Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Health Psychology Copyright 1997 by the American PsychologicalAssociation,Inc.

1997, Vol. 16, No. 2, 137-146 0278-6133/97/$3.00

Self-Esteem and the Relation Between Risk Behavior and Perceptions


of Vulnerability to Unplanned Pregnancy in College Women
Gabie E. Smith, Meg Gerrard, and Frederick X. Gibbons
Iowa State University

This article reports 2 studies testing the hypothesis that individuals with high self-esteem are
more likely than those with low self-esteem to interpret information about their personal
vulnerability to health risks in a self-serving manner. Study 1 used an experimental paradigm
to demonstrate that self-esteem moderates the influence of review of sexual and contraceptive
behavior on college women's perceptions of vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy (N = 125).
Study 2 used a longitudinal design to demonstrate that self-esteem also moderates the relation
between naturally occurring changes in college women's sexual behavior and changes in their
risk perception (N = 273). Together, these studies provide evidence that people with high
self-esteem use self-serving cognitive strategies to maintain their risk perceptions.

Key words: perceived vulnerability, risk perceptions, self-esteem

Several comprehensive reviews of the literature on health make judgments about their own likelihood of pregnancy in
behavior have supported the hypothesis that perceptions of a series of hypothetical situations in which both frequency of
vulnerability motivate precautionary behavior (e.g., Becker, sexual intercourse and contraceptive method were manipu-
1974; Harrison, Mullen, & Green, 1992; Janz & Becker, lated (Gerrard & Luus, 1995). Results indicated that the
1984). The effect has been documented for a wide variety of women combined information about frequency of inter-
preventive behaviors including receiving immunizations, course and contraception correctly (i.e., they recognized that
blood pressure screenings, and preventive medical assess- the two interact to determine risk) and that their risk
ments, and returning for follow-up appointments with estimates were relatively accurate. Rothman et al. (in press)
physicians. Given this evidence, questions concerning the reported similar evidence of accuracy in a study in which
determinants and accuracy of these perceptions warrant college students' estimates of their risk for 11 different
examination. At present, however, research on these issues health hazards correlated .71 with the actual probability of
presents an apparent contradiction (Rothman, Klein, & occurrences of these events. Additional evidence that risk
Weinstein, in press). On the one hand, there is ample perceptions reflect risk has recently been reported in a
evidence that people underestimate their risk of negative longitudinal study of adolescents' initiation of three risk
events relative to others (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986; Weinstein, behaviors (e.g., reckless driving and smoking; Gerrard,
1980, 1982). On the other hand, there is emerging evidence Gibbons, Benthin, et al., 1996). As expected, increases in
that people use information to make reasonably accurate risk adolescents' participation in these risk behaviors were
estimates of their absolute risk (Gerrard, Gibbons, Benthin, followed by increases in their perceptions of vulnerability to
& Hessling, 1996; Gerrard & Luus, 1995; Rothman et al., in the associated negative consequences (e.g., automobile
press). accidents and lung disease). In short, these studies suggested
that people do understand the relation between risk behav-
Determinants and Accuracy of iors and vulnerability and can be fairly accurate in estimat-
VulnerabilityEstimates ing the relative risks of different courses of action.

In a recent study of predictors of perceptions of vulnerabil- Illusion o f Unique Invulnerability


ity to unwanted pregnancy, college women were asked to
Numerous studies have also demonstrated that people
tend to think that they are less vulnerable than others to
Gabie E. Smith, Meg Gerrard, and Frederick X. Gibbons, negative events (Gerrard, Gibbons, & Warner, 1991; Perloff
Department of Psychology, Iowa State University. & Fetzer, 1986; Quadrel & Fischhoff, 1993; Taylor &
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Brown, 1988; Weinstein, 1987). In other words, individuals'
Health Grant 48165-01. ability to make accurate estimates is tempered by their
We would like to thank Bret Pelham for comments on a draft of
this article, and Daniel W. Russell for consultation regarding the efforts to maintain what is commonly called an illusion of
data analyses. unique invulnerability, or optimistic bias. Optimistic bias
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to has been demonstrated by using both absolute estimates of
Gabie E. Smith, who is now at the Department of Psychology, Guild risk and perceptions of risk relative to others (e.g., Wein-
Center, Frostburg State University, Frostburg, Maryland 21532. stein, 1980, 1984). Not surprisingly, a variety of factors
Electronic mail may be sent via Intemet to g_smith@fre.fsu.umd.edu. appear to contribute to this perception.

