Sunteți pe pagina 1din 15

Review Article

Nutrition (Micronutrients) in Child Growth and Development: A


Systematic Review on Current Evidence, Recommendations and
Opportunities for Further Research
Mohammad Yawar Yakoob, MD, MS, DS,* Clifford W. Lo, MD, PhD†

ABSTRACT: Objective: An important aspect of malnutrition is deficiency of different micronutrients during


pregnancy or early childhood. We systematically reviewed the role of nutrition in child growth (weight or
height gain) and development. Methods: A comprehensive literature search was done on PubMed/Cochrane
Library browsing through 38,795 abstracts until December 31, 2016 to select systematic reviews/meta-
analyses and individual randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of micronutrient supplementation. Results:
Micronutrients studied included iron, iodine, folate, zinc, calcium, magnesium, selenium, vitamin D, vitamin
A, vitamin B complex, and multiple micronutrients. We summarize evidence with details and results of RCTs,
highlight strengths/weaknesses, and critically interpret findings. Effects of breastfeeding-promotion, food-
supplementation (complementary and school feeding), conditional-cash-transfers, and integrated nutrition/
psychosocial interventions are discussed. Conclusion: Based on this evidence we make policy and pro-
grammatic recommendations for supplementation to mothers and children at high-risk of deficiency.
(J Dev Behav Pediatr 38:665–679, 2017) Index terms: micronutrient supplementation, pregnancy, child growth and development.

T he future of human societies depends on children


being able to achieve their optimal growth and de-
first 2 years of life. This requires a diet that provides suf-
ficient nutrients for normal, healthy growth.3 Maternal
velopment. The Lancet Series on Child Development undernutrition can lead to low birth weight (LBW), pre-
identified 4 risk factors in resource-poor countries where term births, and small-for-gestational age babies.4 Preterm
the need for intervention is urgent in terms of improving and LBW infants are at increased risk of major impair-
child development, 3 of which were nutrition-related: ments, as well as below-average cognitive abilities and
stunting, iodine deficiency, and iron deficiency anemia. greater behavioral problems at school age, even in the
These, along with other undernutrition factors, are com- absence of obvious neurological deficits.5 Therefore,
mon during pregnancy and early childhood in resource- interventions can be targeted at the periconceptional
poor countries. It is important to address nutrition-related phase and during pregnancy.
risks to support optimal development in early childhood The development of the brain occurs at an earlier
with implications throughout life.1,2 stage than the rest of the body. This can be illustrated by
Inadequate maternal and early child nutrition may limit the fact that a newborn’s brain is about 10% of the body
the development of important aspects of the brain’s ar- weight, whereas in the adult, it is only 2%. By the age of
chitecture. The brain is vulnerable to nutritional influen- 1 year, the child will be about 15% of the final body
ces from the preconception period as well as the earliest weight but the brain will be already about 70% of that of
stages of pregnancy from the time the neural plate is a young adult. By 2 years of age, the brain will be about
formed leading to a neural tube, a process that is complete 77% of its final weight, whereas the body is about 20% of
from 21 to 28 days after conception.3 The brain also the adult level.6 After birth, the key window of oppor-
grows very rapidly in the last trimester of pregnancy and tunity for interventions is during the first 24 months of
life, since the brain development is in an important
From the *Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public phase during this period. This is the period of peak in-
Health, Boston, MA; †Department of Nutrition, Children’s Hospital Boston,
Boston, MA. cidence of growth faltering, micronutrient deficiencies,
Received June 2016; accepted June 2017. and infectious illnesses in resource-poor countries.7 After
Disclosure: The authors declare no conflict of interest. this period, the effects of malnutrition on stunting may
Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations be irreversible, and some of the functional deficits may
appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this become permanent.8 Exclusive breastfeeding is recom-
article on the journal’s Web site (www.jdbp.org). mended for the first 6 months of life, after which com-
Address for reprints: Mohammad Yawar Yakoob, MD, MS, DS, Department of plementary foods should be introduced. Therefore,
Nutrition, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Avenue,
Boston, MA 02115; e-mail: myyakoob@mail.harvard.edu. interventions like breastfeeding-promotion and comple-
mentary feeding strategies (provision of complementary
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
foods with or without nutrition education is discussed

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 www.jdbp.org | 665


Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
later in this article) that are effective at reducing mal- 2016, and involved screening a further approximate
nutrition during this vulnerable period should be a high 2795 abstracts. The approximate search terms used
priority. Early childhood stunting caused by malnutrition were, for example, (iron) AND (growth OR de-
is associated with adverse developmental outcomes, es- velopment) AND (infan* OR child OR children); (“vita-
pecially, deficits in cognition and school achievement min a”) AND (growth OR development) AND (infan* OR
from early childhood to late adolescence.9–11 child OR children) AND suppl*; (breastfeeding) AND
Malnutrition, especially, micronutrient deficiencies, (growth OR development) AND (infan* OR child OR
can also occur in resource-rich countries. Subtle dif- children) AND (promotion OR support); (food OR
ferences in diet composition can lead to micronutrient feeding) AND (growth OR development) AND (infan*
deficiencies. Micronutrient deficiencies may also in- OR child OR children) AND (complementary OR pre-
teract with the gut microbiome and alter the immune school OR preschool OR school); and (“conditional cash
response of the intestine. For example, iron deficiency transfer*”) AND (growth OR development) AND (infan*
can deplete lactobacilli that convert lactose and other OR child OR children).
sugars to lactic acid and may result in diarrhea, or zinc Studies that reported outcomes on growth (weight
deficiency can decrease richness and species diversity, and height gain) and/or development were selected and
decrease Gram-negative facultative anaerobes and lac- were from both resource-rich and resource-poor coun-
tobacilli, and affect the immune system.12,13 tries. The priority was to obtain systematic reviews and
It is also now widely recognized that nutritional the most recent randomized controlled trials on the
well-being is not only related to dietary adequacy and topic. In the absence of such evidence, observational
quality, but also to the process of feeding and parenting studies were also selected. We have made separate
psychosocial determinants. These include areas like cog- sections of supplementation during pregnancy and
nitive stimulation, caregiver sensitivity and responsiveness during childhood and within each section described the
to the child, and caregiver affect (emotional warmth or outcomes of growth first and then development. We
rejection of the child). It is known that responsive feeding excluded studies of food fortification and where
might have a role in ameliorating child undernutrition micronutrients were part of the diet and not given as
because positive caregiver verbalizations during feeding supplements.
can increase child acceptance of food.14 Similarly, par-
enting styles and feeding practices can influence a child’s RESULTS
body mass index (BMI), for example, indulgent or pro- Role of Micronutrients
tective parenting was associated with higher BMI and The main micronutrients considered in this review in-
authoritative parenting with a healthy BMI.15 Besides, clude iron, iodine, folic acid, zinc, vitamin A and vitamin
studies by Galler et al. and Lozoff et al. have shown that D. Deficiency may occur for more than 1 nutrient;
compromised nutritional status during the first year of therefore, the role of multiple micronutrients (MMNs) is
life is associated with neurocognitive impairments per- also covered in this article (Supplemental Digital Content
sisting into adulthood despite complete catch-up physical 1, http://links.lww.com/JDBP/A143). Other miscellaneous
growth at adolescence.16–18 micronutrients are also discussed in brief like calcium,
This review, therefore, includes the efficacy and ef- magnesium, selenium and vitamin B complex.
fectiveness of nutrition-only–related interventions along
with integrated nutrition-psychosocial interventions af- Iron Supplementation
fecting child growth (weight or height gain) and de- Globally, anemia affects 47.4% of preschool children
velopment. We conclude with considerations for policy and 25.4% of school age children,3 most of which is due
and program planners for large-scale implementation. to iron deficiency. Iron deficiency anemia is, therefore,
one of the most prevalent yet neglected nutritional
METHODOLOGY deficiencies in the world. The influence of iron de-
We did a comprehensive literature search on PubMed ficiency in children has been reviewed in several studies
(MEDLINE) and the Cochrane Clinical Trials using the leading to very similar conclusions. It has been seen that
following search strategy: it leads to poorer developmental outcomes in infancy
(“Micronutrients” [Mesh] OR nutrition OR micro- and childhood, especially, motor, cognitive, social-
nutrient* OR “multiple micronutrient*” OR zinc OR emotional and neurophysiologic development in the
iodine OR calcium OR magnesium OR “vitamin D” short term and in the long term.19–24 The effects of early
OR “vitamin A” OR iron OR “folic acid” OR “vitamin B” iron deficiency include decreased myelination, changes
OR breastfeeding OR food OR “cash transfer*”) AND in dopamine metabolism in the striatum, and alterations
(growth OR development) AND (infan* OR child OR in the energy metabolism of the hippocampus, an area of
children). the brain important for memory.25 New findings also
The last date of search was January 31, 2010. A total of point to the need for more attention to the de-
36,000 abstracts were screened and evaluated. The velopmental effects of prenatal iron deficiency. The most
search for recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses common etiology of reduced iron supply to the fetus is
was updated in December 2016 up to December 31, maternal iron deficiency, and fetal and neonatal iron

