Sunteți pe pagina 1din 7

20 MARKETING INTELLIGENCE & PLANNING 5,3

M
arketers should not place too great a Decision Making and Communication Strategy
burden on the advertising element of Marketing and promotional campaigns are based on
the marketing mix. Advertising has a assumptions about the nature of the persuasion process,
role in creating awareness and comprehension, such as the objectives that must be achieved if consumers
but other aspects of the promotional mix serve are to move from a state of unawareness about a product,
the function of gaining attention and to acquiring positive beliefs and feelings about the product
stimulating action. which will prompt action. However, there is increasing
debate amongst consumer behaviourists as to whether
the common assumptions which guide the setting of
campaign objectives, or the measurement of campaign
success, can be justified. Not only is the generally
accepted process of decision making being attacked, but
research is casting increasing doubt about the nature of

Communication consumer information collection and usage, and the role


of marketer controlled information sources. The
implications of this for management are likely to be of
major importance, necessitating a fundamental re-
and the evaluation of our accepted generalisations.

While some of the rules which make up our framework,


Nature of or understanding, may be accurate representations of
reality, others require further consideration. For example,
based on previous buying experience, a customer may

Consumer believe that a relationship exists between the price charged


for a good and the quality of that product. While this
relationship may exist for some buying situations (such
as where pricing is on a cost-plus basis), it can also mislead
Decision consumers into making inferior choices if applied to all
purchases.

Processes It is therefore worthwhile to pause occasionally to consider


whether the assumptions, or models, we hold about how
consumers choose are accurate conceptualisations of
reality and whether they apply in our own product market.

by Keith Fletcher
Consumer Behaviour Explanations of Choice
The present dominant conceptualisation explaining
consumer behaviour is one founded in cognitive psychology
which perceives individuals as rational information seekers
and users who actively and purposefully purchase products
Introduction as ways of solving consumption problems (Howard, 1977).
An understanding of how consumers buy and choose While this perspective has been criticised (e.g. Foxall,
between alternative products is an essential basis for 1983; Nord and Peters, 1980), it is generally accepted by
creating a marketing mix which efficiently and effectively both academics and practitioners as offering the greatest
matches a product offering and a buyer's needs. The degree of insight and explanation of buying behaviour, and
analysis of consumers' buying behaviour allows the thus the rules and generalisations which flow from it can
marketer to know not only the relevant product attributes be used to guide marketing strategy. This is particularly
and benefits to promote, but also the most effective the case in the area of promotion where marketers need
communication channels to use during the promotion guidance on the choice of media, the design of copy
process. platforms and messages, creation of images, positioning
of brands and many other related issues.
Individual research findings on aspects of consumer
behaviour must, however, be placed within a conceptual Unfortunately, even within this prevailing paradigm, there
framework if worthwhile generalisations are to be drawn, is conflict regarding the applicability of the problem-solving
and the response of consumers to marketing stimuli and information-seeking conception of consumers (Ray,
predicted. 1973; Robertson, 1976). The debate centres on the amount
COMMUNICATION AND THE NATURE OF CONSUMER DECISION PROCESSES 21

