Sunteți pe pagina 1din 22

Journal of New Music Research

ISSN: 0929-8215 (Print) 1744-5027 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nnmr20

Sound mass, auditory perception, and ‘post-tone’


music

Jason Noble & Stephen McAdams

To cite this article: Jason Noble & Stephen McAdams (2020) Sound mass, auditory
perception, and ‘post-tone’ music, Journal of New Music Research, 49:3, 231-251, DOI:
10.1080/09298215.2020.1749673

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2020.1749673

Published online: 13 Apr 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 167

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=nnmr20
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH
2020, VOL. 49, NO. 3, 231–251
https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2020.1749673

Sound mass, auditory perception, and ‘post-tone’ music


Jason Noble and Stephen McAdams
Schulich School of Music, McGill University, Montréal, Canada

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


The term ‘post-tonal’ embodies a broad distinction between musical explorations of new combina- Received 11 August 2019
tions of tones (‘post-tonality’) and explorations of sonic resources other than tones (‘post-tone’). A Accepted 26 March 2020
significant turning-point in post-tone thinking occurred when some composers replaced notes with KEYWORDS
masses of notes, or sound masses, as musical units. Existing definitions of sound mass are reviewed and Sound mass; tone; post-tone;
a new definition drawing on empirical evidence is offered. The perceptual principles that are involved auditory perception; music
in the perception of polyphonic music are demonstrated to also ground sound mass perception, with perception
opposite aesthetic goals achieved through radically different musical organisation.

1. Introduction: post-tonality, post-tone


We want to suggest that within the term ‘post-tonal’
Against our claim that there is no music in nature, it will resides a further broad divide, between two approaches
be objected that there is a wealth of diverse voices which to music that are distinct in concept if not always in prac-
wonderfully enliven nature. Must not the babbling of the tice. There is music that continued (and continues) to
brook, the slap of waves on the shore, the thunder of
avalanches, the raging of the gale have been the incentive accept the premise of Hanslick’s statement above – that
to and prototype of human music? Have all the murmur- tones are the fundamental building-blocks of music –
ing, squealing, crashing noises had nothing to do with and to explore new ways to arrange them. In this cate-
the character of our music? We must in fact reply in the gory, which is post-tonal in the sense of ‘post-tonality,’
negative. All these natural manifestations are nothing but we might list free atonality, serialism, minimalism, and
noise, i.e. air vibrations of incommensurable frequen-
other recent tone-based musics. Different from this, there
cies. Seldom at best, and then only in isolated instances,
does nature produce a tone, i.e. a sound of determinate, is music that broadens the palette of its building blocks
measurable pitch, high or low. Tones, however, are the beyond tones, accepting into its vocabulary a wider, per-
basic conditions of all music. – Hanslick, On the Musically haps unrestricted array of sonic resources. This category
Beautiful (1854/1986, p. 71) [Emphasis added] includes many strains of aleatoric, electronic, and noise-
The common term ‘post-tonal’ implies that music can based musics, and may be considered post-tonal in the
be understood in two great camps separated by a stylistic sense of being ‘post-tone.’ The ‘post’ in post-tone need
and historical dividing line. First, there was tonal music not imply a rejection of all tones, just as post-tonality
(and its historical antecedents). Then there emerged need not reject all tonal elements. But in post-tone music,
some musical styles that, while universally acknowledged the tone is shaken from its position at the centre of the
to encompass an enormous number of different prac- musical universe, just as tonal centricity and functional-
tices, may be defined in contradistinction to tonal music. ity are stripped of their unchallenged governing authority
The ‘post’ in post-tonal, on our reading, implies a con- in post-tonality. Both ‘posts’ represent a broadening of
sequent relationship, a way of thinking that is different domains of musical possibility: of combinatorial possi-
from, but preceded and made possible by, tonality: it need bility on the one hand, and of material possibility on the
not imply a wholesale negation or rejection of all things other.
tonal. Indeed, many post-tonal styles continue to employ The post-tone concept, and the musics that exem-
materials and techniques of tonal music, but with dif- plify it, are the focus of this essay. It fundamentally
ferent aesthetic goals and different methods underlying changed the status of timbre in music, which tradition-
their deployment. ally served as a mere delivery system for tone-based

CONTACT Jason Noble jason.noble@mail.mcgill.ca

© 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group


232 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

musical ideas, a sensuous sonic surface for the ‘deep and our model is sound not literature, sound not math-
structure’ of music, an ornamental or decorative ‘fill’ for ematics, sound not theatre, visual arts, quantum physics,
primary or essential ‘form.’ In post-tone music, timbre geology, astrology or acupuncture’ (1982/1998, p. 298).
assumes a primary musical role. Instead of notes, chords, Jonathan Harvey wrote ‘[i]t is not a question for me of
and rhythms, post-tone music deals in spectromorpholo- forsaking harmony and regarding everything as timbre,
gies, auditory gestalts, sound objects, sound itself. The rather that harmony can be subsumed into timbre’ (2000,
murmuring, squealing, crashing noises disparaged by pp. 13–14).
Hanslick become sources of interest, fascination, beauty, The post-tonality/post-tone distinction is also evident
and legitimate sonic resources for musical creation as in much recent music scholarship. Leigh Landy uses
the tone-centric distinction between musical and non- the term ‘sound-based music’ to denote ‘the art form in
musical sound is dissolved. Robert Erickson could almost which the sound, that is, not the musical note, is its basic
have been responding directly to Hanslick when he unit’ (2007, p. 17). Lasse Thoresen writes about ‘a clear
wrote that ‘multiplicity, sound-source groupings, sound dichotomy: between the pure sound-based music and the
perspective, sound as embedded in the natural order, pure interval-based music’ (2015, p. 76). Józef Chomiński
sound in its physicality and its endless variety, under- describes ‘sonoristics’ (related to Polish sonorism), whose
gird much of the significant music we are hearing today’ musical value is ‘of a purely sonorous origin’ (quoted
(1975, p. 193). in Granat, 2009, p. 822), epitomised by electronic music
The post-tone concept is manifested in well-known which reaches ‘into the very essence of sound’ (p. 825).
moments in twentieth-century music history. Arnold Post-tone thinking is illustrated in the sense of the term
Schoenberg, though perhaps most famous for his devel- ‘transformation’ used by sonic artists such as Denis Smal-
opments in post-tonality, was also an important post- ley (1993) and Trevor Wishart (1996). In Smalley’s article
tone thinker, as evident in his famous developments such ‘Defining Transformations’ (1993), he conceives of trans-
as Sprechstimme (or Sprechgesang), a speech-song hybrid formation as change in the ‘identities’ of sounds. This
form of vocal delivery in which pitch and timbre are may involve ‘source-bonded transformation’ when sound
constantly fluctuating (Griffiths, 2001),and Klangfarben- sources are recognisable, or ‘spectromorphological trans-
melodie,1 in which timbral transformation assumes a formation’ in properties of sounds whose sources are
musical function analogous to the succession of tones in ‘imagined, imaginary, or seem so remote that they appear
melody, (Rushton, 2001), as well as his statement that nonexistent’ (pp. 280–82). Smalley relates his spectro-
‘[t]one color is . . . the main topic, pitch a subdivision’ morphological ideas primarily to electroacoustic music
(1911/1983, p. 421). Luigi Russolo, in his futurist mani- (p. 280), but also states elsewhere that spectromorpho-
festo ‘The Art of Noises’ (1913/1986), calls for musicians logical thinking is appropriate for complex instrumental
to ‘substitute for the limited variety of timbres that the music that
orchestra possesses today the infinite variety of timbres in is more concerned with timbre than harmony (not that
noises’ (p. 28). John Cage’s ‘The Future of Music: Credo’ the borderline between them is very clear), more con-
(1958) states his belief that the use of noise would increase cerned with the motions of texture than with more
through the use of electrical instruments and accepted traditional layers of lines and melodic intervals, more
the possibility of substituting the word ‘music’ for ‘a more concerned with looser and flexible pacings than with
constant, metrically-driven time. (2011, p. 51)
meaningful term: organization of sound’ (p. 55). Edgard
Varèse was also willing to replace ‘music’ with ‘organ- The notion that post-tone spectromorphologies can
ised sound’ (Tedman, 1983, p. 6) and, in his manifesto be created with tone-producing musical instruments is
‘The Liberation of Sound’ (1966), imagined works in intriguing. How does this work? The underlying princi-
which traditional musical conceptions such as melodies ple was articulated in a famous critique of serialism by
would be irrelevant (p. 11).2 Pierre Schaeffer created one of the composers Smalley identifies as amenable to
music using real-world sounds, recorded and manipu- spectromorphological thinking, Iannis Xenakis:
lated in various ways in musique concrète (1966/2017, pp. Linear polyphony destroys itself by its very complex-
31–2). Gérard Grisey declared that ‘[w]e are musicians ity; what one hears is really nothing but a mass of
notes in various registers. The enormous complexity
1 The term Klangfarbenmelodie has (at least) two different senses: Weber- prevents the audience from following the intertwin-
nian Klangfarbenmelodie implies a succession of tones with contrasting ing of the lines and has as its macroscopic effect an
timbre/instrumentation, and may be used to enhance pitch structure. irrational and fortuitous dispersion of sounds over the
Schoenbergian Klangfarbenmelodie implies a transforming timbre that may whole extent of the sonic spectrum. There is conse-
be projected through a single pitch (Rushton, 2001). We refer here to the
Schoenbergian sense.
quently a contradiction between the polyphonic linear
2 This lecture recalls Varèse’s affinity with Futurist poetics; see Stojanović system and the heard result, which is surface or mass.
(2009). (1971, p. 8)
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 233

