Sunteți pe pagina 1din 14

PERSONALITY

10.1177/0146167204264292
Schimmack et al.AND
/ PERSONALITY
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
AND LIFE BULLETIN
SATISFACTION

Personality and Life Satisfaction: A Facet-Level Analysis

Ulrich Schimmack
University of Toronto, Mississauga
Shigehiro Oishi
University of Minnesota
R. Michael Furr
Appalachian State University
David C. Funder
University of California, Riverside

At the global level of the Big Five, Extraversion and Neuroticism happy life with many pleasant and few unpleasant experi-
are the strongest predictors of life satisfaction. However, ences and high life satisfaction. One important finding
Extraversion and Neuroticism are multifaceted constructs that in the SWB literature has been the demonstration that
combine more specific traits. This article examined the contribu- SWB is moderately stable over time and influenced by
tion of facets of Extraversion and Neuroticism to life satisfaction personality traits (Diener & Lucas, 1999). One consis-
in four studies. The depression facet of Neuroticism and the posi- tent finding has been a strong link between Extraversion
tive emotions/cheerfulness facet of Extraversion were the stron- and Neuroticism and SWB (cf. Diener & Lucas, 1999).
gest and most consistent predictors of life satisfaction. These two Despite considerable progress, several questions about
facets often accounted for more variance in life satisfaction than the relation between personality and life satisfaction
Neuroticism and Extraversion. The findings suggest that mea- remain. Few studies have examined whether other per-
sures of depression and positive emotions/cheerfulness are neces- sonality traits also are related to SWB, and even fewer
sary and sufficient to predict life satisfaction from personality studies have compared the predictive validity of
traits. The results also lead to a more refined understanding of Extraversion and Neuroticism to those of other person-
the specific personality traits that influence life satisfaction: ality traits (cf. DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). Hence, it is cur-
Depression is more important than anxiety or anger and a rently unclear whether Extraversion and Neuroticism
cheerful temperament is more important than being active or are sufficient or necessary to capture personality influ-
sociable. ences on SWB. The present article takes a closer look at
this question by comparing the Big Five to a more
comprehensive set of more specific personality traits.
Keywords: personality; life satisfaction; Big Five; subjective well- The Components of Subjective Well-Being (SWB)
being; depression
SWB researchers distinguish an affective and a cogni-
Research in the past 20 years has made considerable tive component. The affective component of SWB is
defined as the hedonic balance of pleasant and unpleas-
progress in the assessment of personality traits (Costa &
McCrae, 1992; Goldberg, 1990, 1997). Structural analy- ant experiences. An individual with many pleasant and
ses of personality traits in diverse cultures reveal five
g l o b a l d i m e ns i o ns o f i nd i v i d u a l d i ffe r e nc e s : Authors’ Note: The data gathering for Study 3 was supported by Na-
Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, tional Institute of Mental Health Grant R01-MH42427 to David C.
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (McCrae & Funder. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
Costa, 1997). These dimensions are known as the Big to Ulrich Schimmack, Department of Psychology, University of
Toronto, Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, On-
Five. tario, L5L 1C6, Canada; e-mail: uli.schimmack@utoronto.ca.
Researchers also have made progress in the study of
PSPB, Vol. 30 No. 8, August 2004 1062-1075
subjective well-being (SWB; Diener & Larsen, 1984; DOI: 10.1177/0146167204264292
Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). High SWB implies a © 2004 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

1062
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1063

few unpleasant experiences has high SWB. In contrast, relation between personality traits and life satisfaction
an individual with few pleasant and many unpleasant may even refine the definition of the affective compo-
experiences has low SWB. Personality traits have a strong nent of SWB. For example, Schimmack (2003) found
influence on the affective component of SWB. that aggregated experiences of happiness were a better
Neuroticism is in part a disposition to experience more predictor of life satisfaction than a combined aggregate
anger, fear, and sadness, and Neuroticism also is related of happiness, pride, and affection, suggesting that
to frequent experiences of other unpleasant emotions happiness should be weighted more heavily in a measure
(Izard, Libero, Putnam, & Haynes, 1993; Watson & of the affective component of SWB.
Clark, 1992). Extraversion is a more multifaceted con-
struct, but one important facet of Extraversion is the dis- Personality and Life Satisfaction
position to experience more pleasant emotions (Costa & In studies of the relation between personality traits
McCrae, 1992; Izard et al., 1993; Watson & Clark, 1992). and life satisfaction, Neuroticism and Extraversion typi-
Not surprisingly, Extraversion and Neuroticism are cally emerge as the strongest predictors (Diener &
strong predictors of the affective component of SWB (cf. Lucas, 1999; Schimmack, Diener, & Oishi, 2002;
Diener & Lucas, 1999). This finding is by no means triv- Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, Dzokoto, & Ahadi,
ial. For example, Costa and McCrae (1980) demon- 2002). However, this conclusion is limited by several
strated that personality measures predicted hedonic bal-
factors. First, most of the studies relied exclusively on
ance 10 years later. Costa and McCrae proposed an
self-report measures. Hence, previous studies may have
influential model in which Extraversion leads to more
overestimated the strength of the relation between per-
pleasant experience and Neuroticism leads to more
sonality traits and life satisfaction due to the influence of
unpleasant experiences. This model implies that
response sets and styles (cf. Schimmack, Bockenholt, &
Extraversion and Neuroticism are important determi-
Reisenzein, 2002). Second, previous studies have
nants of SWB. However, this model focused exclusively
assessed personality at the level of broad and global per-
on the affective component of SWB.
sonality traits. It is possible that these studies have over-
Relatively few studies have examined the influence of
looked the contribution of more specific personality
personality traits on the cognitive component of SWB.
traits to life satisfaction.
The cognitive component of SWB is assessed with life sat-
For example, Neuroticism represents the shared vari-
isfaction judgments (e.g., “I am satisfied with my life.”).
ance between anxiety, hostility, and depression but it
Life satisfaction items ask respondents for a subjective
does not represent the unique variance in anxiety, hostil-
evaluation of their lives. Respondents can use any infor-
ity, and depression that is not shared with the other
mation that they deem relevant. Hence, in theory, life
dimensions. It is possible that some of this specific vari-
satisfaction judgments could be influenced by a variety
ance is related to life satisfaction. Furthermore, it is pos-
of personality traits. For example, if people judged life
sible that some of the more specific dimensions are more
satisfaction based on their incomes, conscientiousness
highly correlated with life satisfaction than are the global
would be a predictor of life satisfaction because consci-
dimensions used in previous investigations. In the most
entiousness predicts higher incomes (Judge, Higgins,
extreme case, it is possible that a single specific dimen-
Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999).
sion can account for the relation between personality
The main aim of the present study is to provide a
more detailed examination of the personality traits that and life satisfaction.
influence life satisfaction. This article focuses on the cog- Hierarchical Taxonomies of Personality
nitive component of SWB for a simple reason. The affec-
tive component of SWB is defined by a researcher. If a Scientific taxonomies of individual differences are
researcher defines hedonic balance as the balance of faced with a trade-off between bandwidth and fidelity or
happiness and sadness, then personality dispositions to parsimony and precision (Hampson, John, & Goldberg,
experience happiness and sadness are by definition the 1986; John, Hampson, & Goldberg, 1991; Paunonen,
most important predictors. If another researcher 1998; Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). Increasing the num-
includes anxiety and anger in the definition and assess- ber of dimensions allows a more accurate description of
ment of hedonic balance, then dispositions to experi- individual differences but makes the assessment of indi-
ence these emotions are bound to be additional predic- vidual differences more laborious. The Big Five offer a
tors. In contrast, life satisfaction judgments are solution to this problem. Assessing fewer than five
subjective judgments that allow respondents to use what- dimensions lowers accuracy because the five dimensions
ever information they deem relevant to evaluate their provide largely independent information about differ-
own lives. Hence, there is no a priori relation between ent aspects of individual differences. Assessing more
personality traits and life satisfaction. Examining the than five dimensions decreases parsimony because addi-
1064 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

