Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Human Development


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/earlhumdev

The relationship between exclusive breastfeeding and infant development: A T


6- and 12-month follow-up study
Hye Jeong Choia, Su Kyoung Kangb, Mi Ra Chunga,b,

a
Sesalmaul Research Institute, Gachon University, Gyeonggi-do, South Korea
b
Department of Early Childhood Education, Gachon University, Gyeonggi-do, South Korea

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Background: Breastfeeding offers several advantages for infants with research suggesting that it benefits de-
Cognitive development velopment, including cognitive and language development. However, limited research has examined the benefits
Communication development in infants under one year.
Exclusive breastfeeding duration Aims: To investigate the association between breastfeeding duration and infant development.
Formula feeding
Study design: A longitudinal study conducted over 12 months.
Social development
6–12 months
Subjects: Two hundred fifty-five mothers and their infants living in South Korea were seen at three time-points
based on infants' age (4, 6, and 12 months).
Outcome measures: Breastfeeding data were collected at 4 and 6 months. The Korea-Developmental Screening
Test for Infants & Children was administered at 6 and 12 months to measure developmental milestones. We
analyzed the relationship between breastfeeding duration in the first 6 months and infant development at 6 and
12 months using logistic regression analysis.
Results: Compared to infants who did not breastfeed at all, infants who were exclusively breastfed until 4 months
of age followed by mixed breastfeeding had a better communication and social interaction at 6 months, and
better cognition, communication, and social interaction at 12 months. Exclusive breastfeeding until 6 months of
age had no apparent impact on the outcomes at 6 and 12 months.
Conclusions: Exclusive breastfeeding up to 4 months of age followed by mixed breastfeeding might maximize the
effects of infant development in their first year after birth. Breastfeeding programs should effectively commu-
nicate that exclusive breastfeeding for at least 4 months benefits infant development.

1. Introduction the child ages, being just 55.8% at 4 weeks of age, 38.1% at 4 months,
and 18.3% at 6 months of age. The exclusive breastfeeding rate for
According to the National Survey on Fertility, Family Health and 6 months of age in South Korea is about half of the global average.
Welfare in Korea [1] between 2000 and 2012 in South Korea, from Breastfeeding offers several advantages for infants. In particular,
9.5% to 32.3% infants aged 5–6 months were receiving their nutrition breast milk is known to strengthen infants' immune system and help in
from breastfeeding only1, which may reflect increasing interest in the development of their brain and cognitive function. Attempts to
breastfeeding. The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends that empirically understand the effects of breastfeeding duration in general
mothers worldwide exclusively breastfeed (i.e., not using any formula are ongoing. Recent studies on the effects of breastfeeding have noted
or food) for the infant's first 6 months to achieve optimal growth, de- declines in infantile infection rates [3,4], improvements in cranial
velopment, and health. According to data released by the Korea Com- nerve development [5,6], and enhanced physical and mental develop-
mittee for UNICEF in 2016 [2], the exclusive breastfeeding rate in ment [7–10] and cognitive and language development [11–15]. Breast-
South Korea is high immediately after birth but gradually decreases as fed infants develop language and process language stimuli during the

Abbreviations: K-DST, Korea-Developmental Screening Test for Infants & Children; SES, socioeconomic status; ANOVA, one-way analyses of variance; aOR, adjusted
odds ratio; LCPUFA, long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids; DHA, docosahexaenoic acid

Corresponding author at: Department of Early Childhood Education, Gachon University, 1342 Seongnam-daero, Sujeong-gu, Seongnam-si, Gyeonggi-do, South
Korea.
E-mail address: mrchung@gachon.ac.kr (M.R. Chung).
1
Family Health and Welfare in Korea [1] investigated the rate of infants aged 5–6 months who were more breast fed than formula fed, including infants who were
exclusively breastfed.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.08.011
Received 30 May 2018; Received in revised form 15 August 2018; Accepted 28 August 2018
0378-3782/ © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
H.J. Choi et al. Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

