Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Qual Life Res

DOI 10.1007/s11136-016-1403-6

The fatter are happier in Indonesia


Kitae Sohn1

Accepted: 25 August 2016


Ó Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Abstract Introduction
Purpose Although obesity and happiness are known to be
negatively related in the developed world, little attention Although obesity is generally associated with the devel-
has been paid to this relationship in the developing world. oped world, people in the developing world are no longer
We thus investigated the relationship in Indonesia and safe from it [1, 2]. Obesity is of concern because it is
attempted to explain the underlying rationale. related to diverse chronic diseases such as cardiovascular
Methods We considered about 12,000 respondents aged disease, Type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, stroke,
15? for each gender obtained from the Indonesian Family dyslipidemia, osteoarthritis, and several types of cancer. As
Life Survey 2007 by relating a measure of happiness to a result, obese people (BMI C 30) incur medical costs
weight-related measures in ordered probit models. which are about 30 % higher than those incurred by people
Results The relationship between obesity and happiness with a BMI \ 25 [3]. Obesity is not merely a health issue.
was positive in Indonesia, and this relationship was robust. Obesity is negatively related to labor market outcomes,
Our evidence suggests that the contrasting results for the particularly among women [4]. As a result, obesity inevi-
two worlds result from affordability of obesity. That is, tably influences socioeconomic aspects, such as income
while even low socioeconomic status (SES) individuals in and health inequalities, health insurance, disability benefits,
the developed world can afford to be obese, only high SES taxation, and negative externalities [5, 6].
individuals in the developing world can do. When obesity is negatively related to health and eco-
Conclusions Our findings imply that obesity prevention in nomic outcomes, it naturally influences various aspects of
the developing world requires different measures than life. Happiness is often used as a summary measure of life
those used in the developed world. evaluation, where happiness is defined as the degree to
which a person positively evaluates the overall quality of
Keywords Obesity  Happiness  Developing country  his present life as a whole [7]. Happiness is appealing
Socioeconomic status  Indonesia Family Life Survey  because it is generally the ultimate goal in life, while
Culture income, which is a typical measure of life evaluation in
economics, is a means to achieve happiness. In addition,
happiness, as part of quality of life, considers the totality
of life, whereas income concerns only one aspect of life.
By examining how obesity relates to happiness, one can
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this understand the pervasive effects of obesity on life. As
article (doi:10.1007/s11136-016-1403-6) contains supplementary
material, which is available to authorized users. expected from the negative relationship of obesity to
health and economic outcomes, many studies have found
& Kitae Sohn that in general, obesity is negatively related to happiness
ksohn@konkuk.ac.kr
(or life satisfaction) and positively to depression.
1
Department of Economics, Konkuk University, 120 Research has demonstrated this relationship in the USA
Neungdong-ro, Gwangjin-gu, Seoul 05029, South Korea [8–11], Britain [12], Switzerland [13], Germany [14–16],

