Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

This article was downloaded by: [University of Prince Edward Island]

On: 16 November 2014, At: 14:23


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/urqe20

Development of an Instrument to Assess Cognitive


Processes in Physical Education Classes
a b b
Melinda A. Solmon , Amelia M. Lee & Amelia M. Lee
a
Department of Kinesiology , University of Maryland , USA
b
Department of Kinesiology , Louisiana State University , USA
Published online: 22 Feb 2013.

To cite this article: Melinda A. Solmon , Amelia M. Lee & Amelia M. Lee (1997) Development of an Instrument to Assess
Cognitive Processes in Physical Education Classes, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68:2, 152-160, DOI:
10.1080/02701367.1997.10607991

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1997.10607991

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport
© 1997 bythe American Alliance for Health,
Physical Education, Recreation and Dance
Vol. 68, No.2, pp. 152-160

Development of an Instrument to Assess Cognitive Processes


in Physical Education Classes
Melinda A. Solmon andAmelia M. Lee
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

Using the mediating process paradigm (Doyle, 1977) as a framework, the need to develop reliable and valid instruments for
assessing cognitive processesis apparent, and that was the purpose of this study. Participants (N = 819) completed the Cognitive
Processes Questionnaire in Physical Education (CPQPE), as well as three other instruments addressing dispositional goal
orientation, perception of motivational climate, and beliefs about causes ofsuccess in physical education. The fioe-factor
structure that emerged in an exploratory factor analysis produced an acceptablefit with the data in the confirmatory factor
analysis. The subscales of the CPQPE were related to a task-involved goal perspective and the belief that success is attributed to
motivation and effort. The results indicate the CPQPE is a valid and reliable instrument that can provide valuable information
about the teaching and learning process.

Key words: cognitive mediation, cognition, physical edu- In recognizing the link between what teachers do
cation and what students learn cannot be explained by a unidi-
rectional two-step model, investigators have employed a

A ttem p ts to understand or define effective teaching


were initiated within a performance-based model
or a process-product paradigm. Direct relationships be-
mediating process paradigm (Doyle, 1977) to increase
knowledge about how students learn from teaching. This
research is grounded in the belief that learning from
tween teacher behavior (process variables) and student teaching can best be understood by viewing the activi-
achievement (product variables) were examined. In gen- ties and involvement of learners during classes as medi-
eral, this research used trained observers and low ating variables. This extension, suggested by Doyle
inference coding instruments to quantify types and fre- (1977), reflected a change in perspective of the teach-
quencies of teacher behaviors which were then corre- ing and learning process from one that examines teacher
lated with student outcomes. Although this line of behaviors to one that studies what students do to learn.
inquiry generated a large number of studies and pro- Equipped with a clear understanding of how student
vided valuable information about what occurs in process variables impact learning, we might then be able
classes, it has been criticized on a variety of bases. Most to frame meaningful questions and derive worthwhile
notably, the conclusions drawn were not consistent solutions about instructional conditions that are most ef-
(Doyle, 1977), and this simplistic two-dimensional model fective. Doyle (1977), while acknowledging the contribu-
lacks the complexity necessary to explain the mechanism tion of initial studies incorporating a mediational
for effective teaching (Marx & Winne, 1987). Research- paradigm, criticized this work for its reliance on overt
ers in educational settings now acknowledge that under- measures of mediation, such as time-utilization and task-
standing the multifaceted process of teaching and completion rates. He advocated emphasizing the inter-
learning requires an approach more complex than sim- nal cognitive processes operating when students are
ply examining variables related to teacher behavior and actively engaged with content rather than relying totally
student achievement. on overt behaviors. In this context, cognitive processes
are defined as student thoughts or cognitions that im-
pact learning, including their beliefs, perceptions, expec-
Submitted: December 19, 1995 tations, levels of attention, motivation, and use of
Accepted: July 22, 1996 strategies (Wittrock, 1986).
The move toward studying covert cognitive variables
Melinda A. Solman is with the Department of Kinesiology at the
University of Maryland. Amelia M. Lee is with the Department of
recommended by Doyle (1977) requires the assessment
Kinesiology at Louisiana State University. of mental processes that cannot be detected by observa-

152 RaES: June 1997


Solmon andLee

tion and, therefore not easily measured. Investigators ego-involved goals reference their success relative to the
must rely to a large degree on measures of self-report, performance of others and consequently are more likely
and problems associated with accepting self-reports as to focus on their ability and performing better than
primary data sources are well documented (Bainbridge, other students in the class. They tend to display lower
1979; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). However, it is evident that levels of persistence and effort during learning and of-
when self-report data are carefully collected using appro- ten choose tasks to avoid challenge (Solmon, 1996).
priate guidelines, they can provide valid, reliable, and Dispositional and situational factors have been used
valuable information for researchers and practitioners to assess goal perspective. Measures of dispositional goal
(Ericsson & Simon, 1980; Howard, 1981; Lee & Solmon, orientation (Duda, 1992) assess individual differences
1992; Locke &Jensen, 1974) or tendencies to be task- or ego-involved. Although it is
Despite the challenge associated with measurement, acknowledged that individuals vary in individual dispo-
several researchers have undertaken the study of stu- sitions for developing task- or ego-involved states, situ-
dents' cognitive processes and the relationship of these ational factors have the potential to enhance or
processes to student outcomes (Peterson, 1988; Wittrock, constrain those goal states (Ames, 1992). Perceptions of
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

