Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, 409–420

doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw073
Advance Access Publication Date: 10 May 2016
Original article

Neurocognitive Development and Mental Health

Parental age and attention-deficit/hyperactivity


disorder (ADHD)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


Susanne Hvolgaard Mikkelsen,1,* Jørn Olsen,2,3 Bodil Hammer Bech4
and Carsten Obel1,5
1
Department of Public Health, Section for General Medical Practice, 2Department of Clinical Medicine,
Clinical Epidemiolgy, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark, 3Department of Epidemiology, Fielding
School of Public Health, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA, 4Department of Public Health,
Section for Epidemiology and 5Center of Collaborative Health, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
*Corresponding author. Susanne Hvolgaard Mikkelsen, Ph.d. Student, MHSc., RN, Institute of public health, Department of gen-
eral medical practice, Aarhus University, Bartholins Allé 2, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark. Email:susanne.hvolgaard.mikkelsen@ph.au.dk
Accepted 16 March 2016

Abstract
Background: Previous studies have suggested that young mothers more often have chil-
dren with ADHD. We used sibling comparisons to examine the nature of this association
and to investigate if this association is explained by early environment or genetic and
socioeconomic factors.
Methods: A large population-based cohort including all singletons born in Denmark
from 1 January 1991 through 31 December 2005 was followed from birth until 30 April
2011. Data were available for 94% (N ¼ 943 785) of the population. Offspring ADHD was
identified by an ICD-10 diagnosis of Hyperkinetic Disorder (HKD). We used sibling-
matched Cox regression to control for genetic and socioeconomic factors.
Results: In the population cohort we found that children born by parents aged 20 years
or younger had more than twice the risk of being diagnosed with ADHD compared with
children with parents between 26 and 30 years of age. When comparing full siblings the
associations were attenuated, but we found a trend of increased risk of ADHD with
decreasing maternal age, which was not seen for paternal age.
Conclusions: Sibling comparisons suggested that the associations between both mater-
nal and paternal age and ADHD are partly explained by common genetic and socioeco-
nomic factors. The trend of increased risk of ADHD with decreasing maternal age, but
not with paternal age, may be linked to pregnancy or early-life environmental factors.
Even though only a smaller part of the association can be attributed to environmental
factors, there is a public health interest to support young parents through their first years
of parenthood.

Key words: Maternal age, paternal age, parental age, hyperkinetic disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder, sibling design

C The Author 2016; all rights reserved. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Epidemiological Association
V 409
410 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

Key messages

• Children diagnosed with ADHD were more likely to have younger parents than children without ADHD.
• The highest risk of ADHD was found when both parents were very young.
• Sibling comparisons suggested that the association between both young maternal and paternal ages and ADHD

mainly can be accounted for by genetic and socioeconomic factors.


• In the area of public health, focus on delaying fertility without ameliorating the background risks experienced by

young parents and their children may not be fully effective in reducing offspring ADHD. Public policy initiatives
should be targeted at attention to young parents and individual support of at-risk parents and their children.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


Introduction in some, but not all, studies.18 The probably most robust
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of associations so far are the links between intrauterine
the most common psychiatric disorders in childhood.1,2 growth retardation and prematurity and subsequent
During recent decades a trend towards an increase in ADHD in the offspring.18.21,22 Another consistently found
ADHD diagnosis has been observed,36 with a current esti- association is that children with ADHD have younger par-
mated prevalence of 3–7% among children and adoles- ents compared with children without ADHD,23–28 but sur-
cents.1,7,8 This trend may to some extent be related to prisingly few studies have aimed to explore the nature of
changes in diagnostic criteria, earlier age at diagnosis, this association by including both parents in their
increased clinical awareness and media attention.1,4,9–11 analyses.27
There is robust evidence from a wide range of study de- Although the association has not been studied in depth,
signs that the development of ADHD has a strong genetic a number of biological hypotheses have been suggested. An
component.12 First-degree relatives of those with ADHD imbalance between the competing nutritional needs of the
are two to eight times more likely than relatives of un- mother and child may play a role; and, further, children
affected individuals to also show ADHD symptoms.13 of young mothers are exposed to different amounts of sex-
Twin studies have found higher concordance rates for related hormones compared with children of older
ADHD among monozygotic twin than dizygotic twin mothers.29,30 A likely alternative explanation is that the as-
pairs, with a heritability estimate around 80%.13,14 sociation simply reflects genetic and socioeconomic factors
However, heritability estimates include not only genetic in- accompanying young parenthood.31–36 A recent Swedish
fluences, but also the effects of gene-environment inter- study using sibling and cousin comparisons to partly con-
action. Genetic predisposition in parents may lead to trol for unmeasured genetic and environmental confound-
inherited predisposition in children with expression trig- ing, supported that the association is mainly explained by
gered by environmental factors.15. genetic factors contributing to both young maternal age at
ADHD constitutes a major social and economic chal- childbirth and ADHD in offspring.37 If this is the case, a
lenge, and identification of potentially preventable envir- mother may confer risk to her children regardless of
onmental factors is of major public health interest. whether she gives birth as a teenager or later in life, which
Environmental exposures in utero as well as in early life has been supported by studies finding that maternal age at
have been shown to be important for lifetime health, but to first birth was more predictive of later-born children’s psy-
date only few plausible environmental causes have been chiatric problems than maternal age at the child’s own
identified for the development of ADHD, leaving limited birth.35,37,38 Maternal age at her first birth could be seen
room for prevention. as a marker of genetic and social factors related to ADHD
There is an increasing focus on potential perinatal fac- in young mothers.
tors related to mental health in offspring.16,17 A systematic The crucial question is whether this higher risk in off-
review of pre- and perinatal risk factors for ADHD impli- spring of younger mothers constitutes a potential for pre-
cated exposure to tobacco smoke in utero as a suspected vention by delaying fertility, or whether focus should be on
risk factor18 although recent research questions this con- supportive interventions in young parents to improve the
clusion.19,20 Also, exposure to other substances during upbringing environment and thereby reduce symptoms in
pregnancy has been linked to ADHD. Moderate maternal offspring with a genetic predisposition.
alcohol use during pregnancy and exposure to illicit sub- If the association between young maternal age and off-
stances have been associated with increased risk of ADHD spring ADHD is caused by familial factors, a similar
International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2 411