first 6 months of life better than formula-fed infants [16] and may also single birth (i.e., no multiples), and who were not attending childcare.
have higher IQ score during the first year than those fed with formula No infants participating in the study had chronic diseases, such as
[17]. In addition, children breastfed for < 4 months showed an in- cardiovascular diseases or asthma.
creased risk of delayed development in the adaptive, language, and fine Of the original sample of 510, a total of 431 mothers completed the
motor domains during their first three years compared to infants who survey at Time 1 (18.3% dropout rate), 363 at Time 2 (18.6% dropout
were breastfed for > 4 months [14], while continuing breastfeeding for rate), and 298 at Time 3 (21.8% dropout rate). Participants who did not
at least 4 months was associated with increased child cognitive devel- have a completed breastfeeding survey or did not respond to one or
opment [13]. However, Fewtrell, Wilson, Booth, Lucas [18] have sug- more question on the K-DST were eliminated using listwise deletion.
gested that exclusive breastfeeding to 6 months leaves infants lacking in The final sample size was 255. No statistically significant differences
iron, which is of concern given the potential for long-term adverse ef- were found between respondents that dropped out after 4 months
fects on motor, mental, and social development [19,20]. On the other (n = 176) and those that remained in the study for all 12 months
hand, exclusive breastfeeding until at least 6 months has also been re- (n = 255) in terms of sociodemographic variables, including maternal
ported to be positive for development [21,22]. Thus, studies have had age, maternal education, paternal education, and household income.
inconsistent results with respect to the optimal duration of exclusive
breastfeeding. 2.2. Measures
However, most studies have been limited to specific areas of de-
velopment, such as cognition and language. Of the few studies have 2.2.1. Breastfeeding duration
examined the effects of exclusive breastfeeding on infants, most focused Breastfeeding information was collected using one question: “How
on diseases or nutrition [3,4,19,20]. Thus, there is still debate over how have you been feeding your infant until now? (breastfeeding only;
long exclusive breastfeeding should be continued to maximize its ben- formula only; breastfeeding and formula together).” Participants were
efits for infant development. The combination of breast milk, formula, categorized according to their infants' breastfeeding type at 4 and
and supplementary foods from 4 months after birth is both less bur- 6 months of age, as follows: never breastfed, mixed breastfeeding until
densome for the mother and nutritionally sufficient [18]. Recently, in 6 months of age, exclusive breastfeeding until 4 months of age followed by
South Korea, the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine Korea released a mixed breastfeeding, exclusive breastfeeding until 6 months of age. Here,
report encouraging 6 months of exclusive breastfeeding as re- “mixed” means that the infant was both breastfed and fed with formula
commended by the WHO, but in Korea, studies have focused on and mother's milk, while “exclusive” meant that the infant was only
breastfeeding itself; its present status [2,23,24], the factors affecting its breastfed during that period without any formula. Other breastfeeding
duration [25], and the nutrient composition of human milk [26–28]. categories were not represented in the data, and were not presented. All
Korean studies related to effect of breastfeeding and especially to infant infants in the four categories of breastfeeding consumed other liquids or
health and development aspects remain insufficient. Therefore, it would solids food at 6 months, as the 11 infants who did not have other liquids
be meaningful to explore the effects of exclusive breastfeeding duration or solids food were excluded at the sampling stage for homogeneity of
on infant development for the first year after birth. the nutritional environment. In Korea, infants generally begin to con-
Thus, the purpose of this study is to examine the relationships of sume other liquids or solid food at 4–6 months [30].
exclusive breastfeeding duration with the physical, cognitive, commu- Breastfeeding was measured at 4 and 6 months for the following
nicative, and social development of infants at 6 and 12 months of age reasons. In 2011, the WHO released a statement asserting the beneficial
compared with those infants under formula. For this purpose, a re- effects of exclusive breastfeeding for 6 months, changing their existing
search design to track infants throughout the first year of life was de- recommendation of at least 4 months. We chose time points of
veloped. We measured the overall development of infants using the 4 months, at which the breastfeeding rate dropped sharply to < 40%
Korea-Developmental Screening Test for Infants & Children (K-DST), an [2], and 6 months recommended by the WHO for exclusive breast-
effective, reliable, and accredited tool in South Korea for measuring feeding.
infants' physical, cognitive, communicative, and social development
[29]. Our study addresses the prominent gaps in past literature, which 2.2.2. Infant development
has largely focused on cognitive functioning, and did not gather follow- Infant development was assessed using the K-DST [29] at 6 and
up results on early infant development from 6 to 12 months after birth. 12 months. The K-DST is a screening test designed by the Ministry of
In addition, we adjusted for the sociodemographic characteristics of Health and Welfare to assist in monitoring the development of Korean
infants and mothers to maximize the statistical explanatory power of children aged 4 months to 6 years. It contains 5 dimensions, each with 8
the results. items, as follows: gross motor (e.g., “Can turn over while lying down”),
fine motor (e.g., “Shakes toy when a toy is placed in hand”), cognition
2. Methods (e.g., “Follows a rolling ball with eyes”), communication (e.g., “Speaks
babbling like ‘ooh’ and ‘ah’”), and social interaction development (e.g.,
2.1. Participants “Tries to approach familiar people such as family members”). Each item
is scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (cannot perform
The participants were 255 unemployed mothers and their infants at all) to 3 (can perform well). Mothers were asked to assess their infants'
were recruited between August 2015 and May 2017 through the behavior. The K-DST score is calculated by summing the scores of each
Sesalmaul Research Institute, Gachon University. The participants were dimension, and based on these dimension scores, infants' level of de-
followed up three times according to infants' age: 4, 6, and 12 months velopment is categorized into two groups: delayed and normal. Infants
(Times 1, 2, and 3, respectively). All mothers were briefed about re- who scored above −1 standard deviation from the mean were classified
search participation and research ethics, and signed a written consent as being in the normal group, and those who scored below this level
form at each follow-up point. The study was approved by the IRB of were classified as being in the delayed group.
Gachon University (Approval number: 1044396-201705-HR-079-01). This scale has been validated against the Korean-Bayley Scale of
Mothers answered a question about breastfeeding at Times 1 and 2, and Infant Development [31] and the Korean-Wechsler Preschool and Pri-
completed the K-DST at Times 2 and 3. The breastfeeding information mary Scales of Intelligence [32] with Korean children. In this study, the
was collected online. The K-DST was disseminated to participants by a internal consistency reliability (the Cronbach's alpha) was good for the
skilled clinician through home visits; the mothers completed it them- whole K-DST, ranging between 0.82 and 0.85. The internal consistency
selves. Among research participants, only those who satisfied the fol- of subscales ranged from 0.45 to 0.84. A coefficient of at least 0.80
lowing criteria were included in this study: born at term (≥37 weeks), shows good consistency, and a coefficient of < 0.70 shows low

