Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

31.12.

2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in


distress

Photo by Samule Austin/Unsplash


Joseph E Davis is research professor of sociology and chair of the Picturing the Human Colloquy at the Institute
for Advanced Studies in Culture at the University of Virginia. His latest book is Chemically Imbalanced: Everyday
Suffering, Medication, and Our Troubled Quest for Self-Mastery (2020).

After Jenna discovered that her boyfriend was cheating on her, she went into an
emotional tailspin. She was ‘crying all the time’, struggled to attend her university
classes, slept a lot and avoided situations she normally enjoyed.

In recounting her emotional reaction, Jenna stressed to me its unreasonableness. Given


that she and her boyfriend hadn’t been dating for long, she felt that she shouldn’t have
been so upset. After a month, she decided that something was seriously amiss and that
she needed professional help. She recalled her psychiatrist diagnosing depression and

https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 1/6
31.12.2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

telling her the problem might be caused by a chemical imbalance in her brain, for which
she was prescribed an antidepressant.

Jenna found her emotional reactions jarring. They defied her basic assumptions about
herself as confident, mature and self-sufficient. She told me she welcomed the diagnosis
of a neurobiological disorder, which confirmed her problem was ‘real’ – brought on by a
physiological force external to her volition – and that it showed she’s not ‘just a slacker’.

At the same time, Jenna was careful to distance her experience from that of people who
are, in her words, ‘crazy’ or ‘nuts’. Their illness means a loss of control and ability to
function. By contrast, she sees her problem as a common and minor glitch in
neurochemistry. No one, she insisted, should mistake her for the mentally ill.

Jenna was one of 80 diverse volunteers that a research team at the University of Virginia
and I interviewed in Chicago, Baltimore, Boston and two small cities in central Virginia.
We wanted to find out how people deal with common forms of psychological distress
and challenging circumstances, such as: shyness and nervousness in social situations;
underperformance at work or school; struggles after the loss of a significant relationship;
and disappointment with how their lives are unfolding. A majority of our interviewees
had received some form of psychotherapy and/or been diagnosed with a condition such
as depression, social anxiety disorder or attention deficit disorder, and prescribed a
psychiatric medication.

It was striking that many (though not all) at least partly explained their distress in terms
of biological causes, particularly a neurochemical imbalance. Yet thinking of their
problems in this way was a fraught process. Like Jenna, many interviewees sharply
distinguished themselves from the mentally ill and cast the mentally ill in a very
negative light. This stigmatising of people with serious mental illness wasn’t based on
any first-hand experiences; rather, it was motivated by a desire to protect their own
dignity and social standing. To justify the distinction between their own situation and
mental illness, our interviewees rejected the idea that they had an ‘illness’ as such,
detached themselves from any formal diagnosis using statements such as ‘that’s what
the doctor calls it’ or, in some cases, they avoided seeking medical help altogether.

For people like Jenna, who embraced a neurobiological explanation for their problems,
this created a conundrum, which many of them resolved by creating a separate
classification for their own experience – what I have called a ‘third condition’. The
people we spoke to did not give this ‘condition’ a name or explicit meaning. Rather it
emerged in the rhetorical space opened up by the way they framed their personal
https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 2/6
31.12.2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

struggle, distinguishing it from mental illness, on the one hand, and normality, on the
other.

Consider the perspective of another of our interviewees, a young woman I’ll call Piper,
who had been diagnosed with depression. When ‘you think mental illness’, according to
Piper, ‘you think schizophrenia and crazy people, and I’m not crazy, I just get really
nervous.’ In making this distinction, interviewees like Piper and Jenna not only claimed
that they were less impaired than the seriously mentally ill, they also insisted that their
experience was categorically different. Piper said of herself that biologically something
is just ‘a little off’. She has ‘too little or too much or whatever it is that makes you have
these issues’. Distinct from ‘crazy people’, she has control over her mind and her story.
All she needs is a little pill. Yet at the same time, her ‘condition’ is also different from the
mundane challenges that normal people might face. Piper was adamant that her
nervousness in social situations is different from ordinary shyness. Her taking of
medication is warranted. She has a third condition caused by an ‘imbalance’.

The ‘crazy people’ are shadowy, depersonalised figures –


the damaged, uncontrolled ‘other’

To reconcile their perspective with the fact that they had received a psychiatric
diagnosis, many interviewees credited a medical professional or a confidant for
suggesting something like this third condition idea to them. Others emphasised the
ordinariness of their experience and compared it to the types of routine problems that
regular physicians treat. As one interviewee put it, ‘all you have to do is take a pill’.

There are echoes here of the messages conveyed in direct-to-consumer advertising for
psychiatric medication. I’ve analysed the content of these adverts and found that
distress, as listed in symptoms and portrayed in the patients’ stories, is often presented
as a ‘real medical condition’ and yet unlike mental illness. The ads contain no references
to psychiatrists or to the diagnostic manual of mental disorders from the American
Psychiatric Association (APA), nor use of phrases such as ‘mental illness’ or ‘mental
disorder’, and no depictions of those affected as anything but productive and successful
citizens. In the words of our interviewees and in the advertising messages, the ‘crazy
people’ are shadowy, depersonalised figures – the damaged, uncontrolled ‘other’ against
whom the implicit comparisons are being drawn.