association is to be expected between young paternal age Registry (DMBR) and the Danish Psychiatric Central
and ADHD and, further, an additional risk is to be ex- Research Register (DPCR) with psychiatric outcome.
pected when both parents are very young. Few studies We defined a cohort study with follow-up of singletons
have reported on associations between paternal age and born in Denmark from 1 January 1991 to 31 December
ADHD, and results have been conflicting.23,27,39,40 A study 2005. All births by women with permanent residence in
from Sweden reported advanced paternal age to be associ- Denmark (N ¼ 999 713) were identified in the Danish
ated with an increased risk of ADHD.40 In contrast, an- Medical Birth Registry, DMBR. We excluded multiple
other study from Israel found that the paternal age births (n ¼ 37 406) and, after linkage with the DCPR, we
distribution among children with ADHD was similar to excluded additionally 18 522 children with insufficient or
what we see in the general population.39 missing data for the biological father; this left 943 785
We studied offspring ADHD according to maternal as children in the cohort (Figure 1). The study was approved

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


well as paternal age, and combined parental age in a large by the Danish Health Authority and the Danish Data
population-based cohort. We did comparisons of risk be- Protection Agency.
tween full and half siblings to control for unmeasured gen- Maternal and paternal ages at birth were calculated
etic and socioeconomic factors and other shared family based on the civil registration numbers of mothers, fathers
characteristics. and children, and was then divided into five age groups
ranging from 20 years or younger to older than 35 years,
in 5-year intervals.
Methods
Setting Outcome definition
All citizens in Denmark are given a unique civil registration We defined offspring ADHD as children with an ICD-10
number at birth or immigration, by the Danish Central diagnosis of Hyperkinetic Disorder (F90x) after the age of
Population Registry (DCPR). This registry includes infor- 5 years, identified in the DPCR. The ICD-10 classification
mation on date of birth and biological relationships for all system has been used since 1994. The DPCR provides indi-
citizens in Denmark since 1968. Using the civil registration vidual-level data on clinical diagnoses and admission dates
number we linked data from the Danish Medical Birth for all inpatient psychiatric hospital admissions since 1969

Figure 1. Flowchart.
412 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

and outpatient visits at psychiatric hospitals and clinics splines with five knots, which are the default knot values
since 1995. based on Harrell’s recommended percentiles.41 The adjusted
HR was in addition adjusted for parity, maternal smoking
during pregnancy and gender. As maternal and paternal
Statistical analyses
ages are highly correlated, we adjusted for the other parent’s
Maternal and paternal ages were studied separately. We age by including the age difference between parents in the
used maternal or paternal age between 26 and 30 years as model; this covered both the parental age difference and the
the reference group. In a subanalysis, maternal and pater- age of the other parent as a confounder.43
nal ages were further divided into three age groups— < 26, Gestational age and birthweight are assumed to be po-
26–30, > 30 years—to study parental age combining both tential intermediate factors and were therefore not ad-
maternal and paternal age. We used maternal and paternal justed for in the final models,44 but in subanalyses we

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


ages > 30 years as the reference group. Finally, to examine examined if the association between maternal age and
the full childbearing age range, maternal and paternal ages ADHD was modified by gestational age or birthweight.
were studied as continuous variables using cubic splines.41 Further, we examined whether associations of interest
Measured covariates included: gender (girls, boys); parity were modified by parity, gender or birth year.
(1, > 1); maternal smoking during pregnancy (yes, no); and
birth-year groups (1-year intervals).
We examined the relative risk (RR) of being diagnosed Sibling-matched analyses
with ADHD associated with the exposure variable of inter- Two sibling subcohorts were identified as children born in
est, using Cox regression. Cox regression estimates hazard the time period 1 January 1991 to 31 December 2005. One
rate ratios comparing the different exposure groups, included full siblings; the other included half siblings with
assuming the hazard ratio remains constant over time. We the same biological mother. Full sibling comparisons pro-
evaluated the proportional hazard assumption by graphical vide stronger adjustment for shared genetic and time-stable
assessment of log-log plots, and the assumption was ac- environmental factors compared with half sibling compari-
ceptable. Results are reported as hazard ratios (HR) with sons. Only families with at least two siblings, of which at
95% confidence intervals (CIs). The Cox regression least one child was diagnosed with ADHD, were included,
allowed us to control for entry at different time points, corresponding to 6436 families in the full sibling cohort
varying length of follow-up and changing rates of ADHD and 1795 families in the half sibling cohort.
over time. Since ADHD is a rare outcome resulting in low The sibling-matched design extends the Cox regression
hazard rates, the relative HR will approximate RR and model by stratifying on the set of either full or half siblings.
hazard ratios can be interpreted in terms of additional cu- In the stratified Cox regression model, each family has its
mulative risk. Each child contributed with risk time begin- own baseline rate function reflecting the family’s shared
ning at the age of 5 years old and ending at diagnosis, genetic and environmental factors and thus adjustment for
immigration, death or end of follow-up on 30 April 2011, all factors that are shared within each sibling set.
whichever came first. To control for the lack of independ- Additionally, the sibling comparisons allow us to control
ence of children within the same family, we used robust for the age difference between parents among full siblings
variance estimation in the population cohort analyses. We
and maternal age at birth of first child among both full and
did regular cohort analyses and then compared results with
half siblings. Only families with at least two siblings dis-
both full and half sibling-matched analyses. All statistical
cordant for parental age group contributed with informa-
analyses were performed with Stata/SE 12.
tion on the association between parental age and ADHD.
The adjusted sibling-matched analyses were controlled for
Population cohort analyses the same covariates as in the population cohort and, in
addition, adjustment for the increasing prevalence of
To adjust for changing diagnostic criteria, differences in as-
ADHD was achieved by including calendar time in a cubic
sessment methods over time, changing age at first child-
spline with five knots.41
birth and the changing prevalence of ADHD during the
study period, we adjusted for calendar time by including
separate baseline diagnostic rates (strata) for each birth Results
year.1,10,42
We estimated crude and adjusted HR within the popula- Population cohort
tion cohort. Maternal and paternal age were studied cat- In the population cohort, 1.30% of the children had a hos-
egorically and as continuous variables expressed as cubic pital diagnosis of ADHD (1.99% boys and 0.58% girls,
International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2 413

