Sunteți pe pagina 1din 7

Pain 139 (2008) 610–616

www.elsevier.com/locate/pain

Disruption of cognitive function in Fibromyalgia Syndrome q


Bruce D. Dick a,b,c,*, Michelle J. Verrier a,c, K. Troy Harker d, Saifudin Rashiq a,c
a
Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, 8-120 Clinical Sciences Building, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alta., Canada T6G 2B7
b
Department of Psychiatry, University of Alberta, Canada
c
Multidisciplinary Pain Centre, University of Alberta Hospital, Canada
d
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Canada

Received 5 February 2008; received in revised form 5 June 2008; accepted 17 June 2008

Abstract

Accumulating evidence points to significant cognitive disruption in individuals with Fibromyalgia Syndrome (FMS). This study
was carried out in order to examine specific cognitive mechanisms involved in this disruption. Standardized experimental paradigms
were used to examine attentional function and working memory capacity in 30 women with FMS and 30 matched controls. Cog-
nitive function was examined using performance on these tests and between group results were analysed in the context of important
psychological and behavioural measures. Performance of standardized everyday attentional tasks was impaired in the FMS group
compared to controls. Working memory was also found to be impaired in this group. Stimulus interference was found to be signif-
icantly worse in the FMS group as the demands of the tasks increased. These effects were found to exist independent of the measures
of mood and sleep disruption. However, when pain levels were accounted for statistically, no differences existed between groups on
cognitive measures. These findings point to disrupted working memory as a specific mechanism that is disrupted in this population.
The results of this study suggest that pain in FMS may play an important role in cognitive disruption. It is likely that many factors,
including disrupted cognition, play a role in the reduced quality of life reported by individuals with FMS.
Ó 2008 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Fibromyalgia Syndrome; Attention; Working memory; Cognitive disruption; Chronic pain

1. Introduction with FMS. Sleep disturbance is a common complaint in


FMS [31], and depression is also frequently reported
A high proportion of individuals who suffer from [6,16,27]. Both these factors are known to have strong
Fibromyalgia Syndrome (FMS) report cognitive deficits associations with cognitive disruption [14,25]. There
that often include attention and memory dysfunction [7]. are also other factors that are known to play important
While these reports have been noted in clinical settings roles in the disruptive effects of chronic pain syndromes
for many years [13], the research literature aimed at including FMS such as pain-related anxiety, fear of
understanding the mechanisms underlying these prob- pain, and catastrophizing [10,11].
lems has grown slowly. There are many factors that Importantly, a study by Park et al. strengthens the
could be linked to cognitive deficits noted in individuals argument that individuals with FMS do indeed experi-
ence marked cognitive disruption over and above the
effects of depression and anxiety. They also show that
q
Financial support: Intramural department funding. these effects appear to be specific to particular cognitive
*
Corresponding author. Address: Department of Anesthesiology functions including memory and vocabulary deficits.
and Pain Medicine, 8-120 Clinical Sciences Building, University of
Their findings also suggest that these deficits are likely
Alberta, Edmonton, Alta., Canada T6G 2B7. Tel.: +1 780 407 1097;
fax: +1 780 407 7461. not a result of pervasive cognitive deficits secondary to
E-mail address: bruce.dick@ualberta.ca (B.D. Dick). mood disorders or other factors [21].

0304-3959/$34.00 Ó 2008 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.06.017
B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616 611

