Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

This article was downloaded by: [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY]

On: 17 March 2015, At: 12:02


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Clinical Neuropsychologist


Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ntcn20

The boston qualitative scoring system


for the rey-osterrieth complex figure: A
study of children with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder
a a b a c d
Deborah A. Cahn , Ann C. Marcotte , Robert A. Sten , James
a d e d
E. Arruda , Natacha A. Akshoomoff & Isabell C. Leshko
a
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior , Brown University
School of Medicine , Providence, Rhode Island
b
Department of Pediatrics , Memorial Hospital of Rhode Island ,
Pawtucket, Rhode Island
c
Department of Clinical Neurosciences , Brown University School of
Medicine , Providence, Rhode Island
d
Rhode Island Hospital , Providence, Rhode Island
e
Georgia State University , Atlanta, Georgia
Published online: 08 Nov 2007.

To cite this article: Deborah A. Cahn , Ann C. Marcotte , Robert A. Sten , James E. Arruda , Natacha
A. Akshoomoff & Isabell C. Leshko (1996) The boston qualitative scoring system for the rey-osterrieth
complex figure: A study of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 10:4, 397-406, DOI: 10.1080/13854049608406700

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13854049608406700

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015
The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1385-4046/96/1004-397$12.00
1996, Vol. 10, NO.4, pp. 397-406 0 Swets & Zeitlinger

The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the


Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: A Study of Children
with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder*
Deborah A. Cahn', Ann C. Marcottelr2,Robert A. S t e n ~ ' , ~James, ~ , E. A r r ~ d a ' . ~Natacha
, A.
Akshoomoff5, and Isabel1 C. Leshko4
'Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University School of Medicine, Providence, Rhode
Island, *Department of Pediatrics, Memorial Hospital of Rhode Island, Pawtucket, Rhode Island, 3Department
of Clinical Neurosciences, Brown University School of Medicine, Providence, Rhode Island, 4RhodeIsland
Hospital, Providence, Rhode Island, and 'Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

ABSTRACT

The Boston Qualitative Scoring System (BQSS) for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) was
utilized to examine the qualitative features of ROCF performance of children with Attention Deficit Hyper-
activity Disorder (ADHD). Thirty-nine children with ADHD were compared to age-matched controls (n =
39) on their reproduction of the ROCF. ADHD children performed more poorly than did control children
on measures of attention to detail, expansion, accuracy, and neatness. Sensitivity and specificity of individ-
ual BQSS measures for discriminating ADHD from control subjects were determined, and a logistic regres-
sion model was derived, yielding an overall sensitivity of 64% and specificity of 97% for the classification
of ADHD. Eighty-one percent of all children were correctly classified. Cross-validation of this model on
an independent sample of ADHD and control subjects revealed good predictive accuracy. These findings
suggest that the BQSS may be a useful measure in the neuropsychological evaluation of children with
suspected ADHD.

Attention Deficit H y p e r a c t i v i t y D i s o r d e r sin Card Sorting Task (Chelune, Ferguson,


(ADHD) is a constellation of developmentally Koon, & Dickey, 1986; Gorenstein, Mammato,
inappropriate behaviors, including hyperactivity, & Sandy, 1989; Reader, Harris, Schuerholz, &
inattention, and impulsivity, that arises in child- Denckla, 1994), verbal fluency tasks (Felton,
hood and impairs social, academic, and/or occu- Wood, Brown, Cambell, & Harter, 1987; Grod-
pational functioning (American Psychiatric As- zinsky & Diamond, 1992), and the Porteus
sociation, I 994). Several neuropsychological Mazes (Grodzinsky & Diamond, 1992; Homa-
studies of children with ADHD have demon- tidis & Konstantareas, 1981; Kuehne, Kehle, &
strated impairment on some tasks that are be- McMahon, 1987). However, not all studies have
lieved to reflect problems with executive func- demonstrated executive dysfunction in subjects
tioning, such as integration, planning, organiza- with ADHD (Barkley & Grodzinsky, 1994;
tion, and inhibition. For example, children with Reader et al., 1994). A possible explanation for
ADHD have been shown to perform more these disparate findings is that studies have var-
poorly than d o matched controls on the Wiscon- ied tremendously in subject selection (e.g., oper-