respectively) (Table 1). We found that both decreasing ma- children with mothers aged 29 years or fathers aged 32
ternal and paternal ages were associated with higher risk of years, a higher risk of ADHD was found with decreasing
being diagnosed with ADHD; the associations were margin- maternal or paternal age (Figures 2 and 3) For children
ally attenuated after adjustment for the measured covariates. with an older father (> 30 years), the younger the mother
Children with mothers or fathers 20 years of age or younger the higher risk of ADHD. The risk increased by 25%
had more than twice the risk of ADHD (HR ¼ 2.24, 95% CI (HR ¼ 1.25, 95% CI 1.18–1.33) among children born to
2.07–2.42 and HR ¼ 2.28, 95% CI 2.03–2.57, respectively) mothers aged 26–30 years and by 98% (HR ¼ 1.98, 95%
(Table 2) compared with children with parents between 26 CI 1.81-2.15) among children born to mothers aged below
and 30 years of age. The associations between the catego- 26 years (Table 3). Among children born to mothers older
rized maternal as well as paternal age and ADHD followed a than 30 years, the risk of ADHD was also affected by pa-
‘dose-response’ relation with increasing risk of ADHD with ternal age (Table 3). The combination of both a young

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


decreasing parental age (test for trend P-value < 0.001). mother and a young father resulted in the highest risk for
Additionally, we found almost similar associations ADHD (HR ¼ 2.31, 95% CI 2.16–2.46). In the population
when maternal and paternal ages were studied as continu- cohort, we did not find interaction by gestational age,
ous variables expressed as cubic splines. Compared with birthweight, gender, parity or birth year.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the population cohort

Variable All births No. (%) ADHD-diagnose Maternal age, mean Paternal age, mean
No. (%) (95% CI) (95% CI)

Total no. (%) 943 785 (100) 12 294 (1.30) 29.43 (29.42–29.44) 32.32 (32.31–32.34)
Gender no. (%)
Female 458 749 (48.61) 2647 (0.58) 29.63 (29.61–29.64) 32.32 (32.30–32.34)
Male 485 036 (51.39) 9647 (1.99) 29.64 (29.62–29.65) 32.33 (32.31–32.34)
Birthweight, no. (%), grams
 2000 13 224 (1.40) 281 (2.12) 29.74 (29.65–29.83) 32.50 (32.39–32.61)
2001–2500 23 953 (2.54) 467 (1.95) 29.36 (29.29–29.42) 32.03 (31.95–32.11)
2501–3000 111 746 (11.84) 1723 (1.54) 29.09 (29.06–29.12) 31.91 (31.87–31.94)
3001–3500 307 588 (32.59) 3827 (1.24) 29.35 (29.33–29.37) 32.10 (32.08–32.12)
3501–4000 318 112 (33.71) 3845 (1.21) 29.78 (29.76–29.80) 32.43 (32.41–32.45)
>4000 158 909 (16.84) 2013 (1.27) 30.27 (30.25–30.29) 32.81 (32.78–32.83)
Missing 10 253 (1.09) 138 (1.35) 30.08 (29.99–30.17) 32.99 (32.87–33.10)
Gestational age, no. (%), full weeks
< 32 6581 (0.70) 144 (2.19) 29.86 (29.74–29.99) 32.58 (32.43–32.73)
32–36 38 441 (4.07) 708 (1.84) 29.52 (29.47–29.57) 32.18 (32.12–32.24)
37–40 634 759 (67.26) 8218 (1.29) 29.66 (29.65–29.67) 32.35 (32.34–32.37)
> 40 250 395 (26.53) 3015 (1.20) 29.59 (29–57.29.60) 32.27 (32.25–32.29)
Missing 13 609 (1.44) 209 (1.54) 29.27 (29.19–29.35) 32.22 (32.11–32.32)
Apgarscore 5 min
9 67 232 (7.12) 1070 (1.59) 29.59 (29.56–29.63) 32.16 (32.12–32.21)
10 86 3277 (91.47) 11 031 (1.28) 29.63 (29.62–29.64) 32.33 (32.32–32.34)
Missing 13 276 (1.41) 193 (1.45) 29.70 (29.61–29.78) 32.69 (32.59–32.80)
Parity
1 406 476 (43.07) 5565 (1.37) 27.76 (27.75–27.78) 30.54 (30.53–30.56)
2 342 850 (36.33) 4245 (1.24) 30.22 (30.21–30.23) 32.85(32.84–32.87)
>2 177 717 (18.83) 2255 (1.27) 32.82 (32.80–32.84) 35.40 (35.38–35.43)
Missing 16 742 (1.77) 229 (1.37) 29.10 (29.03–29.18) 32.02 (31.93–32.11)
Smoking during pregnancy
Non smoker 676 117 (71.64) 6661 (0.99) 29.89 (29.88–29.90) 32.56 (32.55–32.57)
Smoker 219 876 (23.30) 4957 (2.25) 28.85 (28.83–28.87) 31.59 (31.56–31.61)
Missing 47 792 (5.06) 676 (1.41) 29.54 (29.50–29.58) 32.37 (32.32–32.42)
Birthyear
1991–95 318 085 (33.70) 4839 (1.52) 28.87 (28.86–28.89) 31.63 (31.61–31.65)
1996–2000 318 521 (33.75) 5025 (1.58) 29.67 (29.65–29.68) 32.36 (32.34–32.38)
2001–2005 307 179 (32.55) 2430 (0.79) 30.38 (30.37–30.40) 33.01 (32.99–33.03)
414 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

