Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

The Relationship of Personality

to Affective Disorders
A Critical Review

Hagop S. Akiskal, MD; Robert M. A. Hirschfeld, MD; Boghos I. Yerevanian, MD

\s=b\ Although characterologic constellations such as obses- widely believed that psychopathologic states arise from
sionalism, dependency, introversion, restricted social skills, enduring, preexisting, and predisposing traits that con¬
and maladaptive self-attributions are popularly linked to the stitute the premorbid personality. However, while depres¬
pathogenesis of depressive disorders, the evidence in support sive episodes may be superimposed on cyclothymic, dys-
of this relationship remains modest. Indeed, many of these thymic, dependent, or compulsive personalities, affective
attributes may reflect state characteristics woven into the psychoses may also arise out of personality structures that
postdepressive personality. Current evidence is strongest for are either unremarkable or insufficiently deviant to war¬
introversion as a possible premorbid trait in primary nonbipo- rant personality diagnoses.4,5
lar depressions. By contrast, driven, work-oriented obsessoid, The postulated relationship between personality and
extroverted, cyclothymic, and related dysthymic tempera- affective states has been developed largely from clinical
ments appear to be the precursors of bipolar disorders. Other reconstruction of the premorbid histories of patients stud¬
personalities, while not necessarily pathogenic in affective ied during, or subsequent to, an illness episode.6 Although
disorders, nevertheless may modify the clinical expression of these clinical approaches have generated valuable insights,
affective disorders and their prognosis. they have been subject to the bias inherent in such meth¬
(Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983;40:801-810) ods.7 The assumption that personality disturbance precedes
affective episodes has yet to be demonstrated in large-scale,
rigorous prospective research. In fact, current evidence
physicians conceptualized illness, including
Hippocratic
melancholia,
premorbid
in dimensional terms as an
characteristics.1 In modern
outgrowth of
times, full-scale
suggests that such disturbance not uncommonly represents
the sequel of incompletely remitted affective episodes.8"11
This critical assessment of the relationship between
application of this dimensional concept to psychiatric disor¬ personality and affective illness will focus on previous
ders led Kretschmer to hypothesize that the "endogenous research efforts conducted within these méthodologie con¬
psychoses are nothing other than marked accentuation of straints (as well as attempts to circumvent them), and on
normal types of temperament."2 This line of reasoning is emerging clinically relevant findings about this complex
also typical of dynamic thinking that considers manifest relationship. The literature review is selective, chosen to
psychopathology as the symptomatic expression of charac- highlight specific méthodologie issues.
terologic tendencies of developmental origin.3 Thus, it is TERMINOLOGIC AND CONCEPTUAL ISSUES

Accepted for publication Nov 29, 1982. Terms like temperament, character, and personality have
From the Affective Disorders Program, Departments of Psychiatry been used almost synonymously to subsume more or less
(Drs Akiskal and Yerevanian) and Pharmacology (Dr Akiskal), University of enduring response sets. The term temperament, long asso¬
Tennessee College of Medicine, Memphis; Sleep Disorders Center, Labora- ciated with humoral theory, is best reserved for genetically
tory of Neurophysiology, Baptist Memorial Hospital, Memphis (Dr Akis- or constitutionally determined tendencies. Character, by
kal); and the Clinical Research Branch and Center for the Study of Affective
Disorders, National Institute of Mental Health, Alcohol, Drug Abuse and contrast, generally refers to the learned attributes orig¬
Mental Health Administration, Rockville, Md (Dr Hirschfeld). Dr Yereva- inating in developmental experiences within the family
nian is now with the Affective Disorders Unit, Department of Psychiatry,
structure. Of the three terms, personality has the broadest
University of Rochester, NY.
Reprint requests to Suite 633, Department of Psychiatry, University of meaning, embracing the joint contribution of inborn tem¬
Tennessee, 66 N Pauline St, Memphis, TN 38163 (Dr Akiskal). peramental and acquired characterologic determinants of

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


psychologie and behavioral traits. Within this definition, ality patterns are the expression of a major affective dis¬
personality disturbances can be incidental to, causative in, position. Periodic anger outbursts30 and periodic obsessio¬
or a sequel of adult psychiatric disorders. Just as one can nal symptoms31 are reported to respond to lithium carbon¬
speak of personality disturbances that are secondary to the ate, suggesting their kinship to bipolar affective disorder.
experience of a disabling medical disorder, it is legitimate to However, monozygotic-dizygotic twin differences in rates of
speak of personality disturbances secondary to affective obsessionalism have not provided unequivocal support for
disorder; these sequelae could be short term or long term. hereditary contributions to this personality type.20
However, in clinical discussions, developmentally ac¬ The foregoing considerations suggest that personality
quired components of personality are usually given more and affective disorder are linked in a variety of ways.4,5
weight. Despite Freud's statement that "endowment and Authors in both the psychodynamic and behavioral tradi¬
chance determine a man's fate—rarely or never one of these tions have generally assumed that specific character traits
powers alone,"12 the psychogenetic point of view has domi¬ provide vulnerability to affective episodes; whether this
nated the conventional psychologic literature.3,13 Current vulnerability is viewed as involving developmental fixation
evidence indicates that hereditary-genetic factors do con¬ points6 or deeply ingrained maladaptive self-attribu¬
tribute to personality development; according to Körner, "it tions,13,32 the evidence in its support has generally come
is likely that genetic differences in the biologic sense create from retrospective reconstruction of personality attributes.
genetic differences in the psychoanalytic sense."14 Ben¬ Other authors79 have limited their hypotheses to more
jamin has gone so far as to state that "not only can innate immediately observable relationships between personality
differences in drive organization, in ego functions, and in and affective illness, and have postulated that personality,
maturational rates determine different responses to objec¬ while not necessarily involved in pathogenesis, may never¬
tively identical experiences, but they can also help deter¬ theless modify the clinical expression or the outcome of the
mine what experiences will be experienced."15 illness. Neokraepelinian psychiatrists,2,4 on the other hand,
The most compelling evidence for the contribution of have taken the stand that the personalities observed in the
heredity to personality formation is provided by adoption affectively ill represent either genetically attenuated tem¬
studies. For instance, the adopted-away offspring of so- peramental forms of the illness or secondary social compli¬
ciopathic persons, who share their biologic parents' genes cations of recurrently incapacitating affective episodes. We
but are reared by nonsociopathic adoptive parents, have a shall next examine the evidence for each of these view¬
rate of personality disorder that exceeds the risk in the points.
general population.16 Monozygotic-dizygotic differences THE CHARACTEROLOGIC PREDISPOSITION
have suggested that heredity significantly contributes to
TO AFFECTIVE EPISODES
the introversión-extraversión dimension17 and to "orality"
in men and hysterical traits in women.18 By contrast, it has In broad terms, this approach presupposes that certain
been reported that low self-confidence, shyness, passive- learned characterologic propensities are etiologic antece¬
dependence, and depressive tendencies exceed chance ex¬ dents of affective illness. Research in this area has ranged
pectation in the daughters of alcoholics only when raised from clinical explorations to studies employing sophisti¬
with their biologic fathers, suggesting that developmental cated measurements.
experiences are the dominant determinants for such traits Clinical Explorations
in this group.19 Many of these issues are reviewed by
Rainer.20 Early analytic investigations of the origins of psychiatric
Other approaches that may provide insight into biologic syndromes emphasized constitutionally determined fixa¬
contributions involve the demonstration that certain per¬ tion points, implicating faults in the maturation of key
sonality types are strongly correlated with certain biologic biologic drives in etiology.33 Despite the astuteness of these
markers characteristic of primary affective disorders.21 For clinical observations and the eagerness to formulate etiol¬
instance, recent studies have shown that subgroups of pa¬ ogy in the metabiologic language of the day, Freud and his
tients with characterologic depression22 and borderline early followers were limited to a metapsychologic level of
personality23 have rapid eye movement (REM) latencies in discourse by the lack of the requisite biologic technology.
the range for primary depressives, but significantly shorter Abraham34 is usually credited as being the first to suggest
than those of noncase and nonaffective personality controls. that persons prone to depressive episodes resembled obses¬
Other proposed research strategies include familial loading sional neurotics in having prominent "anal" traits, but
for primary affective disorders, unequivocal response to differed in having high levels of "oral" dependency. Abra¬
such interventions as tricyclic antidepressants, monoamine ham's report is prototypical of clinical-dynamic approaches
oxidase (MAO) inhibitors, lithium carbonate, or sleep in the following ways: (1) affective disorder was treated as a
deprivation. For instance, cyclothymic personalities were unitary condition, (2) insights gained from the clinical study
found to have a significantly higher proportion of manic- of six manic depressives were generalized, (3) neither the
depressive biologic kin than a nonaffective personality dependent (affective state) nor the independent (character)
disorder group.24 Brief hypomanic responses to tricyclic variable was measured by reliable, objective, and standard¬
antidepressants25 or sleep deprivation26 are largely limited ized instruments, (4) no control groups were utilized to test
to primary mood disorders, and can therefore be used clini¬ the specificity of the character type to affective type, (5)
cally to distinguish affectively based dysthymic tempera¬ despite the fact that information about premorbid personal¬
ments22,27 from the large universe of chronically dysphoric ity was obtained retrospectively, it was postulated that
characterologic disturbances subsumed under the broad dynamics (condensed in a character diagnosis) precede
rubric of dysthymia in DSM-III.28 Likewise, Klein et al29 psychopathology (affective episodes), and (6) it was as¬
have argued that "hysteroid dysphoria" has a strong bio¬ sumed that the co-occurrence of certain characterologic
logic core because of its responsivity to MAO inhibitors. traits and affective episodes indicated a causal relationship.
Finally, unequivocal response of patients with personality Abraham did not consider the possibility that they could
disorders to lithium carbonate in a placebo crossover design both spring from the same etiologic foundations.
lends modest support to the contention that certain person- Although some of these criticisms also apply to studies by