137
138 SMITH, GERRARD, AND GIBBONS

First, it appears that when asked to estimate their own and participate in a variety of self-serving or self-protective
others' risk, people are more likely to pay attention to the cognitive distortions and illusions that appear to be similar
risk-increasing factors of others (e.g., genetic predisposi- to those associated with optimistic bias (for reviews, see
tions, unhealthy behaviors) than their own risk factors, Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Taylor & Brown, 1988). For example,
leading to the conclusion that others are at greater risk than individuals with high self-esteem are more likely than those
they are (Weinstein & Lachendro, 1982). Second, when with low self-esteem to exhibit enhanced impressions of
asked to consider their personal risk of a specific disorder, their attributes and abilities (Pelham, 1991; Schrauger &
people appear to conjure up an image of someone who is Terbovic, 1976), and they are less likely to accept informa-
particularly vulnerable to that disorder and then compare tion suggesting that they have performed poorly or have
themselves with that image (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986). In been negligent (Brockner, 1979; Shrauger & Kelly, 1988).
comparison with this high-risk image, the self is seen as Thus, it appears that self-enhancing biases are one way that
relatively invulnerable. Such findings may represent unmoti- high self-esteem individuals validate and maintain positive
vated cognitive errors in judgment caused, for example, by self-perceptions and expectations.
comparison with salient stereotypic victims (Weinstein, Research that has specifically assessed health-relevant
1980). It has also been suggested, however, that optimistic cognitions has also demonstrated the use of self-enhancing
bias is the result of a motivated process designed to maintain strategies among high self-esteem individuals. For example,
subjective well-being (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986; Taylor, Wood, Boney-McCoy, Gibbons, and Gerrard (1995) found that in
& Lichtman, 1983; Weinstein, 1987; Wills, 1981). More spite of comparable levels of risk behavior, high self-esteem
specifically, when considering their own risk, individuals women reported lower perceived vulnerability to sexually
apparently are motivated to attend to information about transmitted diseases than did low self-esteem women. In
themselves in a self-serving manner (Kulik & Mahler, 1987; addition, this study documented high self-esteem women's
Weinstein, 1984), focusing on their own risk-reducing use of a self-serving strategy. When high self-esteem women
factors (e.g., diet, exercise, evidence of hereditary longevity) were asked to identify behaviors or factors that increased
and others' risk-increasing factors (Weinstein & Klein, their risk of contracting sexually transmitted diseases, they
1995). made more positive ratings of themselves on two additional
Gerrard, Gibbons, et al. (1991) provided evidence of the dimensions (their personalities and their pregnancy preven-
motivational component of optimistic bias among women tion effeorts) than did women with low self-esteem. Another
Marines. This is a group with a high frequency of inter- study relevant to the current research examined the reaction
course, and therefore, a high risk of pregnancy (Gerrard & of smokers who had joined smoking cessation groups (and
Warner, 1994). Consistent with previous research (Wein- therefore made a public commitment to abstain) and then
stein, 1983), the process of behavioral review used in this relapsed (Gibbons, Eggleston, & Benthin, 1997). In this
study resulted in lower perceptions of vulnerability to study, smokers with high self-esteem, but not those with low
pregnancy. However, this effect was significant only among self-esteem, lowered their estimation of the health risks
those women for whom pregnancy was undesirable. In short, associated with smoking after relapsing. In sum, each of
when their risk and preventive behaviors were both made these findings suggests that people with high self-esteem
salient, the women who considered pregnancy particularly react to information that threatens their perception that their
undesirable focused more on their preventive behaviors, own preventive behaviors are efficacious in a more self-
thereby justifying their preexisting optimistic bias (cf. serving fashion than do people with low self-esteem.
Weinstein & Klein, 1995).

Self-Esteem Current Studies


Previous research and theorizing suggests one factor that The current studies were designed to examine the effect of
may moderate the relation between risk behavior and risk self-esteem on individuals' interpretations of their health
perceptions (i.e., acknowledgment of risk) is self-esteem. risk behavior. Study 1 was an experimental investigation of
For example, Campbell and her colleague (Campbell, 1990; the effect of self-esteem on women's estimates of vulnerabil-
Campbell & Lavallee, 1993) have demonstrated that individu- ity to unplanned pregnancy following review of their sexual
als with high self-esteem have more clearly defined and and contraceptive behavior. This study used a manipulation
more stable self-schemas than do individuals with low of the salience of sexual and contraceptive risk behaviors by
self-esteem. As a result, high self-esteem individuals are having half of the participants review the risk behaviors they
more likely to interpret self-relevant information in a had engaged in with each of their sexual partners (cf.
manner that is consistent with their self-schemas, and those Gerrard, Gibbons, et al., 1991). In this study, we predicted an
schemas by definition tend to be favorable. Similarly, high interaction between self-esteem and review such that review
self-esteem individuals have been shown to be more likely would have more impact on the perceived vulnerability of
than low self-esteem individuals to reject information that is low self-esteem women than on the perceptions of high
inconsistent with their perceptions and opinions (Cohen, self-esteem women. We also conducted analyses to explore
1959; Knight & Nadel, 1986). the relation between review, perceptions of birth control
More generally, high self-esteem individuals have been efficacy, and perceptions of vulnerability. Study 2 was a
shown to be more likely than low self-esteem individuals to longitudinal assessment of the effect of self-esteem on the
SELF-ESTEEM AND PERCEIVED VULNERABILITY 139