Table 2. Population cohort;crude and adjusted associations between maternal and paternal age and ADHD

Population cohort: risk of ADHD, HR (95%CI)

Maternal age Paternal age

Age groups, years Crude HR*1 Adjusted HR*2 Crude HR*1 Adjusted HR*2
No. ¼ 943 785 No. ¼ 883 933 No. ¼ 943 785 No. ¼ 883 933

 20 2.43 (2.27–2.61) 2.24 (2.07–2.42) 2.42 (2.16–2.71) 2.28 (2.03–2.57)


21–25 1.54 (1.47–1.61) 1.49 (1.42–1.56) 1.52 (1.44–1.60) 1.49 (1.40–1.57)
26–30 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.)
31–35 0.83 (0.79–0.88) 0.80 (0.76–0.85) 0.83 (0.79–0.87) 0.78 (0.74–0.82)
> 35 0.84 (0.78–0.90) 0.78 (0.72–0.84) 0.79 (0.75–0.83) 0.61 (0.57–0.65)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


P-value*3 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001

*1Crude estimate stratified by birth year.


*2Adjusted estimate (parity 1/> 1; smoking no/yes; parental age difference; gender), stratified by birth year.
*3Test for trend.

Figure 2. Population cohort; crude and adjusted association between maternal age as a continuous variable expressed as cubic spline and ADHD.

Sibling cohort in the half sibling cohort (P-value 0.21 and 0.56,
In the full sibling cohort we found a weaker ‘dose- respectively).
response’ relation with higher risk of ADHD with decreas- Further, results from the full sibling cohort suggested
ing maternal age (P-value < 0.05). Compared with children that a sibling born when both parents were < 26 years old
born to mothers aged between 26 and 30 years, the risk of had 61% (HR ¼ 1.61, 95% CI 1.13–2.30) higher risk of
ADHD was increased by 28% (HR ¼ 1.28, 95% CI 0.94– ADHD compared with a later-born sibling with both par-
1.73) among children born to mothers aged 20 years or ents being older than 30 years old. Compared with a sib-
younger (Table 4). Maternal age above 30 years remained ling with both parents above 30 years, a sibling born
associated with lower risk of ADHD in the offspring earlier in the parents’ lifespan with only a father above 30
(Table 4). In the half sibling cohort, the same tendency of a years had a 32% (HR ¼ 1.32, 95% CI 1.12–1.56) and a
‘dose-response’ relation was seen(P-value 0.30). We 70% (HR ¼ 1.70, 95% CI 1.26–2.29) higher risk of
found no indication of a ‘dose-response’ relation be- ADHD if the mother was 26–30 years old or younger than
tween paternal age and ADHD either in the full sibling or 26 years, respectively (Table 5). Conversely, in full siblings,
International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2 415

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


Figure 3. Population cohort; crude and adjusted association between paternal age as a continuous variable expressed as cubic spline and ADHD.

Table 3. Population cohort; crude and adjusted combined effect of parental age on risk of ADHD

Population cohort: risk of ADHD, HR (95%CI)

Maternal age, years Paternal age, years

Crude HR*1 No.: 943 785 < 26 26–30 >30 P-value*3


< 26 2.40 (2.27–2.54) 1.83 (1.72–1.95) 1.98 (1.82–2.14)
26–30 1.69 (1.51–1.89) 1.23 (1.17–1.30) 1.21 (1.14–1.28)
> 30 2.02 (1.59–2.57) 1.25 (1.14–1.38) 1.00 (ref.) <0.001
Adjusted HR*2 No.: 883 933
< 26 2.31 (2.16–2.46) 1.85 (1.73–1.97) 1.98 (1.81–2.15)
26–30 1.63 (1.45–1.84) 1.29 (1.22–1.37) 1.25 (1.18–1.33)
> 30 1.76 (1.38–2.26) 1.23 (1.11–1.36) 1.00 (ref.) <0.001

*1Crude estimate stratified by birth year.


*2Adjusted estimate (parity 1/> 1; smoking no/yes; gender), stratified by birth year.
*Test for trend.

born to mothers aged above 30 years, decreasing paternal including comparisons between children with different de-
age was no longer associated with an additional risk for grees of genetic similarity support the hypothesis that
ADHD. (Table 5). part of the population-wide association between young
maternal age as well as young paternal age and offspring
ADHD may be explained by family-related factors. The as-
Discussion sociation may partly be caused by genetic risk factors, as
In the population-based cohort we found that young ma- we observed the weakest association in the full sibling
ternal age as well as young paternal age were associated comparison, which provides a stronger adjustment for
with offspring ADHD, with the highest risk found when shared genetics than the half sibling comparison. Initiating
both parents were younger than 26 years old. In the sibling parenting at a young age may be a proxy for behavioural
comparison, the association between maternal age and genetic risk factors shared among members of the same
ADHD was weaker, with the weakest association observed family and transmitted from parents to their offspring,
in full siblings. We found no association between contributing to young parental age as well as offspring
paternal age and ADHD in full or half siblings. The results ADHD.
416 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

Table 4. Sibling cohorts; crude and adjusted associations between parental age and ADHD

Cohorts: risk of ADHD, HR (95% CI)

Full siblings Half siblings

Age groups, years Crude HR*1 Adjusted HR*2 Crude HR*1 Adjusted HR*2

Maternal age No. ¼ 12 659 No. ¼ 11 853 No. ¼ 3549 No. ¼ 3281
20 1.16 (0.90–1.51) 1.28 (0.94–1.73) 1.33 (0.81–2.20) 1.38 (0.78–2.45)
21–25 1.10 (0.96–1.26) 1.15 (0.98–1.34) 1.16 (0.87–1.55) 1.24 (0.90–1.73)
26–30 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.)
31–35 0.77 (0.67–0.89) 0.73 (0.62–0.85) 0.80 (0.58–1.10) 0.89 (0.62–1.27)
> 35 0.65 (0.49–0.87) 0.61 (0.44–0.85) 0.71 (0.39–1.29) 0.79 (0.41–1.54)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