666 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
deficiency adversely affects developing brain-behavior these reviews and the findings for iron-replete children
systems.23 have been conflicting. Gahagan et al.40 retrospectively
Iron Supplementation in Pregnancy examined short- and long-term growth in iron-sufficient
A recent systematic review by Chmielewska et al.26 (IS) infants from a randomized trial of iron supplemen-
that studied prenatal iron supplementation during preg- tation among breastfed, low- to middle-income Chilean
nancy showed no effect of iron supplementation on the infants. Growth trajectories between iron supplemented
mental development or the behavioral status of their and usual nutrition groups did not differ during or after
children. Individual studies found from our literature supplementation indicating no adverse effect of iron in IS
search reported similar findings. In a randomized study infants. Similarly, in Bangladesh, iron was found to have
by Frith et al.27 from Bangladesh, mothers from 14 weeks no impact on the rate of length or weight gain for infants
of gestation to 12 weeks postpartum were assigned into aged 6 to 12 months or when stratified by baseline he-
3 groups: 30 mg Fe 1 400 mg folic acid, 60 mg Fe 1 400 moglobin concentration.41 However, Lind et al. from
mg folic acid, and MMNs. Compared with 30 mg Fe, 60 secondary analysis of 6-month old Indonesian infants
mg Fe decreased the quality of maternal-infant feeding participating in a randomized trial showed that weight-
interaction by approximately 10%. This could be because for-age Z score (WAZ) from 6 to 12 months and mean
of a higher dose of iron inhibiting zinc absorption lead- WAZ at 12 months were significantly lower in the iron-
ing to decreased plasma zinc concentrations, gastroin- replete iron-supplemented group compared with
testinal side effects, and increased maternal morbidity iron-replete non–iron-supplemented infants.42 In another
because of reduced immunity or higher bacterial viru- recent randomized trial43 on breastfed infants enrolled at
lence. MMNs compared with 30 mg of iron did not im- 1 month and supplemented from 4 months, medicinal
prove interaction but reduced early postpartum distress. iron was associated with a small but significant reduction
This may be related to the effect of micronutrients on in length gain and a trend toward reduced weight gain at
significant neurotransmitters. A randomized controlled the end of supplementation at 9 months. However,
trial from Australia28 by Zhou et al. on prenatal 20 mg/ provision of medicinal iron or iron-fortified cereal did
d iron supplementation from 20 weeks of gestation to improve the iron status in this study.
delivery compared with placebo group found no differ- We identified a few reviews on the relationship be-
ence in the mean composite IQ score or any of its sub- tween iron and developmental outcomes. The results
scales or in the proportion of children with IQ 1 or 2 SD were mixed. Reviews34,36,39,44,45 of oral, preventive ,and
below the mean at 4 years of age after multiple con- therapeutic iron supplementation in children showed
founder adjustments such as sex, birth order, gestational reductions or improvement in cognitive and motor de-
age, maternal age, maternal education, paternal educa- velopment deficits, and improvement in memory and
tion, home environment, and breastfeeding. Another learning in iron-deficient or anemic children, particu-
Chinese study by Li et al. on prenatal iron and folic acid larly, with longer-duration, lower-dose regimens. The
supplementation (60 mg iron and 400 mg/d folic acid) review by Low et al. reported improvement in global
compared with folic acid alone showed no effect on the cognitive scores, IQ, attention, and concentration.44
Bayley mental development index and Bayley psycho- Similarly, a recent review by Lam and Lawlis46 was based
motor development index (PDI) of children at 3, 6, and on 2 trials, of which 1 reported improvement in in-
12 months of age.29,30 telligence and attention after iron supplementation,47
Iron Supplementation in Infancy/Children whereas the other reported higher increase in IQ score
Petry et al.,30 Ramakrishnan et al.,31,32 US Preventive in weekly versus daily supplementation but no differ-
Services Task Force,33 Baumgartner et al.,34 Pasricha ence between weekly and placebo.48 Another review,
et al.,35,36 Vucic et al.37 and Sachdev et al.38 did meta- based on 5 RCTs, suggested a possible moderate im-
analyses/systematic reviews of randomized controlled provement in PDI score that was most evident at 12
intervention trials of the impact of iron supplementation months of age with no effect on mental development or
on child growth and showed that iron had no significant behavior of children.26 Another systematic review by
effect on child growth. There was no significant effect of Sachdev et al.49 comprising 15 randomized studies on
iron therapy on any of the anthropometric variables 2827 children showed that after supplementation the
studied: weight-for-age, weight-for-height, height-for-age, combined mental development score standardized mean
mid-upper arm circumference, skinfold thickness, and difference (SMD) (random model) was 0.30 (95% CI,
head circumference.30,38 Overall, effect sizes were 0.09 0.15–0.46; p , .001). Baseline anemia and iron de-
[95% confidence interval (CI), 20.07 to 0.24] for height ficiency anemia were responsible for heterogeneity. For
and 0.13 (95% CI, 20.05 to 0.30) for weight.31 Iron IQ scores (4 trials on children $ 8 years of age), the
therapy, however, did significantly improve hemoglobin combined SMD was 0.41 (95% CI, 0.20–0.62, p , .001).
levels in these reviews.30,31 Also, another 2006 review39 There was no effect of iron on motor development
showed that with iron supplementation, weight gains score. In contrast, other recent reviews concluded that
were adversely affected in iron-replete children and the iron supplementation in children under 2 years of age
effects on height were inconclusive. Many studies, in- did not have an effect on Bayley MDI (MD 1.65, 95% CI
cluding randomized trials, have been published after 20.63 to 3.94; 6 studies, n 5 1093) and PDI (1.05, 21.36

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 667
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
to 3.46; 6 studies, n 5 1086).30,35,50 A review by Guo groups. Five other quasi-experimental or observational
et al. reported no difference in global cognitive perfor- studies were found. A nonrandomized intervention
mance in primary school children after iron supple- study76 compared psychological development of infants
mentation51 and similarly, US Preventive Services Task aged 3 to 18 months for 300 mg/d of KI (n 5 133) during
Force review found mostly nonsignificant results for the first trimester of pregnancy versus no iodine sup-
developmental test scores after 3 to 12 months of follow- plements (n 5 61). This study found that children of
up.33 A Cochrane review52,53 on iron therapy in children iodine-supplemented mothers had a more favorable
less than 3 years of age with iron deficiency anemia in- psychometric assessment than those of the unexposed
cluded 5 trials and studied measures of psychomotor group. They had higher scores on the PDI (p 5 .02) and
development within 30 days of commencement of the Behavior Rating Scale. However, this study was not
therapy. The combined difference in pretreatment to controlled for different confounding factors but is the
posttreatment change in Bayley PDI between iron and best evidence available. Another recent intervention
placebo groups and in Bayley MDI was also non- study77 of pregnant women with mild hypothyroxinemia
significant, but the days elapsed since the start of treat- reported delayed neurodevelopmental outcomes among
ment were few. children of nonsupplemented mothers. The mean de-
velopmental quotient of children of mothers with thy-
Iodine Supplementation roxine levels above the 20th percentile at the beginning
Iodine is a clear example of a nutrient that, if deficient of gestation (4–6 weeks) was 101.8 6 9.7 versus 87.5 6
during a critical period of brain development, has long- 8.9 in nonsupplemented mothers. The timing of the start
term adverse consequences.3 Iodine deficiency is the of intervention was potentially important; supplementa-
single most important cause of preventable brain damage tion starting at 12 to 14 weeks of gestation was associ-
and mental retardation. It is required for the production ated with worse scores than supplementation starting at
of thyroid hormones that are essential for brain de- 4 to 6 weeks. In a small pilot study in France (n 5 44) of
velopment, and the fetus, newborn, and young child are prenatal 150 mg/d of iodine supplementation from , 10
particularly vulnerable to iodine deficiency. Severe io- weeks of gestation to 3 months postpartum, scores in
dine deficiency during gestation results in fetal brain Bayley Scales of Infant Development were not different
damage that is irreversible by mid-gestation, commonly in exposed at 24 months compared with unexposed
leading to cretinism and impaired cognitive function in group.78 However, 2 other studies79,80 from Spain
children.54 Mild-to-moderate maternal hypothyroxinemia reported that women with self-reported iodine intake of
may also result in suboptimal neurodevelopment.55 Even 150 mg/d compared with ,100 mg/d had infants with
mild-to-moderate deficiency in children has been asso- lower PDI (PDI , 85, OR 5 1.8, 1.0–3.3). The compar-
ciated with lower IQs and more disruptive behavior.56–58 ison range of iodine intake was narrower in these studies
Iodine Supplementation During Pregnancy with potential for misclassification in reporting intakes.
Multiple systematic reviews and meta-analyses have Meta-analysis comprising some of these non-RCTs plus
studied maternal iodine supplementation.55,59–65 The additional ones found that iodine supplementation be-
findings from the trials have also been mixed. In 1 trial fore or early in pregnancy compared with control had an
from Peru, there was no difference between iodine and average effect size of 0.51 (8 non-RCTs) for mental de-
placebo groups in gross development of newborns, but velopment score that was smaller (d 5 0.17) for late
in subgroup observational analysis, iodine-supplemented pregnancy supplementation (4 non-RCTs).64
mothers had infants with higher IQ scores (85.6 6 13.9 Iodine Supplementation in Children
compared with 74.4 6 14.8, p 5 .002) and lower psy- Childhood iodine supplementation has been
chological age retardation (15.5 6 11.6% compared with covered in multiple systematic reviews and meta-
26.6 6 14.1%, p , .01).66–68 In the trial from Papua New analyses.59–61,64,81 The Cochrane review81 on iodine
Guinea, incidence proportion of cretinism in iodine- supplementation in children under 18 years of age in
supplemented villages was 2% versus 6% in placebo iodine deficiency areas included a total of 26 studies.
group (relative risk 5 0.27, 0.12–0.60, p 5 .002) up to Different forms of iodine supplementation were used in
a follow-up period of 15 years after intervention.69–72 these studies, including iodized salt, iodized oil (oral
RCTs on pregnant women with mild thyroid dysfunction form), intramuscular oil, iodized water, etc. Body weight
found no effect on children’s IQ status or cognitive was given as an outcome in 4 studies82–85: weight in-
functioning through 3 or 5 years with levothyroxine creased by 5.2% (after 4 months) to 27.5% (after 22
treatment versus none started before 18 weeks of ges- months) in the intervention groups (oral-iodized oil) and
tation.73,74 Similarly, another recent randomized in- 3.4% to 13.2% (both after 10 months) in the control
tervention study75 comprising 3 groups (iodized salt in groups. In 3 of these 4 trials, increases were slightly
cooking and at the table, 200 mg of potassium iodide greater in the intervention group than in the control, but
(KI)/d, 300 mg/d of KI) evaluated maternal iodine sup- were nonsignificant. Increase in height was also given in
plementation in 131 pregnant women during the first 4 studies.82–84,86 It was 2.7% (after 10 months) to 7%
trimester and found no effect on children’s neurological (after 22 months) in the intervention (oral-iodized oil)
development nor effect modification by treatment group and 2.3% (after 10 months) to 7.9% (after 22

