Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ergon

The effects of “finger pointing and calling” on cognitive control processes


in the task-switching paradigm
Kazumitsu Shinohara a, *, Hiroshi Naito a, Yuko Matsui b, Masaru Hikono b
a
Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, 1-2, Yamadaoka, Suita, Osaka 5650871, Japan
b
Institute of Nuclear Safety System, Incorporated, 64 Sata, Mihama, Mikata-gun, Fukui 9191205, Japan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: “Finger pointing and calling (FPC),” also known simply as “pointing and calling,” is an operational
Received 29 July 2011 procedure to prevent human error and has been used extensively in Japanese industry. Although the
Received in revised form effectiveness of FPC has been widely recognized, cognitive processes underlying FPC have not been
28 July 2012
thoroughly investigated. The current study focused on the effect of FPC on cognitive control processes
Accepted 8 August 2012
Available online 24 August 2012
responsible for the supervisory attentional system including the retrieval and activation of working
memory. In the experiment, a task-switching paradigm was adopted. Twenty participants had to make
a binary decision about two presented digits according to one of three task rules. Task rules to be applied
Keywords:
Finger pointing and calling
were presented by a cue immediately before presenting the digits. Participants took part in a mixed rule
Human error prevention session in which they had to switch the task rule from one trial to the next, and a single rule session in
Task switching which a specific rule was applied throughout the session. FPC to the cue was incorporated. Results
Attention showed that in the mixed rule condition, the reaction was significantly faster when FPC was performed
Working memory compared to when FPC was not performed. Even though the reaction became faster in the mixed rule
condition with FPC, the accuracy of response was not affected. However, the preparation time, which was
required for retrieving and activating the rule used for the current trial, was affected by performing FPC.
In addition, the level of subjective mental workload did not change by applying FPC, which suggests that
FPC was nonintrusive to the main task. These findings suggest that FPC facilitates the cognitive control
processes of the supervisory attentional system, and that FPC was especially useful for the signal that
requested operators to select something from memorized alternatives according to the content of the
signal.
Relevance to industry: The findings of this study provide evidence of the effectiveness of FPC and
contribute to encouraging the introduction of FPC to real working situations as a tool to prevent oper-
ational errors. The findings can also serve to evaluate the effectiveness of FPC by evaluating to what
extent cognitive control processes are included when an operator has to check or respond to signals. If
a check and response to a signal demands cognitive control processes, it seems to be effective to
incorporate FPC into the checking and response behavior. Furthermore, the findings of this study may
contribute to updating policies regarding the application of FPC and to improving safety training
programs in which FPC is incorporated in the training process.
Ó 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and sometimes executing simple physical responses such as


pressing a button or touching a screen. While the physical load for
In modern working situations, most working processes are operators has been minimized, operators still have to manage an
computerized and automated by implementing information and enormous amount of information compiled by the information
communication technologies (ICT), and most tasks executed by systems. An improved humanecomputer interface contributes to
human operators mainly involve monitoring the status of processes reducing the operator’s information-processing load by mediating
between a human operator and a computer system, but the human
operator is still usually responsible for making and carrying out
* Corresponding author. Tel./fax: þ81 6 6879 8034.
E-mail addresses: sinohara@hus.osaka-u.ac.jp (K. Shinohara), naito@hus.osaka-
critical decisions and controls. If a human operator makes erro-
u.ac.jp (H. Naito), matsui@inss.co.jp (Y. Matsui), hikono.masaru@inss.co.jp neous decisions and/or makes mistakes in control operations, and if
(M. Hikono). the system does not deal with this human error, it is impossible to

0169-8141/$ e see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2012.08.004
130 K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136