Here we see a crucial difference between music made whose organisation emphasises totality and minimises
of notes, and music made of masses of notes: somehow, the individual identities of constituent parts. Many great
in certain musical arrangements, the identities of tones works from the sound mass canon exploit a perceptual
appear to self-immolate, turning attention away from interplay between local and global listening strategies.
their relational properties as discrete entities and onto the For example, Ligeti’s Lontano (1967) opens with a sin-
sound mass.3 Although much has been written about the gle flute note, initiating a musical line in the traditional
artistic intentions and compositional methods of its com- sense. Gradually, more and more lines are added within a
posers, sound mass – which we hold to be one of the most narrow ambitus, and eventually the multiplicity of voices
significant developments in post-tone musical thinking may become so overwhelming to a line-focused listening
– has been surprisingly little studied from a perceptual strategy that a global gestalt may emerge in the wake of a
point of view. In this paper, we aim to lay a foundation for conventionally contrapuntal texture.
a perceptually grounded understanding of sound mass, in Like the more general term mass, which has a diverse
the hopes that future studies will empirically evaluate and array of meanings and connotations in various con-
apply the concepts articulated here. A necessary first step texts, sound mass (or soundmass, sound-mass) is used
is to define what we mean by ‘sound mass,’ which, as we in many different ways by scholars, and massed effects
will see, has proven to be no easy task. have been achieved musically in many different ways
It must be said at the outset that composers asso- by composers. It should be acknowledged that some
ciated with sound mass, including Witold Lutosławski have rejected the term, such as eminent Ligeti scholar
(1913–1994), Iannis Xenakis (1922–2001), György Ligeti Jane Piper Clendinning, in whose groundbreaking study
(1923–2006), and Krzysztof Penderecki (1933–2020), ‘The Pattern-Meccanico Compositions of György Ligeti’
each developed their own aesthetic agendas and musical (1993) the word ‘mass’ never appears. This avoidance
discourses. They do not constitute a ‘school’ in any tra- may be due to perceived oversimplifying or pejorative
ditional sense, and their individual approaches to sound connotations of the term, perhaps interpreted to imply
mass comprise only a subset of each of their composi- a coarse flattening-out of musical nuance, or an impli-
tional practices. They, and later composers who adopted cation that Ligeti, Lutosławski, Penderecki, and Xenakis
comparable practices, are referred to here as ‘sound mass were all doing the same thing. However, a review of the
composers’ as a matter of convenience, but this is not to literature reveals many uses of the term that account for
minimise the significant differences between them and musical variety and subtlety. In the following section, a
their respective œuvres. sample of 31 definitions of sound mass, gleaned from
books, articles, theses, and dissertations, will be exam-
ined and summarised (Table 1). Only passages judged
2. Defining ‘sound mass’
to have the character of definitions are considered here:
The premise of the sound mass aesthetic, as we inter- texts that use the term without attempting to define it are
pret it, is that many component parts group into a single excluded from the present discussion. The content of the
auditory percept; but the grouping need not, and indeed reviewed definitions will be critiqued in light of original
cannot, be complete. If it were as complete as the fusion of empirical research, and a new definition, informed both
partials into a human voice, for example, then the sense by historical usage and by our experimental observations,
of multiplicity would be lost and instead of a mass one will be offered.
would perceive a single sound source. If such percepts
were considered to be sound masses, then the concept
would provide no basis to distinguish a single tone on 2.1. Definitions of ‘sound mass’ in the literature:
a flute from the opening chord of Ligeti’s Atmosphères analysis and critique
(1980) and would be so general as to be of little use to The reviewed definitions suggest that the quintessen-
music theory or analysis. Furthermore, it is too reductive tial feature of sound mass is an emphasis on the
to say that listeners cannot hear the individual parts of sonic whole with a corollary deemphasis or attenua-
a sound mass: it is often possible to do so with focused tion of its constituent parts. Masses are denoted as
attention, just as one can strain to ‘hear out’ an indi- wholes [1;5],4 totalities or total textures [9;11;15;24],
vidual voice amid the din of the crowd. But there is a aggregates or aggregations [5;10;13;26], amalgams [8],
sense that to do this is to miss the point of the music, global entities [5;12;15], and congealed structures [27].
They may be conceived vertically, horizontally, or both
3 Danuta Mirka makes a similar point: ‘while all the effort of the serialist com-
posers was focused on individual tones, their audiences neglected that level
of musical phenomena and concentrated instead on large sets of tones’ 4 For ease of reference, numbers in square brackets in this section refer to
(1997, p. 6). Table I.
234 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

Table 1. Sources of reviewed definitions of ‘sound mass.’ collective spectromorphological behaviours or structures
Textbooks in which smaller elements may be subsumed [8;11] and
[1] Benward and Saker (2015, p. 347) as entities whose component parts are subordinate to the
[2] Kostka (2012, p. 233)
[3] Roig-Francoli (2008, pp. 280–281) overall effect [23;26]. They are characterised in terms of
Scholarly Books, Chapters, and Treatises fusion [3;11;18;27], saturation [30], oversaturation [29],
[4] Edwards (2001, p. 326) and the choric effect [5].
[5] Erickson (1975, pp. 175–193)
[6] Mirka (1997, pp. 8–10; 20)
[7] Schaeffer (1966/2017, pp. 112–13) 2.1.1. Density
[8] Smalley (2011, pp. 50–1, 55)
[9] Xenakis (1971, p. 9) One of the concepts most commonly invoked in relation
Articles to sound mass is density, which is mentioned in 15 of the
[10] Bernard (1999, p. 3)
[11] Drott (2011, pp. 7–8; 16; 28–9; 36–7) 31 reviewed definitions [1;3;5;6;8;10;11;13;14;15;20;21;
[12] Harley (1995, p. 221) 23;25;29] and is sometimes identified as a primary
[13] Harley (1998, p. 65) parameter of sound mass composition [3;23]. A requi-
[14] Klein (1967, pp. 123–24)
[15] Reiprich (1978, p. 167) site threshold of density for sound mass is sometimes
[16] Reynolds (2013, p.205) posited but seldom defined [1;20;23], and density is
[17] Strawn (1978, pp. 140; 143)
[18] Truax (1990, pp. 123-24) sometimes identified negatively as what is left when tra-
[19] Wen-Chung (1966, p. 3) ditional parameters such as harmony and rhythm are
Theses and Dissertations
[20] Besharse (2009, pp. 54–5) neutralised [25;29]. Authors vary in associating the term
[21] Foy (1994, pp. 1; 16; 23–5) with vertical pitch density [10], horizontal event den-
[22] Fulford (1979, pp. 2–3) sity [13], and timbral, spectral, or colouristic density
[23] Hoogewind (2000, p. 34)
[24] Iverson (2009, pp. 2–4) [8;14;15]. Textures, especially canonic [15;29] or poly-
[25] Lefresne (2005, pp. 100–1) phonic/micropolyphonic textures [1;5;11;21;23], are fre-
[26] Roberts (1978, p. 5)
[27] Smoot (1986, pp. 2–3) quently described in terms of density or implied to be
[28] Stephenson (1988, pp. 2–3) dense even if this word does not appear in the definition
[29] Svard (1991, abstract; pp. 22–3)
[30] Van der Slice (1980, pp. 9–10) [24;26]. Other terms might be interpreted as proxies for
[31] Wood (1974, pp. 7–8) vertical density, especially cluster, which appears in 10 of
the 31 definitions [1;2;3;4;6;8;20;21;24;29] and is some-
times identified as an exemplar or prototype of sound
[22;28;31]. Authors frequently emphasise the contrast mass [2;20;21;24] or a synonym for sound mass [29].
between micro-level construction and macro-level per- Terms related to dense deployment of pitches appear
ception [9;12;15;18;24], as well as the importance of frequently, including references to the packing or fill-
internal properties [6;18;19;20]. Such characterisations ing of regions of pitch space with semitones [3;10;21;25],
highlight stark differences between the poietic level of microtonal intervals [3;6;21], and glissandi [4;6;21]. The
production and the esthesic level of reception in sound importance of span, compass, or ambitus in delimiting
mass (Nattiez, 1987/1990, pp. 11–12), which is some- the range or identity of the mass is sometimes emphasised
times described in terms of a discrepancy between what [10;11]. A number of metaphorical images related to
is seen in the score (if there is one) and what is heard density appear frequently in these definitions, including
in the music [3;29], sometimes in terms of the diffi- blocks [3;5;6;14;17;20;23;25;26], clouds [5;13;14], webs
culty or impossibility of perceiving discrete elements [5;11;24], and knots [8]. In general, an enormously rich
[1;3;5;11;12;15;30], and sometimes in terms of the irrel- vocabulary of metaphorical imagery is used to describe
evance or minimised importance of discrete elements, sound mass. This will be the subject of a future paper.
especially pitch content [2;4;6;18;20;23;24;26]. In spite
of their diminished importance, such discrete elements 2.1.2. Complexity
are often precisely notated in more-or-less conventional Another recurring concept in this literature is complexity,
scores. In other cases, composers have adopted graphical mentioned in 10 out of 31 definitions [1;3;8;11;12;13;15;
notation for sound masses, especially the characteristic 18;23;26]. Sometimes the term applies to pitch formations
solid black bands that indicate filled regions of the pitch [3], sometimes to textural or contrapuntal intricacy
spectrum [3]. Some authors emphasise subjective aspects [12;18;23], and sometimes to sonorities or spectral con-
of sound mass perception such as psychological impact tent [8;13]. It is often identified as a requisite condi-
[2], the role of the listener’s attention in adopting a global tion of sound mass [1;15;23;26], although, as with den-
listening perspective [11;15], and recognition of the mass sity, the threshold is usually left undefined. The related
as a unit or fundamental event in spite of changing concepts of rhythmic intricacy or microprocesses [1;5],
surface features [22;30]. Sound masses are described as extreme activity [2], and extremely rapid succession of
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 235