P-value*3 < 0.05 < 0.05 0.22 0.30
Paternal age No. ¼ 12335 No. ¼ 11572 No. ¼ 3289 No. ¼ 3083
 20 0.83 (0.58–1.18) 0.91 (0.60–1.37) 1.01 (0.74–1.38) 0.93 (0.57–1.52)
21–25 0.92 (0.79–1.06) 1.03 (0.86–1.22) 1.07 (0.89–1.28) 1.02 (0.77–1.34)
26–30 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.) 1.00 (ref.)
31–35 0.97 (0.85–1.11) 0.90 (0.77–1.05) 1.01 (0.83–1.23) 1.14 (0.85–1.53)
> 35 0.79 (0.61–1.01) 0.74 (0.53–1.02) 1.06 (0.83–1.35) 1.34 (0.77–2.30)
P-value*3 0.37 0.21 0.95 0.56

*1Crude estimate (adjusting for birth date by cubic spline).


*2Adjusted estimate (birth date by cubic spline; parity 1/> 1; smoking no/yes; gender; other parents age).
*3Test for trend.

Table 5. Sibling cohorts; crude and adjustedcombined effects of parental age on risk of ADHD

Risk of ADHD; HR (95% CI)

Maternal age, years Paternal age, years

Full siblingcohort

Crude HR*1 No.: 12 769 < 26 26–30 >30 P-value*3


< 26 1.30 (0.96:1.77) 1.33 (1.04–1.70) 1.65 (1.28–2.14)
26–30 1.08 (0.78–1.50) 1.32 (1.10–1.58) 1.26 (1.09–1.45)
> 30 0.95 (0.40–2.27) 1.05 (0.83–1.33) 1.00 (ref.) 0.72
Adjusted HR*2 No.: 11 967
< 26 1.61 (1.13–2.30) 1.50 (1.13–1.99) 1.70 (1.26–2.29)
26–30 1.35 (0.93–1.96) 1.45 (1.18–1.79) 1.32 (1.12–1.56)
> 30 0.79 (0.29–2.13) 1.09 (0.84–1.42) 1.00 (ref.) 0.32
Half siblingcohort
Crude HR*1 No.: 3730
< 26 1.96 (1.00:3.84) 1.33 (1.04–1.70) 2.04 (1.06–3.94)
26–30 1.28 (0.70–2.33) 1.32 (1.10–1.58) 1.16 (0.75–1.81)
> 30 1.06 (0.40–2.82) 1.05 (0.83–1.33) 1.00 (ref.) 0.25
Adjusted HR*2 No.: 3683
< 26 1.80 (0.86–3.80) 1.34 (0.68–2.65) 1.85 (0.89–3.86)
26–30 1.18 (0.60–2.29) 1.43 (0.86–2.37) 1.01 (0.62–1.67)
> 30 1.50 (0.51–4.38) 0.82 (0.43–1.56) 1.00 (ref.) 0.16

*1Crude estimate(adjusting for birth date by cubic spline).


*2Adjusted estimate (birth date by cubic spline: parity 1/> 1; smoking no/yes; gender).
*3Test for trend.

Young parents represent a selected group of young peo- history of hyperactivity problems32 and they may transmit
ple, and many of their characteristics may increase the risk the genetic susceptibility to ADHD in the offspring even
of ADHD in offspring. Compared with those who become though the severity of their own symptoms would not
parents at an older age, young parents more often have a reach the diagnostic threshold. This hypothesis is also
International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2 417

supported by research reporting that young people with child especially in the first years, which may explain this
ADHD behaviour are found to have earlier sexual debut finding.
and have more risky sexual behaviour with more sexual This study supports that the association between young
partners, and thus a higher probability of becoming par- maternal age and offspring ADHD is due to risk factors at
ents at an earlier age.2,32,34,45–49 a family level, mainly accounted for by genetic factors, but
If the association between young maternal age and environmental factors or gene-environment interactions
ADHD is explained by a genetic predisposition, the timing might also play a role in the aetiology of ADHD. It has
of the birth in the mothers life may not be the critical factor been suggested that children with some genetic vulnerabil-
in determining offspring ADHD.35,36,50–52 If such a mater- ity to develop ADHD can be more susceptible to environ-
nal disposition to initiate childbearing at a younger age ex- mental risk factors, for example adversities in the perinatal
plained the observed association in the population-based environment, exposure to alcohol and cannabis, and ad-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