avoid problems and accidents. Therefore, efforts are continuing to out FPC, to point to the stimulus, to call out the status of the
be made to reduce the risk of human operator error. These efforts stimulus indicated, or to do nothing before making a response to
include improving the user-interface design and implementing fail- the visual stimulus. The results showed that carrying out FPC
safe and fool-proof concepts to the system. In addition to these significantly reduced errors. Haga et al. (1996) conducted
measures, it is important to train and encourage operators to a psychological experiment which was based on Kiyomiya et al.
incorporate a procedure that reduces the possibility of making (1965) and found that FPC did actually reduce the occurrence of
mistakes. human error (Experiment 1). Moreover, they conducted another
Even in the modern industry that has been highly computerized experiment (Experiment 2) to examine how FPC affected the
and automated, the performance of human operators remains an prevention of human error when the reaction was activated by an
important factor for accident prevention and efficiency improve- improper visual signal, and the results indicated that FPC signifi-
ment. For example, human error in maintenance tasks is an cantly reduced this type of human error. Haga et al. (1996)
important problem for ensuring safety and reliability in many explained that the effectiveness of FPC was associated with (1)
industries, such as in aviation, nuclear power plants, chemical a confirmed visual contact with the target, (2) a continuous
plants, medical services, mining, etc (Dhillon and Liu, 2006). In the orientation of attention to the target, as well as facilitated memo-
airline industry, human error in the maintenance activities causes rization by recall and vocalization of the target, (3) an enhanced
accidents and other troubles such as delays in aircraft availability. cognitive process by using multiple sensory modalities, and (4) an
Latorella and Prabhu (2000) stated that human error in aviation increased activation by using muscular movement. The findings
maintenance tasks includes defective components, missing obtained in these previous studies have served as the evidence to
components, wrong components, incorrect configurations, incor- justify the implementation of FPC in work situations as a measure
rect assembly sequences, functional defects, tactile defects, and to prevent human error.
procedural defects. Latorella and Prabhu (2000) proposed several One of the important potencies of FPC is to intentionally make
approaches to reduce human error: (1) training, (2) job design and a worker control each step of an action sequence. As a result, the
organizational considerations, (3) workspace and ambient envi- behavior of FPC slows down and its accuracy is thus promoted. FPC
ronment design, (4) task equipment and information design, and is useful when emphasizing accuracy over speed. FPC is particularly
(5) automation. Training for improving performance while pre- useful for some habitual behaviors such as confirming that the
venting human error has been extensively studied (e.g., Brunstein doors and windows are locked before going outside. By checking
and Gonzalez, 2011; Chan and Chiu, 2009; Czaja and Drury, 1981; the locks on each door and window using FPC, the risk to forget to
Gramopadhye et al., 1998; Liang et al., 2010; Nalanagula et al., lock any doors and windows can be reduced. In other words,
2006). In the present study, we are focusing on a procedure to human error such as action slips (Norman, 1981) is likely to be
check the actions necessary to perform tasks required in a working averted when using FPC. However, FPC is not useful when
situation. It can be introduced into industry through the training for emphasizing speed over accuracy. For example, when we play
human operators. sports or video games, we have to take prompt and immediate
actions. In this case FPC is not useful or even harmful because FPC
1.1. Finger-pointing and calling blocks the smooth execution of the action sequence. Perhaps FPC is
useful in most situations in real life because in many ways we have
In Japanese industry, finger pointing and calling (FPC), which is to do something accurately at the cost of speed.
called “shisa-kosho” or “shisa-kanko” in Japanese, have been Recently, there has been an increased need to study the cogni-
traditionally practiced as a tool for preventing human error. FPC is tive process underlying FPC because some negative effects of
an operational procedure to ensure accurate information acquisi- traditional FPC have been revealed in Japanese industry. For
tion and recognition and/or to perform motor responses accurately. example, in situations where there are a lot of signals to be checked
In the typical FPC, an operator first makes visual contact with the and an operator is asked to perform FPC with all of them, the
target (e.g. meters, lamps, buttons, etc.) to be checked or to be frequency of carrying out FPC is increased, and more time is needed
controlled manually, stretches the arm out in the direction of the for it. In this situation, an operator will be very busy, and an
target, points at the target with the index finger, and calls the name unacceptably high load may be induced by performing FPC,
and/or the status of the target aloud. When an operator needs to resulting in an increase in operator fatigue and/or human errors
manipulate the target, an operator actually manipulates it after induced by time pressure. Because FPC essentially emphasizes
completing the procedure mentioned above. There are many vari- accuracy over speed, workers inevitably feel conflicted about using
ants of FPC among industrial fields in Japan. FPC developed spon- FPC when both speed and accuracy are required to do work.
taneously from on-site work practices of the Japanese rail industry One solution for such a situation is to modify the FPC procedure.
and was later regulated as an official procedure to confirm that By modifying the procedure, FPC is feasible in a high intensity load
conditions were safe. Nowadays, FPC is accepted as an effective situation while still being able to maintain its effectiveness at
procedural tool for preventing human error and is widely used in a similar level as when FPC is performed in the traditional way. To
many industries in Japan. Moreover, FPC is systematically incor- achieve this goal, it is necessary to clarify the cognitive processes
porated in many occupational safety training systems. For example, underlying FPC and to identify a component cognitive process that
the Japan Industrial Safety and Health Association offers a safety can be modified without negatively affecting the overall effective-
training program to predict underlying hazards in a work situation, ness of FPC. It may be possible to make FPC more effective by
and FPC is incorporated in this program. modifying it in order to facilitate one of the component cognitive
Although FPC is accepted as an effective procedure for pre- processes included in it. For example, Watanabe et al. (2005) sug-
venting human error and is widely used in industry, the effective- gested the modification of FPC based on the findings of psycho-
ness of FPC has not been sufficiently studied, aside from a few logical studies on action events. In their experiment, when
published reports (Haga et al., 1996; Kiyomiya et al., 1965). In the participants carried out FPC along with operation instructions
research by Kiyomiya et al. (1965), which was the earliest experi- presented sequentially, they were asked to perform the actual
mental study of FPC according to Haga et al. (1996), participants action each instruction referred to. This modification was devised
were required to respond to one of five visual stimuli presented in based on the studies of “subject-performed tasks (SPTs)”
random order. In addition, participants were asked either to carry (Englekamp, 1998; Nilsson, 2000). In the SPTs studies, participants
K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136 131