* Dr. Cahn is now at the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of
Medicine. Portions of this paper were presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the National Academy of
Neuropsychology, San Francisco, CA, November, 1995.
Address correspondence to: Ann C. Marcotte, Ph.D., Department of Pediatrics, Memorial Hospital of Rhode
Island, 111 Brewster St., Pawtucket, RI, 02860, USA.
Accepted for publication: January 26, 1996.
398 DEBORAH A. CAHN ET AL.

ational definition of ADHD, co-morbid learning to assess process aspects of ROCF reproduction,
disabilities) and test-related performance vari- we undertook this exploratory study to examine
ables (e.g., differences in test versions, adminis- the ability of the BQSS to differentiate children
tration, and scoring). with ADHD from children without ADHD. A
One test that has been used to assess the exec- number of hypotheses were generated. These
utive aspects of visuomotor integration is the were based upon the suggested executive dys-
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) (Cor- function thought to characterize the cognitive
win & Blysma, 1993; Lezak, 1995; Osterrieth, impairments in ADHD, and upon clinical expe-
1944; Rey, 1941). This test requires the subject rience using the ROCF with the traditional 36-
to copy a complex figure, engaging visuomotor point scoring system (Lezak, 1995) applied to
integration and visuoperception abilities in con- children.
cert with planning and organization. Grodzinsky We anticipated qualitatively different perfor-
and Diamond (1992) reported that boys with mances on selected BQSS measures in children
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

ADHD performed more poorly than did normal with ADHD as compared to normal control chil-
control subjects on their copy of the ROCF. In dren despite average constructional and visuo-
contrast, several other studies have reported spatial functioning in the ADHD children. As-
negative findings (Barkley & Grodzinsky, 1994; sumed deficits in inhibitory and attentional pro-
Frost, Moffitt, & McGee, 1989; Reader et al., cesses led us to expect poorer scores on the mea-
1994). One explanation for these discrepant sures of Accuracy and Neatness in copying the
findings may be that traditional quantitative figure. Difficulty in generating efficient and or-
scoring systems were used and, as suggested by ganized strategies were expected to result in a
Reader et al. (1994), these scoring systems may lower score on the Planning score, which
not be sensitive enough to capture organiza- assesses the overall organization of the produc-
tional aspects of the ROCF. tion, including such aspects as the order in
Recently, the Boston Qualitative Scoring Sys- which particular elements are drawn and the
tem (BQSS) (Stern et al., 1994) was developed placement on the page of the entire production.
in order to capture specific impairments in the Two measures included in the BQSS, Vertical
process by which the ROCF is reproduced. The and Horizontal Expansion, were hypothesized to
system includes 17 dimensions upon which the be impaired in the ADHD group, possibly re-
drawing is judged, and uses an extensive set of flecting difficulty inhibiting graphomotor output
criteria, templates, and example productions in and self-monitoring.
deriving each score. The dimensions of the
BQSS were chosen based on clinical observa-
tions of a wide range of patients with focal and METHOD
diffuse neurobehavioral disorders, as well as on
the literature pertaining to visuoconstructional Subjects
functioning and visuospatial learning and mem-
ory. The BQSS has been shown to have very ADHD Subjects
Potential subjects were identified retrospectively
good interrater reliability (Stern et al., 1994) and from clinic files and were included in the study if
is particularly useful because it provides a vari- they met DSM-IV (APA, 1994) criteria for ADHD
ety of scores, each assessing specific qualitative and the inclusionary criteria described below. All
features of a production and the processes em- clinic subjects were seen for evaluation in a Neuro-
ployed. Akshoomoff and Stiles (1995) have re- developmental Evaluation Clinic at a metropolitan
cently demonstrated that the BQSS is a useful university-affiliated medical center in Rhode Is-
tool for assessing visuospatial constructional land. Each child was evaluated individually by a
Child Neuropsychologist (AM) and a pediatrician.
and organizational skills in normal children. Prior to their evaluation, each subject’s school re-
Because planning and organizational skills cords, including all past evaluations, were ob-
are purported to be affected in ADHD, and be- tained. Each child received comprehensive neuro-
cause the BQSS has been developed specifically psychological testing (including measures of ex-
BOSTON QUALITATIVE SCORING SYSTEM IN ADHD 399