123
Qual Life Res

and Finland [17]. All studies involved developed Main variables


countries.1
In contrast, two well-known facts suggest that the rela- Happiness was measured by the response to the following
tionship between obesity and happiness is likely to be question: ‘‘Taken all things together how would you say
positive in the developing world. First, obesity and things are these days—would you say you were very
socioeconomic status (SES) are positively related in the happy, pretty happy, or not too happy?’’ This question is
developing world [18–20]. Second, SES and happiness are identical to that of the US General Social Survey and is
positively related there [21–24]. These two facts suggest nearly identical to that of the Euro-barometer Survey Ser-
that affordability of obesity differs between the two worlds. ies. Note that the IFLS question referred to three possible
In the developed world, even low SES individuals can responses (very happy, pretty happy, and not too happy) in
afford to be obese if they so desire. In fact, obesity is more itself, but respondents chose one of four possible responses
prevalent among low, rather than high, SES individuals (very happy, happy, unhappy, and very unhappy). We
[25]. In contrast, in the developing world, it is not evident combined the categories of unhappy and very unhappy
whether low SES individuals can afford to be obese even if because only 0.29 % of men and 0.33 % of women in our
they wanted to. In this case, obesity can be a costly signal sample said that they were very unhappy (Table 1). We
of high status, as only high SES people can afford [26]. assigned a value of 1 to very unhappy and unhappy, 2 to
Following this conjecture, we aimed to determine how happy, and 3 to very happy. The variable of happiness in
obesity was related to happiness in Indonesia, a large IFLS4 appears to be valid because factors that are known to
populous developing country. Furthermore, unlike other be associated with happiness in other countries have been
studies, we examined men and women separately because also associated with happiness in IFLS4 to similar degrees
there is no a priori reason to assume that the relationship is [21–23, 27].
the same for both genders. Weight was measured by trained nurses using Seca
Model 770 scales. Various measures are available to
classify obesity, but BMI is widely used in the social sci-
Methods ences for its simplicity and availability [28]. Following the
World Health Organization’s suggestion [29], we used the
Source of data following BMI cut-off points for Asians: underweight
defined as BMI \ 18.5, normal weight as
The main dataset was the Indonesian Family Life Survey 18.5 B BMI \ 23, overweight as 23 B BMI \ 25, and
(IFLS), an ongoing longitudinal survey. Data collection for obesity as BMI C 25. We used BMI in not linear but
the first IFLS survey (IFLS1) began in 1993, involving dummy form to detect any nonlinear relationship between
over 22,000 individuals from 7224 households in 13 pro- BMI and happiness. BMI in IFLS4 was accurate because
vinces. The IFLS selected 13 provinces to maximize rep- specially trained nurses measured height and weight,
resentation of the population (83 %), capture the cultural thereby reducing measurement error and bias in self-re-
and socioeconomic diversity within Indonesia, and be cost- ported height and weight. We considered a range of
effective. A total of 321 enumeration areas were randomly covariates (listed in ‘‘Statistical analysis’’ section) and
selected from the 13 provinces. Within a selected enu- presented the descriptive statistics of these variables in
meration area, households were randomly selected. Four Table 1. Appendix B describes all covariates, and for more
follow-ups were conducted in 1997 (IFLS2), 1998 information, an interested reader may consult the user’s
(IFLS2?), 2000 (IFLS3), and 2007 (IFLS4). Respondent manual available at http://www.rand.org/labor/FLS/IFLS.
attrition was not a serious issue: the re-contact rate of the html. Appendix C explains why we used BMI and the
original dynasties was 93.6 % in IFLS4. Although the obesity cut-off point.
survey is longitudinal, we only analyzed IFLS4 because it
was the only survey to include a variable of happiness and Inclusion and exclusion criteria
other variables of interest, not included in the other follow-
ups. In IFLS4, 29,055 respondents aged 15? provided valid
answers to the happiness question. We only included
respondents who received a secular, rather than Islamic,
education to ensure greater sample homogeneity. We fur-
1
The World Bank uses gross national income per capita to classify ther excluded a small number of respondents whose highest
economies into four income categories. In this study, developed
education level was difficult to compare with others’ (adult
countries correspond to the upper-middle- or high-income economies,
and developing countries to the lower-middle- or low-income education and open university) and another small number
economies. of respondents with missing and unreasonable values (to

123
Qual Life Res

Table 1 Descriptive statistics


Gender Men Women
% or Mean (SD) % or Mean (SD)

Happiness
Very happy 6.1 % 6.8 %
Happy 85.0 % 85.4 %
Unhappy 8.7 % 7.5 %
Very unhappy 0.29 % 0.33 %
Height (cm) 162.1 (6.5) 150.8 (5.8)
Weight (kg) 57.1 (10.7) 52.5 (10.4)
BMI (kg/m2) 21.7(3.6) 23.0 (4.2)
Underweight (BMI \ 18.5) 16.9 % 12.4 %
Normally weighted (18.5 B BMI \ 23) 53.2 % 42.8 %
Overweight (23 B BMI \ 25) 13.3 % 15.5 %
Obese (BMI C 25) 16.6 % 29.3 %
Demographic variable
Age 37.2 (15.4) 36.8 (15.3)
Years of schooling 8.5 (4.3) 7.7 (4.6)
Single, separated, divorced, or widowed 29.0 % 30.1 %
Married 71.1 % 69.9 %
Non-Javanese 58.0 % 59.0 %
Javanese 42.0 % 41.0 %
Non-Islamic 11.3 % 11.1 %
Islamic 88.7 % 88.9 %
Not working 15.5 % 42.5 %
Working 84.5 % 57.5 %
Rural residence 45.8 % 45.2 %
Urban residence 54.2 % 54.8 %
Province fixed effects (not listed but controlled for)
Socioeconomic status variable
Consumption of eggs per week (days) 3.0 (2.1) 2.9 (2.1)
Consumption of fish per week (days) 3.6 (2.4) 3.5 (2.5)
Consumption of meat per week (days) 1.8 (1.8) 1.6 (1.8)
Consumption of dairy products per week (days) 1.4 (2.3) 1.4 (2.4)
Ln(Wealth) 17.11 (2.00) 17.09 (2.06)
Subjective wealth level 1 5.5 % 4.8 %
Subjective wealth level 2 24.4 % 23.7 %
Subjective wealth level 3 53.4 % 53.0 %
Subjective wealth level 4 15.5 % 17.2 %
Subjective wealth level 5 1.1 % 1.0 %
Subjective wealth level 6 0.18 % 0.22 %
Objective health variable
Grip strength (kg) 36.44 (9.81) 22.97 (7.93)
Lung capacity (L) 3.84 (0.94) 2.61 (0.66)
Hemoglobin level (g/dL) 14.63 (1.65) 12.60 (1.46)
No hypertension 73.9 % 74.9 %
Hypertension 26.1 % 25.1 %
# Words recalled 8.9 (3.7) 8.5 (3.9)
Subjective health variable
# Depression symptoms 3.1 (1.6) 3.2 (1.7)
# Acute diseases 2.1 (2.0) 2.3 (2.1)
Very healthy 11.3 % 9.6 %