tones [29] are sometimes invoked. Some authors empha- is largely attributable to the composer Edgard Varèse
sise large numbers of parts, events, or sound sources who coined it in a lecture in 1936, later published in the
[1;5;9;11;18;20;29], and statistical or stochastic distribu- manifesto ‘The Liberation of Sound’ (1966):
tions [9;18;24;30]. When new instruments will allow me to write music as
I conceive it, taking the place of the linear counterpoint,
2.1.3. Timbre and the inversion of hierarchies the movement of sound-masses, of shifting planes, will
Many authors observe that sound mass inverts tradi- be clearly perceived. When these sound-masses collide
tional hierarchies of musical parameters, diminishing or the phenomena of penetration or repulsion will seem
to occur. Certain transmutations taking place on certain
obliterating the role of erstwhile ‘primary’ parameters planes will seem to be projected onto other planes, mov-
of discrete pitch and rhythm [10;25;26], and emancipat- ing at different speeds and at different angles. There will
ing ‘secondary’ parameters such as sound value, texture, no longer be the old conception of melody or interplay of
dynamics, volume, gesture, register, and especially tim- melodies. The entire work will be a melodic totality. The
bre [3;6;13;15;23;24;29]. Several authors [4;5;6] explicitly entire work will flow as a river flows. (p. 11)
address the role of sound mass in diminishing the status Varèse seems to have used the term ‘sound mass’ in a
of the tone as the axiomatic unit of music, emphasising somewhat broader and looser sense than many other
instead new timbres and playing techniques. Klein even authors [27]. As Wen-Chung (1966) characterises it, a
claims that Varèse’s sound-mass music is ‘anti-harmonic’ Varèsian sound mass is ‘a body of sounds with certain
[14]. Cross-modal imagery of colour [3;15;24;29] and specific attributes in interval content, register, contour,
blurring [29] are invoked, as is diminished clarity of dis- timbre, intensity, attack and decay’ [19]; as Reynolds
tinctions between categories that had previously been (2013) notes, it follows that, in Varèse’s terminology,
considered separate, such as tone and noise [4;6;8;10] or ‘[o]ne must be prepared to consider any sound ele-
timbre and harmony [8]. Out of this body of definitions, ment . . . to have a sonic palpability meriting the term
only three mention homogeneity [5;6;24], though Smoot “mass”’ [16].
paraphrases it in the concept of ‘fused ensemble timbre’
[27] in which individual component instruments cannot 2.1.6. Other exceptions
be discerned. This relative lack of emphasis on homo- There are several definitions (or parts of definitions) in
geneity is significant in light of our experimental findings, this literature that seem to run contrary to the others:
summarised below. Schaeffer, in his Treatise on Musical Objects [7], invokes
the term mass as an approximate synonym for spectral
2.1.4. Time and space distribution or bulk, and characterises it as a property
Relations between sound mass, time, and space (literal of all sounds rather than a special category. Besharse
or metaphorical) are also emphasised in many of these describes soundmass as ‘essentially chordal in nature’
definitions. Authors differ somewhat in characterisations [20] and Kostka identifies polychords played fortissimo
of sound mass behaviour, with some claiming that they as a type of sound mass [2]. In emphasising chordal
remain fairly constant or static over a duration of time natures or structures as essential characteristics, these
[13;20;22], and others emphasising continuity, fluctua- authors run counter to the more frequently asserted posi-
tion, transformation, transmutation, contoured motions tion that sound mass diminishes the role of discrete pitch,
through time, plastic moulds of time, and constant or interval, and harmony.
dynamic change [5;8;9;15;18;19;31]. Some authors state
that sound mass may be either static or dynamic, or both
2.2. Experimental research: fusion, density,
static and dynamic [3;11;23;29]. The importance of pitch
complexity, homogeneity
or register space in sound mass identification is empha-
sised [13], and spatial metaphors such as plane, field, In spite of the profusion of theoretical and analytical
zone, volume, location, motion, movement, mobility, col- research on sound mass, there is an almost total absence
lision, penetration, and interpenetration are frequently of empirical study on sound mass perception in the pub-
invoked [5;6;7;11;14;16;17;19;22]. lished literature. Eric Drott (2011) offers an exemplary
analysis of Ligeti’s Kyrie that stands apart in its appli-
2.1.5. Varèse cation of the principles of auditory streaming, but does
While the term ‘sound mass’ (masse sonore) had been so in an ‘admittedly unsystematic manner’ (p. 9) without
a French locution in various discourses since the mid- empirical corroboration. Nearly all authors acknowledge
nineteenth century,5 the contemporary usage of the term the perceptual telos of this music, but tend to do so based
on intuition or self-examination of their own perceptions
5 Thanks to John Rea for drawing our attention to this fact. rather than intersubjective perceptual study. Therefore,
236 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

the generalizability of many claims in the theoretical components’).6 Density, Complexity, and Homogene-
literature remains to be demonstrated. ity were chosen based on use in the literature as
A first step towards addressing this situation was well as our intuitions about which semantic categories
taken in an experiment conducted at the Music Per- were most relevant to sound mass fusion. The terms
ception and Cognition Lab at McGill University (Noble, were presented on continuous sliders without explicit
2018, pp. 120–53). A complete account of this exper- numerical calibration as continuous ranges between
iment is beyond the scope of the present discussion: these terms and their opposites (completely fused–not
here we present selected findings germane to the sub- fused at all; dense–sparse; complex–simple; homoge-
ject at hand. Thirty-eight listeners heard forty excerpts neous–heterogeneous). They were subsequently coded in
of twentieth- and twenty-first-century music featuring the computer on a scale from 0 to 1. The definitions we
sound mass and related fusion-based aesthetics (e.g. gave for these terms did not specify musical parameters
granular synthesis, spectralism). Participants included such as pitch, rhythm, or timbre. Rather, listeners were
roughly equal numbers of musicians and non-musicians, left to freely decide which musical or sonic attributes con-
but statistical differences in their responses did not war- tribute to their impressions of density, complexity, and
rant treating them separately in the results reported homogeneity. After the rating task was complete, listen-
here. Excerpts were chosen to include a wide variety ers verbally described their method of rating for each of
of contrasting musical/sonic attributes, representing var- these categories.
ious ways sound mass may be achieved composition- Significant variation across excerpts was observed for
ally (see Section 3, below). Additionally, some excerpts all four rating categories, suggesting that listeners associ-
that were judged by the authors to have similar musi- ated them with variable properties in the music. Signifi-
cal/sonic attributes to the sound mass examples but to cant correlations between fusion and the other categories
be less perceptually assimilated were included as counter- were also observed, suggesting that density, complexity,
examples, such that a range of degrees of sound mass and homogeneity do indeed relate meaningfully to sound
fusion were represented in the stimuli. Excerpts were mass fusion. However, there were some big surprises
approximately 15 s in duration and maintained rela- regarding the nature and robustness of these correla-
tively consistent textures over their durations. Listeners tions. If the content of the published definitions of sound
were provided the following non-technical explanation of mass reviewed above is an index, one would expect all
‘sound mass’: three categories to correlate positively with sound mass
Sound mass exists when multiple sound events or sources fusion, with density having the greatest effect, complex-
are heard as a single meaningful unit. Examples are ity the next greatest, and homogeneity the least. This is
encountered on a daily basis: a large crowd of people, not at all what was observed. Each of the graphs in Fig-
a flock of birds, rustling leaves, traffic noise, shattering ures 1–3 presents the stimuli in ascending order for aver-
glass, etc. In each of these cases, the individual sound
sources are no longer heard as individuals, but they
aged Fusion ratings, with Fusion presented as a dotted
contribute to the sound of the whole (i.e. the sound line and Density, Complexity, and Homogeneity, respec-
mass). tively, superposed as a solid black line, to demonstrate the
Music, especially contemporary music, frequently makes correlations between them. A positive but relatively mod-
use of this phenomenon, creating textures in which many est correlation (r = .491) was found between sound mass
notes, sounds, instruments, voices, etc. are grouped into fusion and density (Figure 1).
a mass. Musical sound masses may be very different from A robust negative correlation (r = −.828) was
one another in their sound quality, organization, and observed between sound mass fusion and complexity
behavior, but they always involve multiple sounds being
(Figure 2). The strongest relationship was a robust pos-
grouped into a unit. In this study, we will refer to this
grouping of sounds into a mass as fusion. There may be itive correlation (r = .865) between sound mass fusion
degrees of fusion, as some sounds may be grouped more and homogeneity (Figure 3). Table 2 lists the stimuli
strongly than others. It is possible for some of the sounds with numbers corresponding to the graphs (x axis), in
that you hear at any given time to fuse into a mass while descending order for Fusion ratings. Based on listeners’
others retain their individual identities, as when a single verbal descriptions, homogeneity is strongly associated
voice stands out from a crowd.
with timbre, complexity is most associated with rhythm
Listeners provided one rating for each musical excerpt
on each of four different scales: Fusion (as defined
in the above explanation), Density (defined as ‘com- 6 There were also two other blocks in the experiment, in which participants
pactness of sound components’), Complexity (‘intri- rated the same stimuli along batteries of other semantic scales derived from
the published literature on sound mass. It is beyond the scope of this paper
cacy or interconnectedness of sound components’), to report on these other blocks and their subsequent analyses, but they are
and Homogeneity (‘degree of similarity between sound detailed in Noble (2018) and will be the subject of a future paper.
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 237

Figure 1. Ratings for 40 excerpts for Fusion and Density, arranged in increasing order for Fusion ratings.

Figure 2. Ratings for 40 excerpts for Fusion and Complexity, arranged in increasing order for Fusion ratings.

and/or texture, and density is interpreted both verti- responses lies in a confusion of perceived density, com-
cally as number of layers and horizontally as rhythmic plexity, and homogeneity with acoustical density, com-
organisation, as well as in terms of metaphorical imagery plexity, and homogeneity. In particular, the negative
(especially spatial imagery). correlation between complexity and fusion may seem
counterintuitive because sound masses are often char-
acterised by highly complex musical constructions and
2.2.1. Perceptual vs. acoustical attributes acoustical signals, but it makes sense in terms of the limits
A possible explanation for the observed differences of of our auditory information processing capabilities. After
emphases between published definitions and listener a certain point, we are no longer able to parse incoming

Figure 3. Ratings for 40 excerpts for Fusion and Homogeneity, arranged in increasing order for Fusion ratings.
238 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

Table 2. Sources of the 40 excerpts.