pressive and receptive language, visual perception, 8.5 (SD= 1.7) years, and a gender distribution of
visual motor integration, memory, executive func- 20 boys and 19 girls. The NC children were all in
tioning, and attention), several parent and teacher regular school placements and did not have a his-
behavioral rating forms were completed, and a full tory of major medical illness, psychiatric illness,
neurological examination was performed on each developmental disorder, or significant visual or
child. All subjects had been administered the auditory impairments. All children were tested in
ROCF as part of the neuropsychological evalua- San Diego, California, in either a laboratory, local
tion. community center, or local middle school, and all
Selection for study inclusion was made without came from primarily middle-class families and
knowledge of the subject’s performance on the were representative of the local community in
ROCF. Furthermore, BQSS scores were not avail- terms of race and gender.
able or used in the clinical diagnosis of ADHD. All
subjects had obtained a Full Scale IQ score of Instrumentation
greater than 80 (on either the WISC-I11 or Stanford All subjects were individually administered the
Binet) within the last 3 years. If no IQ score was ROCF using a standardized procedure. Control
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

available, a child was included only if she or he subjects received copy and immediate recall trials
had never been referred for special educational as part of a brief assessment protocol (Akshoomoff
testing, had demonstrated average abilities on stan- & Stiles, 1995). The ADHD group received copy,
dardized group achievement tests, and had no out- immediate recall, and 30-min delayed recall trials
standing deficits on recent report cards. All chil- as part of a larger neuropsychological evaluation.
dren had English as their primary language. For the present study, only copy conditions were
Potential subjects were excluded if they were included. All subjects were instructed to copy the
receiving any special educational services in ROCF as carefully as possible. Children were told
school at the time of evaluation, or if they had a that at specific intervals they would be given a dif-
documented history of head injury, seizure disor- ferent colored pen to continue their drawing. Pens
der, neuromuscular disorder, psychiatric problems, were switched approximately every minute, or
learning disability, motor disorder (e.g., tics, Tou- when the subject began to draw a new part of the
rette’s Syndrome), and sensory problems. No sub- figure ( e g , to detect fragmentation of a specific
ject was on medication for the treatment of ADHD element of the figure). Children were not allowed
at the time of evaluation. Vision and hearing were to rotate the model or the blank sheet of paper, and
screened at the time of evaluation, and no subject were told to indicate to the examiner when they
exhibited deficits on either test. Subjects were not were finished with the copy. All productions were
excluded on the basis of gender or handedness. scored by trained research assistants who were
The resulting ADHD group was comprised of blind to group membership and to the hypotheses
39 (32 male, 7 female) children between the ages being explored in the study.
of 6 and 12 years (mean age = 8.3; SD = 1.9). This Each production was scored using the BQSS
ratio (4: 1) of male to female is consistent with gen- (Stern et al., 1994). Table 1 outlines and describes
der ratios in the community reported for this disor- the 17 BQSS dimensions. The 17 scores were de-
der (APA, 1994). Furthermore, we did not antici- rived to assess pertinent qualitative features of
pate gender differences on the ROCF in this age ROCF production, including the following: the
group, based on the findings of Akshoomoff and presence, accuracy, and placement of major com-
Stiles (1995). The ADHD subjects performed ponents of the figure (i.e., configural elements,
within normal limits on the Developmental Test of clusters, details); fragmentation of key elements;
Visual Motor Integration (Beery, 1989) (mean overall planning; reduction and expansion of the
standard score = 95.7; SD = 9.8) and on the Hooper entire figure; rotation; perseveration; confabula-
Visual Organization Test (Hooper, 1958) (mean z tion; neatness; and asymmetry. Many of the scores
score = 0.0; SD = .81). are rated by comparison to specific criteria (and
example productions) included in the manual.
Normal Control ( N C ) Subjects Some scores (e.g., Horizontal and Vertical Expan-
Control subjects were selected from a larger sam- sion) are rated by use of transparency template
ple of children who had received the ROCF as part overlays. The version of the scoring system used in
of another study investigating the normal develop- the present study (September 1994 Version) dif-
ment of visuospatial abilities in children (Akshoo- fered from the version described by Stern et al.
moff & Stiles, 1995). Each control subject was (1994) in that each score could receive a possible
matched in age to a child in the ADHD group, re- score ranging from 0 to 4, with 0 representing poor
sulting in 39 control subjects with a mean age of performance and 4 representing good performance;
400 DEBORAH A. CAHN ET AL.

