Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

Children’s heart rate variability as stress indicator: Association with


reported stress and cortisol
Nathalie Michels a,∗ , Isabelle Sioen a,b , Els Clays a , Marc De Buyzere c , Wolfgang Ahrens d ,
Inge Huybrechts a,e , Barbara Vanaelst a,b , Stefaan De Henauw a,f
a
Department of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Ghent University, De Pintelaan 185, 2 Blok A, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium
b
Research Foundation – Flanders, Egmontstraat 5, B-1000 Brussels, Belgium
c
Department of Cardiology, University Hospital Ghent, De Pintelaan 185, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium
d
Bremen Institute for Prevention Research and Social Medicine (BIPS), University of Bremen, Achterstrasse 30, D-28359 Bremen, Germany
e
Dietary Exposure Assessment Group, International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC/WHO), 150 Cours Albert Thomas, 69372 Lyon CEDEX 08, France
f
Department of Health Sciences, Vesalius, Hogeschool Gent, Keramiekstraat 80, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Stress is a complex phenomenon coordinated by two main neural systems: the
Received 14 November 2012 hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal system with cortisol as classical stress biomarker and the auto-
Accepted 25 August 2013 nomic nervous system with heart rate variability (HRV) as recently suggested stress marker. To test
Available online 2 September 2013
low HRV (5 minute measurements) as stress indicator in young children (5-10y), associations with
self-reported chronic stress aspects (events, emotions and problems) (N = 334) and salivary cortisol
Keywords:
(N = 293) were performed. Peer problems, anger, anxiety and sadness were associated with lower root
Biomarker
mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) and high frequency power (i.e. lower parasympathetic
Children
Heart rate variability
activity). Anxiety and anger were also related to a higher low frequency to high frequency ratio. Using
Questionnaire multilevel modelling, higher cortisol levels, a larger cortisol awakening response and steeper diurnal
Salivary cortisol decline were also associated with these HRV patterns of lower parasympathetic activity. Conclusion:
Stress Low HRV (lower parasympathetic activity) might serve as stress indicator in children.
© 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction person is unable to handle the persistent situation (chronic and


uncontrollable stress), the sustained, chronic arousal can trigger
Chronic stress is linked to psychological and physiological health long-term physiological, emotional, and behavioural disturbances
complaints through behavioural responses and changes in the (Koolhaas et al., 2011; Ursin & Eriksen, 2004).
neuroendocrine system (Cohen, Janicki-Deverts, & Miller, 2007). Next to subjective reports (by questionnaires), also biomarkers
Consequently, an adequate measurement of chronic stress is nec- can be used to measure stress. Two major neuroendocrine systems
essary. Stress is an adaptive, dynamic state that is composed of have been shown to adapt the organism to stress situations: the
several aspects. The initiating stimulus is the ‘stressor’. This is hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis and the autonomic nervous
the environmental demand, challenge or event. When being con- system (Charmandari, Tsigos, & Chrousos, 2005). The first system
fronted with these stressors, people evaluate whether this is a starts in the hypothalamus by secretion of the corticotropin-
potential threat. This is the stress appraisal or perceiving phase. releasing hormone that stimulates the pituitary which eventually
When homeostasis is threatened i.e. when there is a discrepancy stimulates the adrenal cortex to secrete cortisol as hormonal
between what is expected or the ‘normal’ situation and what end product. The autonomic nervous system starts in the spinal-
is happening in reality, a physiological and psychological cop- cord/brainstem, results in secretion of adrenaline/noradrenaline as
ing response will be initiated that induces arousal. Only if the main hormonal end products and can be divided in the sympa-
thetic system that prepares the body for a fight or flight and the
parasympathetic system that brings the body back from an emer-
Abbreviations: CAR, cortisol awakening response; ChiBS, children’s body compo- gency status to a resting status. In times of stress, this autonomic
sition and stress; HF, high frequency; HRV, heart rate variability; LF, low frequency; system will give priority to cardiovascular tone and high blood
nu, normalized units; PA, parasympathetic activity; pNN50, percentage of consec- pressure, respiration and release of energy substrates, while it will
utive normal RR intervals differing more than 50 ms; RMSSD, root mean square of
temporarily suppress digestion, growth, reproduction and immu-
successive differences; SA, sympathetic activity.
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +32 9 332 36 85; fax: +32 9 332 49 94. nity. These two major pathways can be reflected by the biomarker
E-mail address: nathalie.michels@ugent.be (N. Michels). cortisol and heart rate variability (HRV), respectively.

0301-0511/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.08.005
434 N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440