1986). Using questionnaires and stimulated recall inter- motivational climate have been used to study how situ-
views as indicants of students' thoughts, Peterson and her ational factors impact goal perspective and subsequent
colleagues (Peterson & Swing, 1982; Peterson, Swing, motivational patterns (Ames & Archer, 1988; Seifriz,
Stark, & Waas, 1984) concluded that students' reports of Duda, & Chi, 1992). The evidence is clear, from both
their thoughts were more accurate predictors ofstudent dispositional and situational measures, that a task ori-
achievement than observer estimates of time on task. In entation, more than an ego orientation, is positively re-
physical education classes, both stimulated recall inter- lated to the use of active cognitive engagement strategies
views (Lee, Landin & Carter, 1992) and questionnaires in both academic classrooms (Ames & Archer, 1988) and
(Solmon & Lee, 1996) have been employed as measures physical education (Solmon & Boone, 1993). Task ori-
of student cognition, and the results of these studies in- entation is also associated with the belief that success and
dicate that students are aware of their cognitive processes effort covary (Ames & Archer, 1988), while individuals
and able to report them with sufficient detail to yield adopting an ego-involved perspective are more likely to
valuable information about how they learn from teach- attribute success to ability (Duda & Nicholls, 1992).
ing. It has been established that cognitive variables, such The investigation of cognitive processes impacting
as students' reports of their attention, use of strategies, learning has the potential to contribute significantly to
and motivational levels, impact measures of student en- the knowledge base about teaching and learning. How-
gagement, student effort, and achievement in physical ever, the reliable assessment of cognitive processes re-
education classes (Solmon & Boone, 1993; Solmon & mains an issue in this line of research. Measures used in
Lee, 1996). The measurement of cognitive variables in previous research have been developed for specific stud-
these studies has produced informative data about how ies, and although reliability coefficients are reported,
students mediate instruction in physical education set- there has been no attempt to standardize an instrument
tings, and the results in this line of research are encour- for use across multiple studies. The purpose of this study
aging. However, more consistent and reliable assessment was to develop an instrument that assesses the cognitive
ofvariables related to student cognition is needed for use processes of students in physical education classes that
in physical education classes. will provide investigators with a reliable tool for measur-
Goal perspective theory (Nicholls, 1984, 1989) has ing cognitive variables that impact achievement.
been used as a framework for studying cognitive engage-
ment, and investigations using this construct have sup-
ported the notion that student reports of their
cognitions are related to the achievement goal orienta- Method
tion they adopt. Considerable research supports the
notion of student goals as antecedents of motivated be- Participants
havior and effective approaches to learning. These goals
serve to activate patterns of cognition related to how Participants in this study were 819 students (417 boys,
students attend to, interpret, and respond to learning 412 girls) in physical education classes in public elemen-
activities. Two contrasting sets of goals, "task" and "ego" tary (n =228) and middle schools (n =591) in the south-
have been identified (Nicholls, 1984, 1989). Students eastern United States. The students, enrolled in 32
adopting task-involved goals define success in self-refer- different classes taught by 19 teachers in 12 schools, were
enced terms. They are more likely to focus on learning in grades 4 through 8 and ranged in age from 10-14
through effort, show an intrinsic interest in learning years, Mage = 12.4 years, SD = 1.36. The school systems
activities, and use effective learning strategies (Ames & granted perrnission to conduct the study, and partici-
Archer, 1988). On the other hand, students adopting pants' parents gave informed consent prior to the study.

ROES: June 1997 153


Solmon andLee

Instrument Development sis, the form was administered to a small group of stu-
dents. Standardized instructions were given, and stu-
Items for the Cognitive Processes Questionnaire in dents completed thequestionnaire. They were then
Physical Education (CPQPE) were derived through a interviewed informally in small groups concerning any
series of steps. A pool of 60 potential items was gener- items they had difficulty with as well as their impressions
ated from several sources. Items were drawn from ques- of the instrument. Based on these interactions, no ad-
tionnaires used in previous studies as well as open-ended ditional modifications were deemed necessary.
student responses in stimulated recall interviews (Lee et
al., 1992; Peterson et al., 1984; Solmon & Boone, 1993; Procedure
Solmon & Lee, 1996). Additional items were adapted
from the Learning and Study Strategies Inventory The instrument was administered to the participants
(Weinstein, Palmer, & Shulte, 1987). The pool of poten- during their regularly scheduled physical education
tial questions was screened using a computer software classes by one of the primary investigators or a gradu-
program to ensure the wording used in the items was at ate student who had been trained to do so. At the be-
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