tional dimensions tend to be intercorrelated and Extraversion that predicts life satisfaction was activity
provide partially redundant information. level. In this case, researchers would examine other vari-
However, the optimal number of dimensions ulti- ables such as achievements, career choices, and so forth.
mately depends on the weights attached to parsimony Similarly, different aspects of Neuroticism suggest differ-
and precision. If precision were at a premium, it would ent theories of SWB. The facets anxiety and vulnerability
be desirable to increase the number of dimensions to would suggest responsiveness to stress and uncertainty as
describe individual differences even if these dimensions determinants of life satisfaction, whereas the depression
were somewhat redundant (i.e., they share variance with facet suggests a lack of meaning and purpose in life as
each other). Indeed, standard personality question- causes of low life satisfaction.
naires such as the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) The search for lean predictors also can lead to a
and the 300-item version of the IPIP (Goldberg, 1997) leaner assessment of personality in future studies of life
allow researchers to describe personality in terms of the satisfaction. If the personality variance in life satisfaction
Big Five or in terms of six specific facets of each of the Big could be assessed with a few lean scales, researchers
Five dimensions. Henceforth, we refer to these scales as could routinely include personality measures in their
the Lean Thirty. studies, although it is impossible to include the NEO-PI-
Due to the hierarchical relation between the Big Five R or the IPIP-300 in each study. To illustrate, a complete
and the Lean Thirty, the latter scales contain all of the assessment of the Big Five with the NEO-PI-R or the IPIP
information about the Big Five dimensions. At the same takes about 45 min. Few researchers would be willing to
time, they also provide additional information that is lost include these measures if they were not genuinely inter-
by aggregating them into the Big Five. Paunonen (1998) ested in personality. However, the assessment of a single
warned that “aggregating personality traits into their lean dimension takes only about 2 min. If a few lean
underlying personality factors could result in decreased dimensions were sufficient to capture the personality
predictive accuracy due to the loss of trait-specific but variance in life satisfaction, more researchers could rou-
criterion-valid variance” (p. 538). Hence, researchers tinely assess these dimensions in their studies. The inclu-
may be discarding important information when they first sion of personality measures in more studies of SWB
assess the Lean Thirty and then aggregate lean scales would be helpful to address the third-variable problem
into the Big Five. Similarly, shorter questionnaires that in correlational research. For example, a researcher may
assess only the global Big Five dimensions may fail to find a significant correlation between financial satisfac-
include more specific personality factors that influence tion and life satisfaction, suggesting that financial satis-
life satisfaction. To address these concerns, it is impor- faction influences life satisfaction. The interpretation of
tant to examine the relation between the Lean Thirty this finding would change dramatically if this correlation
and life satisfaction. were no longer significant after controlling for a person-
A better understanding of the relation between the ality trait (e.g., Neuroticism). In this case, it would be
Lean Thirty and life satisfaction provides several benefits more likely that personality influences life satisfaction
for future SWB research. First, the results of these studies and financial satisfaction.
are necessary for more refined theories of the personal-
Theoretical Predictions
ity influences on SWB. The main advantage of broad
constructs such as Neuroticism and Extraversion is that We had several predictions about personality traits
they predict a wide variety of behaviors and experiences that influence life satisfaction. These predictions were
with a relatively small set of personality dimensions. largely derived from the mediator model of personality
However, broad traits are more difficult to understand influences on life satisfaction (Schimmack, Diener, et al.,
than are more specific traits. As noted by Costa and 2002; Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, et al., 2002). The
McCrae (1998), the understanding of a broad trait like mediator model assumes that Extraversion and
Neuroticism is based on the specific traits that define it. Neuroticism have a strong influence on the affective
Hence, finding the specific traits of a global factor that component of SWB. Extraverts have a more positive
relate most closely to life satisfaction leads to a better hedonic balance (pleasant minus unpleasant affect)
understanding of personality influences on life than introverts, and neurotic individuals have a more
satisfaction. negative hedonic balance than emotionally stable indi-
For example, imagine that the main facet of viduals (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Diener & Lucas, 1999).
Extraversion that predicts life satisfaction was warmth/ The mediator model also assumes that people rely on
friendliness. In this scenario, researchers would pursue their hedonic balance to judge life satisfaction. People
future research by examining the role of social relations, who have a more positive hedonic balance judge their
interactions, friendships, and so forth, as determinants lives to be more satisfying. Finally, the mediator model
of life satisfaction. Now, imagine that the main facet of postulates that the influence of Extraversion and
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1065