relation between naturally occurring changes in risk behav- nient would it be for you to get pregnant in the next year?" (1 = not
ior and risk estimates. at all, 7 = extremely).
Risk behavior. A pregnancy risk index was created, which
consisted of the product of the woman's frequency of intercourse
Study 1 and the actual failure rate of her usual contraceptive method
(Hatcher et al., 1992). Frequency of sexual intercourse was
Method measured by using the item "How often do you have sexual
intercourse?" (response scale from 1 = currently, I am not sexually
Participants in Study 1 were 125 sexually active, unmarried active to 6 = more than three times a week; see Table 1). The
female students at a large public university in the Midwest who failure rate for contraceptive methods was 3% for oral contracep-
agreed to participate in a study of health behavior. The mean age of fives, 12% for condoms, 18% for withdrawai, and 20% for the
participants was 19 years old at the time of the study. These women rhythm method. Higher values on the pregnancy risk index indicate
were selected from a pool of undergraduates who completed the ineffective contraceptive behaviors. For example, the pregnancy
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) and responded to risk for a woman who reported having intercourse "at least once a
questions about their sexual activity and contraceptive use in large month, but not as often as once a week" a n d who used oral
group screening sessions. contraceptives would be 4 (frequency) × 3 (the actual failure rate
for oral contraceptives), or 12. This index has previously been
Procedure and Measures demonstrated to be a valid predictor of pregnancy in a high-risk
sample (Gerrard & Warner, 1994).
Review. Data were collected from groups of 10 to 15 women,
with participants separated by empty seats to increase privacy. The Results
order of the questions was manipulated such that half of the women
completed a detailed report of their risk-increasing and risk- The mean self-esteem score o f participants was 54.9
decreasing behaviors prior to estimating their vulnerability to (SD = 13.51, range = 10-70). For illustrative purposes,
unplanned pregnancy (review condition; cf. G-errard, Gibbons, et mean responses for high and low self-esteem women are
al., 1991), which gave the following instructions: presented in Table 1 using a median split. The most
Starting with your first sexual partner, indicate ALL the commonly reported birth control methods were oral contra-
periods of time you were ~exually active. For each period of ceptives (53.6% o f participants) and condoms (35.2%); the
time indicate.., the length of the relationship, the frequency remaining participants reported using either withdrawal
of intercourse, and the method of contraception used . . . . (9.6%) or rhythm (1.6%). The modal response on reported
Please start with your first sexual partner, and work forward.
frequency o f intercourse was "at least once a week, but not
The order of the measures for women in the review condition was more than three times a w e e k " (38.4% o f participants). The
perceived efficacy of birth control methods, attitudes toward mean on the pregnancy risk index was 34.04 (SD = 24.93,
pregnancy (e.g., inconvenience and unhappiness if one were to range = 3-108). High self-esteem women tended t o be
experience pregnancy), review of sexual and contraceptive behav- slightly riskier on these dimensions, although there were no
iors for each partner (e.g., level of commitment, sexual activity), significant differences on the variables reported in Table 1
report of specific contraceptives consistency for current method,
(all ps > . 14). The mean response to the perceived vulnerabil-
and perceived vulnerability estimates. The other half of the women
estimated their vulnerability prior to reporting these behaviors (no ity item was 20.77 (SD = 20.12).
review condition). The order of the questionnaire materials for the Hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to test
women in the no review condition was perceived vulnerability the influence o f actual risk behavior, risk-relevant review,
estimates, perceived efficacy of birth control methods, attitudes and self-esteem on perceptions o f risk to unplanned preg-
toward pregnancy, review of sexual and contraceptive behaviors nancy. The order of variables entered into the regression was
for each partner, and report of contraceptive consistency for current as follows: Step 1 - - p r e g n a n c y risk index, serf-esteem, and
method. In Study 1, we used a 7-point Likert-type response scale review condition; Step 2---Self-Esteem × Review, Re-
with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (1 = strongly disagree, view × Risk, and Self-Esteem × Risk interaction terms; and
7 = strongly agree). Each of the measures is described below.
Perceived vulnerability. Participants responded to a question
concerning their personal vulnerability to experiencing an un- 1 We used absolute perceptions of vulnerability in Study 1 and
planned pregnancy (i.e., "What do you think the likelihood is that comparative perceptions of vulnerability in Study 2. We have used
you will have an unplanned pregnancy in the next year?") by using the term perceived vulnerability to refer both to the estimation of
a 1-100 scale, with higher values indicating greater vulnerability.I risk in absolute terms and to perceived vulnerability compared with
Perceived efficacy of birth control methods and attitudes toward others. Absolute vulnerability is usually assessed with items like
pregnancy. Because perceptions of vulnerability to unplanned "What is the likelihood that you will experience...?", whereas
pregnancy are likely to reflect perceptions of the efficacy of comparative vulnerability is operationaiized as "Compared to most
contraception, we also asked the women how effective they thought people your age, what is the likelihood that you will experi-
their method of birth control was. For example, one question we ence...?" Although optimistic bias has been demonstrated by
asked was "How effective are condoms in preventing pregnancy?" using both constructs (e.g., Welnstein, 1980, 1984), little is known
(1 = extremely effective, 7 = not at all effective). This variable was about whether people store information regarding their vulnerabil-
used as a covariate in one set of regression analyses and was also ity in absolute or comparative terms (Gladis, Michela, Waiter, &
used in correlational analyses. In addition, in order to address the Vaughan, 1992; Weinstein, 1984). The literature does suggest,
motivational components of the relation between self-esteem and however, that the two constructs are not redundant, for example,
perceived vulnerability, motivation to avoid unplanned pregnancy correlations between the two across different negative events have
was also measured with the following question: "How inconve- ranged from .22 to .75 (Weinstein, 1984).
140 SMITH, GERRARD, AND GIBBONS

Table 1
S t u d y 1: C o n t r a c e p t i v e a n d S e x u a l B e h a v i o r a n d A t t i t u d e s a s a F u n c t i o n o f S e l f - E s t e e m
High self-esteem Low self-esteem Whole sample
(n = 61) (n = 64) (N = 125)
Measure M SD M SD M SD
Failure rate of usual birth control method= 8.57 5.74 7.22 5.39 7.88 5.58
Frequency of intercourse b 4.51 1.04 4.42 .89 4.46 .96
Perceived efficacy of usual birth controlc 1.82 1.20 2.08 1.17 1.95 1.19
Number of partners 3.52 4.18 2.58 1.82 3.04 3.23
Pregnancy risk index a 37.34 26.20 30.89 23.43 34.04 24.93
Inconvenience of pregnancye 5.97 1.71 6.21 1.55 6.09 1.62
=Response scale for failure rate was 3% for oral contraceptives, 12% for condoms, 18% for
withdrawal, and 20% for the rhythm method, bResponse scale for frequency of intercourse was 1 =
currently, I am not sexually active; 2 = less than once a semester; 3 = at least once p e r semester, but
not as often as once a month; 4 = at least once a month, but not as often as once a week; 5 = at least
once a week, but not more than three times a week; and 6 = more than three times a week.
eResponse scale for perceived efficacy was 1 = extremely effective in preventing pregnancy to 7 =
not at all effective in preventing pregnancy, aThe pregnancy risk index was the product of the
failure rate of usual birth control method and frequency of intercourse measures, with high scores
indicating higher pregnancy risk. eResponse scale for inconvenience question was 1 = not at all
inconvenient to 7 = extremely inconvenient.

