Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

Gabriel Alvarez <gaboinkl@gmail.com>

The neuroscience of anxiety


1 mensaje

Scott Young <newsletter@scotthyoung.com> 26 de noviembre de 2019, 14:24


Para: Gabriel <gaboinkl@gmail.com>

Hello Gabriel,

Few things are as unpleasant as anxiety. With the explosion of interest in


mindfulness meditation and books like The Subtle Art of Not Giving a F*ck,
I’m hardly alone in wanting to worry less.

Recently, I read Anxious by NYU neuroscientist Joseph LeDoux. LeDoux is


an expert on a lot of the neural processes behind fear and anxiety, and this
book offers fascinating insights into the underlying mechanisms that make
miserable.

At the same time, however, Anxious isn’t an easy read. Many sections
involved deciphering neural circuit diagrams, trying to keep track of
countless acronyms for minuscule brain regions. This essay is my attempt
to summarize some of the main insights of the book to help my own
understanding of anxiety and for anyone else who wants to worry less.

Fear and Consciousness


A huge chunk of the opening of the book is devoted to what, at first glance,
seems to be a rather abstract problem: do the parts of the brain that are
active when we observe fearful behavior really make us feel fear?

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 1/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

If you’ve followed any pop-sci articles on neuroscience, you’ve probably


heard that the amygdala, an almond-shaped chunk of brain beneath the
cortex, is the brain’s “fear center.” Scare someone (or a mouse in a
laboratory) and the amygdala gets activated.

This has led to a wave of reporting that center our feelings of fear inside
this little knob of brain tissue. This is unfortunate, in LeDoux’s estimation,
because it’s not at all clear that signals processed by the amygdala are
conscious at all.

Instead, LeDoux prefers the term “survival circuit” since, while it’s clear that
the amygdala is involved in responding to threats, it’s not clear that activity
here directly generates conscious feelings. Fear, as an experience, likely
happens elsewhere, possibly in higher cortical areas of the brain that work
with attention, thinking, imagining our futures and remembering our pasts.

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 2/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

Put another way, if a threat falls in the amygdala, but there’s nobody there
to feel it, does it actually make you fear? To LeDoux, anxiety you don’t
consciously experience isn’t anxiety at all.

Side note: Fear, anxiety and worrying have different,


technical meanings. Fear is the feeling associated with
imminent danger. Anxiety is the feeling of uncertain threat.
Worrying is anxious and repetitive thinking.

The Adaptive Unconscious and Fear


This division between nonconscious survival circuitry and conscious
experience is reflected in more than just anxiety. In Strangers to Ourselves,
psychologist Timothy Wilson argues that much of our mental operation
exists outside of conscious thought.

The idea of an unconscious mind isn’t new, of course. Freud famously


argued that our unconscious minds was a surreal dreamscape full of
repressed desires about genital jealousy and wanting to romance our
mothers.

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 3/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

This new picture of an unconscious, what Wilson calls the adaptive


unconscious is much more mundane. The adaptive unconscious manages
the business of living, often guiding our behavior without our awareness.

What we experience, of our mental processing, may not be just


the tip of the iceberg, it may be merely a snowflake resting atop
that tip.

A classic demonstration of the adaptive unconscious are priming


experiments. If you flash an image, and then quickly flash a kind of
masking stimulus, subjects cannot consciously report what they saw. You
can tell they can’t consciously report it, because when asked to guess
about what it was, they do no better than chance.

Nonetheless, if that image is of a frightening face, unconscious threat


circuits are mildly activated. Although you may not be able to consciously
notice any difference, behavioral responses, such as muscle movements in
the face, can be measured.

Experiments such as these suggest that a lot of sophisticated mental


processing not only happens beneath our everyday awareness, it is
completely inaccessible to our conscious mind.

Where Do Feelings of Anxiety Come From?

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 4/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

If threat circuitry isn’t directly conscious then, how does it impact our
experience of anxiety which very much is conscious? In this case it’s likely
that while the threat circuitry may not result in conscious experiences itself,
it may trigger other mental processes which do cause us to worry.