were required to memorize verbal commands and to perform the intervenes in the activation process of schemas. The supervisory
action that each verbal command indicated, with the result that the attentional system, which is equivalent to the central executive of
performance of an action facilitated the recall of verbal commands. the working memory system (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baddeley
It was predicted that this action rehearsal with FPC would facilitate and Logie, 1999), has many cognitive functions such as coordina-
encoding the sequence of instructed actions. The results showed tion of the memory system, control of encoding and retrieval
that the implementation of rehearsed actions with FPC had a posi- strategies, switching of attention, and mental manipulation of
tive effect on memory performance. material in slave systems (Baddeley and Logie, 1999).
Furthermore, by identifying why FPC is effective for preventing Task switching is known to be closely related to the central
human error in terms of cognitive science, the implementation of executive function (Gopher et al., 2000; Kramer et al., 1999;
FPC in work situations is taken as an evidence-based safety Monsell, 2003; Rogers and Monsell, 1995) and has been extensively
measure. It also makes safety training programs that include FPC studied. In the typical task switching paradigm, several tasks are
more persuasive. prepared and participants are asked to perform one of them. When
participants have to change the task from one trial to the next, they
1.2. Hypothetical effect of FPC on cognitive control processes have to retrieve and reactivate the task set that is necessary to
perform a specific task. Several studies (Baddeley et al., 2001; Bryck
Fig. 1 depicts the hypothetical effect of FPC on cognitive control and Mayr, 2005; Emerson and Miyake, 2003; Miyake et al., 2004;
processes, which is based on the supervisory attentional system Saeki and Saito, 2004) involved experiments in which task
(SAS) model proposed by Norman and Shallice (1986). In this switching was combined with articulatory suppression. Articula-
model, human behavior was controlled by two systems: contention tory suppression involves saying an irrelevant word repeatedly and
scheduling and supervisory attentional system (SAS). Finger is a commonly used technique to impose a load on the phonological
pointing has an effect on the perceptual stage of information. By information processing of the working memory system. Task
pointing a finger, visual attention is oriented and focused at the switching performance has been shown to interfere with articula-
target position with more certainty where the information is dis- tory suppression, suggesting that task switching is based on
played. The effect of FPC on the orientation of visual attention was phonological representation of a task set or rule which is necessary
examined in previous studies (Shinohara et al., 2009; Ariga and to perform the task. This task set or rule has to be retrieved and
Watanabe, 2009). Shinohara et al. (2009) examined the effect of activated every time participants prepare the incoming task using
FPC by using a spatial cueing paradigm (Posner, 1980). FPC was a rule that is different from the one used for the previous task.
performed to the cue indicating the position of an incoming visual Even though no studies have yet been done that examine the
stimulus. The results showed that FPC, particularly pointing, facil- effect of FPC on supervisory attentional control or the central
itated the detection of the visual target. Ariga and Watanabe (2009) executive, these preceding findings obtained in the study of
examined various pointing gestures as a cue to trigger an automatic working memory suggest that vocalizing the target information
attentional shift and found that gesture cues caused a reflexive aloud (i.e. doing the “calling” in FPC) are related to the selective
attentional shift to the pointed position and that indexical pointing activation process of memorized information which is essential for
gestures had a relatively persistent cueing effect. In addition, performing the task. Thus, FPC is presumed to facilitate the activity
Watanabe et al. (2005) argued that visual contact with the target to of the supervisory attentional system and as a result, intentional
be operated contributed to a positive effect of FPC with an action by monitoring of the activation of schemas is expected to be more
facilitating the planning of an action sequence. It is thought that efficient. Even when the operator is familiar with the situation and
encoding of visual information and related cognitive processes is the operator’s behavior can be automatically executed, the chance
facilitated by focusing visual attention on the target by finger to intervene in an intentional control of the supervisory attentional
pointing. system may be enhanced by vocalizing the target information
According to the SAS model, perceived information is thought to aloud. It may contribute to preventing slip-type human errors
trigger several schemas, with the result that some behaviors which (Reason, 1990).
are associated with the activated schema are evoked. Coordination The purpose of this study was to examine the cognitive
of activated schemas is responsible for the contention scheduling. processes in FPC by focusing on the facilitating effect of FPC on the
These processes are relatively automatic and are underlying in supervisory attentional system or the central executive of the
familiar situations. On the contrary, when the perceived informa- working memory system. A task-switching paradigm was used and
tion is novel or unfamiliar, the supervisory attentional system FPC was incorporated. FPC was performed to the task cue indicating
the task rule which was used in the current trial, and the perfor-
mance with and without FPC was compared. It was hypothesized
that the performance would be better when FPC was performed,
particularly in the condition when the task rule was randomly
switched from one trial to the next because FPC would support the
function of the supervisory attentional system and facilitate the
process of retrieval and activation of task rules.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty people (10 females and 10 males), ranging in age from


