Sunteți pe pagina 1din 15

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2020, 53, 10–24 NUMBER 1 (WINTER)

The importance of multiple exemplar instruction in the


establishment of novel verbal behavior
DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE
ENDICOTT COLLEGE AND H.O.P.E. CONSULTING, LLC

JONATHAN TARBOX
UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN CALIFORNIA AND FIRSTSTEPS FOR KIDS

According to traditional linguistic accounts language, and its generative nature, cannot be taught.
From a behavior analytic perspective, language is like any other behavior; it is learned and amenable
to change. Based upon Skinner’s radical behavioral analysis of verbal behavior, specific procedures
have been designed to promote novel verbal relations. However, despite the strength and utility of this
approach, using behavioral principles to understand the generativity of language has been challenging.
Dependent upon the specific theory (e.g., stimulus equivalence, relational frame theory, bidirectional
naming) within the radical behavioral orientation, researchers arrange unique procedures to evaluate
the variables responsible for this phenomenon. This paper presents the commonalities and differences
of two procedures (i.e., multiple exemplar training, multiple exemplar instruction) with examples of
research highlighting the use of both in producing generativity. Further, it describes how multiple
exemplar instruction is independent from other procedures leading to this outcome, and concludes
by providing recommendations for both research and practice.
Key words: generativity, intervention, multiple exemplar instruction, multiple exemplar
training, verbal behavior

Traditional linguistic accounts ascribe the prov- which comprised all human language. In keeping
enance of language to innate human abilities and with a radical behavioral orientation, Skinner pos-
account for the generative nature of language as a ited that verbal behavior is like any other behavior
function of internal mechanisms or hypothetical in that it is learned via the individual’s experiences.
constructs (Chomsky, 1959; Pinker, 1994). Thus, verbal behavior can be both explained and
According to these explanations, language is changed via analyses of behavior–environment
unlike other forms of behavior, in that this capac- relations. Since Skinner’s original writing, much
ity cannot, and need not, be taught. One’s biolog- research employing behavior-analytic principles to
ical make-up prepares them to learn language investigate the development and remediation of
(Pinker, 1994). The concerning implication is language has been published (e.g., Akers et al.,
that language may not be ameliorated through 2019; Carr & Miguel, 2013; Frampton &
intervention (LaFrance & Miguel, 2014). In Shillingsburg, 2018; Marcon-Dawson, Vicars, &
1957, B. F. Skinner published a behavior-analytic Miguel, 2009; Oah & Dickinson, 1989;
account refuting this notion. He proposed a tax- Sautter & LeBlanc, 2006; Shafer, 1995; Stauch,
onomy of analytic units (i.e., verbal operants) LaLonde, Plavnik, Bak, & Gatewood, 2017;
Wiskow, Matter, & Donaldson, 2018). Taken
together, the results of these studies contradict the
The authors would like to thank Caio Miguel for his idea of language as an innate and immoveable
suggestions on a previous version of this manuscript. entity.
Address correspondence to: Danielle LaFrance, Despite the strength and utility of Skinner’s
H.O.P.E. Consulting, LLC., 7949 California Avenue, Fair
Oaks, CA 95628 (email: dlafrance@hopeconsulting.net) analysis of verbal behavior, some may argue that
doi: 10.1002/jaba.611 using behavioral principles to understand the
© 2019 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
10
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 11

inherent complexity of an adult’s language has referred to as bidirectional naming or BiN


been challenging. This complexity, in part, has to (Miguel, 2016). Each approach also has its own
do with the generativity of language (Törneke, methodological traditions. That is, a researcher’s
2010). That is, typically developing individuals framework (i.e., stimulus equivalence, RFT,
often demonstrate novel, and advanced BiN), determines the sets of procedures used to
(e.g., metaphors, analogies) forms of language evaluate the variables responsible for generativity.
without the need for specific teaching. This capac- In stimulus equivalence, research predominantly
ity to demonstrate novel, untaught, and advanced relies upon matching-to-sample (MTS) task prepa-
response forms is a critical feature defining mature rations, in which several sample (i.e., conditional)
language repertoires. However, explanations of and comparison (i.e., discriminative) stimuli are
this repertoire appealing only to stimulus control, presented according to a four-term contingency.
generalization, and motivating operations This strategy has been shown to lead to the devel-
(Skinner, 1957) may be unsatisfactory opment of classes whose members share the same
(e.g., Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). function (e.g., Dougher, Augustson, Markham,
For the purposes of this paper, generativity is Greenway, & Wulfert, 1994; Griffith, Ramos,
used as an umbrella term referring to demonstra- Hill, & Miguel, 2018; Lillie & Tiger, 2019;
tions of any novel responses, including stimulus Saunders, Drake, & Spradlin, 1999; Sidman &
and response generalization, equivalence, and Tailby, 1982). In RFT, the establishment of rela-
emerging operants (i.e., not directly trained). In tional frames and the genesis of derived behavior is
contrast, the term generalization is used to refer to typically examined via an arranged history of multi-
instances in which either previously trained ple exemplar training (MET; e.g., Baltruschat
responses are occasioned by stimuli differing from et al., 2011; Baltruschat et al., 2012; Berens &
those in training (i.e., stimulus generalization), or Hayes, 2007; Gould, Tarbox, O’Hora, Noone, &
instances in which slight variations in response Bergstrom, 2011; Luciano, Becerra, & Valverde,
topography are occasioned by similar stimulus 2007; Najdowski, Bergstrom, Tarbox, & St. Clair,
conditions (i.e., response generalization). Equiva- 2017; Najdowski, St. Clair, Fullen, Child, Per-
lence is used to refer to demonstrations of new sicke, & Tarbox, 2018; Persicke, Tarbox,
behavioral relations following training of other Ranick, & St. Clair, 2012; Sivaraman, 2017; Tar-
relations (e.g., matching A to C after being taught box, Zuckerman, Bishop, Olive, & O’Hora,
to match A to B and A to C), and emergence is 2011). Thus, multiple exemplar training is utilized
used to refer to the demonstration of new response within both the stimulus equivalence and RFT
forms in stimulus conditions in which other approaches. When studying BiN (Miguel, 2016),
response forms have been taught (e.g., tacting or researchers used multiple exemplar instruction
manding following echoic training). (MEI; e.g., Arntzen & Almas, 2002; Carroll &
Behavior-analytic researchers have begun con- Hesse, 1987; Eby, Greer, Tullo, Baker, & Pauly,
ducting investigations examining possible mecha- 2010; Greer, Stolfi, Chavez-Brown, & Rivera-
nisms for language generativity (e.g., Miguel, Valdes, 2005; Greer, Stolfi, & Pistoljevic, 2007;
2016, 2018; Miguel et al., 2015; Ming & Stewart, Olaff, Ona, & Holth, 2017; Speckman, Greer, &
2017; Moran, Walsh, Stewart, McElwee, & Rivera-Valdes, 2012) to produce interdependence
Ming, 2015; Stewart, McElwee, & Ming, 2013). between speaker and listener repertoires, or
Much of the work done on this topic is based between verbal operants within the same reper-
upon one of three distinct theoretical frameworks: toire. What follows is a brief description of both
stimulus equivalence (Sidman, 1971; 2000), rela- MET and MEI and a short review of some of the
tional frame theory (RFT; Hayes et al., 2001), or research highlighting the use of both in producing
naming (Horne & Lowe, 1996; 1997) also generativity. A discussion of MEI’s independence
12 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

