Sunteți pe pagina 1din 14

Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Promoting high-leverage writing instruction through an early


childhood classroom daily routine (WPI): A professional development
model of early writing skills
Chenyi Zhang ∗ , Gary E. Bingham
Georgia State University, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study presents an initial investigation of a professional development intervention model for pro-
Received 11 April 2017 moting preschool teachers’ high-leverage writing instruction through a modification of the “Morning
Received in revised form 13 May 2019 Meeting Time” (MMT) classroom routine. Using a quasi-experimental design, 14 teachers and 112 chil-
Accepted 2 June 2019
dren were assigned to intervention and comparison conditions. Intervention group teachers received a
Available online 3 July 2019
four-hour workshop and one in-classroom coaching session focused on modifying existing MMT routine
activities to implement interactive writing instruction. Results indicate that intervention group teach-
Keywords:
ers achieved high intervention implementation fidelity and significantly improved the quality of their
Early writing
Early literacy
writing instruction (Cohen’s d ranges from 1.22 to 2.36). Further, despite a relatively brief intervention
Writing instruction phrase (three months), intervention group children showed greater gains in name writing, letter writing,
Professional development and letter naming skills than comparison group children (Cohen’s d ranges from 1.28 to 1.59). Findings
Early childhood provide initial evidence for the merit of utilizing typically occurring classroom routines, such as Morning
Meeting, for embedding explicit teaching and writing opportunities for young children.
© 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction development. Further, despite the importance of writing oppor-


tunities in early childhood settings to children’s writing attempts
Children’s writing development (e.g., name writing, letter and learning of literacy skills (Gerde, Bingham, & Pendergast, 2015;
writing, and spelling) before they enter formal schooling, is con- Guo, Justice, Kaderavek, & McGinty, 2012), recent research docu-
currently and longitudinally associated with pre-reading skills ments that preschool teachers’ writing instruction occurs relatively
(Kendeou, van den Broek, White, & Lynch, 2009; National Early infrequently and is, generally, of low quality (Bingham, Quinn,
Literacy Panel, 2008), such as phonological awareness and letter & Gerde, 2017). Although writing instruction is often included
knowledge (Diamond, Gerde, & Powell, 2008; Dickinson, McCabe, as a component of targeted literacy instruction in early literacy
Anastasopoulos, Peisner-Feinberg, & Poe, 2003), and predictive of professional development (PD) interventions that employ com-
both later reading and writing achievement (Aram & Biron, 2004; prehensive language and literacy PD approaches (e.g., Landry,
Hammill, 2004; National Early Literacy Panel, 2008; Shatil, Share, & Anthony, Swank, & Monseque-Bailey, 2009; Wasik & Hindman,
Levin, 2000). The reciprocal relationship between children’s early 2011; Powell & Diamond, 2011), it is difficult to know how specific
writing and reading processes (e.g., Bloodgood, 1999; Clay, 2001; instructional practices impact children’s writing development. One
Diamond et al., 2008; Morris, Bloodgood, Lomax, & Perney, 2003) recent approach to professional learning suggests the importance
highlights the importance of children’s early writing experiences, of focusing on high-leverage teaching practices (HLTP)—a core set
which serve as a context for using and developing a range of literacy of practices that have the greatest impact on children’s learning of
skills in meaningful ways (Diamond et al., 2008). specific early skills (Ball, Sleep, Boerst, & Bass, 2009; Hlas & Hlas,
Although emerging evidence highlights the importance of chil- 2012). An HLTP PD approach highlights certain core instructional
dren’s writing skills to later achievement, less is known about practices within a very specific purpose (e.g., interactive, shared
how children’s exposure to writing instruction supports writing book reading to promote children’s language development). HLTP
PD designs should focus on accommodating teachers’ background
knowledge and experiences in a manner that facilititates that newly
∗ Corresponding author. introduced instructional practices fit within existing classroom
E-mail address: czhang15@gsu.edu (C. Zhang). routines and are relatively easy for teachers to implement. Such

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.06.003
0885-2006/© 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 139

an approach increases the likelihood that teachers will enact prac- support. Taken together, the findings of these studies suggest that
tices with high fidelity (Ball et al., 2009). This study presents an children can learn important writing and pre-reading skills through
exploration of a PD intervention model developed within an HLTP interactions with adults who draw attention to the process of
approach to promote the quality of teachers’ writing instruction writing and support children in their emergent writing abilities.
and, subsequently, children’s writing skills through the modifica- However, given research documenting that teachers infrequently
tion of a large group Morning Meeting Time (MMT) routine. Given model or scaffold writing in preschool classrooms, it is important to
consistent associations between children’s writing and pre-reading develop PD intervention models for promoting the frequency and
skills (i.e., letter knowledge, print knowledge and phonological quality of teachers’ writing instruction.
awareness), we also explored whether the intervention may have
an impact on children’s pre-reading skills. 1.2. Developing a PD model within a high-leverage teaching
practice framework
1.1. Promoting the quality of early writing instruction
A fundamental challenge in early childhood professional devel-
Existing research on early childhood classrooms in the U.S. doc- opment is improving the quality of teachers’ instructional practices
uments that children have access to a variety of writing materials, in ways that positively impact children’s development. This chal-
which often include a classroom writing center (Gerde et al., 2015). lenge is illustrated by findings from early reading first programs
The availability of these items align with existing professional rec- as well as other language and literacy PD interventions that
ommendations and teaching standards that encourage, or even have employed comprehensive approaches to increasing classroom
mandate, preschool classrooms to provide writing materials (e.g., quality (Dickinson & Caswell, 2007; Landry et al., 2009; Powell &
Head Start performance standards, U.S. Department of Health and Diamond, 2011). Comprehensive models often include intensive
Human Services, 2008) and to create a learning center where chil- workshops and/or coaching for teachers aimed at implementing
dren can practice writing (Copple & Bredekamp, 2009). Researchers a full range of evidence-based literacy instruction of multiple, if
and professionals agree that providing and displaying attractive not all, early literacy domains (e.g., oral language, pre-reading, and
writing materials in classrooms are necessary (Neuman, Copple, & emergent writing). In many of these approaches, teachers imple-
Bredekamp, 2000) for engaging children in writing activities (Rowe ment various literacy and language instructional practices (e.g.,
& Neitzel, 2010) and encouraging children’s self-initiated writing book reading, phonological awareness instruction) across differ-
attempts (O’Leary, Cockburn, Powell, & Diamond, 2010). ent activity contexts either sequentially or simultaneously (e.g.,
Despite the prevalence of materials in classrooms, teachers Dickinson & Caswell, 2007; Jackson et al., 2007; Landry et al.,
relatively infrequently engage in activities that actively support 2009; Powell & Diamond, 2011). Although valuable, comprehen-
children’s writing (Gerde et al., 2015; Zhang, Diamond, & Powell, sive approaches are considerably time-consuming and expensive
2015). Gerde et al.’s observational study demonstrates that very for delivery (e.g., Dickinson, Freiberg, & Barnes, 2011) and do not
few teachers model or scaffold writing regularly as part of their necessarily improve children’s language and literacy skills signif-
daily routine, classroom teaching, or during children’s play. In addi- icantly (Diamond, Justice, Siegler, & Snyder, 2013; Jackson et al.,
tion, when teachers were observed modeling or scaffolding writing, 2007; Lonigan, Farver, Phillips, & Clancy-Menchetti, 2011).
they often did so in ways that were implicit (i.e., they failed to Difficulty with producing consistent teacher and child out-
draw attention to their actions) or of relatively low quality (i.e., they comes likely relates to multiple factors, including the fact that
rarely scaffolded children in ways that produced an understanding some approaches ask teachers to implement too many prac-
of writing processes). Few teachers prompted children to identify tices (e.g., book reading, phonological awareness instruction, and
the name and sound of letters when writing letters or spelling writing) or have limited focus on key practices that are easily
words, which is an important instructional practice for building implemented by early childhood educators who often have little
children’s understanding of how letters work in written language. time to plan instruction (Dickinson et al., 2011). Further, because
Social-cultural theory suggests that adult support and guidance preschool teachers are often encouraged to provide support of all
are indispensable to children’s writing development (Vygotsky, domains of children’s development with the least instructional
1978). The importance of adult guidance is demonstrated in stud- time (Bustamante, Hindman, Champagne, & Wasik, 2018), time
ies documenting that children write more letters and spell longer and staffing constraints, are considerable barriers to PD as teachers
words with parents (DeBaryshe, Buell, & Binder, 1996) or teach- receive very little paid time for planning or professional develop-
ers’ help (Rieben, Ntamakiliro, Gonthier, & Fayol, 2005) than when ment (Powell, 2013). These factors are one reason that teachers
they write independently. Several home-based intervention stud- may vary significantly in their fidelity of intervention imple-
ies (Levin & Aram, 2010), for example, demonstrate the importance mentation across strategies that they are trained to implement
of parents’ clarification and explanation about writing conventions (e.g., Bingham, Culatta, & Hall-Kenyon, 2016; Powell, Diamond,
(e.g., letter formation, word, and letter spacing) to children’s devel- Burchinal, & Koehler, 2010). Research documents that teachers
opment of handwriting and spelling skills. may need considerable time to practice and effectively implement
Given the consistent associations between writing and pre- newly introduced literacy instruction in ways that promote chil-
reading skills (e.g., Molfese, Beswick, Molnar, & Jacobi-Vessels, dren’s literacy and language skills (Landry, Anthony, Swank, &
2006; Diamond et al., 2008), teachers’ effective writing instruc- Assel, 2011).
tion likely leads to children’s development of pre-reading skills in A recent approach emphasized for impactful professional devel-
addition to writing skills. In one of very few preschool writing inter- opment projects is to focus on high-leverage teaching practices
vention studies, Aram and Biron (2004) document that children (HLTP; Ball et al., 2009; Grossman, Hammerness, & McDonald,
who receive explicit small group writing support and instruction 2009). Generally speaking, HLTPs are conceptualized as classroom
evidenced significantly higher word writing and decoding skills practices and teaching pedagogies that, based on existing empir-
(i.e., phonological awareness, letter knowledge, and orthographic ical studies, produce the greatest and most consistent impacts on
awareness skills) than children receiving small group joint book children’s learning and development when implemented properly
reading instruction or business as usual instruction. The small and frequently. These practices are fundamental to teacher’s class-
group writing instruction focused on supporting children to write room instruction and beneficial to children’s learning both distally
their names, the names of friends, and familiar words while the and proximally, yet unlikely to be learned through teacher’s own
teacher provided letter-name and letter-sound correspondence experience (Grossman et al., 2009). HLTPs should also address and
140 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

Table 1
The list or high-leverage writing instruction.