43
H.J. Choi et al. Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

consistency [33]. Among the five dimensions, the cognition (Cronbach's Table 1
alpha = 0.45) showed low internal consistency; however, Gregory [34] Demographic characteristics of participants (N = 255).
suggested that there are no absolute standards for the Cronbach's alpha Variables N (%)
and that tests with reliabilities of 0.70 or lower can still have utility as
measures of individual differences. In this light, the standardized K-DST Infant
Gender (male) 130 (51.0)
still deemed to be a reliable scale for assessing the overall development
Birth order (first) 234 (91.8)
of infants and was used without excluding the cognition dimension. Gestational age at birth (37–42 weeks) 251 (98.4)
Birth weight (g), mean (SD) 3.3 (0.4)
2.3. Confounding factors Kaup index (kg/m2), mean (SD)
6 months 17.4 (2.1)
12 months 17.0 (1.9)
Confounding factors that appear to be related to breastfeeding style
Mother
and infant development were included in the models as control vari- Maternal age (years), mean (SD) 33.1 (5.9)
ables to rigorously examine the predictive effect of breastfeeding Education
duration on infant development: socioeconomic status (SES; maternal Less than or equal to high school 16 (6.3)
College 191 (74.9)
education, paternal education, and family income), mother's age at
Greater than College 48 (18.8)
birth, birth order, birth weight, and gestational age [8,10,35–37]. In- Monthly household income
formation about these variables was obtained via an online ques- < $2600 60 (23.5)
tionnaire at the 4-month visit. < $3500 69 (27.1)
< $4400 56 (22.0)
< $5300 33 (12.9)
2.4. Statistical analysis
≥$5300 37 (14.5)
Duration of breastfeeding
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics 23.0. Never 19 (7.5)
First, the demographic characteristics of the participants were de- Mixed breastfeeding for 6 months 130 (51.0)
scribed as frequencies, percentages, means, and standard deviations. Exclusive breastfeeding for 4 months, followed by mixed 82 (32.2)
breastfeeding
Second, the differences in infant development scores among breast- Exclusive breastfeeding for 6 months 24 (9.4)
feeding groups were examined using one-way analyses of variance K-DST at 6 months, mean (SD)
(ANOVAs) with Duncan's multiple range test as a post-hoc test. Third, Gross motor 15.4 (4.0)
the relationship between exclusive breastfeeding duration and infant Fine motor 21.7 (2.2)
Cognition 20.4 (2.4)
development was analyzed using multiple binary logistic regression
Communication 18.9 (3.2)
analysis. We accounted for confounding factors (SES, mother's age at Social interaction 19.0 (2.5)
time of birth, birth order, birth weight, gestational age) in this analysis K-DST at 12 months, mean (SD)
to isolate the specific benefits of breastfeeding duration. Gross motor 20.3 (4.7)
Fine motor 22.2 (2.1)
Cognition 22.1 (1.8)
3. Results Communication 21.2 (3.0)
Social interaction 21.7 (2.5)
The descriptive statistics for this study are presented in Table 1. The
sample comprised 255 Korean infants and their mothers. Note. Some data were missing for birth weight (n = 2), Kaup index at 6 months
(n = 9), and Kaup index at 12 months (n = 5). K-DST response options range
3.1. Breastfeeding duration and infant development from 0 to 3.