123
Qual Life Res

Table 1 continued
Gender Men Women
% or Mean (SD) % or Mean (SD)

Somewhat healthy 76.5 % 74.9 %


Somewhat unhealthy 12.0 % 15.2 %
Unhealthy 0.26 % 0.32 %
Did not have a general check-up in the past 5 years 85.2 % 86.2 %
Have a general check-up in the past 5 years 14.8 % 13.8 %
Did not visit a doctor in the past 4 weeks 89.7 % 80.8 %
Visited a doctor in the past 4 weeks 10.3 % 19.2 %
Do not have health benefits or insurance 72.3 % 74.6 %
Have health benefits or insurance 27.7 % 25.4 %
Na 11,926 12,910
a
Sample sizes slightly varied depending on covariates

list only a few, outside the following ranges, After estimating the relationship between obesity and
120 cm B height B 200 cm, 30 kg B weight B 150 kg, happiness, we attempted to understand the relationship by
10 B BMI B 40, 70 mm Hg B systolic blood pres- using the following specification:
sure B 300 mm Hg, and 50 mm Hg B diastolic blood wi ¼ c1 Underwti þ c2 Obesei þ Demoi c3 þ ei ð2Þ
pressure B 200 mm Hg). The final sample size was about
12,000 observations for each gender. where wi refers to the goodness of individual i’s life, the c
to coefficients to estimate, and e to an error term. wi
Statistical analysis includes each element of SES, Obj.Health, and Sub-
j.Health, one at a time. For continuous and dichotomous
Since the measure of happiness was ordinal, we used an dependent variables, we used OLS; for multi-level (more
ordered probit model. This model facilitates comparisons than 2) discrete dependent variables, we used an ordered
between our results and those in the literature. We esti- probit model and reported the mean marginal effects for the
mated the following model: response of very happy. Note that all dependent variables
in this specification were covariates in specification (1).
yi ¼ b0 Underwti þ b1 Overwti þ b2 Obesei þ xi b3 þ ei ; and
Thus, by estimating specification (2), we do not argue that
ei jUnderwti ; Overweighti ; Obesei ; xi  Normalð0; 1Þ these factors explained the remaining portion of the rela-
ð1Þ tionship between obesity and happiness. The remaining
portion was likely to be driven by unobservable factors,
where yi is the latent variable of individual i’s happiness.
and by definition, we could not control for them. The
Underwt refers to being underweight, Overwt to being
purpose of specification (2) was to suggest that obese
overweight, and Obese to being obese; the normal-weight
people enjoyed better lives than non-obese people in terms
group is the reference group. x is a vector of covariates,
of SES and health, and consequently, they were happier.
including demographic variables (Demo), SES variables
This argument implies that unobservable factors responsi-
(SES), objective health indicators (Obj.Health), and sub-
ble for the remaining portion were those positively related
jective health indicators (Subj.Health). We considered
to a good life. Each dependent variable is not a perfect
these sets of covariates because they are known to be
indicator of SES or health, but collectively, they can draw a
closely related to happiness in general [30] and in
reasonable picture of one’s life.
Indonesia [21–23, 31], and because, as revealed below,
they played an important role in explaining the positive
relationship between obesity and happiness. e refers to a
random error, and the b to the coefficients to estimate; b2 Results
was the coefficient of interest. We applied sampling
weights to all estimations to make them nationally repre- Descriptive results
sentative. We also clustered standard errors at the county
level to account for possible correlation within a county. Table 1 presents descriptive statistics. Because the number
Because an ordered probit is a nonlinear model and the b of variables was large, we briefly explained only some
are difficult to interpret, we presented the mean marginal variables. Most Indonesians were happy, which reduced
effects for the response of very happy. variation in our dependent variable. This feature reinforces