High-leverage writing instruction Targeted teaching practices in intervention Research evidence

• Interactive writing practices Through frequent writing attempts in a meaningful


Writing practices • Repeated writing practices context, children can develop awareness of being a writer
• Included both teacher-led (writing along with teachers’ (Clay, 1975; Rowe, 2008), understand the function of
writing) and child-led writing practices (children’s writing as a communication tool, and to practice writing
independent writing attempts) actions (Dennis & Votteler, 2013).

• Explanation of writing tools, e.g., “I am going to use my Children need frequent exposure to environmental print,
Explanation of writing marker to write January on my whiteboard.” its function, and familiar words in order to develop
processes • Explanation of writing as a form of communication, e.g., understanding of print and how it works (Pollo, Kessler, &
“Good morning class! Let’s write hello on our whiteboard Treiman, 2009).
and show it to our friends to welcome them to school.”
• Explanation of writing tasks, e.g., “Friends, yesterday we Teachers need to utilize environmental print in teaching
talked about trees. Let’s write the word tree together on and frequently model writing to demonstrate the
our whiteboard.” formation of letters, how letters represent sounds and can
• Modeling writing with explanation of print knowledge, be used for spelling words, and learn writing conventions
and decoding skills, e.g., “What’s the weather today? (Dennis & Votteler, 2013).
Sunny. Good, I am going to write down Sunny here from
left to right with the first letter capitalized”

Scaffolding with explicit • Supporting children’s writing attempts with explicit Writing is cognitively challenging task that requires
decoding skills instruction focused on letter knowledge and/or phonemic children to coordinate different literacy skills, such as
awareness (letter names of letter sound), e.g., “A letter to letter knowledge and phonological awareness (Molfese
your mom? Let’s write together. What is the first letter we et al., 2011), in addition to necessary motor skills (Gerde,
need to write? What letter makes the /m/ sound? Yes. How Bingham et al., 2012) when completing a writing task (e.g.,
about /a/ sound? Great! What is the last letter you hear? name writing, letter writing, and spelling).
Studies show that teachers’ explicit instruction
significantly impacts children’s learning of decoding skills,
which in turn may influence children’s early writing
development (Puranik, Lonigan, & Kim, 2011). As such,
teachers need to scaffold children’s writing attempts by
building children’s print knowledge, letter knowledge, and
phonemic awareness skills.

incorporate teachers’ level of experience, expertise, and profes- also enables teachers to evaluate and further support children’s
sional development learning needs in a manner that encourages writing development. Teachers’ modeling and demonstration of
teacher participation and reciprocity. Attention to teachers’ back- the writing process is considered an important strategy for help-
grounds (e.g., educational and work experiences) and classroom ing young children develop an understanding of writing language
characteristics (e.g., existing resources, curriculum and children’s (Clay, 2001). High-quality modeling involves teachers’ explana-
skills) are important when determining whether the practices tion of, or attention to, the process of writing through the use
being delivered through PD can be learned by teachers and imple- of think aloud, self-talk, or questioning that draws attention to
mented by them. Such factors are also important for encouraging what teachers are doing in ways that promote children’s current
teachers’ personal reflection on the effectiveness of HLTPs in pro- understanding of writing products and processes (Roskos, Tabors,
moting children’s skills and the impact of PD on their practices & Lenhart, 2009). In addition, given that writing is a complex
(Zhang & Cook, 2019). act (i.e., one must coordinate multiple developmental systems:
motor, language, orthographic, and cognitive), children may need
teachers’ supports when coordinating multiple literacy skills (e.g.,
1.3. The Writing Promotion Intervention (WPI) model
letter name and sound correspondence, and print knowledge) to
complete a writing task (Cabell, Tortorelli, & Gerde, 2013; Piasta,
The Writing Promotion Intervention (WPI) model was devel-
Purpura, & Wagner, 2010; Puranik & Lonigan, 2014). Although
oped within an HLTP framework. The theory of change in this model
research suggests that teachers provide support of handwriting
focuses on a core set of research-based HLTP writing instruction
skills, such as hand-over-hand guidance of forming recognizable
practices that can be implemented during a typically occurring rou-
letters (Gerde et al., 2015), they provide considerably less support
tine classroom activity (i.e., morning meeting). Teachers are more
around other writing practices (Bingham et al., 2017). There-
likely to implement writing instruction regularly and with high
fore, the scaffolding practices in our intervention focused on the
fidelity, if they find such practices easy and convenient to imple-
use of explicit literacy instruction to support children’s writing
ment. Implementing such strategies should then, in turn, lead to
attempts through helping them coordinate multiple literacy skills
improvements in children’s writing. We explain the WPI model
(see Table 1).
in depth below as well as the research base that supports this
The WPI model is centered on one activity context, Morning
approach.
Meeting Time (MMT), as the instructional routine in which teach-
The WPI instructional model is centered on three core HLTPs
ers implement the HLTP. During a typical preschool day, MMT
that research suggests are important to children’s writing develop-
often serves as the very first teacher-led large group activity and
ment: (a) offering interactive writing opportunities, (b) explaining
teachers often complete a set of routinized daily tasks during this
and demonstrating writing processes, and (c) scaffolding children’s
instructional routine (Bustamante et al., 2018), such as checking
writing attempts with explicit literacy instruction. Early literacy
attendance, talking about the date and weather, discussing the
research highlights the importance of providing interactive and
daily schedule or reviewing the classroom theme or area/unit of
engaging literacy activities to children (Gerde, Bingham, & Wasik,
study (e.g., fall, farm animals, and transportation). Research demon-
2012; Ilgaz, Hassinger-Das, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2014; Zhang
strates that morning meeting practices are common in preschool
& Quinn, 2018). Writing activities should be interactive so that chil-
programs, particularly Head Start, and can be used as an inter-
dren may actively participate and initiate writing attempts, which
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 141

vention context (Hindman & Wasik, 2012). For example, in their 1.4. Research questions
study of 34 Head Start classrooms, Zhang, Diamond et al. (2015)
document that almost all classrooms had a routinized MMT large As a first step toward building evidence of promise for this inter-
group circle time before, or at a time separate from, large group vention approach, this study aimed to promote children’s writing
book reading. Importantly, the average length of the MMT (13 min) skills by making modifications to teachers’ MMT practices. Two
was significantly longer than a typical large group book read- research questions were addressed in this initial investigation: (1)
ing activity (9 min), suggesting that teachers were spending more To what extent does guiding teachers to implement writing instruc-
time in these routinized practices instead of evidence-based strate- tion during MMT lead to higher quality writing interactions in early
gies such as shared book reading. Given research documenting childhood classrooms? We hypothesized that MMT, as a typical
that teachers’ general literacy instruction is of relatively low qual- large group activity, could be used as a meaningful context for
ity (Justice, Mashburn, Hamre, & Pianta, 2008) and that teachers’ promoting core practices of teachers’ writing instruction. (2) Does
speech and literacy instruction varies across different classroom infusing writing instruction into MMT promote children’s early
activities and routines (Gest, Holland-Coviello, Welsh, Eicher-Catt, reading and writing skills? We hypothesized that MMT promotes
& Gill, 2006), MMT appears to be an instructional context that could teacher–child writing interactions in ways that increase opportu-
be leveraged for meaningful and explicit writing experiences. This nities for children to practice and explore writing skills. Children’s
assumption is bolstered by studies documenting that, in contrast writing experiences during MMT time will lead to improvements
to book reading talk that tends to include relatively richer lan- in their early reading and writing skills.
guage and content (as teachers focus on story structures, plots, and
oral language skills, see Neuman & Dwyer, 2009; Dickinson, Hofer,
2. Method
Barnes, & Grifenhagen, 2014), teacher’s speech during MMT typi-
cally includes short and repetitive utterances that do not change
2.1. Participants
significantly over the semester (e.g., “What is the date today?” or
“What is the weather today?”) (Zhang, Diamond et al., 2015). With
Fourteen Pre-Kindergarten classrooms from four childcare cen-
such research in mind, the relatively low quality of MMT instruc-
ters in a southeastern urban area serving children from low-income
tional activities offer a promising context for implementing writing
families (i.e., over 80% of enrolled children receive free meals) were
instruction because many teachers are familiar with activities that
recruited for this study. Participating classrooms maintained a full-
could provide opportunities to include or embed literacy teaching
day schedule. Each classroom had one lead teacher and an assistant
of early reading and writing skills, including letter writing (e.g.,
teacher. The 14 lead teachers (all female) and 112 children from
writing children’s names or letters from children’s names), phono-
their classrooms (eight children per classroom) participated in this
logical awareness (e.g., clapping syllabus in children’s names), print
study. The majority of teachers were African American (one Cau-
knowledge (e.g., pointing to the words of the month, and children’s
casian teacher), had experience working with young children (six
name cards), or spelling (e.g., writing messages or simple words
years average experience), and had a Bachelor’s degree in either
related to the curricular theme or topic of study).
early childhood education (11 teachers) or from another discipline
Although little research has examined MMT as an instructional
(three teachers). Participating children were all African Ameri-
context to promote early writing skills, teachers implementation
can and spoke English as their primary language. Eleven children
of a Morning Message routine, as part of MMT, has been docu-
did not complete the post-intervention because they had stopped
mented by Hindman and Wasik (2012) as an activity context for
attending the child care centers or because of sustained school
building children’s decoding related skills. In a pilot intervention
absence. Because the results of data analyses with and without
study, they trained 19 Head Start teachers to implement explicit lit-
the missing children were the same, the distribution of missing
eracy instruction using a Morning Message routine. Teachers were
children was across both conditions and generally evenly dis-
trained to generate one or two sentences with children about a vari-
tributed across classrooms, and no significant difference was found
ety of topics (e.g., weather, special event, and holiday) and write
between the 11 children and other children’s pre-intervention per-
these sentences on a whiteboard. Teachers were also trained to
formance, we report the results of data analyses on 101 children (55
model writing in ways that provided explicit literacy instruction
girls, mean age = 55.45 months, SD = 3.62), who completed assess-
(e.g., letter knowledge, print knowledge and letter-sound aware-
ments at both data points, for better interpretation of intervention
ness). Although this exploratory study did not have a comparison
impacts.
group or a large sample size, a significant relation emerged between
teachers’ letter knowledge instruction during MMT and children’s
growth of letter knowledge across the year. 2.2. Recruitment procedure
The WPI model has a more specific focus of infusing writing
instruction into MMT through the modification of existing morn- Ten childcare centers were identified for initial participation in
ing routines based on teachers’ own preference and children’s this study from a list of Georgia Pre-Kindergarten Programs where
perceived interests. It is similar to the naturalistic instructional over 50% of enrolled children received free meals. Center directors
approach used in intervention studies of children with spe- of each center were contacted via emails and phone calls to explain
cial needs, in which researchers embed teaching of target early the research project. Four centers agreed to participate. Other cen-
skills (e.g., communication skill) into typically occurring activities ters who were contacted did not participate due to existing early
(Snyder et al., 2015). The WPI model was implemented in large intervention projects already occurring in their programs. Upon
group routine for all children in a classroom rather than small- the directors’ agreement to participate in the study, researchers
group or free-choice routines for individualized instruction because visited each center and explained to teachers the study’s purpose
(a) teachers spend considerable time in these settings during the and procedures. All teachers (both lead and assistant teachers) in
preschool day (Early, Iruka, Ritchie, & Barbarin, 2010) and (b) teach- the four centers consented to participate. After gaining teachers’
ers in this study had MMT as part of their daily schedule but not consent, researchers distributed a letter explaining the study and
small group instruction. Lastly, we provided a brief four-hour work- parental permission forms to the parents of all children in target
shop to introduce HLTP writing instruction to teachers and guided classrooms. Among children whose parents agreed to participate,
teachers to modify existing daily activities to infuse HLTP writing we randomly selected eight children (four boys and four girls) per
instruction into MMT. classroom. After recruitment into the study, centers were assigned
142 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