668 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
months) in the control group. Moreover, 3 prospective Zinc Supplementation
double-blind supplementation intervention versus Zinc is involved in the activity of over 200 enzymes, in
placebo studies87,88 were conducted on 7- to 10-year-old particular, those associated with synthesis of DNA and
severely iodine-deficient Moroccan children (n 5 71; RNA. An estimated 17.3% of the world population has
10-month study of iodized salt), moderately deficient 10- inadequate zinc intakes.92 Although zinc plays essential
to 12-year-old Albanian children (n 5 310; 6-month developmental roles both before and after birth, its role
study; 400 mg iodine as oral iodized oil), and mildly in the brain is poorly understood. In animal studies, it has
deficient 5- to 14-year-old South African children (n 5 been shown that zinc deficiency during gestation and
188; 6-month study; 2 doses of 200 mg iodine as oral- lactation results in irreversible cognitive impairment.3
iodized oil). At follow-up, supplementation did not have Zinc deficiency in infants may also compromise behav-
a significant effect on somatic growth among South iors necessary for cognitive functioning including activ-
African children but increased WAZ and height-for-age Z ity and attention.93
scores (HAZ) in Moroccan and Albanian children.
Shrestha84 showed significant improvement in hand Zinc Supplementation During Pregnancy
grip between intervention and control groups (32 vs The Cochrane review on zinc supplementation during
23.3% after 10 months). The difference in sitting/stand- pregnancy by Ota et al.94 has reported birth outcomes and
ing ability was also significant. The same study reported growth outcomes. Based on a meta-analysis of 14 RCTs
significant differences in ball-throwing abilities with no involving 5643 women, zinc supplementation during
differences in pegboard and balancing tests after 4 pregnancy was found to have no significant difference in
months. Huda 200185 evaluated 12 different cognitive low birth weight (LBW) (relative risk 5 0.93; 95% CI,
and motor function tests without any significant differ- 0.78–1.12), head circumference (7 RCTs), or mid-upper
ences between the 2 groups. IQ was measured in 1 study arm circumference (3 RCTs). Infant WAZ score was sim-
by Bautista et al.82 There were no significant differences ilar at 6 months in the 2 groups based on 2 RCTs,95,96 but
in increase in IQ, improvement in school grades, or in by 13 months, no-zinc group showed significantly higher
perceptual-motor development between intervention scores based on 1 RCT.95 Weight-for-height was also not
and control groups. Improvement in cognitive perfor- different between the 2 groups at 6 months. In a recent
mance was also nonsignificant between the 2 groups as trial of antenatal micronutrient supplementation,97 the
measured by Untoro et al.89 However, children who mean height of school age children in rural Nepal of
were more iodine-deficient exhibited greater increase in mothers in the iron 1 folic acid 1 zinc group was 113.93
cognitive performance than those who were more io- (65.79) cm, compared with 113.33 (65.32) cm in iron 1
dine replete. folic acid group (nonsignificant). The mean weight was
A randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial90 of also 18.12 (62.36) kg in the intervention versus 18.05
iodine supplementation was conducted in Dunedin, New (62.32) kg in the iron 1 folic acid group. Another ma-
Zealand, among 184 children aged 10 to 13 years who ternal supplementation study in Peru showed no effect on
were mildly iodine-deficient. Children were randomly linear growth, but it was associated with greater weight
assigned a daily tablet of 150 mg iodine or placebo for 28 gain through age 1 year of infancy (p , .001).98
weeks. Iodine supplementation significantly improved A meta-analysis by Warthon-Medina et al.99 showed no
scores for 2 of the 4 cognitive subsets (picture concepts effects of maternal zinc supplementation on the child’s
[p 5 .023] and matrix reasoning [p 5 .040]) but not for cognitive domains of intelligence, executive function, and
letter-number sequencing (p 5 .480) or symbol search motor skills. In the Cochrane review on maternal zinc
(p 5 .608). The overall cognitive score of the iodine- supplementation,100 2 trials101,102 measured child de-
supplemented group was 0.19 SDs higher than that of velopment outcomes. Hamadani et al.101 found that among
the placebo group (p 5 .011). After 28 weeks of supple- 168 Bangladeshi infants studied at 13 months, the zinc
mentation, iodine status improved in intervention group, group had significantly worse mental development, PDI
whereas the placebo group remained iodine-deficient. scores, emotional tone, and cooperation compared with
Another randomized double-blind trial of iodine supple- no-zinc group, with infant approach, activity, and vocali-
mentation in iodine-deficient 10- to 12-year-old children in zation showing no differences. The other RCT102 that fol-
Albania91 showed that compared with placebo, iodine lowed 355 infants of African-American mothers at 5 years
treatment significantly improved performance on 4 of 7 of age found no significant differences for differential
tests: rapid target marking, symbol search, rapid object abilities, visual or auditory sequential memory scores, Knox
naming, and Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices cube, gross motor scale, and grooved pegboard scores.
(p , .0001). Meta-analysis based on these 2 studies59 Another review by Hess and King103 reports data from
showed a significant improvement in global cognitive a study in Peru104 where some improvements in infant
scores (adjusted SMD 5 0.27, 0.10–0.44, p 5 .002) and in neurobehavioral development (novelty preference) was
perceptual reasoning (0.55, 0.05–1.04, p 5 .03) with sig- observed in zinc-supplemented group at 6 months of age.
nificant between-study heterogeneity for the latter out- Zinc Supplementation in Children
come (I2 5 90%, p 5 .001). There was no benefit for the The impact of zinc supplementation in prepubertal
domains of processing speed index and working memory. and under-5 children on growth has been the subject of

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 669
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
many reviews30,32,105–111 and multiple other studies. In anemia at baseline modified the effect of zinc on HAZ and
a 2009 review on preventive zinc supplementation by WAZ scores. Anemic infants supplemented for 6 months
Brown et al.,105 it was shown that there was a signifi- had significantly higher HAZ (0.17) and WAZ (0.14)
cantly greater change in height among prepubertal chil- scores, with no effect on growth in infants not anemic at
dren who received zinc supplements, with an overall baseline. Among Bangladeshi infants, Fischer Walker et al.
effect size of 0.170 (95% CI, 0.075–0.264), p 5 .001 showed that there was no effect of zinc on the rate of
based on 37 controlled trials (47 data sets). There was no length or weight gain for infants aged 6 to 12 months or
correlation between mean initial HAZ and effect size in even when stratified by baseline hemoglobin (Hb)
this analysis, unlike the results of a previous review106 concentration.41
where a positive response to zinc was only seen among The combined effects of zinc supplementation in
studies that enrolled children with initial mean children on the final MDI and PDI using the Bayley
HAZ , 21.5 z. The reason could be exclusion of studies scores have been reported in the reviews by Brown
of hospitalized, severely malnourished children from the et al.,105 Nissensohn et al.115 and Gogia et al.116 Most
2009 review. Similarly, zinc had a significant positive studies in the Brown et al. review did not present intra-
overall impact on change in weight from 35 trials individual changes in developmental scores during the
(45 data sets): mean effect size of 0.119 (95% CI, 0.048– course of the intervention that allowed only final values
0.190), p 5 .002. The effect on change in weight- to be compared. There was no impact of preventive zinc
for-height Z score (WHZ), although small, was also on final MDI (effect size 5 0.021, 95% CI: 20.133 to
marginally significant.105 Meanwhile, the review by 0.175, p 5 .76) or on final PDI (effect size: 0.025, 95% CI:
Ramakrishnan et al.32 showed that overall weighted 20.149 to 0.198, p 5 .75). We also identified a few other
mean effect for change in height was small and not sig- studies besides those included in this review. Heinig
nificant with zinc supplementation (0.07; 95% CI, 20.03 et al.117 in a double-blind, randomized, placebo-
to 0.17). Similar was the result for weight gain (0.09; 95% controlled trial showed that gross motor development
CI, 20.11 to 0.25). The difference between Brown et al. was not significantly different in zinc-supplemented
and Ramakrishnan et al. analyses is the inclusion/exclu- infants aged 4 to 10 months compared with placebo
sion criteria used. Ramakrishnan et al. included only group based on mean Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS)
studies with children younger than 5 years compared scores. However, in another meta-analysis by Warthon-
with prepubertal children in the review by Brown et al. Medina et al.,99 zinc supplementation during childhood
Brown et al. excluded studies of zinc fortification and had positive impacts on executive function and motor
those studies where SD for change in length was not skills and no effect on intelligence but was limited by
given in the paper, whereas Ramakrishnan et al. included a few data sets.
them. Several studies have been published after these
reviews. Another meta-analysis by Petry et al. showed Folic Acid Supplementation
that zinc supplementation had no effect on childhood Folic acid is involved in nucleotide synthesis, DNA
wasting, stunting, and underweight in children.30 A re- integrity, and transcription that explains its importance
cent randomized, double-blind trial112 in New Delhi, In- for brain development.118 It has an established role
dia, published after these reviews on 2482 children aged periconceptionally in decreasing the incidence of neural
6 to 30 months showed that after 4 months of supple- tube defects. Although the data are very limited, there
mentation, there were no differences between in- are suggestions that an adequate supply of folate is also
tervention (zinc) and placebo groups with respect to important for growth and development at later stages
weight and length gains, and with respect to length-for- because Gross et al.119 found that infants born to moth-
age Z scores, WAZ, and weight-for-length Z scores. An- ers with severe folate deficiency during pregnancy
other community-based randomized study of zinc in showed abnormal or delayed development. Other stud-
preschool children in Iran with retarded linear growth ies, however, have not found associations between folate
showed significantly increased weight gain in children status in the second half of pregnancy and neuro-
compared with placebo, but no effect on mid-upper arm psychological development of children at age 5. This
circumference increment. A highly significant impact may be because of their classification of low-folate status
was seen in boys’ height increment at the end of the based on biochemical measures rather than megaloblas-
follow-up period.113 This trial, however, had a small total tic anemia.120
sample size of 85. Data from another 4 randomized, Folate Use in Pregnancy
double-blind trials in 4 different countries114 show that In a meta-analysis review, folic acid supplementation
zinc supplementation in infants aged 4 to 6 months had during pregnancy had no impact on pregnancy out-
no effect on increments in height or weight during the 6- comes such as preterm birth; however, improvements
month supplementation period, nor on mean HAZ, WAZ, were seen in mean birth weight (mean difference
or WHZ scores. However, in logistic regression analysis, [MD] 5 135.75 g, 95% CI, 47.85–223.68).121 Multiple
infants receiving zinc were less likely to be stunted than reviews have studied maternal folate supplementation or
infants not getting zinc (OR 5 0.80; 95% CI, 0.64–1.0, p 5 intake and effects on cognition and neuro-
.049). Another interesting finding in this analysis was that development.26,122–124 A systematic review on folic acid