Table 2 shows the results of the one-way ANOVA to examine the The gross motor and fine motor dimensions showed no changes after
relationship between exclusive breastfeeding duration and infant de- adjustment.
velopment at 6 and 12 months. Infants who were exclusively breastfed
until 6 months of age had significantly higher mean social interaction 4. Discussion
development scores at 6 months than did infants who never breastfed
(p < .05). At 12 months, infants who exclusively breastfed until 4 and We found that infants who were exclusively breastfed for 4 months
6 months of age had significantly higher mean scores for cognition, followed by mixed breastfeeding showed better development compared
communication, and social interaction development than did infants to infants who were never breastfed. Specifically, they had higher levels
who never breastfed (p < .05). of communication and social interaction at 6 months, and higher cog-
Tables 3 and 4 show the results of the binary logistic regression nition, communication, and social interaction development at
analysis on the K-DST dimensions at 6 and 12 months, before and after 12 months.
adjusting for the confounding variables, respectively. Infants who were According to preceding research on cognition and communication
exclusively breastfed until 4 months had greater odds of having normal development, a longer duration of breastfeeding appears to improve
scores for communication and social interaction development at cognitive and communication development at 18 months [15]. Ad-
6 months compared to infants who never breastfed (adjusted odds ratio ditionally, infants who breastfed for < 4 months had an increased risk
[aOR]: 4.12 [95% confidence interval: 1.11–15.28] and aOR: 6.04 of developmental delay in terms of adaptability and communication at
[1.05–34.66], respectively). At 12 months, similar effects were found ages 1–3 years compared to those who were breastfed for > 4 months
for cognition (aOR: 6.66 [1.02–43.63], aOR: 3.93 [1.07–14.40], and [14]. The longer the breastfeeding period, the faster infants appear to
aOR: 1.59 [1.59–42.05], respectively). No significant effects were reach developmental milestones such as polysyllabic babbling [38], and
found for the breastfeeding categories, including exclusive breast- breastfed infants have an advantage in processing language stimuli over
feeding until 6 months. formula-fed infants [16]. A previous meta-analysis of 20 studies that
Compared with the unadjusted analyses, adjustment for the con- compared breastfeeding with formula-feeding reported that breastfed
founding variables increased the effects of exclusive breastfeeding on infants consistently had higher cognitive development mean scores
communication at 6 months and social interaction at 12 months. than did formula-fed infants [11]. The authors went on to point out that
Furthermore, the effects on social interaction at 6 months and com- increased cognitive development as a result of breastfeeding begins to
munication at 12 months were significant only in the adjusted analyses. appear in early development and persists throughout childhood and

44
H.J. Choi et al. Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

Table 2
Mean and standard deviation of breastfeeding duration and infant development at 6 and 12 months.
Breastfeeding duration p

Exclusive breastfeeding until Exclusive breastfeeding until 4 months of Mixed breastfeeding until Never breastfed
6 months of age age, thereafter mixed breastfeeding 6 months of age

n = 24 n = 82 n = 130 n = 19

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

6 months
Gross motor 17.29 (4.71) 15.71 (3.86) 14.92 (3.89) 15.26 (4.41) NS
Fine motor 22.08 (2.28) 22.01 (2.06) 21.58 (2.15) 21.11 (2.33) NS
Cognition 20.75 (2.97) 20.66 (1.91) 20.24 (2.48) 20.21 (2.25) NS
Communication 19.71 (3.51) 19.37 (2.95) 18.63 (3.18) 18.31 (3.28) NS
Social interaction 19.92b (2.02) 19.50a,b (2.21) 18.68a,b (2.80) 18.37a (1.77) < .05

12 months
Gross motor 19.17 (4.93) 20.90 (4.07) 20.13 (5.00) 20.21 (5.22) NS
Fine motor 22.08 (2.02) 22.02 (2.03) 22.33 (2.14) 21.84 (2.29) NS
Cognition 22.17b (1.55) 22.28b (1.49) 22.09b (1.84) 20.95a (2.46) < .05
Communication 21.13b (2.13) 22.27b (2.19) 20.94b (3.19) 19.53a (4.25) < .05
Social interaction 21.83b (2.68) 22.27b (1.61) 21.53a,b (2.84) 20.58a (2.87) < .05

a b
Note. Significant differences are denoted with and for Duncan's validation. NS = not significant.

adolescence, and that the longer the breastfeeding period, the greater recommended for all infants for at least four months after birth.
the cognitive developmental benefits of breastfeeding. As noted earlier, the majority of previous studies examined the re-
The relationships of breastfeeding with communication and cogni- lationship between breastfeeding and cognitive abilities, whereas only a
tion development can be explained by a special nutritional component few have examined social interaction. In this study, infants who were
in breast milk not found in formula. There is robust evidence that the exclusively breastfed for 4 months followed by mixed breastfeeding
long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs), especially doc- were found to have higher social interaction scores at both 6 and
osahexaenoic acid (DHA), contained in breast milk have beneficial ef- 12 months compared to infants who were never breastfed. Social in-
fects on brain development. Makrides et al. [5] found that breastfed teraction captures the essential skills infants use in interacting with
infants had higher levels of DHA in their cerebral cortex and better others, such as eye contact and conjugate gaze [29]. It has been re-
scores on measures of visual function and brain development than did ported that the breastfeeding process itself involves mother–infant
formula-fed infants. Crawford [39] similarly reported that DHA is an dyadic interactions that facilitate bonding and enhance interaction
important component of breast milk and supports brain development, [41], and that through breastfeeding, the mother and the infant natu-
while Willatts et al. [40] demonstrated that 10-month-old infants who rally make eye contact, and the warm skin contact and immediate sa-
were fed formula containing LCPUFA showed improved problem-sol- tisfaction of hunger have an increasing effect on interaction with the
ving skills. In addition, breast milk contains thyroxine and nerve infant [42]. In a New Zealand's birth cohort study [43], breastfeeding
growth factors, which can influence neural development and cognitive children were found to have more positive relationships with their
achievement [12]. Because of the neurophysiological merits of breast parents than formula-fed children; more specifically, the longer the
milk, it is perhaps the best proprietary initial nutrition source and is duration of the breastfeeding period, the more likely the child was to