Neuroticism on life satisfaction is almost completely Griffin, 1985). The SWLS is a 5-item scale with good
mediated by hedonic balance; that is, Neuroticism and psychometric properties. The items are (a) “In most ways
Extraversion predict life satisfaction because they influ- my life is close to ideal,” (b) “The conditions of my life
ence affective experiences and people rely on affective are excellent,” (c) “I am satisfied with my life,” (d) “So far
experiences to judge life satisfaction. Based on this I have gotten the important things I want in my life,” and
model, we can predict that facets of Extraversion and (e) “If I could live my life over, I would change almost
Neuroticism that are dispositions to experience pleasant nothing.” Responses were made on a 7-point Likert-type
or unpleasant emotions should be most closely related to scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
life satisfaction. For Neuroticism, these are the facets It is important to note that the SWLS items do not share
anxiety, anger, and depression (Costa & McCrae, 1992). content with the personality items. None of the SWLS
Although we expect all three facets to be correlated with items mention frequent experiences of happiness or
life satisfaction, we also predicted that the depression depression. Participants completed the SWLS at the
facet should be the strongest predictor of life satisfac- beginning of the semester but not at the same time as the
tion. This prediction is based on the assumption that sui- personality measure. They repeated SWLS ratings two
cide attempts are an indicator of low life satisfaction and times in monthly intervals.
the finding that depression is a stronger predictor of sui- Participants distributed questionnaires with the
cide attempts than anxiety (Placidi, Oquendo, Malone, SWLS items to two friends and two family members. Most
Brodsky, Steven, & Mann, 2000; Scocco, Marietta, of the family reports were completed by parents and sib-
Tonietto, Buono, & De Leo, 2000). lings. The informants rated participants’ life satisfaction
For Extraversion, the facet most closely related to and mailed completed questionnaires to the experi-
affective experiences is the positive-emotions facet in the menters. Both self-reports and informant reports of
NEO-PI-R and the equivalent cheerfulness facet in the SWLS had good reliabilities (Cronbach’s α > .80). Fur-
IPIP (Goldberg, 1997). People with a cheerful tempera- thermore, families’ and friends’ reports showed moder-
ment have more positive affective experiences, leading ate convergent validity. The correlation between aver-
to higher levels of life satisfaction. We also predict that aged family reports and friend reports was r = .36. We
the excitement-seeking facet of Extraversion does not combined families’ and friends’ reports to increase the
predict life satisfaction. This prediction is based on validity of informant reports.
empirical data (Oishi, Schimmack, & Diener, 2001) and
the value-as-a-moderator model (Oishi, Diener, Suh, & Results
Lucas, 1999). The influence of excitement-seeking
on life satisfaction depends on situational factors. Simple correlations. Table 1 shows the correlations
Excitement-seekers have high life satisfaction when their between the global and specific personality dimensions
lives are filled with excitement, but they have low life sat- of Extraversion and Neuroticism with life satisfaction.
isfaction when nothing exciting is happening. On aver- Consistent with previous findings, Extraversion and
age, excitement-seeking is not related to life satisfaction Neuroticism were significant predictors of life satisfac-
just like other values and goals are moderators of situa- tion. Furthermore, all subscales of Neuroticism were
tional effects on life satisfaction but not directly related negatively correlated with life satisfaction. However, not
to life satisfaction. all of the Extraversion subscales were consistently
related to life satisfaction. Only warmth, gregariousness,
STUDY 1
and positive emotions were consistently correlated with
life satisfaction. Depression was more highly related to
Method life satisfaction than was Neuroticism and positive emo-
Participants. The data of this study were obtained tions were more highly related to life satisfaction than
from 136 students at the University of Illinois, Urbana- was Extraversion.
Champaign. The sample included 100 female and 36 Stepwise regression analyses. To examine the unique con-
male participants who were on average 20 years old. Stu- tribution of Extraversion and Neuroticism facets to life
dents participated in this data collection as part of a satisfaction, we conducted a series of stepwise regression
course on personality. analysis. These analyses start with the strongest predictor
Materials and procedure. During the first weeks of the and add additional predictors if they explain significant
class, participants completed the NEO-PI-R (Costa & additional variance in the criterion. We set the inclusion
McCrae, 1992). This 240-item questionnaire assesses the criterion to p = .10. Table 2 shows that depression and
Big Five and six facets of the Big Five with 8 items for each positive emotions were the first two predictors in all anal-
facet. Life satisfaction was assessed with the Satisfaction yses with self-reports as criterion. Depression was also the
With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & first predictor in the analyses with informant reports of
1066 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 1: Simple Correlations Between Personality Traits and Life TABLE 3: Incremental Changes in Explained Variance in a Stepwise
Satisfaction Regression Analysis With Global and Specific Predictors

SWLS SWLS
Personality Traits T1 T2 T3 Informant T1 T2 T3 Informant
Step/Predictors ∆R2 ∆R
2
∆R
2
∆R
2

Neuroticism –.45* –.50* –.44* –.29*


Anxiety –.22* –.25* –.21* –.08 1. Neuroticism and
Angry/hostility –.34* –.40* –.35* –.22* Extraversion .24* .26* .22* .13*
Depression –.52* –.52* –.49* –.38* 2. Depression and
Self-consciousness –.35* –.38* –.37* –.22* positive emotions .08* .09* .07* .05*
Impulsivity –.19* –.23* –.16 –.12 1. Depression and
Vulnerability –.31* –.33* –.30* –.21* positive emotions .32* .33* .29* .17*
Extraversion .33* .27* .29* .31* 2. Neuroticism and
Warmth .27* .25* .25* .28* Extraversion .00 .02 .00 .02
Gregariousness .26* .19* .28* .27*
2
Assertiveness .21* .12 .11 .20* NOTE: ∆R = change in explained variance. Significance was tested by
Activity .23* .16 .19* .24* partial F tests. SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, T2 =
Excitement-seeking –.03 –.04 .01 –.01 Time 2, T3 = Time 3.
Positive emotions .40* .41* .38* .28* *p < .05.

NOTE: SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2,


T3 = Time 3. life satisfaction above and beyond depression and posi-
*p < .05. tive emotions. These analyses demonstrate that two fac-
ets of Extraversion and Neuroticism are better predic-
tors of life satisfaction than are Extraversion and
TABLE 2: Incremental Explained Variance in Stepwise Regression Neuroticism themselves.
Analysis

Step/Predictor ∆R
2 What about the other Big Five dimensions? We also exam-
ined whether personality dimensions belonging to the
Self-report T1 other three dimensions of the Big Five added to the pre-
1. Depression .27 diction of life satisfaction. Simple correlations revealed
2. Positive emotions .05
some positive relations of Conscientiousness with life sat-
Self-report T2
1. Depression .26 isfaction. However, partial correlations revealed that
2. Positive emotions .08 Conscientiousness did not explain unique variance in
3. Excitement-seeking .02 life satisfaction after controlling for the influences of
Self-report T3 positive emotions and depression (see Table 4).
1. Depression .24
2. Positive emotions .04 A method-free estimate of personality influences on life satis-
Informant reports
1. Depression .14
faction. One shortcoming of many previous studies was
2. Warmth .04 the exclusive reliance on self-reports to assess personal-
ity and SWB. Table 3 reveals clear evidence for method-
NOTE: T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2, T3 = Time 3. specific variance. Whereas depression and positive emo-
tions explained about 30% of the variance in self-
reported life satisfaction, the two personality traits
explained only 17% of the variance in informant reports
life satisfaction as criterion. However, warmth rather
of life satisfaction. To estimate the contribution of per-
than positive emotions was the second predictor.
sonality traits to life satisfaction independent of method,
Global versus specific predictors. We conducted hierarchi- we compared the simple correlation between self-
cal regression analyses to examine the predictive validity reports and informant reports of life satisfaction to the
of global and specific personality dimensions (see Table part (or semipartial) correlation between the two life sat-
3). In the first analyses, we entered Extraversion and isfaction measures while controlling for depression and
Neuroticism in the first step and then entered depres- positive emotions. The simple correlation between self-
sion and positive emotions in the second step. This anal- reports and informant reports of life satisfaction was r =
ysis was repeated for all four measures of life satisfaction. .49 (N = 136). The part correlation was r = .31. These cor-
The next analyses reversed the order of the predictors. relations imply that the two life-satisfaction measures
Depression and positive emotions predicted life satisfac- share 25% (i.e., .492) of their total variances and 10%
tion above and beyond Extraversion and Neuroticism. (i.e., .312) of their total variances are not shared with the
However, Extraversion and Neuroticism failed to predict personality variables. Hence, 15% (i.e., 25% – 10%) of
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1067