finally, in order to determine if the predicted Self-Esteem × ated the influence of the review process, such that low
Review interaction was moderated by risk, Step 3 included self-esteem women's perceptions of vulnerability were more
the Review × Self-Esteem × Risk interaction term. All affected by the review process than were high self-esteem
three continuous variables (perceived vulnerability, preg- women's perceptions. This relation was not influenced by
nancy risk index, and self-esteem) were standardized by the actual risk behavior o f participants, however.
using z scores prior to regression analyses (el. Aiken & West, Finally, in order to control for the women's perceptions of
1991). the efficacy of their contraceptive methods in the main
As c a n b e seen in Table 2, self-esteem was a significant analyses, we again performed the hierarchical regression
predictor of perceived vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy, analysis described above, this time adjusting for perceived
such that low self-esteem women reported higher vulnerabil- efficacy of contraceptive method, which was entered in the
ity estimates than high self-esteem women, (13 = - . 3 3 , first step of the regression. This analysis demonstrated that
p = .001). Also as predicted, the Self-Esteem × Review perceived efficacy predicted perceived vulnerability, but it
interaction was significant (13 = - . 2 4 , p = .04), such that also revealed a pattern of results that was virtually identical
review did not affect the perceived vulnerability of the high to the primary analyses. Once again, the self-esteem main
self-esteem women, but it increased the vulnerability esti-
mates of the low self-esteem women (see Figure 1).
Consistent with previous findings that optimistic bias is not a Perceived
function of threat (Weinstein, 1980, 1987), the Review × Vulnerability Low Serf-
0.8 Esteem
Self-Esteem × Risk interaction was not significant
(13 = - . 0 8 , p = .47). Thus, as predicted, self-esteem moder-
0.6

0.4
Table 2
S t u d y 1: E f f e c t s o f R i s k Behavior, S e l f - E s t e e m ,
0.2
and Review on Absolute Perceived Vulnerability
to U n p l a n n e d P r e g n a n c y
i O'
Measure fJ t p<
Step 1 -0.2 "
Pregnancy risk index .16 1.75 .08
Self-esteem -.33 -3.76 .001 -0.4 ........................... ..... • High Serf-
Review condition .11 1.30 .19 Esteem
Step 2
Self-esteem × Review -.24 -2.09 .04 41.6
Review × Risk Index .14 1.24 .22 No Review Review
Self-Esteem × Risk Index - . 14 - 1.49 .14
Step 3 Review Condition
Review × Self-Esteem × Risk Index -.08 -0.72 .48
Figure 1. Perceived vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy as a
Note. F(7, 117) = 3.46,p = .002;R 2 = .17. function of self-esteem and review.
SELF-ESTEEM AND PERCEIVED VULNERABILITY 141

effect and the Self-Esteem × Review interaction were the comparative vulnerability in a longitudinal design. Consis-
only significant effects. Thus, adjusting for perceptions of tent with Gerrard, Gibbons, Benthin, et al., (1996), we
the effectiveness of contraceptive methods in preventing hypothesized that increases in pregnancy risk behavior
pregnancies did not alter the relation between self-esteem would be followed by increases in perceived vulnerability to
and behavioral review. unplanned pregnancy. As in Study 1, we also hypothesized
It may be that people with high self-esteem respond to that this effect would be moderated by self-esteem. In
reviewing their risk behavior by selectively focusing on their particular, high self-esteem should dilute the effect of
risk-reducing behaviors; that is, the high self-esteem wom- increased risk behavior on perceptions of vulnerability.
en's reaction to the behavioral review in Study 1 may have Thus, high self-esteem women should be less likely than low
been to focus on their relatively effective contraceptive self-esteem women to adjust their vulnerability estimates
methods (Gerrard, Gibbons, et al., 1991; Rothman et al., in following increases in their risk behavior.
press). In order to explore this possibility, we examined the
effects of self-esteem and review on the relation between
Study 2
perceived efficacy of birth control and perceived vulnerabil-
ity to pregnancy by computing the correlations between Method
these variables for the high and low self-esteem women in
the review and no review conditions separately. Because we
believed that the high self-esteem women in the review Participants and Procedures
condition would be the most likely group to focus on their Participants in this study were college students in an ongoing
contraceptive behavior, we compared the correlation for this longitudinal study of health risk behaviors conducted at a large
group with the combined correlation for women in the other public university in the Midwest (see Gibbons & Gerrard, 1995, for
three cells. This analysis revealed that perceived efficacy of more details).3 Study 2 reports the responses of 273 women who
contraceptive method was predictive of perceived vulnerabil- had complete data for the variables of interest in the spring
ity only among the high self-esteem women in the review semester of their freshman year, or Time 1 (T1), and their fall
condition. The correlation for the high self-esteem women in semester of their sophomore year, or Time 2 (T2). The mean age of
the review condition was .45 (p < .01), compared with r = participants at T1 was 19 years old.
.16 for the other three cells combined; z test for the Students completed the questionnaires individually in private
rooms in a laboratory on campus. At each administration, a
comparison between correlations = 1.62, p = .05, one-
same-sex interviewer greeted the student, escorted her to a private
tailed; all other rs < .29, ns). 2
testing room, explained any changes in the procedures or questions
since the last administration, reminded her of the confidential
Discussion nature of the data, and remained available to answer any questions
that might arise. Each session lasted about 75 rain, for which the
The findings of Study I reveal a main effect of self-esteem students were paid $20.
on perceived vulnerability. However, this effect is primarily
due to the different reactions high and low self-esteem
women had to a review of their risk-relevant behavior. 2 In order to further explore the motivational component of this
Specifically, in the no review condition high and low process, we divided the high self-esteem review and no review
self-esteem women did not differ in their perceptions of groups into those who were highly motivated to avoid a pregnancy
vulnerability, but after reviewing their sexual and contracep- and those who were less motivated. We then examined the same
tive behaviors low self-esteem women had significantly correlation (i.e., between perceived efficacy of birth control and
higher perceived vulnerability estimates than did high perceived vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy) separately for the
self-esteem women. This finding is consistent with the high self-esteem, high motivation women in the review condition
hypothesis that high self-esteem individuals engage in and for the high self-esteem, high motivation women in the no
review condition. High motivation to avoid pregnancy was defined
cognitive adjustments that allow them to buffer the acknowl-
as responding to the inconvenience question with a 7 (61% of the
edgment of actual risks. The pattern of these results is also women); responses of 6 or lower were classified as less motivated.
consistent with past research regarding the mutability of low The correlation for the high self-esteem, high motivation women in
self-esteem individuals' self-perceptions and their depen- the review condition was .59 (p < .01), compared with .08 for the
dence on information for self-definition (cf. Campbell & high self-esteem, high motivation women in the no review
Lavallee, 1993). In addition, Study 1 provides correlational condition; (z test for the difference between correlations = 1.86,
evidence that high self-esteem women were more likely than p = .03, one-tailed). Although the ns in these two cells were small
low self-esteem women to respond to the review by focusing (only 23 and 22, respectively), this post boc analysis is consistent
on the efficacy of their contraceptive behavior even though with the hypothesis that high self-esteem women focus on risk-
the two groups did not differ in birth control behavior. reducing aspects of their behaviors when reviewing their contracep-
tive behavior and that this is particularly true for those high
Study 2 was intended to replicate and extend the findings
self-esteem women who are most motivated to avoid pregnancy.
of Study 1 by determining whether self-esteem also moder- 3 A number of other constructs (e.g., prototype perception,
ates the relation between naturally occurring changes in risk prevalence estimates) and risk behaviors (i.e., alcohol use, smok-
behavior and subsequent alterations in perceptions of vulner- ing, reckless driving) were assessed as part of the larger longitudi-
ability. Specifically, self-esteem and change in pregnancy nal study. Only data relevant to the current hypothesis are reported
risk behavior were used to predict changes in estimates of here.
142 SMITH, GERRARD, ANDG~BONS