of cognitive activity (thinking) that takes place before By 1972, Hansen was able to provide a summary of 28
purchase, and, thus, the nature of the advertising basic models of the decision process in the field of
campaigns needed to influence and promote purchase. marketing. The most widely known of these is probably
that of Strong (1925) who suggested the stages of Attention
Two distinct views have emerged which can be classified - Interest - Desire - Action (AIDA), while Rogers (1962)
into the high-involvement vs. the low-involvement model. suggested a diffusion model with the steps Awareness -
(Involvement is defined as ego involvement, the arousal Interest - Evaluation - Trial - Adoption. It is perhaps the
of the individual's commitment or stands.) Each of these models of Lavidge and Steiner (1961) and Colley (1961)
models draws different conclusions about the type of which had the greatest impact on our thinking about the
advertising claims that are likely to be most effective, the nature of the advertising process.
channels of influence that should be used, and the research
methods relevant to test campaign effectiveness. Lavidge and Steiner, in a brief article, pointed out that
consumers "normally do not switch from disinterested
individuals to convinced purchasers in one step. Rather
they approach the ultimate purchase through a process
High Involvement or series of steps in which the actual purchase is but the
The high-involvement model takes a problem-solving final threshold". They suggested that advertising can be
approach: that individuals strive to attain goals and thought of as a force, which must move people up these
solutions to problems, gather information on and evaluate steps. They describe the sequence as follows:
various alternative solutions to achieve a satisfactory
solution. The assumptions of strict rationality in problem (1) Near the bottom of the steps stand potential
solving, that people strive to maximise utility and have purchasers who are completely unaware of the
perfect knowledge in the search for the optimum solution, existence of the product or service in question.
have long been rejected (Simon, 1956). Instead, it is (2) Closer to purchasing, but still a long way from the
recognised that we "satisfice" in our decision making, cash register, are those who are merely aware of
within certain constraints or "limits of rationality". As its existence.
Simon states:
(3) Up a step are prospects who know what the
It is impossible for the behavior of a single isolated individual product has to offer.
to reach any high degree of rationality. The number of (4) Still closer to purchasing are those who have
alternatives he must explore is so great, the information he favourable attitudes towards the product — those
would need to evaluate them is so vast that even an who like the product.
approximation to objective rationality is hard to conceive.
Individual choices take place in an environment of "givens" (5) Those whose favourable attitudes have developed
— premises that are accepted by the subject as the basis to the point of preference over all the possibilities
for his choice, and behavior is adaptive only within the limits are up still another step.
set by these "givens" (p. 79).
(6) Even closer to purchasing are consumers who
The act of making a decision requires that a goal be couple preference with a desire to buy and the
identified, that information is acquired about several conviction that the purchase would be wise.
alternative ways of achieving the goal, and that a choice (7) Finally is the step which translates this attitude
is made between these alternatives, all conducted within into actual purchase.
the constraints of "bounded rationality". This decision-
process approach is the basis for most of the general They point out that the steps are not necessarily
models of buyer behaviour (Howard and Sheth, 1969; equidistant, that a potential purchaser may move up several
Nicosia, 1966, etc) and is explicitly stated in the majority steps simultaneously, and suggest the hypotheses that the
of "hierarchy of effects" models which attempt to relate greater the psychological or economic involvement, the
specifically to advertising (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961). longer it will take to bring consumers up these steps, and
contrariwise, the less serious the commitment, the more
likely the consumer will go almost "immediately" to the
It is, however, not new. John Dewey, in 1910, was one of
top of the steps.
the first people to consider the stages of decision making
and described them as:
This idea was taken even further by Colley (1962) whose
(1) a difficulty is felt; sequence Unawareness - Awareness - Comprehension -
Conviction - Action is usually referred to as Dagmar after
(2) a difficulty is located and defined; the initials of his monograph publicising the model,
(3) possible solutions are suggested; "Defining Advertising Goals for Measured Advertising
Results". He makes the point that advertising goals are
(4) consequences are considered, and virtually always communications goals, and thus sales in
(5) a solution is accepted. themselves were not a good measure of success. Thus,
22 MARKETING INTELLIGENCE & PLANNING 5,3