from MET, as well as the implications for MEI’s the presentation of an array of several photo-
use is also included. In the hopes of encouraging graphs, with a picture of a dog among them. Fur-
clarity and accuracy, recommendations for both ther, a child may be asked to point to a dog
research and practice are provided. illustrated upon a page in a book. Thus, behaviors
acquired through MET are described as responses
that have come under the control of “stimulus fea-
PROCEDURAL AND TERMINOLOGICAL
tures present in the exemplars used during
DISTINCTIONS
training,” which are also evoked by subsequent
Clear distinctions in terminology are necessary presentations of “novel stimuli that share these
to better understand the extant literature on MET features” (Marzullo-Kerth, Reeve, Reeve, &
and MEI. These distinctions are proffered not Townsend, 2011, p. 280). Novel behavior
only to assist readers in interpreting the studies (or generativity) may then be based upon stimulus
discussed below, but also in a broader sense. More generalization, and potentially enhanced or lim-
specifically, although the terms MET and MEI ited by the extent to which stimuli resemble one
are used interchangeably at times (e.g., Byrne, another (Catania, 2013; Jenkins & Harrison,
Rehfeldt, & Aguirre, 2014; Greer & Ross, 2008), 1960; Young, Krantz, McClannahan, & Poulson,
they refer to two different procedures with distinct 1994). After acquiring the tact “dog” in the pres-
parameters and outcomes. ence of a Golden Labrador, a child may, for
instance, also tact similar dogs (e.g., Chocolate
MET and Black Labradors, Golden Retrievers). In sum-
Most interventions referred to as MET include mary, MET typically consists of the presentation
the presentation of several different exemplars of of different exemplars (Sprague & Horner, 1984)
the same stimulus (Stokes & Baer, 1977). For to a learner whose target response topography typ-
example, when teaching the tact, ‘dog,’ numerous ically remains constant, and training continues
different pictures of dogs are presented to ensure until probes reveal that generalization to
responding comes under the control of all appro- untrained stimuli has occurred.
priate stimuli within that class (Skinner, 1957). Alternatively, MET may also be utilized to
Stimuli presented in this context may be highly teach response generalization (Cooper, Heron, &
(e.g., a Chihuahua, a Boxer, and a Newfound- Heward, 2007; Holth, 2017). In this type of
land) or minimally (e.g., a Husky, an Akita, and a application, discriminative stimuli may remain
German Shepard) discriminable from one the same, while the response topography differs
another, may range in shape, size, and color, may slightly across consecutive trials. When teaching
depict dogs in different positions or orientations intraverbals related to personal information, for
(e.g., laying, sitting, standing), and may have dif- example, several different response topographies
ferent backgrounds (e.g., blank white back- may be appropriate. When teaching a child to
grounds and more natural contexts). Additionally, respond to the question, “what’s your name” there
MET can involve the presentation of stimuli are some different response options, with varia-
depicting the same teaching target in different for- tions in length and structure, that are likely appro-
mats. These stimuli may range from the object priate (e.g., responding with just one’s name,
itself to pictures or abstract line drawings of the responding with a more complete sentence like
object. For instance, if teaching a child to respond “My name is ___”). It should be noted, however,
to the auditory stimulus “dog” as a listener, one that each different response topography serves the
may point to the dog and say “dog” in the pres- same function. That is, although several different
ence of the animal. Other teaching opportunities, response options may produce reinforcement, they
or trials, targeting the same response may entail all belong to the same operant class. In this
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 13

example, each response consists of an intraverbal, verbal antecedent. The delivery of generalized
evoked by a prior verbal stimulus, and maintain- conditioned reinforcement would follow all
ed by generalized conditioned reinforcement correct tacts and echoics. During mand trials,
(Skinner, 1957). This is an important point, and the teacher ensures an appropriate motivating
one that will be revisited in the description of operation (MO; Laraway, Snycerski, Michael, &
MEI below. Thus, MET may be more accurately Poling, 2003; Michael & Miguel, 2020) is in
described as a teaching procedure used to system- place, likely via some sort of deprivation proce-
atically program for generalization (stimulus or dure, and a correct response results in the man-
response) that exemplifies the concept of ‘training ded item.
sufficient exemplars’ as discussed in Stokes and Generally speaking, procedures referred to as
Baer’s (1977) seminal paper. MEI involve “a special kind of task interspersal of
various novel and acquired exemplars across verbal
operant categories … In this arrangement, the
MEI same target is taught simultaneously as two or
Most procedures referred to as MEI in the liter- more different verbal operants” (Sidener et al.,
ature include rapidly and randomly rotating 2010, p.15). The distinguishing feature of MEI in
instructions targeting different verbal operants comparison to MET is that the function of
across a series of consecutive trials. For example, responses across consecutive trials differs. MEI
when teaching a child to respond to the stimulus procedures consist of the rotation of different ver-
‘dog,’ consecutive trials within a block may target bal operants with different functions (e.g., match,
selection responses (i.e., listener behavior1), tacts, point to, tact, multiply controlled tact) across a
echoics, and mands (i.e., speaker behavior) in series of consecutive trials (Greer, Stolfi, et al.,
quick succession. Antecedents and consequences 2005). Often, teaching continues across new sets
are arranged according to Skinner’s (1957) taxon- of stimuli until probe data reveal that teaching
omy such that they come to evoke the specific ver- only one operant produces untrained operants for
bal operants to which they correspond. During a that same response form (e.g., teaching a listener
trial targeting a selection response, for example, an response, such as pointing, results in untrained
array of pictures which includes a picture of a dog speaker behavior, such as a tact).
may be presented. The child may then be
instructed to “find dog,” and a correct selection
results in some form of generalized conditioned APPLICATION AND SIGNIFICANCE
reinforcement, such as praise or a token. During a OF MET
tact trial, the correct vocalization “dog” is As mentioned above, MET has been utilized to
occasioned by the presentation of a nonverbal teach both stimulus and response generalization.
stimulus (i.e., the picture of the dog); whereas Studies targeting stimulus generalization follow-
during an echoic trial, the correct vocalization ing MET have evaluated variations in stimulus
“dog” is occasioned by a teacher saying “dog.” In conditions including changes to physical stimulus
this case, the learner’s correct vocal response must properties (e.g., objects versus pictures), changes
have point-to-point correspondence with the in behavior change agents (e.g., parents, teachers,
siblings, peers), and changes in settings or envi-
1
Listener behavior is not defined as a verbal operant in ronments (e.g., home, school, community). For
Verbal Behavior (Skinner, 1957), and it is not suggested example, Parsonson and Baer (1978) evaluated
that readers begin to refer to listener behavior as such. demonstrations of generalized problem solving
Any reference to listener behavior as a verbal operant in
the current paper is purely for ease of reference and across five young children. Three different activi-
discussion. ties (i.e., hammering, storing items in containers,
14 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