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


Cohen et al,35 their focus on the predisposition to manic- esized to underly minor depressions has received both
depressive illness, without generalizing to all depressions, sociopsychologic (ie, "learned helplessness")47 and socio¬
was a méthodologie advance. These authors hypothesized biologie (ie, attachment as a critical evolutionary role for
that parental attitudes led to the development of ambitious, women)48 interpretations.
driven, and extraverted persons who were superficially In summary, psychoanalytic attempts to delineate the
well adjusted, but concealed strong dependency needs. As characterologic matrix of affective disorders have suffered
the prestige bearers of the family, the life goals of these fu¬ from a number of méthodologie flaws. Also, they have been
ture manic-depressives were set very high, making them more concerned with generating hypotheses than testing
highly susceptible to minor setbacks and prone to respond hypotheses. Definitive testing of these views should be of
with guilt and self-denigration. These findings were par¬ high priority given the influence they continue to exert on
tially supported by Gibson.36 This formulation survived as clinical practice.
the prevalent clinical-dynamic explanation of the character¬
ologic predisposition to bipolar mood swings, despite Jacob- Psychometric Studies
son's warning37 that cyclothymic oscillations in mood were Psychometric studies have generally avoided the méth¬
"endogenous" or temperamental in nature. odologie problems of the psychoanalytic literature with one
Chodoff,6 reviewing the dynamic literature on the depres¬ exception: they are all retrospective.
sion-prone individual half a century after Abraham, con¬ Von Zerssen, in his review of the world literature,4
cluded that a liberal mixture of obsessional and dependent concluded that the "melancholic type" was characterized by
traits was the hallmark of such personalities. While this "orderliness, conscientiousness, meticulousness, high
hypothesis has some support in clinical observations, it value achievement, conventional thinking, and dependency
cannot be proved by clinical disquisition. However, Chodoff on close personal relationships." This pattern was typified
did recognize many of the méthodologie inadequacies of the in the portrayal of the late-onset, usually psychotic, depres¬
early analytic approaches, and paved the way to modern sive as a self-critical, conscientious, hard-working, and
formulations. Persons prone to depression are viewed as socially well-integrated person who had responded to adult
highly dependent on narcissistic supplies from others to losses in a self-punitive and self-denigrating manner. These
maintain their self-regard; hence, they have a lowered findings were consistent across diverse cultural settings.
threshold for developing depression when such supplies are This essentially anankastic aspect of the melancholic
withdrawn as a result of adult object loss. Thus, Arieti and character has been supported by other authors. Cadoret et
Bemporad3 conceptualized depression as a derivative of al49 found that scores on the factor denoting "superego
unsatisfactory intimacy in early attachment leading to self- strength" on CattelPs personality inventory were highest in
esteem that is exquisitely vulnerable to adult viscissitudes: depressives with onset after the age of 40 years; scores were
the depressive finds himself unloved, yet clings to love also high in their "well" relatives compared with those of
objects whom he expects to be all-giving. Strupp et al38 have early-onset depressive probands. Kendell and Discipio,50
recently expressed similar views. In the extreme, the who used the Leyton inventory51 to measure obsessionalism
depressive character is considered to be a "love addict," during illness episodes and on clinical recovery in hospi¬
intolerant of any withdrawal of romantic attachments in talized depressives, could not confirm the age association,
adult life, reminiscent of the "hysteroid dysphoric" women but did find that recovered unipolar (usually psychotic)
described by Liebowitz and Klein39; their short-lived, non- depressives had scores intermediate between those of ob¬
autonomous, crushlike reactions occur in the setting of sessional neurotics and two groups of normal controls. In
histrionic and self-centered demeanor characterized by addition, these authors suggested that manic episodes
flamboyance, exhibitionism, intrusiveness, seductiveness, rarely arose from the substrate of marked obsessionalism or
demandingness, and suicide gestures. obsessional neurosis. This "protection" against mania ac¬
These views are concordant with Bowlby's conceptualiza¬ corded by marked premorbid obsessionalism strengthened
tion40 of depression as an expression of disrupted object their argument that such traits were linked with unipolar
relations in early childhood. However, in his latest writings, depressive illness. This study failed to confirm Videbech's52
Bowlby41 suggests that breaks in early bonds are not and Gittleson's53,54 observations that obsessional symptoms
specific to clinical depression, but generic to a broad intensify during depression in premorbidly obsessional
spectrum of characterologic difficulties that underlie many persons; furthermore, even in nonobsessional persons,
psychopathologic syndromes. Partial support for this posi¬ obsessional symptoms seemed to develop during depres¬
tion has come from studies showing that childhood loss of a sion, especially when depression was severe.
parent predicts high rates of suicide attempts.42,43 In brief, The inverse relationship between obsessionalism and
breaks in early object relations seem to predict a general mania was not replicated in the National Institute of Mental
characterologic disturbance distinct from clinical depres¬ Health (NIMH) Collaborative Study on the Psychobiology
sion11,27; the age at onset of these character disorder-based of Depression.55 Obsessionalism was in fact the only deviant
dysphorias is typically early, dysphoric symptoms are often personality measure associated with mania: manies' scores
described in hyperbolic terms, and clear-cut depressive fell between those of obsessional neurotics and published
episodes with endogenomorphic coloring are uncharacteris¬ norms for the US population. This finding is consistent with
tic. the observation that the extraverted, ambitious, and driven
Another line of thinking about the characterologic vul¬ person with an inordinate capacity for work—a profile
nerability depression can be traced from Freud's for¬
to frequently linked to bipolar illness—has considerable af¬
mulation of hostile introjects,44 to Klein's notions about the finities to the duty-bound and work-addicted compulsive
depressive and paranoid positions,45 to contemporary ob¬ person.5 Thus, the "syntonic" type, the Sjobring descrip¬
servations of depression manifesting in self-punitive cogni¬ tion of the bipolar personality,56 overlaps considerably with
tions in Judeo-Christian cultures and in paranoid-pro- the "anankastic" (or compulsive) personality of the Anglo-
jective mechanisms in some African cultures.46 Why women Saxon literature.57 This is not to say that a unitary personal¬
are presumably more vulnerable to these hostile-depend¬ ity type characterizes the premorbid adjustment of bipolar
ent, self-punitive, and "passive" stances that are hypoth- patients. Apart from the syntonic-anankastic type, "sub-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