Leavitt and Katz reported another important finding newspaper, and public television advertisements. A
regarding the nature of cognitive deficits in FMS [17]. pain-free control group matched for age and gender to
They noted that neurocognitive assessments and some the FMS group was recruited in the same manner. The
previous research protocols [8] have at times not found age matched control group was included to account
cognitive disruption that is proportional to the reported for the cognitive decline related to age-related factors
severity of FMS-related cognitive deficits. They pro- that have been well documented [1,21]. All participants
posed that this variability in findings could have in the FMS group were on stable doses of medication.
occurred if different assessment protocols differentially Participants between ages 18 and 80 years diagnosed
measured certain cognitive mechanisms. They found with FMS and who had experienced pain for at least 6
that when individuals with FMS were given tests aimed months were recruited. Exclusion criteria included hav-
at assessing their ability to deal with stimulus competi- ing a medical history of significant head injury, neuro-
tion, task performance declined markedly as stimulus logical disorder, or disease known to impair
competition increased. This study was very important attentional function (e.g., epilepsy, Multiple Sclerosis,
as it confirmed other findings that as the cognitive load Parkinson’s Disease, chronic migraine/chronic daily
of tasks increase, performance declines significantly in headache).
individuals with chronic pain [5,9]. It also pointed to a
specific measure, the Auditory Consonant Trigram 2.2. Measures
(ACT), that appears highly sensitive to the cognitive
dysfunction reported by individuals with FMS. Demographic: Demographic information related to
Findings of a subsequent study in a general chronic age, gender, and number of years of education was
pain population [4] corroborate well with Leavitt and recorded.
Katz’ findings [17]. In that study, chronic pain was asso- Medical and pain history: Medical diagnoses, etiol-
ciated with marked working memory disruption during ogy, chronicity, and location were recorded along with
the performance of a challenging task involving stimulus a detailed list of participants’ current medications.
competition. Specifically, chronic pain appeared to Pain: A Numerical Rating Scale (NRS) as well as the
interfere with the formation of a memory trace during McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ) [20] were used to
a demanding task involving stimulus competition. measure pain intensity at the time of cognitive testing.
As a result, this study explored whether or not work- Participants completed the NRS by drawing a vertical
ing memory disruption was associated with the decline line across an 11.0 cm horizontal line to represent their
in performance on attentional stimulus competition current pain level. The horizontal line was labeled at
tasks in individuals with FMS using the ACT and other each end with ‘‘no pain” and ‘‘worst pain imaginable”.
measures used in recent studies [3,4,17]. We predicted The MPQ provides measures of the sensory, affective,
that we would find results similar to Leavitt and Katz’ and evaluative characteristics of pain. This question-
[17] in that performance would decline in FMS patients naire is widely used because of its ability to assess a vari-
relative to matched controls on the ACT as stimulus ety of different pain characteristics, its sensitivity to
competition increased. We also hypothesized that per- variations in pain levels, and its sensitivity to variances
formance on everyday attentional tasks would be pre- in the qualities of an individual’s pain.
dicted by stimulus competition and working memory Attentional functioning: The Test of Everyday Atten-
decrements. tion (TEA) is a normed, standardized neuropsychologi-
cal test that measures attentional functioning in the
2. Methods domains of sustained and selective attention as well as
auditory-verbal working memory. It has strong ecologi-
The University of Alberta Health Research Ethics cal (relevance to everyday tasks) and face validity and a
Board reviewed and approved this study. short administration time. TEA subtests vary in terms of
the attentional processes assessed as well as the level of
2.1. Participants difficulty between tasks [22].
In addition to the TEA, the ACT was also adminis-
Informed consent, compliant with the University of tered [28]. This dual task paradigm required participants
Alberta’s Health Research Ethics Board requirements, to remember three item lists of consonant letters after
was obtained from all participants prior to study inclu- performing a distracter task that interfered with the con-
sion. Based on power calculations (expected solidation of information into memory. This test was
power = 0.80) derived from previous studies [3], 30 par- administered by presenting item lists to participants
ticipants diagnosed with FMS using Wolfe’s criteria [32] and asking them to count backwards by 3’s (dual task)
were recruited. Recruitment occurred at the Multidisci- for intervals of 9, 18, or 36 s. Following this distracter
plinary Pain Centre at the University of Alberta Hospi- task, participants were asked to verbally report the
tal and from the general public by means of campus, previously presented consonant lists. Five trials were
612 B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616