although the end goal is behavioural, the best way of In this case, information seeking is often based on trial
setting operational objectives is in terms of intervening of the product and later evaluation, leading to a sequence
variables. He went on to argue that objectives should be of Cognition → Behaviour → Attitude, or, as
clearly stated and should be aimed at the communication Ehrenberg (1974) conceptualised, Attention, Trial,
effects that were wanted, based on his hierarchy of Reinforcement. The sequence of the two hierarchies is
communication effects as given above. shown below:
High-involvement Hierarchy Low-involvement Hierarchy
These "hierarchy of effects" models seem to operate
when the product is central to the value system, when Creation and comprehension Creation of minimal brand
it can be clearly differentiated from other products, and of brand beliefs beliefs
when commitment to the purchase exists. In these cases, Evaluation of beliefs and Purchase decision/trial
the communication aim is to move people up the hierarchy, attitude formation
and certain strategies can be recommended. Awareness Purchase decision Brand evaluation/
and knowledge can be enhanced through high information reinforcement
content message. As consumers are actively seeking Initially, the low-involvement hierarchy was seen as
information, messages can be longer and will need less applying only to limited product classes, but it is possible
repetition. (Awareness rises rapidly in high involvement that high-involvement purchasing may be the exception
situations (Ray, 1973).) This would suggest an emphasis rather than the rule.
on print media. Attitude development is most easily
affected by a high credibility information source (Hovland,
In a low-involvement purchase, consumers are much more
1956), such as friends, or by trial experience with the
likely to make a satisficing decision as more products are
product. Intention to purchase can be manipulated by
seen as similar. They are likely to absorb a few key points
adjusting the perception of product attributes and their
rather than a broad-based information campaign. Because
salience at time of purchase (Fishbein and Azjen, 1975)
of a relative lack of active information seeking about
(i.e. value can be changed by special offers, etc). Long-
brands, selective processes are relatively inoperative, as
term behaviour gains reinforcement from product use, and
beliefs are not strongly held, wider claims will be accepted
advertising may be used by the consumer to support
without evaluation, and visual aspects are likely to be
existing beliefs and decisions, i.e. reduce dissonance
important. This would suggest a communication strategy
(Festinger, 1957).
based around broadcast media, incorporating image as the
copy platform rather than verbal product benefit claims.

Low Involvement A problem arises, however, in deciding which product


The above models are based on the belief that a particular purchases are likely to be high involvement, and which
cognitive structure must be created (i.e. a set of ideas low involvement. A study (Fletcher and Hastings, 1983)
and knowledge) that will lead to a particular attitudinal set into the purchase of life assurance suggested that while
which, in turn, creates a particular behavioural intent. this decision was a matter of interest and involvement to
individuals during the actual decision process, individuals
In other words, behaviour (and decision processes) is had extremely low involvement, both before consideration
determined by the beliefs, opinions and "facts" a person of the equipment and after the commitment had been
processes, by the needs, goals and values he has, and by made. Respondents often forgot the name of the life
the momentary control he has over his behaviour by given assurance company that held their policy, the nature of
features of his cognitive and motivational structure (Marken, the policy and many other details.
1974, p. 506).
A recent research study (Fletcher, 1986) investigated the
This model has been criticised on the grounds that the
first-time acquisition of a video recorder, which at the time
stages need not be uni-directional, linear or sequential.
was in the early stages of its life cycle, expensive and
The particular weakness was the claim that attitudes lead
technically complex. It was hypothesised that this would
to behaviour, which research has been unable to validate.
be a high-involvement purchase leading to the full learning
The low-involvement model has been suggested to explain
hierarchy of Problem Recognition, Search and Evaluation.
purchases where attitude change does not seem to
Instead, consumers had little involvement with the
precede purchase and the cognitive structure stays at a
purchase and followed a sequence closer to the low-
simplistic level.
involvement than high-involvement sequence, and showed
The low involvement hierarchy suggests the buyer may not distinctive use of advertising and information sources,
be particularly committed to his/her brand selection for many which differed from the use predicted by the high-
products. When this is the case, the consumer's selective involvement model.
processes are relatively inoperative and advertising is
passively accepted without undue resistance and without The problems this creates for researchers and marketers
thorough message evaluation (Robertson, 1976). has lead to the development of measures of consumer
COMMUNICATION AND THE NATURE OF CONSUMER DECISION PROCESSES 23