an appropriate reading level for the participants. Items ginning of the session, we explained to students we were
were modified when necessary to be appropriate for the trying to learn how to help people become better physi-
third grade reading level. Statements were counterbal- cal education teachers and that we appreciated their
anced so that some were positively weighted and others help very much. We stressed there were no right or
negatively weighted. This was done for three reasons: (a) wrong answers, but that it was very important they be
to reduce the likelihood of producing socially desirable honest. Participants were assured that no one else would
responses, (b) to encourage participants to read each see their answers. We emphasized the importance of
statement carefully, and (c) to facilitate comprehension reading each statement carefully before marking in any
of the meanings of the items. of the columns and to think about how well each state-
We piloted potential questionnaire items with se- ment described them in their physical education classes.
lected elementary and middle school students using sev- Prior to each administration, we reviewed the response
eral steps. First, we read items aloud to small groups of scale with students to ensure that the meanings were
students, asking whether or not they understood the clear to them, and instructed them to ask questions
items and what they thought the statements meant. At about any item they did not understand.
this stage, we also experimented with potential response Three additional questionnaires were administered
scales. We considered using several types of five-point to participants to provide data concerning the validity
Likert scales that had been used previously, such as hav- of the CPQPE. The response format for these instru-
ing students indicate their level of agreement with state- ments was a five-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly
ments (from strongly disagree to strongly agree), and the Disagree to Strongly Agree. Internal consistency on in-
frequency with which they engaged in a behavior or ac- dividual subscales of these questionnaires on the data
tion (from usually to almost never). Based on input from collected for this study was assessed using alpha coeffi-
students in these informal conversations, combined with cients (Cronbach, 1951). Student goal orientation in
consideration of the phrasing in the statements, we de- physical education was assessed using the 13 items from
cided the most appropriate format for our instrument the Task and Ego Orientation in Sport (TEOSQ; Duda,
was to ask students to indicate how well the statements 1992) modified for use in physical education classes. The
described themselves by selecting one of the following stem for this instrument was "I feel most successful in
responses: "very much like me," "like me," "sorta like physical education class when ... "The reliability esti-
me," "a little like me," or "not like me at all." mates for the Task and Ego subscales were .81 and .83,
Next, we grouped together items we thought ad- respectively.
dressed similar or related areas or concerns and divided Students' perceptions of the motivational climate of
items into two smaller pools according to the direction their classes were assessed by the second questionnaire.
in which they were weighted. The items were randomly Drawing again from instrumentation used in sport set-
ordered for inclusion on the scale, with two stipulations. tings, the 25 items included on this scale were adapted
No more than three consecutive items would be from the Perceived Motivational Climate in Sport Ques-
weighted in the same direction or come from the same tionnaire (PMCSQ; Seifriz, Duda, & Chi, 1992) for use
group of items hypothesized to focus on the same area in physical education classes. "In my physical education
or concern. During the piloting phase, items were elimi- class ... "was the stem for the 25 items on this ques-
nated from the pool to decrease repetition and elimi- tionnaire. Fourteen items comprised the subscale reflect-
nate questions that generated confusion. This process ing the perception of a performance or ego-involved
resulted in a 33-item scale used in this study. After the climate (a = .79), while the remaining 11 items related
questionnaire had been printed using computer soft- to the perception of a mastery or task-involved climate
ware that permitted responses to be scanned for analy- (a = .77)

154 RDES: June 1997


Solmon andLee

The third questionnaire addressed students' beliefs produce an external matrix file as the data source for
or attributions concerning the causes of success in physi- LISREL 7 (SPSS). Maximum likelihood method of esti-
cal education classes. The 20 items on this scale were mation was used. The hypothesized model generated by
adapted for use in physical education classes from scales the EFAwas imposed on the second subsample by speci-
used by Duda and Nicholls (1992). The stem for this in- fying the parameters of the model so that the loadings
strument was "People succeed in physical education class of the items for a particular factor were free to vary, while
when ...." Eight items focused on motivation and ef- the loadings on all other factors were fixed to zero.
fort as the cause for success in physical education classes, Factor Scores. Factor scores, taking into account the
and the reliability estimate for this subscale was .84. Seven weighting of each item that loads on a factor, were cal-
items identified ability as a cause of success, producing a culated for the five subscales. The product of the factor
subscale with an alpha coefficient of .65. The remaining loading and the score of each item loading on a factor
five items attributed success in physical education classes were summed to calculate to factor scores (Stevens,
to deception, such as cheating. The alpha coefficient for 1986). Reliability between the items within each factor
this subscale was .68. or subscale was assessed using alpha coefficients
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