and lacing) were presented to participants. In a multiple baseline design across participants. Base-
baseline, the experimenters provided specific line consisted of probing the participants’ profi-
items for each activity (e.g., a hammer with a tool ciency with vending machine usage. Following
bench, laces with a lace board), along with a baseline, three of the six participants were exposed
description and model of appropriate engagement to the single-instance condition followed by the
(e.g., using the hammer to drive pegs down into multiple-instance condition. For the remaining
the tool bench). Correct participant engagement three participants, baseline was followed by single-
resulted in praise. Then, a generalization probe instance, multiple-instance, and then general-case
was conducted in which the same activities were training. Across all participants, general-case train-
presented, but the specific items associated with ing yielded the highest number of correct
each activity (e.g., the hammer) were removed. responses during generalization probes.
Instead, the experimenters provided participants Overall, the body of literature shows that MET
with other items that could be used as substitutes can be implemented to produce generalization of
(e.g., a cotton spool instead of a hammer). Five to important skills such as imitation (e.g., Garcia,
seven items were probed for each activity. Follow- Baer, & Firestone, 1971), mands (e.g., Najdowski,
ing the probes, the participants were trained to Bergstrom, et al., 2017), conversation (e.g., Garcia-
use the substitute items to engage in each activity. Albea, Reeve, Reeve, & Brothers, 2014; Park &
The substitute items varied across sessions. Gaylord-Ross, 1989), vocational skills (e.g., Horner
Finally, follow-up probes were conducted to assess & McDonald, 1982), sharing (e.g., Marzullo-
for maintenance. Results showed that all partici- Kerth et al., 2011), and helping (e.g., Reeve, Reeve,
pants engaged in little to no problem-solving dur- Townsend, & Poulson, 2007). Moreover, multiple
ing baseline, that training produced generalized exemplars have included several different variations
problem-solving within each activity, and that of stimuli/stimulus conditions, such as peers
trained improvisation responses maintained at fol- (e.g., Hughes, Harmer, Killian, & Niarhos, 1995;
low up. Interestingly, however, problem-solving Najdowski et al., 2018), settings (e.g., Miller &
did not generalize across different activities. Sloane, 1976; Miltenberger et al., 2013), and appa-
Despite this limitation, the results of this study ratuses (e.g., Parsonson & Baer, 1978; Sprague &
support the notion that programming for multiple Horner, 1984). Although a full review of the
stimulus exemplars can lead to the generalization behavior-analytic literature on MET falls outside
of important skills. the scope of the current paper, the sheer volume of
Studies on MET targeting response generaliza- empirical work, as well as the robustness of the
tion teach variations in response topographies, findings, show that MET can be successfully
often in combination with the presentation of sev- applied to produce generativity in the form of
eral different stimulus exemplars. For example, either stimulus or response generalization. Use of
Sprague and Horner (1984) evaluated the effec- teaching procedures that incorporate both stimulus
tiveness of different parameters to teach the gener- and response generalization may be the most effi-
alized use of vending machines to students with cient and effective way to employ MET.
disabilities. Students were taught to use a vending
machine in one of three conditions: a) the use of a
single vending machine (i.e., single instance), b) APPLICATION AND SIGNIFICANCE
the use of three different, albeit similar vending OF MEI
machines (i.e., multiple instances), and c) the use Broadly speaking, the literature investigating
of three different vending machines that sampled the effects of MEI can be categorized into two
the full range of both stimulus and response exem- subtypes: 1) studies aimed at establishing
plars required to obtain items (i.e., general case) via bidirectionality between speaker and listener
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 15

repertoires, and 2) studies aimed at examining 1 stimuli; 3) emergence probes for all other
the utility of MEI in the development of more response types for Set 1 stimuli; 4) MEI for Set
complex forms of generativity (e.g., taking dic- 2 stimuli; 5) post-MEI probes for all untaught
tation and intraverbal spelling) by establishing response types for Set 1 stimuli; 6) matching
interdependence between verbal operants. instruction for Set 3 stimuli; and 7) emergence
Interpretation of this range of complexity is probes of all other response types for Set 3 stim-
perhaps best described as a continuum, with uli. Results showed that after matching training
the establishment of prerequisites (see Greer with one set of stimuli, participants demon-
et al., 2007) for bidirectionality between strated a higher number of correct responses in
speaker and listener repertoires lying at one end listener trials (i.e., point to). Interestingly, it
(i.e., its simplest level), and the emergence of was not until participants received MEI train-
more sophisticated operants (see Eby et al., ing with one set of stimuli that improved accu-
2010, Luke, Greer, Singer-Dudek, & Keohane, racy was observed in speaker trials (i.e., tacts,
2011, and Speckman et al., 2012) lying at its multiply controlled tacts). Following MEI, the
opposite end (i.e., its most complex). A brief researchers observed a high number of correct
review of both research subtypes is provided responses across all response types (i.e., both
below. speaker and listener behavior) for the other two
untrained sets of stimuli.
Several additional studies examining the effects
MEI to Establish Bidirectionality of MEI on BiN have been conducted. All such
In one of the first demonstrations of its kind, investigations have used procedures similar to
Greer, Stolfi, et al. (2005) sought to determine Greer, Stolfi, et al. (2005). Notably, these results
whether MEI would result in transfer between have been replicated across children with different
listener and speaker repertoires with three par- disabilities (e.g., learning delays and developmen-
ticipants with language and developmental tal disabilities, Greer et al., 2007; autism, Fiorile &
delays. Participants did not demonstrate Greer, 2007; Olaff et al., 2017), typically develop-
responding characteristic of BiN (i.e., emitting ing children (Gilic & Greer, 2011), very young
speaker behavior after having learned listener (i.e., 2-year-old) children (Gilic & Greer, 2011),
behavior, or vice versa) prior to the start of the children with varying numbers of prerequisite
study. Dependent measures consisted of correct BiN skills (e.g., echoics, tacts, and listener behav-
visual–visual matches, selections (i.e., pointing ior), and children with extremely limited vocal–
to the correct stimulus in the presence of its verbal repertoires (Fiorile & Greer, 2007).
dictated name), tacts, and multiply controlled Despite different procedural manipulations such
tacts (i.e., tacts when shown a picture and as echoic training during MTS instruction
asked, “What is this?”). The authors collected (Cao & Greer, 2018; Hawkins, Kingsdorf, Char-
data on the dependent variables across three nock, Szabo, & Gautreaux, 2009; Olaff et al.,
sets of five stimuli (for a total of 15 stimuli), in 2017) or matching without echoic training
20-trial blocks (for a total of four different (Gilic & Greer, 2011), results show that BiN did
response types for each stimulus). Target not emerge until after MEI was implemented.
responses were measured across the following Studies have also compared MEI to single-
phases of a multiple baseline design across stim- exemplar instruction (SEI; e.g., Greer et al.,
ulus sets and participants: 1) pre-experimental 2007), which consists of targeting a single verbal
probes for all response types (i.e., matching, operant at a time and teaching other verbal oper-
pointing to, tacts, and impure tacts) across all ants separately and sequentially via a series of sim-
stimuli; 2) matching instruction for Set ple discrimination contingencies (Green, 2001).
16 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