Fusion Ranking Composer Title Stimulus Name Location in Score Time in Recording
40 György Ligeti Atmosphères Atmosphères (2) m. 40 3:45
39 Krzysztof Penderecki Polymorphia Polymorphia (1) R. 8 1:35
38 Iannis Xenakis Mycenae Alpha Mycenae Alpha beginning 0:00
37 Jonathan Harvey Mortuos Plango, Vivos Voco Mortuos Plango – 2:44
36 Jean-Claude Risset Sud Sud n/a 1:55
35 Iannis Xenakis Metastaseis Metastaseis m. 317 8:00
34 Kaija Saariaho Du cristal Du Cristal R. H 3:02
33 Krzysztof Penderecki Threnody to the Victims of Threnody R. 70 9:08
Hiroshima
32 György Ligeti Atmosphères Atmosphères (1) m. 1 0:00
31 Witold Lutosławski Mi-parti Mi-Parti R. 40 10:04
30 Henryk Górecki Symphony No. 3 Gorecki Symphony No. 3 m. 26 1:12
29 Toru Takemitsu Asterism Asterism (1) m. 88 6:50
28 György Ligeti Volumina Volumina R. 1 0:15
27 György Ligeti Requiem Requiem m. 3 0:12
26 Philippe Hurel Six Miniatures en Trompe-l’œil Six Miniatures vi. m. 32 vi. 1:58
25 Veljo Tormis Jaanilaulud Jaanilaulud m. 104 4:22
24 Gérard Grisey Partiels Partiels m. 1 0:05
23 György Ligeti Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe Ligeti Double Concerto (1) m. 32 II: 1:15
and Orchestra
22 Trevor Wishart Vox Vox n/a 2:18
21 Krzysztof Penderecki Polymorphia Polymorphia (2) R. 17 2:50
20 Witold Lutosławski Musique funèbre Musique Funèbre R. 240 9:05
19 Barry Truax The Wings of Nike Wings of Nike n/a II: 0:30
18 Georg F. Haas Hyperion Hyperion R. L 25:08
17 Barry Truax Riverrun Riverrun n/a 2:15
16 György Ligeti Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe Ligeti Double Concerto (3) II: m. 1 II: 0:00
and Orchestra
15 György Ligeti Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe Ligeti Double Concerto (2) II: m. 56 II: 2:04
and Orchestra
14 Witold Lutosławski Double Concerto for Oboe, Harp Lutosławski Double Concerto m. 1 0:00
and Chamber Orchestra
13 Barry Truax Pacific Pacific n/a IV. 9:20
12 Karlheinz Stockhausen Stimmung Stimmung R. 12 mvt. 12, 0:12
11 Stéphane Roy Crystal Music Crystal Music n/a 8:55
10 Franck Bedrossian Tracés d’Ombres Tracés d’Ombres II. m. 5 3:20
9 Francis Dhomont Points de Fuite Points de Fuites n/a 2:18
8 Witold Lutosławski Symphony No. 2 Lutosławski Symohony No. 2 R. 1 0:20
7 Witold Lutosławski Trois Poèmes d’Henri Michaux Trois Poèmes d’Henri Michaux II: R. 48 II: 2:45
6 Iannis Xenakis Pithoprakta Pithoprakta m. 49 2:14
5 György Ligeti Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe Ligeti Double Concerto (4) II: m. 121 4:30
and Orchestra
4 Stéphane Roy Mimetismo Mimetismo n/a 2:20
3 Witold Lutosławski Jeux vénitiens Jeux Vénitiens R. A 0:01
2 Toru Takemitsu Asterism Asterism (2) R. F 10:05
1 Robert Normandeau Clair de Terre Clair de Terre n/a 13: 0:15

information into meaningful units, and increasing acous- mass (Iverson, 2009). With respect to density, it is not
tical complexity results in decreasing perceived complex- guaranteed that those musical constructions or acousti-
ity as superimposed acoustical details cancel each other cal signals that appear to be the densest will necessarily be
out and meld together in a perceptual representation.7 perceived that way. As noted above, the reviewed defini-
Stockhausen made this point in observing that at a cer- tions of sound mass make frequent mention of clusters as
tain point of complexity, sound events lose their rhythmic exemplars of density, with the tacit assumption that chro-
character and instead contribute to the timbre of the matically or microtonally filled pitch space is the densest.
But in a pilot study conducted with Eddy Kazazis at the
7 Lasse Thoresen describes this situation as Music Perception and Cognition Lab at McGill Univer-
a category in which the very complex and the very sim- sity (Noble, 2018, pp. 58–67), in which 22 listeners pro-
ple meet in a paradoxical, ambivalent union. We shall call vided ratings for various groups of notes on scales includ-
this form-element paradoxical complexity—a specific case ing density, it was found that the sheer number of notes,
of the classical coincidentia oppositorum, the unity of oppo- regardless of their distribution in pitch space, was by far
sites. It applies to objects with myriad details, but with a the strongest predictor of perceived density, accounting
perceptually simple overall character. A homogenous accu-
mulation . . . may often qualify for this category. (2015, for over 90% of the variance in the ratings. That is, a group
p. 457) of eight notes tended to have higher ratings for perceived
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 239

density than a group of six notes, whether those notes cannot stop at positing the requirements of density, sim-
were distributed as a chromatic cluster, two smaller clus- plicity, and homogeneity, but must relate these concepts
ters separated by a wide gap, an approximately tertian to perceptual principles. This is the aim of the follow-
chord with more conventional voicing, or various other ing section, with reference to several illustrative examples
distributions (Figure 4). from the sound mass repertoire.
As for perceived homogeneity, its robust correla-
tion with sound mass fusion seems highly intuitive: by
definition, the more heterogeneous an auditory percept, 3. Sound mass and perceptual principles
the less cohesive and fused it is. As such, it is interest-
ing that this term is so much less emphasised in the Although the perceptual principles in question are little-
reviewed body of sound mass definitions than density studied with respect to sound mass, their roles in other
and complexity. types of music have been examined in detail. In particu-
lar, polyphonic music, whose voice leading maintains the
concurrent independence of multiple auditory streams,
2.3. Proposed definition of ‘sound mass’ applies the same principles in reverse: that is, the same
In our opinion, no published definition of which we are principles that allow the lines in a fugue to segregate
aware sufficiently accounts for both poietic and esthesic into separate streams also allow the lines of microp-
sides of the semiological tripartition, and no study of olyphony to assimilate into a mass. In his 2001 paper
which we are aware sufficiently explicates the sound mass ‘Tone and voice: A derivation of the rules of voice-leading
concept in terms of perceptual principles. These are two from perceptual principles,’ David Huron explains the
major aims of the present study. The definition we pro- rules of polyphonic voice-leading in terms of principles
pose is: of auditory perception. He does not claim that voice-
leading rules are or ought to be musical universals, but
Sound mass: A perceptually homogeneous and dense rather states that ‘even if the “rules of voice leading” were
auditory unit integrating multiple sound events or com-
ponents while retaining an impression of multiplicity.
shown to be somehow immutable, their musical rele-
Although their acoustical correlates may be highly com- vance depends entirely on the acceptance of the underly-
plex, sound masses are perceptually simple because they ing perceptual goals . . . different genres might manifest
resist perceptual segregation in one or more parameters different perceptual goals that evoke pleasure in other
(e.g. pitch, rhythm, timbre). ways’ (pp. 56–58). This is just the situation we encounter
The foregoing discussion has described why sound in sound mass music. The same principles that ground
masses are taken to be perceptually dense and homo- tonal voice-leading rules remain operative, but due to
geneous, why they are taken to be auditory units inte- radically different musical organisation, they serve to
grating multiple sound events or components, why they attenuate the perceptual independence of lines and to
are perceptually simple even though their acoustical cor- assimilate them into an auditory unit. Since Huron’s
relates may be highly complex, and why the retention paper provides a systematic overview of these princi-
of an impression of multiplicity is an essential feature. ples, the following discussion will present them as Huron
The remaining qualification – resistance of perceptual does, and then demonstrate their relevance to techniques
segregation in one or more parameters – is complicated of sound mass composition with references to selected
and requires a detailed exposition. Perceptual integration examples of twentieth- and twenty-first-century music.
(qua resistance of segregation) can be effected in many These principles are by no means mutually exclusive, and
different ways: even a cursory review of the sound mass frequently operate together.
canon (or of contemporary music in general) reveals that The principles Huron invokes are:
the techniques composers have used to bring about per-
ceptual integration are numerous and varied. Sound mass (1) Toneness Principle
effects are united, as we have claimed, by their perceptual (2) Principle of Temporal Continuity
density, simplicity, and homogeneity, but the threshold (3) Minimum Masking Principle
values that define what is perceptually dense, simple, and (4) Tonal Fusion Principle
homogeneous vary widely depending on musical context (5) Pitch Proximity Principle
– in terms of covariant factors such as timbre, register, (6) Pitch Co-modulation Principle
dynamics, tempo, rhythmic activity, pitch distribution, (7) Onset Synchrony Principle
and so forth – due to peculiarities of human auditory per- (8) Principle of Limited Density
ception and music cognition. Therefore, if we wish for (9) Timbral Differentiation Principle
our account of sound mass perception to be thorough, we (10) Source Location Principle
240 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

Figure 4. Chords from pilot study, arranged in increasing order for listeners’ ratings for Density.

3.1. Toneness principle (1962), Penderecki calls for a wide variety of noise-based
sounds, including the key-click and typewriter section at
Strong auditory images are evoked when tones exhibit
a high degree of toneness. A useful measure of tone- rehearsal 20.
ness is provided by virtual pitch weight. Tones having the
highest virtual pitch weights are harmonic complex tones
centered in the region between F2 and G5. Tones hav-
3.2. Principle of temporal continuity
ing inharmonic partials produce competing virtual pitch In order to evoke strong auditory streams, use continu-
perceptions, and so evoke more diffuse auditory images. ous or recurring rather than brief or intermittent sound
(Huron, 2001, p. 10) sources. Intermittent sounds should be separated by no
The toneness principle provides the perceptual foun- more than roughly 800 ms of silence in order to ensure
the perception of continuity. (p. 12)
dation for tone-centrism in traditional Western music,
favouring ‘harmonic complex tones’ (multiple partials Temporal continuity is usually a property of sound
related by simple ratios) over inharmonic sounds (irra- masses in toto, but discontinuity in individual parts
tionally related partials) or noise-based sounds (random sometimes helps prevent their constituent sound events
or pseudo-random distribution of energy) as producers from forming separate streams that can be perceptually
of strong auditory images. Another major factor in tone- segregated from the mass. This may be achieved by sepa-
ness is register. Although human hearing extends roughly rating events with rests, as in section 43 of Lutosławski’s
from 20 Hz to 20 kHz, pitch perception is not uniform Symphony No. 2 (1973) in which progressively elon-
throughout this range: towards the bottom end we hear gated rests inhibit internal stream integration within
indistinct ‘rumbling,’ and towards the top we hear indis- parts, favouring instead global integration between parts
tinct ‘sizzling,’ with clear pitch perception residing in the (Figure 5).
range from about 30 Hz to about 4000–5000 Hz (Plack,
2014, pp. 118–19). As Huron observes, we are maximally 3.3. Minimum masking principle
sensitive to pitch between F 2 (87 Hz) and G5 (784 Hz),
roughly coterminous with the range covered by the grand In order to minimize auditory masking within some
staff and the combined ranges of the adult male and vertical sonority, approximately equivalent amounts of
spectral energy should fall in each critical band. For typ-
female voices. Harmonic spectra with fundamentals in ical complex harmonic tones, this generally means that
this range – such as most sounds produced by orches- simultaneously sounding notes should be more widely
tral instruments – have a strong tendency to stand out spaced as the register descends. (p. 18)
as distinct tones, thereby resisting or destroying sound
What Huron refers to as ‘masking’ might be more gener-
mass integration unless their pitch salience is attenuated
ally framed as the interference between vibrations on the
in some way.
basilar membrane (the part of the cochlea that transduces
Sound mass composers have responded to this per-
incoming sound signals into neural impulses), causing
ceptual challenge in several ways, such as by embracing
beating and sensory dissonance.8 The term critical band
extreme registers in their musical vocabularies. Undoubt-
refers to any region approximately one-millimeter long
edly the most famous example of a high-register mass
on the basilar membrane: simultaneous frequencies that
in Penderecki’s œuvre is the opening of Threnody for
fall within a critical band are close enough to physically
the Victims of Hiroshima (1961b), in which the string
interfere with one another. In terms of perceived pitch,
instruments play the highest (indeterminate) pitch avail-
the interval covered per critical band is much greater in
able on their instrument, fortissimo. Penderecki exploits
low registers than in high: as a result, two tones separated
the extreme low register in the opening section of Poly-
morphia (1963), which features very low notes in the 8 Huron’s usage differs somewhat from the way the term ‘masking’ is used
celli and double basses. Another way to subvert the by some other authors: for example, according to Christopher Plack, ‘[m]
toneness principle is to use non-harmonic sounds as asking occurs whenever the activity produced on the basilar membrane by
one sound (the masker) obscures the activity produced by the sound you
musical building blocks. For example, in Fluorescences are trying to hear (the signal)’ (2014, p. 91).
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 241