the earlier version utilized a 1 to 5 scale. AS cal criteria. In forward selection, variables are al-
described above, the present study utilized selected lowed to enter into the logistic regression model if
BQSS variables based on hypotheses regarding the they contribute significantly to the differentiation
impact of ADHD on ROCF performance. The vari- of normal and clinical subjects. In the present
ables chosen for the current investigation included: study, a single BQSS variable was allowed to enter
Configural Accuracy, Cluster Accuracy, Detail the equation at each step if the probability associ-
Presence, Fragmentation, Planning, Vertical Ex- ated with that variable’s “F to enter” was less
pansion, Horizontal Expansion, Perseveration, and than .05. Once the logistic regression equation was
Neatness. derived, an “estimated probability” of having
ADHD was subsequently derived for each subject.
Statistical Analyses These probabilities range from 0.0 to 1.0, with a
Group comparisons were performed on the BQSS value of 0.0 representing “perfect classification”
variables of interest using nonparametric Mann- of a NC subject, and 1.0 representing “perfect
Whitney U tests, where children with ADHD were classification” of an ADHD subject. The most
compared to NC children. Measures of sensitivity commonly used cut-off value for the estimated
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

and specificity were calculated to determine the probability is 0.5 (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989).
overall accuracy of each BQSS measure in identi- and was thus chosen for the current study. There-
fying a child with ADHD. For the present analy- fore, an estimated probability value less than or
ses, each possible BQSS score for an item (i.e., 0, equal to 0.5 classified a subject as NC, while an
1 , 2, 3, 4) was treated as a possible cut-off score estimated probability value greater than 0.5 classi-
and the corresponding sensitivity (i.e., percentage fied a subject as ADHD.
of children with ADHD scoring at or below this To assess the stability of the logistic model, it
score) and specificity (i.e., percentage of NC chil- was then cross-validated with an independent sam-
dren scoring above this score) for the diagnosis of ple of ADHD (n = 15) and NC (n = 15) subjects
ADHD were calculated. A receiver operating char- who were similar in demographic characteristics to
acteristic (ROC) curve was then generated for each the original sample but who were not included in
of the BQSS variables by plotting sensitivity the derivation of the original logistic model.
against specificity for every possible cut-off score. ADHD subjects were drawn from the same clinic
Since it was decided a priori that sensitivity and sample and met the same inclusionary criteria as
specificity were to be of equal importance, the op- the original sample. All ADHD subjects included
timal cut-off score was the point at which the sum in the cross-validation were male. The mean age of
of sensitivity and specificity reached a maximum these ADHD subjects was 9.2 years (SD= 1.7).
value. In the present study, the criterion “gold NC subjects were drawn from the original sample
standard” for correct classification of ADHD was (Akshoomoff & Stiles, 1995) with an effort to
the diagnosis made at the time of the comprehen- match the ADHD cross-validation sample on age.
sive multidisciplinary clinic evaluation. The mean age of the NC subjects was 9.1 (SD=
In addition to determining the optimal cut-off 1.4) years and there were 6 girls and 9 boys. The
score and sensitivity and specificity for each indi- NC and ADHD cross-validation groups did not
vidual BQSS score, a stepwise logistic regression differ significantly in age ( t = -.14, p > .05).
analysis with forward variable selection was per-
formed to determine the combination of BQSS
measures that would provide the most accurate RESULTS
differentiation between NC and ADHD subjects.
Prior to conducting the stepwise logistic regres-
sion, all BQSS scores were dichotomized accord- The medians of the BQSS items of interest at-
ing to the previously derived cut-off scores. Con- tained by both groups and corresponding Mann-
sequently, subjects scoring at or below the cut-off Whitney-U tests are presented in Table 2. NC
were given a value of 0 (i.e., were considered children performed significantly better than chil-
ADHD), while those subjects scoring above the dren with ADHD on Configural Accuracy, Clus-
cut-off were given a value of 1 (i.e., were consid- ter Accuracy, Detail Presence , Vertical Expan-
ered normal). All BQSS variables investigated in
sion, Horizontal Expansion, and Neatness. The
the ROC analyses were entered into the logistic
model. difference between ADHD and NC children on
The stepwise approach to variable selection in- Perseveration approached significance (p < .07).
volves selecting variables for inclusion or exclu- There were no group differences on Fragmenta-
sion in a sequential fashion based solely on statisti- tion or Planning. Figure l displays examples of
BOSTON QUALITATIVE SCORING SYSTEM IN ADHD 40 1

Table 1. Description of Scores for the Boston Qualitative Scoring System (BQSS) for the Rey-Osterrieth Com-
plex Figure (ROCF). *