Table 3
Unadjusted binary logistic regression analyses of K-DST scores at 6 months according to duration of breastfeeding.
Breastfeeding duration p

Exclusive breastfeeding until Exclusive breastfeeding until 4 months of Mixed breastfeeding until Never breastfed
6 months of age age, thereafter mixed breastfeeding 6 months of age

n = 24 n = 82 n = 130 n = 19

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR

6 months
Gross motor 0.28 (0.03–2.72) 0.36 (0.04–296) 0.21 (0.03–1.66) Ref NS
Fine motor 0.82 (0.12–5.51) 0.85 (0.17–4.23) 1.58 (0.32–7.95) Ref NS
Cognition 2.71 (0.23–32.34) 4.71 (0.62–35.78) 2.94 (0.53–16.37) Ref NS
Communication 2.50 (0.51–12.18) 3.83a (1.06-13.79) 1.85 (0.60–5.70) Ref < .05
Social interaction 2.06 (0.31–13.81) 4.94 (0.91–26.71) 1.55 (0.40–6.01) Ref NS

12 months
Gross motor 0.32 (0.08–1.23) 0.73 (0.22–2.43) 0.65 (0.20–2.07) Ref NS
Fine motor 0.82 (0.12–5.51) 0.85 (0.17–4.23) 1.58 (0.32–7.95) Ref NS
Cognition 4.31 (0.41–45.28) 7.50a (1.16–48.56) 1.69 (0.43–6.57) Ref < .05
Communication 3.93 (0.67–23.10) 3.30 (0.94–11.59) 1.85 (0.60–5.70) Ref NS
Social interaction 2.93 (0.48–18.10) 7.02a (1.42-34.63) 1.38 (0.42–4.59) Ref < .05

The reference group is the never breastfed infants. NS = not significant.


a
p < .05.

45
H.J. Choi et al. Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

Table 4
Adjusted binary logistic regression analyses of K-DST scores at 6 months according to duration of exclusive breastfeeding.
Breastfeeding duration p

Exclusive breastfeeding until Exclusive breastfeeding until 4 months of Mixed breastfeeding until Never breastfed
6 months of age age, thereafter mixed breastfeeding 6 months of age

n = 24 n = 82 n = 130 n = 19

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

6 months
Gross motor 0.19 (0.16–2.27) 0.24 (0.02–2.35) 0.16 (0.02–1.54) Ref NS
Fine motor 0.87 (0.13–5.86) 0.90 (0.18–4.61) 1.74 (0.34–8.83) Ref NS
Cognition 2.74 (0.22–33.79) 4.11 (0.52–32.37) 2.87 (0.50–16.49) Ref NS
Communication 2.69 (0.55–13.20) 4.12a (1.11-15.28) 2.01 (0.65–6.29) Ref < .05
Social interaction 2.29 (0.34–15.50) 6.04a (1.05-34.66) 1.68 (0.43–6.60) Ref < .05

12 months
Gross motor 0.28 (0.07–1.13) 0.62 (0.17–2.18) 0.61 (0.18–2.03) Ref NS
Fine motor 0.87 (0.13–5.86) 0.90 (0.17–4.61) 1.74 (0.34–8.83) Ref NS
Cognition 4.41 (0.42–46.68) 6.66a (1.02-43.63) 1.65 (0.42–6.49) Ref < .05
Communication 4.34 (0.73–25.79) 3.93a (1.07-14.40) 2.02 (0.65–6.32) Ref < .05
Social interaction 3.21 (0.52–19.98) 8.17a (1.59-42.05) 1.50 (0.45–5.04) Ref < .05

The reference group is the never breastfed infants.