to treatment and comparison conditions as explained in more detail The PD workshop was given to intervention group teachers
below. after pretest data collection (late January), while comparison group
Centers were located around a large urban metro area in the teachers received typical professional development support from
southeastern US and served children of diverse ethnic groups. their own centers. Fidelity observations occurred a month after
Consistent with Georgia PreK guidelines, all centers followed the the PD workshop (late February). After the completion of post-
Georgia Early Learning and Development Standards (GELDS) and intervention data collection, comparison group teachers received
used Work Sampling as their system for documenting children’s the same materials and training as teachers in the intervention
learning and development. All classrooms also reported imple- group.
menting the Creative Curriculum. Among the four participating
centers, variation existed in classroom/building structure. Two cen- 2.3.2. The HLTP professional learning workshop
ters were designed in a typical fashion in which each PreK class was A four-hour long PD workshop, that identified the three HLTP
located in its own room (i.e., each classroom contained one PreK writing instructional approaches, was developed and delivered to
class). The other two centers (with two classrooms in each center) intervention teachers (see Table 1). The content of the workshop
shared a more open concept floor plan, with classrooms sharing covered: (a) an overview of early writing development and its
a large open-space area, with individual classrooms partitioned association with other early literacy skills, (b) information about
by child-height shelves. Although classrooms in this center were creating a print rich classroom environment, (c) the importance of
separated into distinct areas, with each classroom having a desig- MMT as a writing instructional context, and (d) the demonstration
nated space for large group and learning center activities, teachers of HLTP writing instruction. Because participating classrooms did
in these centers could easily see and observe each other during not have many materials that could be used for interactive writing
teaching and learning activities. This design was taken into account activities during MMT (i.e., small whiteboards for both teachers and
when assigning centers, rather than classrooms, to intervention and children to write on), we provided a set of handheld lightweight
comparison conditions. whiteboards and led a group discussion with teachers to iden-
tify the MMT routine activities that they felt most comfortable
2.3. Intervention procedure modifying in order to most effectively use whiteboards for writ-
ing instruction. Using handheld whiteboards enabled teachers to
2.3.1. Group assignment model and teach writing in an interactive way and provided oppor-
Because the open concept floor plan of two centers made tunities for children to practice writing independently after they
the possibility of intervention contamination likely, we did not saw the teacher model writing. Teachers were encouraged to mod-
assign teachers randomly to comparison or intervention condi- ify MMT to include both teacher-led and children-initiated writing
tions. Rather, we assigned eight teachers and 64 children (mean activities. For example, given that all teachers had a calendar time
age 54.84 months, SD = 3.53, 32 girls) from two childcare centers routine, they were encouraged to write and model writing letters,
(six classrooms from a typical center and two classrooms from the name of the month, or the date on a big whiteboard, instead of
open space sharing center) to the intervention group. Six teachers pointing to the printed calendar, and to prompt children to write
(four classrooms from a typical center and two from an open space the same letters or words on their handheld whiteboards. Another
sharing center) and 48 children from the two centers (mean age way that writing was embedded into already occurring MMT rou-
56.24 months, SD = 3.67, 23 girls) were assigned to the comparison tines was to have teachers take attendance by asking children to
group. Assignment of the open space sharing centers was random, independently write their names (or initial letter(s)) on their hand-
however, to condition. In this way, both comparison and interven- held whiteboards. In addition to modeling writing, teachers were
tion groups had typical and space-sharing classrooms. Participating trained to support children’s writing attempts by scaffolding and
teachers did not know to which group they would be assigned encouraging them, and by accepting writing in any form the child
to when they consented to participate in the research study. No was able to produce (i.e., letters or letter-like shapes). During the
statistically significant group differences of children’s age, gender, modeling and scaffolding process, teachers were encouraged to
or teacher’s education level were detected before the intervention draw attention to the shape of letters in addition to the sound(s)
implementation. that they make. For example, while writing the initial letter for the
The intervention occurred in the spring of the PreK year and month March as part of calendar time, teachers would guide chil-
lasted approximately three months (mid-January to late April). dren to write individual and salient letters sounds while supporting
The authors observed all classrooms two times: at the beginning letter formation (“March begins with M. The M goes up and down,
(pre-intervention) and the end of the Spring semester (post- up and down, like two mountains”).
intervention). Classroom observations were arranged on typical
PreK days at a time arranged with the teachers. Based on the User 2.3.3. Follow-up collaborative coaching session
Guide Manual of selected observational measures (Smith, Brady, & In addition to a four-hour workshop and approximately one
Anastasopoulos, 2008) and previously published research (Gerde month after the PD workshop, intervention teachers received a
et al., 2015), each observation lasted a minimum of 90 min during collaborative coaching session which included an observation of
which the researchers observed MMT, a large group book reading, MMT (about 20 min), and one 15–20-min one-on-one “researcher-
and center time (i.e., free choice). Outside time was not observed teacher conversation” meeting. During the MMT observation, a
and was not counted within the total number of minutes of the researcher completed a fidelity check-list of teachers’ writing
observation. The length of MMT was counted from the time all chil- instruction. After the observation, the researcher briefly met with
dren gathered at a large circle area until more than 50% of children teachers to provide feedback about their implementation of HLTP
were dismissed from the group for other activities. If teachers con- practices during MMT. The meeting focused first on practices
ducted MMT along with a read-aloud session, for coding purposes, that the teacher was implementing well and then on the teach-
the moment teachers started to discuss the book was considered ing practice that needed strengthening or tweaking to be more
as the end of the MMT. Children were assessed by trained research effective. The researcher also answered teachers’ questions and
assistants pre and post-intervention. Children were invited to com- addressed any issues brought up by the teacher (e.g., challenges
plete early language, reading, and writing assessments in a quiet with managing materials). This approach established a teaching
area adjacent to their classrooms (e.g., teachers’ lounge, multipur- partnership between researchers and teachers and provided an
pose room, or staff offices). opportunity for teachers to receive individualized support and feel
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 143

successful in the changes they were making to their instruction. pendent writing attempts: child uses writing in play or activity,
Similar approaches are employed in other intervention studies and child writes at writing center, and child writes letters, num-
have been suggested to be important to improving the quality of bers, or words. During an observation, an observer checked the
teachers’ language and literacy practices (e.g., Buysse, Castro, & items/materials that were present in a classroom or that were
Peisner-Feinberg, 2010). observed during classroom routines/instruction. Because interven-
tion group teachers received handheld whiteboards and markers,
2.4. Measures these writing materials were not counted as existing classroom
writing materials when coding writing materials available in class-
2.4.1. Teachers’ writing instruction rooms. Further, because intervention group children were provided
Three classroom focused instructional measures were used to with interactive writing activities, only children’s independent
assess writing instruction before and after the intervention. Two writing attempts that occurred outside of MMT (e.g., free choice
of these observational measures evaluated the quality of class- time) were counted when observing the children’s independent
room writing experiences (e.g., (1) ELLCO; Smith et al., 2008 and writing attempts domain.
(2) WRITE, Gerde et al., 2015) while the third was a researcher To derive scores, trained research assistants first counted how
developed fidelity measure. ELLCO and WRITE have been used in many items were observed in each sub-domain. Each sub-domain
previous early literacy studies examining the quality of reading was then issued a rating on a four-point scale: “0” if none of the
and writing experiences in early childhood programs serving low- items were observed, “1” if less than 50% of items in a domain were
income communities (e.g., Gerde et al., 2015; Powell et al., 2010). observed, “2” if more than 50% of items in a domain were observed,
The researchers conducted all classroom observations. High inter- and “3” if all items in a sub-domain were observed. Lastly, a score
rater reliability of observational and child assessment measures of each domain was created by adding all sub-domain rating scores
(k > 0.90) were achieved before pre- and post-intervention data col- together. For example, in order to generate the score of the domain
lection. Pre-intervention observation reliability was established in of writing instruction, the research assistants would count how
a classroom that was not participating in the intervention study many strategies teachers were observed implementing for each of
and one classroom that was participating (i.e., classrooms were the three sub-domains. When coding the sub-domains of “expla-
double coded). One intervention classroom was also double coded nation of writing process” (four items), a teacher would be given
post-intervention to establish that strong inter rater reliability was “0” points if she did not explain the writing process. She would
maintained across the course of the intervention. Because results receive “1” point if she used one of the four strategies, 2 points if
revealed strong agreement on the fidelity checklist and other mea- she used two out of four strategies, and “3” points if she used all
sures, classrooms were then coded by only one observer. four strategies. Scores for the sub-domains of “demonstration of
writing” and “scaffolding of writing” were created with the same
2.4.2. Language and literacy classroom instruction and supports approach. In the end, the scores from the three sub-domains were
The Early Language and Literacy Classroom Observation (ELLCO; summed to create a total writing instruction domain. In this study,
Smith et al., 2008) assessed the global quality of language and liter- the ELLCO Print and Early Writing domain score was consistently
acy focused supports and interactions occurring within classrooms. and significantly correlated with the WRITE writing instruction
Trained observers rated classroom literacy environments using 19 domain score pre (r = 0.56, p < 0.05) and post-intervention (r = 0.68,
Likert-scale items (range 1–5) in five domains: classroom struc- p < 0.01).
ture, curriculum, language environment, books and book reading,
and print and early writing. Since this study targeted teachers’ writ-
ing instruction, we only used three items from the Print and Early 2.4.4. Fidelity checklist
Writing domain for the purpose of this study. The three items found In order to observe teachers’ implementation of writing activ-
in this subscale include: early writing environment (i.e., the extent ities and instruction from the PD workshop, an 8-item checklist
to which classroom writing materials promote children’s aware- was developed (see Appendix A). Three items focused on teach-
ness of writing), support for children’s writing (i.e., the extent to ers’ implementation of whiteboard activities. Five additional items
which teachers guide and support children’s writing attempts), and focused on teachers’ implementation of specific teaching strate-
environmental print (i.e., extent to which teachers use environ- gies. During observations of intervention classrooms, these items
mental print to promote children’s print knowledge). According were checked as either present or not present. Ratio scores were
to the ELLCO Users’ Guide. the coefficient alpha for the classroom calculated to capture the extent to which teachers provided writ-
observation total score is 0.90, and 0.86 for the language and liter- ing instruction based on the workshop training. High inter-rater
acy subscale. In this study, the three targeted items were strongly reliability (k > 0.90) was achieved between two researchers before
and significantly related. conducting fidelity observation.