670 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
supplementation/intake122 during pregnancy on neuro- significantly greater in the infants of supplemented
developmental and autism outcomes found 22 studies (2 mothers compared with nonsupplemented group (MD:
RCTs, 18 cohorts, and 2 case-control studies) of which 0.41; 95% CI, 0.18–0.64).
15 showed a beneficial effect, 6 showed no association,
whereas 1 showed a harmful effect. A large retrospective Breastfeeding Promotion Activities
study125 based on 1988 National Maternal Infant Health Human breast milk contains 3 to 5% fat, 0.8 to 0.9%
Survey and the 1991 follow-up supplement in the United protein, 6.9 to 7.2% carbohydrates, and 0.2% mineral
States studied the effect of prenatal folic acid use on early constituents that include sodium, potassium, calcium,
child development. Folate use was associated with sig- magnesium, phosphorus, and chloride.133 Proteins in
nificant reduced odds for moderate risk on gross motor breast milk also include immunoglobulin A, lysozyme,
scale (OR 5 0.5), but was also associated with marginally and albumin related to child’s immune function. There
significant increased odds for moderate risk on the are numerous observational studies linking breast milk
personal-social scale (OR 5 1.8). A population-based to higher scores on cognitive tests and in motor and
cohort study—The Generation R Study—prospectively mental development, especially, in LBW or preterm
assessed maternal folic acid supplement use during the infants134–136 because of breast milk being rich in long-
first trimester by questionnaire.126 Child outcomes were chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs). Studies
assessed at 18 months of age in 4214 toddlers. Results have supported the hypothesis that breast milk promotes
showed that children of mothers who did not use folic brain development, particularly white matter growth.137
acid supplements in the first trimester had a higher risk of Exclusive breastfeeding for the first 6 months is of prime
total behavioral problems (OR 5 1.44; 95% CI, 1.12–1.86). importance as it reduces the incidence of infections,138
Folic acid supplement use also significantly protected but evidence on its effect on child’s growth is conflict-
from both internalizing and externalizing problems, even ing. Some studies report better growth with formula
when adjusted for maternal characteristics.126 milk, whereas others report that breast milk is sufficient
Folate Use in Infancy/Childhood for adequate growth during the first 6 months of life.139
A study by Olney et al.127 showed that combined iron Some studies have also reported higher weight gains
and folic acid supplementation with or without zinc with exclusive breastfeeding in early days of life that
from 5 to 7 months of age in children was associated then transforms in favor of formula milk by 6 to 7 months
with earlier initiation of walking by approximately 1 of life.138,140 Breast milk also contains adiponectin,
month than in children without iron/folate (p 5 .020). a protein that is associated with lower obesity,141 along
Supplementation with MMNs and long-chain poly- with other hormones and growth factors, namely ghre-
unsaturated fatty acids is discussed in the online Sup- lin, resistin, and obestatin that are involved in food intake
plemental Material (Supplemental Digital Content 1, regulation and energy balance.142 It may also, therefore,
http://links.lww.com/JDBP/A143). have a role in reducing obesity. The World Health Or-
ganization currently recommends exclusive breastfeed-
Supplementation with Other Individual ing up to 6 months of age after which complementary
Micronutrients i.e. Vitamin A and Vitamin D foods should be introduced and breastfeeding then
A meta-analysis by Ramakrishnan et al.32 on vitamin A continued up to 2 years of age.143
supplementation on child growth in children under 5 years Multiple reviews on the effect of breastfeeding pro-
of age was based on 17 studies with 19 data sets. The motion activities on child growth exist.144,145 Based on
overall weighted mean effect size for change in height was 16 to 17 data sets between 2006 and 2014, breastfeeding
small and not significant (0.08; 95% CI, 20.18 to 0.34). The promotion had no impact on weight or height z-scores
weighted mean effect size for weight gain was 20.03; 95% but had a modest, significant reduction in body mass
CI, 20.23 to 0.18. Semba et al.128 reported the effect of index (BMI) or WHZ scores in studies from resource-
expanded national vitamin A program among preschool poor countries. However, Kramer et al.146 from their
children in India. The prevalence of stunting, severe large PROBIT trial included in this meta-analysis reported
stunting, underweight, and severe underweight was higher no significant effects of breastfeeding promotion in-
among children who did not receive vitamin A compared tervention on height, BMI, waist or hip circumference,
with those who received it (p , .0001). triceps, or subscapular skinfold thickness. The authors
Vitamin D is another vitamin that is thought to play an present the latest evidence of the effect of prolonged and
important role in brain development and functioning and exclusive breastfeeding on child’s cognitive ability at 6.5
its deficiency may affect growth and development po- years of age.147 The intervention was based on the Baby-
tential of infants and children. Meta-analyses of vitamin D Friendly Hospital Initiative, which was developed by the
supplementation during pregnancy report significantly WHO and UNICEF, whereas the control maternity hos-
greater birth weight and birth length in neonates by pitals and polyclinics continued the practices and poli-
107.6 and 0.3 cm, respectively,129 and reduction in risk cies in effect at the time of randomization. A total of
of LBW.130 A Cochrane review on vitamin D supple- 17,046 healthy breastfeeding infants were enrolled, of
mentation in pregnancy131 reported infant weight at 12 whom 13,889 (81.5%) were followed-up at age 6.5 years.
months of age based on 1 study.132 The weight was The experimental group had higher means on all of the

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 671
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence measures, programs and conditional cash transfers are discussed in
with cluster-adjusted mean difference of 17.5 (10.8 to the online Supplemental Material (Supplemental Digital
114.3) for verbal IQ that was statistically significant. Content 1, http://links.lww.com/JDBP/A143).
Other means for performance IQ and full scale IQ were
higher in the intervention group as well, but non- Integrated Nutrition and Psychosocial
significant. Teachers’ academic ratings were significantly Interventions
higher in the experimental group for both reading and Malnutrition can have a major influence on a child’s
writing. The same authors in another article148 report no behavioral development; for example, iron-deficient
significant treatment effects on either the mother or the children are irritable and tired, and protein deficiency
teacher, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire ratings can lead to lethargy and unhappiness among children
of total difficulties, emotional symptoms, conduct prob- that can reduce their interaction with parents or sib-
lems, hyperactivity, peer problems, or prosocial behav- lings.162,163 Research has shown that in combined nu-
ior or on the supplemental behavioral questions. trition and psychosocial interventions, the psychosocial
support provided by increased stimulation can have
Complementary/Preschool Feeding greater effects on psychological functioning than on
To sustain optimal growth and development of infants physical growth, whereas nutrition supplementation
and children is important for their future and also for would improve both growth and development. The
development of societies in which they live.149 Common combination of stimulation and supplement inter-
factors causing malnutrition in infancy include in- ventions appears to have a greater effect on cognitive
appropriate feeding practices such as lack of exclusive development than either one alone.164 Similar results
breastfeeding and premature introduction of weaning have been shown by large-scale programs including
foods. Later on during childhood, inadequate diets in Integrated Child Development Services (ICDS) Project
quantity and quality are important causes.150 Studies have in India165 and Head Start in the United States166 and
linked household food insecurity with risk of stunting and Chile.167 Other programs include PANDAI (Child De-
underweight in preschool children,151 and also with velopment and Mother’s Care) Project in Indonesia,
negative effects on development and learning poten- PRONOEI in Peru, Hogares Comunitarios de Bienestar
tial.152,153 Similarly, household food security has been as- (Homes of Well-Being) in Columbia, Programa de Ali-
sociated with greater growth among children.154 mentacao de Pre-escolar (PROAPE) in Brazil and In-
Complementary/supplementary feeding interventions are tegrated Programme for Child and Family Development
usually targeted to children aged 6 to 24 months because (IFBECD), and Family Development Programme (FCP)
this is the period of peak incidence of growth faltering, in Thailand. The aim of ICDS in India165 was to improve
micronutrient deficiencies, and infectious illnesses.7 The nutritional and health status of children under age 6.
preschool years (i.e., 1–5 years of age) is also a time of This program combined supplementary feeding for
rapid and dramatic postnatal brain development (i.e., children with health and nutrition education. The pro-
neural plasticity), and of fundamental acquisition of cog- gram had an impact on physical growth of children as
nitive development (i.e., working memory, attention, and evidenced by lower levels of undernutrition in ICDS
inhibitory control). A food-based, comprehensive ap- areas. Children who attended the program regularly also
proach may be more effective and sustainable than pro- scored higher on cognitive tests than nonparticipants.
grams targeting individual nutrient deficiencies. The Head Start program in the United States166 was
A systematic review by Imdad et al. showed significant started to provide comprehensive child development
positive effects/gains of complementary feeding with or services for low-income children and their families. It
without nutrition counseling and nutrition counseling enrolled children aged 3 to 5 years. Snacks and pre-
alone on weight (weighted mean difference 5 0.34 SD, school education were provided, along with health
0.11–0.56; 0.30 SD, 0.05–0.54) and height (0.26 SD, 0.08– care services. No impact was seen on height-for-age,
0.43; 0.21 SD, 0.01–0.41).155 Similar positive findings have but positive effects were noted on the Peabody
been shown for food supplementation in socioeconomi- Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). Effects persisted into
cally disadvantaged children aged 3 months to 5 years in and translated into improved school attainment for
another review156 and in other reviews in children under white and Hispanic children but not African-American
2 years of age157,158 in resource-poor countries. However, children.
in a review published earlier, Dewey and Adu-Afarwuah7
on complementary feeding interventions found that pro- Nutrition Plus Responsive Care/Responsive Feeding
vision of complementary foods (often fortified) had Aboud et al.168 using a cluster randomized field trial in
a positive, but nonsignificant, impact on weight and linear Bangladesh evaluated a 6-session educational program of
growth. In this review, 4 efficacy trials provided data on responsive complementary feeding that emphasized
behavioral development. The provision of a fat-based child self-feeding and maternal responsiveness. A total of
fortified food product or micronutrients alone improved 108 mothers and their 8- to 20-month-old children
gross motor development in Ghana,159 but had no signif- attended the sessions, whereas 95 mothers and children
icant impact in South Africa160 or India.161 School feeding served as controls and received sessions on foods to feed