involvement (Zaichkowsky, 1985), a consideration of its is merely avoiding a "barren environment"; a need or
relationship to perceived risk and importance of purchase problem is not yet sufficiently motivating. In this situation,
(Bloch and Richins, 1983), which is an area worthy of the consumer may not be aware of the learning process,
further investigation. and the influence of the marketer is much more subtle.
The consumer's unwillingness to expend any cognitive
or behavioural effort on the message means it must be
Search during the Decision Process appealing, simple and attention getting.
From the marketer's point of view, what is important is
not simply understanding the nature of the decision This passive information usage was considered by Keil
process, but how this influences the individual's responses (1978) when he noted the importance of introspection,
to advertising communications. where a consumer uses the experience he has previously
accumulated about product and market from past search
Information-processing theorists have focused mainly on and incidental learning. The continual interaction with the
the internal mechanisms by which information is attended market-place, whether purchase takes place or not, is thus
to, interpreted and incorporated into existing knowledge. a gradual learning process. Consumers accumulate
While such a micro focus is helpful for a theoretical knowledge about markets, such as what type of stores
understanding, it has limited direct relevance to the media sell what type of products, what media or organisation
decisions that have to be made regarding the selection supply what kind of information, and what are the generally
of suitable channels, or the timing of suitable messages accepted norms concerning the various types of exchange
for maximum effect. To help in this area, the focus should that occur in the market. This form of learning has obvious
move from the internal processes which lead to message similarities to Aguila's undirected and conditioned viewing
acceptance to the external behaviour which has lead to and stresses the importance of the information
these messages coming to the individual's attention, and environment.
the function that this information serves.
Alderson and Sessions (1962) also comment on this
Aguila (1967) suggests that information acquisition can be learning of market structures over time when explaining
of two major types — purposeful search and undirected the so-called irrationality of many shoppers who seemingly
viewing. These, he suggests, are two ends of a continuum undertake little (external) search:
which can be arbitrarily classified into four "scanning
activities", i.e. undirected viewing, conditioned viewing, . . . in such purchases the housewife may be a more efficient
informal search and formal search. A choice is made as shopper if she spreads her information gathering over time.
to which scanning mode is used on a number of criteria. By buying for a while in a few different stores, she may soon
Formal search is the most effective method of gaining learn their characteristics. As a result, she will be able to
information, but also requires the greatest degree of effort purchase in the first store she enters confident that this is
and attention. Formal scanning could not, therefore, be the retailer who is very likely to meet her needs best (p. 141).
carried out for all informational requirements as it would
be too costly and time consuming. Thus, incidental learning is a means of obtaining
information about a product class and the brands that make
up the product class, and, as such, will later influence
Formal search is likely to take place when knowledge is possibly more structured patterns.
extremely limited, the problem is unstructured and
possibly ill defined, and information is needed to help
clarify the problem, identify alternatives and help in the As mentioned earlier, a common assumption is that the
selection of the "best choice". In this situation, it is likely more formalised search procedures will operate for
that a reasonably complete decision process will be expensive durable goods, and that passive incidental
undertaken, with, therefore, a matching formalised search learning will be reserved for low-price, frequently replaced
process using a number of marketer-controlled information goods, but this assumption is an over-simplification of
sources. reality.

Informal search is carried out when a problem is In the video study mentioned earlier, two main search
recognised, but possible solutions are known and the type patterns could be identified, which were classified into pre-
of information requirements are readily available. Less market and market search. Initially, respondents
information is required and less effort will be spent in conducted informal search of an undirected or conditional
acquiring it. The manufacturer must therefore simplify viewing nature, with little effort. During this pre-market
his/her message and make them widely available. stage, general knowledge and impressions were being
gained, often convincing the respondent that little formal
It can be hypothesised that undirected viewing and search was required, or would be justified. In this stage,
conditioned viewing are employed when there is no no intention to purchase existed and information gathering
immediate need for information and the decision maker was extremely casual.
24 MARKETING INTELLIGENCE & PLANNING 5,3

The "market search" stage followed once a deliberate


decision had been made to gather information, and during
this stage, a reduced set of alternatives was being
considered as to suitability. This stage, however, was often
extremely short and seldom followed a formalised
procedure, remaining casual and limited, with incidental
learning being at a premium. The majority of information
acquisition took place at the passive end of Aguila's
continuum which has obvious implications for a video
marketers' media and copy strategy.