Cortisol is the most commonly used biomarker for stress. Sali- (overall cortisol levels, the CAR and the diurnal decline). As stud-
vary cortisol sampling is stress-free and allows multiple sampling ies on the basal cortisol–HRV relation are almost non-existing, this
throughout the day in a natural environment. The secretion has paper will broaden the knowledge on the equilibrium between
a circadian rhythm with lowest levels around midnight and peak these two homeostasis systems.
production in the early morning. Apart from this circadian rhythm,
a cortisol awakening response (CAR) is elicited by a quick cor- 2. Methods
tisol increase within 30 min after wake up (Fries, Dettenborn, &
Kirschbaum, 2009). In general, the CAR reflects the anticipation of 2.1. Participants and general procedures
the upcoming day by activation of memory representation and by
orientation in time and space. Next to the single cortisol values, Participating children were recruited from the Belgian ChiBS
these cortisol patterns (CAR and the diurnal slope) may serve as study (Children’s Body composition and Stress) that examines the
an index of adrenocortical activity on longer term. The reported association between stress and body composition evolution over 2
associations between stress-exposure and cortisol levels are quite years (2010–2012). Detailed aims, methods and population char-
complex. A hyper-/hypo-cortisolism hypothesis suggested that acteristics were described elsewhere (Michels, Vanaelst, et al.,
recent exposure to a stressor may initially elevate cortisol levels 2012). Children were selected by random cluster sampling (primary
(hypercortisolism with high morning cortisol and steep diurnal school children in the selected city of Aalter, Belgium). Data was
slope), while the axis may develop a counter-regulatory response collected from February till June 2010 (the ChiBS baseline survey)
of cortisol lowering after extended stress exposure (Heim, Ehlert, & when the children were between 5 and 10 years old. The study was
Hellhammer, 2000). In a previous analysis, evidence for hypercor- conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and the project
tisolism was found in our young population since negative events protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Ghent Uni-
and emotional problems were related to a steeper diurnal slope and versity Hospital. Written informed consent was obtained from the
low happiness with a higher overall, morning and evening cortisol, parents.
although peer problems were associated with lower overall cortisol The different measurement modules (i.e. salivary cortisol, HRV,
levels in girls (Michels, Clays, et al., 2012). questionnaires) were optional and were performed under different
Another promising and increasingly used stress marker is HRV. conditions: parents had to make an appointment at the local sports
High HRV is defined as high variability of the distance between park for the HRV measurement and questionnaire administration,
consecutive R peaks of the heart beat signal. This vital variabil- while salivary cortisol was sampled at home. In total, 523 children
ity reflects the heart’s ability to respond to physiological and participated in the ChiBS survey. To enable exclusion of unhealthy
environmental stimuli. Because of the specific autonomic nervous subjects, parents had to fill in a medical questionnaire. Concern-
innervations of the heart, HRV is recognized as a quantitative ing HRV related diseases, one child with a cardiovascular disease
marker of this autonomic nervous system: it is influenced by sym- was excluded and no cases with diabetes were reported. Concern-
pathetic activity (SA) and vagal parasympathetic activity (PA) (Task ing cortisol, no Cushing or Addison patients were found in our
Force of, ESC/NASPE, 1996). These SA and PA innervations on the population. No clinical psychopathologies were reported. Because
sinus node of the heart control the firing of electrical impulses of high quality control for HRV and cortisol data (see respective
that stimulate heart contraction. Importantly, the PA regularly sections) and the different modules being optional, 432 children
sends inhibitory signals e.g. at expiration, with a temporary reduc- had complete HRV data, 310 had complete cortisol data and 484
tion of the heart rate as a result. When this PA innervations are had stress questionnaire data. Consequently, the analyses for HRV
pathologically attenuated, the sinus node will fire at its intrin- versus reported stress (questionnaire data) were performed in 334
sic rate, resulting in a lower variability of the heart rate i.e. low children and for HRV versus salivary cortisol in 293 children. No
HRV. As stress influences this autonomic system, HRV can be used difference in sex, parental education and physical activity was
as an index of chronic stress, e.g. work related stress (Chandola, observed between children included and not included in these two
Heraclides, & Kumari, 2010). Low PA has previously been linked sets of analyses, but those included were somewhat older.
with poor emotion regulation, high stress, decreased stress reactiv- Physical activity was studied as the parental reported hours of
ity and increased stress vulnerability, also in children (Porges, 1995; physical activity per week (both at sports club and outdoors) and
Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, & Maiti, 1994). Moreover, a reduction was used as confounder of HRV. Parental education was used as
of HRV (i.e. reduced PA due to changes in the innervations with proxy variable for the socio-economic status using the Interna-
or without increased SA) is a pathway linked to higher morbidity tional Standard Classification of Education. Further categorization
and mortality (Thayer, Yamamoto, & Brosschot, 2010) and conse- was done into 2 groups: low (=up to secondary education) and high
quently, HRV may be a potential pathway linking stress to ill health (=tertiary education) status.
(Thayer & Brosschot, 2005).
The hypothesis of the present study is that 5-minute HRV 2.2. Heart rate variability
parameters (especially low PA) could serve as chronic stress indi-
cator. Since developmental changes have been shown in HRV with Inter-beat RR-intervals were recorded at a sampling rate of
age-related wave-like increases in PA (Galeev, Igisheva, & Kazin, 1000 Hz with the elastic electrode belt Polar Wearlink 31 using
2002), it is interesting to test this hypothesis also in children. After a Windlink infrared computer transmitter. This low-cost device
all, research on HRV as stress marker is very scarce in this popu- has been validated against an electrocardiogram device in children
lation, although the prevalence of stress/mental health problems (Gamelin, Baquet, Berthoin, & Bosquet, 2008). Each child was indi-
is already high in childhood (Kieling et al., 2011) and HRV has vidually examined in a quiet room in the supine position during
been shown to be a risk factor for disease also in children (Zhou, 10 min. Children were asked to refrain from strenuous physical
Xie, Wang, & Yang, 2012). Children’s HRV will be tested as chronic activity on the measurement day. The heart rate belt was fixed
stress indicator. Low HRV (=low PA) is hypothesized to be asso- around the chest and measurements were started when the signal
ciated with (1) questionnaires on stress-related aspects and (2) was stabilized. Each child was encouraged to breath normally and
hypersecretion patterns of the biomarker salivary cortisol. To cover not to speak or move during the measurement. In the occasion of
several aspects of stress, different questionnaires will be used. Fur- sudden irregular respiration, the registration was cancelled, as such
thermore, measuring cortisol at several time points will enable minimizing breathing influences. Further data processing was done
the consideration of alternative cortisol parameters across the day with the free, professional HRV Analysis Software of the University
N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440 435