Adjusted for mother's age at birth, birth order, birth weight, gestational age (37–42 weeks = 1), and socioeconomic status (paternal education, maternal education,
household income). NS = not significant.
a
p < .05.

perceive their mother's care as affectionate. In addition, similar findings followed by mixed breastfeeding, the infants who received 6 months of
have reported in studies on attachment, which is considered to be im- exclusive breastfeeding had higher means scores on the cognition,
portant in social development, that breastfeeding enhances secure at- communication, and social interaction dimensions at 6 and 12 months
tachment in mother–child relationships by “skin-to-skin contact” [44]. than did the never breastfed group. This indicates a synergistic effect.
Attachment here means a strong affectionate tie with special people in Therefore, continued research on the effects of exclusive breastfeeding
one's life [45]. Consistent with this, Else-Quest et al. [46] reported that for 4 months and 6 months on infant development is needed.
children who were breastfed tend to have stronger emotional bonds The limitations of this study are as follows. First, the monthly
with their mothers; Britton et al. [47] found that mothers who chose household income of 50.6% of the participants was less than $3500.
breastfeeding tended to respond more sensitively to infant cues in Considering that the monthly household income of Korea in 2017 was
mutual interactions during early infancy and that the greater sensitivity reported to be $4200 [50], the mothers in the sample had relatively
of these mothers directly influenced the infant's secure attachment high household income, but the income of the families in our sample
formation. In addition, mothers with high sensitivity were more likely was comparable to the mean household income level of Korea. How-
to breastfeed for longer during infancy [47]. These results suggest that ever, the fact that participants were a middle-class sample could be a
by breastfeeding the infant, the mothers were in physical contact with limitation of this study. Second, due to the small sample size, there are
their children and as they watched the child breastfeed, they felt that limits to our ability to generalize the study results and we need to
their emotions were closely communicated to each other, forming a conduct additional studies to support generalization.
bond and attachment. Korean mothers try to get used to breastfeeding
the infant and want to use the bottle only partially [48]. It is known that 5. Conclusion
breastfeeding rather than bottle feeding using a breast pump helps
mothers produce the proper amount of breast milk and adapt to the A strength of the present study is that we measured the overall
infant's breastfeeding [49]. Although this study did not investigate the development of infants through a 1-year follow-up. Exclusive breast-
variations in breastfeeding, i.e. bottle feeding or breastfeeding, further feeding in infants up to 4 months followed by mixed breastfeeding was
research should look at the variations in breastfeeding and how it re- associated with significantly better infant development: at 6 months,
lates to infant development in the future. the relationship was present for communication and social interaction,
Interestingly, unlike infants who received 4 months of exclusive whereas at 12 months, it had expanded to include cognition. We can
breastfeeding followed by mixed breastfeeding, infants with 6 months conclude that this pattern of breastfeeding maximizes the develop-
of exclusive breastfeeding did not show statistically significant effects mental benefits to infants during the first year of the infant's life.
on all developmental areas. Previous studies have reported that the Further research is, however, needed to support the WHO's re-
longer breastfeeding duration has a positive effect on infant develop- commendation for exclusive breastfeeding up to 6 months, rather than
ment. In contrast, in this study, the effects of exclusive breastfeeding 4 months. However, we are reasonably certain that long-term (at least
duration on infant's cognitive, language, and social development were 6 months) continuous breastfeeding is associated with improved early
no longer significant when mothers continued to exclusively breastfeed infant development. Further expansion of this research to include the
after 4 months. A review by Fewtrell et al. [18] has suggested that the long-term follow-up into early childhood to observe the effects of ex-
benefits of breastfeeding and the benefits of exclusive breastfeeding clusive breastfeeding until 6 months, as well as inclusion of diverse
should be distinguished. However, caution is needed when interpreting racial and ethnic groups could strengthen this study.
the results of this study; in particular, it is possible that the sample size
of the 6-month exclusive breastfeeding group was too small, explaining Acknowledgements
why it did not show statistical significance. The evidence for this is that,
as with the infants who received 4 months of exclusive breastfeeding None.