20 to 39 (M ¼ 24.55), were recruited to take part in this study.
Institutional ethical approval was obtained before the start of the
study and this experiment was conducted in accordance with
ethical guidelines. Informed consent was obtained from all
Fig. 1. Hypothetical effect of finger pointing and calling on cognitive control processes. participants.
132 K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136

2.2. Apparatus participants was also recorded to assess the correctness of


participants’ responses. This stimulus display was erased, and
A Core 2 Duo personal computer with Microsoft Windows XP feedback indicating whether the response was correct or not was
and Cedrus SuperLab 4.0 software, which is used for authoring provided visually.
psychological experiments, were used to control the stimulus
presentation and response data correction. All visual stimuli were 2.4. Finger-pointing and calling
presented on a display monitor (IODATA LCD-AD193GS) set in front
of the participant. The viewing distance was approximately 50 cm. In the FPC condition, participants were asked to perform FPC
Participants responded to the stimuli by pressing one of four with the presented task cue. The index finger of the right hand
buttons on a USB key box (Techno wave KB-IOPAD4). was used for “finger pointing.” The presented task cue was read
aloud by participants for the “calling.” Participants were asked to
carry out FPC in the instructed way; participants had to fixate on
2.3. Task
the task cue, stretch out their right arm in the direction of the task
cue, point to the task cue with their index finger, and read it aloud
The basic task participants were required to perform was to
clearly. This style of FPC is common in industrial working situa-
compare two digits based on the task rule and to select one of them.
tions in Japan.
The task rule was provided at the beginning of each trial, and two
digits were visually presented after the task rule was presented.
2.5. Subjective mental workload assessment
The event sequence in one trial is presented in Fig. 2. At the
beginning of each trial, a task cue indicating the rule of the current
The subjective mental workload was assessed using the check-
trial and four place holders were presented after a 1-s “Ready”
list developed by Shinohara and Kimura (2010). This checklist
message. A task cue appeared at the center of the screen, and the
includes 26 questions with a 7-point rating scale. Each question
four place holders were also presented on each side of the task cue.
represented each of six dimensions of mental workload: mental
The task cue was either “digit,” “size,” or “position” Actually, the
demand, physical demand, time pressure, performance, effort, and
Japanese words “Suji,” “Saizu,” or “Ichi,” which correspond
frustration. These questions were based on the description of
respectively to “digit,” “size,” and “position,” were presented by
subscales of the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) (Hart and
kanji or katakana. “Digit” indicated that participants had to select
Staveland, 1988). The original description of the workload dimen-
the larger of the presented digits. “Size” indicated that participants
sions of NASA-TLX includes several expressions for the feelings of
had to select the digit that was displayed in a larger size. “Position”
mental stress. It has caused difficulties for the respondents because
indicated that participants had to select the digit that was displayed
the characteristics of each workload dimension are hard to
at a vertically higher position than the other digit. Participants were
understand. In the checklist used in this experiment, several scales
allowed to see this task cue until they recalled the rule to be applied
were developed by breaking down each description into simple
in the current trial, and they were asked to press one of the
pairs of adjectives or short sentences. For example, the original
response keys with the index finger of their right hand when they
“mental demand” scale was broken down into four scales, which
wanted to proceed to the stimulus presentation (Response 1 in
were “use your head vs. not use your head”, “easy vs. demanding”,
Fig. 2). The time from the onset of the task cue presentation to the
“simple vs. complex”, and ”exacting vs. forgiving”. The average
participants’ response to proceed was recorded because it was
score was calculated for the scales of the workload dimension and
thought to reflect the time required to prepare the rule internally
was used as the mental workload score. In the study of Shinohara
for the subsequent response.
and Kimura (2010), forty participants were asked to perform an
After the presentation of the task cue and the following 300-
attention-demanding task and to make an assessment of subjective
ms blank interval after the participants’ response, two digits
mental workload by using both the checklist and the original NASA-
were presented in two of four place holders. Participants were
TLX. To examine the correspondence between the results of the
commonly required to respond to the stimulus by pressing either
checklist and those of the NASA-TLX, six correlation coefficients
the “left side” button or the “right side” button allocated on the
between corresponding subscales were calculated. Four of the six
USB response key box with the index or middle finger of their left
correlations were significant (p < .01), “physical demand” (r ¼ .53),
hand (Response 2 in Fig. 2). The key was to be selected on the
time pressure (r ¼ .54), effort (r ¼ .56), and frustration (r ¼ .44),
basis of the rule indicated in the preceding display. Reaction times
while no significant correlation was found for the remaining two
(RTs) from the onset of stimulus presentation to the participants’
scales; “mental demand” (r ¼ .15) and “performance” (r ¼ .28).
response were measured and recorded. The key pressed by
A profile of the subjective mental workload that occurred in
each condition was described based on these six scores.