TABLE 4: Simple and Partial Correlations With Other Big Five related to life satisfaction. Rather, people who are prone
Dimensions to experience more anxiety and anger are also likely to
SWLS experience more depression. This shared risk to experi-
Personality Traits T1 T2 T3 Informant ence more unpleasant emotions in general is captured
in the global trait of Neuroticism, which is strongly
Simple correlations related to life satisfaction. However, a specific disposi-
Openness .16 .05 .07 –.04 tion to experience more anxiety or anger that is not
Agreeableness .14 .17* .15 .06
Conscientiousness .25* .20* .16 .28*
related to more experiences of depression is not related
Partial correlations to life satisfaction, whereas a specific disposition to expe-
Openness .02 –.13 –.08 –.16 rience more depression is related to life satisfaction.
Agreeableness .03 .06 .05 –.03 Hence, it is the global disposition to experience more
Conscientiousness .07 .01 –.03 .16 unpleasant emotions and a specific disposition to experi-
NOTE: Depression and positive emotions were the covariates in the ence more depression that are important for the predic-
partial correlations. SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, tion of life satisfaction. Because both components are
T2 = Time 2, T3 = Time 3. captured in the depression scale, the depression scale is
*p < .05.
both necessary and sufficient for predicting the influ-
ence of Neuroticism-related facets on life satisfaction.
The results also provide important insight into the
the shared variance between the two life satisfaction facets of Extraversion that account for the relation
measures is explained by depression and positive emo- between Extraversion and life satisfaction, although the
tions. In other words, the personality variables account evidence was stronger for self-reported life satisfaction
for 60% (i.e., .15/.25) of the shared variance between than for informant reports of life satisfaction. Consistent
the two life satisfaction measures. The estimate dropped with previous findings and our predictions, excitement-
to 45% when Extraversion and Neuroticism were used to seeking failed to show significant simple correlations
estimate the contribution of personality to life satisfac- with life satisfaction. Hence, the excitement-seeking
tion. In sum, these analyses reveal that depression and facet of Extraversion cannot explain the significant con-
positive emotions account for a large portion of the tribution of Extraversion to life satisfaction. Also consis-
method-invariant variance in life satisfaction. tent with predictions, the positive emotions facet was the
strongest predictor of self-reported life satisfaction.
Discussion Other facets showed significant simple correlations,
which were no longer significant after controlling for
Study 1 examined the personality predictors of life
the shared variance with other Extraversion facets.
satisfaction at two levels of specificity. Consistent with
These findings suggest that the positive emotion facet of
previous findings, Extraversion and Neuroticism were
Extraversion is sufficient to account for the relation
significant predictors of life satisfaction when personal-
between Extraversion and life satisfaction. This finding
ity was assessed at the level of the Big Five (Diener &
has important implications for theories of the link
Lucas, 1999). Analyses at the level of the Lean Thirty
between Extraversion and life satisfaction. Some theo-
revealed that positive emotions and depression were
ries suggest that Extraversion enhances well-being
strong predictors of life satisfaction. Comparisons across
because Extraversion leads to better social relationships.
levels revealed that depression was a stronger predictor
Other theories assume that Extraversion enhances well-
of life satisfaction than was Neuroticism, and positive
being because it is a disposition to have a more cheerful
emotions was a stronger predictor of life satisfaction
temperament. The present findings are more consistent
than was Extraversion, although the latter finding did
with the latter hypothesis.
not hold for informant reports of life satisfaction.
In sum, Study 1 suggested that depression and posi-
The results demonstrate that a more precise assess-
tive emotions are necessary and sufficient to account for
ment of personality at the level of lean facets increased
the relation between personality, as measured by the
the variance that personality traits explain in life satisfac-
NEO-PI-R, and life satisfaction. Study 1 also suggested
tion. Furthermore, the findings deepen the understand-
that these two personality traits account for 60% of the
ing of subjective well-being. Previous theories of SWB
method-invariant variance in life satisfaction.
implicitly assumed that all negative emotions are equally
detrimental for SWB. However, the present findings sug-
gest that a disposition for depression has more severe STUDY 2
effects on life satisfaction than dispositions for other The main purpose of Study 2 was to replicate the find-
unpleasant emotions such as anxiety and hostility. This ings of Study 1 with an alternative personality measure.
finding does not imply that the latter emotions are not Whereas Study 1 used the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae,
1068 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

1992), Study 2 used the IPIP (Goldberg, 1997). TABLE 5: Simple Correlations Between Personality Traits and Life
Satisfaction
Although both questionnaires are designed to assess the
same constructs and show high convergent validity, they SWLS
use different items. If the results of Study 1 can be repli- Personality Traits T1 T2 Informant
cated with a different instrument, it is more likely that
the results are based on the underlying constructs and Neuroticism –.49* –.43* –.26*
not on superficial characteristics of the items used to Anxiety –.40* –.31* –.15
Anger –.38* –.35* –.21*
assess the underlying constructs. Depression –.57* –.55* –.37*
Method Self-consciousness –.35* –.32* –.24*
Immoderation –.23* –.16 –.04
Participants. The data of this study were obtained Vulnerability –.38* –.28* –.19*
Extraversion .42* .35* .30*
from 124 students at the University of Illinois, Urbana-
Friendliness .43* .37* .33*
Champaign. The sample included 88 female and 36 Gregariousness .21* .16 .18*
male participants, who were on average 21 years old. Assertiveness .36* .33* .25*
Activity level .17 .14 .06
Materials and procedure. During the first weeks of the
Excitement-seeking .11 .03 .09
class, participants completed the 300-item IPIP Cheerfulness .51* .46* .32*
(Goldberg, 1997). The questionnaire assesses the six
subscales of the Big Five with 10 items each. The ques- NOTE: SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2.
*p < .05.
tionnaire was designed to assess the same personality
dimensions as the NEO-PI-R, although some dimensions
are named differently (e.g., friendliness = warmth). Most
important, the positive emotions scale of the NEO-PI-R
corresponds to the cheerfulness scale in the IPIP. depression in the prediction of informant reports of life
Although it may be confusing for readers, we decided to satisfaction. In sum, the results replicated Study 1’s find-
use Goldberg’s label when we refer to this facet of ing that depression and cheerfulness are the most
Extraversion in the IPIP. Participants also completed the important predictors of life satisfaction.
standard SWLS (Diener et al., 1985) at the beginning Global versus specific predictors. The analyses followed
(Time 1 [T1]) and the end of the semester (Time 2 the procedure of Study 1. We conducted hierarchical
[T2]). Participants distributed questionnaires with the regression analyses to examine the predictive validity of
SWLS items to two friends and two family members. The global and specific personality dimensions (see Table 7).
informants rated participants’ life satisfaction and In the first analyses, we entered Extraversion and
mailed completed questionnaires to the experimenters. Neuroticism in the first step and then entered depres-
The psychometric properties of these measures were sion and cheerfulness in the second step. The next analy-
similar to those in Study 1. ses reversed the order of the predictors. Depression and
Results cheerfulness predicted life satisfaction above and
beyond Extraversion and Neuroticism. However,
Simple correlations. Table 5 shows the correlations Extraversion and Neuroticism failed to predict life satis-
between the global and specific personality dimensions faction above and beyond depression and cheerfulness.
and life satisfaction. Consistent with previous findings, These analyses demonstrate that depression and cheer-
Extraversion and Neuroticism were significant predic- fulness are necessary and sufficient predictors of life
tors of life satisfaction. Furthermore, nearly all facets of satisfaction.
Extraversion and Neuroticism were significantly corre-
lated with life satisfaction. However, the magnitude of What about the other Big Five dimensions? We also exam-
these correlations varied greatly. Depression and cheer- ined whether personality dimensions belonging to the
fulness were the only subscales that were more highly other three dimensions of the Big Five added to the pre-
correlated with life satisfaction than were Extraversion diction of life satisfaction. Simple correlations revealed
and Neuroticism. positive relations of Openness to Experience and Con-
scientiousness with life satisfaction. However, partial cor-
Regression analyses. Regression analyses followed the relations that controlled for cheerfulness and depres-
same procedure as in Study 1 (see Table 6). The results sion were no longer significant (see Table 8).
for self-reported life satisfaction replicated the findings
of Study 1. Depression and cheerfulness were the first A method-free estimate of personality influences on life satis-
two predictors in stepwise regression analyses. In Study faction. As in Study 1, we used simple and part correla-
2, cheerfulness was also the second predictor after tions between self-reports and informant reports of life
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1069