Score Description

1. Configural Presence The degree to which some representation of each of the six configural elements (i.e.,
rectangle, two diagonals, two bisectors, triangle on right) exists in the production,
regardless of the quality of those representations.
2. Configural Accuracy The quality of each of those configural elements present, based on the completeness
of element, accuracy of placement as related to other elements, size, proportion, etc.
3. Cluster Presence The degree to which some representation of each of the nine clusters (i.e., subunits
of the figure, such as the rectangle with diagonals in the left side of the figure [Clus-
ter I], the circle with three dots) exists in the production, regardless of the quality of
those representations.
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

4. Cluster Accuracy The quality of each of those clusters present, based on the completeness of the cluster,
accuracy of placement and orientation of each cluster’s parts, size, proportion, etc.
5. Cluster Placement The degree to which each of the clusters present is positioned in the proper region of
the figure.
6. Detail Presence The degree to which some representation of each of the six detail elements (i.e., addi-
tional single lines in the figure, not included as either configural elements or clusters)
exists in the production, regardless of the quality of those representations.
7. Detail Placement The degree to which each of the detail elements present is positioned in the proper
region of the figure, especially in relation to configural elements.
8. Fragmentation The degree to which the configural elements and Cluster 1 are drawn as whole units
(e.g., completed in a single pen stroke or before another part of the figure is begun).
9. Planning The overall sense of importance given to the configural rectangle; the degree to
which the configural elements are completed prior to beginning the clusters and de-
tails; and the awareness of external structure, boundaries of the page, and centering
of the figure on the page.
10. Size Reduction The degree to which the figure has been reduced from the size of the original stimu-
lus (using a transparency template for scoring).
1 1. Vertical Expansion The degree to which the figure has been expanded along the vertical axis from the
size of the original stimulus (using a transparency template for scoring).
12. Horizontal Expansion The degree to which the figure has been expanded along the horizontal axis from the
size of the original stimulus (using a transparency template for scoring).
13. Rotation The degree to which the entire figure is rotated on the page (using a transparency
template for scoring).
14. Perseveration The amount of recognizable and inappropriate replication in the figure.
15. Confabulation The amount of additions to the figure that are foreign to the original stimulus and not
recognizable as perseverations.
16. Neatness The degree of care taken in producing a neat production, including the extent of dou-
ble drawn lines or overwriting, self-corrected errors, curved or tremulous lines, etc.
17. Right/Left Asymmetry An assessment of whether the figure is disproportionately distorted or lacking details
on one (right or left) side.

Note. All items are scored on a 0 (poor) to 4 (good) scale, with the exception of RighULeft Asymmetry which is
scored as either None, Right, or Left.
j: Scores are made using specific criteria, exemplars, and templates included in the Manual for the BQSS (avail-

able from Dr. Robert Stern, Neurobehavioral Research, Rhode Island Hospital, 1 10 Lockwood Street, Providence,
Rhode Island, 02903).
402 DEBORAH A. CAHN ET AL

reproductions of the ROCF made by an ADHD expansion were generally quite effective for dis-
subject and by an age-matched control subject. tinguishing between ADHD and NC subjects.

Sensitiviv and Specijicig Logistic Regression Analysis


Table 3 displays the optimal cut-off score and The following variables were entered into the
corresponding sensitivity and specificity for logistic regression in a step-wise manner with
classification obtained for each BQSS measure. forward selection: Configural Accuracy, Cluster
As illustrated in Table 3, the two measures of Accuracy, Detail Presence, Fragmentation, Plan-
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

Fig. 1. Top: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) stimulus. Middle: Copy of the ROCF produced by a 12-
year-old healthy female without Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Bottom: Copy of
the ROCF produced by a 12-year-old male with ADHD diagnosed using DSM-IV criteria by a neuro-
psychologist and a pediatrician. Note the extreme horizontal and vertical expansion in the production
by the child with ADHD.
BOSTON OUALITATIVE SCORING SYSTEM IN ADHD 403

Table 2. Median Scores (Rank Sums) of the Selected BQSS Measures Obtained by the Attention Deficit Hyper-
activity Disorder (ADHD) Group and Normal Control (NC) Group.

