Sunteți pe pagina 1din 3

Abnormal rhythmic Motor hyperactivity is a striking feature of attention-deficit–

hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), manifested by restlessness, fid-


getiness, and unnecessary gross motor movements (Zametkin
motor response in and Ernst 1999). Whereas much research has been devoted to
the neurophysiology of attention, the underpinnings of the
children with motor impairment in ADHD have been relatively neglected
(Barkley 1997). Accumulating evidence points to abnormali-

attention-deficit– ties in the dopaminergic system and participating cortico-stri-


atal-thalamic neural loops (Aylward et al. 1996) reminiscent
of that in Parkinson’s disease (Bergman and Deuschl 2002).
hyperactivity disorder Although ADHD and Parkinson’s disease differ in aetiology,
age, and clinical signs, it might be possible to use experimental
motor paradigms from patients with Parkinson’s disease to
study motor phenomena in ADHD. Patients with Parkinson’s
Hilla Ben-Pazi* MD; disease participating in a repetitive motor task, such as finger
Varda Gross-Tsur MD, Neuropediatric Unit, Shaare Zedek tapping, can accjurately produce low frequencies, but with
Medical Center; higher-frequency stimuli they respond faster than the exter-
Hagai Bergman MD PhD, Department of Physiology, The nal cue, i.e. getting ‘ahead of themselves’ (Nakamura et al.
Hebrew University–Hadassah Medical School; 1978). This ‘hastened’, voluntary response, termed ‘hasten-
Ruth S Shalev MD, Neuropediatric Unit, Shaare Zedek ing phenomenon’, falls within the tremor frequency band of
Medical Center, Jerusalem, Israel. Parkinson’s disease and by extrapolation is considered to
result from a central oscillatory mechanism (Logigian et al.
*Correspondence to first author at Neuropediatric Unit, Shaare 1991). The commonalities between the two disorders led us
Zedeck Medical Center, PO 3235, Jerusalem 91031, Israel. to suggest that an abnormal neuronal oscillatory mechanism
E-mail: shmaryaho@hotmail.com might also have a role in the motor abnormalities of ADHD.
The object of this study was to explore whether children with
ADHD also demonstrate hastening phenomenon.

Method
Children with attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder PARTICIPANTS
(ADHD) have difficulties with motor control, inhibition of We examined two groups of children, both attending the reg-
motor responses, motor flexibility, and motor preparedness. ular school system in a suburban neighbourhood. The group
We proposed that motor abnormalities in ADHD might result, with ADHD consisted of 27 children (21 males, six females)
at least in part, from an abnormal neuronal oscillatory aged from 6 to 14 years 6 months old (mean age 11 years 4
mechanism necessary for motor temporal regulation. The aim months, SD 2 years 2 months) from the Neuropediatric Unit at
of this study was to assess pacing in children with ADHD, by Shaare Zedek Medical Center, Israel, diagnosed with ADHD by
testing for rhythmic abnormalities of motor activity using a an experienced pediatric neurologist according to DSM-IV
tapping test. Twenty-seven children (21 males, six females; aged clinical criteria (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
6 to 14 years 6 months; mean age 11 years 4 months, SD 2 years Disorders; American Psychiatric Association 1994) and stan-
2 months) diagnosed with ADHD according to DSM-IV clinical dard behavioural questionnaires (Conners Abbreviated Rating
criteria, and 33 controls (25 males, eight females; aged 6 to 14 Scale [Goyette et al. 1978] and Child Behavioral Checklist
years 6 months; mean 11 years 1 month, SD 2 years 2 months), [Achenbach et al. 1991]), and who consented to participate.
underwent a finger-tapping test requiring rhythmic responses to The second group consisted of 33 control participants
frequencies from 1 to 6Hz. All participants who were treated on matched for age (mean age 11 years 1 month, SD 2 years 2
a daily basis with methylphenidate (n=22) were medication- months) and sex (25 males, eight females) drawn from a regu-
free on the day of the test. Most of the children with ADHD lar elementary school, without attention or hyperactivity
responded at a constant rate regardless of stimulus frequency, a problems according to their teachers. The 22 children with
phenomenon only seen in a small number of the controls. This ADHD treated with methylphenidate on a regular basis were
specific error pattern, also seen in Parkinson’s disease, has been free of medication for at least 24 hours before the tapping
attributed to an abnormal oscillatory mechanism mediated by test. Children with a significant hearing and visual impair-
dopaminergic fronto-striatal circuitry, which might also be ment or other neurological disorder were excluded before
pathophysiologically relevant for ADHD. testing. We explained the experimental procedure to the
children and parents, and informed consent was received.
The study was approved by the Internal Review Board of the
Shaare Zedek Medical Center.