2.6. Procedure and design

Each participant was individually tested so that this experiment


simulated a working alone situation. In a real working situation in
which several workers are in the same workplace, each worker is
often encouraged to check the other workers’ FPC and to let the
others know when any errors during FPC are found. This mutually-
checking of FPCs has been regarded as one of the advantages of
using FPC. However, a situation in which a worker engages in a task
alone is increasing because of industrial mechanization and
personnel downsizing. In this situation, it is impossible to check the
FPC on site.
At the beginning of the session, instructions for this experiment
Fig. 2. Event sequence in a trial. were provided. A visual aid consisting of a PowerPoint slide show
K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136 133

was used to aid participants in understanding the task. In partic- For the error data, the error rates were calculated for each
ular, the procedure for performing FPC was explained carefully and participant, task rule switching, and FPC presence.
presented visually on the slides, including a demonstration movie
clip that showed the ideal way to perform FPC. This instruction was 3.2. Performance
designed to ensure that FPC would be performed consistently.
Three 30-trial practice sessions of FPC for each rule were Fig. 3 indicates the mean RTs as a function of the FPC condition
carried out immediately after the instruction. In these practice and the rule switching condition. A two-way repeated analysis of
sessions, a single rule was applied throughout each session. In the variance (ANOVA) showed that the main effects of task rule
single rule session, a specific rule which was applied in the switching (F(1,19) ¼ 18.265, p < .001) and FPC presence
experiment was allocated to each participant throughout the (F(1,19) ¼ 5.879, p < .05) were significant. There was a significant
session. That is, each participant experienced only one of three interaction between task rule switching and FPC presence
rules in the single rule session. Following that, a 30-trial practice of (F(1,19) ¼ 6.685, p < .05). A simple main effect was examined, and it
the mixed rule practice session was carried out, in which one of was shown that in the mixed rule condition there was a significant
three rules was applied in each trial and was switched randomly. difference between the FPC condition and the no-FPC condition
At the end of this practice session, an assessment of the subjective (F(1,37) ¼ 12.408, p < .01). When switching of the task rule was
mental workload was conducted. This procedure was aimed at required, reactions became faster by performing FPC. It was also
familiarizing participants with the checklist method of mental shown that a significant difference was found between the task rule
workload assessment. switching conditions both in the FPC condition (F(1,25) ¼ 9.163,
Following the practice sessions, participants took part in four p < .01) and in the no-FPC condition (F(1,25) ¼ 24.249, p < .01),
experimental sessions: the mixed rule with FPC session (120 trials), which indicates that introducing the task rule switching increased
the mixed rule without FPC session (120 trials), the single rule with the RTs regardless of whether FPC was performed.
FPC session (90 trials), and the single rule without FPC session (90 Overall, the error rates were quite low, ranging from .011 to .036.
trials). The four experimental sessions were counterbalanced. After A Friedman test was performed, and it revealed that there was
completing each session, participants were asked to rate their a significant difference among the four conditions (c2(3) ¼ 13.710,
subjective mental workload according to the checklist. p < .01). Scheffe’s test was conducted as a multiple comparison
The independent variables of this experiment were the task rule procedure and revealed that the error rate obtained in the single
switching (mixed vs. single) and the FPC presence (FPC vs. no FPC). rule with FPC condition (.011) was significantly lower than the error
All variables were within participants. The dependent variables rate obtained in the mixed rule without the FPC condition (.036)
were the RT, the percentage of correct responses, and the prepa- (c2(3) ¼ 10.901, p < .05).
ration time for task rules. RT has been used as a major behavioral Fig. 4 shows the preparation time. Two-way repeated ANOVA
measure from the context of human factors research. In particular, showed a significant interaction (F(1,19) ¼ 79.258, p < .001). A
an analysis of RT is useful for examining the cognitive processes simple main effect indicated there was no significant difference in
when a human operator engages in an attention demanding task or the preparation time between the FPC condition and the no-FPC
when they are in an inactive state (e.g., fatigue or boredom). For condition when the task rule switching was required
example, RT has been used to examine the effect of the working (F(1,33) ¼ 3.824, p > .05). When task rule switching was not
memory load on the drivers’ response to a braking lead vehicle required, the preparation time in the FPC condition was signifi-
(Engström et al., 2010), the effect of rehearsal in the memory to cantly longer than that in the no-FPC condition (F(1,33) ¼ 62.534,
a drivers’ performance (Salvucci and Beltowska, 2008), and the p < .001). A simple main effect test also showed that in the FPC
efficiency of the mental information processing of a human oper- condition, the preparation time in the single rule condition was
ator in a state of mental fatigue (Meijman, 1997). significantly longer than that in the mixed rule condition
It was thought in the present study that the preparation time (F(1,37) ¼ 77.581, p < .001). It also indicated that in the no-FPC
included the time to retrieve and activate the memorized task rule condition, on the contrary, the preparation time in the single rule
and the RT included the time to select the response under the rule condition was found to be significantly shorter than the prepara-
activated during the preparation time and to execute the response. tion time in the mixed rule condition (F(1,37) ¼ 8.773, p < .01).