2.4.3. Writing resources and interactions


The Writing Resources and Interactions in Teaching Envi- 2.5. Children’s writing skills
ronments (WRITE; Gerde et al., 2015), a 41-item check list
observational measure, was also used to assess the quality of partic- 2.5.1. Children’s name-writing skills
ipating classrooms writing environments in three specific domains: To assess children’s name writing skill, children were given
(a) classroom writing materials and environmental print, (b) teach- a blank piece of paper and a marker and were asked to write
ers’ writing instruction, and (c) child initiated independent writing their first name. Children’s writing was coded by one research
attempts. The classroom writing materials and environmental print assistant and one of the authors independently (Cohen’s k > 0.90)
domain includes three sub-domains: availability of writing mate- with a coding scheme that ranged from “1” (scribbling) to “9” for
rials (six items), arrangement of writing materials (five items), and completely writing the first name with correct conventions (e.g.,
environmental print (four items). The teachers’ writing instruc- capitalized initial letter). This coding scheme was used previously
tion domain includes three sub-domains: explanation of writing by Diamond et al. (2008) and found reliable with other existing
(four items), demonstration of writing (five items), and scaffold- coding schemes of children’s name writing (Puranik, Schreiber,
ing of writing (five items). The children’s writing attempts domain Estabrook, & O’Donnell, 2014). Inter-rater reliability was checked
is made up of three items focusing on children’s observed inde- every 10 writing samples to maintain high inter-rater reliability.
144 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

2.5.2. Letter writing were shown a sheet with 26 uppercase letters presented in random
Children were given a blank piece of paper and were asked order and asked to identify the names of letters. Children were then
to write 10 letters based on the dictation of the following letter shown a sheet of randomly ordered uppercase letters and conso-
names: B, D, S, T, O, A, H, K, M, and C. The 10 letters selected for this nant digraphs (i.e., ch, sh, th) and asked to produce the sound of
assessment were previously identified among the most familiar let- the letters and digraphs. The PALS-PreK has been used in a variety
ters known by young children (see Justice, Pence, Bowles, Ryan, & of studies of children’s early literacy skills (e.g., Wasik & Hindman,
Wiggins, 2006). This letter writing assessment has been employed 2011; Welsch, Sullivan, & Justice, 2003) and demonstrates strong
in previous studies of young children’s writing with high internal associations with other standardized early literacy assessments,
consistency (c.f. ˛ = 0.93, Puranik & Lonigan, 2011). Children’s letter such as the Test of Early Reading Ability (r = 0.67, p < 0.01 Reid,
writing samples were coded by two research assistants to count the Hresko, & Hammill, 2001)
number of correctly written letters (possible range 1–10). In order
to give credit to children’s writing while accounting for variation in 2.6.3. Print knowledge
children’s fine motor skill, research assistants gave credit to approx- Concepts About Print (CAP; Clay, 1985) was used to assess
imations of children’s written letters and common deviations from children’s print knowledge. For this assessment, consistent with
conventional forms (e.g., letter reversals). Children’s writing of both procedures in previous studies (e.g., Welsch et al., 2003; Diamond
upper and lower-case letters was accepted. et al., 2008), the examiner showed a storybook to children and
asked children to identify the title of the book, the letter, the word,
2.5.3. Invented spelling and the direction of reading story text. Responses were scored as
Using a spelling task developed by Puranik and Lonigan (2011), correct or incorrect with six total points possible. The reliability
children were asked to write six CVC words (i.e., mat, bed, duck, cat, coefficients of this measure have ranged from 0.73 to 0.95 (Clay,
fell, hen) to assess their word spelling skill (possible range 0–42). 1985). This measure has also shown high correlations with pre-
For this task, each child was given a sheet of paper with five blank reading skills in previous studies (e.g., Welsch et al., 2003).
lines. Research assistants asked the child to write, or attempt to
write each word on the blank line. Research assistants repeated the
2.7. Data analyses
word as necessary for the children. Two research assistants coded
children’s writing using a 7-point scale. Children were given “1” if
The primary goals of data analyses were to detect intervention
they did not write but verbally responded with random letters, 2
impacts on both teachers’ instruction and children’s writing and
if they scribbled, 3 if they spelled a word with random letters, 4
other early skills. Analyses are detailed for examining intervention
if they wrote the correct initial or last letter of a word, 5 if they
impacts for both groups below.
correctly spelled both initial and last letter of a word, 6 if they cor-
rectly spelled both initial and last letter with an incorrect vowel
or used invented spelling, and 7 if they correctly spelled a word. 2.7.1. Teachers’ pre and post-intervention writing instruction
This spelling task evidenced high internal consistency (˛ = 0.96). One-way ANOVA analyses revealed no significant group
One research assistant and one of the authors coded all the chil- differences between treatment and comparison teachers’ pre-
dren’s writing samples. The inter-rater reliability of the coding intervention writing instruction (i.e., ELLCO domain of writing
was checked before coding children’s writing samples and was environment and WRITE scores, see Table 2). Hence, in order to
also checked every 10 writing samples to maintain high inter-rater evaluate the intervention impact on teachers’ practices, repeated
reliability, Cohen’s kappa > 0.90. measure ANOVAs (two time points x two groups) were calculated
using raw scores from teachers’ observational data.
2.6. Children’s pre-reading skills
2.7.2. Children’s skills pre- and post-intervention
2.6.1. Phonological awareness Pre-intervention ANOVA analyses revealed that children in the
The Test of Preschool Early Literacy (TOPEL; Lonigan, Wagner, comparison group generally performed better than children in
Torgesen, & Rashotte, 2007) phonological awareness subtest was the intervention group and did so significantly on name writing
used to assess children’s phonological awareness skills. The sub- and print knowledge tasks (see. Table 3). Although children in
test includes two blending and two elision tasks. Children were the comparison group performed significantly better on the name
presented with a set of pictures as prompts first to guide them to writing task, it is important to note that because of the ordinal
complete sound manipulation items, such as identifying the com- nature of the variable, such differences were minimal within an
ponents of compound words, creating a new compound word by instructional context, as children, on average, across both groups,
combining word sounds, or by taking sounds away (e.g., “Look at were using a few letters (but not letter like shapes) to write their
these pictures. Now point to batman without bat.”). In subsequent names (e.g., name writing scores of comparison (M = 7.85, SD, 1.69)
tasks, children were provided verbal prompts (e.g., “If you take and intervention group children (M = 7.11, SD = 1.97)). Most chil-
away bell from the word doorbell, what would you have?”) without dren’s performance on the name writing task, in both groups, was
pictures to assist them. This subtest evidences good test-retest reli- distributed in the range between 6 to 8 before the intervention.
ability (r = 0.83) and high concurrent validity (i.e., blending r = 0.59 However, given such pre-intervention differences, we evaluated
and elision r = 0.65) with the Comprehensive Test of Phonological the associations between the intervention and children’s writing
Processing (Lonigan et al., 2007). The test also demonstrates strong and literacy skills by conducting analyses on children’s change
psychometric properties when used with children from various scores of their pre- and post- assessment performance based on
backgrounds (Webb, Patton-Terry, Bingham, Puranik, & Lederberg, What Works Clearinghouse standards (2017). In order to account
2018). A total score is generated by counting all correct answers for the best method of estimating effects, we conducted data anal-
(range 0–26). yses using both children’s raw change scores (i.e., subtracting
children’s pre-intervention scores from post-intervention scores)
2.6.2. Letter and letter sound awareness and residual change scores (i.e., regressing their pre-intervention
The Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening (PALS-PreK, scores to their post-intervention scores) separately. Analyses using
Invernizzi, Sullivan, Meier, & Swank, 2004) alphabet knowledge the two calculations of change scores yielded the same result.
subtest was used to evaluate children’s letter knowledge. Children Because residual change scores, in comparison to raw change
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 145

Table 2
The descriptive statistics of classroom observation (N = 14).

Variable (range) Comparison (n = 6) Intervention (n = 8) F d

M SD M SD

ELLCO writing environment (0–15) Pre-Int. 9.50 1.22 8.94 1.21 0.74 0.46
Post-Int. 9.75 0.69 12.19 1.49 13.70** 2.00
Writing material (0–9) Pre-Int. 6.17 1.94 6.13 2.10 0.01 0.05
Post-Int. 6.33 1.37 7.38 2.67 0.75 0.47
Writing instruction (0–9) Pre-Int. 5.33 3.08 3.50 3.07 1.22 0.60
Post-Int. 4.17 1.47 7.00 2.62 5.61* 1.28
Children’s writing attempts (0–3) Pre-Int. 2.00 1.10 0.75 1.04 4.76* 1.18
Post-Int. 1.00 0.89 0.75 1.03 0.22 0.25
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

Table 3
Children’s gains in literacy and writing skills pre and post-intervention.