stable" (extraverted or hyperthymic), cyclothymic, and have emerged as relatively robust premorbid attributes in a
even dysthymic temperaments have been reported to pre¬ recent NIMH collaborative study (in progress).57 This study
cede bipolar psychoses.2,22,24 is attempting to address the méthodologie limitations of
Perris58 contrasted the "substable" personality of bipolar previous studies by comparing recovered primary nonbipo-
depressives to the "sub valid" attributes of unipolar depres¬ lar depressives with their recovered first-degree and never-
sives. Subvalidity refers to persons who are bound to ill relatives and population controls.
routine, easy to tire, neurasthenic, cautious, tense, and In summary, predisposing characterologic traits for de¬
meticulous—a characterologic spectrum much wider than pressive illness have yet to be demonstrated in a prospec¬
obsessionalism. tive design. The following attributes represent a distillate
Wittenborn and Maurer,59 who examined personalities at of the current literature on the subject:55 (1) introversion, (2)
the depth of depression and a year later on clinical recovery, lack of self-confidence, (3) nonassertiveness, (4) deficiency
reported a wide range of neurotic characterologic traits in social adroitness, (5) dependence, (6) tendency to worry
that persisted in many but not all apparently recovered and obsessionalism, and (7) pessimism. Although such a
depressives, including (1) a tendency to blame others, (2) a constellation may simply represent nonspecific personality
demanding and complaining attitude, (3) low self-confi¬ incompetence common to a wide variety of psychiatric
dence, and (4) a tendency toward moodiness and worry. The syndromes,66 we submit that the evidence reviewed impli¬
authors concluded that in some patients the tendency cates introversion in nonbipolar conditions, and extraver¬
toward obsessional brooding and moodiness is state de¬ sion and possibly obsessionalsim in bipolar disorders.
pendent and may temporarily persist as an interepisodic PERSONALITY AS A MODIFIER OF AFFECTIVE EPISODES
manifestation of depressive illness; in others this tendency
may represent premorbid traits. It was further speculated This viewpoint refrains from etiologic speculations re¬
that intensification of obsessionalism and denial of anger at garding personality and the pathogenesis of affective disor¬
the onset of a depressive episode may serve a defensive der, limiting itself to the more observable impact of person¬
function in a person overwhelmed by stress and preoc¬ ality on the clinical picture, treatment, and outcome of
cupied with the fear of losing control. Such reasoning has affective episodes (the so-called pathoplasty hypothesis). A
led Telenbach60 to suggest that the traits of orderliness, classic example of this approach is a study by Lazare and
guilt, sociability, and concern for others are a defense Klerman67 in hospitalized depressed women that provided
against depressives' tendency toward disorganization, hos¬ evidence that certain traits modified the symptomatic
tility, and self-preoccupation, which represent their basic expression of the depressive episode. The obsessional
or primary personality traits. woman, overwhelmed by fear of loss of control, presented
Metcalfe61 administered the Eysenck Personality Inven¬ an agitated and more severe depression than the hysteroid
tory (EPI) to women who had recovered from a severe character, whose demanding and seductive stance made her
depression and compared them with controls without such appear less depressed than she actually was. One prediction
history. Although the overall neuroticism scores of the two from this observation is that, contrary to the clinical
groups were similar, two personality attributes seemed to stereotype, persons with histrionic characters may suffer
characterize the experimental group: (1) worry and tense¬ from unrecognized severe depressions and even commit
ness, and (2) rigid, unimaginative, and "habit-bound" atti¬ suicide. Data from the University of Tennessee" support
tudes. The author concluded that the depressive is not this prediction. Also related to the Lazare-Klerman for¬
necessarily one who is prone to the development of depres¬ mulation is Bart's concept of "metadepression,"68 the deep¬
sion, but one who has difficulty recovering from such ening of depression in obsessional persons who subscribe to
illness. This interpretation is in line with Seligman's the Anglo-Saxon work ethic and become demoralized be¬
"learned helplessness" hypothesis,47 and Lewinsohn's em¬ cause of the limitations imposed by their depressive state.
phasis on deficient social skills.62 However, deficits in in¬ Such persons tend to believe they are "weak" or "lazy"
strumental behavior are not necessarily antecedent person¬ rather than ill.
ality attributes; insecurity, passive-dependence, and a gen¬ These data from various authors indicate that the charac¬
eral lack of social assertiveness could be secondary to the ter structure, while not necessarily pathogenic in affective
experience of recurrently incapacitating episodes of de¬ illness, may modify its symptomatic expression. Vaz Serra
pression.63 For the same reason, the cognitive triad of and Polliti69 have recently supported such a position on the
hopelessness, helplessness, and self-denigration32 should be basis of data showing high correlations between neuroti¬
regarded as epiphenomenal until it is shown prospectively cism scores on the EPI70 and the "psychologic" symptoms of
to antedate clinical depression. This sense of defeat and depression, and lack of correlation between such scores and
demoralization29 is conceivably the final common pathway of the "biologic" manifestations of depression. However, one
many neurotic and psychotic disorders, and may not be cannot conclude that neuroticism favors the occurrence of
specific to affective disorders. psychologic symptoms; it is possible that both neuroticism
The work of Beck, Seligman, and Lewinsohn has stimu¬ and psychologic symptoms reflect the same measure.
lated much research on the social and interpersonal roots of Personality factors may also determine prognosis or
depression. In addition to being retrospective, much of this treatment compliance. For example, it has been reported
research13 has been based on nonclinical populations and/or that high neuroticism scores in depression correlate with
depressions falling short of syndromal criteria. Risk factors poor outcome.71 In another study, "unstable" character¬
described for such depressions64—female sex, loss of ologic traits were considered the strongest predictor of
mother during childhood, lack of intimate confiding rela¬ unfavorable social outcome in affective disorders.11 "Double
tionships, disinclination to seek help, and maladaptive self- depressions,"72,73 ie, coexisting depressive personalities and
attributions—therefore may not apply to clinical (syndro¬ major depressive episodes defined by Research Diagnostic
mal) depressions.65 Criteria,74 have also been shown to have poor outcome. It
It is apparent that méthodologie challenges in the area of would appear that in all of these studies, unfavorable
personalityand affective disorders are complex but not outcome reflects the unstable and neurotic traits measured
insurmountable. Indeed, introversion and low sociability by the followup instrument rather than the failure of