The views we encountered in our interviews are consistent with national surveys of
public opinion about mental health. Today, with respect to mental health problems, the
https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 3/6
31.12.2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

lay public much more freely endorses biological causes, the seeking of medical help and
the use of psychoactive medications than in the past. Their views have converged with
the biological perspectives long promoted in public campaigns to reduce the stigma of
mental illness. According to the American anti-stigma organisation Bring Change to
Mind, for instance: ‘The fact is, a mental illness is a disorder of the brain – your body’s
most important organ.’ Among anti-stigma researchers and activists, the evolution in
public attitudes toward biological psychiatry has been celebrated as a sign that the
public has at long last become ‘literate’, with a ‘more scientific’ even ‘sophisticated’
understanding of mental illness.

In promoting a biogenetic causal theory, anti-stigma campaigners – as well as


psychiatrists, the popular media, and others – hoped to convince people that mental
illnesses are ‘just like’ other chronic physical ailments, such as ‘heart disease or
diabetes’, to quote the APA, and could be medically addressed. This approach would, in
turn, alleviate mental illness stigma and foster tolerance by reducing the (allegedly
common) tendency to hold sufferers responsible for their condition. The result, it was
confidently believed, would promote treatment optimism and increased help-seeking.

But something unexpected happened. The public embrace of neurobiology has not led
to a more benevolent orientation toward the mentally ill. Rather, according to an
editorial in the Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience in 2015, ‘well-conducted studies
have concluded, almost uniformly’ that the stigma-reduction strategy of recent decades,
informed by ‘biogenetic attribution of all mental disorders’, has ‘not only not worked,
but also may have worsened public attitudes and behaviour toward those with mental
illnesses’. Such studies have shown that the growing endorsement of biological
causation has continued to exacerbate the stigmatising of mental illness among the
general public, patients and professionals.

What accounts for this paradoxical result?

I believe that our interviews reveal a crucial reason. While people accepted a
neurobiological explanation for their problems, they struggled against the
dehumanising notion that their thoughts, feelings or behaviour were mechanistically
caused. Drawing on clinicians, drug ads and the popular media, the caricature of the
seriously mentally ill served as a pivotal image by which to contrast and affirm their own
control and self-determination. Millions of Americans – and countless others in Western
countries – likely find themselves sharing this or a similar perspective.

https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 4/6
31.12.2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

The idea that one’s distress is primarily caused by a


neurochemical deficiency that can be corrected by a
drug is a fiction

As I argue in my book Chemically Imbalanced (2020), to defeat this othering and reduce
stigma, clinical practice needs to move away from biogenetic causal language.
Psychiatric research doesn’t support the notion of simple cause and effect in mental
health, instead uncovering a far more complex and indeterminate picture of
vulnerabilities. There is no evidence to justify the continued promotion of one-
dimensional theories such as ‘chemical imbalance’. Nor does the beneficial use of
psychiatric medicines require it. In fact, their precise mechanism of action and relation
to troublesome experience remains a mystery. It would be more truthful for mental
health professionals and public health campaigns to acknowledge this.

It’s true, as interviewees such as Jenna made clear, that patients often find biogenetic
language appealing. It provides a way to establish their suffering as both tangible and
unfeigned, and it offers a simple account and positive prognosis for their struggles.
However, the very notion that to be ‘real’, mental suffering must equate with some kind
of physiological malfunction is deeply misguided. Dropping this reductionist premise
could restore the deeper questions of phenomenological experience and meaning. What
makes the difficult experience feel the way it does? How does the experience induce
shame or anxiety or disappointment? How are people’s experiences connected to their
relations with others and the world around them, and how are these relations disrupted?

The idea that one’s distress is primarily caused by a neurochemical deficiency that can
be corrected by a drug is a fiction. The idea that it explains first-person experience or
that it offers some exemption from responsibility is a fiction as well. As the interviews
made clear, people adopted the biogenetic account because they thought it was based in
science. The interviews also show how this account undermines the kinds of
introspection and self-examination that can lead to meaningful self-knowledge.

Mental health treatment needs to re-engage with the language of persons. This means
suspending the detached, third-person stance toward patients, and attending to their
actual experience and circumstances. And it means encouraging patients themselves to
avoid this stance and draw on the normal ways that people make sense of their emotions
and actions. In this everyday language, we intuitively refer to intentions and desires and
a person’s reasons for thinking, acting or feeling as they do. When explaining difficult

https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 5/6
31.12.2020 Let’s avoid talk of ‘chemical imbalance’: it’s people in distress | Psyche Ideas

experience, we don’t ordinarily speak of causation or mechanisms but presuppose of our


own action and that of others some degree of freedom and control, and we pay attention
to relationships, history and social context, such as adverse events, confounding
circumstances or unrealised dreams. Even episodes of psychopathology can be explored
in the language of persons, as when psychologists speak of hallucinations, delusions,
compulsions and so on. Unlike the mechanical picture of a brain misfiring, this language
permits a form of understanding that can bring unusual and challenging mental states
into conversation with a person’s relation to the world.

Recovering this interpretive conversation means sharply circumscribing if not dropping


biogenetic talk. It means seeking understanding, which is what people dealing with
emotional suffering, like those we interviewed, yearn for. An understanding as persons,
embodied and situated in a life-world. An understanding that is the enemy of fear and
requires no othering.

14 JULY 2020

https://psyche.co/ideas/lets-avoid-talk-of-chemical-imbalance-its-people-in-distress 6/6

S-ar putea să vă placă și