Variables (range) Growth Comparison (n = 46) Growth Intervention (n = 55) F d

t d M SD t d M SD

Name writing Pre-Int. 7.85 1.69 7.11 1.97 4.93* 0.46


(0–9) Post-Int. 7.98 1.57 8.29 1.08 1.39 0.24
.85 0.16 5.86** 1.59
Letter writing Pre-Int. 6.40 3.45 5.65 3.70 2.12 0.29
(0–8) Post-Int. 7.44 2.82 7.40 2.88 0.45 0.13
3.94** 1.14 6.12** 1.67
Spelling Pre-Int. 17.48 8.05 16.20 7.36 0.63 0.16
(0–42) Post-Int. 21.80 6.69 20.31 6.19 1.34- 0.23
**
3.87 0.74 4.70** 0.90
PA Pre-Int. 14.78 4.29 13.67 4.61 1.54 0.24
(0–26) Post-Int. 15.85 4.90 15.00 5.61 0.64 0.16
1.78† 0.52 2.17* 0.59
Letter naming Pre-Int. 18.33 8.66 16.24 9.46 2.72 0.33
(0–26) Post-Int. 19.85 7.99 19.80 7.05 0.05 0.04
**
2.86 0.84 4.69** 1.28
Letter sound Pre-Int. 8.33 8.20 8.38 8.08 0.04 0.13
(0–26) Post-Int. 10.52 8.00 11.09 7.60 0.06 0.15
3.20** 0.92 3.73** 1.02
Print knowledge (0–6) Pre-Int. 3.67 1.66 3.13 1.43 5.82* 0.48
Post-Int. 3.73 1.36 3.70 1.37 0.01 0.02
0.41 0.08 2.49* 0.68
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

scores, eliminate potential outliers based on linear regression mod- to the intervention. In the model i represents each individual child
eling (Cronbach & Furby, 1970), we present findings from analyses and j represents each individual classroom.
using residual change scores. In order to accommodate the nested
structure of the data (i.e., children were nested within each teach-
ers’ classroom), we then conducted hierarchical linear modeling 3. Results
(HLM) to examine the intervention impact on children’s pre- and
post-intervention changes in assessments. The model we estimated Observations demonstrated that all participating classrooms
was: had a MMT routine before the intervention began. The aver-
age length of these MMT routines was approximately 16 min,
Intervention group, M = 15.12 min, SD = 7.55, Range 5–26 min, Com-
Child Residual Scoreij =  00 +  01 ∗Groupj +  02 ∗Pre-ELLCO2j
parison group, M = 18.8 min, SD = 4.76, Range 7–25 min. The length
+  03 ∗Pre-WRITE j of MMT increased during the intervention for treatment class-
rooms, but this change was not statistically significant, 21 min after
+  10 ∗Child GENDERij +  20 ∗Child AGE ij + u0j + r ij the intervention, Intervention group, M = 21.87 min, SD = 10.82,
Range 8–40 min, Comparison group, M = 19.00 min, SD = 5.51, Range
11–30 min. It is notable, however, that one teacher from the inter-
In this model, children’s residual change score (level-1 variable) vention group, during the post-intervention observation, had a very
served as an outcome variable explained by teachers’ group assign- long MMT time (40 min). This teacher included a book picture walk
ment (i.e., intervention vs. comparison), pre-intervention teachers’ as part of MMT that was related to her curricular theme, which
ELLCO and WRITE scores (level-2 variables), and children’s age and added considerable time to how long children were on the carpet.
gender (level-1 variables). In this way, we controlled for child-level For most teachers, however, MMT routines evidenced the same
variables that may have contributed to performance (e.g., age and daily routine activities pre and post-intervention (i.e., calendar,
gender) as well as the nature of pre-intervention classroom writ- weather, counting who was present). Hence, findings suggest that
ing environments (e.g., ELLCO and WRITE scores). In the model, i the WPI intervention, generally, only slightly modified the structure
represents each individual child and j represents each individual of teachers’ existing morning routine without altering the general
classroom. The coefficient  01 suggests the group difference due activities that teachers typically focused on during MMT.
146 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

Fig. 1. Changes in teachers’ writing instruction pre- and post-intervention.


Note: The possible range of WRITE writing instruction domain is 0–9, and the possible range of ELLCO writing environment domain is 0–15.

3.1. Changes of teachers’ instruction pre- and post-intervention play time) did not change significantly during the intervention.
Descriptive statistics demonstrate that while children’s indepen-
Descriptive statistics of teachers’ writing instruction (e.g., ELLCO dent writing attempts in the intervention classrooms remained
Print and Early Writing domain and three domains of the WRITE) the same across the intervention period, children’s independent
and comparison results between groups are presented in Table 2. writing in comparison classrooms had a noticeable decrease from
The global quality of classroom writing experiences (i.e., ELLCO winter to spring, t (5) = −2.24, p = 0.08, d = 1.21.
Print and Early Writing domain) and the quality of teachers’ writing Examination of fidelity data revealed that all eight intervention
instruction (i.e., WRITE writing instruction domain) were generally group teachers successfully implemented instruction consistent
low. Despite the presence of relatively abundant writing materials with the WPI PD model. Intervention teachers implemented 62.5%
(i.e., WRITE writing material domain), large variation in writing (five out of eight fidelity items) to 100% of targeted core teach-
instruction was evident among classrooms in both conditions. ing practices. All the teachers incorporated whiteboard writing
One-way ANOVAs were conducted to compare the two groups of activities into MMT (i.e., teachers wrote on the large whiteboard
teachers’ pre- and post-intervention on writing instruction vari- and children used handheld whiteboards to complete some rou-
ables (see Table 2). Although pre-intervention results revealed tinized activities), modeled writing for children, and were observed
more frequent child independent writing attempts in comparison prompting children to write with letter names. Six of the eight
classrooms than intervention classrooms, no significant pre- teachers were observed drawing attention to the ways that letters
intervention differences between groups on writing instruction are formed and prompting children’s writing with letter sounds.
variables were detected. In contrast, post-intervention compar- Five of the eight teachers encouraged children to talk about their
isons suggested that intervention classroom teachers provided writing (e.g., “What did you write?” or “What is this letter?”). As
significantly higher quality writing instruction than comparison a group, teachers implemented close to 90% of targeted writing
group teachers. instruction practices, demonstrating relatively high fidelity to the
We conducted repeated-measure ANOVAs to further investi- WPI intervention approach (see Appendix A).
gate the associations between the WPI intervention and teachers’
writing instruction. In the model comparing group performance on
the ELLCO writing environment domain, both time F (1,12) = 26.01, 3.2. Changes of children’s skills pre- and post-intervention
p < 0.001, d = 2.75 and time x group interactions F (1,12) = 19.11,
p < 0.00, d = 2.36 were statistically significant with large effect size Descriptive statistics, the results of paired T-test by time, and
(Sawilowsky, 2009). These results suggest that, although teachers, ANOVAs between groups are presented in Table 3. Generally speak-
overall, provided significantly better quality writing environment ing, comparison group children outperformed intervention group
at the end of the spring semester, intervention group teachers children in all assessment tasks pre-intervention, with statistically
provided significantly higher quality writing instruction than com- significant differences detected on name writing, and print knowl-
parison teachers after the intervention (see Fig. 1). In the model edge tasks with large effect size. Despite initial group differences,
comparing teachers’ WRITE writing instruction domain scores, the paired T-tests comparisons of children’s pre- and post-intervention
time x group (i.e., intervention vs. comparison group) interaction assessment scores showed that intervention group children’s per-
was statistically significant with a large effect size, F (1,12) = 5.13, formance in all the assessment tasks improved significantly across
p = 0.04, d = 1.22. However, time and group differences were not the intervention phrase with large effect size. Although compari-
significantly different. This result suggests that intervention group son group children also demonstrated improvements in some early
teachers provided significantly more writing instruction than com- literacy skills, children’s name writing and print knowledge scores
parison teachers at the post-intervention time point (see Fig. 1). did not significantly change. Notably, an examination of children’s
Classroom writing materials (i.e., WRITE writing material performance post-intervention revealed that the significant pre-
domain) and children’s independent writing attempts outside intervention advantages of comparison group children in writing
of MMT (WRITE independent child writing domain during free and print knowledge tasks disappeared.
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 147