Figure 5. Witold Lutosławski, Symphony No. 2, section 43.

by, for example, a major third in a low register will fall band may often be desirable. This is a characteristic fea-
within the same critical band and interfere with one ture of the type of sound mass identified by some as
another, but two tones separated by the same interval prototypical, the cluster, which harnesses interference to
in a high register will fall in different critical bands and use sounds that may have high toneness in the register of
will not interfere. As Huron explains, this is why chord maximal pitch salience without disintegrating the mass
voicing in tonal music tends to favour wider intervals at into separate auditory streams. For example, the chord of
the bottom and narrower intervals at the top, minimis- arrival following the famous glissando ascent/expansion
ing sensory dissonance and maximising pitch resolution. at the beginning of Xenakis’s Metastaseis (1952) contains
That being said, most musical sounds have many fre- 46 pitches separated mostly by semitones or whole tones.
quency components, and even sounds with widely spaced Electronic resources, such as the UPIC system devel-
fundamental frequencies may have higher harmonics oped by Xenakis, expand the potential for density even
that fall within the same critical bands, creating inter- further, allowing complete saturation of all frequencies
actions that contribute to phenomena such as sensory within a defined range in compositions such as Mycenae
dissonance. Alpha (1978). Masking also enables sound mass fusion
In sound mass, pitch resolution detracts from the aes- in more active textures, as noted by Drott (2011) who
thetic goal of global integration or fusion, and so the refers to a masking effect between overlapping parts in
mutual interference of frequencies in the same critical micropolyphony.
242 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

3.4. Tonal fusion principle The pitch proximity principle refers to successive rather
than simultaneous tones: at most musical tempi, melodic
The perceptual independence of concurrent tones is
weakened when their pitch relations promote tonal continuity is promoted by small intervals and dimin-
fusion. Intervals that promote tonal fusion include (in ished by larger intervals. It is therefore possible to weaken
decreasing order): unisons, octaves, perfect fifths, . . . the continuity (stream integration) of individual musi-
Where the goal is the perceptual independence of con- cal lines by populating them with large leaps. When such
current sounds, intervals ought to be shunned in direct lines are multiply superposed, the weakness or absence
proportion to the degree to which they promote tonal
fusion. (p. 19)
of integration within each line makes it more likely that
the notes will integrate into a composite mass rather than
This is a circumscribed sense of the term ‘fusion’ which segregating into separate streams. An example is found
does not apply to many types of sound mass. How- at rehearsal 10 in Penderecki’s Dimensions of Time and
ever, it is singularly important for the ‘spectral sound- Silence (1959–60), in which the wide leaps and over-
mass’ described by Besharse (2009), which exploits the lapping ranges of the harp, keyboards, and percussion
auditory system’s strong tendency to group harmon- instruments obliterate any sense of melodic continuity
ically related partials together. Spectral soundmasses, in favour of a massed texture (Figure 6). Pitch proximity
sometimes denoted with different names such as spec- can also be a strong factor in situations where most notes
tral sonorities or spectral chords, may not only use assimilate into a mass, but a few ‘outliers’ are marked to
intervals that promote tonal fusion, but may recreate segregate into a separate stream. For example, in section
the complex structure of real or ideal harmonic spec- 5 of Lutosławski’s Symphony No. 2, the three flutes play
tra in order to maximise perceptual fusion. As Lasse at independent tempi in an overlapping middle register
Thoresen observes, ‘[a] spectral chord is ideally fused at a relatively low dynamic level, promoting strong inte-
to the extent it approaches a sound-object with a tim- gration. After this texture has been established, the flutes
bre’ (2015, p. 343). Fusion of this kind, which occupies occasionally interject high B-flats and A-naturals played
the much-discussed liminal realm between timbre and staccato and forte, which split off distinctly into a separate
harmony, can be achieved synthetically with combina- stream.
tions of sine tones and other electronically generated
sound components, or it can be achieved with instrumen-
tal synthesis, the classic example being Grisey’s Partiels 3.6. Pitch co-modulation principle
(1978) in which the low E of the trombone and dou- The perceptual union of concurrent tones is encouraged
ble bass is orchestrated by assigning the frequencies of when pitch motions are positively correlated. Perceptual
their spectra to other instruments. As Robert Hasegawa fusion is most enhanced when the correlation is precise
points out, with respect to log frequency. (p. 21)

[t]he goal of instrumental synthesis is not a precise repro- This principle, along with the tonal fusion principle, pro-
duction of the trombone sound – which would in any vides the basis for that most famous of voice-leading
case be impossible given the complex spectra of acoustic rules: the ban on parallel fifths and octaves. Huron,
instruments – but rather a hybrid sonority permitting us drawing from experiments by Stephen McAdams (1984,
to hear both the individual instruments and their fusion
1989), states that synchronous, similar motion between
into a unified timbre. (2009, pp. 349–50)
parts causes them to group or fuse perceptually; the ten-
Similar effects can also be achieved with human voices: dency is stronger when the motion is parallel, preserving
Stockhausen’s Stimmung (1968), which is based entirely not only contour but exact pitch intervals. Harmonic par-
on a spectral voicing of a B-flat dominant 9th chord, allelism has been used in some music that would not
adopts a proto-spectral aesthetic grounded in tonal normally be associated with sound mass but that never-
fusion. theless exploits similar fusion-based aesthetic goals, such
as some passages of Ravel’s Boléro (1928). It is an audi-
tory adaptation of the Gestalt ‘law of common fate’ which
3.5. Pitch proximity principle Emilios Cambouropoulos and Costas Tsougras describe
The coherence of an auditory stream is maintained by thus: ‘objects that move together tend to be connected
close pitch proximity in successive tones within the to each other and grouped together’ (2009, pp. 123–24).
stream. Pitch-based streaming is assured when pitch Semblant motion between lines is a musical example of
movement is within van Noorden’s ‘fission boundary’
common fate motion, but not the only one: other types
(normally 2 semitones or less for tones less than 700 ms
in duration). When pitch distances are large, it may be of coherent motion can cause parts to group perceptu-
possible to maintain the perception of a single stream by ally as well. For example, the ‘Shepard Tone’ creates an
reducing the tempo. (p. 24) illusion of perpetual descent through its deployment of
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 243

Figure 6. Penderecki, Dimensions of Time and Silence, rehearsals 9–10.

partials or pitches, with new sounds appearing at the notes with simultaneous onsets, short duration, and often
top to replace those that disappear off the bottom in a high dynamic for accentuation. Punctuation blend is
a kind of musical barber shop pole. Although an ide- well-illustrated at rehearsal 34 of Lutosławski’s Dou-
alised synthetic Shepard Tone will preserve frequency ble Concerto for Oboe, Harp, and Chamber Orchestra
ratios exactly, there are many musical examples in which (1980) (Figure 7).
a similar effect is achieved with only approximate corre-
spondence between the trajectories of the parts, as seen
at rehearsal L in Georg Friedrich Haas’s Hyperion (2006). 3.8. Principle of limited density
If a composer intends to write music in which indepen-
dent parts are easily distinguished, then the number of
3.7. Onset synchrony principle concurrent voices or parts ought to be kept to three or
fewer. (p. 46)
If a composer intends to write music in which the parts
have a high degree of perceptual independence, then syn- Huron explains that our ability to follow independent
chronous note onsets ought to be avoided. Onsets of musical lines follows an un, deux, trois, beaucoup pat-
nominally distinct sounds should be separated by 100 ms tern: once the number of concurrent parts exceeds three,
or more. (p. 40)
our ability to follow them (or even to have a sense of
Huron notes that synchronous onsets between parts can how many there are) decreases dramatically. Therefore, if
promote fusion, as sounds that begin together tend to attenuating the perceptual salience of individual lines is
group together. Whereas synchronous onsets in poly- an aesthetic goal, a simple and much-exploited technique
phonic textures can be a detriment to the perceptual is to have a large number of parts active at the same time.
independence of concurrent lines, in sound mass they Quintessential examples are found in Xenakis’s pioneer-
provide an effective way to promote perceptual fusion. ing treatment of each string instrument as an individual
Onset synchrony is especially characteristic of what Moe part in pieces such as Pithoprakta (1955–56); this prac-
Touizrar and McAdams (in press) refer to as ‘punctuation tice was soon adopted by Penderecki and Ligeti. It is
blend,’ which occurs when several instruments articulate important to note that the sheer number of parts does
244 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

Figure 7. Lutosławski, Double Concerto for Oboe, Harp, and Chamber Orchestra, rehearsal 34.