of Kuopio, Finland (Niskanen, Tarvainen, Ranta-Aho, & Karjalainen, death, family, school and social domains. This validated (reliability
2004). Low frequency (LF) and high frequency (HF) bands were r = 0.69, construct validity = 0.45) 36-item questionnaire measured
analyzed between 0.04–0.15 Hz and 0.15–0.4 Hz, as default (Task the self-reported frequency and timing of neutral and negative
Force of, ESC/NASPE, 1996). The RR series were detrended using events in the last year relevant for this age group and resulted in a
the Smoothness priors method with alpha = 300 and a cubic inter- children’s ‘life change units’ score.
polation at the default rate of 4 Hz was done. The middle 5 min
were manually checked on their quality and if necessary, another 2.6. Problems (parental report)
appropriate 5 min interval was selected. Quality was defined as no
large RR-interval outliers, an equidistance between consecutive RR- Parents were asked to complete 4 subscales of the ‘Strengths
interval points and unimodal, Gaussians RR-interval and heart rate and Difficulties Questionnaire’ (Goodman, 1997) on problems of
distribution graphics. By doing this, disturbing phenomena such as their child over the past six months: emotional problems, conduct
the Valsalva manoeuvre (exhaling while keeping mouth and nose problems, peer problems and prosocial behaviour. Parents could
closed) were excluded. answer on the statements with: ‘not true’ (0), ‘somewhat true’ (1)
Using time domain methods, the root mean square of suc- and ‘certainly true’ (2). Higher scores on the prosocial behaviour
cessive differences (RMSSD) and the percentage of consecutive subscale reflect strengths, whereas higher scores on the other three
normal RR intervals differing more than 50 ms (pNN50) were deter- subscales reflect difficulties. This questionnaire has shown good
mined. Using spectral domain methods, parametric autoregression test–retest stability (r = 0.88) and good validity when compared to
(forward–backward least squares with a personally optimized the Child Behaviour Checklist (r between 0.87 and 0.7) and the
model order) was performed. The power of LF and HF bands in Rutter questionnaire (r = 0.88) (Goodman, 1997; Goodman & Scott,
absolute and normalized (nu, LF or HF divided by ‘LF + HF’) units 1999; Muris, Meesters, & van den Berg, 2003).
and the LF/HF ratio were determined.
Several HRV parameters were used to test which one was most 2.7. Salivary cortisol
convincingly associated with stress and as they can represent
different aspects: HF, pNN50 and RMSSD can reflect the vagal inner- Saliva was collected at home into Salivette synthetic swabs
vation (=PA), LF is dual-innervated (=PA and SA) and the LF/HF specifically designed for cortisol analysis (Sarstedt, Germany). The
ratio has been assumed to represent the sympathovagal balance, participants were asked to collect saliva during two consecu-
although some theories criticize this (Porges, 2007). The HF and LF tive weekdays at four time points: immediately after wake up,
power in normalized units have the advantage of minimizing the 30 min after wake up, 60 min after wake up and in the evening
effect of total power differences (ESC/NAS, 1996). between 7 and 8 PM. For the four consecutive time points, the
within coefficients of variation were 36.2, 33.6, 43.0 and 41.5%,
2.3. Questionnaires on children’s stress respectively. Pearson correlation coefficients between 2 days for
the four time points were respectively 0.399, 0.336, 0.498 and
A broad definition of stress was used by measuring the differ- 0.509 (all p < 0.001). Standardized sampling instructions have been
ent aspects of the stress process: negative events and also outcome published (Michels, Sioen, Huybrechts et al., 2012). The parents
aspects i.e. negative emotions and behaviour. The stress appraisal were also asked to fill in a checklist about instruction compli-
phase (“are you stressed?”) was not measured since this subjective ance. Adherence to the sampling points was strictly requested as
representation is difficult to be answered in children. Children were non-compliance may partially invalidate cortisol results (Kudielka,
individually interviewed by a trained researcher to obtain infor- Broderick, & Kirschbaum, 2003). As we found that parental reported
mation about their life events and emotions. Furthermore, parents ‘time non-compliers’ showed lower cortisol concentrations and
were asked to report on their child’s behavioural and emotional less pronounced CAR than ‘time compliers’ (Michels, Sioen, De
problems over the past 6 months. Vriendt, et al., 2012; Michels, Sioen, Huybrechts, et al., 2012), a
quality control was executed by excluding morning samples col-
2.4. Emotions (child report) lected more than 5 min different from the requested time point
and evening samples not collected between 7 and 9 PM (271 out of
Children had to indicate on a Likert-scale how they mostly feel 3290 samples). Furthermore, samples of corticosteroid-users were
(not only today). For each of the 4 emotions (happiness, anger, anx- also excluded (5 children).
iety and sadness) they could rate themselves from 0 ‘not at all’ to Samples were stored at home in the fridge and were picked up
10 ‘very strong’ as was done by Zimmer-Gembeck, Lees, Bradley at school in the morning after the two sampling days and were kept
and Skinner (2009). To help the children understand these dis- cool during transport. Centrifugated saliva filtrates were assayed in
tinct feelings, four pictures of a social skills training game for very the routine laboratory of the Ghent University Hospital on a Modu-
young children were displayed next to the question (one picture for lar E 170 immunoanalyser system (Roche Diagnostics, Mannheim,
each emotion) (Dupondt, 1992). These basic emotions are already Germany) using the Roche Cobas Cortisol assay, a competitive elec-
understandable during infancy (Flavell, 1999) and can as such be trochemiluminescence immunoassay. More details and reference
uncomplicatedly used in this age-group. The sum of the negative ranges on the cortisol values in this population were published
emotions (anger, anxiety, sadness) was validated against the well- elsewhere (Michels, Sioen, De Vriendt, et al., 2012; Michels, Sioen,
know PANAS-C questionnaire (Laurent et al., 1999) that can be used Huybrechts, et al., 2012).
for children of at least 9 years old. A Spearman correlation of r = 0.48,
p < 0.001 with the negative affect score of the PANAS-C question- 2.8. Statistical analyses
naire was found in a sample of 153 children that were between 9
and 13y old at follow-up. Most statistical analyses were performed using SPSS/PASW ver-
sion 19 (IBM Corp., NY, USA), while the cortisol-HRV regression
2.5. Life events (child report) was performed in the multilevel HLM/2L programme (version 7.0)
using an approach similar to the one published by Adam (2006).
The ‘Coddington Life Events Scale’ for children (Coddington, The significance level was always set to p < 0.05. Standardized
1972, 1999) was used to identify potential physical and mental regression coefficients with their 95% confidence interval are given.
health problems arising from life events: events concerning illness, Due to a skewed distribution, HF power, LF power, LF/HF and
436 N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and sex and age differences for heart rate variability, physical activity, stress questionnaire data and salivary cortisol levels.