completed for each distracter interval. A no-distraction standardized cognitive testing. As the testing related to
condition was also included (0s delay to report). this study was extensive and time consuming (approxi-
Attentional/working memory capacity: The Reading mately 90–120 min), participants in both groups were
Span Test (RST) assesses linguistic and nonlinguistic permitted (and encouraged) to take regular rest breaks
cognitive abilities [2]. It has been proposed to reflect between subtests as needed.
basic cognitive efficiency and attentional resource capac-
ity [23]. This test was selected because it has been widely 2.4. Statistical analysis
used and validated as a measure of verbal working mem-
ory. On each trial of this test, participants were asked to An overall attentional function score was created
read a set of sentences aloud while maintaining one tar- using the sum of all TEA subtest scores as previously
get word in memory for each sentence. Five trials were reported in Dick et al. [3]. In addition, selective attention
administered at each set size, ranging from two to six and sustained attention index scores were calculated as
sentences. reported by Robertson et al. [22]. Multifactorial analy-
The RST was performed by participants using a Tos- ses of variance (MANOVAs) and stepwise multiple
hiba SatelliteTM laptop and was completed using the regressions were carried out using SPSS version 14.0.
instructions found in Daneman and Carpenter [2]. This Planned comparisons were performed using Bonferroni
paradigm also includes dual task performance as it is corrections to control for the inflation of alpha.
completed by reading each sentence out loud and then
reporting whether each sentence is grammatically cor- 3. Results
rect or not. While performing that task, participants
are also required to remember the final word of all sen- No significant differences existed between groups on
tences in the set. After each entire set, participants were measures of age (F[1,58] = 0.00, p = .984) or years of
asked to recall the last word in all the sentences in the education (F[1,58] = 1.28, p = .263). Not surprisingly,
set, preferably in the order in which they were presented. significant differences were detected between groups on
As the test progressed, task difficulty and attentional all measures of pain (all p = .000). Significant between
demand were enhanced by increasing set size from two group differences were also found on measures known
to six sentences. to be related to cognitive disruption including depres-
Two outcome scores were derived for this test. The sion (F[1,58] = 74.50, p = .000), anxiety (F[1,58] =
measure of primary interest was the total RST score 58.16, p = .000), average hours of sleep per night
for the number of words correctly remembered for the (F[1,58] = 5.36, p = .024), and night wakings
whole paradigm, a reflection of working memory func- (F[1,58] = 117.33, p = .000). In addition, groups were
tion. The second score represented participants’ accu- found to differ significantly on reported quality of life
racy at judging whether or not sentences were (F[1,58] = 255.96, p = .000). Participants’ demographic,
grammatically correct. sleep, and medical information can be found in Table
Quality of life (QOL): The 15D was used to measure 1 and questionnaire data, in Table 2.
health-related QOL. This 15-item measure assesses lim- Attentional disruption was examined using
itations in physical activities, social activities, usual role MANCOVAs with measures of depression, anxiety,
activity, general mental health (psychological distress and sleep as covariates. Significant differences were still
and well-being), vitality (energy and fatigue), and gen- found between groups where individuals with FMS per-
eral health perceptions as a result of physical and/or formed more poorly on an overall attentional score
emotional health problems. A utility score between 0
and 1 is derived from these 15 measures [26].
Mental health (anxiety and depression): The Hospital Table 1
Anxiety and Depression (HAD) scale, a brief (14-item) Demographic, sleep, and medication information for all participants
measure, was used to assess participants’ current levels Variable Fibromyalgia group Control group
of depression and anxiety [34]. M (SD) M (SD)
Sleep: Participants reported their average total hours Age (years) 49.60 (12.54) 46.56 (10.00)
of sleep per night along with their average number of Education (years) 14.17 (2.49) 15.20 (4.34)
wakings per night. Mean hours of sleep 6.58 (1.79) 7.48 (1.15)
per night
Mean number of 2.80 (1.82) 1.41 (1.11)
2.3. Procedure wakings per night
Medications
On the day of testing, the informed consent process Opioids (%) 40.00 0.00
was carried out and demographic and medical informa- NSAIDS (%) 33.33 10.00
tion was collected. All data were collected in a standard- Tricyclics (%) 46.67 6.67
Other (%) 83.33 50.00
ized manner by a research assistant trained in the use of
B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616 613