cohort, we would not have expected to find an association verse obstetric and postnatal events. Maternal lifestyle dur-
in the sibling comparisons, as the risk will be the same ing pregnancy might explain the trend of increasing risk of
among siblings. The early childbearing phenotype of the ADHD with decreasing maternal age, observed in full sib-
mother will be the same for all siblings, and the attenua- lings, as young mothers more often tend to engage in risky
tion in the association between maternal age and ADHD behaviours during pregnancy that have been associated
found in the sibling comparison may reflect that this phe- with mental problems in the offspring.24,34 Young mothers
notype is central in the association found in the population are more likely to smoke during pregnancy, which affects
cohort. Although young maternal age was not associated
the developing fetus.57 .However, it is also possible that
with ADHD in full siblings, we did find a trend of increas-
they are involved with other kinds of risky behaviours such
ing risk of ADHD with decreasing maternal age.
as poor nutrition, alcohol and cannabis use. Prenatal ex-
Additionally, when studying the combined effect of paren-
posure to alcohol has been shown to be associated with
tal ages in full siblings, we observed a tendency to maternal
increased risk of ADHD, independently of the effects of
age affecting the association between paternal age and
prenatal smoking exposure.57 Maternal lifestyle and risky
ADHD; the younger the mother, the higher the risk.
behaviour during pregnancy may explain why the trend of
Paternal age did not modify the association between mater-
increased risk of ADHD with decreased age only is
nal age and ADHD.
observed regarding maternal age. In addition, studies of
A study found that young mothers showed more nega-
the relationship between maternal age and low birthweight
tive responsiveness and were more restrictive in controlling
found that children born to very young mothers had a
their children, less supportive, more inadequate in guiding
higher risk of low birthweight,58 and low birthweight has
their children and generally less responsive compared with
been found to be associated with ADHD. We also con-
older mothers.34,35 Further, children of young mothers
ducted subanalyses stratified by gestational age and birth-
were found likely to experience higher levels of maternal
depression, marital conflict and family disruption com- weight, but the estimates did not change significantly
pared with children of older mothers,54 which has been either in the population-based cohort or in the sibling
associated with ADHD.54–56 This trend observed with ma- cohorts.
ternal age may be due to improved maternal parenting If different amounts of sex-related hormones29 or gy-
skills and maturity resulting in a more accepting attitude naecological immaturity explained the association between
towards a second or third child’s behaviour, potentially young maternal age and ADHD, we would have expected
causing fewer symptoms and/or decreased impairment almost similar associations in the sibling cohorts as in the
among later-born siblings. Also, the first child’s behaviour population cohort and, further, we would not have ex-
could have made the mother more robust and competent in pected an additional risk also when the father was very
handling later-born children with similar behaviour. young.
Mother-child attachment will differ among siblings but, The present study has a number of strengths. It is based
explaining part of the increased risk of ADHD with on a complete 20-year follow-up using established diagnos-
decreased maternal age, we would also have expected an tic data obtained from highly reliable nationwide registers,
association between low paternal age and ADHD as a re- based on standardized diagnostic reporting procedures. In
sult of paternal immaturity. However, the finding of a addition, data on both inpatient and outpatient visits were
trend with only maternal age may reflect less paternal in- available for the whole study population. Furthermore, our
volvement in early parenting. Even though there have been study was able to examine the associations in both a full
cultural changes towards more gender equality, pregnancy anda half sibling cohort, corresponding to 6436 and 1795
and breastfeeding keep mothers in closer contact with the families, respectively. Finally, as an important
418 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

methodological strength, we controlled for time trends the other siblings within the family. Parental perceptions,
including the increase in ADHD diagnosis during the study experience and behaviour may impact on the associations
period. of interest at the level of an individual child or family, but
Although the Danish register contains complete infor- these factors may be difficult or impossible to measure and
mation for inpatient as well as outpatient contacts since control for. Also, birth order might play an important role
1995 in the public health system, some children are unrec- when comparing the risk of ADHD among siblings. It is
ognized and thus untreated or treated without contact with possible that a diagnosis given to a child will make it more
the hospital system. Accordingly, ADHD is undiagnosed in likely that an undiagnosed sibling with the disorder will be
the hospital system and we have false-negatives, especially diagnosed and probably also lower the threshold for diag-
for the oldest children in the cohort. Among siblings, false- nosing another. In the sibling design, this would generate
negatives might therefore be more likely among the oldest fewer discordant sibling pairs attenuating the estimates.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


siblings, which might have biased the estimates towards Our results are much in line with the study from Sweden,
the null. We controlled for time trends and do not expect using sibling- and cousin-comparisons, indicating that the
them to influence our estimates significantly. In contrast, association between maternal age and ADHD mainly could
as the literature also reports ADHD being over-diagnosed be explained by genetic confounding,37 as we found the
typically referring to false-positives, we cannot exclude weakest association among full siblings. Another Swedish
non-differential misclassification.10,59,60 If we expect
study reported that siblings with fathers aged 45 years or
increased focus on families with young parents, the likeli-
older had a highly increased risk of ADHD (HR ¼ 13.13,
hood of getting in contact with the paediatric psychiatry
95% CI 6.85-25.16) compared with their siblings born
system and of being diagnosed may differ between parental
when the father was 20 to 24 years old,40 which they suggest
age groups, resulting in diagnostic parental age bias. In this
is consistent with the hypothesis that new genetic mutations
case, an overestimation of the association between young
that occur during spermatogenesis are causally related to
parental age and ADHD cannot be rejected.
offspring ADHD. In contrast, we did not find any associ-
Only 1.30% of the children had an ICD-10 diagnosis of
ations between either young or advantaged paternal age and
Hyperkinetic Disorder (F90x), which is lower than sug-
ADHD. A potential explanation could be failure to control
gested in other studies. In this study, the ICD-10 diagnosis
for the trend of increase in ADHD diagnosis as well as in-
of Hyperkinetic Disorder mainly represents the more se-
crease in parental childbearing age over time in the Swedish
vere cases of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
study.64 In addition, inconsistencies between studies may be
Mental Disorders (DSM-5) definition of attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with some of the less se- due to variation in the demographics of the study partici-
vere cases being missing. pants, sample size and availability of data on potential con-
We excluded less than 2% off the children on the basis founders. Further, in the present study we were able to
of missing data on paternal age. As an association between include data on clinical diagnoses and admission dates for
lonely parenthood and offspring ADHD has been all inpatient psychiatric hospital admissions since 1969 and
observed, we might have excluded a group of children with outpatient visits at psychiatric hospitals and clinics since
higher prevalence of ADHD.54 Compared with the total 1995, comparing 1973 and 2001 in Sweden, respectively.
study population, we found that children with missing
data on paternal age had 0.1% higher prevalence of
Conclusion
ADHD, but including these children in the analysis did not
change the crude associations between maternal age and We found that children diagnosed with ADHD more often
offspring ADHD either in the population cohort or in the had younger parents compared with children without a diag-
half sibling cohort. nosis of ADHD. The risk of ADHD was highest if both par-
Although the sibling design can be a powerful tool for ents were very young. However, the risk attenuated in the
accounting for confounding because of both measured and sibling-comparisons, suggesting that the population-wide as-
unmeasured stable within-family factors,28,40,61–63 the sib- sociation between young parental age and offspring ADHD is
ling comparisons face a limitation, as they cannot account due to risk factors at a family level, mainly accounted for by
for uncontrolled confounding by unmeasured sibling- genetic factors, or that sibling comparisons are too conserva-
varying within-family factors as carry-over effects from tive. As we found a trend of increased risk of ADHD with
one sibling to another. We should therefore carefully con- decreasing maternal age, which was not found according to
sider factors that may affect the exposure and outcome paternal age, we cannot exclude the early-life environment or
differently between siblings. A sibling being diagnosed maternal behaviour related to young maternal age playing a
with ADHD could affect parental behaviour in relation to role in the aetiology of ADHD.
International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2 419

