Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1380-3395/02/2406-745$16.

00
2002, Vol. 24, No. 6, pp. 745±754 # Swets & Zeitlinger

Differences in Executive Functioning Between Alzheimer's


Disease and Subcortical Ischemic Vascular Dementia
Robert L. Yuspeh1, 2, y, Rodney D. Vanderploeg 3, 4, 5, Timothy A. Crowell3, 5, and Michael Mullan 3, 4, 5
1
Department of Psychology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, USA, 2 Department of Psychiatry and
Behavioral Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA,
3
James A. Haley VA Medical Center, Tampa, FL, USA, 4 Roskamp Institute, Tampa, FL, USA, and
5
Memory Disorder Clinic, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA

ABSTRACT

The present study examined the performance of 114 individuals (62 males, 52 females) on a variety of tests
purported to measure executive abilities. Participants were diagnosed with possible or probable Alzheimer's
disease (AD), subcortical ischemic vascular dementia (SVaD), or were normal controls (NoDx). Groups
were matched for age and education, and clinical groups were matched for severity of dementia. Multivariate
and univariate analyses of variance were performed which indicated that the AD and SVaD patients differed
from the NoDx on all measures of executive functioning. Further, the AD group made signi®cantly more
episodic memory errors than the SVaD group. On the other hand, consistent with previous research, the SVaD
group performed signi®cantly better than the AD group on recognition memory, but not on free recall
measures. Present ®ndings suggest that AD patients have more executive self-monitoring problems than
SVaD patients do, but SVaD patients have more retrieval problems (executive memory search), suggesting a
fractionation of executive abilities. Thus, differences between dementia groups depend on the nature of the
executive function assessed.

Alzheimer's disease (AD) and cerebrovascular Studies on the neuropathology of AD indicate


disease are the two most frequent causes of that the pathogenic mechanisms of AD include
dementia (Folstein, 1997). In fact, autopsy studies the preferential deterioration of medial temporal
have indicated that vascular dementia (VaD) lobe or temporal limbic structures and reciprocal
accounts for 17±29% of all dementias, and a corticolimbic connections (Hyman, Arriagada,
mixed group with the neuropathology of both van Hoesen, & Damasio, 1993; Tomlinson et al.,
AD and VaD accounts for another 10±23% 1970) that are critical in the operation of declarative
(Erkinjuntti, Haltia, Palo, Sulkava, & Paetau, memory (Eichenbaum & Otto, 1993). Deteriora-
1988; O'Brien, 1988; Tomlinson, Blessed, & tion of these brain regions and cognitive processes
Roth, 1970). Recent advances in neuroimaging result in the hallmark symptom of AD which is
support an even greater spectrum and prevalence described as poor consolidation of information
of VaD with the frequent disclosure of periven- into long-term memory, and characterized on
tricular white matter degeneration attributed to neuropsychological testing as rapid forgetting or
cerebrovascular disease (Hachinski, 1990). poor delayed memory performance (Hart, Kwentus,

We are saddened to note that Dr. Yuspeh died on January 19th, 2002. Because of that, the corresponding author has
been changed to Dr. Rodney D. Vanderploeg.
Address correspondence to: Dr. Rodney D. Vanderploeg, James A. Haley Veterans Hospital, Psychology Service
(116B), 13000 Bruce B. Downs Blvd., Tampa, FL 33612, USA. Tel.: ‡ 1 813 972 2000 ext. 6728. Fax: ‡ 1 813 903
4814. E-mail: Rodney.Vanderploeg@med.va.gov.
Accepted for publication: August 6, 2001.
746 ROBERT L. YUSPEH ET AL.