To investigate associations between the WPI intervention and attempts with letter-sound support or instruction. One reason for
children’s growth in early reading and writing skills, we con- this finding may relate to the level of early reading and writing
ducted HLM analyses to examine the differences of children’s skills in intervention classrooms. It is possible that because some
residual scores of writing and pre-reading assessments between children in these classrooms had limited letter-sound knowledge
intervention and comparison group children. Results demon- (i.e., pre-phonological writers, Polo et al., 2009) that teachers per-
strate that pre-intervention classroom writing environment and ceived scaffolding writing attempts with letter names (rather than
teachers’ writing instruction were not significantly related to chil- sounds) as more appropriate for children in a whole group setting.
dren’s residual scores, nor was children’s age and gender. In The significant association between intervention implementa-
contrast, a significant intervention effect was found, as interven- tion and changes in teachers’ writing instruction demonstrates
tion group children achieved significantly more gains in name promise of an HLTP focused PD approach. Because delivering con-
writing,  01 = 0.64, SE = 0.22, t (10) = 2.88, p = 0.03, letter writ- tent to early childhood educators is often challenging given time
ing,  01 = 0.65, SE = 0.28, t (10) = 2.29, p = 0.03, and letter naming, and staffing constraints (Whitebrook & Sakai, 2003), an HLTP
 01 = 1.17, SE = 0.50, t (10) = 2.34, p = 0.02 than comparison group approach may prove more effective for many early childhood
children. No significant differences were detected in children’s educators who have little time for planning or professional devel-
residual change scores on other early reading (e.g., phonological opment (Powell, 2013). This approach is important to note in light
awareness) or writing (e.g., invented spelling) tasks. of comprehensive early literacy PD approaches aimed at improv-
ing the totality of the literacy teaching through extensive training in
PD workshops and onsite/online coaching (e.g., Wasik & Hindman,
4. Discussion
2011; Powell et al., 2010). Many existing early literacy PD inter-
vention models follow this rationale in design and implementation
Given the relatively low quality of writing instruction in
with intensive supports for teachers across multiple literacy and
preschool classrooms (Bingham et al., 2017; Gerde et al., 2015) and
language skills (see Dickinson & Caswell, 2007; Landry et al., 2006;
because early writing skills are predictors of later reading and writ-
Powell et al., 2010; Wasik & Hindman, 2011). Compared to such
ing outcomes, it is important to identify instructional approaches
approaches, the WPI model provided substantially shorter PD but
that preschool teachers can implement frequently and with fidelity
demonstrated significant associations with the quality of teachers’
as part of their typical day. In this exploratory study, we developed a
writing instruction. In addition to the specific focus of evidence-
PD intervention model for guiding teachers to infuse high-leverage
based instruction, the flexibility of the WPI model may have also
writing instruction into their existing classroom MMT routine.
supported teachers’ classroom instruction. The intervention was
Through relatively brief professional development experiences (i.e.,
developed to fit within teachers’ existing classroom routines with
a four-hour workshop and a 35–40 min follow up session), imple-
the implementation of the intervention focused on one specific
mentation of the WPI intervention showed significant associations
activity context (i.e., MMT). This intentional design may have
with improvements in teachers’ writing instruction and children’s
assisted teachers in implementing instructional practices in a way
writing skills growth. We discuss research findings in relation to
that provided the least amount of interference to existing teaching
how focusing on writing during MMT may help teachers leverage
responsibilities or daily schedules.
routinized instruction in ways that support children’s early writing
Evidence of MMT routine as an appropriate context in which
skills.
to implement early writing and reading instruction also appears
to be supported by the high fidelity of intervention implementa-
4.1. Infusing high-leverage teaching practice into classroom tion. Because MMT typically occurs daily in preschool classrooms
routines (Bustamante et al., 2018; Zhang, Diamond et al., 2015), it offers an
opportunity to teach early skills that can be promoted by repeated
Analyses revealed significant associations between intervention explicit instruction, frequent exposure, and interactive practices.
implementation and teachers’ writing instruction as evaluated by Another unique feature of MMT is that teachers often implement
both global (ELLCO) and domain-specific (WRITE) observational routines that can involve a meaningful use of environmental print
measures. As we hypothesized, intervention teachers provided to complete certain routinized activities. Although teachers appear
more explicit instruction on writing processes, modeling of writing to need training to intentionally use environmental print to teach
actions, and scaffolding children’s writing. These types of writing literacy skills (e.g., Justice, Kaderavek, Fan, Sofka, & Hunt, 2009),
experiences rarely occur in preschool classrooms, especially those the high fidelity of intervention implementation from this study
serving children from low income backgrounds (i.e., Head Start pro- and previous research (Wasik & Hindman, 2011) demonstrate that
grams, Bingham et al., 2017; Gerde et al., 2015). Teachers writing teachers may find the implementation of evidence-based literacy
supports preintervention are largely consistent with Gerde et al. or writing instruction during regularly occurring MMT routines
(2015) study suggesting that teachers, on average, scaffold chil- less challenging and more convenient to implement than learning
dren’s writing attempts approximately three times during a typical new practices or incorporating new routines into their day. Fur-
preschool morning. The writing environments of classrooms in this ther, because instructional quality during MMT is generally low
study (as evidenced by the presence of writing materials and envi- (see Bustamante et al., 2018), it is important to support teachers
ronmental print), however, were slightly higher than in Gerde et al. in instructional practices that provide engaging MMT experiences
Because the quality of classroom writing environments and writing that enhance children’s learning.
instructional supports are significant, and independent, predictors Importantly, findings from this study revealed that the WPI
of children’s writing skill (see Gerde et al., 2015), findings from this writing intervention approach, which focused primarily on MMT,
study illustrate the importance of providing guidance to preschool did not translate to changes in the availability of classroom writ-
teachers in ways that encourage them to use classroom materials to ing materials or more frequent displays of independent children’s
model writing processes and scaffold children’s writing attempts. writing attempts outside of MMT. This finding is not particularly
Fidelity observations demonstrated that intervention teach- unusual given information suggesting that preschool teachers set
ers implemented almost all teaching strategies introduced in the up learning centers with various types of materials at the beginning
PD workshop. All teachers started modeling writing processes of the semester based on curriculum or early learning standards
and scaffolding children’s writing attempts with attention to let- and then make few substantive changes to the prevalence of
ter names. Fewer teachers, however, scaffolded children’s writing writing materials in learning centers generally or in the writing
148 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

center specifically (Bingham et al., 2017; Gerde, Bingham et al., Despite significant gains in name writing and letter writing
2012). Although we provided intervention group teachers hand- skills, HLM analyses did not detect direct significant associations
held whiteboards, we did not introduce small group or free-choice between the WPI approach and children’s letter sound, phono-
writing activities using these materials outside of MMT. As a result, logical awareness, or invented spelling skills. These insignificant
teachers did not appear to make them available during other times intervention impacts are likely a function of teachers’ limited focus
of the day. Future work should address ways in which teachers on letter-sound correspondence skills during MMT instruction and
can generalize practices learned within this framework to other a relatively limited focus on invented spelling strategies during
instructional opportunities that are provided in early childhood the PD workshop. Teachers may have been more comfortable sup-
settings. porting children’s writing attempts by prompting children’s letter
It is important to note that in order to preserve the objec- knowledge skills (e.g., “We want to write the first letter in the word
tive of MMT as a welcoming time for children in the classroom Mom. Let’s write the letter ‘m.’) than drawing attention to the letter-
(i.e., a time to build community) we did not completely modify sound associations and print (e.g., “What sound do you hear in the
MMT during the intervention. Rather, we encouraged teachers to word mom? Mom has the /m/, /m/ sound. What letter makes the
make decisions about which routine tasks they might modify to /m/ sound? Can you write letter M?”). We noticed more frequent
implement HLTP writing instruction practices based on their own explicit instruction of letter name than letter sound skills during
preferences and perceptions of children’s interests and skills. In fidelity and post-intervention observations. Because linking letter
this way, teachers’ experience and existing knowledge of classroom names to print may be cognitively easier for children in writing
teaching were acknowledged and valued (see. Zhang and Cook, (Zhang, Diamond, & Powell, 2017), teachers may have decided to
2019). As evidenced by high adherence to HLTP practices during the focus on these skills to ensure that all children were able to suc-
intervention, the modification of existing routine activities during cessfully complete the task within a large group context. Attention
MMT rather than replacing these practices with a new set of MMT to phonological awareness skills, including rhyming, blending or
activities may have helped teachers implement the project with segmenting, or syllables, also occurred infrequently and was not a
fidelity. Although this modification did not significantly increase strong focus of the intervention.
the length of morning meeting in intervention classrooms more Secondly, because invented spelling skills are just emerging dur-
than comparison classrooms, the fact that one intervention group ing the preschool years (e.g., Invernizzi & Hayes, 2004; Puranik &
teacher almost doubled her morning meeting time in length, to Lonigan, 2014), it is not emphasized in many preschool/PreK teach-
include writing activities and engage in a “picture walk” of a story- ing standards or curriculum (i.e., GELDS, Georgia Department of
book related to her curricular theme, is of concern. Because teachers Early Care and Learning, 2010). Instruction of invented spelling
may feel the need to squeeze in many instructional opportunities was not highlighted in the PD workshop. Although teachers were
into MMT, it is essential that future MMT work emphasizes the encouraged to model spelling words (e.g., writing theme/unit
importance of capitalizing on existing routinized practices in ways words or calendar words), children were not expected to perform
that increase children’s exposure to writing interactions but does spelling independently given their emerging pre-reading and writ-
not significantly increase children’s large group carpet time. ing skills. This may also explain why teachers’ explicit instruction
of phonological awareness rarely occurred in our intervention. The
4.2. Associations between intervention implementation and majority of invented spelling interventions typically focus on chil-
children’s writing dren in kindergarten, focus on explicit instruction of phonemic
awareness skills, and include frequent structured teaching sessions
Although children in the WPI intervention group performed sig- in small group contexts, which allows teachers to individualize
nificantly lower on writing assessments and print knowledge than instruction in ways that more directly scaffold children’s individual
comparison group children at the beginning of this study, they spelling skills (see Ouellette, Sénéchal, & Haley, 2013).
made significant gains over the intervention on name writing, let- Our findings are generally consistent with Aram and Biron’s
ter writing, and letter naming, but not print knowledge. The lack (2004) results demonstrating how exposing children to inten-
of evidence of improvement on print knowledge likely relates to tional writing opportunities can increase children’s early writing
limited teacher practices to support this skill. Although research skills in comparison to children receiving book reading or typical
documents the importance of practices that draw attention to print instruction. Aram and Biron (2004) documented positive impacts
during storybook reading (e.g., pointing to the story text to demon- from their small group instruction intervention on children’s word
strate how print works; Justice et al., 2009; McGinty et al., 2011), writing and pre-reading skills (i.e., phonological awareness, letter
teachers in this study were more likely to spend time on letter knowledge and orthographic awareness skills). An important dis-
knowledge skills and less likely to focus on how print works. For tinction between our study and Aram and Biron’s (2004) is that their
example, teachers in this study were much more likely to talk about intervention used a university research assistant and small group
the names of letters and their shape than to draw attention to how instruction (i.e., four to six children), while we targeted an exist-
print communicates a messages or moves from left to right. In ing classroom whole group routine where teachers could imbed
addition, an examination of children’s performance on the print writing opportunities. Differences in intervention approach, mea-
awareness assessment illustrates some possible challenges with surement of children’s early reading and writing skills, and the
the measure. Children’s performance suggests a potential ceiling intensity of the intervention (their intervention was approximately
effect on the print knowledge assessment as many children appear twice as long) likely lead to our finding fewer early reading out-
to have mastered basic print knowledge skills, such as understand- comes from our study. Given the relatively limited research on
ing that words rather than pictures tell a story and how one reads approaches to supporting children’s early writing development in
from left to right, by the end of the PreK year. Importantly, given early childhood classrooms, information from both studies demon-
existing literature about the reciprocal relations between children’s strates how including explicit, yet meaningful, opportunities to
early reading and writing processes (e.g., Bloodgood, 1999; Clay, interact with print can build children’s emergent writing skills.
2001; Diamond et al., 2008; Morris et al., 2003; Puranik & Lonigan,
2011), exposure to early writing experiences should contribute 4.3. Feasibility of HLTP model
to children’s understanding of print, letter knowledge, and letter-
sound awareness indirectly through children’s improved writing The HLTP model greatly relies on the researcher-teacher
skills. partnership and collaboration due to its practice-based nature.
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 149