Similar to previous comparisons, participants the emergence of adjective–object mands


exposed to SEI did not demonstrate BiN until (e.g., “I want a red car”) and tacts (e.g., “It’s a
they were also exposed to MEI. Moreover, replica- red car”) via a multiple-probe design across par-
tions have shown that many commonly used ticipants. Experimental conditions were similar
teaching components (e.g., MTS, echoic training, to those conducted in previous studies
vocal modeling) do not produce BiN in isolation (e.g., Fiorile & Greer, 2007; Greer, Stolfi,
(Cahill & Greer, 2014). In contrast, these studies et al., 2005), including: 1) pre-experimental
demonstrate that MEI reliably leads to the devel- probes of adjective–object mands and tacts; 2)
opment of BiN. counterbalanced training of mands and tacts
Taken together, the results of the aforemen- (i.e., teaching either adjective–object mands or
tioned studies (Cahill & Greer, 2014; Fiorile & adjective–object tacts); 3) probes of emergent
Greer, 2007; Gilic & Greer, 2011; Greer, Stolfi, adjective–object mands or tacts (for the previ-
et al, 2005; Greer et al., 2007; Olaff et al., 2017) ously untrained operant). This sequence was
suggest that MEI can be used to effectively establish repeated across three sets of stimuli. Results
functional interdependence between speaker and showed that all four participants demonstrated
listener repertoires. Most notably, these studies untaught adjective–object mands and tacts only
show that the prerequisite skills required for BiN following MEI, but never following SEI. Addi-
(i.e., listener behavior, echoics, and tacts) can be tionally, all participants demonstrated untaught
taught to children with even very limited vocal– adjective–object mands and tacts in probes with
verbal repertoires, and that a subsequent two additional stimulus sets after MEI was
programmed history of MEI can lead to the devel- implemented with the first stimulus set. These
opment of this critical cusp (Bosch & Fuqua, results point to MEI’s effectiveness in teaching
2001; Rosales-Ruiz & Baer, 1997). These findings complex skills. That is, unlike studies using
have important implications for intervention pro- MEI to establish BiN, participants in the pre-
grams teaching language, as BiN has been posited sent study demonstrated skill transfer across
as a necessary prerequisite for the acquisition of two different verbal operants (mands and tacts),
additional language, as well as a foundational build- with an autoclitic frame (i.e., a sentence).
ing block for the subsequent development of more Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer’s (2004) findings
complex skills, such as categorization, problem- have been replicated across participants of dif-
solving, and analogical reasoning (Carp & Pet- ferent ages (e.g., 3 to 7 years of age), with typi-
ursdottir, 2015; Jennings & Miguel, 2017; Kobari- cally and atypically developing children (Luke,
Wright & Miguel, 2014; Lee, Miguel, Darcey, & et al., 2011), children with autism (Nuzzolo-
Jennings, 2015; Lowe, Horne, & Hughes, 2005; Gomez & Greer, 2004; Eby et al., 2010), bilin-
Ma, Miguel, & Jennings, 2016; Miguel et al., gual children (Luke et al., 2011), and across
2015; Miguel & Kobari-Wright, 2013; Miguel, verbal topographies (e.g., taking dictation and
Petursdottir, Carr, & Michael, 2008; Sprinkle & vocal spelling, Eby et al., 2010; Greer, Yuan &
Miguel, 2013). Gautreaux, 2005; regular and irregular past
tense verbs, Greer & Yuan, 2008; noun and
adjective forms containing suffixes ending in
Moving Beyond Bidirectionality ‘-er’, Speckman et al., 2012). Further compari-
Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004) were the sons of the differential effectiveness of SEI and
first to show that MEI can be used to extend MEI (Eby et al., 2010; Greer & Yuan, 2008;
the bidirectionality between speaker and lis- Speckman et al., 2012) are consistent with pre-
tener repertoires to more complex responses. vious research, showing that emergence was not
The authors evaluated the effects of MEI on observed until participants received MEI.
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 17

Collectively, the literature suggests that MEI be limited by the extent to which the training stim-
can be used to develop bidirectionality between uli resemble stimuli in novel contexts. Although
speaker and listener repertoires, to teach children this outcome is meaningful, its potential for lead-
complex language skills, and to establish novel ing to additional generativity remains limited. In
verbal behavior without explicit teaching contrast with MET, the mechanisms responsible
(i.e., taking dictation, intraverbal spelling, and for the effects of MEI are not well understood,
autoclitic frames containing prepositions and though the authors cited in this review do suggest
comparatives). As bidirectionality between speaker a few potentials (e.g., transformation of function,
and listener repertoires is merely a prerequisite for abstract stimulus control, joint stimulus control,
the development of an adequate vocal–verbal and the transformation of establishing operations).
repertoire and speaking “with understanding” If MEI achieves its effects due to the establishment
(Miguel, 2016, p.129), MEI may have additional of abstract (Becker, 1992) or joint stimulus con-
utility in research and practice. However, more trol2 (Greer, Stolfi, et al., 2005), MEI’s potential
research will need to be conducted before pre- for producing generativity is exponentially greater
requisites necessary to produce the generativity than that of MET. Whereas MET varies exemplars
characteristic of adult language repertoires (e.g., or responses and produces generalization within
intraverbal bidirectional naming, Jennings & the same operant, MEI varies antecedents and con-
Miguel, 2017) can be attained. sequences in a manner that appears to lead to bidi-
rectional relations across speaker and listener
repertoires as well as verbal operants. This is
Recommendations for Research important from a perspective of “learning to
With few exceptions (i.e., Arntzen & Almas, learn,” as the structured opportunities to rapidly
2002; Carroll & Hesse, 1987; Lechago, Carr, respond to different stimuli and MOs in MEI
Kisamore, & Grow, 2015; Olaff et al., 2017; could help to establish multiple control over
Sidener et al., 2010), the vast majority of studies responding (Skinner, 1957). The multiple control
published demonstrating the effectiveness of MEI of verbal operants has long been recognized as
in producing functional interdependence origi- essential for developing more complex skills
nate from the same group of researchers (i.e., Eby (Michael, Palmer, & Sundberg, 2011; Miguel,
et al., 2010; Fiorile & Greer, 2007; Greer & 2016), and is similarly necessary when developing
Longano, 2010; Greer, Stolfi, et al., 2005; Greer complex verbal repertoires. However, to date,
et al., 2007; Greer & Yuan, 2008; Greer, Yuan, there are no empirical evaluations of MEI’s under-
et al., 2005; Hawkins et al., 2009; Nuzzolo- lying mechanisms. Future studies examining this
Gomez & Greer, 2004; Speckman et al., 2012). It question would lead to a greater understanding of
should be noted that some investigations con- MEI’s effects, and could potentially provide clarity
ducted by independent research laboratories have regarding the application of this procedure, to
failed to replicate the outcomes described above optimize learning outcomes in practice.
(e.g., Lechago et al., 2015; Sidener et al., 2010). Although a history of training with both MET
Additional independent replications on the effects and MEI can lead to generativity, this outcome is
of MEI are warranted to better understand the achieved in different ways. The main differences
strengths of this procedure. are 1) procedural arrangements and 2) the
Generalization appears to be the behavioral
2
mechanism responsible for the effects of MET. If In Greer, Stolfi, et al., 2005, joint stimulus control is
this is the case, MET leads to just that— referred to as instances in which either 1) a single stimulus
comes to exert control jointly over several different
appropriate responding to untrained stimuli in responses, or 2) a single response comes under the control
novel conditions. This responding, however, may of several different establishing operations.
18 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