TESTING PROCEDURE
Each child was seated comfortably in front of a laptop comput-
er in a quiet room. The child was asked to tap on the space bar
immediately after the simultaneously computer-generated
visual and auditory stimuli appeared. The combined use of
simultaneous visual and auditory stimuli was to help the child

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2003, 45: 743–745 743


to stay on task during the testing session. The stimuli appeared Results
at the following frequencies: 1, 2, 2.5, 3, 3.5, 4, 5, and 6Hz Children with ADHD had difficulty modulating their response
(stimuli per second). The visual stimulus was a 2cm black rate in accordance with the frequency of the stimulus (Fig.
diamond and the auditory stimulus was a 35dB bleep, both 1c, d) unlike the controls, who changed their responses in tan-
lasting 50ms. Forty stimuli were presented for each frequen- dem with the stimulus (Fig. 1a, b). Children with ADHD tended
cy; there was a 3-second interval between frequencies. The to tap faster than the stimulus presentation, thereby ‘getting
testing session lasted 7 minutes. Because most of the chil- ahead of themselves’. Fifteen of the 27 children with ADHD ful-
dren, both controls and those with ADHD, failed to follow filled all three criteria for hastening phenomenon, in compari-
the 6Hz stimuli, it was excluded from the analysis. son with 2 of the 33 controls (p<0.05). The hastened frequency
hovered between 2.8 and 4.7Hz (mean 3.8Hz, SD 0.6Hz).
RECORDING ANALYSIS (1) Recurrent mistakes: children with ADHD who demon-
We used customized programmes written in Matlab (Matworks, strated hastening phenomenon (n=15) made errors on 2 to 7
Beltsville, MD, USA) previously employed for analysis of the fin- of the 10 stimulus frequencies (mean 3.9Hz, SD 1.3Hz), in
ger-tapping responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease and comparison with 0 to 4 (mean 0.5Hz, SD 1.1Hz) of the controls
in adults without disabilities (Elazary et al. 2003). (p<0.05). (2) Rapid response: all children with hastening phe-
Hastening phenomenon was described as a fast and con- nomenon had a rapid response. Tapping responses of children
stant response frequency, but previous studies did not with ADHD were faster than those of controls, i.e. 8.9% (SD
provide a quantitative measure of this phenomenon. We 23.3%) in comparison with –6.7% (SD 32%) respectively
defined hastening phenomenon operationally according to (p<0.05); this effect could be attributed to the 15 children with
the following criteria. (1) Recurrent mistakes: median tap- hastening phenomenon (mean 20.7%, SD 17%). The remain-
ping frequency faster or slower by 0.5Hz than the stimulus ing 12 children with ADHD responded similarly to controls
frequency on at least 2 of the 10 frequencies. (2) Rapid (ADHD mean –5.7%, SD 22.4%; controls mean –6.7%, SD 32%;
response: at least one of the median tapping frequencies p=0.92). (3) Constant response: the slope of median errors by
more than 0.5Hz above the requested stimulus. To deter- frequency was horizontal (range 0.15 to 0.38; mean 0.1, SD
mine whether the tapping responses were faster or slower 0.2) demonstrating a response frequency that was, for the most
than that of the stimulus frequency, we subtracted the num- part, constant regardless of the stimulus (Fig. 2).
ber of fast mistakes from the number of slow mistakes, divid- We found a significant inverse correlation between age and
ed by the total number of responses and multiplied by 100. fast tapping responses for children with ADHD (r2=0.53,
A positive percentage value indicated an overall tapping rate p<0.05), a correlation not found among the controls (p=0.36).
faster than the stimulus frequency. (3) Constant response: There was no correlation between hastening phenomenon and
linear regression slope of the median mistaken frequencies sex or handedness.
hovering around zero (less than 0.5) instead of an expected
slope of one. Discussion
Statistical difference between the groups was determined by More than half of the children with ADHD demonstrated a
using χ2 test, t-test, ANOVA, and linear and logistic regression. response pattern that was constant and faster than the input