Measures vulnerability (Gerrard, Gibbons, Benthin, et al., 1996).


Change in behavior was assessed in the same manner,
Self-esteem was again assessed by means of the Rosenberg entering risk behavior at T1 in Step 1 and risk behavior at T2
Self-Esteem OScale (using a 7-point Likert-type response scale).
in Step 2. In sum, Step 1 included perceived vulnerability
Pregnancy risk was an index consisting of the product of the
woman's frequency of intercourse over the last 6 months (an estimates at T1, pregnancy risk behavior, and serf-esteem;
open-ended item) and the failure rate of her usual contraceptive Step 2 included pregnancy risk behavior at T2; and in Step 3,
method. Perceptions of comparative vulnerability to unplanned the interaction of self-esteem and pregnancy risk behavior at
pregnancy was assessed by a single item, "Compared to others T2 was entered. Once again, all variables were continuous
your age, how likely is it that you will have an unplanned and were standardized by using z scores prior to regression
pregnancy at some time in the future?" Responses to this item were analyses.
recorded by using a 7-point scale (1 = much less likely than others, As can be seen in Table 5, risk estimates at T1 predicted
7 = much more likely than others). risk estimates at T2, (13 = .58, p < .0001). In addition, T1 to
T2 change in risk behavior predicted T1 to T2 change in
Results vulnerability estimates, such that women who increased
their risk behavior also increased their vulnerability esti-
The mean self-esteem score of the participants was 55.81
mates (13 = .26, p < .0001). More important, self-esteem
(SD = 8.30). Participants reported high levels of optimistic
moderated the effect of changes in risk behavior on changes
bias (i.e., low levels of comparative vulnerability) as re-
in vulnerability estimates as indicated by the significant
fleeted in the mean of the comparative vulnerability item at
interaction between self-esteem and the pregnancy risk
T1 (M = 2.2, SD = 1.35). The mean frequency of inter-
index (13 = - . 1 0 , p = .03). In particular, low self-esteem
course over the previous 6 months increased from 14.92 to
women who increased their risk behaviors increased their
18.36 between T1 and T2 (p < .02), with the high and low
vulnerability estimates to a greater degree than did high
self-esteem women reporting equal increases (p > .28). The
self-esteem women who increased their risk behavior (see
participants had a mean of 81.05 on the pregnancy risk
Figure 2). The slopes for both groups were significantly
index. As in Study 1, there were no differences in the
different from zero; for low self-esteem women, 13 = .26,
pregnancy risk index or in optimistic bias at T1 or T2 for
t(269) = 4.88, p < .001, and for high self-esteem women,
high and low seif-esteem women (p > .57). As seen in Table
13 = .10, t(269) = 2.04,p = .04.
3, participants in Study 2 had a mean number of 3.75 sexual
partners by T2, a number comparable to that reported by the
participants in Study 1. Participants reported that they Discussion
became sexually active around 16 years of age on average.
Correlations between the measures in Study 2 are reported in The results of Study 2 indicate that changes in risk
Table 4. behavior are associated with changes in comparative vulner-
A hierarchical regression was performed to examine the ability estimates and that the strength of this association is
effects of self-esteem and of changes in risk behavior moderated by self-esteem. More specifically, although in-
between T1 and T2 on changes in comparative vulnerability creases in risk behavior are followed by increases in
estimates from T1 to T2. The criteria variable in the comparative vulnerability estimates for both groups, the
regression were perceived vulnerability estimates at T2, effect is significantly stronger for women with low self-
with perceived vulnerability estimates at T1 entered in Step esteem than for those with high self-esteem. These findings
1. Thus, the analyses predicted change in perceptions of replicate the results of Study 1 and extend those findings in

Table 3
Study 2: Contraceptive and Sexual Behavior and Attitudes as a Function o f Self-Esteem
High self-esteem Low self-esteem Whole sample
(n = 140) (n = 133) (N = 273)
Measure M SD M SD M SD
Failure rate of usual birth control method (T1)a 9.63 1 4 . 7 9 6.82 4.70 8.29 10.99
Failure rate of usual birth control method (T2) 8.06 9.29 6.91 4.70 7.49 7.41
Frequency of intercourse over 6-month period
from T1 to T2 20.85 28.28 16.48 21.94 18.65 25.33
Number of partners 3.79 4.53 3.00 3.99 3.75 4.26
Pregnancy risk index (T1)b 81.88 130.12 80.19 128.58 81.05 129.14
Pregnancy risk index (T2) 108.75 170.60 90.09 141.41 99.62 156.99
Note. There were no differences for any of these variables for high and low self-esteem participants
(allps > .07). T1 = Time 1; T2 = Time 2.
aScale for failure rate at T1 and T2 was 3% for oral contraceptives, 12% for condoms, 18% for
withdrawal, and 20% for the rhythm method. ~I'he pregnancy risk index was the product of the
failure rate of usual birth control method and frequency of intercourse measures, with high scores
indicating higher pregnancy risk.
SELF-ESTEEM AND PERCEIVED VULNERABILITY 143