(Cronbach,1951).
Data Analysis Correlational Analyses and Group Differences. To pro-
vide information 'concerning utility of the instrument
Exploratory Factor Analysis. Prior to analyzing re- with elementary and middle school populations, data
sponses on the instrument, scores on the reversal items relative to descriptive statistics and group differences
were transformed so that a score of 5 was indicative of are presented. Pearson product correlation coeffi-
the most positive response, and a score of 1 indicated cients were used to investigate relationships between
the least positive response for all items. The total sample the subscales of the CPQPE with the measures of goal
of 819 participants was randomly spilt to produce two perspective, causes of success, and perception of mo-
subsamples, one for an exploratory factor analysis (EFA; tivational climate. Differences among groups of stu-
n = 409), and the other for a confirmatory factor analy- dents based on school level and teacher were analyzed
sis (CFA; n = 410). An iterative principal components using multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs)
analysis was used for the EFA. This data reduction tech- with each subscale of the CPQPE as a dependent vari-
nique was used to delineate constructs or underlying fac- able. Significant effects were followed up using analy-
tors of cognitive processes assessed by this instrument. ses of variance.
The number of factors that constitute a solution for the
EFA is determined based on several criteria. Consider-
ations include the number of eigenvalues that exceed
1, the percentage ofvariance accounted for by the num- Results
ber of factors, inspection of the scree plot to determine
when the eigenvalues begin to level off, and the cohe- Exploratory FactorAnalysis
siveness of the items within factors identified.
After the number of factors had been established, Five factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 emerged
a varimax rotation was used to simplify interpretation from the principal components factor analysis of the 33
of the factors. This rotation was selected because of its items on the CPQPE. The items and their factor load-
tendency to produce a factor pattern in which a small ings from the varimax rotation are presented in Table 1.
number ofvariables load high on one factor and low on The five factors accounted for 48.3% of the total variance.
the others (Stevens, 1986). The varimax rotation is a This percentage is comparable to the variance accounted
rigid rotation that does not allow factors in a solution for in other EFAs conducted in physical education set-
to be correlated. Because of the possibility that the tings (Papaioannou, 1994).
subscales of the CPQPE could be related, an oblique Items loading on the first factor centered on self-
rotation, direct olimin, that allows factors to be corre- regulated learning behaviors, reflecting a willingness to
lated, was also conducted. The same factor pattern was learn new activities, exert effort during practice, and
produced by both of the rotations, with all variables load- persist when difficulty is encountered. The focus of the
ing on the same factors with similar loadings, indicat- items loading on the second factor is a sense of confi-
ing the solution was stable. Factor loadings for each of dence or efficacy with regard to the activities in physi-
the items on the instrument are generated for the num- cal education. It is important to note that six of the
ber offactors extracted, and these reflect the magnitude seven items loading on this factor are reversal items,
or weighting of the item on each factor. Loadings of .40 on which a response of "not like me at all" was trans-
are considered to be significant (Stevens, 1986). formed from a 1 to a 5 for analysis. Although the man-
ConJirmatoryFactor Analysis. To conduct the CFA, the ner in which these items are stated reflects a lack of
PRELIS program was used on the second subsample to efficacy, a high score on this subscale would be indica-

RDES: June 1997 155


Solmon andLee

tive of a high level of confidence or efficacy rather than fourth factor consists of items that reflect a tendency to
a low one. The focus of the items on the third factor is avoid working in physical education class. These are all
concentration or paying attention during instruction and reversal items, and low scores (rather than higher ones)
practice. Four of the six items on this scale are also re- indicate an unwillingness to engage in class activities.
versal items, and the scoring has been adjusted so that Items loading on the fifth factor center on the use of
high scores correspond to high levels of attention. The strategies to learn skills in physical education class.

Table 1.Principal components factor analysis ofthe cognitive processes questionnaire (CPQPE)

Items Factors

2 3 4 5
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

Self-regulation
I findthat new games and skills arefun once you give them atry .70 .02 .09 .05 .04
I liketo learnnew and differentgames and skills .69 .16 .06 .24 .08
I feel like I can dowell if I try hard .66 .17 .01 .33 .07
When I practice a skill, I try to figure out .62 .14 .13 .02 .26
If I am not good in practice, I keep trying hard what I am doing wrong .62 .18 .08 .14 .08
I try to remember the important thingsthe teachersays about a skill
when 1am practicing .61 .09 .36 .02 .08
When I am practicing skillsin PE class, I try to get better each time .60 .00 .35 .11 .07
I work hard during practiceinPE class .60 .07 .28 .24 .21
If I don'tunderstand how or whatto do,I ask the teacherfor help .53 -.03 .23 .03 .17
When 1make mistakes during practice, I sayto myself "I cando better" .52 .23 .00 .10 .33

Confidence-efficacy
It is hard for meto correctthe things the teachersays I as doing wrong .02 .69 .20 .03 .10
During PE class I give up when the skill is hard .31 .62 .25 .28 -.09
When I cannot do a skill in PE, it is because it is too hard .10 .60 -.06 .11 .02
When the teachertells me what I am doing wrong I donot understand it, .07 .56 .24 .08 -.08
When I cando a new skill in PE, I think it is because I am lucky .14 .52 .11 .26 -.20
I feel bad when myskillsarenot as good asmyclassmates -.12 .48 .04 .29 .03
When I listen and watch the teacherexplain a skill, I think "Oh, I can dothat" .22 .43 .16 .22 -.15