Other studies of external search support this finding of


limited search for durable goods (Brandt, 1978; Newan
and Staelin, 1973), especially when search is measured
by the number of shops visited (Le Grand and Udell, 1964;
Dommermuth and Cundiff, 1967).

Information Sources
The accepted wisdom regarding the use of information
sources during the decision process is that the mass media
are most used by individuals during the initial stages of
the decision process (Mason, 1964; Beal and Rogers,
1957), while personal sources are most used in the later
evaluation stages (Engel, Blackwell and Kegerries, 1969;
Kohn-Berning and Jacoby, 1974).

A conclusion from this is that the mass media should be


used mainly to create awareness and comprehension, and
that it has little ability to persuade or influence attitudes.
This has lead to the development of the concept of
"agenda setting", that the role of mass media is to focus
consumers' attention on the values, products, brands or
attributes to think about, rather than try to persuade
consumers what to think (Sutherland and Galloway, 1981).
A later study (Ghorpade, 1986) tested this agenda-setting
function and concluded that advertising can focus attention
on which attributes of a product to think about (i.e. salience
in the audience's mind), and that this transfer of salience
can lead to intended behavioural outcomes, such as buying.

Other authors have suggested that the mass media may


not even have this limited role. Lazer and Bell (1966)
reported that awareness of innovation came from more
personal communications, and if more information is then
required, the mass media tend to be consulted. O'Brien
(1971) comments that "it is particularly interesting that
commercial information (advertising) has no direct
influence on ultimate purchase . . . such influence begins
solely with personal sources" (p. 40).

The video study similarly found an extremely limited use


of the mass media. The most dominant influence
throughout the decision process was personal influence.
Friends and relatives were used most frequently, and
seemed to have by far the most influence. In the pre-
market stage, friends helped supply knowledge about the
general market, and generally stimulated interest. They,
COMMUNICATION AND THE NATURE OF CONSUMER DECISION PROCESSES 25