46
H.J. Choi et al. Early Human Development 127 (2018) 42–47

Funding [20] J.S. Halterman, J.M. Kaczorowski, C.A. Aligne, P. Auinger, P.G. Szilagyi, Iron de-
ficiency and cognitive achievement among school-aged children and adolescents in
the United States, Pediatrics 107 (2001) 1381–1386.
This work was supported by the Gachon University Research Fund [21] C. Gertosio, C. Meazza, S. Pagani, M. Bozzola, Breastfeeding and its gamut of
of 2018 [GCU-2018-0357]. benefits, Minerva Pediatr. 68 (2016) 201–212.
[22] M.S. Kramer, R. Kakuma, Optimal Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding (Review),
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2012, John Wiley & Sons, New Jersey,
Declarations of interest 2012, http://cochranelibrary-wiley.com/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD003517.pub2/
epdf (accessed 24 July 2018).
None. [23] S.J. Byun, A study on performance of breastfeeding by employed mother, Korean J.
Women Health Nurs. 1 (1995) 137–152.
All authors have approved the final article. [24] M.K. Lee, J.H. Kim, Breastfeeding method in mothers of 4 week old infants, Korean
J. Women Health Nurs. 8 (2002) 257–267.
Ethical approval [25] J.H. Yeo, Influencing factors in breast feeding duration, Korean J. Women Health
Nurs. 11 (2005) 142–147.
[26] J.O. Koo, K.S. Choi, W.G. Kim, Longitudinal study of growth, energy and protein
The study was approved by the IRB board of Gachon University metabolism of Korean breast fed and formula fed infants from 1 to 3 post-
(Approval number: 1044396-201705-HR-079-01). partum months, Korean J. Community Nutr. 1 (1996) 47–60.
[27] M.H. Choi, S.J. Moon, H.S. Ahn, An ecological study of changes in the components
of human milk during the breast feeding and the relationships between the dietary
References behavior of lactating women and the growth of breast-fed infants, J. Nutr. Health 24
(1991) 77–86.
[1] S.K. Kim, Y.K. Kim, H.R. Kim, J.S. Park, C.Y. Son, Y.J. Choi, Y.W. Kim, G.E. Lee, [28] K.A. Kang, S.J. Kim, The growth, developmental status of breastfed infants, Kor.
A.R. Yoon, The 2012 National Survey on Fertility, Family Health & Welfare in Parent Child Health J. 9 (2006) 33–42.
Korea, Korea Institute for Health and Social Affairs, Seoul, 2012, https://www. [29] B.L. Eun, H.J. Jung, G.H. Kim, S.G. Kim, S.W. Kim, Y.G. Kim, Y.A. Kim, J.S. Kim,
kihasa.re.kr/common/filedown.do?seq=16475 (accessed 16 January 2018). J.G. Kim, C.T. Kim, I.K. Sung, S.M. Sin, G.J. Oh, H.J. Yu, H.J. Yu, S.J. Lim, J.H. Lee,
[2] E.J. Choi, E.J. Park, H.R. Kim, M.A. Oh, N.H. Lee, J.H. Choi, Survey on H.I. Jung, J.E. Choi, Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants & Children,
Breastfeeding in Korea, The Korean Committee for Unicef, Seoul, 2016, http:// Ministry of Health and Welfare, Disease Control Center, Seoul, 2014, http://
www.unicef.or.kr/news/report_list.asp#609 (accessed 14 January 2018). cfile202.uf.daum.net/attach/2630134A54040392357D0C (accessed 23 February
[3] L. Duijts, V.W. Jaddoe, A. Hofman, H.A. Moll, Prolonged and exclusive breast- 2017).
feeding reduces the risk of infectious diseases in infancy, Pediatrics 126 (2010) [30] Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention & Ministry of Health and Welfare,
18–25. Korea health statistics 2009: Korea national health and nutrition examination
[4] M.S. Kramer, R. Kakuma, The optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding, survey IV, Chungbuk, (2009) https://knhanes.cdc.go.kr/knhanes (accessed 23 July
Protecting Infants Through Human Milk, Springer, Boston, 2004, pp. 63–77. 2018).
[5] M. Makrides, M.A. Neumann, R.W. Byard, K. Simmer, R.A. Gibson, Fatty acid [31] H.W. Park, K.H. Cho, H.J. Choe, Standardization of Korean Bayley Scales of Infant
composition of brain, retina, and erythrocytes in breast-and formula-fed infants, Development (K-BSID-2), Korean J. Clin. Psychol. Dev. 16 (2003) 121–134.
Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 60 (1994) 189–194. [32] H.W. Park, K.J. Kwak, K.B. Park, Korean Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
[6] S.C. Deoni, D.C. Dean, I. Piryatinsky, J. O'muircheartaigh, N. Waskiewicz, Intelligence (K-WPPSI), Special Education, Seoul, 1996.
K. Lehman, M. Han, H. Dirks, Breastfeeding and early white matter development: a [33] J.C. Nunnally, Psychometric Theory, second ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, 1978.
cross-sectional study, Neuroimage 82 (2013) 77–86. [34] R.J. Gregory, Psychological Testing: History, Principles and Applications, Allyn &
[7] S.J. Carling, M.M. Demment, C.L. Kjolhede, C.M. Olson, Breastfeeding duration and Bacon, Boston, 1992.
weight gain trajectory in infancy, Pediatrics 135 (2015) 111–119. [35] N.K. Angelsen, T. Vik, G. Jacobsen, L.S. Bakketeig, Breast feeding and cognitive