Harkins, & Taylor, 1988; Paulsen et al., 1988; viously mentioned, a number of these symptoms
Welsh et al., 1996). are also found in AD (Berg, 1988; Pirozzolo,
In contrast, VaD preferentially results from Christense, Ogl, Hansch, & Thompson, 1981).
multiple deep strokes or vascular-induced peri- A number of research studies have examined
ventricular white matter changes in frontal- neuropsychological features that differentiate AD
subcortical regions (Aharon-Peretz, Cummings, from vascular dementias. Our review of these
& Hill, 1988; Hachinski, 1990; Ishii, Nishihara, & studies will focus on aspects of executive function-
Imamura, 1986). Particularly when there is a ing, and will attempt to differentiate vascular
minimal history of prior clinical strokes, a dementias (an heterogeneous group with and
differential diagnosis between AD and VaD is without clinical strokes, and with varying degrees
dif®cult. In addition, the more recently identi®ed of involvement of cortical gray matter, subcortical
subcategory of subcortical vascular dementia gray matter, and subcortical WMLs) from studies
or ischemic vascular dementia (i.e., individuals that have a relatively pure subcortical white matter
with cognitive de®cits who have no history of dementia group (e.g., Binswanger's disease [BD]).
clinical strokes, but who on neuroimaging Bernard et al. (1992) evaluated letter and
studies have multiple white matter lesions semantic ¯uency as well as episodic memory in
[WMLs]) has complicated the clinical diagnostic BD and AD groups and normal controls. Groups
issue. Neuropsychologically, although some were matched on age and education. In addition,
mental slowing has been reported in AD (Gordon AD and BD groups also were matched on severity
& Carson, 1990; Vrtunski, Patterson, Mack, & of dementia using the Mini-Mental State Examina-
Hill, 1983), a greater decline in mental or tion (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh,
information processing speed and ef®ciency has 1975). The dementia groups were comparable
been shown to correlate with white matter on semantic category ¯uency and letter category
involvement in VaD and cerebrovascular disease ¯uency, but were worse than controls on these
(Boone et al., 1992; Junque et al., 1990; Kluger, measures. On an episodic memory task, although
Gianututsos, Leon, & George, 1988; Matsubayashi, both dementia groups performed more poorly
Shimada, Kawamoto, & Ozawa, 1992; Ylikoski than controls, the AD group in turn performed
et al., 1993). worse than the BD group.
Other than the aforementioned de®cits in A number of other studies have compared AD
information processing speed and ef®ciency groups with groups of patients diagnosed with a
associated with WMLs, problems with motor VaD (Barr, Benedict, Tune, & Brandt, 1992;
sequencing (Junque et al., 1990), frontal executive/ Carew, Lamar, Cloud, Grossman, & Libon, 1997;
self-regulatory abilities (Bernardin, Rao, Haughton, Kertesz & Clydesdale, 1994; Lafosse et al., 1997;
Yertkin, & Ellington, 1991; Boone et al., 1992), Lamar et al., 1997; Libon et al., 1997; Mendez,
the emergence of primitive re¯exes (Junque et al., Cherrier, & Perryman, 1997; Padovani et al.,
1990), and problems with memory retrieval 1995). Unfortunately, in all these studies, the VaD
(Vanderploeg, Yuspeh, & Schinka, 2001) have groups are heterogeneous, composed of individ-
been reported. Furthermore, Gupta et al. (1988) uals with and without clinical strokes and having
reported that patients with WMLs and mild levels neuroimaging ®ndings that include cortical and
of cognitive impairment have de®cits limited subcortical lesions (gray and white matter) in
to sustained and divided attention; while those various combinations. In all studies, AD and VaD
with moderate levels of cognitive impairment groups were matched on age, education, and
also have problems with general slowing of per- severity of dementia (MMSE scores or Mattis
formance (i.e., bradykinesia and bradyphrenia), Dementia Rating Scale [MDRS] total scores;
dif®culty in the rate of learning, problems or- Mattis, 1988). Consistently, no differences were
ganizing material to be learned, and poor recall found between groups on measures of semantic
consistency across learning trials. This speci®c category ¯uency. However, although not all
pattern of de®cits has been described as a frontal- studies cited reported statistically signi®cant
subcortical dementia. Unfortunately, as pre- differences, with the exception of the Barr et al.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 747