Table 2 Table 3
Questionnaire data for all participants Cognitive test performance outcome means for all participants
Measure Possible Fibromyalgia Control Measure Fibromyalgia Control
group group group group
Score M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
range
Test of Everyday Attention (TEA)
Pain rating (NRSa) 0–10 6.52 (2.64) 0.34 (0.78) Overall score 95.07 (14.55) 106.97 (10.85)*
McGill Pain Auditory verbal 18.53 (5.37) 21.80 (4.33)**
Questionnaire (MPQb) working memory
Sensory index 0–42 27.23 (4.60) 0.00 (0.00) Selective attention 29.40 (6.08) 32.70 (5.98)
Affective index 0–14 5.40 (2.97) 0.00 (0.00) Sustained attention 27.07 (4.33) 29.70 (3.53)
Pain rating index (total) 0–78 45.77 (8.85) 0.00 (0.00)
Reading Span Test (RST)
Quality of life (15Dc) 0–1 0.64 (0.10) 0.96 (0.04)
Working memory score 47.74 (17.72) 61.83 (12.72)***
Anxiety (HADd) 0–21 9.60 (3.54) 2.97 (3.19)
Grammatical 95.43 (3.79) 96.57 (2.53)****
Depression (HADd) 0–21 8.07 (4.46) 0.80 (1.16)
judgment score
a
NRS, Numerical Rating Scale.
b Auditory Consonant
MPQ, McGill Pain Questionnaire.
c Trigram (ACT)
15D, Quality of life utility score.
d 0s 14.91 (0.29) 14.63 (0.99)
HAD, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.
9s 10.35 (3.38) 12.30 (2.59)
18 s 8.74 (4.21) 10.73 (2.53)
36 s 7.26 (4.47) 10.13 (3.15)*****
derived from the TEA (F[5,54] = 4.32, p = .002), the *
p = .002.
TEA auditory verbal working memory index **
p = .006.
(F[5,54] = 3.71, p = .006), the RST score for accuracy ***
p = .001.
****
of judging whether or not sentences were grammatically p = .010.
*****
correct (F[5,47] = 3.42, p = .010), the total RST score p = .005.
for number of words correctly remembered
(F[5,47] = 5.36, p = .001), the ACT score at a delay of
36 s (F[5,47] = 3.94, p = .005), and the ACT Total Score could have played a role in at least some of these non-
(F[5,47] = 2.51, p = .043), but not at intervals of 9 significant differences.
(F[5,47] = 1.46, p = .220), or 18 s (F[5,47] = 1.48, In order to examine the effects of working memory
p = .214). TEA measures of selective and sustained function and stimulus interference on attentional func-
attention were found to be significantly worse in the tion, stepwise regressions were used (Table 5). The
FMS group using a MANOVA without the above RST score for the number of words correctly
covariates (selective: F[1,58] = 4.49, p = .038; sustained
F[1,58] = 6.65, p = .012) but were not significantly dif-
ferent when the MANCOVA was carried out (selective: Table 4
F[5,58] = 1.19, p = .329; sustained F[5,58] = 2.25, Cognitive test performance outcome means based on opioids use for
all participants in the FMS group
p = .062). When the NRS pain score was entered as a
covariate, none of the cognitive scores between groups Measure Fibromyalgia Fibromyalgia
group group
on the TEA, RST, or ACT were significantly different.
Taking opioids No opioids
Means for group results on cognitive tests can be found M (SD) M (SD)
in Table 3.
Test of Everyday
While it can be challenging to statistically account for Attention (TEA)
the effects of medication on cognitive function, a post- Overall score 101.56 (10.01) 92.29 (15.48)
hoc ANOVA was carried out to compare cognitive func- Auditory verbal 22.67 (4.39) 16.76 (4.81)*
tion between individuals with FMS who were taking working memory
Selective attention 27.89 (6.15) 30.05 (6.09)
opioids (n = 9) and those who were not (n = 21). This
Sustained attention 28.89 (3.26) 26.29 (4.56)
analysis found that the individuals taking stable doses
of opioids performed significantly better on the Audi- Reading Span Test (RST)
Working memory score 48.11 (19.48) 42.57 (16.09)
tory Verbal Working Memory Index of the TEA Grammatical judgment score 96.71 (1.98) 94.88 (4.23)
(F[1,28] = 9.99, p = .004). No statistically significant dif-
ferences were found between these subgroups on any Auditory Consonant
Trigram (ACT)
other TEA, RST, or ACT measures. However, individ- 0s 15.00 (0.01) 14.86 (0.36)
uals in the opioid subgroup performed better on average 9s 11.22 (2.24) 9.71 (3.47)
on all of those measures except on the TEA Selective 18 s 9.11 (4.05) 8.19 (4.05)
Attention Index (Table 4). It may be that a lack of sta- 36 s 8.44 (3.56) 6.38 (4.39)
tistical power due to the sample size of these subgroups *
p = .004.
614 B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616