TABLE 6: Incremental Explained Variance in Stepwise Regression TABLE 8: Simple and Partial Correlations With Other Big Five
Analysis Dimensions
2
Step/Predictor ∆R SWLS
Personality Traits T1 T2 Informant
Self-report T1
1. Depression .32 Simple Correlations
2. Cheerfulness .07 Openness .18 .22* –.07
Self-report T2 Agreeableness .14 .19* .00
1. Depression .30 Conscientiousness .35* .33* .14
2. Cheerfulness .04 Partial correlations
Informant reports Openness –.04 .06 –.25*
1. Depression .13 Agreeableness .03 .12 –.09
2. Cheerfulness .03 Conscientiousness .13 .11 –.04
NOTE: T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2. NOTE: Depression and cheerfulness were the covariates in the partial
correlations. SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, T2 =
Time 2.
*p < .05.
TABLE 7: Changes in Explained Variance in a Hierarchical Regres-
sion Analysis With Global and Specific Predictors

SWLS
2
Step/Predictors T1 ∆R T2 ∆R2 Informant ∆R2 Discussion
1. Neuroticism and Study 2 replicated the key findings of Study 1 with an
Extraversion .28* .20* .10* alternative measure of personality. This finding is impor-
2. Depression
and cheerfulness .11* .15* .06*
tant because increasing numbers of predictors increase
1. Depression the likelihood of a Type I error (Paunonen, 1998). The
and cheerfulness .39* .35* .16* results of Study 2 rule out this possibility. Even when per-
2. Neuroticism sonality dimensions were assessed with a different ques-
and Extraversion .00 .01 .01
tionnaire, the same lean dimensions as in Study 1 pre-
2
NOTE: ∆R = change in explained variance. Significance was tested by dicted life satisfaction. Moreover, Study 2 found that
partial F tests. SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale, T1 = Time 1, T2 = depression and cheerfulness were the first two predic-
Time 2.
*p < .05. tors of informant reports of life satisfaction.

STUDY 3

satisfaction to examine the method-invariant contribu- Study 3 extends the previous two studies by using mul-
tion of personality to life satisfaction. To increase the tiple personality measures, whereas life satisfaction was
validity of the self-report measures, we averaged the two assessed only by self-reports. The data were collected as
self-report measures. The simple correlation between part of the Riverside Accuracy Project (Funder, 1995;
self-reports and informant reports of life satisfaction was Furr & Funder, 1998). In this project, students rated
r = .57 (N = 124). The part correlation was r = .41 (N = their personality on the complete NEO-PI-R. In addi-
124). These correlations imply that the two life satisfac- tion, parents and peers also rated students’ personality
tion measures share 33% (i.e., .572) of their total vari- on the NEO-PI-R. As a result, it was possible to examine
ances and 17% (i.e., .412) of their total variances are not whether the previous findings would generalize to per-
shared with the personality variables. Hence, 16% (i.e., sonality measures based on informant reports. Study 3
33% to 17%) of the shared variance between the two life also addressed another limitation of the previous stud-
satisfaction measures is explained by depression and ies. Namely, most participants in the previous studies
cheerfulness. In sum, the personality variables account were women. Hence, it is uncertain whether the results
for 49% (i.e., .16/.33) of the shared variance between generalize to male participants. Indeed, Herringer
the two life satisfaction measures. The estimate dropped (1998) suggested that the assertiveness facet of
to 30% when Extraversion and Neuroticism were used to Extraversion predicts life satisfaction for men but not for
estimate the contribution of personality to life satisfac- women. Study 3 included a larger number of male partic-
tion. In sum, the analyses replicated the findings of Study ipants, which allowed examining the possibility of sex
1 that depression and cheerfulness account for about differences in the contribution of personality traits to life
half of the method-invariant variance in life satisfaction. satisfaction.
1070 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 9: Simple Correlations Between Personality Ratings by Self, TABLE 10: Incremental Explained Variance in Stepwise Regression
Peers, and Parents With Life Satisfaction Analysis
2
Self Peers Parents Step/Predictor ∆R
Personality Traits (N = 143) (N = 129) (N = 108)
Self-report
Neuroticism –.48* –.23* –.27* 1. Depression .27
Anxiety –.33* –.16 –.23* 2. Positive emotions .08
Anger/hostility –.31* –.10 –.17 3. Vulnerability .04
Depression –.52* –.28* –.18 Peer-report
Self-consciousness –.40* –.22* –.28* 1. Positive emotions .14
Impulsivity –.03 –.13 –.10 2. Depression .04
Vulnerability –.47* –.18* –.22* Parent reports
Extraversion .42* .33* .24* 1. Assertiveness .13
Warmth .25* .25* .14 2. Anger/hostility .03
Gregariousness .31* .26* .21*
Assertiveness .35* .20* .36*
Activity .08 .15 .11
Excitement-seeking .22* .15 –.05 personality replicated the importance of depression and
Positive emotions .41* .37* .27*
positive emotions, which were the only predictors that
*p < .05. added unique variance. However, parent reports failed
to replicate this pattern. Rather parents’ ratings of asser-
tiveness and anger/hostility were the two predictors with
Method unique incremental variance.