NC ADHD Mann-Whitney U Statistic


Configural Accuracy 1.0 (1827.0) 0.0 (1254.0) 1047.0**
Cluster Accuracy 1.0 (1782.5) 1.0 (1298.5) 1002.5**
Detail Presence 4.0 (1829.0) 3.0 (1252.0) 1049.0**
Fragmentation 1.0 (1415.5) 2.0 (1665.5) 635.5
Planning 2.0 (1623.5) 1.0 (1457.5) 843.5
Vertical Expansion 4.0 (2046.5) 2.0 (1034.5) 1266.5* *
Horizontal Ex pans i on 4.0 (2025.0) 2.0 (1056.0) 1245.0**
Perseveration 2.0 (1712.5) 1.0 (1368.5) 932.5
Neatness 2.0 (1765.5) 1.0 (1315.5) 985.5*
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

* p < .05, **p < .01.

Table 3. Sensitivity and Specificity for the BQSS Measures.

Cut-off score' Sensitivity Specificity


Configural Accuracy 0 69% 64%
Cluster Accuracy 1 97% 26%
Detail Presence 3 77% 59%
Fragmentation 2 74% 26%
Planning 0 21% 92%
Vertical Expansion 2 64% 97%
Horizontal Expansion 3 82% 67%
Perseveration 2 77% 46%
Neatness 1 62% 67%

'Those subjects scoring at or below the cut-off score were considered ADHD; those scoring above the cut-off
score were considered Normal.

ning, Vertical Expansion, Horizontal Expansion, logistic model. Eighty-one percent of all sub-
Perseveration, and Neatness. Using an a priori jects were correctly classified using this model.
probability value of .05 for inclusion of vari- The classification results, based upon the logis-
ables in the model, the following logistic equa- tic regression model are presented in Table 4.
tion for computing the probability of being clas- To assess the stability of the logistic model,
sified with ADHD was produced: the classification procedure was cross-validated
with an independent sample of 30 subjects as
Estimated Probability for the diagnosis of described above. When the dichotomized scores
e_
x for Vertical Expansion and Detail Presence
ADHD= _
l+e (based on the optimal cut-off scores) of these
subjects were entered into the logistic model,
where x = 4.46 + (- 4.72 Vertical Expansion) + 67% of ADHD and 80% of NC subjects were
(- 2.26 Detail Presence). Using a cut-off proba- correctly classified. The final results of the clas-
bility estimate of 0.50, 64% of ADHD and 97% sification procedure are presented in Table 4.
of NC subjects were correctly classified by the
404 DEBORAH A. CAHN ET AL.

Table 4. Classification Table of ADHD and NC Subjects Based on the Logistic Regression Model.

Classification by the Logistic Model


Clinical Diagnosis ADHD NC
Original Sample
ADHD 25/39 (64%) 14/39 (36%)
NC 1/39 (3%) 38/39 (97%)

Cross-validation Sample
ADHD 10115 (67%) 5/15 (33%)
NC 3/15 (20%) 12/15 (80%)
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

DISCUSSION obscured any possible group differences. That


is, children in this age group (6 to 12 years of
The present study sought to examine differences age) may exhibit poor planning ability when the
between children with ADHD and age-matched adult-based BQSS criteria are employed.
control children on the reproduction of the Akshoomoff and Stiles (1995) have developed a
ROCF. To this end, the BQSS, a scoring system modified planning score that is more sensitive to
providing quantitative analysis of specific quali- age-related changes in planning performance
tative features of visuoconstruction, was utilized and that takes into account strategies that chil-
to capture potential impairments in executive dren use in starting their drawing that may ex-
functioning. The results of this study revealed plain differences in the final organization of the
that children with ADHD but otherwise average reproduction.
visuomotor and visuoperceptual skills did, in- The results of the sensitivity and specificity
deed, display impairments on their reproduc- analyses revealed that specific BQSS measures
tions of the ROCF that reflected underlying dif- discriminated between ADHD and NC children.
ficulties with accuracy, attention to detail, and The BQSS items found to have the best
inhibition, relative to normal controls. Specifi- discriminative ability were those of Horizontal
cally, children with ADHD earned lower scores and Vertical Expansion. It is possible that these
on BQSS measures of Configural and Cluster measures may reflect impairment in planning
Accuracy, Detail Presence, Vertical and Hori- ability and impulsivity inasmuch as expansion
zontal Expansion, and Neatness. Contrary to a of the figure suggests difficulty anticipating the
priori hypotheses, the ADHD children were not future requirements of the task, impairments in
impaired relative to normal controls on the self-monitoring, and behavioral dyscontrol. The
BQSS Planning measure. logistic model derived in this study demon-
There are several possible explanations for strated that the measures most effective for the
this unexpected finding. First, because the differentiation of ADHD from NC children were
BQSS was initally developed for adult popula- Vertical Expansion and Detail Presence. In ag-
tions, it is possible that the Planning measure is gregate, these variables resulted in 64% sensitiv-
not appropriate for use with child populations as ity an 97% specificity. In the cross-validation,
currently defined. It has been reported (Akshoo- the classification accuracy was moderate, with a
moff & Stiles, 1995) that the BQSS Planning correct classification of 67% of the children
measure was not sufficiently sensitive to devel- with ADHD and 80% correct classification of
opmental trends. Second, as Table 2 indicates, NC children. The variables comprising the lo-
both subject groups performed poorly on this gistic model appear to tap the organization and
item, suggesting that a floor effect may have attention to detail problems that are thought to
BOSTON QUALITATIVE SCORING SYSTEM IN ADHD 405