Median Interquartile range Sex Age

Heart rate variability (N = 432) Correlation coefficient


Mean heart rate 82 75–98 B<G −0.415*
Time domain parameters
RMSSD (ms) 71 53–100 B>G 0.141*
pNN50 (%) 41 27–55 B>G 0.207*
Autoregression
LF power (ms2 ) 443 239–800 B>G 0.031
HF power (ms2 ) 428 224–787 B>G 0.161*
LFnu (normalized units) 47 38–56 −0.161*
HFnu (normalized units) 46 34–56 0.211*
LF/HF ratio 1.02 0.71–1.58 −0.209*
Physical activity (N = 432) (self reported h/week) 14 10–20 B>G 0.115*
Stress questionnaire data (N = 334)
Emotions
Happiness (Likert scale 0–10) 8 6–10 −0.221*
Anger (Likert scale 0–10) 3 1–5 0.126*
Anxiety (Likert scale 0–10) 1 0–3 B<G −0.039
Sadness (Likert scale 0–10) 2 0–4 B<G 0.008
Negative event score last 12 months 39 7–72 0.094
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire
Emotional problems (0–10) 2 1–4 −0.042
Conduct problems (0–6) 1 0–2 B>G −0.052
Peer problems (0–10) 1 0–2 −0.017
Prosocial behaviour (0–8) 7 6–8 B<G 0.076
Salivary cortisol (N = 293)
Immediately after wake up (nmol/l) 12.11 9.74–14.47 0.129*
30 min after wake up (nmol/l) 12.77 9.90–15.66 0.124*
60 min after wake up (nmol/l) 7.93 5.96–11.00 0.116*
Evening (nmol/l) 1.63 1.17–2.44 0.181*

HF, high frequency spectral power; LF, low frequency spectral power; LF/HF, ratio of low frequency power to high frequency power; pNN50, percentage of consecutive normal
RRI differing more than 50 ms; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences; B, boys; G, Girls.
*
p < 0.05.

cortisol concentrations were log-transformed. Chi-square statis- in the model, the CAR became a separate coefficient that could
tic and Mann–Whitney U statistic were used to test the selection be predicted independently of the other parameters, such as the
bias for categorical (sex and parental education) and continuous diurnal slope. A dummy variable for day of measurement (first or
variables (age and physical activity) respectively by comparing the second day) was included to account for possible systematic corti-
included and excluded population. Mann–Whitney U and spear- sol differences across days. The reported day-specific wake up time,
man correlation were used in detecting sex and age differences considered as a cortisol-confounder, was also included as a level 1
(age used as a continuous parameter) in physical activity, HRV, parameter because of its day-dependence. HRV parameters were
questionnaire and cortisol parameters. entered as a level 2 predictor of each of the relevant level 1 pre-
Linear regression was used to analyze the relation between the dictors (intercept, slope and CAR) and these models were adjusted
children’s HRV (dependent variable) and stress questionnaire data: for potential confounders (age, sex, physical activity and wake up
(1) the emotions happy, angry, anxious and sad, (2) the negative time). Because of multicollinearity, the HRV parameters were not
event score during the last 12 months and (3) emotional prob- entered simultaneously.
lems, conduct problems, peer problems and prosocial behaviour.
The analyses were adjusted for age, sex and physical activity as 3. Results
these were important HRV determinants in this population sam-
ple (Michels, Vanaelst, et al., 2012). Analyses were stratified by sex Information was available on both HRV and salivary cortisol
when a significant interaction with sex was present. for 293 children (50.5% boys) and information on both HRV and
Hierarchical linear modelling was used to analyze the relation stress questionnaires for 334 children (51.2% boys). The children
between the children’s cortisol pattern and their HRV, adjusted were almost evenly distributed over the age range 5–10. Tertiary
for potential confounders (age, sex, physical activity and wake up parental education was seen in 71.4%. Descriptive statistics on the
time). It is a variant of multiple linear regression useful for data used variables are shown in Table 1. Sex differences were found in
with a nested design, which is the case in this study as repeated HRV, stress questionnaires and physical activity parameters with
cortisol measurements were obtained for each participating child. boys having higher HRV, more physical activity and more conduct
Therefore, a two-level model on the dependent variable ‘cortisol’ problems, while girls having a higher mean heart rate and reporting
was used with the intra-individual parameters modelled at level 1 more anxiety and sadness. HRV parameters representing PA, cor-
(cortisol pattern created by time of day) and the inter-individual tisol values, physical activity and self-reported anger increased by
parameters (HRV data and the personal characteristics as possible age, while the other HRV parameters and self-reported happiness
confounders) at level 2. At level 1, the child’s cortisol values were decreased.
predicted by the time of day, to estimate the shape of each child’s
cortisol curve during the day. Time of day values were expressed 3.1. HRV versus stress related questionnaire data
as ‘number of hours since wake up’ for each participant each day
and centred to midday as six hours post-awakening. To charac- In Table 2, HRV parameters were tested for associations with
terize the CAR, a design variable was created assigning the value questionnaire data on stress aspects. The total negative event
1 to the sample taken 30 min after wake up, and the value 0 to score, happiness, prosocial behaviour and emotional and conduct
all other samples. By representing the CAR as a separate variable problems were not associated with HRV (data not shown). Sex
N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440 437

Table 2
Linear regression predicting heart rate variability parameters by stress questionnaire data adjusted for age, physical activity and sex or stratified by sex (N = 334).

Time-domain HRV parameters Autoregression HRV parameters

RMSSD (ms) pNN50 (%) LF (ms2 ) HF (ms2 ) LFnu HFnu LF/HF

Emotions
Anger (both sexes)a bèta −0.111 0.072 −0.052 −0.147 0.098 −0.121 0.114
p 0.037 0.174 0.316 0.006 0.070 0.022 0.036
Anxiety (boys) bèta 0.080 0.063 0.097 0.043 0.095 −0.043 0.073
p 0.293 0.403 0.206 0.573 0.213 0.572 0.337
Anxiety (girls) bèta −0.219 −0.160 −0.120 −0.250 0.190 −0.223 0.208
p 0.006 0.040 0.131 0.002 0.019 0.008 0.009
Sadness (boys) bèta −0.024 −0.018 0.011 −0.001 0.054 0.007 0.017
p 0.754 0.809 0.891 0.991 0.480 0.927 0.821
Sadness (girls) bèta −0.200 −0.220 −0.127 −0.195 0.118 −0.112 0.118
p 0.012 0.006 0.111 0.014 0.138 0.151 0.132