Attention-concentration
When the teacheris talking, I find myself thinking about otherthings .11 .27 .68 .16 .13
During class, I talk to myfriends when I should bepracticing .10 .24 .65 .19 .14
I listen closely when the teacherexplains a skill during the PE lesson .43 .00 .60 .02 .15
I miss important thingsthe PE teachersays because I am not paying attention .24 .38 .58 .12 -.09
When I practice, I try to think onlyabout the skill I am working on .41 -.04 .47 .03 .09
I onlytry hard when the teacheris looking at me .29 .27 .46 .24 -.17

Willingness to engage
I would ratherstayin the classroom than goto PE class .20 .19 .14 .64 .06
I feel like I can't dowell no matter how hard I try .25 .35 -.02 .54 .01
In PE class I try to stayin the backofthe line sothat
I won't have to take as many turns .29 .30 .24 .54 -.16
I onlyliketo do games and activities that I am good at .07 .26 .32 .48 .15
I avoid practicing anyway I can .24 .27 .35 .40 -.15

Strategies
I try to go overthe right way to perform the skill I learn in PE in mymind at home .05 -.07 .32 .15 .64
When I am practicing a skill,I try to think how it is like something I already know .24 -.10 -.15 .00 .61
I talkto myself during practiceto helpme do better .17 .04 -.02 -.23 .57
When the teacher explains a skill,I practicethe skill in mymind .37 -.04 .18 .05 .53
1try to practiceskills I learn in PE class at home .22 . -.30 .28 .35 .49

Percent of variance 26.9 10.0 4.6 3.7 3.0


Eigenvalue 8.9 3.3 1.5 1.2 1.0

156 ROES: June 1997


Solmon andLee

To obtain estimates of internal consistency for each Interrelationships Between CPQPE Subs cales. Generally,
subscale of the CPQPE, alpha coefficients were calcu- moderate correlations characterize the relationships
lated. In the order the subscales are presented, they are among the subscales of the CPQPE. The self-regulation
.87, .75, .79, .72, and .66. subscale and the attention-concentration subscales ex-
hibited moderate, positive correlation coefficients with
Confirmatory FactorAnalysis each other as well as the three remaining subscales. A
moderately strong relationship between confidence-ef-
The indexes of fit from the CFA are presented in ficacy and willingness to engage was also evident. The
Table 2. Multiple indicators were used to assess how well use of strategies was unrelated to expressions of efficacy
the hypothesized model fit the second sample. The chi and confidence and only minimally related to willing-
square statistic has been used in the past, with a signifi- ness to engage during practice. The pattern of relation-
cant value indicating the hypothesized model is differ- ships evident among the CPQPE factors suggests the
ent from, or does not fit, the data. However, it is now subscales assess distinct but related factors that stu-
recognized that the sensitivity of the chi square statistic dents express about their experiences in physical edu-
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

to sample size and violations of model assumptions (spe- cation classes.


cifically linearity, multinormality, and additivity), have Goal Orientation. The five subscales of the CPQPE
rendered its use inappropriate (Byrne, 1989). Overly positively related to students' tendency to express a dis-
stringent adherence to the chi square statistic can pro- positional task orientation. The strongest relationship
duce over fitting of the model, or fitting the model to was evident between the self-regulation subscale, while
trivial characteristics of the data. the least substantial association was with confidence-ef-
Alternatives to the chi square statistic used in this ficacy. The relationships between the five subscales and
x
study include the 2 j df ratio, the goodness of fit index an ego-involved goal perspective were all in the nega-
(GFI), the adjusted goodness of fit (AGFI, adjusted for tive direction, but these relationships were of smaller
degrees of freedom), the total coefficient of determina- magnitude than the positive relationships to task-involve-
tion (TCD) , and the root mean square residual (RMSR). ment. The coefficients between ego-involvement and
x
A universally accepted value for the 2 j dfratio has not confidence-efficacy and willingness to engage failed to
been established, but the lower the ratio the better the reach statistical significance at the p < .001 level, and
model fit. Proposed acceptable values range from less while the remaining coefficients are statistically signifi-
than 2 to less than 5 (Byrne, 1989). Values for the GFI, cant, the relationships reflected are small.
AGFI, and the TCD indexes range from zero to 1, with Perception ofMotivational Climate. The pattern of re-
values closer to one indicating a better model fit. Val- lationships between the perception of the motivational
ues for the RMSR also range from zero to 1, but for this climate with the subscales produced an interesting par-
indicator, lower values indicate a better fit. Values of less allel to the pattern of relationships with dispositional
than .05 are considered to be indicative of a good fit goal perspective. Positive associations between the per-
(Byrne, 1989), but values between .05 and .10 are gen- ception of a mastery climate and all subscales were evi-
erally considered to be in the acceptable range. It is im- dent, as with a task-involved goal perspective. However,
portant to note that the assessment of the model fit is a the coefficients suggest that dispositional goal perspec-
subjective process in which all indicators are considered, tive has a stronger association with self-regulation and
rather than accepting or rejecting the hypothesized use of strategies, while a mastery climate has a stronger
model on the basis ofa single assessment. The results of association with confidence-efficacy.
the CFA indicate that imposing the hypothesized model The perception of a performance-oriented climate
on the second sample produced an acceptable fit. was negatively related to self-regulation, confidence-ef-
ficacy, attention-concentration, and willingness to en-
Correlational Analyses gage, but unrelated to the use ofstrategies. Though these