3. Results

There were 7992 responses collected in the experiment. Partic-


ipants made 8000 actual responses, but 8 responses (.1%) failed to
be recorded. Of the responses, 199 (2.5%) were incorrect. When FPC
was required, the participants accurately conducted FPC and only
a few FPC errors were observed throughout the experiment.

3.1. Preliminary data processing

Preliminary data processing was conducted for RT data and


preparation time data. Any RTs and preparation times for correct
trials which were longer than three standard deviations above
a participant’s mean RT and mean preparation time in that condi-
tion were defined as an outlier and were replaced with the value of
the mean plus three standard deviations. This procedure is called
“windsorizing” (Barnett and Lewis, 1978; Ratcliff, 1993) for
adjusting a distribution for the outliers. Furthermore, a log trans- Fig. 3. Mean reaction times as a function of task rule switching condition and finger
formation was conducted on the RT and preparation time. pointing and calling condition. Error bars indicate standard errors.
134 K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136

1989, 1990), FPC may be explicitly helpful for processes other than
the activation process.
As for the preparation time (Fig. 4), there was no difference
between the FPC conditions under the mixed rule condition. This
suggests that FPC does not affect the preparation process in the
preparation time. According to the study (Rogers and Monsell,
1995) using the task switching paradigm, there are two prepa-
ration components, one is associated with the intentional
(endogenous) preparation and the other is associated with the
stimulus-driven (exogenous) completion of the preparation. The
preparation process in the interval between the presentation of
a task rule and the stimulus onset was an intentional one. Thus,
FPC did not affect an intentional type of preparation. However,
performing FPC enabled the participants to respond faster (Fig. 3),
indicating that FPC positively affected the process after presenting
the stimulus, which included the stimulus-driven completion of
the preparation, the application of the prepared rule, and the
response execution. It means that FPC does not affect the prepa-
ration of the task rule before the stimulus onset, and that the task
rule that is activated with FPC is processed faster after the stim-
Fig. 4. Mean preparation times as a function of task rule switching condition and
finger pointing and calling condition. Error bars indicate standard errors.
ulus onset. As a result, the reaction time is shortened.
As for the single rule condition, there was significant difference
between the FPC conditions. The preparation time under the single
3.3. Subjective mental workload assessment rule condition was significantly longer in the FPC condition than
under the no FPC condition. This difference in preparation time
Fig. 5 shows the characteristics of subjective mental workload does not derive from the effect of FPC on the task switching process
(SMWL) for each experimental session, and indicates that “effort” because no task rule switching was needed under the single rule
was consistently the most critical aspect for all experimental condition. This result may reflect some of the other aspects of the
sessions. Two-way repeated ANOVA was conducted for each score functions of FPC.
of subjective mental workload. In common, the main effects of task One possible interpretation is that the participants used FPC as
rule switching were significant (F(1,19) ¼ 5.78 w 42.87, p < .05), a measure to resist the low-intensity mental load. Under the single
and neither the main effects of FPC presence (F(1,19) ¼ .08 w 2.37, task condition, the participants repeatedly made binary responses
p > .05) nor the interactions were significant (F(1,19) ¼ .06 w .92, based on the one task rule. A situation entailing a low-intensity
p > .05). These results indicate that the requirement of task rule
switching increased the level of mental workload regardless of
whether FPC was conducted or not.

4. Discussion

This research employed a task-switching paradigm to examine


the effects of FPC on cognitive control, which is responsible for the
supervisory attentional system including selective retrieval and
activation of the memorized task rules. Results showed that
carrying out FPC improved performance when the task rule had to
be switched, suggesting that FPC facilitated the cognitive control
process.
As indicated in Fig. 3, RT in the mixed rule condition was shorter
when FPC was applied than when FPC was not applied, while there
was no difference in the accuracy of response between the FPC
condition and the no-FPC condition. This result suggests that per-
forming FPC facilitated the task rule switching without sacrificing
the accuracy of response. It is notable that the difference in RT is
significant only in the mixed rule condition, which suggests that
the FPC prominently facilitated the activation process relating to
the task switching. Even in the single rule condition in which one
task rule is applied continuously, vocalizing the task rule seemed to
be effective for keeping it active in the working memory. However,
this may not be particularly remarkable because keeping one rule
active is generally so easy that it is sufficient to use sub-vocal
rehearsal or inner speech. However, in some situations, such as
when the information to be memorized is so long and/or compli-
cated that storing information in the working memory is difficult,
or when irrelevant sound or speech disrupts phonological infor- Fig. 5. Scoring profiles of subjective mental workload (SMWL) as a function of task
mation in the working memory (Salamé and Baddeley, 1986, 1987, rule switching condition and finger pointing and calling condition.
K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136 135