Participants. The data of this study were obtained as Global versus specific predictors. A set of hierarchical
part of the Riverside Accuracy Project (Funder, 1995; regression analyses compared the predictive power of
Furr & Funder, 1998). The total sample included 146 Neuroticism and Extraversion with the predictive power
participants (82 women, 64 men). Due to missing data, of the depression and positive emotions facets (see Table
the correlations are based on smaller subsets of partici- 11). Self-reports replicated previous findings that the
pants (see Table 9). facets produced a significant increase in explained vari-
ance in addition to Extraversion and Neuroticism. How-
Materials and procedure. Participants completed the ever, in contrast to the previous studies, Extraversion and
NEO-PI-R and rated life satisfaction on the SWLS Neuroticism also contained unique variance that was not
(Diener et al., 1985). In addition, parents and peers explained by the facets. Peer reports were more consis-
completed informant reports of participants’ personal- tent with theoretical predictions: Facets produced an
ity on the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). increase in predictive validity but global scales did not
Results contain any unique variance. Parent ratings showed a
different pattern. Neither subscales nor global scales
Simple correlations. Table 9 shows the correlations contained any significant unique variance.
between self-reports of life satisfaction and the three sets
of personality ratings. The self-ratings of personality rep- Sex Differences
licate the findings in Studies 1 and 2, namely, depression Herringer (1998) found that assertiveness predicted
and positive emotions were the facets that correlated life satisfaction for men but not for women. We ran three
most highly with life satisfaction. Peer reports showed regression analyses to test this prediction. In each regres-
the same pattern. However, parent reports showed the sion model, sex and assertiveness were entered as predic-
highest correlations for self-consciousness and assertive- tors before the product of sex and assertiveness was
ness. Parent reports also failed to demonstrate a signifi- entered in a second step. A significant increase in
cant correlation between depression and life satisfac- explained variance would indicate that assertiveness
tion, whereas the expected relation between positive interacts with sex in the prediction of life satisfaction.
emotions and life satisfaction was significant. Self-rated assertiveness failed to replicate Herringer’s
Stepwise regression analyses. To clarify the unique contri- (1998) results, R2 change = .01, F(1, 139) = 1.58, p = .21.
bution of lean dimensions, we computed stepwise Parent and peer ratings of assertiveness also did not pro-
regression analyses (see Table 10). Self-ratings repli- duce significant interactions with sex in the prediction
cated earlier findings that depression and positive emo- of life satisfaction, F < 1.00. We also found no evidence
tions were the first two predictors entered in the equa- that sex moderated the relationship between
tion. In addition, vulnerability emerged as a third Extraversion, Neuroticism, depression, and positive
predictor with unique variance. Peer reports of emotions with life satisfaction.
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1071