be the cardinal features of the disorder. It should One limitation of the current study is that the
be noted that Horizontal Expansion alone pro- model employed only provides information with
vided 84% sensitivity and 67% specificity, but regard to the differentiation of ADHD from nor-
did not load into the logistic model, most likely mal children. The differential diagnosis of
because of shared variance with the other mea- ADHD often involves ruling out other childhood
sures (i.e., Vertical Expansion). disorders, such as learning disabilities and con-
Despite average visuomotor and visual inte- duct disorder. Barkley (1990) has suggested that
gration abilities, the children with ADHD in the behavioral rating scales such as the Revised
current study demonstrated impairments on their Conners Behavioral Rating Scales (Goyette,
reproductions of the ROCF. Analysis of their Conners, & Ulrich, 1978) and Child Behavior
productions using the BQSS suggests that un- Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) play
derlying impairments in accuracy, impulse con- an important role in the diagnosis of ADHD.
trol, and organization, reflecting the executive The role of neuropsychological testing in the
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

dysfunction associated with this disorder, re- diagnosis of ADHD has been disputed. This
sulted in the children with ADHD obtaining may, in part, reflect the fact that the criteria for
scores that were significantly lower than the the diagnosis of ADHD are behaviorally rather
scores obtained by normal, age-matched chil- than cognitively delineated (APA, 1994). The
dren. These findings are consistent with those of generally equivocal findings of neuropsycholog-
other researchers who have found that ADHD ical test performance in children with ADHD
children perform more poorly than normal chil- may be a function of the heterogeneity and
dren when task demands are complex (Barkley, comorbidity of other disorders seen in this syn-
1988; Chelune et al., 1986; Douglas, 1983; Fel- drome. Although the logistic model derived in
ton et a]., 1987; Gorenstein et al., 1989; Grod- this study provided good diagnostic accuracy, it
zinsky & Diamond, 1992; Homatidis & Kon- may perhaps be further enhanced by adding
stantareas, 1981; Kuehne et al., 1987; Reader et other neuropsychological measures of executive
al., 1994). dysfunction as well as behavioral rating scales.
The ability to detect these executive impair- Future studies examining the predictive ability
ments was enhanced by the use of a scoring sys- of a spectrum of psychometric and behavioral
tem sensitive to these process variables, and measures may result in a statistical model with
may have been overlooked if the traditional sin- very high sensitivity and specificity for the clas-
gle score (i.e., 36 point) system (Lezak, 1995) sification of ADHD.
had been employed. Given the high frequency The current exploratory study lends prelimi-
with which the ROCF is used in pediatric neuro- nary support for the inclusion of neuropsycho-
psychology evaluations, the current study lends logical measures such as the ROCF in the com-
support for the introduction of the use of the prehensive evaluation of ADHD. Future studies
BQSS in scoring the ROCF with this population should be undertaken to better understand the
of children. predictive properties of qualitative neuropsycho-
As previously discussed, however, modifica- logical instruments such as the BQSS in discri-
tions to the present system may need to be made minating ADHD from other common childhood
to accommodate issues in visuospatial skill de- disorders.
velopment (Akshoomoff & Stiles, 1995). Never- In summary, several BQSS measures appear
theless, the current study provides initial support to have some utility in detecting underlying
for the use of the BQSS in assessing visuo- qualitative differences in executive functioning
constructional and executive functioning diffi- between ADHD and NC children. Improved pre-
culties in ADHD. Indeed, two of the most sensi- dictive accuracy might result from the inclusion
tive measures, Horizontal and Vertical Expan- of developmentally driven measures of planning
sion, can be readily assessed (i.e., in less than 1 that take into consideration factors that affect
min) with BQSS templates (Stern et al., 1994). children’s performance on this task (Akshoo-
moff & Stiles, 1995) as well as behavioral rating
406 DEBORAH A. CAHN ET AL.