Table 4 Time 2
Study 2: Correlations Between Measures Used Perceived
in the Analyses Vu~era~y
0.4
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 Low Self-
0.3 Esteem
1. Seif-esteem (T1)
2. Perceived vulnerability (T1) -.06 -- 0.2
3. Pregnancy risk index (T1) .06 .37*
4. Perceived vulnerability (T2) -.09 .59* .24* -- 0.1
5. Pregnancy risk index (T2) .07 .23* .48* .35* - - High Self-
0
Note. T1 = Time l; T2 = Time 2. Esteem
*p < .001. -0.1
-0.2

two ways. First, whereas Study 1 addresses the impact of -0.3


self-esteem on vulnerability estimates after risk and preven- -0.4
tive behaviors had been made salient through review, Study
-0.5
2 demonstrates that self-esteem also influences vulnerability
estimates as people pass through naturally occurring changes Decreased Increased
in their actual risk. It would appear, then, that in general,
people are aware that changes in their risk behavior influ- Change in Contmcept~ Risk Behavior
From Time 1 to "Fwne2
ence their vulnerability to negative consequences of these
behaviors but that high self-esteem acts as a buffer that dulls Figure 2. Changes in comparative risk estimates regarding un-
this awareness. Second, whereas Study 1 demonstrates that planned pregnancy as a function of self-esteem and naturally
self-esteem moderates the relation between risk behavior occurring changes in risk behavior.
and absolute vulnerability estimates, Study 2 extends this
finding to the relation between risk behaviors and compara-
tive judgments of vulnerability.
Study 2 also provides insight into another issue of General Discussion
interest--the question of whether high self-esteem women's The current studies add to a growing body of evidence
(relative) optimism or low self-esteem women's (relative) suggesting that people with high self-esteem use self-serving
pessimism is a more accurate assessment of their actual strategies to maintain their optimistic biases regarding their
vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy. In this study, both the health risks. When given the opportunity, high self-esteem
high and the low self-esteem women thought that they were
individuals discount health information that is contrary to
far less likely than "other women their age" to experience an
their perceptions that they are engaging in effective precau-
unplanned pregnancy both at T1 and at T2. It is reasonable to
tionary behaviors (Gerrard, Kurylo, & Reis, 1991). When
assume, then, that these women's responses to the compara-
forced to acknowledge their less-than-perfect precautionary
tive risk questions at both times reflect an illusion of unique
behaviors, they react by enhancing their self-perceptions on
invulnerability. Thus, although increases in their risk behav-
other unrelated dimensions (Boney-McCoy et al., 1995).
ior are associated with decreases in their optimism, these
changes were not sufficient to eliminate the women's When they fail at an attempt to decrease a specific risk
perception that they were less vulnerable to unplanned behavior, they are more likely than those with low self-
pregnancy than were other women. esteem to lower their estimates of the risks associated with
that behavior (Gibbons et al., 1997).
In the current studies, we used both an experimental
manipulation and longitudinal data to demonstrate that high
Table 5 self-esteem can mitigate the acknowledgement of the rela-
Effects of Self-Esteem and Naturally Occurring Changes tion between risk behavior and vulnerability to negative
in Pregnancy Risk Behavior on Comparative Perceived
consequences of that risk behavior, a self-serving process
Vulnerability (Study 2)
that has been shown to be associated with self-esteem
Measure ~ t p< maintenance (Tennen & Affleck, 1993). For example, Gib-
Step 1 bons et al. (1997) reported that those participants who
Pregnancy risk index (T1) .03 0.57 .57 responded to their failed attempt to quit smoking by
Self-esteem -.06 - 1.26 .21 reducing the amount of risk they associated with smoking
Risk estimate (T1) .58 1 0 . 8 7 .0001 maintained their self-esteem, those who did not evidenced a
Step 2
Pregnancy risk index (T2) .26 4.90 .0001 decrease in self-esteem. More specifically, Gibbons et al.
Step 3 found that high self-esteem participants in the smoking
Serf-Esteem X Pregnancy Risk (T2) -.10 -2.16 .04 cessation clinics who relapsed later altered their perceptions
Note. F(5, 268) = 38.28, p < .0001; R 2 = .42. T1 = Time 1; of risks associated with smoking, and this alteration was
T2 = Time 2. related to self-esteem maintenance 5 months later.
144 SMITH, GERRARD, AND GIBBONS