outcomes of variation. Therefore, as one might Finally, the effects of other types of procedural
expect, the type of generativity observed and the parameters (e.g., prompts, schedules of reinforce-
extent to which generativity occurs differ with ment) on MET and MEI, when combined in
each procedure. Research focused on the specific teaching packages, remains largely unexplored.
parameters of each procedure (e.g., teaching single Studies seeking to answer these types of questions
stimulus exemplars using MEI, teaching multiple could produce some indication as to when indi-
exemplars combined with MEI) and the resulting vidual learners would benefit from each sequence
outcomes (i.e., BiN, transfer across operants, or combination of teaching procedures, as well as
stimulus generalization) is necessary. If, for which teaching strategies enhance or hinder
instance, stimulus generalization is observed to generativity.
improve following the use of MEI, this may be
the preferred teaching procedure as it leads to two
(versus one) desired outcomes (i.e., both BiN and RECOMMENDATIONS FOR PRACTICE
stimulus generalization). The results of such inves- Despite the need for further research, using
tigations would help to refine our understanding MEI as an intervention to produce generativity is
of the necessary components of each procedure promising. Existing research suggests that the
and potentially help to streamline teaching by greatest benefit of MEI lies in its ability to pro-
maximizing efficiency. mote functional interdependence (Skinner, 1957)
It remains unclear how much exposure to MEI between speaker and listener repertoires and
is required before generative verbal behavior between verbal operants (Gamba, Goyos, & Pet-
becomes a generalized operant. Individual partici- ursdottir, 2015). This has significant implications
pant differences seem to determine the amount of for treatment programs targeting language delays
explicit teaching required for a successful outcome and is particularly appealing for behavior analysts
(e.g., Fiorile & Greer, 2007; Greer & Longano; charged with delivering necessary intervention to
2010; Greer, Stolfi, et al., 2005; Olaff et al., 2017). children with developmental disabilities. Time
For instance, one of the three participants in Olaff spent in intensive intervention is finite. Therefore,
et al. (2017) required MEI with two sets of stimuli teaching strategies that optimize learning and
prior to demonstrating emergence of both speaker quickly develop skill repertoires which allow
and listener responses at criterion with a third and learners to respond to natural contingencies and
final set of stimuli. A second participant was taught continue to learn in these contexts absent of sup-
two sets of stimuli using MEI, but did not meet cri- portive services are needed (LaFrance &
terion in naming probes with a third and final set Miguel, 2014).
of stimuli, and a third participant never met criterion In the early stages of language development, it
regardless of the number of sets taught in MEI. Dif- has been observed that individuals use and respond
ferent skills also seem to require different amounts of to language in a manner consistent with the func-
exposure to MEI before generativity is demonstrated tional independence of verbal operants (Hall &
(Eby et al., 2010). Similarly, different procedural Sundberg, 1987; Horne & Lowe, 1996;
variations of MEI appear to produce different Lamarre & Holland, 1985; Nuzzolo-Gomez &
degrees of complexity in emergent responses. In fact, Greer, 2004; Skinner, 1957). Otherwise stated,
Lechago et al. (2015) concluded that their partici- emitting a response as one verbal operant
pants’ failures to demonstrate novel response forms (e.g., saying ‘water’ as a mand) will not guarantee
following MEI may have been due to the complexity the correct emission of that same response under
of the response required (i.e., intraverbal categoriza- different stimulus conditions (e.g., saying ‘water’
tion) following training of relatively simple response as a tact). Over time, however, typically developing
forms (selection and tacts). individuals learn to respond appropriately in a
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 19

variety of contexts, and emit the same response intervention pertains to generalization, MET
topography across a multitude of stimulus condi- may be the procedure of choice. Until further
tions without additional explicit teaching. This research investigating the aforementioned ques-
suggests that at some point, functional tions is published, it should not be assumed
interdependence between verbal operants that either procedure will lead to both func-
develops, allowing the individual to respond in tional interdependence and generalization.
novel (i.e., generative), appropriate, and adaptive A compounding factor is related to the use
ways to his or her environment. BiN may be the of the terms MET and MEI in the current
behavioral cusp (Rosales-Ruiz & Baer, 1997) behavior-analytic literature. More specifically,
responsible for this new flexibility in responding the term MEI has been found to appear in arti-
(Greer, Pohl, Du, & Moschella, 2017), and the cles describing procedures in alignment with
genesis of early generative verbal behavior. This, in MET, and vice versa (e.g., Byrne et al., 2014).
turn, may be key to the development of incidental This particular issue will likely not be resolved
learning, which is necessary for a complete lan- until such a time that a terminological change
guage repertoire. Thus, BiN is worthy of special is proposed for one or both terms and is subse-
attention, and should occupy an important place quently adopted by the behavior-analytic com-
in language intervention. The use of procedures munity. This is an onerous task, as changing
such as MEI may assist in developing BiN, leading the behavior-analytic vernacular once terms are
to incidental learning, more complex language already widely in use can be a lengthy process
(Greer et al., 2017), and effective remediation of (although, see Laraway et al., 2003, and
language delays. Miguel, 2016 for examples).
The place that MEI holds in relation to One final recommendation is a cautionary
other similar procedures (e.g., SEI, discrimina- note. Absent a terminological change, behavior
tion training, MET) is worthy of discussion. analysts need not only be precise in the ways
When contrasting MET and MEI procedures they utilize MET and MEI in intervention, but
more specifically, some important trends with also in the way they speak about, and train
implications for practice emerge. As each proce- others in the use of these procedures. As a par-
dure may lead to generativity to varying degrees ticular field’s language bears upon the cultural
(Lechago et al., 2015), the question of when to practices of its scientists (Chiesa, 1994; Day,
use a specific procedure arises. For example, 1983), this point cannot be emphasized
multiple experiences with different exemplars enough. The verbal community directly influ-
of the same stimuli, as is characteristic of ences the way science is conceptualized, which
MET, likely leads to a form of stimulus gener- in turn, affects experimental analysis and appli-
alization, with each response producing a com- cation. After all, “a particularly desirable conse-
mon consequence (i.e., serving the same quence of this [verbal] control occurs when
function). MEI, on the other hand, consists of people are equipped by it with new behavior
the training of multiple operants with respect connected with practices of research which can
to the same stimuli, and other operants. Given then be brought to bear upon an analysis of the
the current body of evidence related to the environmental control of behavior” (Day,
application and outcomes of each procedure, 1983, p. 100). Precision with respect to the use
we recommend that practitioners attempting to of these terms may result in more careful analy-
produce either functional interdependence sis and could help sharpen our understanding
between speaker and listener repertoires or of generativity resulting from these procedures,
between verbal operants consider using MEI to lead to more effective application, and optimize
achieve this outcome. If, however, an language intervention. Perhaps, for now, the
20 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