(1992) study, VaD groups routinely performed account for some of the discrepant ®ndings
more poorly on letter ¯uency than AD groups. In reported here.
discussing poorer letter ¯uency performance in Consequently, due to the inconsistent ®ndings
VaD than AD participants, despite comparable previously cited, as well as the heterogeneity of
semantic ¯uency, Carew et al. (1997) concluded the VaD groups employed in the aforementioned
that the reduced letter ¯uency output by the VaD studies, the purpose of the present study is to
participants is consistent with search-retrieval attempt to clarify the differential performance on
de®cits, whereas the reduced output on semantic traditional measures of executive functioning in
¯uency tasks in AD participants may be second- groups of AD and SVaD (e.g., VaD where neuro-
ary to degraded semantic knowledge. Consistent imaging reveals only periventricular white
with the conclusions of Carew et al. (1997), matter alterations), matched on overall severity
Lafosse et al. (1997) reported that the volume of of dementia. In addition, aspects of episodic
WMLs was signi®cantly (inversely) correlated memory, such as memory errors (i.e., intrusion
with letter ¯uency. They hypothesized that and false positive errors) and memory retrieval
decreased ¯uency was secondary to executive ability, will be examined as these facets of
dysfunction in the ability to organize effective memory rely on intact executive abilities.
retrieval strategies due to subcortical dysfunction.
These studies also reported that AD subjects
provided more intrusive responses on free or cued METHODS
recall or false positive errors on recognition
episodic memory measures. Participants
Other ®ndings reported in these studies are Participants consisted of 114 individuals (62 males, 52
mixed. For example, Kertesz et al. (1994) reported females) selected from a larger sample of individuals
referred to a memory disorders clinic for neuropsycho-
VaD patients performed signi®cantly worse than
logical evaluation. An attempt was made to match the
AD patients on the Motor Performance subtest of three groups of participants (AD, SVaD, and controls)
the MDRS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale± on age, education, gender, and for the clinical groups on
Revised (WAIS±R; Wechsler, 1981) Picture the severity of dementia using the MMSE (Folstein
Arrangement and Object Assembly subtests, and et al., 1975). Because probable AD participants were
the Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1982) generally more impaired than the SVaD participants, to
Writing subtest. Further, Mendez et al. (1997) match the AD and SVaD samples, the AD group was
composed of some possible AD and some probable AD
reported VaD patients performed signi®cantly
participants. Although patients diagnosed with possible
worse than AD patients on Part A of the Trail AD have cognitive impairment which may not be as
Making Test (TMT; Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). severe as probable AD patients, these participants were
However, although Padovani et al. (1995) reported included in the AD group because in a multidisciplinary
VaD patients made signi®cantly more persevera- diagnostic consensus meeting it was determined that
tive errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test their cognitive impairment could not be attributed to
(WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, any other medical etiology which could be determined
by serum laboratory values (e.g., thyroid disease) or
1993), they found no signi®cant differences
medical history (e.g., cardiovascular disease). Further,
between AD and VaD patients on a number of patients did not meet criteria for a psychiatric disorder
measures of attention and executive functioning according to Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental
which included Part A of the TMT. Similarly, Disorders-Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) which could
Barr et al. (1992) reported no signi®cant diffe- account for their cognitive impairment (e.g., depres-
rences on either Part A or B of the TMT. It is sion). Both probable and possible AD subjects had no
important to remember that in the studies history of clinical strokes and had MRI studies that
revealed only very mild WMLs, that is, no more WMLs
reviewed here, the VaD groups had both cortical
than might be expected for their age. AD participants
and subcortical vascular lesions on imaging (n ˆ 47) met NINCDS/ADRDA criteria for either
studies. Thus, the attribution of VaD neuro- possible (n ˆ 25) or probable (n ˆ 22) AD (McKhann
psychological ®ndings to subcortical pathology et al., 1984). Participants in the SVaD group (n ˆ 29)
cannot be determined with certainty, and may met the NINDS/AIREN criteria for VaD (Roman et al.,
748 ROBERT L. YUSPEH ET AL.

Table 1. Characteristics of the Matched AD, SVaD, and NoDx Groups.


AD (n ˆ 47) SVaD (n ˆ 29) NoDx (n ˆ 38)

Characteristic M SD M SD M SD
Age (years) 76.3 5.2 74.1 8.2 73.6 7.6
Educ. (years) 12.9 2.5 13.2 4.4 13.8 2.8
MMSE 1 24.2 2.0 25.2 2.7 28.9 1.1
Gender 2 (% male) 40.4% 65.5% 63.2%
1
Note. NoDx > SVaD ˆAD.
2
NoDx ˆ SVaD > AD.

1993). Further, SVaD subjects had neuroimaging data score of Trial 3 minus Trial 1 performance. Memory
that revealed extensive WMLs, with no indication of retention was evaluated by examining the delayed
cortical lesions. The normal control group (NoDx; recall score. Episodic memory retrieval was evaluated
n ˆ 38) was composed of individuals who showed no by the CERAD list learning recognition score de®ned
indication of cognitive problems on clinical examina- as Correct Yes responses minus any false positive
tion or on neuropsychological testing and did not meet responses. More speci®cally, this recognition test
diagnostic criteria for any neurological, psychiatric, or consists of the 10 words of the CERAD word list
alcohol/drug abuse diagnosis based on criteria in the learning test presented among 10 distractor words
DSM-IV; (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). (Morris et al., 1989) which are not semantically related
The overall evaluation suggested a tendency to over- to the targets. Memory retrieval is seen as re¯ecting an
attend to normal cognitive aging issues (i.e., the normal executive memory search process, and recent positron
controls might best be described as `worried well'). emission tomography research suggests that memory
Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics and search or a `retrieval attempt' is associated with frontal
MMSE performance of these three groups of partici- functioning (Kapur et al., 1995). Finally, episodic
pants. Groups did not differ in age or education. Groups memory errors were evaluated by comparing groups
did differ in MMSE performance [F(2, 111) ˆ 62.82, on the sum of intrusive responses on the CERAD list
p < .0001]; however, Tukey's HSD post hoc analyses learning and recall trials and false positive errors on the
revealed that the AD and SVaD participants did not recognition trial. This memory errors score is seen as a
differ on MMSE performance. The three groups measure of an executive memory self-monitoring
differed somewhat on gender composition [X2(2, ability.
N ˆ 114) ˆ 6.32, p < .05], with the AD group having a
higher proportion of females than the other two groups.