672 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
without any intervention of responsive complementary 6 to 24 months. The undernourished intervened children
feeding. At follow-up, the percent of self-fed mouthfuls (intervention for 12 months) showed significantly greater
and the number of responsive verbalizations were sig- improvements in their MDI (4.6 6 2.0; p , .01), response
nificantly higher in the intervention group compared to examiner (p 5 .001), and cooperation (p 5 .005)
with controls. Mouthfuls of food eaten by children and compared with undernourished controls. Change in PDI
weight were equivalent in the 2 groups. (3.5 6 2.2; p 5 .06), emotional tone (p 5 .03), maternal
knowledge about rearing (p , .001), and vocalization
Nutrition Plus Stimulation (p 5 .04) also approached significance between the 2
Gardner et al. assessed the effects of zinc supple- groups. Two other efficacy studies on this topic found
mentation and psychosocial stimulation given together from the WHO document164 include 1 from Bogota,
or separately on the psychomotor development of un- Colombia, and the other from Cali, Colombia. In the
dernourished Jamaican children.169 Zinc was given as 10 Bogota study on undernourished children, there was
mg elemental zinc daily for 6 months, whereas the psy- no added benefit of the combined supplementation-
chosocial stimulation program focused on improving stimulation interventions on psychological development,
maternal-child interactions. There were 4 groups in this but it had a greater effect on physical growth than sup-
study: zinc and stimulation, zinc alone, stimulation alone, plementation alone.175 The study from Cali showed that
and a control group. Developmental quotient and all combined supplementation and stimulation had the
subscale scores declined in all the 4 groups after 6 greatest effects on psychological development, whereas
months of the study period probably because the chil- supplementation alone (before initiating the preschool)
dren were from poor, disadvantaged backgrounds where had no effect on psychological development.176
other micronutrient deficiencies may coexist. In multi-
level model, children who received both zinc and stim-
ulation had the highest developmental quotients after DISCUSSION/CONCLUSIONS
the intervention. There was no main effect of zinc sup- This review has great implications for the promotion
plementation alone, although stimulation alone also had of child growth and development, especially in
a significant effect on development quotient. There was resource-poor countries. There are some clear messages
also a significant interaction between zinc and stimula- emanating from this review. It highlights the role of
tion on the hand and eye subscale that indicated that micronutrients and other nutrition interventions like
children having both treatments had the highest scores. breastfeeding and complementary feeding. Iron folate is
Zinc alone, however, also benefited the hand and eye routinely used during pregnancy in resource-poor
subscale. Stimulation alone had significant main effects countries and its role in preventing and treating ane-
on hearing and speech and performance. Neither in- mia is well established, preventing numerous compli-
tervention benefited locomotor development. cations in the mother and newborn. Folate during
Another recent study by the same group170–173 ran- pregnancy is also well known for preventing neural
domly assigned 129 stunted children aged 9 to 24 months tube defects and should be given periconceptionally as
to 4 groups: control, nutrition supplemented (milk-based well. Almost no studies have assessed the impact of iron
formula), stimulated, or both treatments in poor Kingston and/or folate during pregnancy on short- and long-term
(Jamaica) neighborhoods. A fifth group of nonstunted development of children, possibly because of its proven
children matched for baseline characteristics was also beneficial role in anemia and further randomized con-
studied. After 2 years, supplementation and stimulation trolled trials (RCTs) being unethical. There is a debate
had independent benefits on the children’s development currently on replacing iron folate with multiple micro-
and the effects were additive. The group receiving both nutrients (MMNs) during pregnancy in resource-poor
treatments caught up to the nonstunted children. Four countries. A major concern is increase in birth weight
years after the end of the 2-year intervention, a battery of in the setting of limited cesarean facilities that may lead
cognitive function, school achievement, and fine motor to increased mortality later in pregnancy i.e., during
tests were performed. The perceptual-motor factor labor. Besides this, other procedures such as symphy-
showed a significant benefit from stimulation, and sup- siotomies during delivery can be debilitating and
plementation benefitted only those children whose subsequent impaired maternal health can affect
mothers had higher verbal IQs. However, each in- child development balancing out the benefits of
tervention group had higher scores than the stunted a well-nourished child. More evidence for MMNs sup-
control subjects on more tests than would be expected by plementation is needed in remote or rural areas of
chance (supplemented and both groups on 14 of 15 tests, resource-poor countries before a recommendation for
p 5 .002; stimulated group in 13 of 15 tests, p 5 .01). universal use can be made. Iodine should be given to all
There was no longer an additive effect of combined mothers in iodine-deficient areas, mostly through the
treatments at this 4-year follow-up. The third study we fortification of salt. Routine use of zinc during
found174 looked at the addition of psychosocial stimula- pregnancy is not recommended because of null results
tion to the nutrition interventions (food packets) for on newborn or other growth or developmental out-
malnutrition in undernourished Bangladeshi children aged comes. There are no separate data evaluating zinc

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 673
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
supplementation in zinc-deficient mothers. Studies are nutrition supplementation would improve both growth
needed, particularly in resource-poor countries. and development. Studies have shown that combination
Iron should be supplemented to all iron-deficient of stimulation and supplement interventions appears to
children, and mass supplementation is not recom- have a greater effect on cognitive development than ei-
mended because it does not appear to have utility for ther one alone.164 Similar results have been shown by
improving growth or developmental outcomes in iron- large-scale programs in different countries including In-
replete children. However, iodine should be provided to tegrated Child Development Services (ICDS) Project in
all children in iodine-deficient areas. Data on the impact India165 and Head Start in the United States.166 Therefore,
of preventive zinc on growth are conflicting. Before nutrition and psychosocial interventions should be com-
large-scale recommendation for routine preventive sup- bined to achieve optimal benefits in large-scale programs.
plementation is made, its impact on other outcomes The need of the hour is to translate these recom-
such as mortality and morbidity, specifically because of mendations into large-scale, community-based public
infections, needs to be studied. More RCTs are needed to health programs and increase their coverage, especially
assess the impact of zinc on growth, especially height, for the most impoverished pregnant women and chil-
among children who are anemic or are stunted. The role dren in resource-poor countries. Other research oppor-
of vitamin A supplementation in children in preventing tunities also exist. More evidence is needed for MMNs
all-cause and infection-specific mortality have been well supplements compared with iron folate, particularly in
demonstrated in recent reviews.177 It should be part of regions that do not have appropriate health care delivery
large-scale programs for children at risk despite con- facilities. Zinc and vitamin D supplementation in preg-
flicting results on growth. It is hypothesized that by re- nancy may be important for women deficient in these
ducing incidence of infections like diarrhea and measles, nutrients but current evidence does not show any con-
it should have a positive effect on growth and de- vincing benefits for child growth and development. Al-
velopment as well. There is only 1 study evaluating the though we recommend vitamin A supplementation for
use of folic acid for child development outcomes and children at risk in resource-poor countries, its effects on
there is no substantial evidence to recommend it rou- growth are conflicting. Furthermore, there are no studies
tinely with iron in iron-deficient children. of vitamin A supplementation evaluating developmental
All children should be exclusively breastfed up to the outcomes. Observational studies could be done to ob-
first 6 months of life based on its protective effect on serve effects on these outcomes because according to
infections and also optimal growth, despite some studies new evidence,177 further RCTs of vitamin A supple-
on growth being conflicting. It is important that weaning mentation would be unethical in children. Zinc supple-
foods be started in the right quantity and quality to have mentation in children also has a potential role to play,
positive impacts on child growth and development, with particularly in preventing stunting, but more evidence is
breast milk continued up to 2 years of age. More data are needed before it can be recommended routinely. Its role,
needed on the impact of complementary feeding inter- however, in preventing infections like diarrhea and
ventions on early child development. Provision of com- pneumonia in children is strong.178
plementary foods also had a positive impact on growth. In conclusion, micronutrient supplementation plays
With maternal education for complementary feeding, also a significant role in child growth and development either
there was an additional increase in weight and height. during pregnancy or early childhood. In making policy
Studies from Africa and South Asia generally showed recommendations or guidelines, it is important to rec-
positive results for growth, whereas those in other regions ognize that micronutrients also provide benefits beyond
were more variable. This could be explained by higher those of growth and development. This can potentially
food insecurity rates in Africa and South Asia. Other include reduction in incidence of infections and mortal-
interventions such as managing poverty and improving ity such as for zinc and vitamin A supplementation in
sanitation and health care need to be coupled with com- children. This review has highlighted current evidence
plementary feeding interventions to have a more sub- and further areas of research opportunities related to
stantial impact on child growth and development. The micronutrient deficiencies, and supplementation efforts
key challenge would, however, be to introduce high- to overcome these deficiencies.
quality programs that incorporate these interventions that
are sustainable when delivered on a large scale. REFERENCES
It should be noted here that research has shown that
1. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, et al. Developmental
nutrition interventions combined with psychosocial potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries.
interventions such as parent psychosocial stimulation, Lancet. 2007;369:60–70.
parenting programs, caregiver responsiveness, etc. can 2. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk
have an additive or even synergistic effects on child de- factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet.
2007;369:145–157.
velopment. In combined nutrition and psychosocial
3. Benton D. The influence of dietary status on the cognitive
interventions, the psychosocial support provided by in- performance of children. Mol Nutr Food Res. 2010;54:457–470.
creased stimulation can have greater effects on psycho- 4. Abu-Saad K, Fraser D. Maternal nutrition and birth outcomes.
logical functioning than on physical growth, whereas Epidemiol Rev. 2010;32:5–25.