Table 5
Regression values for working memory predictors of overall attentional function score
Measure B SE B Beta t score Significance (p)
Reading Span Test (RST)
Working memory score .45 .09 .56 5.14 .000
Auditory Consonant Trigram (ACT)
Total score .67 .18 .45 3.78 .000

remembered, representing working memory function, in a general chronic pain population [4] and in a
was a significant predictor of overall TEA attentional group of individuals with FMS [17].
function (p = .000). The ACT total score also signifi- As seen in Park et al.’s findings [21], we were able to
cantly predicted this measure of attentional function distinguish between global cognitive dysfunction and
(p = .000). The RST score for accuracy on the grammat- specific processes involved in our findings. In this study,
ical judgment task was not a significant predictor of working memory capacity predicted overall attentional
attentional function. function. A measure associated with a complex language
processing did not predict attentional disruption. This
4. Discussion may be an important differential distinction from a the-
oretical perspective. One possibility for this difference
A number of very important differences were found could be that despite the complexity of the language
between groups on psychological and cognitive mea- task, the semantic processing required by the RST task
sures. While demographic measures were not signifi- was more of an automatic cognitive process than the
cantly different between groups, there was a very large working memory task of the RST. Automatic cognitive
discrepancy between groups on measures related to processing has been noted previously to be less inter-
mood, sleep disruption, and pain. In the context of these rupted by pain than controlled processing [10,11].
findings, it is not surprising that there was a dramatic With regard to the limitations inherent in this study,
difference between groups on a measure of quality of life we acknowledge that many factors can play a role in the
where individuals in the FMS group reported signifi- effects that we have studied. In designing this study, we
cantly reduced quality of life. endeavoured to account for a number of factors known
As there is a well established link between cognitive to play a role in cognitive function. Given the potential
disruption and each of depression, anxiety, and sleep that medications taken by individuals with FMS could
disruption, we sought to ascertain whether or not cogni- play a role in our findings, we attempted to carefully
tive dysfunction in FMS would be a primary result of record medications taken by all participants. Unfortu-
these factors rather than pain. When these factors were nately, it is difficult to statistically control for the effects
statistically accounted for, the differences in cognitive of these medications. Our finding that FMS participants
function still remained between groups. Of note, stimu- taking stable doses of prescribed opioids performed sig-
lus competition was only disrupted when the cognitive nificantly better on a measure of auditory verbal work-
load associated with the ACT task was the highest. This ing memory than those with FMS not taking opioids
finding corresponds well with previous findings [5,9]. supports previous research. There is a strong evidence
However, when pain scores were accounted for, these from previous studies that cognitive function is either
differences disappeared. This provides considerable evi- not affected or it is improved in individuals taking anal-
dence that chronic pain may play an important role in gesics [19,29]. That our FMS subgroup taking opioids
the disruption of cognitive processes in FMS. performed better (though not statistically significantly
Further, our prediction that stimulus interference better) on all cognitive measures except on a measure
and working memory disruption would have a strong of selective attention suggests that further research on
association with tasks that tapped everyday atten- this issue is warranted. It is important to be aware that
tional function was strongly borne out. Thus, it the potential effects of opioids and other medications
appears that impaired working memory function is taken by participants on cognitive function is complex
disrupted by chronic pain. This effect was seen and that further study is needed.
between groups in this study even after assumed influ- Further, it is recognized that testing-related increases
ential factors such as depression and sleep disruption in pain and fatigue could play a confounding role in the
were accounted for. It also appears that stimulus com- between-group differences reported here. We suggest
petition may be an important factor that is involved that such difficulties with pain and stamina are endemic
in the process that results in observable and reported to having a chronic pain syndrome and almost surely
cognitive impairment. These findings in individuals play a role in the functional impairments and reduced
with FMS are consistent with the previous findings quality of life reported by this population. However,
B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616 615