123
Qual Life Res

our argument that the statistically significant positive overweight because they were not statistically significant in
relationship was not a false positive. Indonesians were almost all estimations. Therefore, in a statistical sense, the
short (on average, 162 cm for men and 151 cm for women) reference group consisted of normal-weight persons;
and light (on average, 57 kg for men and 53 kg for however, the reference group essentially consisted of
women). Men and women exhibited a large difference in underweight, normal-weight, and overweight persons. We
height, but not as much in weight. The mean BMI was thus collectively referred to these three groups as the non-obese
greater in women than in men. Underweight and obese group. For illustration purposes, Table A-1 in Appendix A
individuals coexisted in Indonesia: 16.9 % of men and presents the results with the full set of covariates (i.e.,
12.4 % of women were underweight, while 16.6 % of men Columns 5 and 10 of Table 2).
and 29.3 % of women were obese. When we added only a constant (Column 1), relative to
Figure 1 provides an intuitive understanding of the rela- normal-weight men, obese men are 3.25 % points (or
tionship between BMI and happiness by gender. BMI is 53.7 % relative to the mean) more likely to say very happy.
categorized in 1 kg/m2, and the circles represent BMI-hap- When we introduced Demo (Column 2), b2 decreased by
piness cells. The diameter of each circle is proportional to the 43 %. When we added SES, b2 further decreased by 30 %
cell size. The added lines are fitted regression lines, weighted (Column 3); objective wealth and subjective wealth
by cell sizes. The average level of happiness was almost the explained most of the reduction. Although these two
same for both genders, but the slope of the relationship was variables did not represent all SES variables, we doubt that
steeper for men than for women, suggesting that happiness adding more SES variables would reduce b2 discernably
was more closely related to BMI in men than in women. This because we already controlled for a critical SES variable,
finding anticipates our baseline results. i.e., subjective wealth [32]. Furthermore, when we added
SES variables related to the consumption of protein-rich
Baseline results foods, b2 only slightly decreased.
When we added Obj.Health, b2 decreased by 7 %
Table 2 lists the marginal effect of obesity on happiness, (Column 4), and adding Subj.Health caused b2 to decrease
and each column heading indicates the set of covariates by 13 % (Column 5). As a result, b2 in Column 5 was much
added to those in the previous column. Panel A lists the smaller than in Column 1. However, the size of b2 in
results for men, and Panel B for women. We incrementally Column 5 was not small: relative to normal-weight men,
added sets of covariates to assess the degree to which each obese men were still 1.00 % points (or 17 % relative to the
set affected the association between obesity and happiness, mean) more likely to say very happy. This positive rela-
while omitting their coefficients to save space. We also tionship between obesity and happiness stands in stark
omitted the coefficients on being underweight and contrast to the negative relationship in the developed

Fig. 1 Relation between BMI


and happiness. Notes: BMI is
categorized in 1 kg/m2, and the
circles represent BMI-happiness
cells. The diameter of each
circle is proportional to the cell
size. The added lines are fitted
regression lines weighted by
cell sizes

123
Qual Life Res

Table 2 Marginal effect of


Covariates 1 2 3 4 5
obesity on happiness by gender:
Constant ?Demo ?SES ?Obj.Health ?Subj.Health
baseline results
Panel A: men
BMI C 25 (/10) 0.325 0.185 0.129 0.120 0.105
(0.043)*** (0.041)*** (0.038)*** (0.040)*** (0.041)**
N 11,926 11,924 11,924 11,387 11,387
Pseudo R2 0.005 0.048 0.075 0.074 0.091
6 7 8 9 10
Panel B: Women
BMI C 25 (/10) 0.083 0.114 0.078 0.057 0.051
(0.036)** (0.037)*** (0.037)** (0.039) (0.038)
N 12,909 12,909 12,909 12,608 12,608
Pseudo R2 0.001 0.042 0.065 0.068 0.080
The reference group was individuals with 18.5 B BMI \ 23. We omitted the coefficients on BMI \ 18.5
and 23 B BMI \ 25. We applied cross-section person weights with attrition correction. Standard errors
clustered at the county level are in parentheses
* p value \0.10; ** p value \0.05; *** p value \0.01

world. Furthermore, the reduction in b2 owing to the Obesity and a good life
introduction of covariates implies that compared to non-
obese men, obese men enjoyed higher SES and better In Appendix D, we reinforced our evidence that obesity
health and consequently were happier. We elaborated this was positively related to happiness among Indonesians and
implication in the next subsection. The decreasing rate of argued that this relationship was not explained by the two
the reduction in b2 and the large number of covariates following accounts applicable to the developed world: the
suggest that adding more observable variables would not low willpower of the obese and the norm of obesity. So,
markedly reduce b2. what makes the obese in Indonesia happier? The previous
Different results were found in women (Panel B). When subsection provided the reason for women, but some por-
we added only a constant (Column 6), obese women were tion of the positive relationship for men remain to be
happier than normal-weight women by 0.83 % points explained. In the previous subsection, we suggested that
(12.2 % relative to the mean). When we added Demo unobservable factors made Indonesian men both obese and
(Column 7), b2 increased, but the inclusion of SES reduced happier. The positive relationship between obesity and SES
b2 (Column 8). When we added Obj.Health, b2 was no in the developing world suggests that the obese in
longer statistically significant, not even weakly (Column Indonesia enjoy better lives than the non-obese. This sub-
9). This result remained the same when we added Sub- section presents evidence consistent with this. This evi-
j.Health (Column 10). Hence, the relationship between dence is of importance because it implies that only high-
obesity and happiness for women was also positive, but ability people can afford to be obese.
unlike men, it was largely explained by Demo, SES, and Table 3 considers SES indicators in specification (2).
Obj.Health. Columns 1–3 show that relative to normal-weight men,
Because we entered Subj.Health after Obj.Health, it is obese men ate eggs for 0.21 more days (the coefficient
unclear which set of covariates was responsible for the should be divided by 10 for this and others), fish for 0.17
statistical non-significance of b2. Hence, after controlling more days, and meat for 0.35 more days per week.
for Demo and SES, we added Obj.Health and Subj.Health Although these numbers appear small, they were not small
one at a time (Table A-2 in Appendix A). Obj.Health relative to the means: 7.4, 5.1, and 20.5 %, respectively.
reduced b2 more than Subj.Health (to 0.56 % points vs. The size was the largest for meat, which is a relatively
0.68 % points), but even when we added Subj.Health, b2 expensive commodity in Indonesia. There was, however,
became weakly statistically significant. Thus, the choice of little difference between obese and non-obese men
sets of health covariates was not critical. We thus argue regarding the consumption of dairy products (Column 4).
that in women, health explained the positive relationship In addition to the consumption of protein-rich foods, obese
between obesity and health, although demographic and men were wealthier than non-obese men, in terms of both
SES factors also contributed to the explanation. objective or subjective wealth. Relative to normal-weight