RESULTS
Procedure
Subjects were administered an expanded Consortium Two multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA)
to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease comparing the AD, SVaD, and NoDx groups were
neuropsychological test battery (CERAD; Morris conducted to assess the overall between-group
et al., 1989). Executive functioning was assessed with effects and to reduce Type I error rate. The ®rst
a variety of traditional executive measures including:
the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWA;
compared groups on the traditional executive
Benton, Hamsher, & Sivan, 1983), the CERAD 60 s measures: COWA, Animal Fluency, Color-Word
Animal word list generation task, the Color-Word score Stroop score, Symbol Digit Modalities, Trails A,
from the Stroop Color-Word test (Golden, 1978), the and Trails B. The second MANOVA compared
Symbol-Digit Modalities Test (Smith, 1991), and Parts groups on the memory measures, including the
A and B of the TMT, (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). memory executive measures: recognition memory
Aspects of episodic memory were also assessed score and memory errors.
with the 10-item 3-trial CERAD word list learning test
(Morris et al., 1989). Amount of learning was assessed
The MANOVA on six traditional executive
by Trial 3 performance. Total words learned was measures was signi®cant, Wilks' Lambdaˆ 0.362,
assessed with the sum of Trials 1 to 3 score. Rate of [F (12, 212) ˆ 11.37, p < .001]. With the ®nding of
learning or learning slope was assessed by a computed a signi®cant overall multivariate effect, planned
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 749

Table 2. Performance of AD, SVaD, and NoDx Groups on Traditional Executive Measures.
AD (n ˆ 47) SVaD (n ˆ 29) NoDx (n ˆ 38)

Characteristic M SD M SD M SD
COWA 22.76 11.03 21.14 8.49 34.87 12.33
Animal Naming 7.49 3.35 8.57 3.20 16.39 4.77
Stroop Color-Word 14.09 8.38 13.25 9.83 24.78 10.79
SDMT 22.84 9.84 19.01 6.17 38.42 10.60
Trails A 80.40 48.71 79.92 64.37 45.47 18.87
Trails B 314.42 182.33 304.27 177.61 128.68 81.18
Trails BÿTrials A 239.42 161.44 221.64 151.39 83.21 68.64
Note. COWA ˆControlled Oral Word Association Test; SDMT ˆ Symbol Digit Modalities Test; Trails A ˆTrail
Making Test, Part A; Trails B ˆ Trail Making Test, Part B; Controls > AD ˆ SVaD on all measures.

univariate comparisons (one-way ANOVAs) that the AD and SVaD groups differed from the
were completed to assess group differences for NoDx group, but not from each other.
individual measures (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). The MANOVA on memory measures also was
Subsequent univariate ANOVAs were also all signi®cant, Wilks' Lambdaˆ 0.303, [F (14, 210)ˆ
signi®cantly different between groups. Table 2 12.27, p < .001). With the ®nding of a signi®cant
summarizes the ®ndings. Tukey's HSD indicated overall multivariate effect, planned univariate
that the AD and SVaD groups differed from the comparisons (one-way ANOVAs) were com-
NoDx group, but not from each other on all pleted to assess group differences for individual
executive measures. To emphasize the executive measures. Subsequent univariate ANOVAs were
nature of the Trail Making task, Trails B minus also all signi®cantly different between groups.
Trails A, a computed measure of cognitive ¯exibil- Table 3 summarizes the ®ndings. Tukey's HSD
ity controlling for the motor speed component, was indicated that the NoDx group outperformed both
also examined in a one-way ANOVA. The the AD and the SVaD groups on all episodic
ANOVA was signi®cant [F(2, 111) ˆ 15.58, memory measures except two. The AD group had
p < .001] and post hoc Tukey's HSD indicated more memory errors (Sum of intrusion and false

Table 3. Performance of AD, SVaD, and NoDx Groups on Episodic Memory and Memory-Based Executive
Measures.
AD (n ˆ 47) SVaD (n ˆ 29) NoDx (n ˆ 38)

Characteristic M SD M SD M SD
Trial 3 1 4.96 1.82 4.86 1.16 7.74 1.25
Total Words 1 12.53 3.98 11.66 3.20 18.87 3.39
Slope 1 1.89 1.49 2.28 1.16 3.16 1.13
Delayed Words1 2.11 1.70 2.41 1.92 5.82 1.80
Recognition 2 7.99 1.16 9.29 0.71 9.66 0.48
Memory Errors 3 3.47 3.03 1.79 1.68 1.21 1.91
Note. Trial 3 ˆ Total words recalled on Trial 3; Total Words ˆ Sum of Words Recalled on Trials 1 to 3; Slope ± Trial
3 minus Trial 1 raw score difference; Delayed Words ˆ Words Recalled after a Delay; Recognition ˆ Total
correct Yes responses minus False Positive responses on the Recognition Trial; Memory Errors ˆ total
number of intrusive responses on the learning and recall trials plus false positive responses on the recognition
memory trial. Memory Errors can be seen as an executive dysfunction in the self-monitoring of memory.
1
Controls > AD ˆ SVaD.
750 ROBERT L. YUSPEH ET AL.