most conservative approach is to reserve the this front may in turn also lead to a more com-
term MEI only for procedures aimed at teach- plete account of generativity, and a science of
ing functional interdependence by rotating behavior better prepared to confront its critics.
between trials focusing on speaker behavior, lis-
tener behavior, or the emission of distinct ver-
bal operants, while reserving the term MET REFERENCES
only for procedures aimed at improving stimu- Akers, J. S., Retzlaff, B. J., Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D.,
lus or response generalization through the sys- Kaminski, A. J., & DeSouza, A. (2019). An evalua-
tion of conditional manding using a four-component
tematic modification of stimuli or responses. multiple schedule. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
35, 94–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-018-
0099-9.
CONCLUSION Arntzen, W., & Almas, I. K. (2002). Effects of mand-tact
versus tact-only training on the acquisition of tacts.
As a whole, the science of behavior analysis Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 419–422.
has received criticism for its inability to account https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-419.
for the generative nature of language Baltruschat, L., Hasselhorn, M., Tarbox, J.,
Dixon, D. R., Najdowski, A. C., Mullins, R. D., &
(e.g., Chomsky, 1959; Hayes et al., 2001; Gould, E. (2011). Addressing working memory in
Törneke, 2010). If behavior analysis is to children with autism through behavioral intervention.
become a more mainstream science, ubiqui- Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 267–276.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.008.
tously recognized and respected, it must
Baltruschat, L., Hasselhorn, M., Tarbox, J.,
develop a complete explanation of all behavior. Dixon, D. R., Najdowski, A., Mullins, R. D., &
To do so, the generativity of language so often Gould, E. (2012). The effects of multiple exemplar
attributed by linguists and psychologists alike training on a working memory task involving sequen-
tial responding in children with autism. Psychological
(Chomsky, 1959; Pinker, 1994) to innate Record, 62, 549–562. https://doi.org/10.1007/
abilities and hypothetical constructs, must be BF03395820.
explained via the individual’s learning history. Becker, W. (1992). Direct instruction: A twenty-year
review. In R. West & L. Hamerlynck (Eds.), Design
BiN may be the most basic skill needed for educational excellence: The legacy of B.F. Skinner
for this characteristic of language (Greer & (pp. 71–112). Sopris West: Longmont, CO.
Longano, 2010; Miguel, 2016; 2018), and Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable
MEI may represent an empirically validated comparative relations: Experimental evidence for a rela-
tional operant. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40,
demonstration of how behavior–environment 45–71. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.7-06.
interactions can be arranged to produce gener- Bosch, S., & Fuqua, R. W. (2001). Behavioral cusps: A
ativity in language, especially for those lacking model for selecting target behaviors. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 123–125. https://doi.
it. The distinctions made above, as well as the org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-123.
brief review of the literature and discussion Byrne, B. L., Rehfeldt, R., & Aguirre, A. A. (2014). Eval-
related to each procedure is proffered in the uating the effectiveness of the stimulus pairing obser-
hopes of more clearly distinguishing MEI from vation procedure and multiple exemplar instruction
on tact and listener responses in children with autism.
other procedures sharing common terms with The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 30, 160–169. https://
it (i.e., MET), stimulating additional research doi.org/10.1007/s40616-014-0020-0.
on this topic, and providing clarifications Cahill, C. S., & Greer, R. D. (2014). Actions vs. words:
How we can learn both. Acta de Investigación
related to practice. Further investigations criti- Psicolôgica, 4, 1716–1745. https://doi.org/10.1016/
cally evaluating this procedure may very well S2007-4719(14)70976-7.
lead to an understanding of the mechanisms Cao, Y., & Greer, R. D. (2018). Mastery of echoics in
by which MEI produces its effects, as well as Chinese establishes bidirectional naming in Chinese
for preschoolers with naming in English. The Analysis
how it is related to different levels of simple of Verbal Behavior, 34, 79–99. https://doi.org/10.
and complex responding. Contributions on 1007/s40616-018-0106-1.
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 21