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire


Peer problems (boys) bèta −0.164 −0.129 −0.166 −0.187 0.046 −0.064 0.060
p 0.029 0.085 0.034 0.017 0.541 0.383 0.412
Peer problems (girls) bèta −0.008 0.031 −0.019 0.058 −0.096 0.112 −0.105
p 0.917 0.704 0.809 0.472 0.232 0.156 0.182

HF, high frequency spectral power; LF, low frequency spectral power; LF/HF, ratio of low frequency power to high frequency power; nu, normalized units; pNN50, percentage
of consecutive normal RRI differing more than 50 ms; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences.
a
Not stratified by sex since the sex interaction factor was not significant.

interaction effects were found for the effect of anxiety (on RMSSD 0.06
p = 0.010; on pNN50 p = 0.043, on HF p = 0.008; on HFnu p = 0.022), 0.04

corsol intercept
sadness (on pNN50 p = 0.041; on HF p = 0.029) and peer problems 0.02

(bèta ln)
(for RMSSD p = 0.007; for HF p = 0.025): associations for anxiety and 0
sadness were observed only in girls and for peer problems only in -0.02
boys. Anger, anxiety, sadness and peer problems were all associ- -0.04
ated with lower PA reflected by lower RMSSD (ˇ = −0.111 [−0.180; -0.06
−0.042], ˇ = −0.219 [−0.366; −0.072], ˇ = −0.200 [−0.343; −0.057], -0.08
ˇ = −0.164 [−0.315; −0.013] respectively) and HF (ˇ = −0.147 p=0.040* p=0.009* p=0.011* p=0.282 p=0.114 p=0.215 p=0.185
RMSSD pNN50 HF LF HFnu LFnu LF/HF
[−0.251; −0.043], ˇ = −0.250 [−0.401; −0.099], ˇ = −0.195 [−0.346;
−0.044], ˇ = −0.187 [−0.328; −0.046], respectively), while anxiety 0.006
was also related to higher LF/HF (ˇ = 0.208 [0.065; 0.351]), perhaps 0.004
corsol slope

due to a parallel increased LFnu (ˇ = 0.190 [0.021;0.359]). 0.002


(bèta ln)

0
3.2. HRV versus cortisol -0.002

-0.004
Fig. 1 illustrates the significant associations between sali-
-0.006
vary cortisol and HRV. Both overall cortisol levels, the diurnal
p=0.091 p=0.033* p=0.040* p=0.531 p=0.145 p=0.145 p=0.173
cortisol slope and the CAR were associated with HRV. Higher RMSSD pNN50 HF LF HFnu LFnu LF/HF
overall cortisol levels and a steeper diurnal decline were nega-
tively associated with the PA markers RMSSD (ˇ = −0.044 [−0.002; 0.08
−0.086] for overall cortisol), pNN50 (ˇ = −0.066 [−0.016; −0.115];
corsol awakening response

ˇ = −0.005 [−0.001; −0.010], respectively) and HF (ˇ = −0.052 0.06


[−0.007; −0.097]; ˇ = −0.005 [−0.001; −0.010], respectively). A
0.04
larger CAR was associated with a higher LFnu (ˇ = −0.067 [0.011;
(bèta ln)

0.123]) and LF/HF ratio (ˇ = 0.062 [0.013; 0.111]). 0.02

0
4. Discussion
-0.02
Our results in 5–10 year old children are in line with the sug-
gestion of low HRV (=low PA) as stress indicator by its reflection of -0.04
p=0.654 p=0.789 p=0.679 p=0.367 p=0.183 p=0.012* p=0.022*
chronic stress-induced changes in the autonomic nervous system RMSSD pNN50 HF LF HFnu LFnu LF/HF
(Task Force of, ESC/NASPE, 1996; Thayer & Brosschot, 2005).
First of all, low PA and possibly also high SA were related to Fig. 1. Associations between children’s cortisol and heart rate variability adjusted
higher reports of certain stress aspects in our child population: peer for age, sex, physical activity and wake up time (N = 293). Standardized coefficients
(in natural logarithm) of different cortisol patterns are shown: cortisol intercept
problems, anger, anxiety and sadness were associated with lower (panel A), cortisol diurnal slope (panel B) and cortisol awakening response (panel
PA. Anxiety was also associated with higher LFnu and LF/HF that C). Filled bars are the heart rate variability parameters representing the parasym-
may reflect more sympathetic over parasympathetic dominance. pathetic activity, empty bars are those representing the sympathetic activity or a
Since a reduction of HRV (i.e. reduced PA with or without increased combination of both parasympathetic and sympathetic activity. HF, high frequency
spectral power; LF, low frequency spectral power; LF/HF, ratio of low frequency
SA) is a pathway of increased morbidity and mortality (Thayer et al.,
power to high frequency power; nu, normalized units; pNN50, percentage of consec-
2010), HRV may be a potential pathway linking stress to ill health utive normal RRI differing more than 50 ms; RMSSD, root mean square of successive
(Thayer & Brosschot, 2005). differences.
438 N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440