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


affective symptoms to resolve. These authors concluded that social recovery lagged behind
Outcome is also related to noncompliance, which is itself symptomatic recovery. It is possible, however, that these
largely determined by personality factors. For instance, it interpersonal maladjustments in part reflect neurophysio-
has been suggested that noncompliance with lithium car¬ logic disturbances that are now known to persist for many
bonate therapy is most likely in predominantly hyper- months after symptomatic recovery.84 The practical implica¬
thymic bipolar patients who believe they achieve consider¬ tions of these findings are to continue antidepressants for
able creativity during "highs."75,76 eight to nine months beyond symptomatic improvement and
Finally, the characterologic disturbance may play an to provide short-term interpersonal psychotherapy.85,86
indirect etiologic role by favoring the occurrence of life
events that precipitate depression in vulnerable individu¬
Chronic Sequelae
als, or creating interpersonal circumstances that maintain Cassano et al78 have recently distinguished between re¬
the chronicity of depression. Some support for this sugges¬ sidual affective symptoms, which may persist for a few
tion was obtained by Weissman and Paykel," who noted that months after an acute episode and are often, although not
impaired marital communication and related interpersonal invariably, associated with short-term social maladjust¬
disturbances during illness episodes alienate spouses and ment, and a permanent "depressive defect." The latter can
retard recovery from depression. Similar data have been occur if environmental, psychological, or iatrogenic factors
reported in a cohort of chronic primary unipolar depres¬ impede the delicate recovery phase. Such chronic personal¬
sives.77 ity changes are partly a continuation of the interpersonal
PERSONALITY AS A COMPLICATION maladjustment seen during acute and residual illness
OF AFFECTIVE ILLNESS phases, and partly the psychologic consequence of demoral¬
ization,87 failure, insecurity, dependence, pessimism, and
This approach is the converse of the clinical-dynamic an overall melancholy outlook resulting from repeated
stand in that personality attributes of depressed subjects illness episodes.88
are viewed as sequelae of affective illness. Within this Kraines,89 who devoted many years of prospective clinical
approach, however, one must distinguish between personal¬ observation to the natural history of affective disorders
ity changes during an affective episode, short-term inter¬ before the era of thymoleptic therapy, has described the
personal maladjustments following an affective episode, gradual evolution of postdepressive personality and neuro¬
and long-term personality changes resulting from recur¬ tic complications. One common pattern includes patients
rent affective episodes.78 who are demanding, manipulative, and hostile; others are
State-Dependent Changes irritable, chronically anxious, hypochondriacal, even ago¬
raphobic; another group is dependent and nonassertive; and
Significant personality disturbance has been found on a some patients become rigidly entrenched in work to the
variety of sensitive instruments administered during clini¬ exclusion of pleasure and social life. Obviously, a great deal
cal depression; these measures tend to change toward of overlap exists between these patterns.
published norms on clinical recovery.55,79,80 For instance, These chronic sequelae of affective disorder occur pre¬
many Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales dominantly in high-episode frequency or chronic forms of
other than D (depression) are elevated during clinical unipolar conditions. Such sequelae have been observed in
depressions;81 and neuroticism scores on the EPI are signifi¬ chronically depressed children who manifest hyperactivity
cantly elevated during depression and decreased in mania.55 and conduct disorder.90,91 The interepisodic personality of
Finally, comparing the same depressives during an episode the manic or bipolar patient is often thought to be remark¬
and following recovery, Hirschfeld et al63 reported signifi¬ ably nonneurotic, or free of characterologic disturbance.4,92
cant drops in measures of orality and interpersonal de¬ However, recent clinical observations*1"4 have documented
pendency, suggesting that such traits are strongly state the existence of considerable characterologic disturbances,
dependent. leading to social deterioration, in the long-term course of
Personality change during mania82 is so flagrant that it one third of bipolar patients.
does not require sensitive instruments for documentation.
PERSONALITY AS AN ATTENUATED EXPRESSION
Janowsky et al83 reported that the "interpersonal maneu¬ OF AFFECTIVE ILLNESS
vers" of manic patients, compared with those of other
psychotic patients, involved testing of limits, projection of According to this approach, enduring personality pecu¬
responsibility, detecting others' soft spots, attempts to liarities in affectively ill persons represent milder or alter¬
divide staff, flattering behavior, and ability to evoke anger. native expressions of the basic illness. A trait-state con¬
Impulsive action (eg, in marital and financial matters) as a tinuum is postulated between premorbid personality and
result of poor judgment during both mania and depression82 clinical episodes, and it is assumed that both personality
is probably the most serious personality disturbance in the and affective episodes are basically the expression of the
acute phase of affective illness. Because such action reflects same genetic or constitutional endowment.
psychotic disturbance rather than free choice, it is incum¬ The Classic Typology
bent on the clinician to prevent it, whenever feasible, by
direct and firm therapeutic approaches, eg, chemotherapy, Kraepelin82 was among the first authors to pay systematic
hospitalization, or urging the spouse or significant others to attention to the premorbid characteristics of affectively ill
take responsibility for finances. persons. In his attempt to account for the recurrence of
affective episodes, he postulated the existence of certain
Short-term Sequelae enduring personality characteristics, from which the more
The Weissman and Paykel study9 provided documentation psychotic affective states arose. Thus, depressive, manic,
ofsignificant interpersonal dysfunction despite apparent irritable (mixed), and cyclothymic (circular) personalities
symptomatic recovery. Clinically recovered depressives were considered the temperamental bases of the respective
had incomplete resumption of marital roles, and were full-blown forms of the illness. Kraepelin's hypothesis was
unable to communicate appropriate assertiveness freely. based on the observation that affective temperaments oc-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


curred in the premorbid histories of the majority of manic-
depressive probands who returned to their basic tempera¬
ment rather than to "normality" on remission; furthermore,
such temperaments—without progression to full-blown
illness—were overrepresented in the biologic relatives of
manic-depressive probands.
00 0 0
Hyperthymia DOOuQdO
Schneider's work95 on "psychopathic" (ie, deviant in a
statistical sense) personalities extended Kraepelin's obser¬
vations and led to descriptions of the depressive and hypo¬
manic types—the two basic temperaments in the kraepe-
linian scheme. Schneider's position was empirical and oper¬
11 III I III! Ill
Cyclothymia
W
ational; unlike Kraepelin, he did not genetically link these
two affective personalities to affective disorder.
Kretschmer,2 who subscribed to the kraepelinian concept
of a unity of affective psychoses, did not sharply demarcate
the affective temperaments. He defined a unitary cycloid or hiiminiii
cyclothymic type conforming to the following characteris¬ Predominantly Depressed
tics: (1) sociable, good-hearted, kind, and easygoing, (2) Cyclothymia
elated, humorous, lively, and hot-tempered, (3) intro¬
verted, quiet, calm, serious, and gentle. It was postulated
that all cycloids met the first criterion, that predominantly
hypomanic cycloids more often behaved according to the
traits listed under item 2, and predominantly depressed
lili lililí HI il
cycloids conformed to the third description. Kretschmer Subaffective Dysthymia Tricyclic
hypothesized that the cycloid personality was the tempera¬ Continuum between hyperthymic, cyclothymic, and dysthymic dis¬
mental precursor of manic-depressive psychosis, just as the orders. White rectangles represent elevated periods; gray rec¬
schizoid type was related to schizophrenic psychosis. He tangles, depressed periods; in subaffective dysthymia elevated
further postulated "all thinkable transitions and shadings period is in response to tricyclic challenge.
between health and disease,"2 ie, a continuum between
"healthy" cyclothymes, temperamentally "ill" cyclothymes
(the cycloids), and "psychotic" cyclothymes (manic-depres¬ characterologic disorders. These observations provide sup¬
sives). port for the kraepelinian hypothesis that the cyclothymic
The views of Sheldon et al96 were similar to Kretschmer's. personality is a genetically determined attenuated form,
They claimed that most manic-depressives would have a and sometimes a precursor, of manic-depressive psychosis.
predominance of "viscerotonia" (endomorphy) and "somato- These findings have been replicated in another clinical
tonia" (mesomorphy), with minimal features of "cerebro- setting,98 and most importantly, in a carefully conducted
tonia" (ectomorphy). Combining some of the temperamen¬ study of a nonpatient population.99 As expected, nonpatient
tal characteristics of these constitutions, one can derive a cyclothymics were, on the average, ten years younger than
composite cycloid type characterized by alternatives be¬ cyclothymic patients.
tween (1) sloth and energetic drivenness; (2) sociophilia or In other Tennessee studies,23,27 a subgroup of thymolep-
people-orientation social withdrawal; (3) aggressivity tic-responsive dysthymic probands (who generally met the
under alcohol "friendly" drinking; (4) poor sleep habits DSM-III criteria for dysthymia and the schneiderian crite¬
deep sleep; (5) extraversion introversion; and (6) need for ria95 for depressive personality) differed from a control
action need for people or solitude when troubled. group of primary unipolar depressives in having signifi¬
The thrust of these pioneering studies was that constitu¬ cantly higher rates of familial bipolar illness and hypomanic
tional factors determined both the temperament and the responses to tricyclic antidepressant therapy. Based on
manifest affective psychosis. Although "hereditary taint" such findings, a continuum between dysthymic and cy¬
was reported in many of these classic studies, their meth¬ clothymic temperaments was postulated100 (Figure). This is
odologies were retrospective and flawed by sampling bias concordant with Kretschmer's classic observations on the
and the absence of control groups. To provide more vigorous depressive temperament "which can swing to great ex¬
data on this issue, a prospective study24 was carried out at tremes. The path over which it swings is a wide one ... it
the University of Tennessee with a mental health center does . .
swing towards the cheerful side, but not so often
.