scales (Barkley, 1990).Continued exploration of and reading disability. Brain and Language, 31,
the clinical and research utility of this instru- 171-184.
Frost, L. A., Moffitt, T. E., & McGee, R. (1989). Neu-
ment with different child populations appears ropsychological correlates of psychopathology in
warranted. an unselected cohort of young adolescents. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 307-313.
Gorenstein, E. E., Mammato, C. A., & Sandy, I. M.
REFERENCES (1989). Performance on inattentive-overactive chil-
dren on selected measures of prefrontal-type func-
tion. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45,619-632.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual Goyette, C . H., Conners, C. K., & Ulrich, R. F.
for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child (1 978). Normative data on Revised Conners Parent
Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Ver- and Teacher Rating Scales. Journal of Abnormal
mont, Department of Psychiatry. Child Psychology, 6,221-236.
Akshoomoff, N. A., & Stiles, J. (1995).Developmen- Grodzinsky, G. M., & Diamond, R. (1992). Frontal
Downloaded by [JAMES COOK UNIVERSITY] at 12:02 17 March 2015

tal trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: lobe functioning in boys with attention-deficit hy-
Copying a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, peractivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychol-
364-377. ogy, 8,427-445.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic Homatidis, S . , & Konstantareas, M. M. (1981). As-
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th sessment of hyperactivity: Isolating measures of
ed.). Washington, DC: Author. high discriminant ability. Journal of Consulting
Barkley, R. A. (1988). Attention. In M. G. Tramon- and Clinical Psychology, 47,533-541.
tana & S. R. Hooper (Eds.), Assessment issues in Hooper, H. E. (1958). The Hooper Visual Organiza-
child neuropsychology (pp. 145-176). New York: tion Test Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psy-
Plenum. chological Services.
Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied lo-
disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. gistic regression. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kuehne, C., Kehle, T. J., & McMahon, W. (1987).
Barkley, R. A., & Grodzinsky, G. M. (1994). Are tests Differences between children with attention deficit
of frontal lobe functions useful in the diagnosis of disorder, children with specific learning disabili-
Attention Deficit Disorders? The Clinical Neuro- ties, and normal children. Journal of School Psy-
psychologist, 8, 121-139. chology, 25,161-166.
Beery, K. E. (1989). The Developmental Test of Vi- Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment
sual-Motor Zntegration: Administration, scoring (3rd ed.). New York, N Y Oxford University Press.
and teaching manual (3rd ed.). Cleveland, OH: Ostenieth, P. A. (1944). La test d'une figure com-
Modem Curriculum Press. plexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30,206-353.
Chelune, G. J., Ferguson, W., Koon, R., & Dickey, T. Reader, M. J., Harris, E. L., Schuerholz, L. J., &
0. (1986). Frontal lobe disinhibition in attention Denckla, M. B. (1994). Attention deficit disorder
deficit disorder. Child Psychiatry and Human De- and executive dysfunction. Developmental Neuro-
velopment, 16,221-234. psychology, 10,493-512.
Corwin, J., & Blysma, F. W. (1993). Translation: Psy- Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans les cas
chological examination of traumatic encaphalo- d'encephalopathie traumatique. Archives de
pathy and the Complex Figure Copy Test. The psychologie, 28, 286-340.
Clinical Neuropsychotogist, 7 , 3-21. Stern, R. A., Singer, E. A,, Duke, L. M., Singer, N.
Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attentional and cognitive prob- G., Morey, C. E., Daughtrey, E. W., & Kaplan, E.
lems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsy- (1 994). The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for
chiafry (pp. 280-329). New York: Guilford Press. the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description
Felton, R. H., Wood, F. B., Brown, I. S., Cambell, S . and interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsy-
K., & Harter, M. R. (1987). Separate verbal mern- chologist, 8,309-322.
ory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder

S-ar putea să vă placă și