we also suggest that such differences are a reflection of Acknowledgements


the reality of such medical conditions and strengthen
the ecological validity of the findings reported here. We thank all the individuals who participated in this
Future research could incorporate a number of shorter study. We are also grateful to Peggianne MacKenzie
testing sequences to account for differences in test- and Sherry Lippa in the Multidisciplinary Pain Centre
related fatigue. for their assistance during the recruitment phase of this
Previous research had noted an association between study.
memory deficits and mental clarity with poorer mental No financial support or benefit related to this re-
health in individuals with FMS [14]. In that study, the search has been received by the authors which could cre-
results pointed to the vulnerability of this population ate a conflict of interest or the appearance of a potential
to cognitive disruption. Unfortunately, these effects are conflict of interest.
often discounted or disputed when patients present with
these complaints. Patients are often accused of malinger-
ing or exaggeration when making these reports. Our References
findings suggest that these effects are real consequences
of FMS. They also suggest that this constellation of [1] Badgio PC, Worden BL. Cognitive functioning and aging in
women. J Women Aging 2007;19:13–30.
symptoms can result in tremendous negative effects on
[2] Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working
a person’s quality of life. Studies that have examined memory and reading. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 1980;19:450–66.
the phenomenology of FMS strongly support this [12]. [3] Dick B, Eccleston C, Crombez G. Attentional functioning in
The accumulating evidence points to a biological sub- fibromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis, and musculoskeletal pain
strate that may be associated with cognitive function patients. Arthritis Rheum 2002;47:639–44.
[4] Dick BD, Rashiq S. Disruption of attention and working memory
that is significantly affected by chronic pain. The exact
traces in individuals with chronic pain. Anesth Analg
nature of the biological ‘hardware’ affected by chronic 2007;104:1223–9.
pain remains unknown. Sephton et al. have reported [5] Eccleston C. Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental
that FMS and the cognitive symptoms associated with investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in
it could be biologically related to a disorder of the neu- the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behav Res Ther
1995;33:391–405.
roendocrine stress response [25]. There has also been a
[6] Epstein SA, Kay G, Clauw D, Heaton R, Klein D, Krupp L, et al.
recent suggestion that there may be evidence that cogni- Psychiatric disorders in patients with fibromyalgia: a multi-centre
tive deficits in FMS are associated with a dysfunction of investigation. Psychosom 1999;40:57–63.
central processing related to low arousal and impaired [7] Glass JM. Cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia: and chronic
filtering mechanisms [33]. Interactions between immune fatigue syndrome new trends and future directions. Curr Rheu-
matol Rep 2006;8:425–9.
and neural activities have also been investigated in indi-
[8] Grace GM, Nielson WR, Hopkins M, Berg MA. Concentration
viduals with FMS by Lekander and colleagues [18]. That and memory deficits in patients with Fibromyalgia Syndrome. J
study found that immune system activity was associated Clin Exp Neuropsyc 1999;21:477–87.
with cortical areas responsible for attentional function, [9] Grisart J, Van der Linden M, Bastin C. The contribution of
affect, and pain perception. Some evidence exists of dif- recollection and familiarity to recognition memory performance in
chronic pain patients. Behav Res Ther 2007;45:1077–84.
ferences in brain morphology in FMS [15,24], but these
[10] Grisart J, Van der Linden M. Conscious and automatic uses of
findings require further study and replication. Unfortu- memory in chronic pain patients. Pain 2001;94:305–13.
nately, much remains to be learned about the biological [11] Grisart J, Van der Linden M, Masquelier E. Controlled processes
mechanisms involved in this effect. The findings reported and automaticity in memory functioning in fibromyalgia patients:
here in addition to the broader literature that has relation with emotional distress and hypervigilance. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol 2002;24:994–1009.
explored cognitive deficits in individuals with chronic
[12] Hallberg LR-M, Carlsson SG. Coping with Fibromyalgia: a
pain show a clear need for a stronger theoretical frame- qualitative study. Scand J Caring Sci 2000;14:29–36.
work regarding cognitive mechanisms affected by [13] Jamison RN, Sbrocco T, Parris WCV. The influence of problems
chronic pain. with concentration and memory on emotional distress and daily
Imaging studies in FMS have noted differences in activities in chronic pain patients. Int J Psychiatry Med
1988;18:183–91.
cerebral activity including higher neural recruitment
[14] Katz RS, Heard AR, Mills M, Leavitt F. The prevalence and
during cognitive tasks [7,30]. Much remains to be clinical impact of reported cognitive difficulties (Fibrofog) in
learned from those techniques as equipment and exper- patients with rheumatic disease with and without Fibromyalgia. J
imental paradigms evolve and improve. Each additional Clin Rheumatol 2004;10:53–8.
report provides information that adds a piece to the [15] Kuchinad A, Schweinhardt P, Seminowicz DA, Wood PB, Chizh
BA, Bushnell MC. Accelerated brain gray matter loss in
complex puzzle of how chronic pain disrupts attention
fibromyalgia patients: pemature aging of the brain? J Neurosci
in FMS. The accumulating evidence suggests that many 2007;27:4004–7.
factors must be taken into account as we strive to under- [16] Landrø NI, Stiles TC, Sletvold H. Memory functioning in patients
stand and treat FMS and improve the quality of life of with primary Fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy
those with this problem. controls. J Psychosom Res 1997;42:297–306.
616 B.D. Dick et al. / Pain 139 (2008) 610–616