123
Qual Life Res

Table 3 Relationship between


Dep. var. 1 2 3 4 5 6
obesity and socioeconomic
Egg Fish Meat Dairy Ln(Wealth) Subj. Wealth
status for men
BMI \ 18.5 (/10) -0.799 -1.016 -0.731 0.861 -0.764 -0.012
(0.466)* (0.448)** (0.434)* (0.513) (0.623) (0.012)
18.5 B BMI \ 23 Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
23 B BMI \ 25 (/10) 0.193 0.313 1.327 -0.135 1.937 0.038
(0.514) (0.741) (0.485)*** (0.795) (0.596)*** (0.018)**
BMI C 25 (/10) 2.119 1.681 3.501 0.192 5.169 0.051
(0.402)*** (0.676)** (0.491)*** (0.617) (0.583)*** (0.014)***
Mean dep. var. 2.86 3.32 1.71 1.35 17.12 0.014
2
Pseudo or adj. R 0.082 0.169 0.139 0.086 0.113 0.042
The sample size was 11,924. We controlled for all demographic variables. We applied cross-section person
weights with attrition correction. Standard errors clustered at the county level are in parentheses
* p value \0.10; ** p value \0.05; *** p value \0.01

men, obese men were 68 % (=exp(0.5169) - 1) wealthier men had weaker grip strength, smaller lung capacity, and
and 0.51 % points (41 % relative to the mean) more likely smaller hemoglobin levels.
to feel that they were the richest. Not surprisingly, under- Regarding subjective health indicators, obese men and
weight men ate fish (possibly eggs and meat as well) fewer normal-weight men did not exhibit a statistically significant
days per week than normal-weight men. difference for the number of symptoms of depression and the
Health indicators also suggest that life is better for obese number of acute diseases although c2 displayed the expected
than non-obese men. Table 4 shows objective health indi- sign (Columns 1 and 2). Relative to normal-weight men,
cators and Table 5 subjective health indicators. c2 was however, obese men were 1.8 % points more likely to report
generally positive and statistically significant. Columns that they were very healthy (Column 3), 5.0 % points more
1–3 of Table 4 show that relative to normal-weight men, likely to have had a general check-up in the past five years
obese men had 2.4 kg greater grip strength, 0.20 L greater (Column 4), 2.2 % points more likely to have visited a doctor
lung capacity, and 0.54 g/dL greater hemoglobin levels. over the past four weeks (Column 5), and 3.7 % points more
Relative to the mean, each was 6.5, 5.1, and 3.7 %. Rela- likely to have health benefits or health insurance (Column 6).
tive to normal-weight men, obese men were 23 % points Relative to the mean, each was 16, 34, 22, and 13 %. Rela-
more likely to be hypertensive (Column 4) but recalled 0.4 tive to normal-weight men, underweight men suffered more
more words; relative to the mean, each was 89 and 4.8 %. acute diseases, reported to be less healthy, and were less
Conversely, relative to normal-weight men, underweight likely to undergo a general check-up.