674 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
5. Innis SM. Nutrition and its Impact on Psychosocial Child maternal distress in Bangladesh. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90:
Development: Preterm Infants. British Columbia, Canada: 141–148.
University of British Columbia; 2003. Available at: http://www. 28. Zhou SJ, Gibson RA, Crowther CA, et al. Effect of iron
child-encyclopedia.com/documents/InnisANGxp.pdf. Accessed supplementation during pregnancy on the intelligence quotient
July 7, 2017. and behavior of children at 4 y of age: long-term follow-up of
6. Dekaban AS. Changes in brain weights during the span of human a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:1112–
life: relation of brain weights to body heights and body weights. 1117.
Ann Neurol. 1978;4:345–356. 29. Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternal multimicronutrient
7. Dewey KG, Adu-Afarwuah S. Systematic review of the efficacy and supplementation on the mental development of infants in rural
effectiveness of complementary feeding interventions in western China: follow-up evaluation of a double-blind,
developing countries. Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4(suppl 1):24–85. randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e685–92.
8. Martins VJ, Toledo Florencio TM, Grillo LP, et al. Long-lasting 30. Petry N, Olofin I, Boy E, et al. The effect of low dose iron and zinc
effects of undernutrition. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2011;8: intake on child micronutrient status and development during the
1817–1846. first 1000 Days of life: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
9. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, et al. Early childhood stunting is Nutrients. 2016;8:E773.
associated with poor psychological functioning in late 31. Ramakrishnan U, Aburto N, McCabe G, et al. Multimicronutrient
adolescence and effects are reduced by psychosocial stimulation. interventions but not vitamin a or iron interventions alone
J Nutr. 2007;137:2464–2469. improve child growth: results of 3 meta-analyses. J Nutr. 2004;
10. Martorell R, Rivera J, Kaplowitz H, et al. Long-term consequences of 134:2592–2602.
growth retardation during early childhood. In: Hernandez M, Argente 32. Ramakrishnan U, Nguyen P, Martorell R. Effects of micronutrients
J, eds. Human Growth: Basic and Clinical Aspects. Amsterdam, the on growth of children under 5 y of age: meta-analyses of single and
Netherlands: Elsevier Science Publishers BV; 1992:143–149. multiple nutrient interventions. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:191–203.
11. Daniels MC, Adair LS. Growth in young Filipino children predicts 33. McDonagh M, Cantor A, Bougatsos C, et al. Routine Iron
schooling trajectories through high school. J Nutr. 2004;134: Supplementation and Screening for Iron Deficiency Anemia in
1439–1446. Pregnant Women: A Systematic Review to Update the US
12. Reed S, Neuman H, Moscovich S, et al. Chronic zinc deficiency Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation. Rockville, MD:
alters chick gut microbiota composition and function. Nutrients. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Evidence Syntheses, formerly
2015;7:9768–9784. Systematic Evidence Reviews; 2015.
13. Biesalski HK. Nutrition meets the microbiome: micronutrients and 34. Baumgartner J, Barth-Jaeggi T. Iron interventions in children from
the microbiota. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2016;1372:53–64. low-income and middle-income populations: benefits and risks.
14. Bentley ME, Wasser HM, Creed-Kanashiro HM. Responsive feeding Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2015;18:289–294.
and child undernutrition in low- and middle-income countries. 35. Pasricha SR, Hayes E, Kalumba K, et al. Effect of daily iron
J Nutr. 2011;141:502–507. supplementation on health in children aged 4-23 months:
15. Shloim N, Edelson LR, Martin N, et al. Parenting styles, feeding a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled
styles, feeding practices, and weight status in 4-12 Year-old children: trials. Lancet Glob Health. 2013;1:e77–86.
a systematic review of the literature. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1849. 36. Thompson J, Biggs BA, Pasricha SR. Effects of daily iron
16. Waber DP, Bryce CP, Fitzmaurice GM, et al. Neuropsychological supplementation in 2- to 5-year-old children: systematic review
outcomes at midlife following moderate to severe malnutrition in and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2013;131:739–753.
infancy. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:530–540. 37. Vucic V, Berti C, Vollhardt C, et al. Effect of iron intervention on
17. Waber DP, Bryce CP, Girard JM, et al. Impaired IQ and academic growth during gestation, infancy, childhood, and adolescence:
skills in adults who experienced moderate to severe infantile a systematic review with meta-analysis. Nutr Rev. 2013;71:386–
malnutrition: a 40-year study. Nutr Neurosci. 2014;17:58–64. 401.
18. Lozoff B, Smith JB, Kaciroti N, et al. Functional significance of 38. Sachdev H, Gera T, Nestel P. Effect of iron supplementation on
early-life iron deficiency: outcomes at 25 years. J Pediatr. 2013; physical growth in children: systematic review of randomised
163:1260–1266. controlled trials. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9:904–920.
19. Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Su J, et al. Iron deficiency anemia in 39. Iannotti LL, Tielsch JM, Black MM, et al. Iron supplementation in
infancy and reach and grasp development. Infant Behav Dev. early childhood: health benefits and risks. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;
2009;32:366–375. 84:1261–1276.
20. Monga M, Walia V, Gandhi A, et al. Effect of iron deficiency 40. Gahagan S, Yu S, Kaciroti N, et al. Linear and ponderal growth
anemia on visual evoked potential of growing children. Brain trajectories in well-nourished, iron-sufficient infants are
Dev. 2010;32:213–216. unimpaired by iron supplementation. J Nutr. 2009;139:2106–
21. Thomas DG, Grant SL, Aubuchon-Endsley NL. The role of iron in 2112.
neurocognitive development. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:196–222. 41. Fischer Walker CL, Baqui AH, Ahmed S, et al. Low-dose weekly
22. Santos JN, Lemos SM, Rates SP, et al. Hearing abilities and supplementation of iron and/or zinc does not affect growth
language development in anemic children of a public daycare among Bangladeshi infants. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63:87–92.
center. Pro Fono. 2008;20:255–260. 42. Lind T, Seswandhana R, Persson LA, et al. Iron supplementation of
23. Lozoff B. Iron deficiency and child development. Food Nutr Bull. iron-replete Indonesian infants is associated with reduced weight-
2007;28(4 suppl):S560–S571. for-age. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97:770–775.
24. McCann JC, Ames BN. An overview of evidence for a causal relation 43. Ziegler EE, Nelson SE, Jeter JM. Iron status of breastfed infants is
between iron deficiency during development and deficits in improved equally by medicinal iron and iron-fortified cereal. Am J
cognitive or behavioral function. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:931–945. Clin Nutr. 2009;90:76–87.
25. Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. 44. Low M, Farrell A, Biggs BA, et al. Effects of daily iron
Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2006;13:158–165. supplementation in primary-school-aged children: systematic
26. Chmielewska A, Dziechciarz P, Gieruszczak-Bialek D, et al. Effects review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. CMAJ.
of prenatal and/or postnatal supplementation with iron, PUFA or 2013;185:E791–E802.
folic acid on neurodevelopment: update. Br J Nutr. 2016. [Epub 45. Hermoso M, Vucic V, Vollhardt C, et al. The effect of iron on
ahead of print]. cognitive development and function in infants, children and
27. Frith AL, Naved RT, Ekstrom EC, et al. Micronutrient adolescents: a systematic review. Ann Nutr Metab. 2011;59:
supplementation affects maternal-infant feeding interactions and 154–165.

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 675
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
46. Lam LF, Lawlis TR. Feeding the brain - the effects of micronutrient 63. Zhou SJ, Anderson AJ, Gibson RA, et al. Effect of iodine
interventions on cognitive performance among school-aged supplementation in pregnancy on child development and other
children: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Clin clinical outcomes: a systematic review of randomized controlled
Nutr. 2017;36:1007–1014. trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98:1241–1254.
47. Baumgartner J, Smuts CM, Malan L, et al. Effects of iron and n-3 64. Bougma K, Aboud FE, Harding KB, et al. Iodine and mental
fatty acid supplementation, alone and in combination, on development of children 5 years old and under: a systematic
cognition in school children: a randomized, double-blind, placebo- review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2013;5:1384–1416.
controlled intervention in South Africa. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96: 65. Henrichs J, Ghassabian A, Peeters RP, et al. Maternal
1327–1338. hypothyroxinemia and effects on cognitive functioning in
48. Sungthong R, Mo-Suwan L, Chongsuvivatwong V, et al. Once- childhood: how and why? Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2013;79:152–162.
weekly and 5-days a week iron supplementation differentially 66. Kevany J, Fierro-Benitez R, Pretell EA, et al. Prophylaxis and
affect cognitive function but not school performance in Thai treatment of endemic goiter with iodized oil in rural Ecuador and
children. J Nutr. 2004;134:2349–2354. Peru. Am J Clin Nutr. 1969;22:1597–1607.
49. Sachdev H, Gera T, Nestel P. Effect of iron supplementation 67. Pretell EA, Caceres A. Impairment of mental development by
on mental and motor development in children: systematic review iodine deficiency and its correction. In: Stanbury JB, ed. The
of randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8: Damaged Brain of Iodine Deficiency. New York, NY: Cognizant
117–132. Communication; 1994. 187–191.
50. Szajewska H, Ruszczynski M, Chmielewska A. Effects of 68. Pretell EA, Torres T, Zenteno V, et al. Prophylaxis of endemic
iron supplementation in nonanemic pregnant women, infants, goiter with iodized oil in rural Peru: preliminary report of hte
and young children on the mental performance and effect on the development of newborn children. In: STanbury JB,
psychomotor development of children: a systematic review Kroch RL, eds. Human Development and Thyroid Gland. New
of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91: York, NY: Plenum Publishing Corporation; 1972:246–265.
1684–1690. 69. Connolly KJ, Pharoah PO, Hetzel BS. Fetal iodine deficiency and
51. Guo XM, Liu H, Qian J. Daily iron supplementation on motor performance during childhood. Lancet. 1979;2:1149–1151.
cognitive performance in primary-school-aged children with and 70. Pharoah PO, Connolly KJ. Effects of maternal iodine supplementation
without anemia: a meta-analysis. Int J Clin Exp Med. 2015;8: during pregnancy. Arch Dis Child. 1991;66:145–147.
16107–16111. 71. Pharoah PO, Connolly KJ. A controlled trial of iodinated oil for the
52. Logan S, Martins S, Gilbert R. Iron therapy for improving prevention of endemic cretinism: a long-term follow-up. Int J
psychomotor development and cognitive function in children Epidemiol. 1987;16:68–73.
under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane 72. Pharoah PO, Buttfield IH, Hetzel BS. Neurological damage to the
Database Syst Rev. 2001;CD001444. fetus resulting from severe iodine deficiency during pregnancy.
53. Wang B, Zhan S, Gong T, et al. Iron therapy for improving Lancet. 1971;1:308–310.
psychomotor development and cognitive function in children 73. Lazarus JH, Bestwick JP, Channon S, et al. Antenatal thyroid
under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane screening and childhood cognitive function. N Engl J Med. 2012;
Database Syst Rev. 2013;CD001444. 366:493–501.
54. de Escobar GM, Obregon MJ, del Rey FE. Iodine deficiency and 74. Casey BM, Thom EA, Peaceman AM, et al. Treatment of subclinical
brain development in the first half of pregnancy. Public Health hypothyroidism or hypothyroxinemia in pregnancy. N Engl J Med.
Nutr. 2007;10:1554–1570. 2017;376:815–825.
55. Trumpff C, De Schepper J, Tafforeau J, et al. Mild iodine 75. Santiago P, Velasco I, Muela JA, et al. Infant neurocognitive
deficiency in pregnancy in Europe and its consequences for development is independent of the use of iodised salt or iodine
cognitive and psychomotor development of children: a review. supplements given during pregnancy. Br J Nutr. 2013;110:
J Trace Elem Med Biol. 2013;27:174–183. 831–839.
56. Muela Martinez JA, Garcia Leon A, Torres Barahona R, et al. Effects 76. Velasco I, Carreira M, Santiago P, et al. Effect of iodine prophylaxis
of the iodine deficiency on intellectual variables among children. during pregnancy on neurocognitive development of children
Psicothema. 2008;20:279–284. during the first two years of life. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;
57. Zhukov AO. Mental development disorders and attention-deficit 94:3234–3241.
syndrome caused by iodine deficiency: a clinical and 77. Berbel P, Mestre JL, Santamaria A, et al. Delayed neurobehavioral
epidemiological study. Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. development in children born to pregnant women with mild
2007;107:4–16. hypothyroxinemia during the first month of gestation: the
58. Pineda-Lucatero A, Avila-Jimenez L, Ramos-Hernandez RI, et al. importance of early iodine supplementation. Thyroid. 2009;19:
Iodine deficiency and its association with intelligence quotient in 511–519.
schoolchildren from Colima, Mexico. Public Health Nutr. 2008; 78. Brucker-Davis F, Ganier-Chauliac F, Gal J, et al. Neurotoxicant
11:690–698. exposure during pregnancy is a confounder for assessment of
59. Taylor PN, Okosieme OE, Dayan CM, et al. Therapy of endocrine iodine supplementation on neurodevelopment outcome.
disease: impact of iodine supplementation in mild-to-moderate Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2015;51:45–51.
iodine deficiency: systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J 79. Murcia M, Rebagliato M, Iniguez C, et al. Effect of iodine
Endocrinol. 2014;170:R1–R15. supplementation during pregnancy on infant neurodevelopment
60. Farebrother J, Naude CE, Nicol L, et al. Iodised salt and iodine at 1 year of age. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173:804–812.
supplements for prenatal and postnatal growth: a rapid scoping of 80. Rebagliato M, Murcia M, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, et al. Iodine
existing systematic reviews. Nutr J. 2015;14:89. supplementation during pregnancy and infant neuropsychological
61. Farebrother J, Naude CE, Nicol L, et al. Systematic review of development. INMA Mother and Child Cohort Study. Am J
the effects of iodised salt and iodine supplements on prenatal Epidemiol. 2013;177:944–953.
and postnatal growth: study protocol. BMJ Open. 2015;5: 81. Angermayr L, Clar C. Iodine supplementation for preventing
e007238. iodine deficiency disorders in children. Cochrane Database Syst
62. Donnay S, Arena J, Lucas A, et al. Iodine supplementation during Rev. 2004:CD003819.
pregnancy and lactation. Position statement of the working group 82. Bautista A, Barker PA, Dunn JT, et al. The effects of oral iodized oil
on disorders related to iodine deficiency and thyroid dysfunction on intelligence, thyroid status, and somatic growth in school-age
of the Spanish Society of Endocrinology and Nutrition. Endocrinol children from an area of endemic goiter. Am J Clin Nutr. 1982;35:
Nutr. 2014;61:27–34. 127–134.