[8] B.J. Paine, M. Makrides, R.A. Gibson, Duration of breast-feeding and Bayley's development at age 1 and 5 years, Arch. Dis. Child. 85 (2001) 183–188.
Mental Developmental Index at 1 year of age, J. Paediatr. Child Health 35 (1999) [36] E.F. Maggi, L.C. Magalhães, A.F. Campos, M.C.F. Bouzada, Preterm children have
82–85. unfavorable motor, cognitive, and functional performance when compared to term
[9] A. Sacker, M.A. Quigley, Y.J. Kelly, Breastfeeding and developmental delay: find- children of preschool age, J. Pediatr. 90 (2014) 377–383.
ings from the millennium cohort study, Pediatrics 118 (2006) 682–689. [37] R. Flacking, F. Dykes, U. Ewald, The influence of fathers' socioeconomic status and
[10] C. Jardí, C. Hernández-Martínez, J. Canals, V. Arija, C. Bedmar, N. Voltas, paternity leave on breastfeeding duration: a population-based cohort study, Scand.
N. Aranda, Influence of breastfeeding and iron status on mental and psychomotor J. Public Health 38 (2010) 337–343.
development during the first year of life, Infant Behav. Dev. 50 (2017) 1–11. [38] M. Vestergaard, C. Obel, T.B. Henriksen, H.T. Sørensen, E. Skajaa, J. Østergaard,
[11] J.W. Anderson, M.B. Johnstone, D.T. Remley, Breast-feeding and cognitive devel- Duration of breastfeeding and developmental milestones during the latter half of
opment: a meta-analysis, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 70 (1999) 525–535. infancy, Acta Paediatr. 88 (1999) 1327–1332.
[12] E.B. Isaacs, B.R. Fischl, B.T. Quinn, W.K. Chong, D.G. Gadian, A. Lucas, Impact of [39] M.A. Crawford, The role of essential fatty acids in neural development: implications
breast milk on intelligence quotient, brain size, and white matter development, for perinatal nutrition, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 57 (1993) 703–710.
Pediatr. Res. 67 (2010) 357–362. [40] P. Willatts, J.S. Forsyth, M.K. DiModugno, S. Varma, M. Colvin, Effect of long-chain
[13] M.A. Quigley, C. Hockley, C. Carson, Y. Kelly, M.J. Renfrew, A. Sacker, polyunsaturated fatty acids in infant formula on problem solving at 10 months of
Breastfeeding is associated with improved child cognitive development: a popula- age, Lancet 352 (1998) 688–691.
tion-based cohort study, J. Pediatr. 160 (2012) 25–32. [41] P. Burgard, Critical evaluation of the methodology employed in cognitive devel-
[14] W.H. Oddy, M. Robinson, G.E. Kendall, J. Li, S.R. Zubrick, F.J. Stanley, opment trials, Acta Paediatr. 92 (2003) 6–10.
Breastfeeding and early child development: a prospective cohort study, Acta [42] H.S. Kim, The Mystery of mother's Milk, Ed Tech, Seoul, 1993.
Paediatr. 100 (2011) 992–999. [43] D.M. Fergusson, L.J. Woodward, Breast feeding and later psychosocial adjustment,
[15] V. Leventakou, T. Roumeliotaki, K. Koutra, M. Vassilaki, E. Mantzouranis, P. Bitsios, Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 13 (1999) 144–157.
P.M. Kogevinas, L. Chatzi, Breastfeeding duration and cognitive, language and [44] M. Klaus, Mother and infant: early emotional ties, Pediatrics 102 (1998)
motor development at 18 months of age: Rhea mother–child cohort in Crete, 1244–1246.
Greece, J. Epidemiol. Community Health 69 (2015) 232–239. [45] L.E. Berk, Child Development, Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, 2003.
[16] R.T. Pivik, R.A. Dykman, H. Jing, J.M. Gilchrist, T.M. Badger, The influence of [46] N.M. Else-Quest, J.S. Hyde, R. Clark, Breastfeeding, bonding, and the mother-infant
infant diet on early developmental changes in processing human voice speech sti- relationship, Merrill-Palmer Q. 49 (2003) 495–517.
muli: ERP variations in breast and milk formula-fed infants at 3 and 6 months after [47] J.R. Britton, H.L. Britton, V. Gronwaldt, Breastfeeding, sensitivity, and attachment,
birth, Dev. Neuropsychol. 31 (2007) 279–335. Pediatrics 118 (2006) 1436–1443.
[17] D.L. Drane, J.A. Logemann, A critical evaluation of the evidence on the association [48] S.H. Kim, Process of breast feeding mother's adaptation, Korean J. Women Health
between type of infant feeding and cognitive development, Paediatr. Perinat. Nurs. 15 (2009) 186–195.
Epidemiol. 14 (2000) 349–356. [49] H.S. Kim, The Guideline Book for Breastfeeding, Hyunmoonsa, Seoul, 2001.
[18] M. Fewtrell, D.C. Wilson, I. Booth, A. Lucas, Six months of exclusive breast feeding: [50] KOSIS, Monthly income and expenditure per household, (2017), http://kosis.kr/
how good is the evidence? BMJ 342 (2011) 1–5. statHtml/statHtml. do?orgId=101&tblId=DT_1L9H002&vw_cd=MT_ZTITLE&list_
[19] C.J. Chantry, C.R. Howard, P. Auinger, Full breastfeeding duration and risk for iron id=G_A_4_1_1&seqNo=&lang_mode=ko&language=kor&obj_var_id=&itm_id=&
deficiency in US infants, Breastfeed. Med. 2 (2007) 63–73. conn_path=MT_ZTITLE (accessed 4 July 2018).

47

S-ar putea să vă placă și