One way this has been suggested is through bodily feedback. You
experience some fearful stimulus, your amygdala and related areas react
quickly and increases your heart rate. You feel your heart beating faster
and your mind interprets this as fear.

While the bodily-feedback loop is a popular theory, LeDoux doubts it can


be the total explanation. This feedback is probably just too slow for it to be
the main pathway by which threat circuitry turns into conscious feelings.

Instead it might be that the amygdala triggers patterns in higher cortical


areas which we can consciously experience as fear. In this case, the
feelings we experience as anxiety are a downstream effect of the threat
circuitry that we don’t experience directly.

Still, the coupling between threat-circuitry and feelings of fear is not perfect.
As already discussed, it’s possible to activate the threat-circuitry mildly
without creating any conscious awareness (although behavioral responses
are still observable). Conversely, it might also be possible to feel anxious
even if there are no threat-response circuits firing at all.

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 5/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

The overall feeling of dread and constant worrying that characterizes a lot
of anxiety may not need a trigger from threat circuits to become active. It
may be a purely higher-order mental phenomenon. As a result,
interventions that work by clamping down on threat-circuits may not help
much in these cases.

Threat-Circuits and Anti-Anxiety Drugs


This bifurcation of systems: a nonconscious threat-response system as
well as consciously felt fear, may have implications on our ability to treat
anxiety.

For instance, LeDoux notes that drug companies haven’t always had great
success at discovering anti-anxiety medications. When something does
help with anxiety, it’s often discovered by accident, rather than design.

The separation of threat circuitry from conscious fear may partly explain
why. Drugs are often selected by their ability to reduce fear-like behavior in
laboratory animals. In doing so, however, they may be operating on
nonconscious rather than conscious circuitry. This may not help as much
when the goal is to treat conscious feelings of anxiety.

Exposure Therapy
This perspective might also have implications for improving therapy as
well. Exposure therapy is a fairly successful therapy for combating anxiety.
It works by exposing a patient to the object of their fears. When they
experience fear but nothing bad follows, the fear will be a little less next
time. LeDoux notes that around 70% of patients do get some help from
exposure therapy.

Success isn’t perfect, however. Spontaneous recovery, whereby a


previously extinguished fear returns back, seemingly without reason, can
occur. Stress or trauma can also trigger remissions.

One reason for this seems to be that the memory for threat and the
memory for safety are actually two distinct neural circuits. When you initial
learn the threat, and then later extinguish the threat circuit through

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 6/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

exposure, this doesn’t work by erasing the original memory, but by creating
a second memory designed to suppress the first one. It’s as if, instead of
erasing the fearful picture, you’ve merely painted over it with a fresh coat of
paint, one that might get scratched off later by accident.

Interestingly this memory for safety that suppresses the original threat
response may be more context-sensitive than the threat circuit itself. If you
learn not to panic at a social gathering while with your friend, you may still
panic when that person isn’t there to accompany you. Reducing this
context-sensitivity requires many different exposures in varying situations,
otherwise the experience may not transfer.

Side note: Context-specificity shows up in another context—


learning. In Ultralearning, I reviewed research showing that
the things we learn tend to be more specific and context-
dependent than we expect. That this shows up in so many
different contexts suggests it is a fairly general principle of
how our brains work.

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 7/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

Why We Get Stuck in Anxiety


Anxiety can be self-reinforcing. Say you’re terrified of public speaking. You
have an upcoming speech and you start feeling worse and worse about it.
Finally, you come up with an excuse to avoid speaking. Now you feel
better, whatever you were worried might happen can’t happen.

This actually creates a pattern of negative reinforcement. By removing


something unpleasant (your anxious feelings) you reinforce the behavior
that created it. Since what you fear never happens, the pattern to avoid
what you fear gets stronger each time you feel anxious.

In nature, this pattern probably works fairly well. A rabbit that almost gets
eaten while near a certain watering hole may never go back there. This
may result in a little thirst, but it’s better than being eat by a crocodile.