not in itself guarantee sound mass integration. If musi- In this example, despite the large number of parts, the
cal organisation emphasises collective properties such as emergent properties of the music in toto facilitate the kind
metrical synchronisation and tonal harmonic structure, of segmental analytical listening (and, in the language of
then sound mass fusion may be evaded even in music embodied music cognition, ‘mimetic participation’) that
with a large number of parts. Arnie Cox illustrates this sound mass negates.
point with reference to Thomas Tallis’s 40-voice motet
Spem in Alium (1570): 3.9. Timbral differentiation principle
The more complex the auditory scene, the greater the
If a composer intends to write music in which the parts
challenge for mimetic comprehension. For example, in
have a high degree of perceptual independence, then
the case of Tallis’s forty-voice motet ‘Spem in alium,’
each part should maintain a unique timbral character.
when most or all of the voices are singing at once,
(p. 49)
mimetic participation with particular voices becomes
largely impossible. And yet there is still a straightforward As noted above, timbral homogeneity is a decisive factor
mimetic invitation in connection with the composite in sound mass fusion. Composers have intuitively under-
vocal exertions. The meter, rhythms, and tempo emerge
from a recurring general pattern of vocal exertions and
stood this, frequently either writing for timbrally homo-
these combine with a shared strength (acoustic intensity) geneous ensembles (e.g. string orchestras) or deploying
and manner of articulation. At this level, mimetic partic- timbres in homogeneous subsets of mixed ensembles,
ipation in effect is with the composite emergent musical as, for example, Lutosławski does in his Cello Concerto
entity. (2016, pp. 49–50) (1996). When attempting to achieve sound mass fusion
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 245

Figure 8. Xenakis, Terretektorh, seating plan.

with heterogeneous ensembles, dynamics, articulations, string-dominated to a brass-dominated timbral mass in


and other playing techniques serve important roles. For mm. 93–98.
example, sounds played very softly and without vibrato
lose some of the cues that mark their timbral salience
and therefore allow them to blend more easily, as seen 3.10. Source location principle
at rehearsal 40 of Lutosławski’s Mi-Parti (1976). Alterna- If a composer intends to write music in which the parts
tively, the heterogeneity of ensemble timbres can be over- have a high degree of perceptual independence, then it is
whelmed with fortissimo dynamics and noise, as in the helpful to separate in space the various sound sources for
each part. (p. 51)
tutti climax at rehearsal F of Toru Takemitsu’s Asterism
(1969). This principle is less consistently applied than others in
The interaction between timbre and dynamics can both traditional and sound mass music, probably due
also be used to nuanced aesthetic effect, allowing for a to logistical constraints. As Huron notes, while spatial
kind of timbral counterpoint in a heterogeneous massed distribution is occasionally applied in antiphonal forma-
ensemble texture. Ligeti exploits this throughout Lontano tions, performers of polyphonic music are often reluctant
(1967), with timbral layers coming into the foreground to use dispersed locations even though this would help
through crescendi before submerging back into a pianis- preserve the perceptual independence of their parts. For
simo massed ensemble texture: two striking moments of sound mass, the converse of the source location princi-
this kind are the emergent foregrounding of the violins ple would imply that fusion is promoted by minimising
in m. 11 which are then displaced by the flutes and oboes distance between performers, but since so many canon-
a measure later, and the dramatic overturning from a ical sound mass pieces are for large orchestras, there are
246 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

physical limits on how closely performers can be posi- ‘tendency masks’ over defined temporal and frequency
tioned. However, spatial positioning of sound sources ranges (Truax, 1990). These are readily visible in a spec-
occasionally figures as an artistic parameter in sound trograph of the piece, appearing as clearly-defined geo-
mass music, for example to create the illusion of spa- metrical shapes (Figure 9).
tial movement. A famous example is Xenakis’s Terretek-
torh (1969), in which the musicians of the orchestra
3.12. Addendum 2: rhythmic Irregularity
are interspersed among the audience (Figure 8). In this
piece, according to Maria Anna Harley, ‘[t]he proximity Another point not explicitly emphasised by Huron
of the sound creates a new aural experience strength- (2001)9 is metrical regularity, which provides regular
ened by the movement of sound masses in space’ (1994, accent patterns to which listeners can entrain, and a sim-
p. 300). plifying framework for more complex surface rhythms.
Since clear metre promotes segmental listening – break-
ing up the music’s temporal continuity into strong and
3.11. Addendum 1: event duration
weak beats, bars, and so forth – its aesthetic function may
Several other factors not directly addressed in the above run contrary to the fusion-based aesthetic of sound mass.
list deserve mention here. An additional important Many sound mass composers have avoided it accordingly,
aspect of sound mass, which frequently sets it in con- for example by avoiding synchronised metres or pulses
tradistinction to conventional polyphonic practice, is between parts, even if there may be some implication
duration of its constituent sound events. Huron remarks of pulse or metre within parts individually. Superpos-
that the vast majority of notes in Western tonal music ing multiple layers with different metres and/or inde-
are between 150 ms and 1 s in duration. Sound mass, pendent tempi will tend to override cues for metrical
on the other hand, frequently employs vastly elongated entrainment provided by any given part, resulting in what
durations: the opening chord of Ligeti’s Atmosphères, Jadwiga Paja has described as ‘diffusional aggregations’
at the notated tempo of 40 bpm, lasts 24 s before the and ‘polymorphic polyphony’ that create an ‘intermedi-
first change in orchestration, and over three times that ate dimension’ between the horizontal and the vertical
long before the first chord change. At the opposite end (1990, pp. 186–89). This type of organisation is particu-
of the temporal spectrum, sound mass can be created larly characteristic of Lutosławski’s ‘repeat cell’ sections,
from very large numbers of very short sound events. quasi-indeterminate constructions that typically assign
The shortest event that we can produce or perceive as each musician a cell of musical material which they may
a rhythmic element is about 100 ms, corresponding to repeat freely at their own individual tempi, without coor-
a maximum rhythmic frequency of about 10 events per dinating with the rest of the ensemble. The contents of
second (Hz) (London, 2004, p. 27). At frequencies above Lutosławski’s repeat cells usually feature rhythmic group-
roughly 20 Hz, we perceive pitch. This leaves a region ings of different lengths and accent patterns between the
that Edward Large (2015) has called a ‘no-man’s-land’ players, as is the case at the beginning of Jeux vénitiens
between 10 and 20 Hz: too slow to be perceived as pitch (1960–61).
and too fast to be perceived as rhythm. Auditory expe-
rience in this range can be ambiguous, characterised by
3.13. Addendum 3: source bonding
a rough kind of pseudo-continuity in which events are
neither clearly integrated nor clearly segregated. Perhaps Another aspect of auditory perception not addressed
because of perceptual ambiguity and difficulty of produc- by Huron also deserves mention: source bonding, Smal-
tion, this liminal region traditionally played little role in ley’s term for recognition of a sound’s source cause. It
music (excepting ornamental figures like trills and drum- is relevant here because electronic technology makes it
rolls). However, it has been of great interest to sound possible to introduce recorded samples of ‘real-world’
mass composers: for instance, Ligeti exploited the liminal sound masses, such as the sounds of herds of animals,
region between rhythm and pitch at length in his harpsi- crowds of people, wind, water, and various other envi-
chord piece Continuum (1970), which consists of a steady ronmental sources into a musical context. This has been
stream of notes to be played as fast as possible. an important feature of electroacoustic compositional
In the electronic technique of granular synthesis, there practices such as musique concrète and soundscape com-
are often no ‘parts’ as such but only brief, discon- position. For example, in Trevor Wishart’s Vox 5 (1986), a
tinuous events, distributed stochastically. For example, human voice phonating on a voiced alveolar fricative /z/
Barry Truax’s Riverrun (1987) is produced entirely with
very brief sine tones or frequency-modulated sine tones 9 Huron addresses this issue extensively elsewhere, however. See, for example,
(sometimes as short as 1 ms) distributed with geometrical Sweet Anticipation (2006, pp. 175–202).
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 247

Figure 9. Spectrograph of Barry Truax’s Riverrun.

transforms into the sound of a swarm of bees and then resist segregation, the perceptual principles identified
back into a human voice. by Huron as grounding polyphonic voice leading were
reviewed, followed by several addenda. It was demon-
strated that in most cases sound mass exploits the same
4. Conclusion principles in reverse, emphasising the musical totality by
This essay has distinguished two expansions of post- exceeding or subverting the perceptual independence of
tonal music: an expanded range of combinations of its concurrent parts.
tones in ‘post-tonality,’ and an expanded palette of sonic It would be no overstatement to say that post-tone
resources in ‘post-tone’ music. It has focused on the thinking has revolutionised music, overturning our most
latter, which, in displacing the tone as the de facto fun- basic assumptions about what music is, how it is created,
damental unit of music, has enabled an emancipated, how it behaves. The implications of this revolution are
primary role for musical parameters traditionally consid- clearer in the early years of the twenty-first century than
ered to be secondary, especially timbre. A turning point ever before, as contemporary and popular musics alike
in the development of post-tone thinking occurred when focus ever-more on timbre, with vastly expanded elec-
some composers in the 1950s and 1960s stopped think- tronic and acoustic resources granting musicians every-
ing in terms of combinatorics of tones and turned instead where access to timbral combinations unimaginable in
to global perceptual properties of masses of tones, or even the recent past. We also have access to a broader
sound masses. Although much has been written about and richer understanding of music perception and cog-
poietic technical methods and artistic goals of sound nition than ever before, with an ever-expanding base of
mass composers, esthesic questions of sound mass per- experimental methods and empirical findings. This essay
ception have largely been relegated to introspection and has attempted to lay an empirically informed theoret-
intuition on the part of authors rather than empiri- ical foundation for understanding one important type
cally grounded study. To address this state of affairs, this of timbre-based music, in the hopes that future study
essay reviewed a corpus of extant definitions of sound will explore the suggestions made here through per-
mass, reported some empirical findings that challenge the ceptual experiments and musical analysis. Such stud-
emphases of that corpus, and offered a new definition ies could perhaps answer the pejorative or oversimpli-
that departs from them in emphasising homogeneity, dif- fying connotations that have dogged the term ‘sound
ferences between acoustical and perceptual density and mass’ in the past by highlighting the rich diversity of
complexity, and resistance to segregation in one or more musical gestalts that it can encompass, predicated on
musical parameters. To further explicate how masses percepts of density, simplicity, and homogeneity and
248 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