Carp, C. L., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2015). Intraverbal Garcia-Albea, E., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., &
naming and equivalence class formation in children. Brothers, K. J. (2014). Using audio script fading and
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 104, multiple-exemplar training to increase vocal interac-
223–240. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.183. tions in children with autism. Journal of Applied
Carr, J. E., & Miguel, C. F. (2013). The analysis of verbal Behavior Analysis, 47, 325–343. https://doi.org/10.
behavior and its therapeutic applications. In 1002/jaba.125.
G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis. Gilic, L., & Greer, R. D. (2011). Establishing naming in
Volume 2. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological typically developing two-year-old children as a func-
Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13938-013. tion of multiple exemplar speaker and listener experi-
Carroll, R. J., & Hesse, B. E. (1987). The effects of alter- ences. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 157–177.
nating mand and tact training on the acquisition of https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393099.
tacts. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 5, 55–65. Gould, E., Tarbox, J., O’Hora, D., Noone, S., &
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392820. Bergstrom, R. (2011). Teaching children with autism a
Catania, A. C. (2013). Learning (5th ed.). Cornwall-on- basic component skill of perspective-taking. Behavioral
Hudson, NY: Sloan Publishing. Interventions, 26, 50–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Chiesa, M. (1994). Radical behaviorism: The philosophy and bin.320.
the science. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc. Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B.F. Skinner’s Verbal learners with autism: Advances in stimulus control
Behavior. Language, 35, 27–58. technology. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Disabilities, 16, 72–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Applied Behavior Analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle 108835760101600203.
River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc. Greer, R. D., & Longano, J. (2010). A rose by naming:
Day, W. (1983). On the difference between radical and How we may learn how to do it. The Analysis of Ver-
methodological behaviorism. Behaviorism, 11, bal Behavior, 26, 73–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/
89–102. Retrieved from. http://www.jstor.org/stable/ BF03393085.
27759016. Greer, R. D., Pohl, P., Du, L., & Moschella, J. L.
Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E. M., Markham, M. R., (2017). The separate development of children’s lis-
Greenway, D. E., & Wulfert, E. (1994). The transfer tener and speaker behavior and the intercept as
of respondent eliciting and extinction functions behavioral metamorphosis. Journal of Behavioral and
through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Brain Science, 7, 674–704. https://doi.org/10.4236/
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 331–351. jbbs.2017.713045.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1994.62-331. Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior anal-
Eby, C. M., Greer, R. D., Tullo, L. D., Baker, K. A., & ysis: Inducing and expanding new verbal capabilities in
Pauly, R. (2010). Effects of multiple exemplar children with language delays. New York, NY:
instruction on transformation of stimulus function Pearson.
across written and vocal spelling responses by stu- Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-
dents with autism. The Journal of Speech-Language Valdes, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to
Pathology – Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 20–31. speaker component of naming in children as a func-
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100262. tion of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of
Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134. https://doi.org/10.
naming in children with no prior tact responses as a 1007/BF03393014.
function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction. Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., & Pistoljevic, N. (2007). Emer-
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71–87. https:// gence of naming in preschoolers: A comparison of
doi.org/10.1007/BF03393048. multiple and single exemplar instruction. European
Frampton, S. E., & Shillingsburg, M. A. (2018). Teach- Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 109–131. https://doi.
ing children with autism to explain how: A case for org/10.1080/15021149.2007.11434278.
problem-solving? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Greer, R. D., & Yuan, L. (2008). How kids learn to say
51, 236–254. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.445. the darnedest things: The effect of multiple exemplar
Gamba, J., Goyos, C., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2015). The instruction on the emergence of novel verb usage.
functional independence of mands and tacts: Has it The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 103–121. https://
been demonstrated empirically? The Analysis of Verbal doi.org/10.1007/BF03393060.
Behavior, 31, 10–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Greer, R. D., Yuan, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005). Novel
s40616-014-0026-7. dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a
Garcia, E., Baer, D. M., & Firestone, I. (1971). The multiple exemplar instructional history. The Analysis
development of generalized imitation within topo- of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116. https://doi.org/10.
graphically limited boundaries. Journal of Applied 1007/BF03393012.
Behavior Analysis, 4, 101–102. https://doi.org/10. Griffih, K. R., Ramos, A. L., Hill, K. E., & Miguel, C. F.
1901/jaba.1971.4-101. (2018). Using equivalence-based instruction to teach
22 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

piano skills to college students. Journal of Applied Science + Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Behavior Analysis, 51, 201–219. https://doi.org/10. 978-1-4939-0401-3_16.
1002/jaba.438. Lamarre, J., & Holland, J. G. (1985). The functional
Hall, G. A., & Sundberg, M. L. (1987). Teaching mands independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the
by manipulating conditioned establishing operations. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 5–19. https://
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 5, 41–53. https://doi. doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1985.43-5.
org/10.1007/BF03392819. Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A.
Hawkins, E., Kingsdorf, S., Charnock, J., Szabo, M., & (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe
Gautreaux, G. (2009). Effects of multiple exemplar them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied
instruction on naming. European Journal of Behavior Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414. https://doi.org/10.
Analysis, 10, 265–273. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 1901/jaba.2003.36-407.
15021149.2009.11434324. Lechago, S. A., Carr, J. E., Kisamore, A. N., &
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Grow, L. L. (2015). The effects of multiple exemplar
Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of instruction on the relation between listener and
human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer/ intraverbal categorization repertoires. The Analysis of
Academic Plenum. Verbal Behavior, 31, 76–95. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Holth, P. (2017). Multiple exemplar training: Some s40616-015-0027-1.
strengths and limitations. The Behavior Analyst, 40, Lee, G. P., Miguel, C. F., Darcey, E. K., &
225–241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-017-0083-z. Jennings, A. M. (2015). A further evaluation of the
Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of effects of listener training on the emergence of
naming and other symbolic behaviors. Journal of the speaker behavior and categorization in children with
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241. autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19,
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185. 72–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.04.007.
Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1997). Toward a theory of Lillie, M. A., & Tiger, J. H. (2019). Acquisition and gen-
verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of erative responding following print-to-braille construc-
Behavior, 68, 271–296. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab. tion response training with sighted learners. Journal of
1997.68-271. Applied Behavior Analysis, 52, 286–298. https://doi.
Horner, R. H., & McDonald, R. S. (1982). A compari- org/10.1002/jaba.516.
son of single instance and general case instruction in
Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Nam-
teaching a generalized vocational skill. Journal of the
ing and categorization in young children: III. Vocal
Association for the Severely Handicapped, 7, 7–20.
tact training and transfer of function. Journal of the
https://doi.org/10.1177/154079698200700302.
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83, 47–65. https://
Hughes, C., Harmer, M. L., Killian, D. J., & Niarhos, F.
doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.31-04.
(1995). The effects of multiple-exemplar self-
instructional training on high school students’ gener- Luciano, C., Becerra, I. G., & Valverde, M. R. (2007).
alized conversational interactions. Journal of Applied The role of multiple-exemplar training and naming
Behavior Analysis, 28, 201–218. https://doi.org/10. in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Jour-
1901/jaba.1995.28-201. nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87,
Jenkins, H. M., & Harrison, R. H. (1960). Effect of dis- 349–365. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.08-06.
crimination training on auditory generalization. Jour- Luke, N., Greer, R. D., Singer-Dudek, J., &
nal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 246–253. https:// Keohane, D. D. (2011). The emergence of autoclitic
doi.org/10.1037/h0041661. frames in atypically and typically developing children
Jennings, A. M., & Miguel, C. F. (2017). Training as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The
intraverbal bidirectional naming to establish general- Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 141–156. https://doi.
ized equivalence class performances. Journal of the org/10.1007/BF03393098.
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 108, 269–289. Ma, M. L., Miguel, C. F., & Jennings, A. M. (2016).
https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.277. Training intraverbal naming to establish equivalence
Kobari-Wright, V. V., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). The class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analy-
effects of listener training on the emergence of cate- sis of Behavior, 105, 409–426. https://doi.org/10.
gorization and speaker behavior in children with 1002/jeab.203.
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, Marcon-Dawson, A., Vicars, S. M., & Miguel, C. F.
431–436. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.115. (2009). Publication trends in The Analysis of Verbal
LaFrance, D. L., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). Teaching verbal Behavior: 1999-2008. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
behavior to children with autism spectrum disorders. 25, 123–132. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393076.
In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & Marzullo-Kerth, D., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., &
J. L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early intervention Townsend, D. B. (2011). Using multiple exemplar
for autism spectrum disorders, Autism and child psycho- training to teach a generalized repertoire of sharing to
pathology series (pp. 403–436). New York: Springer children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
MULTIPLE EXEMPLAR INSTRUCTION 23