Furthermore, HRV showed moderate associations with the problem data was in a sex-dependent manner related to cortisol in
other stress marker cortisol, although both represent two differ- a previous analysis (Michels, Clays, et al., 2012). Consequently, sex
ent neural stress systems: the autonomic nervous system and the differences should always be considered in stress measurements.
hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis, respectively. Higher sali-
vary cortisol levels and diurnal slopes were associated with lower 4.2. HRV versus cortisol
PA. Larger CAR was associated with higher LFnu and LF/HF: a higher
sympathetic over parasympathetic dominance might be caused Since both HRV and cortisol have been shown to reflect subjec-
by an increased SA since PA parameters were not significantly tive stress reports, the second hypothesis was that HRV and cortisol
decreased. could show good agreement in their status. Low HRV (i.e. low PA)
When comparing the relations of the different HRV parameters, was indeed related to specific cortisol patterns that have previ-
HF and RMSSD were most significant in the relation with stress ously been associated with stress questionnaires in our population,
questionnaires and HF en pNN50 were most significant in rela- namely a steeper diurnal decline and sometimes higher cortisol val-
tion with cortisol. Consequently, this confirms our hypothesis that ues (Michels, Clays, et al., 2012). Low HRV (a higher sympathetic
low PA can indicate stress. HF could be the preferred stress indica- over parasympathetic dominance) was in our study also related to
tor. Indeed, Porges et al. have previously stated that especially low a higher CAR which was positively associated with life stress in a
vagal tone (PA) is linked with poor emotion regulation, high stress, recent meta-analysis (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). Up to now, only few
decreased stress reactivity and increased stress vulnerability, even studies examined the HRV–cortisol relation and those handled par-
in early life (Porges, 1995; Porges et al., 1994). ticularly the CAR. Higher CAR values have been associated with low
HF and LF but no changes in LF/HF cross-sectionally (Stalder, Evans,
4.1. HRV versus stress questionnaire data Hucklebridge, & Clow, 2011) and with low HF longitudinally (Eller,
2007). Nevertheless, also non-significant findings have been pub-
In a previous publication, significant associations were found lished (Gunnar, Porter, Wolf, Rigatuso, & Larson, 1995; Johnson,
between salivary cortisol and our stress questionnaire data namely Hansen, Sollers, Murison, & Julian, 2002). Studies have also been
the negative event score, emotional problems, peer problems and done on parallelism in stress reactions of these two biomarkers.
happiness (Michels, Clays, et al., 2012). In the current publica- A natural occurring stressor of examination could both increase
tion, we confirm that low HRV (low PA) can be predicted by peer cortisol and decrease HRV in students (Sgoifo et al., 2003), as was
problems, although the HRV associations were most prominent for the case for a laboratory stressor in children (Doussard-Roosevelt,
the negative emotions. More specifically, evidence was highest for Montgomery, & Porges, 2003). Furthermore, the baseline HF sta-
the negative emotion ‘anxiety’: anxiety was related with lower PA tus might determine the cortisol response to stress, although there
parameters (RMSSD, pNN50, HF and HFnu) and a higher LFnu and are inconsistent findings (Gunnar et al., 1995; Johnson et al., 2002;
LF/HF ratio. Consequently, anxiety was related with a lower PA and Smeets, 2010).
a lower sympathetic over parasympathetic dominance (possibly Despite the shown associations between HRV and cortisol,
due to a concurrently higher SA). Indeed, a vagal circuit (PA) in the their reaction on stress may still differ. For instance, a dissocia-
physiological regulation of emotions has previously been outlined tion has been reported in the reactivity of both neural systems
(Porges et al., 1994). Literature is especially abundant on the HRV to repeated stressors with a lower cortisol stress response after
relationship with anxiety, e.g. anxiety in adults and children was several repeated exposures (=habituation), while the HRV reac-
related to both low PA and high SA in a recent review (Friedman, tion remained high (Schommer, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2003).
2007). Peer problems are not a direct measurement of stress itself. Besides, the differential associations of HRV and cortisol with stress
Nevertheless, they can elicit chronic stress by the long-lasting expo- questionnaire data (as we have shown here) may indicate activa-
sure to bullying and by threatening the establishment of close and tion differences in the systems underlying both biomarkers. As a
enduring interpersonal relationships (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). result, a combination of cortisol and HRV measurements could be
Since the arousal will sustain until the reason for the arousal is considered when examining population differences in stress sta-
eliminated, these difficult situations will not lead to phasic arousal tus. When already implementing salivary cortisol measurements,
but to sustained, chronic arousal. the HRV measurements do not add high additional burden to the
HRV could not be predicted by the negative event score. Since examiner and the participant as the measurement is very short and
the event questionnaire only measured the occurrence of events is well accepted by the children.
and not the subjective stress perception or coping, it is not
known whether the reported events were perceived as stress- 4.3. Strengths and limitations
ful and how the children dealt with it (Miller, Chen, & Zhou,
2007). Consequently, this could have reduced the ability to observe This paper contributes to the scarce literature linking HRV and
stronger associations. Although HRV could theoretically be influ- cortisol. Important strengths of this study are (1) the standardized
enced by behavioural factors like sociality and conduct disorders and quality controlled HRV measurements with several calculated
(Beauchaine, 2001), HRV could not be predicted by prosocial parameters, (2) the comparison of HRV with both questionnaire
behaviour, emotional problems and conduct problems in our study. data and cortisol, (3) the use of sophisticated analyses with cor-
The observed stress–HRV relations showed sex differences for rection and stratification, (4) the use of different questionnaires to
certain questionnaire data: anxiety and sadness were only related cover the different aspects of stress and (5) the repeated cortisol
to HRV parameters in girls, while peer problems were only related measures enabling the consideration of several alternative param-
to HRV parameters in boys. Underlying mechanisms could be the eters across the day.
sex differences that have been demonstrated in the used HRV Concerning the questionnaires, we need to consider that over-
parameters (Michels, Vanaelst, et al., 2012), in psychological func- or underreporting is possible and that a broad definition of stress
tioning and development of children (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003) has been used. Although we tried to standardize the interview-
and in handling stressful situations (Hampel & Petermann, 2005). ing conditions in the absence of parents and schoolteachers, there
Sex differences in HRV-anxiety have also been reported with sig- remains a questionable validity and repeatability of such ques-
nificances only in women and girls (Greaves-Lord et al., 2010; tionnaire data in young children. Validated emotion self-report
Grossman, Wilhelm, Kawachi, & Sparrow, 2001). This phenomenon questionnaires almost exclusively exist for older children (start-
occurs also in other stress relations as our happiness and peer ing around 9), therefore we opted for a short questionnaire with
N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440 439