manifested in an enormously vast array of compositional Cox, A. (2016). Music & embodied cognition: Listening, moving,
formations. Analyses could approach post-tone textures feeling & thinking. Indiana University Press.
through this lens, comparing them systematically on the Dhomont, F. (1996). Points de Fuite. On Cycle de L’Errance
[CD]. Empreintes DIGITALes.
basis of perceptual principles and showing how juxtapo- Drott, E. (2011). Lines, masses, micropolyphony: Ligeti’s Kyrie
sitions and transformations of such textures may be used and the ‘crisis of the figure’. Perspectives of New Music, 49(1),
in the deployment of musical form. Future work (cur- 4–46. https://doi.org/10.7757/persnewmusi.49.1.0004
rently underway) will also explore perceptual and cogni- Edwards, J. M. (2001). North America since 1920. In K.
tive differences among sound mass types, demonstrating Pendle (Ed.), Women & music: A history (pp. 314–386).
Indiana University Press.
how the exploitation of the different perceptual princi-
Erickson, R. (1975). Sound structure in music. University of
ples enumerated here may affect experiences of emotion, California Press.
meaning, and so forth. The ultimate hope is that such Foy, R. (1994). Textural transformations: The instrumental
studies will provide clearer insight to how sound mass music of Krzysztof Penderecki, 1960–1973 [Unpublished doc-
and other post-tone musics are perceived by audiences, toral dissertation]. Johns Hopkins University.
which would be invaluable to composers and perform- Fulford, W. D. (1979). An analysis of sound masses in ‘hyper-
prism,’ ‘octandre,’ and ‘intégrales,’ three chamber works by
ers seeking to engage audiences in the media-saturated Edgard Varèse [Unpublished master’s thesis]. California
landscape of the twenty-first century. State University.
Gorecki, H. (1992). Symphony No. 3. Boosey & Hawkes. (Com-
posed 1976).
Acknowledgements Górecki, H. (1999). Symphony no. 3 [Recorded by J. Kozlowska
The authors wish to thank the two anonymous reviewers for and Warschauer Philharmonie, conducted by K. Kord] [CD].
valuable comments on the manuscript, as well as Bennett K. Philips Classics.
Smith for programming the listening experiments, Channey Granat, Z. (2009). Rediscovering ‘sonoristics’: A ground-
Phung for running subjects, and the members of the Music Per- breaking theory from the margins of musicology. In Z.
ception and Cognition Lab at McGill University for valuable Blažeković & B. D. Mackenzie (Eds.), Music’s intellectual his-
feedback. tory (pp. 821–833). Répertoire International de Littérature
Musicale.
Griffiths, P. (2001). Sprechgesang [Encyclopedia entry]. Grove
Funding Music Online. https://doi.org/10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.
article.26465
This work was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Grisey, G. (1978). Partiels. Milano. (Composed 1976).
Research Council of Canada (RGPIN 312774) and the Fonds Grisey, G. (1998). La musique: le devenir des sons. In D.
de recherche du Québec—Société et culture (2017-SE-205667), Cohen-Levinas (Ed.), Vingt-cinq ans de création musicale
as well as a Canada Research Chair (950-223484) awarded to contemporaine: L’Itinéraire en temps reel (pp. 291–300).
Stephen McAdams and a Vanier Canada Graduate Scholarship L’Itinéraire/L’Harmattan. (Original work published in 1982).
(770-2012-0129) awarded to Jason Noble. Grisey, G. (1999). Partiels [Recorded by Ensemble Court-
circuit]. On Les Espaces Acoustiques [CD]. Accord.
Haas, G. F. (2006). Hyperion. Universal Edition.
References Haas, G. F. (2007). Hyperion [Recorded by SWR Sinfonieorch-
ester Baden-Baden und Freiburg, conducted by H. Zender].
Bedrossian, F. (2008). Tracés d’Ombres. Billaudot.
On Donaueschinger Musiktage 2006, Vol. 2 [CD]. Neos.
Bedrossian, F. (2013). Tracés d’Ombres [Recorded by Ensemble
Hanslick, E. (1986). On the musically beautiful (G. Payzant,
2e2 m]. On Manifesto [CD]. Aeon.
Trans.). Hackett Publishing Company. (Original work pub-
Benward, B., & Saker, M. (2015). Music in theory and practice,
lished 1854).
vol. II (9th ed.). McGraw Hill.
Harley, J. (1995). Generative processes in algorithmic com-
Bernard, J. (1999). Ligeti’s restoration of interval and its sig-
position: Chaos and music. Leonardo, 28(3), 221–224.
nificance for his later works. Music Theory Spectrum, 21(1),
https://doi.org/10.2307/
1–31. https://doi.org/10.2307/745918
1576078
Besharse, K. (2009). The role of texture in French spectral music
Harley, M. A. (1994). Spatial sound movement in the instru-
[Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Illinois at
mental music of Iannis Xenakis. Journal of New Music
Urbana-Champaign.
Research, 23(3), 291–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/0929821
Cage, J. (1991). The future of music: Credo. In R. Kostelanetz
9408570661
(Ed.), John Cage, an anthology (pp. 54–57). Da Capo Press.
Harley, M. A. (1998). Music of sound and light: Xenakis’s
(Original work published 1958).
Polytopes. Leonardo, 31(1), 55–65. https://doi.org/10.2307/
Cambouropoulos, E., & Tsougras, C. (2009). Auditory streams
1576549
in Ligeti’s continuum: A theoretical and perceptual approach.
Harvey, J. (1990). Mortuos Plango, Vivos Voco. On Denis Smal-
Journal of Interdisciplinary Music Studies, 3(1–2), 119–137.
ley, Mesias Maiguashca, Gareth Loy, Kaija Saariaho, Jonathan
http://users.auth.gr/emilios/papers/jims2009.pdf
Harvey [CD]. Wergo.
Clendinning, J. P. (1993). The pattern-meccanico compositions
Harvey, J. (2000). Spectralism. Contemporary Music Review,
of György Ligeti. Perspectives of New Music, 31(1), 192–234.
19(3), 11–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/07494460000640331
https://doi.org/10.2307/833050
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 249

Hasegawa, R. (2009). Gérard Grisey and the ‘nature’of har- London, J. (2004). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of
mony. Music Analysis, 28(2–3), 349–371. https://doi.org/ musical meter. Oxford University Press.
10.1111/j.1468-2249.2011.00294.x Lutosławski, W. (1958). Musique funèbre. Polskie Wydawn.
Hoogewind, M. J. (2000). Compositional techniques of sound Muzyczne.
mass in selected works of György Ligeti [Unpublished master’s Lutosławski, W. (1963). Trois Poèmes d’Henri Michaux. Polskie
thesis]. California State University. Wydawn. Muzyczne.
Hurel, P. (1991). Six Miniatures en Trompe-l’Œil. Gérard Billau- Lutosławski, W. (1973). Symphony No. 2. Polskie Wydawn.
dot Éditeur. Muzyczne. (Composed 1965-1967).
Hurel, P. (1995). Six Miniatures en Trompe-l’Œil [Recorded by Lutosławski, W. (1976). Mi-Parti. Chester Music.
Ensemble Intercontemporain, conducted by E. Spanjaard]. Lutosławski, W. (1978). Jeux vénitiens. Moeck Verlag. (Com-
On Six Miniatures en Trompe-l’Œil; Leçon de choses; Opicit; posed 1961).
Pour l’Image [CD]. IRCAM. Lutosławski, W. (1981). Double Concerto for Oboe, Harp and
Huron, D. (2001). Tone and voice: A derivation of the rules of Chamber Orchestra. Chester Music. (Composed 1979-80).
voice-leading from perceptual principles. Music Perception, Lutosławski, W. (1987). Double Concerto for Oboe, Harp
19(1), 1–64. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2001.19.1.1 and Chamber Orchestra [Recorded by H. Holliger, U.
Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology Holliger, and Symphonie-Orchester des Bayerischen Rund-
of expectation. The MIT Press. funks, conducted by W. Lutosławski]. On Lutosławski con-
Iverson, J. J. (2009). Historical memory and György Ligeti’s ducts Lutosławski [CD]. Philips.
sound-mass music 1958–1968 [Unpublished doctoral disser- Lutosławski, W. (1994a). Musique Funèbre [Recorded by Pol-
tation]. The University of Texas at Austin. ish Radio National Symphony Orchestra, conducted by W.
Klein, L. (1967). Twentieth-century analysis: Essays in minia- Lutosławski]. On Symphonic Variations; Symphony no. 1;
ture. Music Educators Journal, 53(8), 123–125. https://doi.org/ Musique Funèbre; Symphony No. 2 [CD]. EMI Classics.
10.1177/002743216705300834 Lutosławski, W. (1994b). Symphony No. 2 [Recorded by Pol-
Kostka, S. (2012). Materials and techniques of post-tonal music ish Radio National Symphony Orchestra, conducted by W.
(4th ed). Prentice Hall. Lutosławski]. On Symphonic Variations; Symphony no. 1;
Landy, L. (2007). Understanding the art of sound organization. Musique Funèbre; Symphony No. 2 [CD]. EMI Classics.
The MIT Press. Lutosławski, W. (1996). Trois Poèmes d’Henri Michaux [Recor-
Large, E. (2015, August 1–5). President’s address presen- ded by Polish Radio National Symphony Orchestra, con-
tation. Conference of the Society for Music Percep- ducted by W. Lutosławski]. On Preludes & Fugue; Trois
tion and Cognition, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN. Poèmes d’Henri Michaux; Paroles Tissées; Postlude no. 1 [CD].
http://www.musicperception.org/pastprograms/SMPC- EMI Classics.
2015.pdf Lutosławski, W. (1996). Cello Concerto. Chester Music. (Com-
Lefresne, B. (2005). Applications of Chaos theory and fractal posed 1970).
geometry in the music of György Ligeti [Unpublished master’s Lutosławski, W. (2000a). Jeux vénitiens [Recorded by Pol-
thesis]. University of Ottawa. ish Radio National Symphony Orchestra, conducted by W.
Ligeti, G. (1965). Requiem. H. Litolff’s Verlag. (Composed Lutosławski]. On Symphony nos. 1 & 2; Symphonic Varia-
1963-65). tions; Musique Funèbre; Concerto for Orchestra; Jeux Véni-
Ligeti, G. (1969). Lontano. Schott Music Corp. (Composed tiens; Livre: Pour Orchestre; Mi-Parti [CD]. EMI
1967). Classics.
Ligeti, G. (1970). Continuum. B. Schott’s Söhne. (Composed Lutosławski, W. (2000b). Mi-Parti [Recorded by Polish Radio
1968). National Symphony Orchestra, conducted by W. Lutosławski].
Ligeti, G. (1973). Volumina. H. Litolff’s Verlag/C.F. Peters. On Symphony nos. 1 & 2; Symphonic Variations; Musique
(Composed 1962-1963/1967). Funèbre; Concerto for Orchestra; Jeux Vénitiens; Livre: Pour
Ligeti, G. (1974). Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe and Orches- Orchestre; Mi-Parti [CD]. EMI Classics.
tra. B. Schott’s Söhne. McAdams, S. (1984). Spectral fusion, spectral parsing, and the
Ligeti, G. (1980). Atmosphères. Universal Edition. (Composed formation of auditory images [Doctoral dissertation, Stanford
1961). University]. http://www.music.mcgill.ca/ smc/McAdams_
Ligeti, G. (1985). Requiem [Recorded by L. Poli, B. Ericson, 1984_PhDThesis.pdf
Chor des Bayerischen Rundfunks, and Sinfonie-Orchester McAdams, S. (1989). Segregation of concurrent sounds. I:
des Hessischen Rundfunks Frankfurt, conducted by M. Gie- Effects of frequency modulation coherence. The Journal
len]. On Requiem für Sopran, Mezzosopran, zwei gemischte of the Acoustical Society of America, 86(6), 2148–2159.
Chöre und Orchester; Aventures; Nouvelles aventures: für 3 https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398475
Sänger und Instrumentalisten [CD]. Wergo. Mirka, D. (1997). The sonoristic structuralism of Krzysztof Pen-
Ligeti, G. (1987). Double Concerto for Flute, Oboe and Orches- derecki. Music Academy in Katowice.
tra [Recorded by G. von Bahr, T.Lännerholm, and Swedish Nattiez, J. J. (1990). Music and discourse: Toward a semiol-
Radio Symphony Orchestra, conducted by E. Howarth]. ogy of music (C. Abbate, Trans.). Princeton University Press.
[CD]. BIS. (Original work published 1987).
Ligeti, G. (1994). Atmosphères [Recorded by Berliner Philhar- Noble, J. (2018). Perceptual and semantic dimensions of
moniker, conducted by J. Nott]. On The Ligeti Project II sound mass [Doctoral dissertation, McGill University].
[CD]. EMI Classics. https://mcgill.ca/mpcl/files/mpcl/noble_2018_phdthesis.pdf
Ligeti, G. (2006). Volumina [Recorded by G. Zacher]. On Clear Normandeau, R. (2001). Clair de Terre [CD]. Empreintes DIG-
or Cloudy [CD]. Deutsche Grammophon. ITALes.
250 J. NOBLE AND S. MCADAMS

