
der Spracherwerb auslandischer Krashen, S., Butler, J., Birnbaum, R. &
Arbeiter. Tubingen: Gunter Narr. Robertson, J. (1978). Two studies
Corder, S. (1978). Language distance and in language acquisition and
the magnitude of the language language learning. ITL: Review of
Applied Linguistics, 39-40, 73-92.

97
Larsen-Freeman, D. (1978). An ESL index learning: issues and approaches.
of development. TESOL Quarterly, Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
12(4), 439-448. Selinker, L. (1969). Language transfer.
Larsen-Freeman, D. & Long, M.H. (1991). General Linguistics, 9, 67-92.
An introduction to second Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. IRAL,
language acquisition research. 10, 209-231.
London: Longman. Tarone, E. (1979). Interlanguage as
Lightbown, P., Spada, N. & Wallace, R. chameleon. Language Learning,
(1980). Some effects of instruction 29, 181-191.
on child and adolescent ESL Tarone, E. (1982). Systematicity and
learners. In R. Scarcella & S. attention in interlanguage.
Krashen (Eds.), Research in Language Learning, 32, 69-84.
second language acquisition, pp. Tarone, (1990). On variation in
162-172. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury interlanguage: A response to
House. Gregg. Applied Linguistics, 11,
McLaughlin, B. (1987). Theories of 392-400.
second language learning. Zobl, H. (1980a). The formal and
London: Edward Arnold. developmental selectivity of L1
McLaughlin, B. (1990a). Restructuring. influence on L2 acquisition.
Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 113- Language Learning, 30(1), 43-57.
128. Zobl, H. (1980b). Developmental and
McLaughlin, B. (1990b). The relationship transfer errors: Their common
between first and second bases and (possibly) differential
languages: language proficiency effects on subsequent learning.
and language aptitude. In B. TESOL Quarterly, 14(4), 469-479
Harley, P. Allen, J. Cummins & M.
Swain (Eds.) The development of
second language proficiency.
Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Pfaff, C. (1987). First and second
language acquisition processes.
Cambridge, Mass.: Newbury
House.
Posner, M.I. & Snyder, C.R.R. (1975).
Attention and cognitive control. In
R.L. Solso, (Ed.), Information
processing and cognition: The
Loyola symposium. Hillsdale. N.J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schmidt, R. (1992). Psychological
mechanisms underlying second
language fluency. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 14,
357-385.
Schumann, J. (1978). Social and
psychological factors in second
language acquisition. In J.
Richards (Ed.), Understanding
second and foreign language

98
99

S-ar putea să vă placă și