a b
Figure 1: Recording of tapping
Stimulus Adequate median response
Tapping response
test to frequencies in control
6 5
Output frequency (Hz)

and child with ADHD.


5 Tapping tested from 1 to 6Hz
Frequency (Hz)

4
4 in ascending and descending
3 order. (a) In control child,
3
2 response rate of finger-
2 tapping pattern (dots)
1 1 follows pace of the stimulus
(line). (b) When output is
0
100 200 0 1 2 3 4 5 plotted against input
Time (s) Input frequency (Hz) frequency, control child
c d demonstrated adequate
median response (squares),
Adequate median response
matching requested
6 5
Output frequency (Hz)

Errors frequencies, and slope


5
Frequency (Hz)

4 approaches 1. (c) For the child


4 with ADHD, however, there is
3 typical fixed frequency of
3
2 response rate (dots)
2 unrelated to input stimulus
1 1 (line). (d) Most tapping
responses defined as errors
0
100 200 0 1 2 3 4 5 (triangles), with an error
Time (s) Input frequency (Hz) slope approaching 0.

744 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2003, 45: 743–745


frequency. This untoward ‘hastened’ voluntary response, fixed frequency can affect voluntary repetitive motor activity.
termed hastening phenomenon, has been documented in There is evidence for the role of central pattern generators in
patients with Parkinson’s disease and might reflect an underly- the execution of rhythmic voluntary movements (Cohen et al.
ing oscillatory pathology in subcortical circuits (Logigian et al. 1988). Even the higher cortical function of writing is rhyth-
1991). The hastened response that we found in ADHD is, in our mic, with a clear preferred frequency (Kunesch et al. 1989). A
opinion, a manifestation of motor abnormalities in ADHD and delineation of the underlying oscillatory mechanisms involved
is indicative of neither impulsivity nor an inability to follow in voluntary movements in children with ADHD might, there-
the task because it occurred in a distinct, constant, and rapid- fore, lead to a better comprehension of the motor abnormali-
ly paced manner. Hastening phenomena were associated ties inherent in this disorder, with diagnositic and therapeutic
with younger children in the ADHD group but not the controls, implications.
suggesting that it is not a variant of normal development.
DOI: 10.1017/S0012162203001385
Rubia et al. (1999) found that boys with ADHD tapped sig-
nificantly faster at 2.5Hz but not at lower frequencies. They Accepted for publication 18th July 2003.
concluded that their data could not support the hypothesis
of an accelerated internal pacemaker responsible for their Acknowledgements
We thank Mrs Tamar Etzioni, principal, and the teachers and students
fast response style (Rubia et al. 1999). In our participants,
of Tzora Elementary School for their cooperation and participation;
tapping abnormalities were also uncommon at frequencies and Gilad Fernandens for statistical analysis. This study was
lower than 2Hz and were elicited mainly at higher frequen- supported in part by a grant from the Joshua Family Foundation, and
cies. We would argue that children with ADHD can follow the National Institute of Psychobiology in Israel, founded by the
slow rhythmic stimuli but at higher frequencies their volun- Charles E Smith Family.
tary motor response is influenced by a dysfunctional central References
oscillatory mechanism. Another abnormal oscillatory pat- Achenbach TM, Howell CT, Quay HC, Conners CK. (1991) National
tern has been shown by Mann et al. (1972), who found that survey of problems and competencies among four- to sixteen-
children with ADHD have abnormal EEG theta activity (4 to year-olds: parents’ reports for normative and clinical samples.