TABLE 11: Changes in Explained Variance in a Hierarchical Regres- anxiety facets. None of the eight Extraversion items
sion Analysis With Global and Specific Predictors belongs to the positive emotions facet. Most of the items
Self Peers Parents tap activity level, assertiveness, and gregariousness. All
Predictors ∆R2 ∆R
2
∆R
2
items of the BFI were slightly modified to start with the
lead in “I tend to be . . . .” For example, the original BFI
1. Neuroticism and Extraversion .35* .14* .10*
item “I see myself as someone who has an assertive per-
2. Depression and positive emotions .04* .05* .02
1. Depression and positive emotions .35* .18* .09* sonality” was modified to read “I tend to have an assertive
2. Neuroticism and Extraversion .04* .00 .03 personality.” To include measures of depression and pos-
2
itive emotions, we created two scales by taking items
NOTE: ∆R = change in explained variance. Significance was tested by
partial F tests.
from the NEO-PI-R and the IPIP depression and positive
*p < .05. emotions/cheerfulness scales. The positive emotions
items were as follows: I tend to “be a cheerful and high-
spirited person,” “radiate joy,” “have a lot of fun,” “be eas-
Discussion ily amused,” “joke around,” “bubble with happiness,”
“amuse my friends,” “be in a good mood,” and “laugh
Study 3 examined the relation between personality
easily.” The depression items were as follows: I tend to
and life satisfaction using multiple personality measures.
“feel hopeless,” “feel discouraged,” “feel lonely,” “feel
The results were somewhat more mixed than in the pre-
sad,” “experience a deep sense of guilt and sinfulness,”
vious studies but in general were supportive of our
“blame myself when anything goes wrong,” and “tend to
hypotheses. On the positive side, self-reports of person-
be in a bad mood.” All scales had satisfactory reliability
ality replicated once more that depression and positive
(Cronbach’s α > .70).
emotions predict unique variance in life satisfaction
above and beyond the influence of other facets and the Results and Discussion
global traits. In addition, Study 3 demonstrated that this
Table 12 shows the simple correlations between life
finding generalizes to peer ratings of personality. How-
satisfaction and the five global and the two specific per-
ever, parent reports of personality failed to show the
sonality traits. Consistent with previous studies,
predicted pattern.
Extraversion and Neuroticism were more strongly corre-
lated with life satisfaction than the other global dimen-
STUDY 4 sions. Consistent with the previous three studies, positive
The previous studies used long questionnaires that emotions and depression were more highly correlated
were designed to assess facets of the Big Five. However, with life satisfaction than Extraversion and Neuroticism.
commonly researchers use shorter questionnaires if they A stepwise regression analysis showed that positive emo-
want to measure the Big Five without an assessment of tions and depression were the only personality traits that
the facets. Study 4 compared the predictive validity of explained unique variance in life satisfaction (see Table
the depression and positive emotions facets to a popular 13). Hierarchical regression analysis showed that
brief measure of Extraversion and Neuroticism—the 44- depression and positive emotions explained 8% of
item Big Five Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991; unique variance in life satisfaction, whereas Neuroticism
John & Srivastava, 1999). and Extraversion accounted for 0% unique variance in
life satisfaction. Another noteworthy finding was that the
Method single depression item of the BFI Neuroticism scale was
Participants. Three hundred and forty four (255 more highly correlated with life satisfaction (r = –.34)
female, 89 male) students at the University of Toronto than the eight-item Neuroticism scale (r = –.30) and a
participated in this study for course credit. seven-item Neuroticism scale that excluded the one
depression item (r = –.27). In sum, the results provide
Materials and procedure. Life satisfaction was assessed
further evidence that Neuroticism and Extraversion are
with the SWLS (Diener et al., 1985) at the beginning of
multifaceted constructs and that the depression and pos-
the semester. Personality was assessed at the end of labo-
itive emotion facets are necessary and sufficient for the
ratory experiments during the following 6 months. Per-
assessment of personality influences on life satisfaction.
sonality was assessed with a slightly modified version of
the Big Five Inventory (John, Donahue, et al., 1991). The
measure was designed to provide a short and reliable GENERAL DISCUSSION
measure of the Big Five. An examination of the eight- Four studies of the relation between the facets of
item Neuroticism scale revealed only one item of the Extraversion and Neuroticism and life satisfaction indi-
depression facet of Neuroticism (“is depressed and cated that the depression facet of Neuroticism and the
blue”). Most of the items belong to the vulnerability and positive emotions/cheerfulness facet of Extraversion
1072 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 12: Simple Correlations Between Personality and Life Satis- Implications of the Findings for the Assessment of
faction (Study 4)
Personality in SWB Research
Personality (N = 344) SWLS
The present findings have implications for the assess-
Neuroticism –.30* ment of personality in future studies of SWB. Many con-
Depression –.39* temporary personality studies use a Big Five question-
Extraversion .24* naire to assess personality. The present results suggest
Positive emotions .36* that this approach is not very economical. A comprehen-
Openness .17*
Agreeableness .17*
sive assessment of the Big Five that also provides a reli-
Conscientiousness .07 able assessment of facets requires more than 200 items.
As demonstrated, only 16 NEO-PI-R or 20 IPIP items are
NOTE: SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale. needed to assess the influence of personality on life satis-
*p < .05.
faction. By focusing on the most important facets,
researchers can increase predictive validity with 10% of
the items. Even compared to a short measure of the Big
TABLE 13: Changes in Explained Variance in a Hierarchical Regres- Five, assessing depression and positive emotions is more
sion Analysis With Global and Specific Predictors economical and increases explained variance. Hence,
(Study 4)
one main contribution of this study is the demonstration
Step/Predictors SWLS that two of the Lean Thirty personality dimensions are
sufficient to capture the influence of personality traits
1. Neuroticism and Extraversion .12* on life satisfaction.
2. Depression and cheerfulness .08*
1. Depression and cheerfulness .20* Implications of the Findings for Theories of SWB
2. Neuroticism and Extraversion .00
2
Our findings also help to refine theories of personal-
NOTE: ∆R = change in explained variance. Significance was tested by ity influences on life satisfaction. Neuroticism and espe-
partial F tests. SWLS = Satisfaction With Life Scale.
*p < .05. cially Extraversion are broad personality traits that incor-
porate several more specific personality traits. The
present findings suggest that SWB researchers should
focus on the affective facets of Extraversion and
Neuroticism. In contrast, assertiveness and sociability do
were the most consistent predictors of life satisfaction.
not add to the prediction of life satisfaction. Hence,
These two facets explained unique variance in life satis-
research that examines the biological and environmen-
faction in all seven analyses with self-reports with four
tal factors that lead to a more cheerful temperament are
independent samples. The depression facet was also a
more likely to contribute to the understanding of per-
significant predictor of unique variance in informant
sonality influences on life satisfaction than studies that
reports of life satisfaction in both studies that included examine the causes of assertiveness. Similarly, our results
informant reports of life satisfaction. Positive emotions suggest that the extensive literature on depression pro-
contributed to the prediction of informant reports in vides more useful information for theories of life satisfac-
one of two studies. Study 3 replicated the finding with tion than the equally vast literature on anxiety or aggres-
peer reports of depression and positive emotions, sion. For example, the strong influence of depression
although parent reports of personality failed to replicate rather than anxiety and vulnerability suggests that a lack
the same pattern. All but one analysis (parent reports in of meaning is more detrimental to life satisfaction than
Study 3) showed that depression and positive emotions/ stress and worries.
cheerfulness explained unique variance in life satisfac- One concern regarding our findings may be that our
tion above and beyond Extraversion and Neuroticism, results are self-evident and do not represent an empiri-
and only one analysis (self-reported personality in Study cal fact. Accordingly, life satisfaction by definition
3) showed that Extraversion and Neuroticism predicted implies a cheerful personality disposition and the
variance in life satisfaction that was not predicted by absence of depression. We believe that this is not a valid
depression and positive emotions/cheerfulness. These criticism. Many important empirical findings can be
findings suggest that depression and positive emotions/ intuitively obvious. For example, it was probably not very
cheerfulness are necessary and sufficient to maximize surprising to find universal sex differences in mate pref-
the prediction of life satisfaction from personality traits. erences (Buss, 1989) but this finding is nevertheless an
The first two studies suggested that these two facets important empirical finding. Similarly, our findings may
account for half of the method-invariant variance in life be intuitively obvious but they present an empirical fact.
satisfaction. Participants could have used any type of information
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1073

that they wanted to judge life satisfaction. The fact that that only two dimensions produce reliable direct effects
these judgments were predicted by depression rather on life satisfaction. However, it is possible that other per-
than anxiety and by positive emotions rather than asser- sonality dimensions influence life satisfaction indirectly
tiveness constitutes an empirical fact about the nature of and in interaction with situational factors. For example,
personality influences on life satisfaction. Even if we con- several studies, including the present studies, demon-
cede that the significant contribution of depression and strated that excitement-seeking is not correlated with
positive emotions/cheerfulness is obvious, it is also note- life satisfaction (Oishi et al., 2001). However, Oishi et al.
worthy that other personality traits failed to explain addi- (2001) demonstrated that excitement-seeking interacts
tional variance in life satisfaction. Wouldn’t our intu- with situational factors in the prediction of life satisfac-
itions predict that somebody who is disposed to tion. Excitement-seekers had higher levels of life satisfac-
experience more depression, anxiety, and anger has tion on an exciting day and lower levels of life satisfac-
lower life satisfaction than somebody who is only dis- tion on a boring day. It is likely that many other
posed to experience more depression? Contrary to this personality dimensions interact with situational factors
seemingly obvious prediction, we found that anxiety and in the prediction of life satisfaction. Such interaction
anger did not contribute to life satisfaction after effects are important for a complete understanding of
controlling for their association with depression. personality influences on life satisfaction. However, the
direct effects of depression and positive emotions/
Implications for Gender Differences in Subjective Well-Being
cheerfulness are particularly important because they
Gender differences in personality and life satisfaction influence people’s well-being across a wide range of
provide a concrete example of the importance of our situations (Diener & Larsen, 1984).
theoretical refinement. Numerous studies have demon-
Limitations
strated universal sex differences in Neuroticism (Costa,
Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001). Given the strong influ- One limitation of our studies is the composition of
ence of Neuroticism on life satisfaction, one would our samples. All studies relied on undergraduate stu-
expect that women have lower life satisfaction than men. dents in North America. Schimmack, Radhakrishnan,
Contrary to this prediction, studies of life satisfaction et al. (2002) found that Extraversion and Neuroticism
consistently fail to show gender differences (Diener were weaker predictors of life satisfaction in less individ-
et al., 1999). One explanation for this finding could be ualistic cultures. Hence, it is possible that other person-
that women score higher in Extraversion to compensate ality traits are more important predictors in other popu-
for the higher levels of Neuroticism. However, women lations and in different cultures. We hope that our
are not more extraverted than men (Costa et al., 2001). initiative of looking at facets encourages future
Hence, personality theories that postulate influences of researchers in different cultures to examine this issue.
Extraversion and Neuroticism on life satisfaction cannot Finding different personality predictors of life satisfac-
explain why women have the same level of life tion in other populations would not undermine the
satisfaction as men. present findings. Rather, it would provide further evi-
Our finding that life satisfaction is influenced by dence that culture moderates the relation between
depression and positive emotions/cheerfulness rather personality traits and life satisfaction.
than Neuroticism and Extraversion provides a simple Another limitation is our assessment of life satisfac-
solution to this problem. An extensive cross-cultural tion with one measure—the SWLS (Diener et al., 1985).
study of gender differences in the facets of Extraversion Different results could have emerged with another mea-
and Neuroticism showed that women score slightly sure of life satisfaction. However, there are several rea-
higher on the depression facet (d = .23) and the positive sons to believe that the present findings generalize to
emotions facet (d = .24) than men (Costa et al., 2001). other measures of life satisfaction. First, different life sat-
Given the similar magnitude of these effects and the isfaction measures are highly correlated with each other
opposing effects of depression and positive emotions on (Andrews & Withey, 1976). Second, the SWLS is a five-
life satisfaction, the two effects cancel each other out. item scale, which increases the generalizability to other
Hence, the absence of gender differences in life satisfac- life satisfaction measures. Third, the SWLS has high face
tion is consistent with the evidence regarding gender dif- validity (“I am satisfied with my life.”). Hence, it is diffi-
ferences in the facets of Extraversion and Neuroticism cult to image how another measure could assess life satis-
that predict life satisfaction. faction and not show the same correlations with
Clarification: Personality as a Moderator personality traits as the SWLS.
A related concern is the level of abstraction of our life
The present studies examined direct, linear effects of satisfaction measure. It is well known that global traits
personality traits on life satisfaction. We demonstrated such as Extraversion are better predictors of global out-
1074 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