positive errors) than either the NoDx or the SVaD (Carew et al., 1997; Lafosse et al., 1997; Lamar
groups (p < .05).1 Similarly, the AD group et al., 1997; Libon et al., 1997; Mendez et al.,
performed more poorly on recognition memory 1997; Padovani et al., 1995). In the one study of
than either the NoDx or the SVaD groups BD (a pure SVaD) versus AD (Bernard et al.,
(p < .05), although the two patient groups were 1992), no differences were found on either
comparable on delayed recall performance.2 This COWA or semantic ¯uency.
pattern of memory performance indicates memory In contrast to the ®ndings on more traditional
retrieval problems in the SVaD group and executive measures, within memory measures
impaired consolidation abilities in the AD group. indicative of aspects of executive functioning,
two patterns emerge between dementia subtypes.
First, the AD group made more episodic memory
DISCUSSION errors (intrusions and false positive errors)
suggesting that individuals with AD have more
The primary objective of the present study was to executive self-monitoring problems than either
compare individuals diagnosed with possible or control subjects or those diagnosed with SVaD.
probable AD with those diagnosed with a SVaD These ®ndings are consistent with previous
predominantly affecting subcortical white matter. research (Almkvist, 1994; Gainotti & Marra,
When these subjects are matched on age, educa- 1994; Helkala, Luluman, Soininen, & Riekkinen,
tion, and degree of overall dementia severity, 1989; Vanderploeg, Yuspeh, & Schinka, 2001).
traditional measures of executive functioning Helkala et al. (1989) suggest that AD patients
did not differentiate dementia groups, although make these types of episodic memory errors as
both patient groups had more dif®culty than a a result of an inability to inhibit irrelevant
demographically matched control group (NoDx). information and because of increased sensitivity
Although previous studies have reported differ- to interference. An alternative explanation, that
ences between AD and VaD on COWA, that was memory errors are made simply because of poor
only the case in studies where the VaD group memory functioning, seems unlikely. If memory
contained both cortical and subcortical lesions is poor and executive self-monitoring is intact,
there is no reason for an individual to provide
intrusions or false positive errors. With a pure
1
memory de®cit, only the amount of correct
We combined Intrusions on free recall and false positive
information recalled or recognized should be
errors on recognition recall as both re¯ect the same type
of memory monitoring problem. We also looked impaired. If the information is not in memory,
separately at intrusions and false positive errors and it cannot be recalled or recognized; but there
found that the pattern of memory error ®ndings is similar would be no reason to produce inaccurate or
across the three diagnostic groups for both intrusions and false information unless executive monitoring is
false positives. The AD group made more intrusion and de®cient as well. Although we believe this
false positive errors than the control group, while the explanation accounts for the memory errors, it is
SVaD group fell at an intermediate level. When the two
possible that if subjects with memory dif®culties
types of memory errors are combined the pattern is
identical, but then the AD group produces more adopt a moderate or liberal recognition response
combined errors than both the SVaD and control groups, criterion, they may have an elevated number of
who do not differ from each other. false positive errors while still failing to identify a
2
We used an adjusted recognition score (adjusting the normal number of correct targets.
overall number of correct yes responses by subtracting The second difference in performance was that
the number of false positive responses) because otherwise the SVaD group performed better than the AD
a good recognition hit score could simply re¯ect an
group on recognition memory, although the two
individual with memory impairment af®rming all
recognition items. However, the AD group performed patient groups were comparable on delayed recall
more poorly than both SVaD and control groups on both performance. This pattern of memory performance
the adjusted recognition score and on the unadjusted indicates that some new episodic information
number of correct yes (i.e., recognition hits). was stored within the LTM of the SVaD subjects
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 751