Paja, J. (1990). The polyphonic aspect of Lutosławski’s music. Smoot, R. J. (1986). The synthesis and manipulation of fused
Acta Musicologica, 62(2/3), 183–191. https://doi.org/10.2307/ ensemble timbres and sound masses by means of digital signal
932632 processing [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. The Ohio
Penderecki, K. (1961a). Dimensions of time and Silence. Her- State University.
mann Moeck Verlag. (Composed 1959-61). Stephenson, T. (1988). The musical language of Edgard Varese
Penderecki, K. (1961b). Threnody for the Victims of Hiroshima. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Surrey.
Ernst Eulenburg Inc. Composed 1960. Stockhausen, K. (1969). Stimmung. Universal Edition. (Com-
Penderecki, K. (1962). Fluorescences. Hermann Moeck Verlag. posed 1968).
(Composed 1961-62). Stockhausen, K. (2007). Stimmung [Recorded by Theatre
Penderecki, K. (1963). Polymorphia. Hermann Moeck Verlag. of Voices, conducted by P. Hillier] [CD]. Harmonia
(Composed 1961). Mundi.
Penderecki, K. (2001). Threnody for the Victims of Hiroshima Stojanović, D. (2009). Work of Edgard Varèse and ‘futurist
[Recorded by Polish Radio National Symphony Orchestra, music’: Affinities (and differences). New Sound: International
conducted by K. Penderecki. On Orchestral Works [CD]. Magazine for Music, 34(1), 50–61. https://www.researchgate.
EMI Classics. net/publication/236159652_Dragana_Stojanovic-Novicic_
Penderecki, K. (2012). Polymorphia [Recorded by Aukso Work_of_Edgard_Varese_and_’Futurist_Music’_Affinities_
Orchestra, conducted by K. Penderecki]. On Threnody for and_Differences_New_Sound_International_Magazine_
the Victims of Hiroshima; Popcorn Superhet Receiver; Poly- for_Music_34_no_1_2009_50-61
morphia; 48 Responses to Polymorphia [CD]. Strawn, J. (1978). The intégrales of Edgard Varèse space,
Nonesuch. mass, element, and form. Perspectives of New Music, 17(1),
Plack, C. (2014). The sense of hearing (2nd ed). Taylor & Fran- 138–160. https://doi.org/10.2307/832662
cis. Svard, L. (1991). Illusion in selected keyboard works of György
Ravel, M. (1929). Bolero. Editions Durand. (Composed 1928). Ligeti [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Johns Hopkins
Reiprich, B. (1978). Transformation of coloration and density University.
in György Ligeti’s Lontano. Perspectives of New Music, 16(2), Takemitsu, T. (1969). Asterism. C.S. Peters.
167–180. https://doi.org/10.2307/832681 Takemitsu, T. (2015). Asterism [Recorded by Toronto Sym-
Reynolds, R. (2013). The last word is: Imagination: A study phony Orchestra, conducted by S. Ozawa]. On Masterworks
of the spatial aspects of varèse’s work (Part I: Writ- of the 20th Century [CD]. Sony Music.
ten evidence). Perspectives of New Music, 51(1), 196–255. Tedman, K. (1983). Edgard Varèse: Concepts of organized sound
https://doi.org/10.7757/persnewmusi.51.1.0196 [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Sussex.
Risset, J. C. (1987). Sud (B). On Sud; Dialogues; Inharmonique; Thoresen, L. (2015). Emergent musical forms: Aural explo-
Mutations [CD]. INA. GRM. rations. University of Western Ontario.
Roberts, G. (1978). Procedures for analysis of sound masses Tormis, V. (1996). Jaanilalud. Werner Chappell Music.
[Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Indiana University. Tormis, V. (2010). Jaanilalud [Recorded by Estonian Philhar-
Roig-Francoli, M. (2008). Understanding post-tonal music. monic Chamber Choir, conducted by P. Hillier]. On Baltic
McGraw-Hill. Runes [CD]. Harmonia Mundi.
Roy, S. (1996a). Crystal Music. On Kaleidos [CD]. Empreintes Touizrar, M., & McAdams, S. (In press). Perceptual facets of
DIGITALes. orchestration in The Angel of Death by Roger Reynolds:
Roy, S. (1996b). Mimetismo. On Kaleidos [CD]. Empreintes Timbre and auditory grouping. In P. Lalitte (Ed.), Analyse
DIGITALes. musicale et perception: Actes des Journées d’Analyse Musicale
Rushton, J. (2001). Klangfarbenmelodie [Encyclopedia entry]. 2016. Editions de l’Université de Bourgogne.
Grove Music Online. https://doi.org/10.1093/gmo/ Truax, B. (1987). Riverrun. On Digital Soundscapes [CD]. Cam-
9781561592630.article.15094 bridge Street Records. (Composed 1986).
Russolo, L. (1986). The Art of Noises (B. Brown, Trans.). Mono- Truax, B. (1990). Composing with real-time granular sound.
graphs in Musicology, No. 6. New York: Pendragon Press. Perspectives of New Music, 28(2), 120–134. https://doi.org/
(Original work published 1913). 10.2307/833014
Saariaho, K. (1990). Du cristal. Edition Wilhelm Hansen AS. Truax, B. (1991a). Pacific. On Pacific Rim [CD]. Cambridge
Saariaho, K. (2012). Du Cristal [Recorded by Los Angeles Phil- Street Records. (Composed 1990).
harmonic Orchestra, conducted by E. P. Salonen]. On Kaija Truax, B. (1991b). The Wings of Nike. On Pacific Rim [CD].
Saariaho: Works for Orchestra [CD]. Ondine. Cambridge Street Records. (Composed 1987).
Schaeffer, P. (2017). Treatise on musical objects: An essay across Van der Slice, J. D. (1980). An analysis of György Ligeti’s atmo-
disciplines (C. North & J. Dack, Trans.). University of Cali- sphères [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of
fornia Press. (Original work published 1966). Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Schoenberg, A. (1983). Theory of harmony (R. Carter, Trans.). Varèse, E., & Wen-chung, C. (1966). The liberation of sound.
University of California Press. (Original work published Perspectives of New Music, 5(1), 11–19. https://doi.org/
1911). 10.2307/832385
Smalley, D. (1993). Defining transformations. Interface, 22(4), Wen-Chung, C. (1966). Open rather than bounded. Perspec-
279–300. https://doi.org/10.1080/09298219308570638 tives of New Music, 5(1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.2307/832383
Smalley, D. (2011). Klang, Morphologien, Spektren: Spektro- Wishart, T. (1990). Vox 5. On Electric Phoenix – Vox [CD].
morphologie in der Instrumentalimusik (original English Virgin Classics. (Composed 1986).
version, provided by the author). In L. Haselböck (Ed.), Wishart, T. (1996). On sonic art. Harwood Academic
Klangperspektiven (pp. 45–72). Wolke Verlag Hofheim. Publishers.
JOURNAL OF NEW MUSIC RESEARCH 251

Wood, D. E. (1974). A paradigm for the study and perfor- Xenakis, I. (1987a). Metastasis [Recorded by Orchestre National
mance of intégrales and hyperprism, two instrumental works de l’ORTF, conducted by M. Le Roux]. On Eonta; Metastasis;
by Edgard Varèse [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Ball Pithoprakta [CD]. Le Chant du Monde.
State University. Xenakis, I. (1987b). Pithoprakta [Recorded by Orchestre
Xenakis, I. (1967a). Metastaseis. Boosey & Hawkes. (Composed National de l’ORTF, conducted by M. Le Roux]. On Eonta;
1953-1954). Metastasis; Pithoprakta [CD]. Le Chant du
Xenakis, I. (1967b). Pithoprakta. Boosey & Hawkes. (Com- Monde.
posed 1955-1956). Xenakis, I. (1987). Mycenae Alpha 1978. Perspectives of
Xenakis, I. (1969). Terretektorh. Editions Salabert. Composed New Music, 25(1–2), 12–15. https://www.jstor.org/stable/
1965-66. 833090
Xenakis, I. (1971). Formalized music: Thought and mathematics Xenakis, I. (2001). Mycenae Alpha. On CCMIX [CD].
in composition. Indiana University Press. Mode.

S-ar putea să vă placă și