Analysis, 44, 279–294. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. Najdowski, A. C., St. Clair, M., Fullen, J. A., Child, A.,
2011.44-279. Persicke, A., & Tarbox, J. (2018). Teaching children
Michael, J., & Miguel, C. F. (2020). Motivating opera- with autism to identify and respond appropriately to
tions. In J. O. Cooper, T. E. Heron, & the preferences of others during play. Journal of
W. L. Heward (Eds.), Applied Behavior Analysis (3rd Applied Behavior Analysis, 51, 890–898. https://doi.
ed., pp. 372–394). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. org/10.1002/jaba.494.
Michael, J., Palmer, D. C., & Sundberg, M. L. (2011). Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence
The multiple control of verbal behavior. The Analysis of untaught mands or tacts of novel adjective-object
of Verbal Behavior, 27, 3–22. https://doi.org/10. pairs as a function of instructional history. The Analy-
1007/bf03393089. sis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 63–76. https://doi.org/10.
Miguel, C. F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirectional 1007/BF03392995.
naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 125–138. Oah, S., & Dickinson, A. M. (1989). A review of empiri-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2. cal studies of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal
Miguel, C. F. (2018). Problem-solving, bidirectional nam- Behavior, 7, 53–68. https://doi.org/10.1007/
ing, and the development of verbal repertoires. Behav- BF03392837.
ior Analysis Research and Practice, 18, 340–353. Olaff, H. S., Ona, H. N., & Holth, P. (2017). Establish-
https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000110. ment of naming in children with autism through
Miguel, C. F., Frampton, S. E., Lantaya, C. A., multiple response-exemplar training. Behavioral
LaFrance, D., Quah, K., Meyer, C. S., … Development Bulletin, 22, 67–85. https://doi.org/10.
Fernand, J. K. (2015). The effects of tact training on 1037/bdb0000044.
the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of Park, H. S., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1989). A problem solv-
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 104, 96–118. ing approach to social skills training in employment
https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.167. settings with mentally retarded youth. Journal of
Miguel, C. F., & Kobari-Wright, V. V. (2013). The Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 373–380. https://doi.
effects of tact training on the emergence of categori- org/10.1901/jaba.1989.22-373.
zation and listener behavior in children with autism. Parsonson, B. S., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Training gener-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 669–673. alized improvisation of tools by preschool children.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.62. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 363–380.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1978.11-363.
Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus
Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M.
categorization by preschool children. Journal of the
(2012). Establishing metaphorical reasoning in chil-
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383–405.
dren with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disor-
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008-89-383.
ders, 6, 913–920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.
Miller, S. J., & Sloane, H. N. (1976). The generalization 2011.12.007.
effects of parent training across stimulus settings.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 355–370. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York:
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-355. Harper Collins Publishers.
Miltenberger, R. G., Roberts, J. A., Ellingson, S., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., Townsend, D. B., &
Galensky, T., Rapp, J. T., Long, E. S., & Poulson, C. L. (2007). Establishing a generalized rep-
Lumley, V. A. (2013). Training and generalization of ertoire of helping behavior in children with autism.
sexual abuse prevention skills for women with mental Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 123–136.
retardation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.11-05.
385–388. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-385. Rosales-Ruiz, J., & Baer, D. M. (1997). Behavioral cusps:
Ming, S., & Stewart, I. (2017). When things are not the A developmental and pragmatic concept for behavior
same: A review of research into relations of difference. analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50, 429–455. 533–544. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-533.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.367. Saunders, R. R., Drake, K. M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1999).
Moran, L., Walsh, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Equivalence class establishment, expansion, and mod-
Ming, S. (2015). Correlating derived relational ification in preschool children. Journal of the Experi-
responding with linguistic and cognitive ability in mental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 195–214. https://doi.
children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research org/10.1901/jeab.1999.71-195.
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 32–43. https://doi. Sautter, R. A., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2006). Empirical appli-
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.015. cations of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior with
Najdowski, A. C., Bergstrom, R., Tarbox, J., & St. humans. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 35–48.
Clair, M. (2017). Teaching children with autism to https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393025.
respond to disguised mands. Journal of Applied Behav- Shafer, E. (1995). A review of interventions to teach a
ior Analysis, 50, 733–743. https://doi.org/10.1002/ mand repertoire. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12,
jaba.413. 53–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392897.
24 DANIELLE L. LAFRANCE and JONATHAN TARBOX

Sidener, T. M., Carr, J. E., Karsten, A. M., Stauch, T., LaLonde, K., Plavnick, J. B., Bak, M. Y., &
Severtson, J. M., Cornelius, C. E., & Gatewood, K. (2017). Intraverbal training for indi-
Heinicke, M. R. (2010). Evaluation of single and viduals with autism: The current status of multiple
mixed verbal operant arrangements for teaching control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33, 98–116.
mands and tacts. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 26, https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-017-0079-5.
15–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393079. Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2013). Language
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equiva- generativity, response generalization and derived rela-
lences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, tional responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
5–13. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1401.05. 29, 137–155. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393131.
Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the rein- Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol-
forcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146. https://doi.org/10. Analysis, 10, 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
1901/jeab.2000.74-127. 1977.10-349.
Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimina- Tarbox, J., Zuckerman, C. K., Bishop, M. R.,
tion vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing Olive, M. L., & O’Hora, D. P. (2011). Rule-
paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, governed behavior: Teaching a preliminary repertoire
37, 5–22. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5. of rule-following to children with autism. The Analy-
Sivaraman, M. (2017). Using multiple exemplar training sis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 125–139. https://doi.org/
to teach empathy skills to children with autism. The 10.1007/BF03393096.
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 337–346. https://doi.
Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An introduction to
org/10.1007/s40617-017-0183-y.
relational frame theory and its clinical application.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Cop-
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc.
ley Publishing Group.
Speckman, J., Greer, R. D., & Rivera-Valdes, C. (2012). Wiskow, K. M., Matter, A. L., & Donaldson, J. M.
Multiple exemplar instruction and the emergence of (2018). An evaluation of lag schedules and prompting
generative production of suffixes as autoclitic frames. methods to increase variability of naming category
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28, 83–99. https:// items in children with autism spectrum disorder. The
doi.org/10.1007/BF03393109. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 34, 100–123. https://doi.
Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. (1984). The effects of org/10.1007/s40616-018-0102-5.
single instance, multiple instance, and general case Young, J. M., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., &
training on generalized vending machine use by mod- Poulson, C. L. (1994). Generalized imitation and
erately and severely handicapped students. Journal of response-class formation in children with autism.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 273–278. https://doi. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 685–697.
org/10.1901/jaba.1984.17-273. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-685.
Sprinkle, E. C., & Miguel, C. F. (2013). Establishing
derived textual activity schedules in children with Received September 26, 2017
autism. Behavioral Interventions, 28, 185–202. Final acceptance June 19, 2019
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1365. Action Editor, Tiffany Kodak

S-ar putea să vă placă și