cohort that had never been hospitalized, but had serious, and not so far, it lingers over in the unhappy direction."2*30'
chronic interpersonal difficulties. Of this sample, 10% met In this framework, some forms of dysthymic disorder are
the criteria for cyclothymia, ie, short-lived subsyndromal conceptualized as special instances of cyclothymia where
biphasic mood swings (falling short of the definitions by brief hypomanic swings are apparent only on tricyclic
Feighner et al97 of depression and mania). Thus, what gave challenge.100 Recently, Turner and King101 have also classi¬
the appearance of a characterologic disturbance proved to fied dysthymia in the bipolar spectrum. Descriptive criteria
be a phenomenologically milder form of bipolar illness. The proposed elsewhere88 suggest that some personalities with
study also demonstrated heterogeneity within the cyclo¬ introverted-obsessoid features, who are habitually brood¬
thymic realm; 50% were predominantly depressive, 10% ing, guilt-ridden, gloomy, pessimistic, self-denigrating,
predominantly hypomanic, and 40% had a more equal anhedonic, and tend to oversleep—features reminiscent of
mixture of depressive and hypomanic swings. Further¬ Schneider's depressive personality—might be suffering
more, the family histories, prospective course, and phar¬ from a genetically attenuated but lifelong form of affective
macologie response to tricyclics in these cyclothymic pro- disorder. Careful evaluation of their lethargy will often
bands were indistinguishable from those of classic bipolar I reveal psychomotor inertia that is worse in the morning.
manic-depressives, but were distinct from controls with Furthermore, while initially introverted, these patients

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


sometimes appear extroverted and driven for short periods neurotic characterologic traits common to a large universe
of time (most typically in response to tricyclic challenge). of psychiatric dysfunction.
Temperament and Polarity of Illness PERSONALITY AS AN ORTHOGONAL DIMENSION
TO AFFECTIVE DISORDER
The studies reviewed thus far were conducted within the
framework of the kraepelinian unification82 of affective Given the complexities in elucidating the relationship
temperaments and affective psychoses into a genetic con¬ between personality and affective disorders, the clinician
tinuum wherein transitions are postulated to occur within may simply elect to note both conditions on separate axes.11
affective temperaments (as in the full-blown forms of the This will assure that neither the affective nor the personal¬
illness) and between temperaments and psychosis. The ity disturbance are overlooked. This is indeed the position
unipolar-bipolar dichotomy led to a corresponding dichoto- endorsed by DSM-III.28
mization of affective temperaments. To prove that unipolar The complexities of the interphase between personality
and bipolar psychoses were distinct entities, investigators and affective disorders are perhaps best exemplified by a
proceeded to demonstrate biologic and psychologic differ¬ group of difficult patients who exhibit joint manifestations
ences between these groups. Many early studies focused on of long-standing affective and flagrant personality disorder.
temperamental differences. For instance, Leonhard and They are variously referred to as neurotic, charactero¬
associates102 showed a preponderance of subdepressive tem¬ logic,22 or chronic intermittent (minor)74 depressions. Typ¬
peraments in the parents and offspring of unipolar pro- ically, the affective symptoms are mild or subsyndromal and
bands, and hypomanic and cyclothymic temperaments in pursue an intermittent chronic course, although full syn-
those of bipolar probands. Perris' classification58 of unipolar dromal episodes are often superimposed on this low-grade
subjects as subvalid (bound to routine, easily tired, cau¬ pattern22; in many of these patients personality distur¬
tious, tense, and meticulous) and bipolar subjects as sub- bances may be more visible and take the form of manip-
stable (naive, frank or open, and interested in their fellow ulativeness, temper tantrums, impulsivity, episodic prom¬
men) has already been commented on; however, it is uncer¬ iscuity, repeated conjugal failure, drug-seeking behavior,
tain if these traits are familial. dilettantism, and suicide gestures or even attempts. Many
A recent study103 conducted at Washington University such patients suffer from dysthymic,22 cyclothymic,24 rapid
attempted to adduce support for the dichotomous position. cycling, atypical, or bipolar II disorders5 (axis I), but they
On the 16-Personality Factor (16-PF) Questionnaire by Cat- simultaneously meet the DSM-III criteria for borderline
tell et al,104 unipolar probands had low scores on factor A and related personality disorders (axis II).28 Although such
(loaded for cyclothymia) compared with controls; thus, the a biaxial classification is a reasonable approach to the
hypothesis that unipolar probands would show a high diagnostic dilemmas presented by these patients, it does
frequency of cyclothymia was not supported. Furthermore, not resolve the question of which psychopathology is pri¬
unipolar probands and "well" first-degree relatives had high mary.88 Prospective followup studies suggest that the af¬
scores on factors that indicated they were shy (H), reserved fective component is often primary.5,10,2124 Some patients
(A), moved by feelings (L), tense (Q4), and apprehensive and considered to have borderline personality because of an "un¬
guilt prone (0). All traits except the last are known to have stable sense of self" may acquire "ego stability" on mainte¬
genetic determinants.104 In brief, the personality attributes nance therapy with lithium carbonate.5 In other adult
of the unipolar depressive were considered to be non- patients with joint characterologic and chronic affective
cyclothymic, consisting of a constellation of largely geneti¬ disturbances, interpersonal maladjustment persists de¬
cally determined traits on the 16-PF. However, these find¬ spite energetic chemotherapy.23 In these patients, personal¬
ings are far from definitive because personality measure¬ ity disorder may have developmental roots and can be
ment was undertaken at the end of a depressive episode, considered to be truly orthogonal to the affective axis."
which may bias the results in the direction of low cy¬ Recent limited evidence from thymoleptic treatment of
clothymia. children with both chronic depressive and conduct disor¬
Gershon et al105 found no evidence for the dichotomous ders indicates that both conditions are controlled by such
position in their genetic investigation, which showed that treatment,90,91 and raises the hope of prevention of the long-
cyclothymic, but not depressive, personalities were over- term personality disturbance seen in early-onset affective
represented in the first-degree relatives of affective pro- disorders.
bands. By contrast, in the study by Angst et al,106 a higher The belief that long-term psychotherapy may prevent
genetic loading for a spectrum of affective personality further affective episodes or a chronic affective denouement
disorders was shown in the bipolar group, supporting the is based on the premise that characterologic disturbances
postulate of a bipolar genetic substrate for hyperthymic, are of primary etiologic significance in affective disorders.3
cyclothymic, and dysthymic temperaments discussed ear¬ The evidence reviewed in this article suggests that more
lier. may be gained by focusing psychotherapy on the social
In summary, current evidence tends to support the clas¬ maladjustments that occur in close temporal contiguity
sic notion that cyclothymic, hyperthymic, and, to some with affective episodes. Thus, brief, practical psychothera-
extent dysthymic personalities represent genetically atten¬ peutic approaches aimed at conjugal, familial, financial, and
uated expressions of the major affective disorders. There vocational matters should prove useful in decreasing post-
may be little correspondence of type of affective tempera¬ depressive (and postmanic) personality disturbances before
ment to polarity of affective illness. The question of speci¬ they crystallize into chronic maladaptive habits. For exam¬
ficity of personality to polarity of affective illness may have ple, by promoting improved communication between
arisen as a result of the methodologically questionable prac¬ spouses, psychotherapy may help to resolve interpersonal
tice of lumping together the large universe of nonen¬ conflicts that may hinder recovery from depressive epi¬
dogenous depressions under the "unipolar" rubric. Any dif¬ sodes.85 It is reasonable to suggest that such pragmatic
ferences disclosed between bipolar and unipolar subjects approaches could enhance or consolidate the therapeutic
(the latter broadly defined) may simply reflect the differ¬ gains achieved through pharmacologie symptom control.85,86
ences between affective personalities and a wide array of An important goal of psychologic treatment is to protect the