mental load can have some negative effects such as reduced vigi- References
lance. As Haga et al. (1996) suggested, one of the positive effects of
FPC is an increased physiological arousal due to muscular move- Ariga, A., Watanabe, K., 2009. What is special about the index finger?: the index
finger advantage in manipulating reflexive attentional shift. Japanese Psycho-
ment. The participants may have adopted a strategy to expressly do logical Research 51, 258e265.
FPC since the single rule session was so easy and monotonous. Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., Adlam, A., 2001. Working memory and the control of
However, in order to expressly do FPC was time-consuming, and action: evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General 130, 641e657.
the preparation time under the FPC condition was longer than that Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G.J., 1974. Working memory. In: Bower, G.H. (Ed.), The
under the no FPC condition. Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory 8.
In the subjective mental workload assessment (Fig. 5), it is note- Academic Press, New York, pp. 47e89.
Baddeley, A.D., Logie, R.H., 1999. Working memory: the multiple-component model.
worthy that the effect of FPC was not significant whereas the effect of In: Miyake, A., Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of
task switching was significant. As mentioned above, it has been Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
problematic to perform FPC too frequently because it increases the bridge, pp. 28e61.
Barnett, V., Lewis, T., 1978. Outliers in Statistical Data. Wiley, New York.
workload for operators. In the present study, participants performed
Brunstein, A., Gonzalez, C., 2011. Preparing for novelty with diverse training.
FPC frequently in the FPC condition, and the rate of FPC in the present Applied Cognitive Psychology 25 (5), 682e691.
experiment was thought to be higher than that in a real work situ- Bryck, R.L., Mayr, U., 2005. On the role of verbalization during task set selection:
switching or serial order control? Memory & Cognition 33, 611e623.
ation. Nevertheless, FPC did not increase the level of subjective
Chan, A.H.S., Chiu, C.H.Y., 2009. The effects of visual lobe training with easy and
mental workload and had a positive effect on task performance. difficult targets on changes of visual lobe shape characteristics and visual search
Conducting FPC in a rigid way is likely to be required regardless of performance. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 39, 851e859.
the working situation, particularly when conducting FPC is inten- Czaja, S.J., Drury, C.G., 1981. Training programs for inspection. Human Factors 23,
473e484.
sively encouraged in a real industrial situation. When human oper- Dhillon, B.S., Liu, Y., 2006. Human error in maintenance: a review. Journal of Quality
ators have to sequentially or simultaneously perform many tasks and in Maintenance Engineering 12, 21e36.
they cannot afford to do any other tasks, FPC will be omitted. Even Emerson, M.J., Miyake, A., 2003. The role of inner speech in task switching: a dual-
task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language 48, 148e168.
when FPC is required in such situations, it will be superficial and Englekamp, J., 1998. Memory for Actions. Psychology Press, Hove, England.
ineffective in error prevention. In this case, the procedure for con- Engström, J., Aust, M.L., Viström, M., 2010. Effects of working memory load and
ducting FPC has to be refined to reduce its burden. However, when repeated scenario exposure on emergency braking performance. Human
Factors 52, 551e559.
human operators have to perform easy, monotonous, or repetitive Gopher, D., Armony, L., Greenshpan, Y., 2000. Switching tasks and attention policies.
tasks, introducing FPC will help them keep up their activation. In this Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129, 308e339.
case, an overt way of conducting FPC may be welcomed. That is, FPC Gramopadhye, A., Bhagwat, S., Kimbler, D., Greenstein, J., 1998. The use of advanced
technology for visual inspection training. Applied Ergonomics 29, 361e375.
can be more acceptable by flexibly adjusting how it is carried out in
Haga, S., Akatsuka, H., Shiroto, H., 1996. Laboratory experiments for verifying the
accordance with the working situation. The crucial factors of FPC effectiveness of “finger-pointing and call” as a practical tool of human error
acceptability have not been studied, and further research is needed prevention. Japanese Association of Industrial/Organizational Psychology Jour-
nal 9, 107e114 (in Japanese with English abstract).
on this issue because it is very important to create an FPC procedure
Hart, S.G., Staveland, L.E., 1988. Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): results
that is more acceptable than the existing one and to widely of empirical and theoretical research. In: Hancock, P.A., Meshkati, N. (Eds.),
disseminate the FPC procedure into the industrial world. Human Mental Workload. Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 139e183.
In summary, the present study revealed that FPC certainly Kiyomiya, E., Ikeda, T., Tomita, Y., 1965. Hukuzatu sentaku hannou ni okeru sagyo
houhou to performance tono kankei ni tuite - Shisa-Kanko no kouka ni tuite no
facilitates the cognitive control processes of the working memory, yobiteki kentou (Procedure of complex selection response and performance:
particularly the supervisory attentional system or the central a preliminary study on the effect of “finger-pointing and calling”). Bulletin of
executive. The findings obtained in this study may have some Railway Labour Science Research Institute 17, 289e295 (in Japanese).
Kramer, A.F., Hahn, S., Gopher, D., 1999. Task coordination and aging: explorations of
practical implications for improving FPC in actual working situa- executive control processes in the task switching paradigm. Acta Psychologica
tions. For example, FPC seems to be effective when an operator who 101, 339e378.
is checking a visual display not only has to memorize the content of Latorella, K.A., Prabhu, P.V., 2000. A review of human error in aviation maintenance
and inspection. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26, 133e161.
the display but also to select one behavior from several possible Liang, G.-F., Lin, J.-T., Hwang, S.-L., Wang, E.M., Patterson, P., 2010. Preventing human
alternatives in response to the content of the display and other errors in aviation maintenance using an on-line maintenance assistance plat-
situational conditions. This is because FPC can support the retrieval form. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 40, 356e367.
Meijman, T.F., 1997. Mental fatigue and the efficiency of information processing in
and activation of memorized content. We can assess the relative
relation to work times. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 20, 31e38.
effectiveness of FPC in individual cases based on the degree of Miyake, A., Emerson, M.J., Padilla, F., Ahn, J.-C., 2004. Inner speech as a retrieval aid
involvement of cognitive control processes. Moreover, these find- for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the
random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica 115, 123e142.
ings are useful for developing more persuasive and effective safety
Monsell, S., 2003. Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, 134e140.
training programs because they are empirically derived evidence of Nalanagula, D., Greenstein, J.S., Gramopadhye, A.K., 2006. Evaluation of the effect of
the effectiveness of FPC. feedforward training displays of search strategy on visual search performance.
International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 36, 289e300.
5. Conclusions Nilsson, L.G., 2000. Remembering actions and words. In: Tulving, E., Craik, F.I.M.
(Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford University Press, New York,
pp. 137e148.
The current study revealed that when FPC was carried out to Norman, D., 1981. Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review 88, 1e15.
a task cue that indicated the task rule to be applied, it facilitated the Norman, D., Shallice, T., 1986. Attention to action: willed and automatic control of
subsequent response. This indicates that the use of FPC can facili- behavior. In: Davidson, R.J., Schwartz, G.E., Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-
regulation. Advances in Research and Theory 4. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1e18.
tate the retrieval and activation of a specific task rule that is Posner, M.I., 1980. Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
selected from three alternatives. In other words, FPC facilitates the Psychology 32, 3e25.
cognitive control processes responsible for the supervisory atten- Ratcliff, R., 1993. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological
Bulletin 114, 510e532.
tional system or the central executive of the working memory Reason, J., 1990. Human Error. Cambridge University Press, New York.
system. The findings of this study are expected to contribute to Rogers, R.D., Monsell, S., 1995. Costs of a predictable switch between simple
improving the application of FPC in real working situations; the cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124, 207e231.
Saeki, E., Saito, S., 2004. Effect of articulatory suppression on task-switching perfor-
contributions include encouraging the introduction of FPC, evalu- mance: implications for models of working memory. Memory 12, 257e271.
ating the relative effectiveness of FPC, and updating FPC imple- Salamé, P., Baddeley, A.D., 1986. Phonological factors in STM: similarity and the
mentation policies and safety training programs in industry. unattended speech effect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24, 263e265.
136 K. Shinohara et al. / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 43 (2013) 129e136