that could not be retrieved without retrieval cues LTM stores (retrieval success; Kapur et al., 1995).
(i.e., recognition testing). The memory retrieval Medial temporal activity is associated with retrieval
problems in the SVaD group stand in contrast success, rather than retrieval attempt, likely re¯ect-
to apparent memory consolidation/storage pro- ing a role in reactivation of previously stored
blems in the AD group ± where minimal informa- information (Nyberg, McIntosh, Houle, Nilsson,
tion could be detected in LTM on either delayed & Tulving, 1996). In contrast, retrieval search is
recall or recognition testing. The memory retrieval frontally mediated and may be somewhat later-
de®cit noted in the SVaD group is consistent with alized: (a) right frontal for episodic information,
retrieval problems associated with VaD (Kershaw, and (b) left prefrontal for semantic information
Yuspeh, Vanderploeg, Gold, & Cohen, 1997; (Buckner et al., 1995; Tulving, Kapur, Craik,
Vanderploeg, Yuspeh, Dupree, Schinka, & Cohen, Moscovitch, & Houle, 1994).
1997; Vanderploeg, Yuspeh, & Schinka, 2001; Future research with larger sample sizes will
Yuspeh, Vanderploeg, & Schinka, 1999), and be needed to con®rm the pattern of differences in
other subcortically based dementias such as executive functioning exhibited between these
Huntington's disease (Massman, Delis, Butters, two dementia groups. Furthermore, future studies
Levin, & Salmon, 1990; Paulsen et al., 1995; should sample a wider range of both traditional
Randolph, Braun, Goldberg, & Chase, 1993), and and nontraditional executive measures, employ-
Parkinson's disease (Jacobs et al., 1995; Stern, ing functional neuroimaging information if
Richards, Sano, & Mayeaux, 1993). possible, and include the types of dementias
Although AD subjects may also have some employed in the present study, in addition to other
dif®culty with memory retrieval in that traces dementias such as vascular dementia of a more
of information may be present in LTM but be cortical etiology, Huntington's disease, Parkin-
insuf®ciently strong to support recall or recogni- son's disease, Lewy body dementia, and fronto-
tion, the SVaD group would appear to have more temporal dementia. Nevertheless, taken together,
severe retrieval problems than the AD subjects. the present ®ndings suggest a fractionation of
The SVaD subjects have comparable performance executive abilities. Thus, executive functioning
to the AD subjects on other measures of executive differences between dementia groups depend
functioning including measures of processing on the nature of the executive ability being
speed and ef®ciency that would be important in assessed.
encoding. In addition, the SVaD group's rate of
learning (an aspect of encoding) was comparable
to the AD group, while both patient groups had REFERENCES
an impaired learning slope compared to the
controls. Therefore, the SVaD participants are Aharon-Peretz, J., Cummings, J.L., & Hill, M.A.
likely to have comparably weak memory traces (1988). Vascular dementia and dementia of the
in LTM. However, unlike the AD subjects, for Alzheimer type. Cognition, ventricular size, and
the SVaD group the recognition paradigm pro- leuko-ariosis. Archives of Neurology, 45, 719±721.
Almkvist, O. (1994). Neuropsychological de®cits in
vided suf®cient support to retrieve these memory
vascular dementia in relation to Alzheimer's
traces. disease: Reviewing evidence for functional similar-
The memory retrieval problems experienced ity or divergence. Dementia, 5, 203±209.
by SVaD patients are likely the result of dif®culties American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
with executive memory search, as opposed to the and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
executive problems with self-monitoring seen Washington, DC: Author.
with the AD group. Recent positron emission Barr, A., Benedict, R., Tune, L., & Brandt, J. (1992).
Neuropsychological differentiation of Alzheimer's
tomography research suggests that there are two
disease from vascular dementia. International
components to successful memory retrieval: (a) a Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 621±627.
prefrontally mediated memory searching process Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., & Sivan, A.B. (1983).
(retrieval attempt), and (b) successful reactivation Multilingual aphasia examination (3rd ed.). Iowa
of memory information from posterior cortical City, IA: AJA Associates.
752 ROBERT L. YUSPEH ET AL.