676 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
83. Furnee CA. Prevention and Control of Iodine Deficiency: Studies 103. Hess SY, King JC. Effects of maternal zinc supplementation on
on the Efficacy of Oral Iodized Oil [doctoral thesis]. pregnancy and lactation outcomes. Food Nutr Bull. 2009;30
Wageningen, the Netherlands: Wageningen Agricultural (1 suppl):S60–S78.
University; 1994. 104. Osendarp SJ, West CE, Black RE. The need for maternal zinc
84. Shrestha RM. Effect of Iodine and Iron Supplementation on supplementation in developing countries: an unresolved issue.
Physical, Psychomotor and Mental Development in Primary J Nutr. 2003;133:817S–27S.
School Children in Malawi [doctoral thesis]. Wageningen, the 105. Brown KH, Peerson JM, Baker SK, et al. Preventive zinc
Netherlands: Wageningen Agricultural University; 1994. supplementation among infants, preschoolers, and older
85. Huda SN, Grantham-McGregor SM, Rahman KM, et al. prepubertal children. Food Nutr Bull. 2009;30(1 suppl):S12–S40.
Biochemical hypothyroidism secondary to iodine deficiency is 106. Brown KH, Peerson JM, Rivera J, et al. Effect of supplemental zinc
associated with poor school achievement and cognition in on the growth and serum zinc concentrations of prepubertal
Bangladeshi children. J Nutr. 1999;129:980–987. children: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am J
86. Todd CH, Dunn JT. Intermittent oral administration of potassium Clin Nutr. 2002;75:1062–1071.
iodide solution for the correction of iodine deficiency. Am J Clin 107. Nissensohn M, Sanchez-Villegas A, Fuentes Lugo D, et al. Effect of
Nutr. 1998;67:1279–1283. zinc intake on growth in infants: a meta-analysis. Crit Rev Food Sci
87. Zimmermann MB, Jooste PL, Mabapa NS, et al. Treatment of iodine Nutr. 2016;56:350–363.
deficiency in school-age children increases insulin-like growth factor 108. Stammers AL, Lowe NM, Medina MW, et al. The relationship
(IGF)-1 and IGF binding protein-3 concentrations and improves between zinc intake and growth in children aged 1-8 years:
somatic growth. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2007;92:437–442. a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2015;69:
88. Pearce EN. Iodine deficiency in children. Endocr Dev. 2014;26: 147–153.
130–138. 109. Imdad A, Bhutta ZA. Effect of preventive zinc supplementation on
89. Untoro J. Use of Oral Iodized Oil to Control Iodine Deficiency in linear growth in children under 5 years of age in developing
Indonesia [doctoral thesis]. Wageningen, the Netherlands: countries: a meta-analysis of studies for input to the lives saved
Wageningen Agricultural University; 1999. tool. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(suppl 3):S22.
90. Gordon RC, Rose MC, Skeaff SA, et al. Iodine supplementation 110. Mayo-Wilson E, Imdad A, Junior J, et al. Preventive zinc
improves cognition in mildly iodine-deficient children. Am J Clin supplementation for children, and the effect of additional iron:
Nutr. 2009;90:1264–1271. a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2014;4:
91. Zimmermann MB, Connolly K, Bozo M, et al. Iodine e004647.
supplementation improves cognition in iodine-deficient 111. Mayo-Wilson E, Junior JA, Imdad A, et al. Zinc supplementation for
schoolchildren in Albania: a randomized, controlled, double-blind preventing mortality, morbidity, and growth failure in children
study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:108–114. aged 6 months to 12 years of age. Cochrane Database Syst Rev.
92. Bailey RL, West KP Jr, Black RE. The epidemiology of global 2014:CD009384.
micronutrient deficiencies. Ann Nutr Metab. 2015;66(suppl 2):22–33. 112. Taneja S, Strand TA, Sommerfelt H, et al. Zinc supplementation for
93. Black MM. The evidence linking zinc deficiency with children’s four months does not affect growth in young north Indian
cognitive and motor functioning. J Nutr. 2003;133(5 suppl 1): children. J Nutr. 2010;140:630–634.
1473S–6S. 113. Mozaffari-Khosravi H, Shakiba M, Eftekhari MH, et al. Effects of
94. Ota E, Mori R, Middleton P, et al. Zinc supplementation for zinc supplementation on physical growth in 2-5-year-old children.
improving pregnancy and infant outcome. Cochrane Database Biol Trace Elem Res. 2009;128:118–127.
Syst Rev. 2015:CD000230. 114. Dijkhuizen MA, Winichagoon P, Wieringa FT, et al. Zinc
95. Osendarp SJ, van Raaij JM, Arifeen SE, et al. A randomized, supplementation improved length growth only in anemic infants
placebo-controlled trial of the effect of zinc supplementation in a multi-country trial of iron and zinc supplementation in South-
during pregnancy on pregnancy outcome in Bangladeshi urban East Asia. J Nutr. 2008;138:1969–1975.
poor. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;71:114–119. 115. Nissensohn M, Sanchez-Villegas A, Fuentes Lugo D, et al. Effect of
96. Dijkhuizen MA, Wieringa FT. Vitamin a, Iron and Zinc zinc intake on mental and motor development in infants: a meta-
Deficiency in Indonesia: Micronutrient Interactions and Effects analysis. Int J Vitam Nutr Res. 2013;83:203–215.
of Supplementation [thesis]. Wageningen, the Netherlands: 116. Gogia S, Sachdev HS. Zinc supplementation for mental and motor
Wageningen University; 2001. development in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012:
97. Stewart CP, Christian P, LeClerq SC, et al. Antenatal CD007991.
supplementation with folic acid 1 iron 1 zinc improves linear 117. Heinig MJ, Brown KH, Lonnerdal B, et al. Zinc supplementation
growth and reduces peripheral adiposity in school-age children in does not affect growth, morbidity, or motor development of US
rural Nepal. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90:132–140. term breastfed infants at 4-10 mo of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84:
98. Iannotti LL, Zavaleta N, Leon Z, et al. Maternal zinc 594–601.
supplementation and growth in Peruvian infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 118. Reynolds E. Vitamin B12, folic acid, and the nervous system.
2008;88:154–160. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:949–960.
99. Warthon-Medina M, Moran VH, Stammers AL, et al. Zinc intake, 119. Gross RL, Newberne PM, Reid JVO. Adverse effects on infant
status and indices of cognitive function in adults and children: development associated with maternal folic acid deficiency. Nutr
a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2015;69: Rep Intern. 1974;10:241–248.
649–661. 120. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Chapman VR, et al. Folate status of
100. Mahomed K, Bhutta Z, Middleton P. Zinc supplementation for mothers during pregnancy and mental and psychomotor
improving pregnancy and infant outcome. Cochrane Database development of their children at five years of age. Pediatrics.
Syst Rev. 2007:CD000230. 2005;116:703–708.
101. Hamadani JD, Fuchs GJ, Osendarp SJ, et al. Zinc supplementation 121. Lassi ZS, Salam RA, Haider BA, et al. Folic acid supplementation
during pregnancy and effects on mental development and during pregnancy for maternal health and pregnancy outcomes.
behaviour of infants: a follow-up study. Lancet. 2002;360:290–294. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013:CD006896.
102. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Ramey SL, et al. Effect of zinc 122. Gao Y, Sheng C, Xie RH, et al. New perspective on impact of folic
supplementation of pregnant women on the mental and acid supplementation during pregnancy on
psychomotor development of their children at 5 y of age. Am J neurodevelopment/autism in the offspring children—a systematic
Clin Nutr. 2003;77:1512–1516. review. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0165626.