Correlation coefficients indicating the strength and


direction of relationships between the CPQPE Table 2. Confirmatory factor analysis indexes of fit
subscales and the other instruments are shown in
Table 3. Because of the large number of participants, Chi-square 1075.33*
coefficien ts of ±.12 are significant at the p < .001 level. Degrees of freedom ratio 485
Chi-square-degrees of freedom 2.22
Relationships of this magnitude are obviously oflimited
Goodness-of-fit index .86
practical significance, indicating that one variable ac- Adjusted goodness-of-fit index .84
counts for approximately 1.5% of the variance of the Root mean square residual .06
other measure. For this reason, the relationships are Total coefficient of determination .99
evaluated based on their relative strength rather than
levels of statistical significance. * p « .001.

ROES: June 1997 157


Solman andLee

coefficients are relatively low, they reflect stronger nega- low-up ANOVAs indicated that there were differences
tive relationships than those evident with regard to dis- between classes on all subscales: self-regulation, F(18,
positional ego-involvement. 800) = 11.72 (P< .001); confidence-efficacyF(18, 800)
Causes ofSuccess. Higher scores on the five subscales = 4.73 (P< .001); attention-concentrationF(l8, 800) =
were positively associated with the belief that success in 11.05 (P< .001); willingness to engage F(18, 800) = 10.92
physical education classes is due to motivation and effort. (P < .001); strategies F(18, 800) = 5.24 (P < .001). The
Beliefs that success in physical education is caused by intent of this analysis was to determine if students in dif-
ability or deception were negatively related to self-regu- ferent settings with different teachers responded to the
lation, confidence-efficacy, attention-concentration, and instrument in a varied fashion. It was beyond the scope
willingness to engage, but unrelated to use of strategies. of this study to draw conclusions based on comparisons
between classes, so class means are not reported.
Group Differences

The MANOVA testing for differences between el-


Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

ementary and middle school students revealed signifi- Discussion


cant differences between those groups, A = .82, F(5, 813)
= 36.42 (P < .001). The follow-up ANOVAs for each of The purpose of this paper was to describe the de-
the five subscales indicated that the elementary students velopment of an instrument for assessing the cognitive
had higher scores on each component: self-regulation, processes of studen ts in physical education classes, with
F(l, 817) = 115.15 (P < .001); confidence-efficacy F(l, the intent of providing investigators with a reliable tool
817) = 29.34 (P < .001); attention-concentration F(l, to measure cognitive variables that mediate achieve-
817) = 132.13 (P< .001); willingness to engage F(l, 817) ment. A five-factor solution emerged from the explor-
= 95.02 (p<.OOl); strategiesF(l, 817) = 8.86 (p<.002). atory factor analysis, and the confirmatory factor analysis
A MANOVA was also used to test for differences indicated that imposing the hypothesized model on a
among students in different teachers' classes, and dif- second sample of data produced an acceptable fit, con-
ferences were evident: A = .58, F(90, 4000) p « .001. Fol- firming the five-factor structure of the instrument. The

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and Pearson product moment correlation coefficients for CPQPE factor scores and othervariables

Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1.Self-regulation 1.00

2. Confidence-efficacy .35 1.00

3.Attention-concentration .64 .45 1.00

4.Willingness to engage .54 .60 .53 1.00

5. Use of strategies .52 -.04 .34 .12 1.00

6.Task orientation .50 .21 .37 .31 .31 1.00

7. Ego orientation -.14 -.05 -.15 -.08 -.13 -.12 1.00

8. Masteryclimate .41 .31 .33 .31 .22 .36 -.12 1.00

9. Performance climate -.23 -.31 -.27 -.28 -.05 -.21 .33 -.50 1.00

10. Success to motivation .45 .30 .36 .31 .22 .40 -.08 .43 -.24 1.00

11. Success to ability -.17 -.26 -.20 -.28 -.02 -.14 .27 -.20 .38 -.14 1.00

12. Success to deception -.35 -.32 -.36 -.33 -.10 -.24 .21 -.33 .44 -.45 .59 1.00

Variable means and 23.08 14.24 12.38 10.17 8.19 27.11 16.18 40.18 41.69 30.73 20.57 12.83
standard deviations (4.94) (3.08) (3.09) (2.22) (2.49) (4.82) (5.31) (6.73) (8.59) (5.81) (4.67) (4.03)
(in parentheses)