Our results suggest that young parenthood could be a 14. Lichtenstein P, Carlstrom E, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Anckarsater
marker of genetic traits and, if so, delaying fertility will not H. The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuro-
be fully effective in reducing offspring ADHD. Public pol- psychiatric disorders in childhood. Am J Psychiatry 2010;167:
1357–63.
icy initiatives should still be targeted at attention to young
15. Thapar A, Langley K, Fowler T et al. Catechol O-methyltransfer-
parents to support parenting as well as guidance on healthy
ase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial
lifestyle during pregnancy among young mothers. behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
order. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:1275–78.
16. Buss C, Entringer S, Wadhwa PD. Fetal programming of brain
Funding development: intrauterine stress and susceptibility to psycho-
Susanne Hvolgaard Mikkelsen was supported by a full PhD fellow- pathology. Sci Signal 2012;5:7.
ship at Aarhus University, Denmark. 17. Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Prenatal program-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


ming of human neurological function. Int J Pept 2011;2011:
Conflict of interest: None.
837596.
18. Linnet KM, Dalsgaard S, Obel C et al. Maternal lifestyle factors
References in pregnancy risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and
1. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. associated behaviors: review of the current evidence. Am J
The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and Psychiatry 2003;160:1028–40.
metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:942–48. 19. Lindblad F, Hjern A. ADHD after fetal exposure to maternal
2. Brown RT, Freeman WS, Perrin JM et al. Prevalence and assess- smoking. Nicotine Tob Res 2010;12:408–15.
ment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in primary care 20. Obel C, Zhu JL, Olsen J et al. The risk of attention deficit hyper-
settings. Pediatrics 2001;107:E43. activity disorder in children exposed to maternal smoking during
3. Atladottir HO, Gyllenberg D, Langridge A et al. The increasing pregnancy - a reexamination using a sibling design. J Child
prevalence of reported diagnoses of childhood psychiatric dis- Psychol Psychiatry2016;16:532–37.
orders: a descriptive multinational comparison. Eur Child 21. Lindstrom K, Lindblad F, Hjern A. Preterm birth and attention-
Adolesc Psychiatry 2015;24):173–83. deficit/hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildren. Pediatrics
4. Kjeldsen BV, Jensen SO, Munk-Jorgensen P. Increasing number 2011;127:858–65.
of incident ADHD cases in psychiatric treatment. Acta Psychiatr 22. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ.
Scand 2012;126:151–52. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who
5. Joelsson P, Chudal R, Gyllenberg D et al. Demographic charac- were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA 2002;288:728–37.
teristics and psychiatric comorbidity of children and adolescents 23. McGrath JJ, Petersen L, Agerbo E, Mors O, Mortensen PB,
diagnosed with ADHD in specialized healthcare. Child Pedersen CB. A comprehensive assessment of parental age and
Psychiatry Hum Dev 2015, Sep 23. [Epub ahead of print.]. psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 2014;71:301–09.
6. Increasing prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyper- 24. Gustafsson P, Kallen K. Perinatal, maternal, and fetal character-
activity disorder among children - United States, 2003 and 2007. istics of children diagnosed with attention-deficit-hyperactivity
MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2010;59:1439–43. disorder: results from a population-based study utilizing the
7. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H. The British Child and Adolescent Swedish Medical Birth Register. Dev Med Child Neurol
Mental Health Survey 1999: the prevalence of DSM-IV disorders. 2011;53:263–68.
J Am Acad Child Adolesct Psychiatry 2003;42:1203–11. 25. Galera C, Cote SM, Bouvard MP et al. Early risk factors for
8. Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyper- hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age
activity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics 17 months to 8 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011;68:1267–75.
2012;9:490–99. 26. Silva D, Colvin L, Hagemann E, Bower C. Environmental risk
9. Parner ET, Schendel DE, Thorsen P. Autism prevalence trends factors by gender associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
over time in Denmark: changes in prevalence and age at diagno- disorder. Pediatrics 2014;133:e14–22.
sis. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008;162:1150–56. 27. Chudal R, Joelsson P, Gyllenberg D, Lehti V, Leivonen S,
10. Batstra L, Nieweg EH, Pijl S, Van Tol DG, Hadders-Algra M. Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, et al. Parental age and the risk of atten-
Childhood ADHD: a stepped diagnosis approach. J Psychiatr tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide, population-
Pract 2014;20:169–77. based cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
11. Hansen SN, Schendel DE, Parner ET. Explaining the increase in 2015;54:487–94.e1
the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: the proportion at- 28. Harden KP, Lynch SK, Turkheimer E et al. A behavior genetic in-
tributable to changes in reporting practices. JAMA Pediatr vestigation of adolescent motherhood and offspring mental
2015;169:59–62. health problems. J Abnorm Psychol 2007;116:667–83.
12. Thapar A, Cooper M, Jefferies R, Stergiakouli E. What causes at- 29. Orlebeke JF, Knol DL, Boomsma DI, Verhulst FC. Frequency of
tention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Arch Dis Child parental report of problem behavior in children decreases with
2012;97:260–65. increasing maternal age at delivery. Psychol Rep 1998;82:
13. Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE et al. Molecular genetics of at- 395–404.
tention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57: 30. Zumoff B, Strain GW, Miller LK, Rosner W. Twenty-four-hour
1313–23. mean plasma testosterone concentration declines with age in
420 International Journal of Epidemiology, 2017, Vol. 46, No. 2