Observation of teachers’ existing instructional practices and class- us to provide stronger evidence for generalizing the findings to a
room routines enables researchers to understand and identify larger population.
specific areas of practice that will be the most malleable to change
(Zhang and Cook, 2019). Focusing on a few core teaching practices
5.1. Future research
may accommodate the challenge that many teachers have in early
care settings to find adequate time and staffing for professional
Although promising, some issues should be addressed in future
learning opportunities (Whitebrook & Sakai, 2003). In addition, it
writing intervention work to enhance intervention effects. First,
may help work around the fact that teachers have little time for
given variability in some teaching practices encountered in this
planning or implementing new learning activities (Powell, 2013).
intervention study, additional attention should be given to ways
However, because this model involves individualized support and
that teachers might use various scaffolding strategies that are
collaborative problem solving, a high researcher to teacher ratio
attuned to children’s developing early writing and reading skills.
may be necessary to ensure successful implementation of this PD
For example, teachers should model and support writing letters
model. In this study, the researcher to teacher ratio was roughly one
with attention to the letter name and sound, particularly if children
researcher to six teachers. Given the needs of frequent classroom
have limited letter sound awareness knowledge. Including more
observations and on-site coaching, more professional mentors are
information about which letters and sounds are acquired more eas-
needed when working with a larger group of teachers.
ily (see Justice et al., 2006) would help teachers plan more effective
This HLTP intervention, as illustrated by the WPI model, does
instruction for children in their class. In this way, children could be
not aim to change existing classroom dynamics by extending the
better supported in their letter knowledge skills while being chal-
amount of time children sit on the carpet or at the table for learning.
lenged to consider letter-sound associations (Cabell et al., 2013).
In contrast, this approach integrates teachers’ existing knowledge
These types of supports will likely benefit children’s early reading
and teaching preference into PD and promotes the evidence-based
and writing skills, particularly their invented spelling skills that
core practices beneficial to both teachers (i.e., personal profes-
showed little improvement in the current study.
sional development) and children (i.e., growth in early skills). Thus,
Second, future research is needed to examine ways in which
implementation of the HLTP intervention should consider teach-
to increase children’s interest and motivation for writing activ-
ers’ existing teaching responsibilities and children’s developmental
ities outside of MMT. Although intervention teachers suggested
stages. Further, sufficient time should be provided to teachers to
that children were more motivated to write and interested in writ-
help them implement practices. Asking teachers to implement mul-
ing activities after the WPI intervention, classroom observations
tiple interventions targeting different developmental skills within
revealed little change in the prevalence or nature of children’s
a short period of time may create challenges for teachers to suc-
individual writing attempts in other classroom contexts (i.e., free
cessfully learn and practice new strategies.
choice/center time). Given research documenting that even very
young children demonstrate a unique interest in writing experi-
ences (Rowe & Neitzel, 2010), attention to how children’s interest
5. Limitations
or motivation in writing may be impacted by changes in teachers’
writing practices is an important area of future research. Attend-
As an initial investigation of the WPI approach, we utilized a
ing to ways in which teachers might embed additional writing
quasi-experimental design, which limits our ability to causally infer
opportunities in other classroom experiences is important to their
about program impacts (Shadish & Luellen, 2005). To explore the
emergent writing skills (Zhang & Quinn, 2018).
feasibility and effectiveness of a PD model with a new theoreti-
Finally, future research should investigate differentiated sup-
cal framework (i.e., HLTP), we only implemented the intervention
port that teachers may need in order to implement HLTP writing
in four childcare centers. While this study included teachers and
practices within their classrooms. Although there was little vari-
children from diverse background, the small sample size of teach-
ation in teachers’ educational backgrounds in our sample, it is
ers and their placement in certain early childhood sites (i.e., open
possible that teachers with varying educational backgrounds and
concept floor plans) made it difficult to conduct a randomized
expertise may differ in their need for more intensive support in
comparison trial (RCT) intervention study. Despite attempts to
order to identify and then implement HLTP practices effectively.
maximize the equality of intervention and comparison condi-
Because the HLTP framework we employed allowed teachers to
tions in relation to study participants, and a random assignment
tailor instructional practices to existing routines, variation in teach-
of an open floor concept center into treatment and comparison
ers’ experiences, beliefs about children’s writing development, and
conditions, children in intervention and comparison conditions sig-
educational backgrounds may have played a role in how they
nificantly differed at the onset of this study on name writing and
implemented instruction. Additional attention should be given in
print knowledge skills. Although we applied regression adjustment
future research to the level of support that teachers require to
(i.e., residual of children’s growth in writing and literacy skills)
implement HLTP writing practices in a manner that is both faithful
in data analyses, the pre-intervention group differences limit our
to the intervention and that evidences strong instructional quali-
ability to draw causal inferences. Center level differences in chil-
ties.
dren’s skills at the beginning of this study may have influenced
classroom dynamics and the results of our study, including teach-
ers’ implementation of certain WPI instructional strategies (i.e., 6. Conclusion
letter-sound instruction). Due to these limitations, caution should
be taken when interpreting these results and generalizing to other The WPI writing intervention model is one of few PD inter-
settings. A larger sample size and RCT design would allow for a ventions focusing on preschool teachers’ writing instruction. By
stronger evaluation of relations among specific types of teach- embedding writing instruction in a typically occurring classroom
ers’ writing instruction and children’s writing and literacy skills. context (Morning Meeting), and providing a workshop with a spe-
A larger scale study would also allow us to further investigate cific focus on teaching, the WPI intervention successfully promoted
teacher- or child-level factors that may have impacted or mod- teachers’ writing instruction and children’s name writing, letter
erated results. In addition, because children in this study were writing, and letter naming. Although a small-scale study, results
primarily from lower-income communities in an urban area, addi- demonstrate the promise of utilizing Morning Meeting time for the
tional research in more diverse early learning settings may enable explicit teaching of early writing and reading skills.
150 C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151

Acknowledgements Diamond, K. E., Gerde, H. K., & Powell, D. R. (2008). Development in early literacy
skills during the pre-kindergarten year in head start: Relations between
growth in children’s writing and understanding of letters. Early Childhood
This research project is supported by National Academy of Edu- Research Quarterly, 23, 467–478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.05.
cation/Spencer Foundation Post-doctoral Fellowship, 2016. 002
Diamond, K. E., Justice, L. M., Siegler, R. S., & Snyder, P. A. (2013). Synthesis of IES
research on early intervention and early childhood education. (NCSER 2013–3001)
Appendix A. This report is available on the IES website at http://ies.ed.gov/. Washington,
DC: National Center for Special Education Research, Institute of Education
Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Intervention Fidelity Check-List Dickinson, D. K., & Caswell, L. (2007). Building support for language and early
Writing routine activities: literacy in preschool classrooms through in-service professional development:
Effects of the literacy environment enrichment program. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 22, 243–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.03.
1. Did the teacher use the whiteboard to complete daily tasks? 001
2. Did children use whiteboards to complete daily tasks? Dickinson, D. K., Freiberg, J. B., & Barnes, E. M. (2011). Why are so few interventions
really effective? A call for fine-grained research methodology. In S. B. Neuman,
3. Were writing interactions routinized during the morning meet- & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 3) (pp.
ing? 337–357). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Writing instruction: Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D.
(2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The
4. Did the teacher model the writing process for children?
interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print
5. Did the teacher explain letter formation when writing or knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
prompting children to write? 95, 465–481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.465
Dickinson, D. K., Hofer, K. G., Barnes, E. M., & Grifenhagen, J. B. (2014). Examining
6. Did the teacher explain letter names when writing or prompting
teachers’ language in head start classrooms from a systemic linguistics
children to write? approach. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 231–244. http://dx.doi.org/
7. Did the teacher explain letter sounds/phonological awareness 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.006
when writing or prompting children to write? Early, D. M., Iruka, I. U., Ritchie, S., Barbarin, O. A., et al. (2010). How do
pre-kindergarteners spend their time? Gender, ethnicity, and income as
8. Did the teacher encourage children to talk about or share their predictors of experiences in prekindergarten classrooms. Early Childhood
writing? Research Quarterly, 25, 177–193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.10.
003
Georgia Department of Early Care and Learning. (2010). Georgia early learning and
Note: Each item is rated as absent (0) or present (1) development standards Retrieved from. http://www.gelds.decal.ga.gov/search.
aspx
Gerde, H. K., Bingham, G. E., & Pendergast, M. L. (2015). Reliability and validity of
References the writing resources and interactions in teaching environments (WRITE) for
preschool classrooms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 31, 34–46. http://dx.
Aram, D. (2010). Writing with young children: A comparison of parental and doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.12.008
maternal guidance. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 4–19. http://dx.doi.org/ Gerde, H., Bingham, G., & Wasik, B. (2012). Writing in early childhood classrooms:
10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01429.x Guidance for best practices. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40, 351–359.
Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10643-012-0531-z
among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Gest, S. D., Holland-Coviello, R., Welsh, J. A., Eicher-Catt, D. L., & Gill, S. (2006).
Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 588–610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq. Language development subcontexts in Head Start classrooms: Distinctive
2004.10.003 patterns of teacher talk during free play, mealtime, and book reading. Early
Ball, D. L., Sleep, L., Boerst, T. A., & Bass, H. (2009). Combining the development of Education and Development, 17, 293–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
practice and the practice of development in teacher education. The Elementary s15566935eed1702 5
School Journal, 109, 458–474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/596996 Guo, Y., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., & McGinty, A. (2012). The literacy
Bingham, G. E., Culatta, B., & Hall-Kenyon, K. M. (2016). Examining the impacts of environment of preschool classrooms: Contributions to children’s emergent
systematic and engaging early literacy (SEEL): Attention to teacher practices literacy growth. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 308–327. http://dx.doi.org/
and classroom effects across kindergarten. Journal of Research in Childhood 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01467.x
Education, 30, 494–512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2016.1216021 Grossman, P., Hammerness, K., & McDonald, M. (2009). Redefining teaching,
Bingham, G. E., Quinn, M., & Gerde, H. K. (2017). Examining early childhood re-imagining teacher education. Teachers and Teaching, 15, 273–289. http://dx.
teachers’ writing practices: Associations between pedagogical supports and doi.org/10.1080/13540600902875340
children’s writing skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 39, 35–46. http:// Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.01.002 Children, 70, 453–468. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001440290407000405
Bloodgood, W. J. (1999). What’s in a name? Children’s name writing and literacy Hindman, A. H., & Wasik, B. A. (2012). Morning message time: An exploratory
acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 342–367. study in head start. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40, 275–283. http://dx.
Bustamante, A. S., Hindman, A. H., Champagne, C. R., & Wasik, B. A. (2018). Circle doi.org/10.1007/s10643-011-0459-8
time revisited: How do preschool classrooms use this part of the day? The Hlas, A., & Hlas, C. (2012). A review of high-leverage teaching practices: Making
Elementary School Journal, 118, 610–631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/697473 connections between mathematics and foreign language. Foreign Language
Buysse, V., Castro, D. C., & Peisner-Feinberg, E. (2010). Effects of a professional Annual, 45, 76–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2012.01180.x
development program on classroom practices and outcomes for latino dual Ilgaz, H., Hassinger-Das, B., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Language for
language learners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 94–206. http://dx. reading. In P. Brooks, V. Kempe, & G. J. Golson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language
doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.10.001 development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cabell, S. Q., Tortorelli, L. S., & Gerde, H. K. (2013). How do I write . . .? Scaffolding Invernizzi, M., & Hayes, L. (2004). Developmental-spelling research: A systematic
preschoolers’ early writing skills. The Reading Teacher, 66, 650–659. http://dx. imperative. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 216–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.
doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1173 1598/RRQ.39.2.4
Clay, M. M. (1975). What did I write? Beginning writing behaviour. Auckland, NZ: Invernizzi, M., Sullivan, A., Meier, J., & Swank, L. (2004). Phonological awareness
Heinemann. literacy screening for prekindergarten (PALS-Pre-K). Charlottesville, VA:
Clay, M. M. (1985). The early detection of reading difficulties. Portsmouth, NH: University of Virginia.
Heinemann. Jackson, R., McCoy, A., Pistorino, C., Wilkinson, A., Burghardt, J., Clark, M., et al.
Clay, M. M. (2001). Change over time in children’s literacy development. Portsmouth, (2007). National evaluation of early reading first: Final report. Washington, DC:
NH: Heinemann. U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences; U. S.
Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2009). Developmentally appropriate practice in early Government Printing Office.
childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8 (3rd ed.). Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. (2009). Accelerating
Washington, DC: NAEYC. preschoolers’ early literacy development through classroom-based
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change” or should we. teacher–child storybook reading and explicit print referencing. Language,
Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–80. Speech, and Hearing Service in Schools, 40, 67–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/
DeBaryshe, B., Buell, M., & Binder, J. (1996). What a parent brings to the table: 0161-1461(2008/07-0098)
Young children writing with and without parental assistance. Journal of Justice, L. M., Pence, K., Bowles, R. B., Ryan, B., & Wiggins, A. (2006). An
Literacy Research, 28, 71–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862969609547911 investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old
Dennis, L. R., & Votteler, N. K. (2013). Preschool teachers and children’s emergent children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21,
writing: Supporting diverse learners. Early Childhood Education Journal, 41, 374–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.07.010
439–446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10643-012-0563-4
C. Zhang, G.E. Bingham / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49 (2019) 138–151 151