158 ROES: June 1997


Solmon andLee

alpha coefficients for each of the factors or subscales ing, a high level of attention and concentration, a will-
exceed the minimal criteria of r = .60 for measures of ingness to engage in class activities, and the use of strat-
internal consistency. However, in subsequent revisions of egies to learn skills. To a lesser degree, task-involvement
the scale, we will increase the number of items related was associated with expression of a high level of confi-
to willingness to engage and the use of strategies to in- dence and efficacy. Similar, but generally weaker, rela-
crease the reliability of these factors. tionships were evident with the perception of a
The following discussion situates this instrument mastery-oriented climate. An exception to this pattern
within the context of relevant literature. Examining the resulted on the confidence-efficacy subscale, where the
relationships between the subscales of the CPQPE and relationship between that subscale and the perception
established measures of goal perspective and beliefs of a mastery climate exceeded that of dispositional task
about success demonstrates how this instrument can involvement. This suggests that the motivational environ-
extend our understanding of the cognitive processes stu- ment structured by the teacher plays a significant role
dents employ to mediate instruction in physical educa- in reported levels of confidence and efficacy.
tion classes. We conclude by proposing prospective uses Of particular interest is the pattern evident with re-
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

of the CPQPE, providing a framework for using the in- gard to measures of an ego-involved goal perspective and
strument to answer meaningful questions about how the scores on the CPQPE subscales. Although weak nega-
teachers can structure learning environments in physi- tive relationships were evident between dispositional ego-
cal education classes to maximize cognitive engagement. involvement and some subscales of the CPQPE, those
The correlation coefficients representing the asso- negative relationships intensified when individuals per-
ciations among the subscales of the CPQPE indicate the ceived a performance-oriented climate. Not only are
factors represent distinct but related aspects of students' these results consistent with previous work, suggesting
experiences in physical education classes. The pattern that individuals in performance-oriented climates are
of relationships between the strategies subscale and the likely to focus on ability rather than effort and avoid
other factors merits special consideration, as this was the challenging tasks (Ames, 1992; Solmon, 1996), they also
only subscale not moderately related to all other suggest that a performance-oriented climate can reduce
subscales. The relationship between strategies and will- engagement in physical education classes for some indi-
ingness to engage was weak, although statistically signifi- viduals and, in this case, seemed to be a stronger influ-
cant, and confidence-efficacy was unrelated to the ence than disposi~onal ego involvement.
reported use of strategies. A parallel can be between Differences between school levels and classes suggest
these results and those reported by Solmon & Lee (1996). the CPQPE can provide valuable information about vari-
In their study, low skilled students, rather than more ables that mediate achievement in physical education
skilled ones, tended to report using strategies to learn classes. Although the intent of this study was to develop
skills. They suggested that students of higher skill levels and validate the CPQPE rather than establish differences
may not need to use strategies, especially if the skill pre- between classes, it is encouraging that students in vari-
sents little difficulty, and they pointed out that analyzing ous settings responded in varied ways. Students in differ-
strategy use at a class level may result in erroneous con- ent classes indicated differing levels of self-regulation,
clusions. Data from this study are consistent with that confidence-efficacy, attention-concentration, willingness
premise, supporting the notion that the use of strategies to engage, and use of strategies in their physical educa-
is a mediating variable that must be investigated carefully tion classes. Although the data collection for this study
in light of contextual or situational factors, entry char- did not include class observations to document different
acteristics, initial ability, and the level of task difficulty. learning environments, the differences evident between
The relationships between the CPQPE subscales and classes suggest that students' levels of cognitive engage-
measures of goal perspective and beliefs about success ment may be related to the learning context. Little re-
in physical education classes support the notion that this search has been conducted to determine how situational
instrument provides reliable, valid information about demands might affect the use of learning strategies.
students' cognitive processes in physical education The results of the analyses in this study, taken to-
classes. The subscales relate to measures of goal perspec- gether, suggest the motivational climate that teachers
tive logically, but the magnitude of the coefficients and create, and events that occur in classes, can, to some
pattern of relationships suggest that the CPQPE provides degree, supersede individual characteristics and dispo-
information not encompassed in the other measures. sitions to encourage effective cognitive engagement in
The relationship between a task-involved goal per- physical education classes. The role of the teacher in
spective and the subscales of the CPQPE is consistent with promoting self-regulatory learning has yet to be system-
a growing body of educational research that suggests task atically explored in physical education, and the develop-
orientation facilitates a high level of cognitive engage- ment of a reliable and valid scale to assess students'
ment. A high level of dispositional task orientation was cognitive processes in physical education classes is an im-
associated with behaviors reflecting self-regulated learn- portant first step in advancing this research agenda. The