The problem is that the avoidance strategy can work too well. It may be
that the situation you’re afraid of is actually safe. By avoiding it, however,
you never experience the disconfirming evidence that you don’t need to be
afraid. In some cases, avoidance itself becomes worse than the thing
you’re afraid of, as you take increasingly costly steps to avoid situations
that might be scary.

Exposure therapy works to disable this feedback loop. Expose yourself to


the thing you’re afraid of, and when nothing devastating happens, you
diminish the fear response the next time. Repeat this and, eventually, the
fear you experience will hopefully be mild enough so that it no longer
interferes with your life.

While this is easiest to understand with phobias, there’s probably a similar


avoidance reinforcement mechanism underlying a lot of more general
anxiety and worrying. An imagined, fearful situation arises, and then you
worry obsessively to find some possible escape. Since most catastrophic
worries never actually happen, you’ve successfully avoided it and so the
worrying behavior is reinforced.

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 8/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

Extinguishing Fears and Changing Beliefs


Modern exposure therapy, however, is usually more than just direct
exposure. It usually also involves cognitive therapy, which involves talking
about your fears. The hope here is to make explicit some of your beliefs so
you can start to question them. Our glossophobe might worry that if he
bombs on stage, his boss will fire him. This may not be realistic, but unless
it’s made explicit, it’s very difficult to evaluate this thought pattern.

According to LeDoux, these two different aspects work on the two different
systems that contribute to feelings of anxiety. Exposure targets the threat-
response circuitry, a largely nonconscious response. Only change
someone’s beliefs, but leave the threat circuits untouched and someone
may still be paralyzed with anxiety even though they know it’s completely
irrational. For instance, you may “know” that standing in a glass elevator is
totally safe, but be petrified looking down over the edge.

Similarly, if you only extinguish the threat circuit, higher-cognitive systems


may reactivate it later. You hear about plane crash and all of a sudden your
worries about flying resurface in ways you can’t control.

Implications of Anxious and Further Thoughts


The existence of parallel nonconscious and conscious states, and indeed,
treating the two as somewhat separate systems that both contribute to
https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 9/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

behavior is an interesting insight I gleaned from the book.

In my own life, I often find myself talking through my problems, either by


myself or with others. Sometimes this process can help. Writing down my
plans, worries, goals and problems in a journal, or discussing it with a
friend, can often lead to new insights or reveal flaws in my thinking that
lead to breakthroughs.

In other cases, overthinking is exactly my problem. Thinking leads to more


thinking which goes nowhere. Particularly in cases where I’m worried, this
thinking becomes compulsive and difficult to control.

This book suggests that neglecting the nonconscious processing may be a


major weakness here. Since many of the mental processes happen outside
of awareness, they can’t really be fixed by thinking about them more.
Alternative approaches, such as exposure, may be more effective in these
cases because they work on the problem more directly.

This book also echos another neuroscience book I reviewed earlier, The
Hungry Brain. In that book, sophisticated circuits control how much we eat
which can explain why dieting is so hard. Like anxiety, these processes
happen without our awareness, yet seem to exert incredible sway over our
lives.

In some ways, the existence of these unseen processes makes things


more difficult. How do you make decisions when many of your actions are
decided by processes you have no access to? How do you effect change
in your life when the causes of your behavior are often invisible? How do
you know yourself when so much is not open to introspection?

These questions don’t have easy answers. But I hope by exploring these,
we might uncover new tools to worry less.

___

Did you find my article “The Neuroscience of Anxiety” helpful or


know somebody who would? I’d really love if you could share it:

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A165129354… 10/11
9/12/2019 Gmail - The neuroscience of anxiety

Best,
-Scott

Unsubscribe | Update your profile | #83-1146 Pacific Blvd., Vancouver, British Columbia V6Z 2X7

https://mail.google.com/mail/u/0?ik=f510d5ad1d&view=pt&search=all&permthid=thread-f%3A1651293549457479203&simpl=msg-f%3A1651293549… 11/11

S-ar putea să vă placă și