Justice, M. L., Mashburn, J. A., Hamre, B., & Pianta, C. R. (2008). Quality of language Puranik, C. S., Lonigan, C. J., & Kim, Y.-S. (2011). Contributions of emergent literacy
and literacy instruction in preschool classrooms serving low-income pupils. skills to name writing, letter writing and spelling in preschool children. Early
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 51–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 465–474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.
ecresq.2007.09.004 2011.03.002
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading Puranik, C. S., Schreiber, M., Estabrook, E., & O’Donnell, E. (2014). Comparison of
comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of name-writing rubrics: Is there a gold standard? Assessment for Effective
oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, Intervention, 40, 16–23.
765–778. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015956 Reid, D. K., Hresko, W. P., & Hammill, D. D. (2001). Test of early reading ability. Ann
Landry, S. H., Anthony, J. L., Swank, P. R., & Monseque-Bailey, P. (2009). Arbor, MI: Academic Therapy Publication.
Effectiveness of comprehensive professional development for teachers of Rieben, L., Ntamakiliro, L., Gonthier, B., & Fayol, M. (2005). Effects of various early
at-risk preschoolers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 448–465. http:// writing practices on reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9,
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013842 145–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902 3
Landry, S. H., Anthony, J. L., Swank, P. R., & Assel, M. (2011). An experimental study Roskos, K. A., Tabors, P. O., & Lenhart, L. A. (2009). Oral language and early literacy in
evaluating professional development activities within a state funded preschool. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
pre-kindergarten program. Reading and Writing, 24, 970–1010. http://dx.doi. Rowe, D. W. (2008). Social contracts for writing: Negotiating shared
org/10.1007/s11145-010-9243-1 understandings about text in the preschool years. Reading Research Quarterly,
Levin, I., & Aram, D. (2010). Mother-child joint writing and storybook reading and 43, 66–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.43.1.5
their effects on kindergartners’ literacy: An intervention study. Reading and Rowe, D., & Neitzel, C. (2010). Interest and agency in 2- and 3-year-olds’
Writing, 25, 217–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9254-y participation in emergent writing. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 169–195.
Lonigan, C. J., Farver, J. M., Phillips, B. M., & Clancy-Menchetti, J. (2011). Promoting http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.2.2
the development of preschool children’s emergent literacy skills: A Sawilowsky, S. S. (2009). New effect size rules of thumb. Journal of Modern Applied
randomized evaluation of a literacy-focused curriculum and two professional Statistical Methods, 8(2), 597–599. http://dx.doi.org/10.22237/jmasm/
development models. Reading and Writing, 24, 305–337. http://dx.doi.org/10. 1257035100
1007/s11145-009-9214-6 Shadish, W. R., & Luellen, J. K. (2005). Quasi-experimental design. In J. L. Green, G.
Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2007). Test of preschool Camilli, P. B. Elmore, A. Skukauskaiti, & E. Grace (Eds.), Handbook of
early literacy (TOPEL). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Complementary Methods in Education Research (pp. 539–550). The American
Molfese, V., Beswick, J., Jacobi-Vessels, J., Armstrong, N., Culver, B., White, J., et al. Educational Research Association. U.S. Department of Health and Human
(2011). Evidence of alphabetic knowledge in writing: Connections to letter and Services (2008). Head Start program performance standards. Retrieved from
word identification skills in preschool and kindergarten. Reading and Writing, https://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/policy/45-cfr-chap-xiii
24, 133–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9265-8 Shatil, E., Share, D. C., & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten
Molfese, V. J., Beswick, J., Molnar, A., & Jacobi-Vessels, J. (2006). Alphabetic skills in writing to grade one literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied
preschool: A preliminary study of letter naming and letter writing. Psycholinguisics, 21, 1–21.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 5–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ Smith, M. W., Brady, J. P., & Anastasopoulos, L. (2008). Early language and literacy
s15326942dn2901 2 classroom observation Pre-K tool. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Morris, D., Bloodgood, J. W., Lomax, R. G., & Perney, J. (2003). Developmental steps Snyder, P. A., Rakap, S., Hemmeter, M. L., McLaughlin, T. W., Sandall, S., & McLean,
in learning to read: A longitudinal study in kindergarten and first grade. M. E. (2015). Naturalistic instructional approaches in early learning: A
Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 302–328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.38.3.1 systematic review. Journal of Early Intervention, 37, 69–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the 1177/1053815115595461
national early literacy panel. Washington, D.C: National Institute for Literacy. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In M. Gauvain,
Neuman, S. B., Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2000). Learning to read and write: & M. Cole (Eds.), Mind and society (pp. 79–91). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Developmentally appropriate practices for young children. Washington, DC: University Press.
National Association for the Education of Young Children. Wasik, B. A., & Hindman, A. H. (2011). The Morning Message in early childhood
Neuman, S. B., & Dwyer, J. (2009). Missing in action: Vocabulary instruction in classrooms: Guidelines for best practices. Early Childhood Education Journal,
Pre-K. The Reading Teacher, 62, 384–392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RT.62.5.2 39(183-), 189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10643-011-0463-z
O’Leary, P. M., Cockburn, M. K., Powell, D. R., & Diamond, K. E. (2010). Head Start Webb, M. L., Patton-Terry, N., Bingham, G. E., Puranik, C. S., & Lederberg, A. R.
teachers’ views of phonological awareness and vocabulary knowledge (2018). Factorial validity and measurement invariance of the test of preschool
instruction. Early Childhood Education Journal, 38, 187–195. http://dx.doi.org/ early literacy-phonological awareness test among deaf and hard-of-hearing
10.1007/s10643-010-0394-0 children and hearing children. Ear and Hearing, 39, 278–292. http://dx.doi.org/
Ouellette, G., Sénéchal, M., & Haley, A. (2013). Guiding children’s invented 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000485
spellings: A gateway into literacy learning. Journal of Experimental Education, Welsch, J. G., Sullivan, A., & Justice, L. M. (2003). That’s my letter! What
81(2), 261–279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2012.699903 preschoolers’ names writing representation tell us about emergent literacy
Piasta, S. B., Purpura, D. J., & Wagner, R. K. (2010). Fostering alphabet knowledge knowledge. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 757–776. http://dx.doi.org/10.
development: A comparison of two instructional approaches. Reading and 1207/s15548430jlr3502 4
Writing, 23, 607–626. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9174-x Whitebrook, M., & Sakai, L. (2003). Turnover begets turnover: An examination of
Pollo, T. C., Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2009). Statistical patterns in children’s early job and occupational instability among child care center staff. Early Childhood
writing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 410–426. http://dx.doi. Research Quarterly, 18, 273–293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-
org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.003 2006(03)00040-1
Powell, D. R. (2013). The Head Start program. In J. Roopnarine, & J. Johnson (Eds.), Zhang, C., & Claire Cook, J. (2019). A reflective professional development
Approaches to early childhood education (6th ed, Vol. 3, pp. 61–78). Columbus, intervention model of early writing instruction. Journal of Early Childhood
OH: Merrill/Pearson. Teacher Education, 40, 117–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10901027.2018.
Powell, D., & Diamond, K. (2011). Improving outcomes of coaching-based 1536903
professional development interventions. In S. B. Neuman, & D. K. Dickinson Zhang, C., Diamond, K., & Powell, D. (2015). Examining the content of Head Start
(Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 3) (pp. 295–307). New York: teachers’ literacy instruction within two activity contexts during large-group
Guilford. circle time. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 29, 323–337. http://dx.
Powell, D. R., Diamond, K. E., Burchinal, M. R., & Koehler, M. J. (2010). Effects of an doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2015.1042124
early literacy professional development intervention on Head Start teachers Zhang, C., Diamond, K., & Powell, D. (2017). Do children learn letter knowledge
and children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 299–312. http://dx.doi. from name-writing: Examining the relation between children’s writing skills
org/10.1037/a0017763 and letter knowledge. Early Child Development and Care, http://dx.doi.org/10.
Puranik, C. S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). From scribbles to scrabble: Preschool 1080/03004430.2017.1343311
children’s developing knowledge of written language. Reading and Writing, Zhang, C., & Quinn, M. (2018). Promoting early writing skills through Morning
24(5), 567–589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9220-8 meeting Routines: Guidelines for best practices. Early Childhood Education
Puranik, C. S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Emergent writing in preschoolers: Preliminary Journal, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10643-017-0886-2
evidence for a theoretical framework. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 453–467.

S-ar putea să vă placă și