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 677
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
123. Veena SR, Gale CR, Krishnaveni GV, et al. Association between 143. World Health Organization. Exclusive Breastfeeding. Available at:
maternal nutritional status in pregnancy and offspring cognitive http://www.who.int/nutrition/topics/exclusive_breastfeeding/
function during childhood and adolescence; a systematic review. en/. Accessed March 21, 2017.
BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2016;16:220. 144. Giugliani ER, Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, et al. Effect of
124. Anjos T, Altmae S, Emmett P, et al. Nutrition and breastfeeding promotion interventions on child growth:
neurodevelopment in children: focus on NUTRIMENTHE project. a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104:
Eur J Nutr. 2013;52:1825–1842. 20–29.
125. Wehby GL, Murray JC. The effects of prenatal use of folic acid and 145. Delgado C, Matijasevich A. Breastfeeding up to two years of age or
other dietary supplements on early child development. Matern beyond and its influence on child growth and development:
Child Health J. 2008;12:180–187. a systematic review. Cad Saude Publica. 2013;29:243–256.
126. Roza SJ, van Batenburg-Eddes T, Steegers EA, et al. Maternal folic 146. Kramer MS, Matush L, Vanilovich I, et al. Effects of prolonged and
acid supplement use in early pregnancy and child behavioural exclusive breastfeeding on child height, weight, adiposity, and
problems: the Generation R Study. Br J Nutr. 2010;103:445–452. blood pressure at age 6.5 y: evidence from a large randomized
127. Olney DK, Pollitt E, Kariger PK, et al. Combined iron and folic trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:1717–1721.
acid supplementation with or without zinc reduces time to 147. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, et al. Breastfeeding and child
walking unassisted among Zanzibari infants 5- to 11-mo old. cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized
J Nutr. 2006;136:2427–2434. trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:578–584.
128. Semba RD, de Pee S, Sun K, et al. The role of expanded coverage of 148. Kramer MS, Fombonne E, Igumnov S, et al. Effects of prolonged
the national vitamin A program in preventing morbidity and mortality and exclusive breastfeeding on child behavior and maternal
among preschool children in India. J Nutr. 2010;140:208S–12S. adjustment: evidence from a large, randomized trial. Pediatrics.
129. Perez-Lopez FR, Pasupuleti V, Mezones-Holguin E, et al. Effect 2008;121:e435–40.
of vitamin D supplementation during pregnancy on maternal 149. Sguassero Y, de Onis M, Carroli G. Community-based
and neonatal outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis supplementary feeding for promoting the growth of young
of randomized controlled trials. Fertil Steril. 2015;103: children in developing countries. Cochrane Database Syst Rev.
1278–1288. e4. 2005:CD005039.
130. Thorne-Lyman A, Fawzi WW. Vitamin D during pregnancy and 150. World Health Organisation. A Critical Link: Interventions for
maternal, neonatal and infant health outcomes: a systematic Physical Growth and Psychological Development. A Review
review and meta-analysis. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2012;26 (Document WHO/CHS/CAH/99.3). Geneva, Switzerland: WHO;
(suppl 1):75–90. 1999.
131. Mahomed K, Gulmezoglu AM. Vitamin D supplementation in 151. Hackett M, Melgar-Quinonez H, Alvarez MC. Household food
pregnancy. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2000:CD000228. insecurity associated with stunting and underweight among
132. Brooke OG, Butters F, Wood C. Intrauterine vitamin D nutrition preschool children in Antioquia, Colombia. Rev Panam Salud
and postnatal growth in Asian infants. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). Publica. 2009;25:506–510.
1981;283:1024. 152. Hernandez DC, Jacknowitz A. Transient, but not persistent, adult
133. Jenness R. The composition of human milk. Semin Perinatol. food insecurity influences toddler development. J Nutr. 2009;139:
1979;3:225–239. 1517–1524.
134. Tanaka K, Kon N, Ohkawa N, et al. Does breastfeeding in the 153. Chilton M, Chyatte M, Breaux J. The negative effects of poverty &
neonatal period influence the cognitive function of very-low-birth- food insecurity on child development. Indian J Med Res. 2007;
weight infants at 5 years of age? Brain Dev. 2009;31:288–293. 126:262–272.
135. Eickmann SH, de Lira PI, Lima Mde C, et al. Breast feeding and 154. Saha KK, Frongillo EA, Alam DS, et al. Household food security is
mental and motor development at 12 months in a low-income associated with growth of infants and young children in rural
population in northeast Brazil. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2007; Bangladesh. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12:1556–1562.
21:129–137. 155. Imdad A, Yakoob MY, Bhutta ZA. Impact of maternal education
136. Dee DL, Li R, Lee LC, et al. Associations between breastfeeding about complementary feeding and provision of complementary
practices and young children’s language and motor skill foods on child growth in developing countries. BMC Public
development. Pediatrics. 2007;119(suppl 1):S92–S98. Health. 2011;11(suppl 3):S25.
137. Isaacs EB, Fischl BR, Quinn BT, et al. Impact of breast milk on IQ, 156. Kristjansson E, Francis DK, Liberato S, et al. Food
brain size and white matter development. Pediatr Res. 2010;67: supplementation for improving the physical and psychosocial
357–362. health of socio-economically disadvantaged children aged three
138. Rebhan B, Kohlhuber M, Schwegler U, et al. Breastfeeding months to five years. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015:
duration and exclusivity associated with infants’ health and CD009924.
growth: data from a prospective cohort study in Bavaria. 157. Lassi ZS, Das JK, Zahid G, et al. Impact of education and provision
Germany. Acta Paediatr. 2009;98:974–980. of complementary feeding on growth and morbidity in children
139. Otaigbe BE, Alikor EA, Nkanginieme KE. Growth pattern of less than 2 years of age in developing countries: a systematic
exclusively breastfed infants in the first six months of life: review. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(suppl 3):S13.
a study of babies delivered at the University of Port Harcourt 158. Sguassero Y, de Onis M, Bonotti AM, et al. Community-based
Teaching Hospital, Rivers State, Nigeria. Niger J Med. 2008;17: supplementary feeding for promoting the growth of children
317–323. under five years of age in low and middle income countries.
140. Spyrides MH, Struchiner CJ, Barbosa MT, et al. Effect of Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012:CD005039.
predominant breastfeeding duration on infant growth: 159. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Brown KH, et al. Randomized
a prospective study using nonlinear mixed effect models. comparison of 3 types of micronutrient supplements for
J Pediatr (Rio J). 2008;84:237–243. home fortification of complementary foods in Ghana: effects
141. Woo JG, Guerrero ML, Altaye M, et al. Human milk adiponectin is on growth and motor development. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:
associated with infant growth in two independent cohorts. 412–420.
Breastfeed Med. 2009;4:101–109. 160. Oelofse A, Van Raaij JM, Benade AJ, et al. The effect of
142. Savino F, Fissore MF, Liguori SA, et al. Can hormones contained a micronutrient-fortified complementary food on micronutrient
in mothers’ milk account for the beneficial effect of breast- status, growth and development of 6- to 12-month-old
feeding on obesity in children? Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2009;71: disadvantaged urban South African infants. Int J Food Sci Nutr.
757–765. 2003;54:399–407.

678 Nutrition in Child Growth and Development Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
161. DhingraP, MenonVP, SazawalS, et al, eds. Effect of Fortification of 170. Grantham-McGregor SM, Powell CA, Walker SP, et al. Nutritional
Milk with Zinc and Iron along with Vitamins C, E, A and supplementation, psychosocial stimulation, and mental
Selenium on Growth, Iron Status and Development in Preschool development of stunted children: the Jamaican Study. Lancet.
Children—A Community-based Double-masked Randomized 1991;338:1–5.
Trial. Report from the 2nd World Congress of Pediatric 171. Grantham-McGregor SM, Walker SP, Chang SM, et al. Effects of
Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition; July 3–7, 2004; Paris, early childhood supplementation with and without stimulation on
France. 2004. later development in stunted Jamaican children. Am J Clin Nutr.
162. KidsHealth. Iron-deficiency Anemia. Available at: http:// 1997;66:247–253.
kidshealth.org/en/parents/ida.html#. Accessed March 21, 2017. 172. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, et al. Effects of early childhood
163. Med-Health.net. Protein Deficiency Diseases. Available at: http:// psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on
www.med-health.net/Protein-Deficiency-Diseases.html. Accessed cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children:
March 21, 2017. prospective cohort study. Lancet. 2005;366:1804–1807.
164. World Health Organization. A Critical Link: Interventions for 173. Walker SP, Grantham-Mcgregor SM, Powell CA, et al. Effects of
Physical Growth and Psychological Development. A Review growth restriction in early childhood on growth, IQ, and
(Document WHO/CHS/CAH/99.3). Geneva, Switzerland: cognition at age 11 to 12 years and the benefits of nutritional
Department of Child and Adolescent Health and Development, supplementation and psychosocial stimulation. J Pediatr. 2000;
WHO; 1999. 137:36–41.
165. ICDS. (Integrated Child Development Services). Project Report. 174. Hamadani JD, Huda SN, Khatun F, et al. Psychosocial stimulation
New Delhi, India: Department of Women and Child improves the development of undernourished children in rural
Development, Ministry of Human Resource Development, Bangladesh. J Nutr. 2006;136:2645–2652.
Government of India; 1995. 175. Super CM, Herrera MG, Mora JO. Long-term effects of food
166. Currie J, Thomas D. Does head start make a difference? Am Econ supplementation and psychosocial intervention on the physical
Rev. 1995;85:341–346. growth of Colombian infants at risk of malnutrition. Child Dev.
167. Davis SM, Sanders SG, FitzGerald CA, et al. Chile: an evidence- 1990;61:29–49.
based preschool intervention for obesity prevention in Head Start. 176. McKay H, Sinisterra L, McKay A, et al. Improving cognitive ability
J Sch Health. 2013;83:223–229. in chronically deprived children. Science. 1978;200:270–278.
168. Aboud FE, Shafique S, Akhter S. A responsive feeding intervention 177. Mayo-Wilson E, Imdad A, Herzer K, et al. Vitamin A supplements for
increases children’s self-feeding and maternal responsiveness but preventing mortality, illness, and blindness in children aged under
not weight gain. J Nutr. 2009;139:1738–1743. 5: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2011;343:d5094.
169. Gardner JM, Powell CA, Baker-Henningham H, et al. Zinc 178. Yakoob MY, Theodoratou E, Jabeen A, et al. Preventive zinc
supplementation and psychosocial stimulation: effects on the supplementation in developing countries: impact on mortality and
development of undernourished Jamaican children. Am J Clin morbidity due to diarrhea, pneumonia and malaria. BMC Public
Nutr. 2005;82:399–405. Health. 2011;11(suppl 3):S23.

Vol. 38, No. 8, October 2017 Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 679
Copyright Ó 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

S-ar putea să vă placă și