Are Optimistic Biases Always Adaptive ? between high and low self-esteem women, the direction of
the differences is such that high self-esteem women take
A number of researchers have suggested that unrealisti- more risks than do low self-esteem women. This is consis-
cally positive self-evaluations and illusions of control and tent with preliminary evidence from two recent studies that
invulnerability are normal and can he adaptive (Abramson & suggested that people with high self-esteem are more likely
Alloy, 1981; Taylor & Brown, 1988). In fact, Taylor and than those with low self-esteem to engage in risky behaviors
Brown have argued that mild levels of these positive such as motorcycle riding, drinking, driving under the
illusions are positively correlated with psychological health. influence of alcohol, and speeding (Gerrard, Gibbons,
They point to evidence that only people suffering from Smith, & Hessling, 1995; Pelham, 1995; Sharp & Getz,
depression and those with extremely low self-esteem do not 1995). More to the point, there is indirect evidence that high
harbor illusions of control and invulnerability (see also self-esteem individuals' self-serving strategies for dealing
Alloy & Ahrens, 1987). The general issue of whether these with their personal vulnerability may reduce motivation to
illusions are adaptive or maladaptive has been the topic of a engage in precautionary behavior. A secondary analysis of
fair amount of speculation (cf. Baumeister, 1989; Colvin & the Boney-McCoy et at. data (1995) revealed that use of the
Block, 1994; Colvin, Block, & Funder, 1995; Gollwitzer & strategy of self-enhancing on other dimensions (when forced
Kinney, 1989; Taylor, 1989; Taylor & Brown, 1994; Taylor, to acknowledge sexual risk behavior) was associated with a
Collins, Skokan, & Aspinwall, 1989). On this issue, Taylor decline in perceived vulnerability. In other words, successful
and Brown (1988) have suggested that information inherent compensatory self-enhancement appears to have facilitated,
in the situation "offsets the effects of illusions and leads or at least to have been accompanied by, a diminution of
people to amend their behavior" (p. 204). For example, they perceived vulnerability.
argued that the need for mastery and the illusion of control Perhaps more suggestive of negative consequences of
can lead people to control their risk behaviors in spite of high self-esteem individuals' self-serving interpretations of
their illusions of invulnerability, and that mild illusions do their behavior is a finding reported in the Gibbons et at.
not interfere with the need to monitor reality. Similarly, (1997) study of members of smoking cessation groups. In
Janoff-Bulman (1989) suggested that people can maintain this research, high self-esteem relapsers showed a signifi-
their broad illusions while accepting concrete evidence of cantly greater drop in commitment to making another
their limitations. If this is the case, the benefits of high cessation attempt than did low self-esteem relapsers. In fact,
self-esteem may outweigh the costs, because the cognitive the decline in perceived vulnerability among relapsers was
biases associated with high self-esteem are kept in check by correlated with a decline in commitment to another attempt
aspects of the situation or environment. at quitting. Thus, it may be the case that high self-esteem
Recently, however, a number of authors have raised the individuals' facility to interpret their behavior in positive
question of whether these positive illusions can be maladap- terms may contribute to future risk behavior either indi-
tive to physical health. Clearly, negative consequences of rectly, by lowering perceptions of vulnerability, or directly,
optimistic bias regarding health risks (e.g., underestimating by lowering willingness to admit and reduce risky behavior.
the likelihood of having an accident while driving under the At any rate, the issue of the relation between perceived
influence of alcohol, or the likelihood of contracting HIV vulnerability and subsequent risk behavior, and the role that
from having unprotected sex) are potentially more danger- self-esteem plays in this relation, are important and defi-
ous than thinking that you are more attractive, socially nitely worthy of future research.
poised, or witty than others think you are. Thus, the question
becomes do illusions of invulnerability have the potential to Limitations and Future Directions
cause people to forego necessary precautionary behaviors
(cf. Flay et al., 1994; Tennen & Affieck, 1993)? More Several limitations of the current studies should be
specifically, does the tendency to interpret behavior in a addressed. Most notably, we did not have the variance
self-serving manner lead to maladaptive complacency regard- available in the behavior of interest to assess whether
ing their precautionary or risk behavior (Haaga & Stewart, differences in high and low self-esteem individuals' risk
1992)? Does it interfere with motivation to practice preven- perceptions translate into differences in risk taking (see
tive measures? What are the implications of high self-esteem footnote 2). The self-enhancing strategies of high self-
women's risk perceptions for actual health behaviors? esteem individuals that lead to altered risk perceptions could
Unfortunately, very little research has addressed the relation also result in greater risk-taking behaviors. Alternatively,
between positive illusions and subsequent risk and precau-
tionary behavior (Gerrard, Gibbons, & Bushman, 1996), and
none of this research has examined the role of self-esteem as 4 The longitudinal data set reported in Study 2 cannot be used to
a moderator of this relation. 4 address this question directly because the participants' sexual risk
Several researchers have demonstrated that children's and preventive behaviors had stabilized between T2 and T3 (6
months after T2). Specifically, there was very little change in
self-esteem is predictive of good health (Mechanic, 1980)
contraceptive method and frequency of intercourse between later
and illness orientations (knowledge of the correct proce- time periods (T2 and T3) as evidenced by the correlations between
dures when experiencing an illness; Lau & Klepper, 1988). failure rate of contraceptive methods at T2 and T3, r = .88,
Although the current studies do not provide evidence of a p < .001, and frequency of intercourse at T2 and T3, r = .58,
significant difference in actual pregnancy risk behaviors p < .001.
SELF-ESTEEM AND PERCEIVED VULNERABILITY 145