Table 4 Relationship between obesity and objective health indicators for men
Dep. var. 1 2 3 4 5
Grip strength (kg) Lung capacity (L) Hemoglobin (g/dL) Hypertension # of words recalled

BMI \ 18.5 (/10) -27.99 -3.276 -3.498 -0.788 -0.656


(2.05)*** (0.309)*** (0.487)*** (0.105)*** (0.917)
18.5 B BMI \ 23 Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
23 B BMI \ 25 (/10) 14.71 1.241 3.419 1.321 3.260
(1.99)*** (0.267)*** (0.468)*** (0.093)*** (1.068)***
BMI C 25 (/10) 23.81 1.956 5.401 2.316 4.306
(2.30)*** (0.235)*** (0.419)*** (0.129)*** (0.785)***
Mean dep. var. 35.40 3.747 14.52 0.284 8.61
N 11,873 11,530 11,809 11,924 11,924
Pseudo or adj. R2 0.391 0.302 0.147 0.164 0.370
We controlled for all demographic variables. We applied cross-section person weights with attrition correction. Standard errors clustered at the
county level are in parentheses
* p value \0.10; ** p value \0.05; *** p value \0.01

123
Qual Life Res

Table 5 Relationship between obesity and subjective health indicators for men
Dep. var. 1 2 3 4 5 6
Depression Symptoms Acute Diseases Healthy General Check-Up Visit Doctor Insurance

BMI \ 18.5 (/10) 0.508 1.854 -0.146 -0.226 0.149 0.148


(0.321) (0.521)*** (0.064)** (0.081)*** (0.091) (0.148)
18.5 B BMI \ 23 Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
23 B BMI \ 25 (/10) -0.552 0.609 0.100 0.138 0.145 0.253
(0.448) (0.618) (0.055)* (0.098) (0.108) (0.122)**
BMI C 25 (/10) -0.486 -0.461 0.176 0.503 0.222 0.366
(0.444) (0.593) (0.064)*** (0.112)*** (0.091)** (0.135)***
Mean dep. var. 3.06 2.01 0.117 0.137 0.106 0.271
Pseudo or adj. R2 0.048 0.045 0.043 0.067 0.017 0.074
The sample size was 11,924. We controlled for all demographic variables. We applied cross-section person weights with attrition correction.
Standard errors clustered at the county level are in parentheses
* p value \0.10; ** p value \0.05; *** p value \0.01

Tables 3, 4, and 5 demonstrate that obesity was posi- The positive relationship in Indonesia suggests that
tively related to SES and health. Therefore, life looked explanations for the negative relationship between obesity
promising for obese men, while the opposite was true for and happiness in the developed world do not apply to
underweight men. Even if one wanted to be overcome by Indonesia. Our evidence suggests that the positive rela-
the temptation of overeating, not all people could afford it. tionship can be explained by improved SES and health
Recall that only 16.6 % of men were obese even with a enjoyed by the obese. These findings have important
BMI of 25 (instead of 30). If obesity represents SES, not implications for the rationality of obesity and public poli-
many people are in a position to be overcome by the cies for obesity prevention in the developing world. As the
temptation. Weak willpower in the face of the temptation is higher prevalence of hypertension among the obese shows
a luxury in Indonesia. (Table 4), obesity does pose a health threat in Indonesia
For each dependent variable, c2 was sometimes small [33]. Therefore, obesity prevention is highly desirable. The
relative to the mean. However, when the relationships for positive relationship between obesity and happiness in
all dependent variables are considered together, the total Indonesia, however, suggests that obesity prevention in the
sum should be substantial. All dependent variables suggest developing world would be more challenging than antici-
that unobservable aspects of life were likely to be much pated. Since this study has not established causality, we do
better for obese than non-obese men. It seems that obesity not argue that a reduction in obesity would automatically
in Indonesia is a luxury good that only high SES people can result in less happiness. Granted, one point is clear: the
purchase. These facts suggest that obesity is a costly signal obese in Indonesia are content with their lives and happier
of high status; it is costly in the sense that obese people than the non-obese. In this situation, it would be difficult to
spend more money on food and suffer more from chronic encourage them to lose weight, particularly if obesity is a
diseases. Otherwise, high SES people, who can afford both costly signal of high status. Although our data did not
slimness and fatness, would be slim. provide direct evidence of this, anthropologists have long
understood this aspect of obesity in traditional societies and
developing countries [34, 35: Chaps. 9–10]. According to
Discussion them, when food shortages are common, slim people are to
be pitied, whereas fat people are to be admired. Fatness is a
This study is not the first to find a positive relationship symbol of health, prestige, and prosperity, and people want
between obesity and happiness in Indonesia. Sohn already to be fat to survive hunger in a risky world. Although we
reported it [21, 22], but he paid scanty attention to it did not locate an anthropological study discussing this
because his aim was to identify factors that were strongly mechanism specifically in the Indonesian context, it is
associated with happiness [21] or to understand why the likely that this mechanism applies to Indonesia, given the
taller were happier [22]. Consequently, he did not check widespread malnutrition in children, the relatively late
the robustness of the relationship between BMI and hap- menarche of adolescent girls, and the short stature of adults
piness, which we did above. He also did not present (two symptoms of malnutrition in childhood) [22, 36–46].
comparisons with developed countries or possible mecha- As demonstrated by the social stigma attached to obesity in
nisms, which we did below. the developed world, however, these symbolic and survival