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


patient (and significant others) from rash and irreversible The Assessment of Personality Types
decisions in major life areas (eg, divorce, inheritance, Conventional personality types have been traditionally
investments, or geographic moves) that can be easily influ¬ considered unreliable because of criterion variance.1" In¬
enced, even dictated, by pathologic mood change. Whether vestigators have attempted to bypass this thorny problem
psychotherapy aimed at modifying certain personality op¬ by recourse to more reliable paper-and-pencil instruments
erations like poor social skills or maladaptive cognitive of personality measurement such as the EPI,™ Leyton,51
styles can prevent de novo affective episodes is a research Mark-Nyman,56 and Lazare-Klerman-Armor112 inventories.
challenge for the future. Another approach might be to use the schneiderian typol¬
FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS ogy,95 which has high interrater reliability for empirically
defined asthénie, explosive, depressive, and affectionless
It is evident from the foregoing presentation that our
personalities."3 Unfortunately, of these personality types,
understanding of the relationship between personality and only the depressive is familiar to a general clinical reader¬
affective disorder has been hampered by méthodologie
limitations. Klein107 has suggested that recent advances in ship.
Even when reliable and valid measures of personality
pathophysiologic understanding of psychiatric disorders attributes in affective disorders are obtained, there re¬
have involved a shift from naturalistic clinical observation mains the question of elucidating their relevance to the
to controlled experimental approaches. Such approaches,
based on modern advances in behavioral genetics, psychom-
pathogenesis of affective episodes. Teasing apart epiphe-
nomenal personality correlates from causal antecedents is
etry, pharmacologie dissection, and other biological mark¬ particularly difficult. Ideally, personality should be as¬
ers, are becoming increasingly relevant to the area of per¬ sessed before affective episodes have occurred. One strat¬
sonality and affective illness.21 These approaches should egy is to study high-risk groups, such as the offspring of
help in reversing the méthodologie shortcomings cited in affectively ill patients, or certain personality types (de¬
this article.
pendent, compulsive, dysthymic, or cyclothymic) consid¬
Heterogeneity of Affective Disorders ered prone to affective episodes, before they have suffered
Unless specific validated subtypes of affective disorder
syndromal affective breakdowns. A related approach is to
compare the personalities of affective probands and their
are examined, future work is unlikely to reveal replicable relatives (with no history of affective episode).
personality findings. In particular, it would be fruitful to CONCLUSIONS
examine personality in affective subtypes defined by famil¬
ial-genetic history, or by such biologic markers as the Given the range of méthodologie difficulties reviewed in
dexamethasone suppression and REM latency tests.108,109 this article, it is understandable that definitive findings
Retrospective Assessment of Personality relating personality to affective disorders have not yet
emerged. The available data permit the following tentative
Assessments have generally been made after the onset of conclusions.
the illness, thereby confounding effects of illness on person¬ 1. Bipolar affective psychoses often arise from the soil of
ality and posing the most serious méthodologie problem in extraverted, cyclothymic, and related dysthymic tempera¬
this area. Personality assessment during an affective epi¬ mental disorders; a driven, work-oriented obsessoid quality
sode is obviously unsatisfactory because of the difficulty of is often present in such temperaments. Those recurrent
separating personality attributes from clinical symptoms, unipolar depressions that are closely linked to bipolar
and the possibility that the affective state may impart a illness may also arise from the same affective temperamen¬
transient affective coloring to the person, or color percep¬ tal foundations.
tions of past personality operations. However, even when 2. Of all the personality attributes hypothesized to pre¬
patients are examined during an episode-free asympto¬ dispose to nonbipolar conditions, introversion appears to be
matic period or an interepisodic period, one must consider the trait for which there is the strongest evidence. Other
the possibility that past affective episodes themselves may attributes such as dependency and negative self-attribu¬
have altered personality. For example, low self-confidence, tions may largely reflect state effects on the postdepressive
interpersonal dependency, and helplessness, often men¬ personality.
tioned as possible predisposing traits, could conceivably 3. Although interpersonal difficulties following affective
result from repeated episodes of depression. Indeed, such episodes tend to be short-lived, in many patients post-
traits may easily develop in the context of any illness that is depressive personality disturbances persist for many
intermittently or chronically incapacitating and requires years. It is conceivable that these disturbances represent
surveillance or support by family members.110 accentuation of premorbid traits, or the incorporation of
It has been suggested that scores similar to those of episode-related affective experiences into the interepisodic
asymptomatic periods might be obtained by instructing phase of the illness.
patients to disregard their current state of illness and to Future research may reveal more complex links whereby
respond according to their "usual selves."50 Contrasting both personality and affective illness emerge as part of a
personality attributes after first affective episodes with multifactorial continuum, including familial-genetic load¬
those following multiple episodes, and obtaining informa¬ ing, developmental vicissitudes, sociocultural background,
tion about premorbid personality from significant others sex effects, and life events. Although there are theoretical
(spouse, parent, or sibling) who have lived with the patient models"4,115 that encompass such variables, from a clinical
for long periods before affective breakdown, are reasonable standpoint they can be considered unwieldy mathematical
but infrequently used approaches. Another useful strategy exercises at this time. Obviously, much work lies ahead in
is to record changes in personality scores from symptomatic designing the appropriate experiments to test the hypoth¬
to more or less asymptomatic periods63; only those personal¬ eses reviewed in this report. For instance, studies that
ity attributes during the asymptomatic period that are sig¬ divide unipolar depressions on the basis of endogeneity,
nificantly deviant from those of population controls would recurrence, familial criteria, and selected biologic markers
conceivably reflect pathology in the premorbid personality. may help to determine whether different personality con-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


stellations predispose to specific subtypes of unipolar ill¬ full range testing of these hypotheses will require experi¬
ness. More generally, research efforts should (1) take the mental methods that go beyond the level of clinical dis-
heterogeneity of affective disorders into consideration, (2)
use reliable personality assessments that are clinically
This investigation was supported by the State of Tennessee Department
cogent, (3) be designed in such a way as to permit timing of of Mental Health and Mental Retardation and the National Institute of
personality assessments most appropriate for the hypoth¬ Mental Health Clinical Research Branch Collaborative Program on the
eses posed, and (4) combine self-assessment, psychiatric Psychobiology of Depression, Clinical Studies.
evaluation and observations on personality by significant Robert Cloninger, MD, and Christy Wright provided valuable comments
others such as parents, classmates, and co-workers. Al¬ in preparing the manuscript.
The Figure is reproduced with permission from Psychiatric Update: The
though naturalistic observations have generated interest¬ American Psychiatric Association Annual Review (Washington, DC,
ing hypotheses linking personality to affective disorders, American Psychiatric Press Ine, 1983, vol 2).