Berg, L. (1988). Mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer Hachinski, V.C. (1990). The decline and resurgence of
type: Diagnostic criteria and natural history. Mount vascular dementia. Canadian Medical Association
Sinai Journal of Medicine, 55, 87±96. Journal, 142, 107±111.
Bernard, B.A., Wilson, R.S., Gilley, D.W., Bennett, D.A., Hart, R.P., Kwentus, J.A., Harkins, S.W., & Taylor, J.R.
Fox, J.H., & Waters, W.F. (1992). Memory failure in (1988). Rate of forgetting in mild Alzheimer's-type
Binswanger's disease and Alzheimer's disease. The dementia. Brain and Cognition, 7, 31±38.
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 230±240. Heaton, R.K., Chelune, G.J., Talley, J.L., Kay, G.G.,
Bernardin, L.J., Rao, S.M., Haughton, V.M., Yertkin, & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Z.F., & Ellington, L.E. (1991). The neuropsycholog- manual: Revised and expanded. Odessa, FL:
ical signi®cance of leukoariosis in a hypertensive Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Helkala, E.L., Luluman, V., Soininen, H., & Riekkinen,
Neuropsychology, 13, 78±79. P.J. (1989). Different error pattern of episodic and
Boone, K.B., Miller, B.L., Lesser, I.M., Mehringer, C.M., semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease and
Hill-Gutierrez, E., Goldberg, M.A., & Berman, N.G. Parkinson's disease with dementia. Neuropsycho-
(1992). Neuropsychological correlates of white- logia, 27, 1241±1248.
matter lesions in healthy elderly subjects: A threshold Hyman, B.T., Arriagada, P.V., van Hoesen, G.W., &
effect. Archives of Neurology, 49, 549±554. Damasio, A. (1993). Memory impairment in
Buckner, R.L., Petersen, S.E., Ojemann, J.G., Miezin, Alzheimer's disease: An anatomical perspective.
F.M., Squire, L.R., & Raichle, M.E. (1995). In R.W. Parks, R.F. Zec, & R.F. Wilson (Eds.),
Functional anatomical studies of explicit and Neuropsychology of Alzheimer's disease and other
implicit memory retrieval tasks. Journal of Neu- dementias (pp. 138±150). New York: Oxford
roscience, 15, 12±29. University Press.
Carew, T.G., Lamar, M., Cloud, B.S., Grossman, M., & Ishii, N., Nishihara, Y., & Imamura, T. (1986). Why do
Libon, D.J. (1997). Impairment in category ¯uency frontal lobe symptoms predominate in vascular
in ischemic vascular dementia. Neuropsychology, dementia with lacunas? Neurology, 36, 340±345.
11, 400±412. Jacobs, D.M., Marder, K., Cote, L.J., Sano, M., Stern,
Eichenbaum, H., & Otto, T. (1993). The hippocampus ± Y., & Mayeaux, R. (1995). Neuropsycholog-
What does it do? Behavioral and Neural Biology, ical characteristics of preclinical dementia in
57, 2±36. Parkinson's patients. Neurology, 45, 1691±1696.
Erkinjuntti, T., Haltia, M., Palo, J., Sulkava, R., & Paetau, Junque, C., Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Bruna, O., Jodar, M.,
A. (1988). Accuracy of the clinical diagnosis of Ribas, J.C., Vinas, J., Capdevila, A., & Marti-Vilata,
vascular dementia: A prospective clinical and post- J.L. (1990). Leuko-ariosis on magnetic resonance
mortem neuropathological study. Journal of imaging and speed of mental processing. Archives of
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 51, 1037± Neurology, 47, 151±156.
1044. Kapur, S., Craik, F.L., Jones, C., Brown, G.M., Houle,
Folstein, M.F. (1997). Differential diagnosis of demen- S., & Tulving, E. (1995). Frontal role of the
tia: The clinical process. The Psychiatric Clinics of prefrontal cortex in retrieval of memories: A PET
North America, 20, 45±57. study. Neuroreport, 6, 1880±1884.
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Kershaw, D.A.J., Yuspeh, R.L., Vanderploeg, R.D.,
Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading Gold, M., & Cohen, D. (1997, August). Differential
the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. memory performance in Alzheimer's and subcort-
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189±198. ical ischemic vascular dementia. Paper presented at
Gainotti, G., & Marra, C. (1994). Some aspects of the 105th annual meeting of the American Psycho-
memory disorders clearly distinguish dementia logical Association, Chicago.
of the Alzheimer's type from depressive pseudo- Kertesz, A. (1982). Western aphasia battery. San
dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Neuropsychology, 16, 65±76. Kertesz, A., & Clydesdale, S. (1994). Neuropsycholog-
Golden, J.C. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. ical de®cits in vascular dementia vs Alzheimer's
Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company. disease: Frontal lobe de®cits prominent in vascular
Gordon, B., & Carson, K. (1990). The basis for choice dementia. Archives of Neurology, 51, 1226±1231.
reaction time slowing in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Kluger, A., Gianututsos, J., Leon, M. de., & George,
and Cognition, 13, 148±166. A.E. (1988). Signi®cance of age-related white
Gupta, S.R., Naheedy, M.H., Young, J.C., Ghobrial, M., matter changes. Stroke, 19, 1054±1055.
Rubino, F.A., & Hindo, W. (1988). Periventricular Lafosse, J.M., Reed, B.R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S.B.,
white matter changes and dementia. Archives of Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W.J. (1997). Fluency and
Neurology, 45, 637±641. memory differences between ischemic vascular
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 753

dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychol- multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurologica Scandi-
ogy, 11, 514±522. navica, 92, 433±442.
Lamar, M., Podell, K., Carew, T.G., Cloud, B.S., Resh, Paulsen, J.S., Salmon, D.P., Monsch, A.U., Butters, N.,
R., Kennedy, C., Goldberg, E., Kaplan, E., & Libon, Swenson, M.R., & Bondi, M.W. (1995). Discrimi-
D.J. (1997). Perseverative behavior in Alzheimer's nation of cortical from subcortical dementias on the
disease and subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. basis of memory and problem-solving tests. Journal
Neuropsychology, 11, 523±534. of Clinical Psychology, 51, 48±58.
Libon, D.J., Bogdanoff, B., Bonavita, J., Skalina, S., Pirozzolo, F.J., Christense, K.J., Ogl, K.M., Hansch,
Cloud, B.S., Resh, R., Cass, P., & Ball, S.K. (1997). E.C., & Thompson, W.E. (1981). Simple and choice
Dementia associated with periventricular and deep reaction time in dementia: Clinical implications.
white matter alterations: A subtype of subcortical Neurobiology of Aging, 2, 113±117.
dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Randolph, C., Braun, A.R., Goldberg, T.E., & Chase,
12, 239±250. T.N. (1993). Semantic ¯uency in Alzheimer's,
Massman, P.J., Delis, D.C., Butters, N., Levin, B.E., & Parkinson's, and Huntington's disease: Dissociation
Salmon, D.P. (1990). Are all subcortical dementias of storage and retrieval failures. Neuropsychology,
alike? Verbal learning and memory in Parkinson's 7, 82±88.
and Huntington's disease patients. Journal of Reitan, R.M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead ±
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, Reitan neuropsychological test battery. Tucson, AZ:
729±744. Neuropsychology Press.
Matsubayashi, K., Shimada, K., Kawamoto, A., & Roman, G.C., Tatemuchi, T.K., Erkunjuntti, T.,
Ozawa, T. (1992). Incidental brain lesions on Cummings, J.L., Masdeu, J.C., Garcia, J.H.,
magnetic resonance imaging and neurobehavioral Amaducci, L., Orgogozo, J.M., Brun, A., Hofman,
functions in the apparently healthy elderly. Stroke, A., Moody, D.M., O'Brien, M.D., Yamaguchi, T.,
23, 175±180. Grafman, J., Drayer, B.P., Bennett, D.A., Fisher, M.,
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale: Professional Ogata, J., Kokmen, E., Bermejo, F., Wolf, P.A.,
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Gorelick, P.B., Bick, K.L., Pajeau, A.K., Bell, M.A.,
Resources, Inc. DeCarli, C., Culebras, A., Korczyn, A.D.,
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, Bogousslavsky, J., Hartmann, A., & Scheinberg, P.
R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical (1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for
diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the research studies: Report of the NINDS-AIREN
NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices International Workshop. Neurology, 43, 250±260.
of the Department of Health and Human Services Smith, A. (1991). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los
Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology, 39, Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
1159±1165. Stern, Y., Richards, M., Sano, M., & Mayeaux, R.
Mendez, M.F., Cherrier, M.M., & Perryman, K.M. (1993). Comparison of cognitive changes in patients
(1997). Differences between Alzheimer's disease with Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease. Archives
and vascular dementia on information processing of Neurology, 50, 1040±1045.
measures. Brain and Cognition, 34, 301±310. Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (1989). Using multi-
Morris, J.C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R.C., Hughes, J.P., variate statistics (2nd ed.). New York: Harper
van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., Mellits, E.D., Clark, Collins.
C., & the CERAD investigators (1989). The Tomlinson, B.E., Blessed, G., & Roth, M. (1970).
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Observations on the brains of demented old
Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsy- people. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 11,
chological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. 205±242.
Neurology, 39, 1159±1165. Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik, F.L., Moscovitch, M., &
Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A.R., Houle, S., Nilsson, L.G., & Houle, S. (1994). Hemispheric encoding/retrieval
Tulving, E. (1996). Activation of medial temporal asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission
structures during episodic memory retrieval. Nature, tomography ®ndings. Proceedings of the National
380, 715±717. Academy of Sciences of the United States of
O'Brien, M.D. (1988). Vascular dementia is under- America, 91, 2016±2020.
diagnosed. Archives of Neurology, 45, 797± Vanderploeg, R.D., Yuspeh, R.L., Dupree, L.W.,
798. Schinka, J.A., & Cohen, D. (1997, February).
Padovani, A., DiPiero, V., Bragoni, M., Iacoboni, M., Memory retrieval in the differential diagnosis of
Gualdi, G.F., & Lenzi, G.L. (1995). Patterns of AD and vascular dementias. Paper presented at the
neuropsychological impairment in mild dementia: 25th annual meeting of the International Neuropsy-
A comparison between Alzheimer's disease and chological Society, Orlando.
754 ROBERT L. YUSPEH ET AL.

Vanderploeg, R.D., Yuspeh, R.L., & Schinka, J.A. Alzheimer's disease from other dementias: The
(2001). Differential episodic and semantic memory CERAD experience. Presented at the 24th annual
performance in Alzheimer's disease and vascular meeting of the International Neuropsychological
dementias. Journal of the International Neuropsy- Society, Chicago.
chology Society, 7, 563±573. Ylikoski, R., Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Salkava, R.,
Vrtunski, P.B., Patterson, M.B., Mack, J.L., & Hill, Raininko, R., & Tilvis, R. (1993). White matter
G.O. (1983). Microbehavioral analysis of the choice changes in healthy elderly persons correlate with
reaction time response in senile dementia. Brain, attention and speed of mental processing. Archives
106, 927±947. of Neurology, 50, 818±824.
Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS±R manual. San Antonio, Yuspeh, R.L., Vanderploeg, R.D., & Schinka, J.A.
TX: The Psychological Corporation. (1999). Episodic and semantic memory dif-
Welsh, K.A., Mirra, S., Fillenbaum, G., Gearing, M., ferences in cortical and subcortical dementias.
Beekly, D., & Edland, S. (1996, February). Neuro- The Clinical Neuropsychologist (Abstract), 13,
psychological and neuropathological differences of 236.

S-ar putea să vă placă și