terminology comprehensible in infancy that showed moderate (Bijzonder Onderzoeksfonds). Barbara Vanaelst and Isabelle Sioen
correlation (r = 0.48) with the validated PANAS-C questionnaire. are financially supported by the Research Foundation – Flanders.
Moreover, the CLES questionnaire has quite a low construct valid- The funding source had no further role in study design; in collec-
ity (0.45) for assessing life events in children. Further criticism is tion, analysis and interpretation of data; in writing of the report;
that we cannot check whether the emotion data really are a reflec- and in the decision to submit the paper for publication. The authors
tion of persistent stress since no further questions on chronicity want to thank the participating children and their parents for their
were included. These issues with the emotions and events ques- voluntary participation.
tionnaires could thwart the relations of questionnaire data with
biological data such as HRV. Concerning salivary cortisol, we only
References
used a subjective measure of time compliance in the cortisol samp-
ling since objective compliance measurements were not feasible Adam, E. K. (2006). Transactions among adolescent trait and state emotion and
in this large population. Nevertheless, accuracy was improved by diurnal and momentary cortisol activity in naturalistic settings. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology, 31(5), 664–679.
stressing the importance of timing and excluding self-reported
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
non-compliers (DeSantis, Adam, Mendelsohn, & Doane, 2010). Con- attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
cerning HRV, measures were only performed in supine position. 497–529.
Since supine position could diminish SA, future research should also Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory:
Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psy-
test basal HRV measures in sitting position as marker of chronic chopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 183–214.
stress (Vybiral, Bryg, Maddens, & Boden, 1989). Although visual Brown, T. E., Beightol, L. A., Koh, J., & Eckberg, D. L. (1993). Important influence of
respiratory observation was done during the registration, breath- respiration on human R-R interval power spectra is largely ignored. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 75(5), 2310–2317.
ing rate was not measured with an extra channel. Breathing rate Chandola, T., Heraclides, A., & Kumari, M. (2010). Psychophysiological biomarkers
was shown to be negatively related with LF and HF power, but had of workplace stressors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 51–57.
no influence on time-domain parameters (Brown, Beightol, Koh, & Charmandari, E., Tsigos, C., & Chrousos, G. (2005). Endocrinology of the stress
response. Annual Review of Physiology, 67, 259–284.
Eckberg, 1993; Penttila et al., 2001). Nonlinearities are more likely Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial
to happen with a low breathing rate (e.g. at 10/minute breath- factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 80(3),
ing rate), a long RRI and in the presence of important respiratory 265–278.
Coddington, R. D. (1972). The significance of life events as etiologic factors in the
sinus arrhythmia (Porta et al., 2000). Nevertheless, the influence
diseases of children. II. A study of a normal population,. Journal of Psychosomatic
of nonlinearities will be minimal in our population as children Research, 16(3), 205–213.
have a general high breathing rate (18–21/min) and short RR- Coddington, R. D. (1999). Coddington Life Events Scales (CLES) technical manual.
Toronto: Multi Health Systems.
interval; and respiratory sinus arrhythmia induced by the Valsalva
Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., & Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological stress and
manoeuvre was more likely abolished. Furthermore, the measure- disease. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association, 298(14),
ment modules were optional resulting in smaller samples when 1685–1687.
combining HRV with cortisol or questionnaire data, although our Crick, N. R., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). The development of psychopathology in
females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and
high quality control was partially responsible for this. Finally, the Psychopathology, 15(3), 719–742.
cross-sectional nature of our results limits the directional state- DeSantis, A. S., Adam, E. K., Mendelsohn, K. A., & Doane, L. D. (2010). Concordance
ments on the relationships. Although our hypothesis was that between self-reported and objective wakeup times in ambulatory salivary cor-
tisol research. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 17(1), 74–78.
chronic stress may result in a simultaneous effect on HRV and Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Montgomery, L. A., & Porges, S. W. (2003). Short-term
cortisol status, interconnections between the two pathways exist stability of physiological measures in kindergarten children: Respiratory sinus
since corticotrophin-releasing factor and noradrenalin can stimu- arrhythmia, heart period, and cortisol. Developmental Psychobiology, 43(3),
230–242.
late each other directly or indirectly e.g. vagal PA innervations of Dupondt, L. (1992). Ik voel me zo.: Ervaringsgericht werken met verhalen en prenten-
the amygdala influence cortisol (Thayer & Brosschot, 2005; Thayer boeken rond gevoelens bij jonge kinderen. Leuven: Centrum voor ervaringsgericht
& Sternberg, 2006). onderwijs vzw.
Eller, N. H. (2007). Total power and high frequency components of heart rate vari-
ability and risk factors for atherosclerosis. Autonomic Neuroscience, 131(1/2),
123–130.
5. Conclusion
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about the mind.
Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45.
We have shown that children’s HRV (especially low parasym- Friedman, B. H. (2007). An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration model of
anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 185–199.
pathetic activity) might serve as a stress indicator since it
Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., & Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The cortisol awakening response
reflects some aspects of their stress status as represented by (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
questionnaire data on negative emotions and peer problems. 72(1), 67–73.
Moreover, HRV showed moderate associations with cortisol, Galeev, A. R., Igisheva, L. N., & Kazin, E. M. (2002). Heart rate variability in healthy
six- to sixteen-year-old children. Human Physiology, 28(4), 428–432.
the marker of the other main neural stress system i.e. the Gamelin, F. X., Baquet, G., Berthoin, S., & Bosquet, L. (2008). Validity of the polar S810
hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis. Nevertheless, measuring to measure R-R intervals in children. International Journal of Sports Medicine,
both pathways is recommended as they might be stimulated 29(2), 134–138.
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note.
differently depending on the stressor or stress outcome. More- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 581–586.
over, further research is necessary to proof HRV as a stress Goodman, R., & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the strengths and difficulties question-
indicator because of inherent problems in assessing complex naire and the child behavior checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 27(1), 17–24.
concepts (adverse life events and emotions) among young Greaves-Lord, K., Tulen, J., Dietrich, A., Sondeijker, F., van Roon, A., Oldehinkel, A.,
children. et al. (2010). Reduced autonomic flexibility as a predictor for future anxiety in
girls from the general population: The TRAILS study. Psychiatry Research, 179(2),
187–193.
Acknowledgements Grossman, P., Wilhelm, F. H., Kawachi, I., & Sparrow, D. (2001). Gender differences
in psychophysiological responses to speech stress among older social phobics:
Congruence and incongruence between self-evaluative and cardiovascular reac-
The project was financed by the European Community within tions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(5), 765–777.
the Sixth RTD Framework Programme Contract No. 016181 (FOOD) Gunnar, M. R., Porter, F. L., Wolf, C. M., Rigatuso, J., & Larson, M. C. (1995). Neonatal
as part of the IDEFICS study and the research council of Ghent stress reactivity: Predictions to later emotional temperament. Child Develop-
ment, 66(1), 1–13.
University (Bijzonder Onderzoeksfonds). Nathalie Michels is finan- Hampel, P., & Petermann, F. (2005). Age and gender effects on coping in children and
cially supported by the research council of Ghent University adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(2), 73–83.
440 N. Michels et al. / Biological Psychology 94 (2013) 433–440