123
Qual Life Res

values of fatness can be reversed once food is no longer Acknowledgments I am grateful to the three anonymous reviewers
scarce. When low SES people can afford to be obese in the for helpful comments and suggestions.
developed world, high SES people purchase slimness Compliance with ethical standards
through costly manners, such as restrained eating, investing
in exercise, and purchasing expensive nutritious food. Conflict of interest All authors have no conflict of interest.
This logic suggests that as long as food is scarce (that is,
Ethical standard Ethics approval was granted from the Institutional
low SES people are underweight), high SES people are Review Board at Rand Corporation (USA) and from the Ethics
unlikely to lose weight on their own. Fatness is admired, Committee at Universitas Indonesia (Indonesia) for the first wave and
and thinness in low SES people is a constant reminder of the Ethics Committee at Universitas Gadjah Mada (Indonesia) for the
the immediate possibility of hunger and starvation. Relat- next three waves.
edly, there appears to be little social pressure on them to
lose weight. This situation is in stark contrast to the
developed world, where thinness is admired, the diet
industry flourishes, and social campaigns constantly pro- References
mote obesity prevention. When willpower is low and social
pressure is weak, obesity does not disappear easily. 1. Prentice, A. M. (2006). The emerging epidemic of obesity in
developing countries. International Journal of Epidemiology,
Therefore, obesity prevention alone would be ineffec- 35(1), 93–99.
tive in the developing world as long as low SES people 2. Popkin, B. M., Adair, L. S., & Ng, S. W. (2012). Global nutrition
remain underweight. Clearly, we need different approaches transition and the pandemic of obesity in developing countries.
than those used in the developed world. For example, the Nutrition Reviews, 70(1), 3–21.
3. Withrow, D., & Alter, D. A. (2011). The economic burden of
emphasis on reducing the risk of chronic diseases in the obesity worldwide: A systematic review of the direct costs of
future would not be an effective strategy for people who obesity. Obesity Reviews, 12(2), 131–141.
face immediate food shortages. Similarly, ‘‘sin’’ taxes on 4. Johansson, E., Bockerman, P., Kiiskinen, U., & Heliovaara, M.
fat and sugar and a reduction in meat consumption may (2009). Obesity and labour market success in Finland: The dif-
ference between having a high BMI and being fat. Economics &
help obese people lose weight, but harm malnourished Human Biology, 7(1), 36–45.
people. Furthermore, the promotion of exercise among 5. Finkelstein, E. A., Ruhm, C. J., & Kosa, K. M. (2005). Economic
people, many of whom are malnourished, is not an causes and consequences of obesity. Annul Review of Public
attractive strategy. In environments such as these, curtail- Health, 26, 239–257.
6. Bhattacharya, J., & Bundorf, M. K. (2009). The incidence of the
ing malnutrition in low SES people by addressing food healthcare costs of obesity. Journal of Health Economics, 28(3),
scarcities could prevent high SES people from becoming 649–658.
obese. This strategy is appealing because it can also 7. Veenhoven, R. (2013). The four qualities of life: Ordering con-
address the dual burden of the disease. At the same time, cepts and measures of the good life. In A. Dell aFave (Ed.), The
exploration of happiness: Present and future perspectives (pp.
we should consider the possibility that once food is easily 67–95). Dordrecht: Springer.
affordable, low SES people become obese while high SES 8. Robert, R. E., William, S. J., Stephane, D., & George, K. A.
people become slim. Future research needs to develop (2002). Are the fat more jolly? Annals of Behavioral Medicine,
obesity prevention programs that are effective and sus- 24(3), 169–180.
9. Graham, C., & Felton, A. (2005). Inequality and happiness:
tainable in the developing world. Insights from Latin America. Journal of Economic Inequality,
Despite our contributions to the literature, we 4(1), 107–122.
acknowledge some limitations. First, the relationship 10. Forste, R., & Moore, E. (2012). Adolescent obesity and life sat-
between obesity and happiness in this study was based on isfaction: Perceptions of self, peers, family, and school. Eco-
nomics & Human Biology, 10(4), 385–394.
correlation rather than causality. If plausible instrumental 11. Wadsworth, T., & Pendergast, P. M. (2014). Obesity (sometimes)
variables (IVs) had been available, we could have esti- matters: The importance of context in the relationship between
mated the causal effects of obesity on happiness. Some obesity and life satisfaction. Journal of Health and Social
previous attempts notwithstanding, we could not find Behavior, 55(2), 196–214.
12. Oswald, A. J., & Powdthavee, N. (2007). Obesity, unhappiness,
plausible IVs. Future research may find plausible IVs and and the challenge of affluence: Theory and evidence. Economic
place the relationship on firmer ground. However, since our Journal, 117(521), 441–454.
main arguments were not based on causality, the lack of 13. Stutzer, A. (2009). Happiness when temptation overwhelms
causality should not undermine the value of this study. willpower. In A. K. Dutt & B. Radcliff (Eds.), Happiness, eco-
nomics, and politics: Toward a multi-disciplinary approach (pp.
Second, although we provided some evidence that obesity 97–126). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
acts as a costly signal, the evidence was circumstantial. 14. Blanchflower, D. G., Landeghem, B., & Oswald, A. J. (2009).
Future research may use clever experiments to obtain Imitative obesity and relative utility. Journal of the European
definitive evidence. Economic Association, 7(2–3), 528–538.