References

1. Adams F (ed): The Genuine Works ofHippocrates. Baltimore, Williams developmental factors in characterological depressions. J Affective Disord
& Wilkins Co, 1939. 1981;3:183-192.
2. Kretschmer E: Physique and Character, Miller E (trans). London, 28. American Psychiatric Association, Committee on Nomenclature and
Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co Ltd, 1936. Statistics: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, ed 3.
3. Arieti S, Bemporad J: Severe and Mild Depression. New York, Basic Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1980.
Books Inc, 1978. 29. Klein DF, Gittleman R, Quitkin F, et al: Diagnosis and Drug
4. Von Zerssen D: Premorbid personality and affective psychoses, in Treatment of Psychiatric Disorders, ed 2. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins
Burrows GD (ed): Handbook of Studies on Depression. Amsterdam, Co, pp 223-575.
Excerpta Medica, 1977, pp 79-103. 30. Sheard MH, Martini J: Treatment of human aggressive behavior:
5. Akiskal HS, Khani M, Scott-Strauss A: Cyclothymic temperamental Four case studies of the effect of lithium. Compr Psychiatry 1978;19:37-45.
disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1979;2:527-554. 31. Van Putten T, Sanders DG: Lithium in treatment failures. J Nerv
6. Chodoff P: The depressive personality: A critical review. Arch Gen Ment Dis 1975;161:255-264.
Psychiatry 1972;27:666-673. 32. Kovacs M, Beck A: Maladaptive cognitive structures in depression.
7. Klerman GL: The relationship between personality and clinical depres- Am J Psychiatry 1978;133:525-533.
sions: Overcoming the obstacles to verify psychodynamic theories. Int J 33. Freud S: Heredity and the aetiology of the neuroses, in Collected
Psychiatry 1973;11:227-233. Papers. New York, Basic Books Inc, 1959, vol 1, pp 138-154.
8. Small I, Small J, Alig V, et al: Passive-aggressive personality disorder: 34. Abraham K: Notes on the psychoanalytic investigation and treatment
A search for a syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 1970;126:973-983. of manic-depressive insanity and allied conditions, in Selected Papers on
9. Weissman M, Paykel ES: The Depressed Woman: A Study of Social Psychoanalysis. New York, Basic Books Inc, 1960, pp 137-156.
Relationships. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1974. 35. Cohen MB, Baker G, Cohen RA, et al: An intensive study of 12 cases
10. WelnerJ, Liss JL, Robins E: Personality disorder: II. Follow-up. Br J of manic-depressive psychosis. Psychiatry 1954;17:103-138.
Psychiatry 1974;124:359-366. 36. Gibson RW: The family background and early life experience of the
11. Akiskal HS, Bitar AH, Puzantian VR, et al: The nosological status of manic-depressive patient: A comparison with the schizophrenic patient.
neurotic depression: A prospective three- to four-year follow-up examina- Psychiatry 1958;21:71-90.
tion in the light of the primary-secondary and the unipolar-bipolar dichoto- 37. Jacobson E: Contributions to the metapsychology of cyclothymic
mies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978;35:756-766. depression, in Greenacre P (ed): Affective Disorders. New York, Interna-
12. Freud S: The Dynamics of Transference, standard ed. London, tional Universities Press, 1953, pp 49-83.
Hogarth Press, 1958, vol 12, p 99. 38. Strupp HH, Sandell JA, Waterhouse GJ, et al: Short-term dynamic
13. Clarkin JF, Glazer HI (eds): Depression-Behavioral and Directive therapies for depression: Theory and research, in Rush J (ed): Short-term
Intervention Strategies. New York, Garland STPM Press, 1981. Psychotherapies for the Depressed Patient: Cognitive, Behavioral, Inter-
14. Korner AF: Some hypotheses regarding the significance of individual personal, and Psychodynamic Approaches. New York, Guilford Publica-
differences at birth for later development. Psychoanal Study Child 1964;19: tions Inc, in press.
58-72. 39. Liebowitz MR, Klein DF: Hysteroid dysphoria. Psychiatr Clin North
15. Benjamin JD: The innate and the experiential in development, in Am 1979;2:555-575.
Brosin HW (ed): Lectures in Experimental Psychiatry. Pittsburgh, Univer- 40. Bowlby J: Childhood mourning and its implications for child psychia-
sity of Pittsburgh Press, 1961, p 34. try. Am J Psychiatry 1961;118:481-498.
16. Crowe R: Adoption studies of antisocial personality. Biol Psychiatry 41. Bowlby J: The making and breaking of affectional bonds: I. Aetiology
1975;10:353-371. and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. Br J Psychiatry
17. Eysenck HJ, Prell DB: The inheritance of neuroticism: An experimen- 1977;130:201-210.
tal study: J Ment Sci 1951;97:441-465. 42. Levi LD, Fales GH, Stein M, et al: Separation and attempted suicide.
18. Torgersen S: The oral, obsessive, and hysterical personality syn- Arch Gen Psychiatry 1966;15:158-164.
dromes: A study of hereditary and environmental factors by means of the 43. Hill O: The association of childhood bereavement with suicide attempt
twin method. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:1272-1277. in depressive illness. Br J Psychiatry 1969;115:301-304.
19. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Knop J, et al: Psychopathology in 44. Freud S: Mourning and Melancholia, standard ed. London, Hogarth
adopted and nonadopted daughters of alcoholics. Arch Gen Psychiatry Press, 1962, vol 14.
1977;34:1005-1009. 45. Klein M: A contribution to the psychogenesis of manic-depressive
20. Rainer JD: Hereditary and character disorders. Am J Psychother states, in Klein M (ed): Contributions to Psychoanalysis 1921-1945. Lon-
1979;33:6-16. don, Hogarth Press, 1948, pp 282-310.
21. Akiskal HS: External validating criteria for psychiatric diagnosis: 46. DeReuck AVS, Porter R (eds): Transcultural Psychiatry. London,
Their application in affective disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 1980;41:6-15. J&A Churchill Ltd, 1965.
22. Akiskal HS, Rosenthal TL, Haykal RF, et al: Characterological 47. Seligman M: Helplessness. San Francisco, WH Freeman and Co,
depressions: Clinical and sleep EEG findings separating 'subaffective 1975.
dysthymias' from 'character-spectrum disorders.' Arch Gen Psychiatry 48. Scarf M: Women and depression. New Republic 1980;183:25-29.
1980;126:973-983. 49. Cadoret RJ, Baker M, Dorzab J, et al: Depressive disease: Personality
23. Akiskal HS: Subaffective disorders: Dysthymic, cyclothymic, and factors in patients and their relatives. Biol Psychiatry 1971;3:85-93.
bipolarII disorders in the 'borderline' realm. Psychiatr Clin North Am 50. Kendell RE, Discipio WJ: Obsessional symptoms and obsessional
1981;4:25-46. personality traits in patients with depressive illnesses. Psychol Med
24. Akiskal HS, Djenderedjian AH, Rosenthal RH, et al: Cyclothymic 1970;1:65-72.
disorder: Validating criteria for inclusion in the bipolar affective group. Am 51. Cooper JE: The Leyton obsessional inventory. Psychol Med 1970;1:
J Psychiatry 1977;134:1227-1233. 48-64.
25. Akiskal HS, Rosenthal RH, Rosenthal TL, et al: Differentiation of 52. Videbech T: A study of genetic facts, childhood bereavement and
primary affective illness from situational, symptomatic, and secondary premorbid personality traits in patients with anacastic endogenous depres-
depressions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979;36:635-643. sion. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1975;52:178-222.
26. King D: Pathological and therapeutic consequences of sleep loss: A 53. Gittleson NL: The effect of obsessions on depressive psychosis. Br J
review. Dis Nerv Syst 1977;38:873-879. Psychiatry 1966;112:253-259.
27. Rosenthal TL, Akiskal HS, Scott-Strauss A, et al: Familial and 54. Gittleson NL: The fate of obsessions on depressive psychosis. Br J