[17] Leavitt F, Katz RS. Distraction as a key determinant of impaired [26] Sintonen H. The 15D instrument of health-related quality of life:
memory in patients with Fibromyalgia. J Rheumal properties and application. Ann Med 2001;33:328–36.
2006;33:127–32. [27] Sletvold H, Stiles TC, Landrø NI. Information processing in
[18] Lekander M, Fredrikson M, Wik G. Neuroimmune relations in primary fibromyalgia, major depression and healthy controls. J
patients with fibromyalgia: a positron emission tomography Rheumatol 1995;22:137–42.
study. Neurosci Lett 2000;282:193–6. [28] Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Pelchat G. Three tests of attention and
[19] Lorenz J, Beck H, Bromm B. Cognitive performance, mood rapid information processing. An extension. Clin Neuropsychol
and experimental pain before and during morphine-induced 1997;2:246–50.
analgesia in patients with chronic non-malignant pain. Pain [29] Tassain V, Attal N, Fletcher D, Brasseur L, Degieux P, Chauvin
1997;73:369–75. M, Bouhassira D. Long term effects of oral sustained release
[20] Melzack R. The mcgill pain questionnaire: major properties and morphine on neuropsychological performance in patients with
scoring methods. Pain 1975;1:277–99. chronic non-cancer pain. Pain 2003;104:389–400.
[21] Park DC, Glass JM, Minear M, Crofford LJ. Cognitive function [30] Williams DA, Gracely RH. Biology and therapy of fibromyalgia.
in Fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis Rheum 2001;44:2125–33. Functional magnetic resonance imaging findings in fibromyalgia.
[22] Robertson IH, Ward T, Ridgeway V, Nimmo-Smith I. The test of Arthritis Res Ther 2006;8:224.
everyday attention. Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company; 1994. [31] Wolfe F. The clinical syndrome of fibrositis. Am J Med
[23] Rosen VM, Engle RW. The role of working memory capacity in 1986;1(3A):7–14.
retrieval. J Exp Psychol Gen 1997;126:211–27. [32] Wolfe F, Smythe HA, Yunus MB, Bennett RM, Bombardier C,
[24] Schmidt-Wilcke T, Luerding R, Weigand T, Jurgens T, Schuierer Goldenberg DL, et al. The American College of Rheumatology
G, Leinisch E, et al. Striatal grey matter increase in patients 1990 criteria for the classification of Fibromyalgia. Arthritis
suffering from fibromyalgia – a voxel based morphometry study. Rheum 1990;33:160–72.
Pain 2007;132(Suppl. 1):S109–16. [33] Young JL, Redmond JC. Fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue, and adult
[25] Sephton SE, Studts JL, Hoover K, Weissbecker I, Lynch G, Ho I, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the adult; a case study.
et al. Biological and psychological factors associated with memory Psychopharmacol Bull 2007;40:118–26.
function in Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Health Psychol [34] Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression
2003;22:592–7. scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;67:361–70.

S-ar putea să vă placă și