Salamé, P., Baddeley, A.D., 1987. Noise, unattended speech and short-term-memory. Shinohara, K., Kimura, T., 2010. Development and validation of an easy-to-answer
Ergonomics 30, 1185e1194. checklist of subjective mental workload. The Japanese Journal of Ergonomics
Salamé, P., Baddeley, A.D., 1989. Effects of background music on phonological short- 46 (Suppl.), 392e393 (in Japanese).
term-memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Shinohara, K., Morimoto, K., Kubota, T., 2009. The effect of “finger-pointing and call”
Experimental Psychology 41, 107e122. on orientation of visual attention. The Japanese Journal of Ergonomics 45, 54e57
Salamé, P., Baddeley, A.D., 1990. The effects of irrelevant speech on immediate free (in Japanese).
recall. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28, 540e542. Watanabe, H., Kawaguchi, J., Naki, Y., Tsukada, T., Hikono, M., Nakamura, H., 2005.
Salvucci, D.D., Beltowska, J., 2008. Effects of memory rehearsal on driver Cognitive psychological approach for an effective Shisa-Kosyo in a field
performance: experiment and theoretical account. Human Factors 50, depended on human memory function. The Japanese Journal of Ergonomics 41,
834e844. 237e243 (in Japanese with English abstract).

S-ar putea să vă placă și