thus, performed the "agenda-setting" functions, but they Personal influence may be more effective than persuasive
also gave information of a more specific nature which would mass communications, but at present mass communication
help in the market-search stage by clarifying potential seems the most effective way of stimulating personal
criteria and alternatives. influence.
Manufacturers have to follow the basic tenets of marketing,
The limited amount of visits to shops or stores which is that consumers must be analysed to find out why they
usually noted in studies of search behaviour (Udell, 1966; buy your product, and how they buy it. With this
Alderson and Sessions, 1962; Newton and Gilmore, 1969; knowledge, communication campaigns can be devised
Bucklin, 1966; William and Dardis, 1972) may lead to the which are complementary to the rest of the marketing
conclusion that they are not useful sources of information. mix, and which will be most efficient in gaining achievable
However, in the video study, shopping, specifically window objectives. Without this knowledge, manufacturers will
shopping, was the second major information source after remain in the position of knowing that half their advertising
friends and relatives (Fletcher, 1987). Respondents had is wasted, but not knowing which half.
a poor opinion of sales assistants' knowledge or veracity
and were generally not consulted until thefinalstages of Finally, eight basic dos and do nots can be listed which
the decision process, and even then had little influence. follow from what has been said:
However, window shopping, or walking round a large
(1) Do not assume involvement. Contrary to your
multiple or department store without talking to assistants, beliefs, consumers are generally not interested
was used to gather information about the range of in your product and could well live without it.
alternatives and the main product features, and seemed
to have a major influence on later choice. This form of (2) Do not assume your product is strongly
information gathering was often conducted walking past differentiated from competing products in the mind
shop windows. In low-involvement situations, passive of the consumer. You may consider your product
search is an ideal way of gaining awareness about products stands alone; the consumer may have a
and brands, as it involves the least effort, suggesting completely different set of values which makes
window and point-of-sale displays can play an important competing products interchangeable.
role. It is possible that this had led to previous researchers (3) Do not assume consumers will be interested
underestimating the importance of this form of search. enough to read your communications, even if they
are placed in front of them. Consumers tend to
collect information in the easiest and most
Conclusion
enjoyable way. This normally means friends and
window shopping.
Marketers should not place too great a burden on the
advertising element of the marketing mix. In both high (4) Do not assume consumers are irrational and fickle
and low-involvement situations, the objectives it can because they make decisions in their own
achieve are limited to creating awareness and idiosyncratic way. Within their own constraints and
comprehension. In the high-involvement situation, this may values, they are being completely rational.
lead to the increased salience of brands, or attributes of (5) Do try and build up an information environment
those brands, which may influence attitude formation and favourable to your company.
later purchase. In the low-involvement situation, continual (6) Do make your product, and information about your
repetition of an advertisement may cause changes in our product, widely and easily available.
perceptions of a brand or product, but in ways that fall
short of persuasion or attitude change (Krugman, 1965). (7) Do ensure distribution is extensive and displays
Persuasion or attitude change will take place in this prominent.
situation either through actual trial of the product or from (8) Do be aware of the importance of communicating
some other easily obtained/trusted source, such as friends a general image of reliability, dependability and
and neighbours, or personal inspection. trustworthiness. This helps consumers believe
the manufacturer's claims they attend to.
Emphasis should be placed on other aspects of the
promotional mix, such as sales promotion and point-of-
sale material, which should serve the function of gaining References
attention and possibly stimulating action, both in low and Aguila, F.G. (1967), Scanning the Business Environment,
high-involvement situations. Personal influence channels Macmillan, London.
and word of mouth (WOM) are also important channels
of information and influence, both in low and high- Alderson, and Sessions, (1962), "Basic Research Report on
Consumer Behavior", in Frank, R.E. et al. (Eds.),
involvement situations, although for different reasons. Qualitative Techniques in Marketing Analysis, Irwin, New
However, as Klapper (1960) has pointed out: York, pp. 129-45.
26 MARKETING INTELLIGENCE & PLANNING 5,3