normal premenopausal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 49. Donahue KL, Lichtenstein P, Lundstrom S et al. Childhood be-
1995;80:1429–30. havior problems and adolescent sexual risk behavior: familial
31. Fraser AM, Brockert JE, Ward RH. Association of young mater- confounding in the child and adolescent twin study in Sweden
nal age with adverse reproductive outcomes. N Engl J Med (CATSS). J Adolesc Health 2013;52:606–12.
1995;332:1113–17. 50. Geronimus AT, Korenman S, Hillemeier MM. Does young ma-
32. Lehti V, Niemela S, Heinze M et al. Childhood predictors of ternal age adversely affect child development? Evidence from
becoming a teenage mother among Finnish girls. Acta Obstet cousin comparisons in the United States. Popul Dev Rev
Gynecol Scand 2012;91:1319–25. 1994;20:585–609.
33. Jaffee S, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Belsky J, Silva P. Why are children 51. D’Onofrio BM, Goodnight JA, Van Hulle CA et al. Maternal
born to teen mothers at risk for adverse outcomes in young age at childbirth and offspring disruptive behaviors: testing the
adulthood? Results from a 20-year longitudinal study. Dev causal hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009;50:1018–28.
Psychopathol 2001;13:377–97. 52. Myrskyla M, Fenelon A. Maternal age and offspring adult
34. Trautmann-Villalba P, Gerhold M, Laucht M, Schmidt MH.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ije/article-abstract/46/2/409/2617180 by guest on 27 February 2019


health: evidence from the health and retirement study.
Early motherhood and disruptive behaviour in the school-age Demography 2012;49:1231–57.
child. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:120–25. 53. Lieb R, Wittchen HU, Hofler M, Fuetsch M, Stein MB,
35. Turley RN. Are children of young mothers disadvantaged be- Merikangas KR. Parental psychopathology, parenting styles,
cause of their mother’s age or family background? Child Dev and the risk of social phobia in offspring: a prospective-longitu-
2003;74:465–74. dinal community study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:859–66.
36. Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Najman JM. Teenage children of teenage 54. Russell G, Ford T, Rosenberg R, Kelly S. The association of at-
mothers: psychological, behavioural and health outcomes from tention deficit hyperactivity disorder with socioeconomic disad-
an Australian prospective longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med
vantage: alternative explanations and evidence. J Child Psychol
2006;62:2526–39.
Psychiatry 2014;55:436–45.
37. Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM et al. Maternal age at
55. Deault LC. A systematic review of parenting in relation to the de-
childbirth and risk for ADHD in offspring: a population-based
velopment of comorbidities and functional impairments in chil-
cohort study. Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1815–24.
dren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
38. Coyne CA, Langstrom N, Rickert ME, Lichtenstein P,
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2010;41:168–92.
D’Onofrio BM. Maternal age at first birth and offspring crimin-
56. Johnston C, Mash EJ. Families of children with attention-deficit/
ality: using the children of twins design to test causal hypotheses.
hyperactivity disorder: review and recommendations for future
Dev Psychopathol 2013;25:17–35.
research. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2001;4:183–207.
39. Gabis L, Raz R, Kesner-Baruch Y. Paternal age in autism spec-
57. Mick E, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Sayer J, Kleinman S. Case-
trum disorders and ADHD. Pediatr Neurol 2010;43:300–02.
control study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and ma-
40. D’Onofrio BM, Rickert ME, Frans E et al. Paternal age at child-
ternal smoking, alcohol use, and drug use during pregnancy. J
bearing and offspring psychiatric and academic morbidity.
Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:378–85.
JAMA Psychiatry 2014;71:432–38.
58. Reichman NE, Pagnini DL. Maternal age and birth outcomes:
41. Smith PL. Splines as a useful and convenient statistical tool. Am
data from New Jersey. Fam Plann Perspect 1997;29:268–72:
Stat 1979;33:57–62.
295.
42. Collishaw S, Maughan B, Goodman R, Pickles A. Time trends in
59. Bruchmuller K, Margraf J, Schneider S. Is ADHD diagnosed in
adolescent mental health. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45:
1350–62. accord with diagnostic criteria? Overdiagnosis and influence of
43. Olsen J, Zhu JL. Re: ‘Advanced parental age and the risk of aut- client gender on diagnosis. J Consult Clin Psychol 2012;80:
ism spectrum disorder’. Am J Epidemiol 2009;169:1406–07. 128–38.
44. Wilcox AJ, Weinberg CR, Basso O. On the pitfalls of adjusting 60. Sciutto MJ, Eisenberg M. Evaluating the evidence for and against
for gestational age at birth. Am J Epidemiol 2011;174:1062–68. the overdiagnosis of ADHD. J Atten Disord 2007;11:106–13.
45. Hosain GM, Berenson AB, Tennen H, Bauer LO, Wu ZH. 61. Lahey BB, D’Onofrio BM. All in the family: comparing siblings
Attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms and risky sexual behav- to test causal hypotheses regarding environmental influences on
ior in young adult women. J Womens Health (Larchmt) behavior. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2010;19:319–23.
2012;21:463–68. 62. Rutter M. Proceeding from observed correlation to causal infer-
46. Ramrakha S, Bell ML, Paul C, Dickson N, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. ence: the use of natural experiments. Perspect Psychol Sci
Childhood behavior problems linked to sexual risk taking in 2007;2:377–95.
young adulthood: a birth cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc 63. Frisell T, Oberg S, Kuja-Halkola R, Sjolander A. Sibling com-
Psychiatry 2007;46:1272–79. parison designs: bias from non-shared confounders and measure-
47. Maughan B, Lindelow M. Secular change in psychosocial risks: ment error. Epidemiology 2012;23:713–20.
the case of teenage motherhood. Psychol Med 1997;27:1129–44. 64. Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development.
48. Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W et al. Developmental trajecto- Mean Age of Mothers at First Childbirth. 2012. http://www.
ries of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents oecd.org/social/family/SF2.3 Mean age of mother at first child-
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA 2002;288: birth - updated 240212.pdf (30 January 2015, date last
1740–48. accessed).

S-ar putea să vă placă și