123
Qual Life Res

15. Clark, A. E., & Etilé, F. (2011). Happy house: Spousal weight 32. Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. A. (2008). Relative
and individual well-being. Journal of Health Economics, 30(5), income, happiness, and utility: An explanation for the easterlin
1124–1136. paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature,
16. Katsaiti, M. S. (2012). Obesity and happiness. Applied Eco- 46(1), 95–144.
nomics, 44(31), 4101–4114. 33. Sohn, K. (2015). Sick but unaware: Hypertension in Indonesia.
17. Böckerman, P., Johansson, E., Saarni, S. I., & Saarni, S. E. Biodemography and Social Biology, 61(3), 298–318.
(2014). The negative association of obesity with subjective well- 34. Brown, P. J. (1991). Culture and the evolution of obesity. Human
being: Is it all about health? Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(4), Nature, 2(1), 31–57.
857–867. 35. Wells, J. C. K. (2010). The evolutionary biology of human body
18. Hruschka, D. J., & Brewis, A. A. (2013). Absolute wealth and fatness: Thrift and control. Cambridge: Cambridge University
world region strongly predict overweight among women (ages Press.
18–49) in 360 populations across 36 developing countries. Eco- 36. Rachmi, C. N., Agho, K. E., Li, M., & Baur, L. A. (2016).
nomics & Human Biology, 11(3), 337–344. Stunting, underweight and overweight in children aged
19. Sohn, K. (2014). Job strenuousness and obesity: The case of a 2.0–4.9 years in Indonesia: Prevalence trends and associated risk
developing country. Journal of Development Studies, 50(9), factors. PLoS One, 11(5), e0154756.
1289–1301. 37. Sohn, K. (2014). Age and size at maturity in Indonesian women:
20. Sohn, K. (2014). Sufficiently good measures of obesity: The case A norm of reaction? American Journal of Human Biology, 26(5),
of a developing country. Journal of Biosocial Science, 46(6), 713–715.
797–817. 38. Sohn, K. (2015). Is leg length a biomarker of early life condi-
21. Sohn, K. (2013). Sources of happiness in Indonesia. Singapore tions? Evidence from a historically short population. American
Economic Review, 58(2), 1350014. Journal of Human Biology, 27(4), 538–545.
22. Sohn, K. (2016). Height and happiness in a developing country. 39. Sohn, K. (2015). The height premium in Indonesia. Economics &
Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(1), 1–23. Human Biology, 16, 1–15.
23. Sohn, K. (2016). The role of spousal income in the wife’s hap- 40. Sohn, K. (2015). The influence of birth season on height: Evi-
piness. Social Indicators Research, 126(3), 1007–1024. dence from Indonesia. American Journal of Physical Anthro-
24. Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2013). Subjective well-being and pology, 157(4), 659–665.
income: Is there any evidence of satiation? American Economic 41. Sohn, K. (2015). The value of male height in the marriage mar-
Review, 103(3), 598–604. ket. Economics & Human Biology, 18, 110–124.
25. Sobal, J., & Stunkard, A. J. (1989). Socioeconomic status and 42. Sohn, K. (2016). Biological standards of living: Age at menarche
obesity: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, vs. height. Annals of Human Biology. doi:10.3109/03014460.
105(2), 260–275. 2016.1147596.
26. Spence, M. (1973). Job market signaling. Quarterly Journal of 43. Sohn, K. (2016). Improvement in the biological standard of living
Economics, 87(3), 355–374. in 20th century Korea: Evidence from age at menarche. American
27. Sohn, K. (2016). Does a taller husband make his wife happier? Journal of Human Biology. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22882.
Personality and Individual Differences, 91, 14–21. 44. Sohn, K. (2016). The influence of climate on age at menarche:
28. Ruhm, C. J. (2012). Understanding overeating and obesity. Augmented with the influence of ancestry. HOMO, 67(4),
Journal of Health Economics, 31(6), 781–796. 328–336.
29. World Health Organization. (2000). The Asia-Pacific perspective: 45. Sohn, K. (2015). Is age at menarche a good predictor of future
Redefining obesity and its treatment. Geneva: World Health body fat? The case of a developing country. Health Care for
Organization. Women International, 16, 1–19.
30. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psy- 46. Sohn, K. (2015). The trend in age at menarche in Indonesia: Birth
chological Science, 7(3), 181–185. cohorts 1944–1988. Journal of Biosocial Science, 47(3),
31. Sohn, K. (2013). Monetary and nonmonetary returns to education 407–412.
in Indonesia. Developing Economies, 51(1), 34–59.

123

S-ar putea să vă placă și