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013


Psychiatry 1966;112:705-708. 86. Klerman GL: Long-term treatment of affective disorders, in Lipton
55. Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL: Personality attributes and affective MA, DiMascio A, Killam KF (eds): Psychopharmacology: A Generation of
disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1979;136:67-70. Progress. New York, Raven Press, 1978, pp 1303-1311.
56. Nyman GE: Variations in personality. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1956;107 87. Klein D: Endogenomorphic depressions: Toward a terminologic revi-
(suppl):1-94. sion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974;31:447-454.
57. Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL, Clayton PJ, et al: Personality and 88. Akiskal HS: Dysthymic disorder: Psychopathology of proposed
depression: Empirical findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry, in press. chronic depressive subtypes. Am J Psychiatry 1983;140:11-20.
58. Perris C: A study of bipolar (manic-depressive) and unipolar recur- 89. Kraines SH; Therapy of the chronic depressions. Dis Nerv Syst
rent depressive psychoses. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1966;42(suppl):7-188. 1967;28:577-584.
59. Wittenborn RJ, Maurer SH: Persisting personalities among de- 90. Staton DR, Brumback RA: Nonspecificity of motor hyperactivity as a
pressed women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977;34:968-971. diagnostic criterion. Percept Mot Skills 1981;52:323-332.
60. Telenbach H: Melancholia, Eng E (trans). Pittsburgh, Duquesne 91. Puig-Antich J: Major depression and conduct disorder in pre-puberty.
University Press, 1980. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1982;2:118-128.
61. Metcalfe M: The personality of depressive patients, in Coppen A, 92. MacVane JR, Lange JD, Brown WA, et al: Psychological functioning
Walk A (eds): Recent Development in Affective Disorders. No. 2. Ashford, of bipolar manic-depressives in remission. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978;35:
Headley Brothers Ltd, 1968, pp 97-104. 1351-1354.
62. Lewisohn P: A behavioral approach to depression, in Friedman RJ, 93. Welner A, Welner Z, Leonard MA: Bipolar manic-depressive disor-
Katz MM (eds): The Psychology of Depression: Contemporary Theory and der: A reassessment of course and outcome. Compr Psychiatry 1975;16:
Research. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1974, pp 97-104. 125-131.
63. Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL, Clayton PJ, et al: Effects of the 94. Akiskal HS, Puzantian VR: Psychotic forms of depression and mania.
depressive state on trait measurement. Am J Psychiatry, in press. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1979;2:419-439.
64. Brown G: The social etiology of depression\p=m-\Londonstudies, in 95. Schneider K: Psychopathic Personalities, Hamilton MW (trans).
Depue RA (ed): The Psychobiology of Depressive Disorders: Implications London, Cassell Ltd, 1958.
for the Effects of Stress. New York, Academic Press Inc, 1979, pp 263-289. 96. Sheldon WH, Hartt EM, McDermo HE: Varieties of Delinquent
65. Hirschfeld RMA, Cross CC: Epidemiology of affective disorders: Youth. New York, Hafner Press, 1970.
Psychosocial risk factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;39:35-47. 97. Feighner JP, Robins E, Guze SB, et al: Diagnostic criteria for use in
66. Von Zerssen D: Personality and affective disorders, in Paykel ES (ed): psychiatric research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972;26:57-63.
Handbook ofAffective Disorders. New York, The Guilford Press, 1982, pp 98. Dunner DL, Russek D, Russek B, et al: Classification of affective
212-228. disorder subtypes. Compr Psychiatry 1982;23:186-189.
67. Lazare A, Klerman G: Hysteria and depression: The frequency and 99. Depue RA, Slater JF, Welfstetter-Kausch H, et al: A behavioral
significance of hysterical personality features in hospitalized depressed paradigm for identifying persons at risk for bipolar depressive disorders: A
women. Am J Psychiatry 1968;124:48-56. conceptual framework and five validation studies. J Abnorm Psychol
68. Bart P: Depression: A sociological theory, in Roman P, Trice H (eds): 1981;90(suppl):381-438.
Explorations in Psychiatric Sociology. Philadelphia, FA Davis Co, 1974. 100. Akiskal HS: Dysthymic and cyclothymic disorders: A paradigm for
69. Vaz Serra A, Pollitt J: The relationship between personality and the high-risk research in psychiatry, in Davis M, Maas J (eds): Affective
symptoms of depressive illness. Br J Psychiatry 1975;127:211-218. Disorders. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press Inc, in press.
70. Eysenck HJ: Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory. Lon- 101. Turner WJ, King S: Two genetically distinct forms of bipolar
don, University of London Press, 1959. affective disorder? Biol Psychiatry 1981;16:417-439.
71. Weissman MM, Prusoff BA, Klerman GL: Personality and the 102. Leonhard J, Korff I, Schultz H: Die Temperamente in due Familien
prediction of long-term outcome of depression. Am J Psychiatry 1978;135: der Monopolaren and Bipolaren Phasischen Psychosen. Psychiatr Neurol
797-800. 1962;143:416-434.
72. Rounsaville BJ, Sholomskas D, Prusoff BA: Chronic mood disorders 103. Wetzel RD, Cloninger RC, Hong B, et al: Personality as a subclinical
in depressed outpatients. J Affective Disord 1980;2:73-88. expression of the affective disorders. Compr Psychiatry 1980;21:197-205.
73. Keller MB, Shapiro RW: 'Double depression': Superimposition of 104. Cattell RB, Eber HW, Tabuoka MM: Handbook for the 16 Personal-
acute depressive episodes on chronic depressive disorders. Am J Psychia- ity Factors Questionnaire. Champion, Ill, Institute for Personality and
try 1982;139:438-442. Ability Testing, 1970.
74. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria for a 105. Gershon ES, Mark A, Cohen N, et al: Transmitted factors in the
Selected Group of Functional Disorders, ed 3. New York, Biometrics morbid risk of affective disorders: A controlled study. J Psychiatr Res
Research Division, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1977. 1975;12:283-299.
75. Jamison KR, Gerner RH, Hammen C, et al: Clouds and silver linings: 106. Angst J, Frey R, Lohmeyer B, et al: Bipolar manic-depressive
Positive experiences associated with primary affective disorders. Am J psychoses: Results of a genetic investigation. Hum Genet 1980;55:237-254.
Psychiatry 1980;137:198-202. 107. Klein DF: Anxiety reconceptualized. Compr Psychiatry 1980;21:
76. Jamison KR, Akiskal HS: Medication compliance in patients with 411-427.
bipolar disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am, in press. 108. Carroll BJ, Greden JF, Frinberg M, et al: Neuroendocrine evalua-
77. Akiskal HS: Factors associated with incomplete remission in primary tion of depression in borderline patients. Psychiatr Clin North Am
depressive illness. J Clin Psychiatry 1982;43:266-271. 1981;4:89-99.
78. Cassano GB, Maggini C, Akiskal HS: Short-term, subchronic and 109. Kupfer DJ, Foster FG, Coble P: The application of EEG sleep for the
chronic sequelae of affective disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am, in press. differential diagnosis of affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1978;135:
79. Lumry AE, Gottesman J: MMPI state dependency during the course 69-74.
of bipolar psychosis. Psychiatry Res 1982;7:59-67. 110. Abram HS: The psychology of chronic illness. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc
80. Liebowitz MR, Stallone F, Dunner DL, et al: Personality features of Sci 1980;447:5-10.
patients with primary affective disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1979;60: 111. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Reliability of clinical criteria for
214-224. psychiatric diagnosis. Am J Psychiatry 1975;132:1187-1192.
81. Carr A: Psychological testing of personality, in Kaplan HI, Freedman 112. Lazare A, Klerman GL, Armor DJ: Oral, obsessive and hysterical
AM, Sadock BJ (eds): Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, ed 3. personality patterns. J Psychiatr Res 1970;7:275-290.
Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co, 1980, pp 940-966. 113. Standage KF: The use of Schneider's typology for the diagnosis of
82. Kraepelin E: Manic-Depressive Illness and Paranoia. Edinburgh, personality disorders: an examination of reliability. Br J Psychiatry 1979;
E&S Livingstone Ltd, 1921. 135:238-242.
83. Janowsky DS, El Yousef K, David JM: Interpersonal maneuvers of 114. Cloninger CR, Rice J, Reich T: Multifactorial inheritance with
manic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1974;131:250-255. cultural transmission and assortative mating: II. A general model of
84. Hauri P, Chernik D, Hawkins D, et al: Sleep of depressed patients in combined polygenic and cultural inheritance. Am J Hum Genet 1979;31:
remission. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974;31:386-391. 176-198.
85. Weissman MM: Psychotherapy and its relevance to the pharmaco- 115. Cloninger CR, Rice J, Reich T: Multifactorial inheritance with
therapy of affective disorders: From ideology to evidence, in Lipton MA, cultural transmission and assortative mating: III. Family structure and the
DiMascio A, Killam KF (eds): Psychopharmacology: A Generation of analysis of separation experiment. Am J Hum Genet 1979;31:366-388.
Progress. New York, Raven Press, 1978, pp 1313-1321.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Chicago Libraries User on 09/01/2013

S-ar putea să vă placă și