these biases could be held in check by environmental or cent initiation and escalation of smoking. Journal of Social
situational constraints and not translate into actual risk- Behavior, 35, 248--265.
taking actions (cf. Taylor & Brown, 1988). Research involv- Gerrard, M., Gibbons, E X., Benthin, A. C., & Hessling, R. M.
ing participants whose risk behavior is in flux is necessary to (1996). A longitudinal study of the reciprocal nature of risk
behaviors and cognitions in adolescents: What you do shapes
address these issues. what you think and vice versa. Health Psychology, 15, 344-354.
In addition, the results of Study 1 suggest that when asked Gerrard, M., Gibbons, E X., & Bushman, B. J. (1996). Relation
to review both risk-increasing and risk-decreasing factors, between perceived vulnerability to HIV and precautionary
high self-esteem individuals focus more on the factors that sexual behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 390--409.
reduce their risks (e.g., perceived efficacy of contraceptive Gerrard, M., Gibbons, E X., Smith, G. E., & Hessling, R. (1995)
methods). The current studies, however, like much of the [Comparison of high and low self-esteem adolescents' risk
research conducted in this area, were not intended to behaviors]. Unpublished raw data.
examine the underlying processes that mediate the relation Gerrard, M., Gibbons, E X., & Warner, T. D. (1991). Effects of
between personality variables, health information, and per- reviewing risk-relevant behavior on perceived vulnerability
ceptions of vulnerability. Future research could provide hard among women Marines. Health Psychology, I0, 173-179.
evidence of the mediating processes that influence percep- Gerrard, M., Kurylo, M., & Reis, T. (1991). Self-esteem, erotopho-
bia, and retention of contraceptive and AIDS information in the
tions of vulnerability. classroom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 368-379.
Gerrard, M., & Luus, C. E, (1995). Judgments of vulnerability to
References pregnancy: The role of risk factors and individual differences.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 85-93.
Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1981). Depression, nondepres- Gerrard, M., & Warner, T. D. (1994). A comparison of women
sion, and cognitive illusions: Reply to Schwartz. Journal of Marine's and college women's HIV-relevant behaviors. Journal
Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 436--447. of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 959-980,
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing Gibbons, E X., Eggleston, T. J., & Benthin, A. C. (1997). Cognitive
and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. reactions to smoking relapse: The reciprocal relation between
Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for dissonance and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
the future: Bias use of statistically relevant information in Psychology, 72, 184--195.
predictions for self versus others. Journal of Personality and Gibbons, E X., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Predicting young adults'
Social Psychology, 52, 366-378. health risk behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Baumeister, R. E (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Journal ogy, 69, 505-517.
of Clinical and Social Psychology, 8, 176-189. Gladis, M. M., Michela, J. L., Walter, H. J., & Vaughan, R. D.
Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and personal health (1992). High school students' perceptions of AIDS risk: Realistic
behavior. Health Education Monographs, 2, 324-508. appraisal or motivated denial? Health Psychology, 11, 307-316.
Boney-McCoy, S., Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Compen- Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. E (1989). Effects of deliberative
satory self-enhancement as a reaction to consideration of health and implementational mindsets on illusion of control. Journal of
risk among persons with high self-esteem. Manuscript submitted Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 531-542.
for publication. Haaga, D. A. F., & Stewart, B. L. (1992). Self-efficacy for recovery
Brockner, J. (1979). The effects of self-esteem, success-failure, and from a lapse after smoking cessation. Journal of Consulting and
self-consciousness on task performance. Journal of Personality Clinical Psychology, 60, 24-28.
and Social Psychology, 37, 1732-1741. Harrison, J. A., Mullen, P. D., & Green, L. W. (1992). A
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and the clarity of the self- meta-analysis of studies of the health belief model with adults.
concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, Health Education Research, 7, 107-116.
538-549. Hatcher, R. A., Guest, F., Stewart, E, Stewart, G. K., Trussell, J.,
Campbell, J. D., & Lavallee, L. F. (1993). Who am I? The role of Cerel, S., & Cates, W. (1992). Contraceptive technology. New
self-concept confusion in understanding the behavior of people York: Irvington.
with low self-esteem. In R. E Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). The benefits of illusions, the threat of
puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 3-20). New York: Plenum. disillusionment, and the limitations of inaccuracy. Journal of
Cohen, A. R. (1959). Some implications of self-esteem for social Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 158-175.
influence. In I. Janis, C. Hovland, P. B. Field, H. Linton, E. Janz, N. K., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief model: A
Graham, A. R. Cohen, D. Rife, R. Abelson, G. Lesser, & B. King decade later. Health Education Quarterly, 11, 1-47.
(Eds.), Personality and persuasibility (pp. 102-120). New Knight, P. A., & Nadel, J. I. (1986). Humility revisited: Self-
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. esteem, information search, and policy consistency. Organiza-
Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 196-206.
mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown Kulik, J. A., & Mahler, H. I. M. (1987). Health status, perceptions
formulation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3-20. of risk, and prevention interest for health and nonhealth prob-
Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive lems. Health Psychology, 6, 15-27.
self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for men- Lau, R. R., & Klepper, S. (1988). The development of illness
tal health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, orientations in children aged 6 through 12. Journal of Health and
1152-1162. Social Behavior, 29, 149-168.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). Mechanic, D. (1980). Education, parental interest, and health
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. perceptions and behavior. Inquiry, 17, 331-338.
Flay, B. R., Hu, E B., Siddiqui, O., Day, L. E., Hedeker, D., Pelham, B. W. (1991). On the benefits of misery: Self-serving
Petraitis, J., Richardson, J., & Sussman, S. (1994). Differential biases in the depressive self-concept. Journal of Personality and
influence on parental smoking and friends' smoking on adoles- Social Psychology, 61, 670-681.
146 SMITH, GERRARD, AND GIBBONS

Pelham, B. W. (1995). [On the limits of illusions: Exploring the (1989). Maintaining positive illusions in the face of negative
costs and hazards of high self-regard]. Unpublished raw data. information: Getting the facts without letting them get to you.
Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgments and Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 114--129.
perceived vulnerability to victimization. Journal of Personality Taylor, S. E., Wood, J. V., & Lichtman, R. R. (1983). It could be
and Social Psychology, 50, 502-510. worse: Selective evaluation as a response to victimization.
Quadrel, M. J., & Fischhoff, B. D. W. (1993). Adolescent (in)vulner- Journal of Social Issues, 39, 19--40.
ability. American Psychologist, 48, 102-117. Tennen, H., & Affleck, G. (1993). The puzzles of self-esteem: A
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. clinical perspective. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. puzzle of low self-regard. New York: Plenum.
Rothman, A. J., Klein, W. M:, & Weinstein, N. D. (in press). Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life
Absolute and relative biases in estimation of personal risk. events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39,
Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 431-457.
Schrauger, J. S., & Kelly, R. J. (1988). Global self-evaluation and
Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility
changes in self-description as a function of information discrep-
to health problems. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5, 441-460.
ancy and favorability. Journal of Personality, 56, 709-728.
Schrauger, J. S., & Terbovic, M. L. (1976). Self-evaluation and Weinstein, N. D. (1983). Reducing unrealistic optimism about
assessments of performance by self and others. Journal of illness susceptibility. Health Psychology, 2, 11-20.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 564-572. Weinstein, N. D. (1984). Why it won't happen to me: Perceptions
Sharp, M. L, & Getz, J. G. (1995). Substance use as impression of risk factors and susceptibility. Health Psychology, 3, 431-457.
management. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility
60-67. to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample.
Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 481-500.
the healthy mind. New York: Basic Books. Weinstein, N. D., & Klein, W. M. (1995). Resistance of personal
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A risk perceptions to debiasing interventions. Health Psychology,
social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychologi- 14, 132-140.
cal Bulletin, 103, 193-210. Weinstein, N. D., & Lachendro, E. (1982). Egocentrism as a source
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusion and of unrealistic optimism. Personality and Social Psychology
well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 8, 195-200.
Bulletin, 116, 21-27. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social
Taylor, S. E., Collins, R. L., Skokan, L. A., & Aspinwail, L. G. psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245-251.

S-ar putea să vă placă și