comes, whereas specific traits such as the depression and DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-
analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psycho-
cheerfulness facets are better predictors of specific out- logical Bulletin, 124, 197-229.
comes (Ahadi & Diener, 1989). Maybe a broader mea- Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Sat-
sure of life satisfaction would be more strongly related to isfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75.
Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-
Extraversion and Neuroticism than to depression and situational consistency of affective, behavioral, and cognitive
cheerfulness. However, we already used the most responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871-883.
Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-
abstract and global measure of life satisfaction possible. being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being:
The SWLS does not constrain what information respon- The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213-229). New York: Rus-
dents use to evaluate their life satisfaction. Hence, it is sell Sage.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective
even more impressive that global life satisfaction is more well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125,
highly related to specific measures of personality rather 276-302.
than global dimensions of personality. Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A
realistic approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652-670.
Another concern could be the influence of social Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (1998). A multimodal analysis of personal
desirability. However, it is unlikely that social desirability negativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1580-1591.
influenced our results. First, social desirability has a neg- Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”:
The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ligible influence on anonymous ratings in personality chology, 59, 1216-1229.
research (Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1992; McCrae & Goldberg, L. R. (1997). International Personality Item Pool: A scientific
Costa, 1983; McCrae, Stone, Fagan, & Costa, 1998). Sec- collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of personality and
other individual differences. Retrieved October 18, 1998, from http://
ond, the use of multiple methods in Studies 1 and 3 fur- 80-ipip.ori.org/ipip/
ther reduces the threat that socially desirable respond- Hampson, S., John, O. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (1986). Category breadth
ing influenced our results. and hierarchical structure in personality studies of asymmetries in
judgments of trait implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
It also has to be noted that our results were not chology, 51, 37-54.
entirely consistent across studies. Future research needs Herringer, L. G. (1998). Facets of Extraversion related to life satisfac-
to examine whether these inconsistencies are due to tion. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 731-733.
Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, O. M. (1993). Stabil-
sampling error or represent meaningful variations. ity of emotion experiences and their relations to traits of personal-
However, there is one important consistent finding ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 847-860.
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. (1991). The Big Five Inven-
across all studies. Researchers can predict as much vari- tory: Versions 4a and 54. Technical report, Institute of Personality
ance in life satisfaction from the Big Five measures, and Social Research, University of California, Berkeley.
based on 240 or 300 items, as they can predict from the John, O. P., Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (1991). The basic level
in personality-trait hierarchies: Studies of trait use and accessibil-
depression and cheerfulness facets, based on 16 or 20 ity in different contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
items. Often, the two facets also predict additional vari- 60, 348-361.
ance that is lost by aggregating facets into global factors. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: His-
tory, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin &
O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp.
REFERENCES 102-138). New York: Guilford.
Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999).
Ahadi, S., & Diener, E. (1989). Multiple determinants and effect size. The Big Five personality traits, general mental ability, and career
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(3), 398-406. success across the life span. Personnel Psychology, 52, 621-652.
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being: McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1983). Social desirability scales: More
America’s perception of life quality. New York: Plenum. substance than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1992). Social desirability scales as mod- 51(6), 882-888.
erator and suppressor variables. European Journal of Personality, 6, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a
199-214. human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evo- McCrae, R. R., Stone, S. V., Fagan, P. J., & Costa, P. T. J. (1998). Identi-
lutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sci- fying causes of disagreement between self-reports and spouse rat-
ences, 12, 1-49. ings of personality. Journal of Personality, 66, 285-313.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of Extraversion and Oishi, S., Diener, E., Suh, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Value as a modera-
Neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy peo- tor in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 67, 157-184.
ple. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678. Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2001). Pleasures and subjec-
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in tive well-being. European Journal of Personality, 15, 153-167.
clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Paunonen, S. V. (1998). Hierarchical organization of personality and
Assessment, 4, 5-13. prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1998). Six approaches to the explication 74, 538-556.
of facet-level traits: Examples from conscientiousness. European Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five factors and facets
Journal of Personality, 12, 117-134. and the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differ- chology, 81, 524-539.
ences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising Placidi, G. P. A., Oquendo, M. A., Malone, K. M., Brodsky, B. E., Ste-
findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322-331. ven, P., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Anxiety in major depression: Rela-
Schimmack et al. / PERSONALITY AND LIFE SATISFACTION 1075

tionship to suicide attempts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, ing process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and
1614-1618. Social Psychology, 82, 1313-1329.
Schimmack, U. (2003). Affect measurement in experience sampling Scocco, P., Marietta, P., Tonietto, M., Buono, M. D., & De Leo, D.
research. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 79-106. (2000). The role of psychopathology and suicidal intention in
Schimmack, U., Bockenholt, U., & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Response predicting suicide risk: A longitudinal study, Psychopathology, 33,
styles in affect ratings: Making a mountain out of a molehill. Jour- 134-150.
nal of Personality Assessment, 78, 461-483. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: Gen-
Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life satisfaction is a eral and specific factors of emotional experience and their rela-
momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The tion to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441-476.
use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personal-
ity, 70, 345-385. Received July 2, 2003
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. Revision accepted October 28, 2003
(2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrat-

S-ar putea să vă placă și