7.5Hz) in the frontal lobes: a frequency that, interestingly, is Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 56: 1–131.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical
within the same range as in this study. Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edn. Washington DC: American
Can a putative mechanism of abnormal oscillatory activity Psychiatric Association.
be reconciled with the evidence of inhibitory deficits in ADHD? Aylward EH, Reiss AL, Reader MJ, Singer HS, Brown JE, Denckla MB.
(Barkley 1997, Moll et al. 2000). If there is indeed decreased (1996) Basal ganglia volumes in children with attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol 11: 112–115.
cortical neuronal inhibition in ADHD (Barkley 1997, Moll Barkley RA. (1997) Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and
et al. 2000), this could release underlying primitive oscilla- executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD.
tory mechanisms between the cortex and the basal ganglia. Psychol Bull 121: 65–94.
Alternatively, abnormal subcortical oscillatory pathways might Bergman H, Deuschl G. (2002) Pathophysiology of Parkinson’s
produce a repetitive stimulus that can secondarily reduce disease: from clinical neurology to basic neuroscience and back.
Mov Disord 17: S28–S40.
intracortical inhibition, as occurs in Parkinson’s disease Cohen AH, Rossignol S, Grillner S. (1988) Neural Control of
(Bergman and Deuschl 2002). Regardless of the precise phys- Rhythmic Movement in Vertebrates. New York: John Wiley.
iological mechanism, neuronal oscillations pulsating at a Elazary AS, Attia R, Bergman H, Ben-Pazi H. (2003) Age-related
accelerated tapping response in healthy population. Percept Mot
Skills 96: 227–235.
Goyette CH, Conners CK, Ulrich RF. (1978) Normative data on
Control revised Conners parent and teacher rating scales. J Abnorm Child
ADHD Psychol 6: 221–236.
15 Kunesch E, Binkofski F, Freund HJ. (1989) Invariant temporal
characteristics of exploratory hand movements. Exp Brain Res
Number of children

78: 539–546.
Logigian E, Hefter H, Reiners K, Freund HJ. (1991) Does tremor
pace repetitive voluntary motor behavior in Parkinson’s disease.
10
Ann Neurol 30: 172–179.
Mann CA, Lubar JF, Zimmerman AW, Miller CA, Muenchen RA.
(1992) Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-
hyperactivity disorder: controlled study with clinical
5 implications. Pediatr Neurol 8: 30–36.
Moll GH, Hienrich H, Trott GE, Wirth S, Rothenberger A. (2000)
Deficient intracortical inhibition in drug naive children with
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is enhanced by
methylphenidate. Neurosci Lett 284: 121–125.
Nakamura R, Nagasaki H, Narabayashi H. (1978) Disturbances of
–0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 rhythm formation in patients with Parkinson’s Disease: Part I.
Slope Characteristics of tapping response to the periodic signals.
Percept Mot Skills 46: 63–75.
Figure 2: Error slopes of controls and children with ADHD. Rubia K, Taylor A, Taylor E. (1999) Synchronization, anticipation
Histograms represent slopes of responses of control group and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally
defined attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Percept Mot
(white) and children with ADHD (black). Note that slopes of
Skills 89: 1237–1258.
controls cluster around 1, indicating an accurate response. Zametkin AJ, Ernst M. (1999) Problems in the management of
Children with ADHD had bimodal distribution of slopes. attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. N Engl J Med 340: 40–46.
Those with fixed response peaked at 0, whereas those with an
accurate response peaked at 1.

Abnormal Rhythmic Motor Response and ADHD Hilla Ben-Pazi et al. 745

S-ar putea să vă placă și