RQES: June 1997 159


Solmon andLee

CPQPE should not be used as a measure of teacher ef- Marx, R. W., & Winne, P. H. (1987). The best tools teachers
fectiveness or as a single measure ofa child's motivational have-their students' thinking. In C. C. Berliner & B. V.
beliefs. Subsequent studies could, however, use the Rosenshine (Eds.) Talks to teachers (pp. 267-304). New
CPQPE in combination with observational methods to York: Random House.
Nicholls, ]. G. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement
investigate the complex nature of relationships among
motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on mo-
teachers actions, students' cognitive processes, and stu-
tivation in education: Vol. 1 Student motivation (pp. 31-56).
dent learning. Initially, the investigation of correlational New York: Academic Press.
relationships between subscales of the CPQPE and mea- Nicholls,]. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic edu-
sures of task structures, student engagement, and student cation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
achievement could lay the foundation for generating Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we
structural equation models from which to infer causal know: Verbal reports on mental process. Psychological Re-
relationships. The CPQPE could also be used with sys- view, 84,231-259.
tematic observation to investigate relationships between Papaioannou, A. (1994). Development of a questionnaire to
students' cognitive processes and a variety of teaching measure achievement orientations in physical education.
Downloaded by [University of Prince Edward Island] at 14:23 16 November 2014

approaches or strategies. In combination with student Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 11-20.
Peterson, P. L. (1988). Teachers' and students' cognitional
interviews, this instrument could be utilized to study the
knowledge for classroom teaching and learning. Educa-
antecedents of individual differences in the use of cog-
tional Researcher, 17( 5), 5-14.
nitive strategies. By using this instrument to explore the Peterson, P., & Swing, S. (1982). Beyond time on task: Stu-
ways students learn from teaching, we can increase our dents' reports of their thought processes during class-
understanding of how teachers can make informed de- room instruction. Elementary SchoolJournal, 82,481-49l.
cisions in their classes to maximize opportunities for Peterson, P. L., Swing, S. R., Stark, K. D., & Waas, G. A. (1984).
cognitive engagement that facilitate student learning. Students' cognitions and time on task during mathemat-
ics instruction. American Educational Research[ournal, 21,
487-515.
Seifriz,].]., Duda,]. L., and Chi, L. (1992). The relationship
References of perceived motivational climate to intrinsic motivation
and beliefs about success in basketball. Journal ofSport and
Exercise Psychology, 14, 375-39l.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: goals, structures, and student
Solmon, M. A. (1996). Impact of motivational climate on stu-
motivation. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 84,261-27l.
dents' behaviors and perceptions in a physical education
Ames, C., & Archer,]. (1988). Achievement goals in the class-
setting. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 88,731-738.
room: Students' learning strategies and motivational pro-
Solmon, M. A., & Boone.]. (1993). The impact of student goal
cesses. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 80, 260-267.
orientation in physical education classes. Research Quar-
Bainbridge, L. (1979). Verbal reports as evidence of the pro-
terlyfor Exercise and Sport, 64,418-424.
cess operator's knowledge. International Journal of Man-
Solmon, M. A., & Lee, A. M. (1996). Entry characteristics, prac-
Machine Studies, 11,411-436.
tice variables, and cognition: Student mediation of in-
Byrne, B. M. (1989). A primer ofUSREL: Basic applications and
struction. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 15,
programmingfor confirmatory factor analytic models. New York:
135-150.
Springer-Verlag.
SPSS, Inc. (1993). USREL 7 andPREUS. Chicago: Author.
Cronbach, L. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal struc-
Stevens,]. (1986). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sci-
ture oftests. Psychometrika, 16,297-334.
ences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Doyle, W. (1977). Paradigms for research on teacher effective-
Weinstein, C. E., Palmer, D. R., & Schulte, A. C. (1987). Learn-
ness. Review ofResearch in Education, 5, 163-198.
ing and study strategies inventory. Clearwater, FL: H & H
Duda,]. L. (1992). Motivation in sport settings: A goal perspec-
Publishing.
tive approach. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.) Motivation in Sport
Wittrock, M. C. (1986). Students' thought processes. In M. C.
and Exercise (pp. 57-91). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wittrock, (Ed.), Handbook on research on teaching (3rd ed.,
Duda,]. L., & Nicholls,]. G. (1992). Dimensions of achieve-
pp. 297-314). New York: Macmillan.
ment motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal ofEdu-
cationalPsychology, 84,290-299.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data.
Psychological Review, 87, 215-25l.
Howard, G. S. (1981). On validity. Evaluation Review, 5, 567-576. Authors' Note
Lee, A. M., Landin, D. K., & Carter, ]. A. (1992). Student
thoughts during tennis instruction. Journal of Teaching in Please address all correspondence concerning this article
Physical Education, 11, 256-267. to Melinda A. Solmon, Department of Kinesiology, Uni-
Lee, A. M., & Solmon, M. A. (1992). Cognitive conceptions of versity of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742.
teaching and learning motor skills. Quest, 44, 57-7l.
Locke, L. E, &]ensen, M. K. (1974). Thought sampling: A study E-mail: ms344@umail.umd.edu
of student attention through self-report. The ResearchQuar-
terly, 45, 263-275.

160 ROES: June 1997

S-ar putea să vă placă și