Beal, G. and Rogers, E. (1957), "Informational Sources in the Kohn-Berning, C. and Jacoby, J. (1974), "Patterns of Information
Adoption Process of New Fabrics", Journal of Home Acquisition in New Product Purchases", Journal of Consumer
Economics, Vol. 49, pp. 630-34. Research, Vol. 1, September, pp. 18-22.
Bloch, P. and Richins, M. (1983), "A Theoretical Model for Krugman, H. (1965), "The Impact of Television Advertising:
the Study of Product Importance Perceptions", Journal of Learning without Involvement", Public Opinion Quarterly,
Marketing, Vol. 47, Summer, pp. 69-81. Vol. 29, pp. 349-56.
Brandt, W.K. (1973), Search Processes for Purchases of Major Lavidge, R.J. and Steiner, G.A. (1961), "A Model for Predictive
Consumer Durables: An Empirical Inquiry, unpublished PhD Measurements of Advertising Effectiveness", Journal of
thesis, Stanford University. Marketing, Vol. 25, October, pp. 59-62.
Bucklin, L.P. (1966), "Testing Properties to Shop", Journal Lazer, W. and Bell, W. (1966), "The Communication Process
of Marketing, Vol. 30, January, pp. 22-7. and Innovation", Journal ofAdvertising Research, Vol. 6 Part
Colley, R. (1961), Defining Advertising Goals for Measured 3, pp. 2-7.
Advertising Results, Association of National Advertisers, New Le Grand, B. and Udell, J. (1964), "Consumer Behavior and
York. the Market Place", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 40, Fall, pp.
Dewey, J. (1910), How We Think, Heath, New York. 32-38.
Markin, R.J. (1974), Consumer Behaviour: A Cognitive
Dommermuth, W.P. and Cundiff, E.W. (1967), "Shopping
Goods, Shopping Centers and Selling Strategies", Journal Orientation, Collier Macmillan, London.
of Marketing, Vol. 31, October, pp. 32-6. Mason, R. (1964), "The Use of Information Sources in the
Process of Adaption", Rural Sociology, Vol. 29, January, pp.
Ehrenberg, A.S.C. (1974), "Repetitive Advertising and the
Consumer", Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 14 No. 2, 40-52.
pp. 25-34. Newman, J. and Staelin, R. (1973), "Information Sources of
Durable Goods", Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 13,
Engel, J., Blackwell, R. and Kegerries, R. (1969), "How
Information is Used to Adopt an Innovation", Journal of April, pp. 19-30.
Advertising Research, Vol. 9, December, pp. 3-8. Newton, A. and Gilmore, D. (1969), "Consumer Behavior in
Carpet Purchasing: Relation to Cognitive and Affective
Festinger, L. (1957), A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance, Harper
Domains", Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 61, February,
Row, New York.
pp. 110-3.
Fishbein, M. and Azjen, I. (1975), Belief, Altitude, Intention Nicosia, F. (1966), Consumer Decision Process, Prentice-Hall,
and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Addison Wesley, Reading, Mass.
Nord, W.R. and Peter, J.P. (1980), "A Behavior Modification
Fletcher, K. and Hastings, W. (1983), "The Relevance of the Perspective on Marketing"; Journal of Marketing, Vol. 44,
Fishbein Model to Insurance Buying", Service Industries
Journal, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 296-307. Spring, pp. 36-47.
O'Brien, T.V. (1971), Tracking Consumer Decision Making",
Fletcher, K. (1986) Search Behaviour: An Analysis of
Information Collection and Usage during the Decision Journal of Marketing, Vol. 33, January, pp. 34-40.
Process, unpublished PhD thesis, University of Strathclyde. Ray, M. (1973), "Marketing Communications and the Hierarchy-
of-Effects", Marketing Science Institute Working Paper,
Fletcher, K. (1987), "Consumers' Use and Perceptions of
Retailer Controlled Information Sources", International Report No. 73-112.
Journal of Retailing, in press. Robertson, T.S. (1976), "Low-commitment Consumer
Foxall, G. (1983), Consumer Choice, Macmillan, London. Behavior", Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 16 No. 2,
pp. 19-24.
Ghorpade, S. (1986), "Agenda Setting: A Test of Advertising's
Rogers, E. (1962), Diffusion and Innovations, Free Press, New
Neglected Function", Journal of Advertising Research,
August/September, pp. 23-7. York.
Simon, H. (1956), Models of Man, Wiley and Sons, New York.
Hansen, F. (1972), Consumer Choice Behavior: A Cognitive
Theory, Free Press, New York. Strong, E.K. (1925), The Psychology of Selling, McGraw-Hill,
Howard, J.A. (1977), Consumer Behavior: Application of Theory, New York, p. 9.
McGraw-Hill, New York. Sutherland, M. and Galloway, J. (1981), "Role of Advertising:
Persuasion or Agenda Setting?", Journal of Advertising
Hovland, C.I. (1957), Communication and Persuasion, Yale
University Press, New Haven. Research, Vol. 21 No. 5, pp. 25-9.
Udell, J. (1966), "Pre-purchase Behaviors of Buyers of Small
Klapper, J.T. (1960), The Effect of Mass Communications, Free
Electrical Appliances'.', Journal of Marketing, Vol. 30,
Press, New York.
October, pp. 50-2.
Keil, G.C. (1978), An Empirical Analysis of New Car Buyers Williams, J. and Dardis, R. (1972), "Shopping Behavior for Soft
External Information Search Behaviour, unpublished PhD Goods and Marketing Strategies", Journal of Retailing, Vol.
thesis, University of New South Wales. 48, Fall, pp. 32-41,126.
Zaichkowsky, J.L. "Measuring the Involvement Construct",
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 12 No. 3, pp. 341-52.
Keith Fletcher is a lecturer in the Department of Marketing at the University of Strachclyde.

S-ar putea să vă placă și