Heim, C., Ehlert, U., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2000). The potential role of hypocortisolism Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress. Neuroscience
in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroen- and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19(2), 225–233.
docrinology, 25(1), 1–35. Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2),
Johnson, B. H., Hansen, A. L., Sollers, J. J., Murison, R., & Julian, J. F. (2002). Heart 116–143.
rate variability is inversely related to cortisol reactivity during cognitive stress. Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). Vagal tone and the
Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(1), 128. physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Kieling, C., Baker-Henningham, H., Belfer, M., Conti, G., Ertem, I., Omigbodun, O., Child Development, 59(2/3), 167–186.
et al. (2011). Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: Evidence for action. Porta, A., Baselli, G., Guzzetti, S., Pagani, M., Malliani, A., & Cerutti, S. (2000). Predic-
Lancet, 378(9801), 1515–1525. tion of short cardiovascular variability signals based on conditional distribution.
Koolhaas, J. M., Bartolomucci, A., Buwalda, B., de Boer, S. F., Flugge, G., Korte, S. IEEE Transactions on Bio-medical Engineering, 47(12), 1555–1564.
M., et al. (2011). Stress revisited: A critical evaluation of the stress concept. Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Dissociation between
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(5), 1291–1301. reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-
Kudielka, B. M., Broderick, J. E., & Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Compliance with saliva adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosomatic
sampling protocols: Electronic monitoring reveals invalid cortisol daytime pro- Medicine, 65(3), 450–460.
files in noncompliant subjects. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(2), 313–319. Sgoifo, A., Braglia, F., Costoli, T., Musso, E., Meerlo, P., Ceresini, G., et al. (2003). Car-
Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Joiner, T. E., Rudolph, K. D., Potter, K. I., Lambert, S., et al. diac autonomic reactivity and salivary cortisol in men and women exposed to
(1999). A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale develop- social stressors: Relationship with individual ethological profile. Neuroscience
ment and preliminary validation. Psychological Assessment, 11(3), 326–338. and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(1/2), 179–188.
Michels, N., Clays, E., De Buyzere, M., Huybrechts, I., Marild, S., Vanaelst, B., et al. Smeets, T. (2010). Autonomic and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress resilience:
(2012). Determinants and reference values of short-term heart rate variability Impact of cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology, 84(2), 290–295.
in children. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 113(6), 1477–1488. Stalder, T., Evans, P., Hucklebridge, F., & Clow, A. (2011). Associations between the
Michels, N., Sioen, I., De Vriendt, T., Huybrechts, I., Vanaelst, B., & De Henauw, S. cortisol awakening response and heart rate variability. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
(2012). Children’s morning and evening salivary cortisol: Pattern instruction ogy, 36(4), 454–462.
compliance and sampling confounders. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 77(1), Task Force of, ESC/NASP. E. (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement,
27–35. physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society
Michels, N., Sioen, I., Huybrechts, I., Bammann, K., Vanaelst, B., De Vriendt, T., of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology.
et al. (2012). Negative life events, emotions and psychological difficulties as Circulation, 93(5), 1043–1065.
determinants of salivary cortisol in Belgian primary school children. Psychoneu- Thayer, J. F., & Brosschot, J. F. (2005). Psychosomatics and psychopathology:
roendocrinology, 37(9), 1506–1515. Looking up and down from the brain. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10),
Michels, N., Vanaelst, B., Vyncke, K., Sioen, I., Huybrechts, I., De Vriendt, T., et al. 1050–1058.
(2012). Children’s Body composition and Stress – the ChiBS study: Aims, design, Thayer, J. F., & Sternberg, E. (2006). Beyond heart rate variability – Vagal regula-
methods, population and participation characteristics. Archives of Public Health, tion of allostatic systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1088,
(70), 17th article, 13pp. 361–372.
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic Thayer, J. F., Yamamoto, S. S., & Brosschot, J. F. (2010). The relationship of auto-
stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psycho- nomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors.
logical Bulletin, 133(1), 25–45. International Journal of Cardiology, 141(2), 122–131.
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & van den Berg, F. (2003). The Strengths and Difficulties Ursin, H., & Eriksen, H. R. (2004). The cognitive activation theory of stress. Psychoneu-
Questionnaire (SDQ) – Further evidence for its reliability and validity in a com- roendocrinology, 29(5), 567–592.
munity sample of Dutch children and adolescents. European Child and Adolescent Vybiral, T., Bryg, R. J., Maddens, M. E., & Boden, W. E. (1989). Effect of passive tilt
Psychiatry, 12(1), 1–8. on sympathetic and parasympathetic components of heart rate variability in
Niskanen, J. P., Tarvainen, M. P., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A. (2004). Soft- normal subjects. American Journal of Cardiology, 63(15), 1117–1120.
ware for advanced HRV analysis. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, Zhou, Y., Xie, G. L., Wang, J. H., & Yang, S. R. (2012). Cardiovascular risk factors signif-
76(1), 73–81. icantly correlate with autonomic nervous system activity in children. Canadian
Penttila, J., Helminen, A., Jartti, T., Kuusela, T., Huikuri, H. V., Tulppo, M. P., et al. Journal of Cardiology, 28(4), 477–482.
(2001). Time domain, geometrical and frequency domain analysis of cardiac Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Lees, D. C., Bradley, G. L., & Skinner, E. A. (2009). Use of
vagal outflow: Effects of various respiratory patterns. Clinical Physiology, 21(3), an analogue method to examine children’s appraisals of threat and emotion in
365–376. response to stressful events. Motivation and Emotion, 33(2), 136–149.

S-ar putea să vă placă și